Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Essay

Plant blindness and the implications for plant


conservation
Mung Balding∗ and Kathryn J.H. Williams† ¶

Office of Environmental Programs, University of Melbourne, Walter Boas Building, Parkville 3010, Australia
†School of Ecosystem and Forest Sciences, University of Melbourne, Baldwin Spencer Building, Parkville 3010, Australia

Abstract: Plant conservation initiatives lag behind and receive considerably less funding than animal
conservation projects. We explored a potential reason for this bias: a tendency among humans to neither
notice nor value plants in the environment. Experimental research and surveys have demonstrated higher
preference for, superior recall of, and better visual detection of animals compared with plants. This bias
has been attributed to perceptual factors such as lack of motion by plants and the tendency of plants to
visually blend together but also to cultural factors such as a greater focus on animals in formal biological
education. In contrast, ethnographic research reveals that many social groups have strong bonds with plants,
including nonhierarchical kinship relationships. We argue that plant blindness is common, but not inevitable.
If immersed in a plant-affiliated culture, the individual will experience language and practices that enhance
capacity to detect, recall, and value plants, something less likely to occur in zoocentric societies. Therefore,
conservation programs can contribute to reducing this bias. We considered strategies that might reduce this
bias and encourage plant conservation behavior. Psychological research demonstrates that people are more
likely to support conservation of species that have human-like characteristics and that support for conservation
can be increased by encouraging people to practice empathy and anthropomorphism of nonhuman species.
We argue that support for plant conservation may be garnered through strategies that promote identification
and empathy with plants.

Keywords: flora appreciation, human dimensions of conservation, public support, zoo chauvinism
Las Implicaciones de Ignorar a las Plantas en su Conservación
Resumen: Las iniciativas de conservación de plantas se quedan atrás y reciben considerablemente menos
financiamiento que los proyectos de conservación de animales. Exploramos una posible razón de esta pref-
erencia: una tendencia entre los humanos a no tomar en cuenta ni valorar a las plantas en el ambiente. La
investigación experimental y los censos han demostrado una mayor preferencia por, una memoria superior
por y una mejor detección visual de los animales en comparación con las plantas. Este sesgo se ha atribuido a
factores de percepción como la falta de movimiento de las plantas y la tendencia de las plantas a combinarse
entre sı́, pero también se atribuye a factores culturales como un mayor enfoque sobre los animales en la
educación biológica formal. En contraste, la investigación etnográfica revela que muchos grupos sociales
tienen lazos fuertes con las plantas, incluyendo relaciones no-jerárquicas de parentesco. Argumentamos que
ignorar a las plantas es común, pero no es inevitable. Si se está inmerso en una cultura afiliada con las
plantas, el individuo vivirá lenguajes y prácticas que incrementan la capacidad de detectar, recordar y
valorar a las plantas, algo menos probable de ocurrir en las sociedades zoocéntricas. Por esto, los programas
de conservación pueden contribuir a reducir este sesgo. Consideramos estrategias que podrı́an reducir este
sesgo y fomentar el comportamiento de conservación de plantas. La investigación psicológica demuestra que
las personas tienen mayor probabilidad de apoyar a la conservación de las especies que tienen caracterı́sticas
humanas y que el apoyo hacia la conservación puede incrementarse si se alienta a las personas a practicar
la empatı́a y el antropomorfismo de especies –humanas. Argumentamos que el apoyo para la conservación
de las plantas puede obtenerse por medio de estrategias que promuevan la identificación con y la empatı́a
hacia las plantas.

¶Address correspondence to K. Williams, email kjhw@unimelb.edu.au


Paper submitted December 7, 2015; revised manuscript accepted April 11, 2016.
1192
Conservation Biology, Volume 30, No. 6, 1192–1199

C 2016 Society for Conservation Biology
DOI: 10.1111/cobi.12738
Balding & Williams 1193

Palabras Clave: apoyo público, apreciación de la flora, chovinismo en los zoológicos, dimensiones humanas de
la conservación

