Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

A Classroom Investigation: Can

Video Improve Intermediate-Level


French Language Students’ Ability to
Learn about a Foreign Culture?
CAROL HERRON CATHLEEN CORRIE
Department of French and Italian Department of French and Italian
Emory University Emory University
Atlanta, GA 30322 Atlanta, GA 30322
Email: cherron@emory.edu Email: ccorrie@learnlink.emory.edu
SÉBASTIEN DUBREIL STEVEN P. COLE
Department of French and Italian Research Design Associates
Emory University 721 E. Ponce de Leon Avenue
Atlanta, GA 30322 Decatur, GA 30030
Email: sdubrei@learnlink.emory.edu Email: spc@cloud9.net

This study examines the effects of video on cultural knowledge at the intermediate level.
Fifty-one intermediate-level French students viewed 8 videos. A pretest/posttest design as-
sessed long-term gains in cultural knowledge and in the learning of cultural practices and
cultural products from exposure to a curriculum with a video component. Eight postvideo tests
measured the students’ ability to retain information and to make inferences. A questionnaire
assessed perceptions of cultural learning. Results indicated a significant gain in cultural
knowledge with posttest scores significantly higher than pretest scores. On the short-answer
and free-recall portions of the 8 postvideo tests, the students’ ability to make inferences or
retain information did not improve significantly in either an advance organizer (AO) or a
non-AO condition. For free recall, scores were significantly higher for mentions of cultural
practices than for products. The students believed that they learned more cultural practices
than products. The results support using video to enhance cultural knowledge.