Introduction Assessing the Evidence for Plant Blindness

Conservation initiatives are biased against plants and Research concerning human–plant relationships is under-
toward mammals and birds (Metrick & Weitzman taken across a range of disciplines, and parallel lines of
1996; Martin-Lopez et al. 2011; Havens et al. 2014), work present contrasting perspectives on the problem
particularly well-studied large-bodied mammal species of plant blindness. Research from psychology and sci-
(Sitas et al. 2009) with forward facing eyes (Smith et al. ence education supports the plant-blindness hypothesis,
2012). The extent of the conservation bias against plants whereas anthropological research points to some soci-
is highlighted by data from the United States showing eties holding strong bonds with plants. We investigated
that “despite plants comprising the majority of the federal whether preference for animals over plants is inevitable.
endangered species list (57%), in 2011 they received less A number of studies provide evidence that humans
than 3.86% of federal endangered species expenditures” have higher preference, superior recall, and visual de-
(Havens et al. 2014:8), a pattern repeated in other nations tection of animals relative to plants. Baird et al. (1984)
(Martin-Lopez et al. 2009; Laycock et al. 2011). Concur- and Wandersee (1986) both surveyed secondary school
rent with this is a dramatic decline in botanical degree students in the United States (sample sizes of 1855 and
programs and courses, a major shortage of government- 136 students, respectively) to test preferences for science
employed botanists, and chronic underfunding of plant topics and found clear preference for studying zoological
conservation in relation to animal conservation (Kramer over botanical topics. Kinchin (1999) tested preferences
et al. 2010). The ongoing loss of plant diversity is ne- of 162 students (U.K. residents) for specimens of an un-
glected by politicians and the media and insufficient re- familiar plant and animal in the classroom and found an
sources are allocated to reverse this trend (Cires et al. overwhelming preference for animal over plant speci-
2013). mens. Schussler and Olzak (2008) compared recall of
Although there are perhaps multiple reasons for this pairs of animals and plants with equal familiarity among
bias in funding and support, human appreciation of 124 psychology students and 203 botany students at a
plants is likely to play a critical role. There is con- U.S. university. Students first rated the attractiveness of
siderable evidence that human perspectives on species plants and animals in images. Recall of these images was
and ecosystems have direct implications for their con- tested following a distraction task. Animal images were
servation. Landholders are more likely to manage rem- recalled significantly more than plant images, and this
nant vegetation in positive ways if they have a liking trend was maintained for both botany and psychology
for that vegetation (Williams & Cary 2002) or feel a students. Most recently, Balas and Momsen (2014) used
sense of connection with nature (Gosling & Williams experimental techniques to test whether plant blindness
2010). Students are more likely to donate money to is due to difficulties in seeing plants. They proposed the
conserve species where they have empathy for that phenomenon may reflect differences in visual memory,
species (Berenguer 2007), and people are more likely suggesting that plant images may be less robustly en-
to support conservation of species they find attractive coded than animal images. They tested this hypothesis
(Knight 2007). making use of a phenomenon known as attentional blink
Evidence that humans more readily detect, appreci- (AB). When 2 images are briefly presented (10–50 ms) in
ate, and support conservation of fauna compared with rapid succession, AB refers to the phenomenon whereby
flora (Bozniak 1994; Uno 1994; Wandersee & Schussler detection of the first target image (T1) compromises the
2001) raises important challenges for plant conservation. ability to detect the second target image (T2) due to in-
Evidence of plant blindness or zoocentrism has been pre- sufficient visual resources. Plant and animal images were
sented across a range of studies from the fields of plant used as T1 targets to measure the impact on detection
biology, psychology, and science education (Wandersee of unrelated T2 images of water. In 640 trials with 24
& Schussler 2001; Hershey 2002; Lindemann-Matthies U.S. female psychology students, detection of animals
2005; Clary & Wandersee 2011), but the implications was significantly higher than plants, and T1 false alarm
of this bias for conservation has been given little atten- rates for plants were significantly higher than animals.
tion. We explored the evidence for this bias and theo- The authors concluded that plant images are harder to
retical accounts of its psychological and cultural basis. detect than animal images.
We drew on our findings to explore the contention At the same time, there is a substantial body of ethno-
that plant appreciation can be promoted through pro- graphic research demonstrating that many individuals
grams that help people identify and empathize with and social groups have strong bonds with plants, includ-
plants. ing some Aboriginal Australian, Native North American,

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016
1194 Plant Blindness

and Maori cultural groups (Descola 2009; Hall 2011). Reasons for Differential Appreciation of Plants and Animals
Many Indigenous Australians have a nonhierarchical kin-
The potential to successfully challenge plant blindness
ship relationship with plants that recognizes human–
depends partly on the causes of observed biases and
plant difference but also a shared ancestry (Rose 1992).
determining whether these factors are open to change.
These kinship relationships have obligations of “respon-
We assessed potential psychological and social forces
sibility, solidarity and care” for plants while recognizing
that contribute to plant blindness or zoocentrism among
the necessity of plant use whereby “killing and harm takes
many social groups. We argue that although plant blind-
place within structure, balanced, and reciprocal relation-
ness has a foundation in biologically based patterns of per-
ships” (Hall 2011:112). People belong to clans based on
ception and cognition, cultural factors play a significant
their lines of descent, whereas particular plant and an-
role in determining whether a given individual notices
imal species are “assigned to clans due to the actions
and values plants.
of the Spirit Ancestors” (Bradley et al. 2006:10). Thus,
Some researchers attribute plant blindness primarily
kinship relationship with certain plant species will differ
to biologically based visual, cognitive, and functional
among individuals depending on which clan the individ-
processes shared across all humans. Wandersee and
ual belongs to, and this kinship will be used in relating
Schussler (2001) suggest that because plants generally do
to the plant species. For example, in the Yanyuwa com-
not move, grow close together, and often have uniform
munity, an individual identified a pandanus palm (Pan-
color, they blend together visually and thus are simply
danus spiralus) as “my mother’s mother’s brother” and
not seen. Evolutionary psychologists suggest that greater
a grey mangrove (Avicennia marina) as “my husband”
attendance to animals over plants may be related to the
(Bradley et al. 2006:9). A similar relationship with plants
functional significance of animals for human survival. The
is part of Native North American and Maori culture; both
animate-monitoring hypothesis suggests humans detect
recognize kinship between humans and plants on the
changes in animals and humans more than plants be-
basis of a shared heritage (Hall 2011). Greater affiliation
cause ancestral hunter gatherers needed to monitor ani-
with plants can exist even in contexts where animals are
mals that may have harmed them or were a food source
plentiful. This is demonstrated by the Ankave of Papua
(New et al. 2007). Humans have historically had a higher
New Guinea who “draw close connection between trees
reliance on animal-based foods than plants (Cordain et al.
and humans” through use in ritual and everyday settings
2000), and it is proposed that this modern trait of greater
(Bonnemere 1998:127). For example, the red juice of
attention to animals than plants is ancestrally derived
Pandanus conoideus has an analogical relationship with
(New et al. 2007).
blood (considered the main agent of bodily growth) that
These hypotheses are consistent with the patterns
informs practices related to maturation, health, procre-
of visual detection biases (Balas & Momsen 2014) and
ation, and male initiation (Bonnemere 1998). Numer-
with evidence that there is a tendency for children to
ous other social groups in North and South America,
recognize nonhuman animals as living earlier than they
Siberia, and Southeast Asia maintain human-like relation-
recognize plants as living. Young children’s conceptions
ships with plants (e.g., friendship, exchange, seduction)
of the natural world are initially anthropocentric (Carey
(Descola 2009).
1985), and they struggle with the concept of plants being
These different lines of research may appear contradic-
alive (Richards & Siegler 1984, 1986; Bebbington 2005).
tory, but a more coherent picture emerges when differ-
By around 10 years old, most children have expanded
ences across discipline, purpose, methods, and popula-
their notion of living things (Carey 1985) from people to
tions of interest are considered. Evidence demonstrating
animals and finally plants (Yorek et al. 2009). Motion has
differential preference and recall for animals over plants
been shown to be a determinant of judging whether a
is grounded in psychological approaches that aggregate
stimulus is alive (Richards & Siegler 1986), giving further
trends at a population level and set out to test specific
weight to this interpretation of the data.
biases in perception. Evidence of strong affiliation with
Other researchers highlight social and cultural fac-
plants is grounded in intensive fieldwork seeking to pro-
tors that may contribute to biases within social groups.
vide an in-depth understanding of practices within a par-
Some researchers attribute poor knowledge of plants
ticular community or social group. Taken together, we in-
(Bebbington 2005; Gatt et al. 2007; Fancovicova &
terpreted these two bodies of work as providing evidence
Prokop 2011) to the focus of biological training. Uno
of general perceptual biases for animals compared with
(1994) found high school biology textbooks devote 14%
plants, suggesting that plant blindness may indeed be a
of their chapters and 20% of their laboratory exercises
factor underpinning biases toward conservation funding
to plants and botanical topics. The term zoochauvinist
and action for animal species, and also as evidence that
was coined to indicate the dominance of an animal focus
such bias is not inevitable because strong bonds with
by biology teachers (Bozniak 1994). Others suggest that
plants are clearly possible within many social groups.
biases against plants reflect much broader worldviews
This latter point is critical: conservation programs may
within some cultures and societies at particular times.
be able to play a role in challenging plant blindness.