FOREIGN LANGUAGE (FL) EDUCATORS 1999; ACTFL Proficiency Guidelines, 1986, 1998;
have reached consensus that culture needs to be Standards, 1996, 1999). The current study sup-
an integral part of FL teaching at all levels (Lange ports an integrated model of language and cul-
& Klee, 2000). Formerly thought of as the fifth ture accepting the notion that cultural profi-
language skill, culture was viewed as information ciency can be defined as follows:
conveyed by the FL rather than as a part of the FL
itself. However, if one views language as social the process of acquiring the culture-specific and cul-
practice, culture becomes the core of FL instruc- ture-general knowledge, skills, and attitudes required
tion (cf. Hinkel, 1999; Kramsch, 1993, 1998). The for effective communication and interaction with in-
positioning of culture at the core of FL study has dividuals from other cultures. It is a dynamic, devel-
opmental, and ongoing process which engages the
led various professional associations to establish
learner cognitively, behaviorally, and affectively.
standards for the teaching and learning of cul-
(Paige, Jorstad, Siaya, Klein, & Colby, 2000, p. 50)
ture (AATF Culture Standards as cited in Lange,
In spite of the renewed emphasis on the central
The Modern Language Journal, 86, i, (2002) position of culture in FL programs, educators
0026-7902/02/36–53 $1.50/0 have yet to agree on how to integrate “language
©2002 The Modern Language Journal learning and cultural inquiry” in the classroom
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 37
(Maxim, 2000). On the side of instructors, linguistic medium because it places a high cogni-
Maxim expressed an urgent need to move be- tive demand on their working memory, they may
yond the dichotomies embedded in the curricu- limit their capacity to process cultural elements
lum: lower division versus upper division, form embedded in the same video.
versus meaning, and cultural fact versus cultural The current empirical study addressed the is-
inquiry. On the student side, the discussion of sue of whether viewing a video, an authentic,
cultural differences—or cross-cultural dialog— discourse-level activity, can help FL students
may challenge learners’ beliefs and values be- achieve the cognitive objective of acquiring cul-
yond the level of comfort (Lange & Klee, 2000). ture-specific knowledge. This goal is one compo-
To address these concerns, some theorists posit nent of the Paige et al. model (2000) that de-
that authentic texts are an excellent medium to scribes culture acquisition as a continual process
teach culture because they take the learner to the engaging learners cognitively, behaviorally, and
heart of the target culture (cf. Omaggio Hadley, affectively. By culture-specific knowledge, Paige
2001). In particular, some scholars praise multi- et al. were referring to the acquisition of knowl-
media technology for its ability to present stu- edge and skills pertaining to one specific culture
dents with long stretches of oral discourse em- (e.g., French, or Francophone culture). In the
bedded in a rich visual context. The rationale current study, the operational definition of cul-
behind the extensive use of multimedia texts is ture-specific knowledge included cultural under-
that in addition to stimulating students’ intrinsic standing, as well as the accumulation of specific
interests, they can also simulate authentic and knowledge. We distinguished between the reten-
up-to-date linguistic and cultural exchanges en- tion of cultural-specific knowledge and under-
countered in the foreign country. In other words, standing on the premise that cultural knowledge
video, and more recently the World Wide Web, denotes factual accumulation (e.g., the Mona
could introduce authentic sociolinguistic ele- Lisa resides in the Louvre), whereas cultural un-
ments into the classroom without the necessity of derstanding engages the student in reflective
travel (cf. Lafford & Lafford, 1997; Muyskens, thinking as well (i.e., the fact that the Mona Lisa,
1994; Shrum & Glisan, 2000). A plethora of in- an Italian work of art, is in the Louvre, a French
structional material has put this increased avail- museum, suggests a relationship between its Ital-
ability of authentic texts to use. As a result, videos ian creator and the French). The current study
and interactive videos (CD-ROM) accompany be- investigates whether a FL program with a video
ginning- and intermediate-level textbooks with component actually facilitates the gathering of
Digital Versatile Disc (DVD) and Internet-based specific cultural knowledge and encourages its
material in pursuit (cf. Bush, 2000; Chun & Plass, examination by intermediate-level FL students.
2000). As Paige et al. (2000) suggested, we realize that
In spite of the proliferation of authentic goals of cultural proficiency extend beyond the
audiovisual materials, other researchers question learning and understanding of one particular
their effectiveness and argue that they might in culture to include general knowledge (the ability
fact not foster cultural acquisition. Lively, Harper, to learn about a culture that is not one’s own) as
and Williams (1998) suggested that the “very es- well as issues of behavior and attitudes. However,
sence of the input text being imbued with native with a paucity of classroom research concerning
culture is what makes accessing the language in whether technology actually enhances the learn-
authentic documents so difficult for students” (p. ing and understanding of cultural information,
82). They urged teachers using authentic texts to we felt justified in executing the study below.
intervene or “mediate” the activity by providing Considering the vast amounts of funding and
students with support for new vocabulary, gram- time directed toward the development of tech-
mar, and cultural information embedded in the nology-enhanced FL instruction and the lesser
text. Chun and Plass (2000) included cognitive amounts allocated for evaluation, we felt the in-
overload among the potential disadvantages of vestigation to be of pressing concern.
the hypermedia environment. Along these lines,
Just and Carpenter (1992) emphasized, accord- REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE
ing to informational processing theory, that hu-
mans are limited in their attention and process- Empirical research on the potential of authen-
ing capacity. In other words, an individual’s tic video to improve FL proficiency is relatively
capacity to process information is finite and, thus, recent. There are studies on the use of video to
limited. Consequently, if students, while viewing improve students’ listening comprehension (Ru-
a video, focus their attention on decoding the bin, 1990; Secules, Herron, & Tomasello, 1992).
38 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
Related research compares listening comprehen- (with out-of-class multimedia activities) signifi-
sion rates for videos using various introductory cantly outperformed the control group (with in-
strategies (Chung, 1999; Herron, 1994; Herron, class multimedia activities) on both the cultural
Cole, York, & Linden, 1998; Herron, Corrie, prequiz and postquiz but that there was no signifi-
Cole, & Henderson, 1999; Herron, Hanley, & cant difference between the two groups in aver-
Cole, 1995). However, classroom research into age cultural gain made over the semester. The use
the use of video to improve cultural learning re- of gain scores in a setting in which the mean on
mains a relatively unexplored area. the pretest scores for the experimental group was
Several pertinent studies have assessed cultural already significantly greater than the mean on
proficiency and the use of video in FL programs. the pretest scores for the control group compli-
Martinez-Gibson (1998), for instance, reported cates interpretation of these results.
that FL students were able to recognize cultural Two additional classroom studies showed an
content of FL commercials when they were com- improvement in cultural knowledge over the
bined with cultural discussion activities before course of a semester for elementary-level French