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016
Balding & Williams 1195

For example, Hall (2011:6) argues that the “zoocentrism her cognitive representations of self” (Schultz 2000:393).
found in Western society pervades all cultural ideas and There is increasing evidence that people who view them-
actions toward the plant kingdom.” Plumwood (1993) selves as part of nature are more likely to exhibit proen-
and Hall (2011) argue that this bias has its foundations vironmental concern and behavior (e.g., Schultz 2000;
in a worldview that positions humans at the peak of a Mayer & Frantz 2004; Davis et al. 2009). We propose that
hierarchy: animals serving man, plants serving animals, a person’s capacity to connect with plants—to perceive
and the inanimate serving plants. In Western society, this human-like traits in plants or to identify or empathize with
construct can be traced from Plato, Aristotle, and the plants—may increase inclusion of plants within sense of
early rationalists through Christian and Cartesian thought self and thus appreciation of plants.
to the present day (Plumwood 1993; Hall 2011). How-
ever, evidence exists of earlier Western societies having
a different relationship with flora. The pagans of pre- Identification with Plants
Christian Europe recognized plants as kin and demon-
strated a relationship of care, solidarity, and responsi- Generally, support for conservation of species is partly
bility much in common with animistic cultures (Hall explained by presence of traits that are human like.
2011). A number of studies identify characteristics of animal
Pathways to plant blindness have not been precisely species that are linked with public support for con-
defined, but it is likely that both biological and cultural servation: aesthetic appeal (Knight 2007); size (Kellert
factors shape human-plant relationships. If biologically 1996); phylogenetical similarity to humans (Samples
based factors make it more difficult for humans generally 1986; Kellert 1996); capacity for feeling, thought, and
to detect, recall, and appreciate plants, an individual’s pain (Kellert 1996); and ecological importance and rar-
perception of plants will also be influenced profoundly ity (Czech et al. 1998). Across these studies, human-
by the social and cultural contexts they inhabit (Fischer like traits—including size, phylogenetical similarly, and
et al. 2011). Cultural processes such as language and sentience—explain a significant proportion of variation
practices affect the ways people develop and organize in support for different animal species. Similarly, pref-
knowledge of their environments, as well as the world erence for animals over plants has been shown to be
views and values they express in relation to other species due to human-like traits of animals, such as movement
(Bang et al. 2007). If immersed in a plant-affiliated culture, (Wandersee 1986; Kinchin 1999), capacity to eat, having
the individual will experience language and practices eyes and faces, communicating with sound, and the lack
that enhance capacity to detect, recall, and value plants, of these characteristics in plants (Wandersee 1986). This
something less likely to occur in zoocentric societies. area of research points to the potential for conservation
Plant conservation programs, therefore, have a role to programs to use strategies that highlight the ways plants
play in promoting experiences, language, and practices are in fact similar to humans.
that challenge zoocentric assumptions common to many The likely success of such approaches is supported
societies. by the observation that cultural groups exhibiting clear
affiliation with plants, appear to have a relationship with
plants via the attribution of shared or human-like traits to
Opportunities to Challenge Zoocentrism
plants. For example, the coconut palm (Cocos nucifera
If the observed differential appreciation and support for family) plays a significant role in the lives of the island
animal and plant conservation is not inevitable, how inhabitants of Nusa Penida near Bali (Giambelli 1998).
can conservation practitioners act to challenge zoocen- The tree and its fruit mark peoples’ lives from birth to
trism? Psychological studies consistently show that edu- death; a tree planted by a man for his child’s first tooth
cation or information alone typically does not result in ritual in effect becomes the child’s alter ego. The tree’s
increased conservation behavior (McKenzie-Mohr et al. life span then parallels humans and in rituals is clothed,
2012). Conservation programs will need more innova- presented food, and addressed as grandmother. Numer-
tive approaches. We evaluated three interrelated and ous terms apply equally to the coconut palm and the
promising lines of research to suggest broad strategies human body (Giambelli 1998). Common in the Amazon,
for promoting plant conservation. among the Indians of Subarctic Canada, and the Inuit
A unifying theme across our analysis is the impor- is the belief that plants and animals are persons pos-
tance of promoting opportunities for people to con- sessing a soul identical to the one they possess; thus,
nect with plants emotionally or cognitively. This is social relations are allowed with them (Descola 2009). A
grounded in emerging research on connectedness the- range of cultures and subcultures can provide insight
ory (see Tam [2013] and Restall and Conrad [2015] for and ideas for supporting greater flora appreciation in
an overview), which suggests that willingness to address a Western context “without fetishizing or appropriat-
environmental issues is related to “the degree to which ing Indigenous people and their culture of connection”
[a person] includes other people and nature within his or (Rose 1999:182).

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016
1196 Plant Blindness

Empathy with Plants evidence that anthropomorphism is used to promote


plant conservation.
There is increasing evidence that connection with nature There is reason for caution in suggesting anthropo-
can change through the practice of empathy (Schultz morphism-based strategies for promoting plant conser-
2000). For example, Berenguer (2007) conducted an ex- vation. Hall (2011) argues there are dangers in sug-
periment demonstrating that empathizing with nature gesting similarities between humans and other species
can increase concern and practical support for conser- where these similarities are nonexistent. Wynne (2007)
vation of species. Participants were randomly assigned argues that anthropomorphizing animal behavior is an
to view photographs of a dead bird on a beach covered outdated Darwinian concept that has no place in con-
in oil or an area with 8 trees that had been cut down temporary psychology. Although clearly a potential con-
and were lying on the ground. Half the participants were servation tool, careful consideration needs to be given to
assigned to a low-empathy condition and were instructed current human interactions with the individual species
to remain detached and objective while viewing the trees and the anthropomorphic projections ascribed (Karls-
or bird. Remaining participants were instructed to em- son 2012; Root-Bernstein et al. 2013) because negative
pathize with the bird or trees, imagining how the bird conservation outcomes are also possible (Root-Bernstein
or tree felt and the impact of this experience. Partici- et al. 2013).
pants who empathized with the other species not only Application of identification, empathy, and anthropo-
expressed greater concern for its welfare but also rec- morphism of plants would be further strengthened if
ommended a higher level of funding allocation to envi- they were grounded in direct experience with plants.
ronmental programs. In both high- and low-empathy con- Direct experience may affect plant-related attitudes and
ditions, concern and financial allocations were lower for behaviors. Experiential outdoor education programs can
trees than for birds. Berenguer (2007:281) notes, “People have a positive effect on students’ environmental atti-
may identify more closely, at a cognitive level, with the tude generally (e.g., Skelley & Zajicek 1998; Bogner &
bird than with the tree, so the level of inclusiveness will Wiseman 2004; Lieflander & Bogner 2014) as well as
be higher in the case of the bird.” broad connection to nature (Ernst & Thiemer 2011).
Although animal advocacy campaigns often appear de- Children participating in outdoor nature programs in-
signed to trigger empathy with threatened species, inten- volving plants increase their appreciation and knowl-
tional use of connectedness or empathy-based strategies edge of plants (Lindemann-Matthies 2005; Fancovicova
is relatively rare in conservation programs (Zylstra et al. & Prokop 2011), and active gardening during childhood
2014; Restall & Conrad 2015). One potential exception fosters positive adult perceptions of the social and intrin-
is the Council of All Beings program, which uses rituals sic values of trees (Lohr & Pearson-Mims 2005). A number
and role playing in which participants give voice to the of plant-focused education programs have positively af-
concerns of nonhuman forms including animals, trees, fected children’s engagement with plants, although the
water, and rocks (Bragg 1996). The effectiveness of this impact of such programs on support for plant conserva-
program for people with limited existing commitment to tion has not been explored. Notable Australian examples
nature conservation is unclear. include the Royal Botanic Gardens Melbourne Children’s
Garden, which is designed to foster children’s delight
in nature and passion for plants (Barley 2009), and the
Appropriate Anthropomorphization of Plants Stephanie Alexander Kitchen Garden Program, which in-
volves children at over 800 Australian primary schools
Anthropomorphism is commonly used in environmental in establishing an organic garden and in the harvesting
discourse (Epley et al. 2007; Tam et al. 2013) and may and cooking of produce (Stephanie Alexander Kitchen
be help strengthen public appreciation of plant species. Garden Foundation 2014).
People anthropomorphize to different degrees, and their Thought needs to be given to the kinds of activities
tendency to anthropomorphize nature predicts moral undertaken in direct-experience programs. For example,
care toward nonhumans in nature (Waytz et al. 2010). householders view their gardens as “part of the domestic
Anthropomorphism of nature enhances connectedness world” (Clayton 2007:223), which signifies an ecology of
to nature (Tam et al. 2013) and action efficacy, which prestige (Grove et al. 2006) in which sustainable prac-
mediate the relationship between anthropomorphism tice is of little concern (Clayton 2007). A survey of 466
and self-reported conservation behavior (Tam 2014). U.S. homeowners revealed only 3.4% believed creating
Numerous authors suggest the use of anthropomor- wildlife habitat and conserving natural resources was
phized narratives as a conservation tool to increase central to the meaning of the term gardener (Kiesling &
the charismatic representation and empathy for indi- Manning 2010). Gardening-based strategies are unlikely
vidual species (Chan 2012; Root-Bernstein & Armesto to effectively promote plant conservation unless such
2013; Root-Bernstein et al. 2013), but we found little human-centric assumptions are challenged.

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016
Balding & Williams 1197

Implementing Identification and Empathy in Plant urgency for action on redressing imbalances between
Conservation Programs plant and animal conservation is, however, clear. In the
face of this challenge, we call for new collaborations
Our analysis points to a number of practical ways to im- between conservation practitioners and social scientists
plement plant conservation programs grounded in pro- to develop ways to implement and systematically eval-
motion of identification and empathy with plants and uate approaches that encourage identification with and
anthropomorphism of plants: enabling direct experience empathy toward plants.
with plants, emphasizing similarities between plants and
humans, and using creative activities to promote empathy
Literature Cited
with plants. The likely effectiveness of these strategies is
consistent with theoretical and empirical evidence; how- Baird JH, Lazarowitz R, Allman V. 1984. Science choices and prefer-
ences of middle and secondary school students in Utah. Journal of
ever, practical application and evaluation is scant. We call
Research in Science Teaching 21:47–54.
for action research (broadly, research that involves exper- Balas B, Momsen JL. 2014. Attention “blinks” differently for plants and
imentation, reflection, and refinement of practices) and animals. CBE Life Science Education 13:437–443.
adaptive management approaches to better understand Bang M, Medin DL, Atran S. 2007. Cultural mosaics and mental mod-
their practical efficacy. els of nature. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
104:13868–13874.
Direct experience with plants that draws attention to
Barley R. 2009. A landscape for children: the Ian Potter Foundation Chil-
individual plants and species may foster the capacity of dren’s Garden at the Royal Botanic Gardens Melbourne. Australasian
individuals to look beyond the amorphous forest, and Parks and Leisure 12:15–17.
promoting discovery of individual plant species may be Bebbington A. 2005. The ability of a-level students to name plants.
critical to creating an empathic connection with plants Journal of Biological Education 39:62–67.
Berenguer J. 2007. The effect of empathy in proenvironmental attitudes
(Wandersee & Schussler 2001). There are many ways
and behaviors. Environment and Behavior 39:269–283.
this might be achieved that extend beyond the taxo- Bogner FX, Wiseman M. 2004. Outdoor ecology education and pupils’
nomic approaches typically used in botanical training, environmental perception in preservation and utilization. Science
for example, gardening to emphasize diverse functions Education International 15:27–48.
of plants, drawing activities to promote close observa- Bonnemere P. 1998. Trees and people: some vital links. Tree products
and other agents in the life cycle of the Ankave-Anga of Papua New
tion, exploration of plant movement, and highlighting
Guinea. Pages 113–131 in Rival L, editor. The social life of trees:
the human-like traits of individual plant species. anthropological perspectives on tree symbolism. Berg Publishing,
Emphasizing evidence-based similarities between Oxford, United Kingdom.
plants and humans is possible because there is increasing Bozniak EC. 1994. Challenges facing plant biology teaching programs.
scientific evidence of human-like traits of flora, such as Plant Science Bulletin 40:42–46.
Bradley J, Holmes M, Marrngawi DN, Karrakayn AI, Wuwarlu JM,
being alive, having intentional motion, food uptake, sex-
Ninganga I. 2006. Yumbulyumbulmantha ki-Awarawu—all kinds
ual activity, communication with neighbors (see Gagliano of things from Country: Yanyuwa ethnobiological classification.
[2013], for communication overview), and recognition Report series 6. Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies Unit
and favoring of kin over strangers (Dudley & File 2007; Research, Brisbane.
Murphy & Dudley 2009). Contemporary scientific under- Bragg EA. 1996. Towards ecological self: deep ecology meets construc-
tionist self-theory. Journal of Environmental Psychology 16:93–108.
standings of plants should be integrated in primary and
Carey S. 1985. Conceptual change in childhood. MIT Press, Cambridge,
secondary school curricula given past evidence of biases Massachusetts.
against plants in school texts (Uno 1994). Chan AAYH. 2012. Anthropomorphism as a conservation tool. Biodi-
Use of creative activities to promote empathy with versity and Conservation 21:1889–1892.
plants can be furthered through collaborations between Cires E, De Smet Y, Cuesta C, Goetghebeur P, Sharrock S, Gibbs D,
Oldfield S, Kramer A, Samain M. 2013. Gap analyses to support ex
plant scientists and artists and such collaborations could
situ conservation of genetic diversity in Magnolia, a flagship group.
play a crucial role in promoting positive emotional con- Biodiversity and Conservation 22:567–590.
nections with plants (Jacobson et al. 2007; Curtis 2011). Clary R, Wandersee JH. 2011. Our human-plant connection. Science
Creative activities such as story writing, drawing, imag- Scope 34(8):32–37.
ining, role playing, and rituals that include anthropomor- Clayton S. 2007. Domesticated nature: motivations for gardening and
perceptions of environmental impact. Journal of Environmental Psy-
phism can encourage active empathy with other species
chology 27:215–224.
including plants (Bragg 1996; Osbourne 2009). Cordain L, Brand Miller J, Eaton SB, Mann N, Holt SH, Speth JD. 2000.
Humans are inextricably linked to conservation agen- Plant-animal subsistence ratios and macronutrient energy estima-
das yet remain a relatively neglected focus in conserva- tions in worldwide hunter-gatherer diets. American Journal of Clin-
tion biology (Mascia et al. 2003; Schultz 2011). Although ical Nutrition 71:682–692.
Curtis DJ. 2011. Using the arts to raise awareness and communicate
theoretical and empirical research suggests the strate-
environmental information in the extension context. Journal of
gies identified here are likely to promote support for Agricultural Education and Extension 17:181–194.
plant conservation, the paucity of practical application Czech B, Krausman PR, Borkhataria R. 1998. Social construction, po-
of these strategies currently prevents effective evalua- litical power, and the allocation of benefits to endangered species.
tion (Zylstra et al. 2014; Restall & Conrad 2015). The Conservation Biology 12:1103–1112.

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016
1198 Plant Blindness

Davis JL, Green JD, Reed A. 2009. Interdependence with the environ- Lindemann-Matthies P. 2005. ‘Loveable’ mammals and ‘lifeless’ plants:
ment: commitment, interconnectedness and environmental behav- how children’s interest in common local organisms can be enhanced
ior. Journal of Environmental Psychology 29:173–180. through observation of nature. Journal of Science Education 27:655–
Descola P. 2009. Human natures. Social Anthropology 17:145–157. 677.
Dudley SA, File AL. 2007. Kin recognition in an annual plant. Biology Lohr VI, Pearson-Mims CH. 2005. Children’s active and passive interac-
Letters 3:435–438. tions with plants influence their attitudes and action toward trees
Epley N, Waytz A, Cacioppo JT. 2007. On seeing human: a three factor and gardening as adults. HortTechnology 15:472–476.
theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review 114:864–886. Martin-Lopez B, Montes C, Ramirez L, Benayas J. 2009. What drives
Ernst J, Theimer S. 2011. Evaluating the effects of environmental ed- policy decision-making related to species conservation? Biological
ucation programming on connectedness to nature. Environmental Conservation 142:1370–1380.
Education Research 17:577–598. Martin-Lopez B, Gonzalez JA, Montes C. 2011. The pitfall-trap of species
Fancovicova J, Prokop P. 2011. Plants have a chance: outdoor educa- conservation priority setting. Biodiversity and Conservation 20:663–
tional programmes alter students’ knowledge and attitudes towards 682.
plants. Environmental Education Research 17:537–551. Mascia MB, Brosius JP, Dobson TA, Forbes BC, Horowitz L, McKean MA,
Fischer A, Langers F, Bednar-Friedl B, Geamana N, Skogen K. 2011. Turner NJ. 2003. Conservation and the social sciences. Conservation
Mental representations of animal and plant species in their social Biology 17:649–650.
contexts: Results from a survey across Europe. Journal of Environ- Mayer FS, Frantz CM. 2004. The connectedness to nature scale: a mea-
mental Psychology 31:118–128. sure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of
Gagliano M. 2013. Green symphonies: a call for studies on acoustic Environmental Psychology 24:503–515.
communication in plants. Behavioral Ecology 24:789–796. McKenzie-Mohr D, Lee N, Schultz PW, Kotler P. 2012. Social marketing
Gatt S, Tunnicliffe SD, Borg K, Lautier K. 2007. Young Maltese children’s to protect the environment: what works. Sage, Thousand Oaks,
ideas about plants. Journal of Biological Education 41:117–121. California.
Giambelli RA. 1998. The coconut, the body and the human being. Metrick A, Weitzman ML. 1996. Patterns of behavior in endangered
Metaphors of life and growth in Nusa Penida in Bali. Pages 133–157 species preservation. Land Economics 72:1–16.
in Rival L, editor. The social life of trees: anthropological perspec- Murphy GP, Dudley SA. 2009. Kin recognition: competition and coop-
tives on tree symbolism. Berg Publishing, Oxford, United Kingdom. eration in Impatiens (Balsaminaceae). American Journal of Botany
Gosling E, Williams KJH. 2010. Connectedness to nature, place attach- 96:1900–1996.
ment and conservation behaviour: testing connectedness theory New J, Cosmides L, Tooby J. 2007. Category-specific attention for an-
among farmers. Journal of Environmental Psychology 30:298–304. imal reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the
Grove JM, Troy AR, O’Neil-Dunne JPM, Burch Jr. WR, Cadenasso ML, National Academy of Sciences 104:16598–16603.
Pickett STA. 2006. Characterization of households and its implica- Osbourne A. 2009. A meeting place: the science, art and writing initia-
tions for the vegetation of urban ecosystems. Ecosystems 9:578– tive. Current Science 97:1547–1554.
597. Plumwood V. 1993. Feminism and the mastery of nature. Routledge,
Hall M. 2011. Plants as persons: a philosophical botany. State University London.
of New York Press, Albany. Restall B, Conrad E. 2015. A literature review of connectedness to
Havens K, Kramer AT, Guerrant Jr. EO. 2014. Getting plant conversation nature and its potential for environmental management. Journal of
right (or not): the case of the United States. International Journal of Environmental Management 159:264–278.
Plant Sciences 175:3–10. Richards DD, Siegler RS. 1984. The effects of task requirements on
Hershey D. 2002. Plant Blindness: “We have met the enemy and he is children’s life judgments. Child Development 55:1687–1696.
us.” Plant Science Bulletin 48:78–84. Richards DD, Siegler RS. 1986. Children’s understanding of the at-
Jacobson SK, Mcduff MD, Monroe MC. 2007. Promoting conservation tributes of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 42:1–22.
through the arts: outreach for hearts and minds. Conservation Biol- Root-Bernstein M, Armesto J. 2013. Selection and implementation of
ogy 21:7–10. a flagship fleet in a locally undervalued region of high endemicity.
Karlsson F. 2012. Critical anthropomorphism and animal ethics. Journal AMBIO 42:776–787.
of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 25:707–720. Root-Bernstein M, Douglas L, Smith A, Verissimo D. 2013. Anthropo-
Kellert S. 1996. The value of life: biological diversity and human society. morphized species as tools for conservation: utility beyond proso-
Island Press, Washington, D.C. cial, intelligent and suffering species. Biodiversity and Conservation
Kiesling FM, Manning CM. 2010. How green is your thumb? Envi- 22:1577–1589.
ronmental gardening identity and ecological gardening practices. Rose D. 1992. Dingo makes us human: life and land in an Aboriginal
Journal of Environmental Psychology 30:315–327. Australian culture. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, United
Kinchin IM. 1999. Investigating secondary-school girls’ preferences for Kingdom.
animals or plants: a simple ‘head-to-head’ comparison using two Rose D. 1999. Indigenous ecologies and an ethic of connection. Pages
unfamiliar organisms. Journal of Biological Education 33:95–99. 175–187 in Low N, editor. Global ethics and environment. Rout-
Knight AJ. 2007. “Bats, snakes and spiders, Oh my!” How aesthetic ledge, London.
and negativistic attitudes, and other concepts predict support for Samples KC, Dixon JA, Gowen MM. 1986. Information disclosure and
species protection. Journal of Environmental Psychology 28:94– endangered species valuation. Land Economics 62:306–312.
103. Schultz PW. 2000. Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective
Kramer AT, Zorn-Arnold B, Havens K. 2010. Assessing botanical capac- taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues
ity to address grand challenges in the United States. Available from 56:391–406.
http://www.bgci.org/usa/bcap (accessed September 2014). Schultz PW. 2011. Conservation means behavior. Conservation Biology
Laycock HF, Moran D, Smart JCR, Raffaelli DG, White PCL. 2011. Eval- 25:1080–1083.
uating the effectiveness and efficiency of biodiversity conservation Schussler EE, Olzak LA. 2008. It’s not easy being green: student recall
spending. Ecological Economics 70:1789–1796. of plant and animal images. Journal of Biological Education 42:112–
Lieflander AK, Bogner FX. 2014. The effects of children’s age and 119.
sex on acquiring pro-environmental attitudes through environ- Sitas N, Baillie JEM, Isaac NJB. 2009. What are we saving? Developing
mental education. The Journal of Environmental Education 45: a standardized approach for conservation action. Animal Conserva-
105–117. tion 12:231–237.

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016
Balding & Williams 1199

Skelley SM, Zajicek JM. 1998. The effect of an interdisciplinary gar- Wandersee JH. 1986. Plants or animals – which do junior high school
den program on the environmental attitudes of elementary school students prefer to study? Journal of Research in Science Teaching
students. HortTechnology 8:579–583. 23:415–426.
Smith RJ, Verissimo D, Isaac NJB, Jones KE. 2012. Identifying Cinderella Wandersee JH, Schussler EE. 2001. Toward a theory of plant blindness.
species: uncovering mammals with conservation flagship appeal. Plant Science Bulletin 47:2–7.
Conservation Letters 5:205–212. Waytz A, Cacioppo J, Epley N. 2010. Who sees human? The stabil-
Stephanie Alexander Kitchen Garden Foundation. 2014. Pleasurable ity and importance of individual differences in anthropomorphism.
food education. Stephanie Alexander Kitchen Garden Foundation, Perspectives on Psychological Science 5:219–232.
Melbourne, Australia. Available from http://www.kitchengarden Williams KJH, Cary J. 2002. Landscape preferences, ecological qual-
foundation.org.au/content/pleasurable-food-education (accessed ity, and biodiversity protection. Environment and Behavior 34:257–
October 2015). 274.
Tam KP. 2013. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: Wynne CDL. 2007. What are animals? Why anthropomorphism is still
similarities and differences. Journal of Environmental Psychology not a scientific approach to behavior. Comparative Cognition &
34:64–78. Behavior Reviews 2:125–135.
Tam KP, Lee SL, Chao MM. 2013. Saving Mr. Nature: Anthropomorphism Yorek M, Sahin M, Aydin H. 2009. Are animals ‘more alive’ than plants?
enhances connectedness to and protectiveness toward nature. Jour- Animistic-anthropocentric construction of life concept. Eurasia Jour-
nal of Experimental Social Psychology 49:514–521. nal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education 5:369–378.
Tam KP. 2014. Anthropomorphism of nature and efficacy in coping Zylstra MJ, Knight AT, Esler KJ, Le Grange LL. 2014. Connectedness
with the environmental crisis. Social Cognition 32:276–296. as a core conservation concern: an interdisciplinary review of the-
Uno GE. 1994. The state of precollege botanical education. The Ameri- ory and a call for practice. Springer Science Reviews 2:119. DOI:
can Biology Teacher 56:263–267. 10.1007/s40362-014-0021-3.

Conservation Biology
Volume 30, No. 6, 2016

You might also like