and after viewing the video. She suggested that students (Herron, Corrie, Cole, & Dubreil, 1999;
the visual aspect of television commercials aids Herron, Dubreil, Cole, & Corrie, 2000). This re-
the students’ comprehension and awareness of search incorporated a within subjects design (re-
cultural information. We note here that the gen- peated measures) to explore the relationship be-
eralizability of these findings is somewhat limited tween cultural proficiency and video viewing for
given that commercials are a very specific genre beginning students. The researchers found that
of video and comprise a relatively small part of a first-year students’ overall cultural knowledge im-
FL instructional curriculum. proved significantly from watching narrative
Research in a FL elementary school program video. In these studies, beginning students of
(Hanley, Herron, & Cole, 1995; Herron & Han- French (FR101 and FR102) completed a pretest
ley, 1992) revealed that using a short video clip as at the beginning of the semester (a different pre-
an advance organizer (AO) to a written passage test was made for each class level). These tests
facilitated student retention of the text’s cultural revealed their overall knowledge of French cul-
information. This research with young children, ture. Half of the items related to little “c” culture
however, entailed the use of video as an introduc- and the other to big “C” culture, with both types
tion to a reading passage containing the same of items being randomly mixed in the test. Vari-
cultural information and did not assess the im- ous theorists (e.g., Brooks, 1968; Seelye, 1994)
pact of using video as the only culture source. have defined little “c” culture as representing as-
In another study, Kitajima and Lyman-Hager pects of lifestyle or patterns of daily living,
(1998) examined the effect of using a 1-minute whereas big “C” culture represents a civilization’s
silent video to teach students Japanese cultural accomplishments in literature and the fine arts,
information. The students, allowed to view the its social institutions, its history, geography, and
video individually and to comment on it as it political systems. The pretests served as posttests
progressed, were able to perceive cultural events at the end of the semester for both FR101 and
from this clip, a finding that suggests that silent 102. Both FR101 and 102 students scored signifi-
video clips enhance students’ discovery processes cantly higher on their respective posttests than
of culturally unique phenomena. This use of si- they did on the pretests. The investigators con-
lent video in the classroom to improve cultural cluded that in-class viewing of narrative video en-
proficiency is intriguing, but somewhat limited hanced first-year French students’ overall knowl-
because it neglects the fundamental audiovisual edge of the target culture.
component of video. In addition, both studies (Herron, Corrie,
Adair-Hauck, Willingham-McLain, and Youngs Cole, & Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000) inves-
(2000) reported the findings of a classroom study tigated whether the students retained more little
in which they assessed the power of technology to “c” culture than big “C” culture from the video-
improve beginning FL students’ cultural knowl- based instructional programs. Although there
edge. The participants were part of a technology- was a statistically significant increase in knowl-
enriched language learning (TELL) program edge of both categories of culture over the course
that included computerized reading and gram- of FR101 and FR102, the findings indicated that
mar programs, as well as a video component. The the students performed significantly better with
video presented brief scenarios performed by na- respect to the little “c” culture items than to big
tive speakers in France and in Guadeloupe. Adair- “C” culture items on both the pretest and the
Hauck et al. reported that the treatment group posttest. The investigators offered several expla-
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 39
nations for the better performance on the little shaped both its form and its content (Lange,
“c” items. Among these, perhaps the students 1999, 2000). The Standards also reconceptualized
were more interested in patterns of living than in the little “c”/big “C” dichotomy and proposed the
cultural achievements because they relate to following three categories: cultural practices,
them easily from their personal association (past products, and perspectives. Practices referred to
or future) with these events. Big “C” culture patterns of social interactions. Products consisted
might appear “high-brow” or removed from their of cultural achievements. Perspectives, the third
daily activities. Only in FR101, however, was the element proposed by the Standards, referred to
gain in little “c” knowledge significantly larger meanings, values, and ideas. In other words, the
than the gain in big “C” knowledge over time. Standards invited students not only to identify cul-
The researchers concluded that patterns of liv- tural practices and products, but also to demon-
ing, which are by definition recurrent, might strate their understanding of the differing per-
have left more of an impression on beginning spectives (meanings) underlying those practices
FR101 students than on FR102 students because and products. We acknowledge that, since the
of the FR101 students’ more limited linguistic 1996 publication, revised Standards have become
abilities. available (Standards, 1999). These updated Stan-
The researchers (Herron, Corrie, Cole, & dards reexamine practices, products, and perspec-
Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000) also assessed tives. Practices consist of daily life interactions;
beginning students’ ability to retain French cul- products refer to “formal culture,” including arti-
tural information from the viewed videos. In ad- facts (e.g., business cards); perspectives comprise
dition to the pretest/posttest instrument, stu- meanings, attitudes, values, and ideas (see Stan-
dents completed a postvideo viewing test dards, 1999, pp. 47–48). The expanded Standards
immediately after viewing each video. Whereas include a description of learning scenarios de-
FR101 students displayed no statistically signifi- signed to accomplish the cultural objectives set
cant differences between the type of culture they forth in the earlier guidelines. They also list stand-
remembered at the short-term level, students in ards for specific foreign languages: Chinese, Clas-
the FR102 sections did significantly better on lit- sical languages, French, German, Italian, Japa-
tle “c” items than on big “C” items on the nese, Portuguese, Russian, and Spanish. Whereas
postvideo viewing tests. Again the investigators we designed the current study prior to the dis-
attributed this discrepancy in findings to the pos- semination of the updated report, our investiga-
sibility that the FR102 students were able to pay tion supports definitions of culture in both Stand-
greater attention than were the FR101 students to ards publications (1996, 1999).
the finer details of cultural information embed- The current research also differed from pre-
ded in the videos for both types of culture, with a vious studies (Herron, Corrie, Cole, & Dubreil,
significant edge in favor of little “c.” 1999; Herron et al., 2000) in its concern with the
The investigators (Herron, Corrie, Cole, & effects of a video component on the cultural pro-
Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000) concluded ficiency of intermediate-level FL students. We
with suggestions for further research. They ac- chose the intermediate level for several reasons.
knowledged that they had only treated one type First, an investigation at this level would build on
of video genre, namely a narrative instructional the previous cultural findings with beginners de-
video, as opposed to a documentary or a longer scribed above. Second, Harlow and Muyskens
film. They emphasized that the participants in (1994), when outlining priorities for intermedi-
both research projects were first-year FL students; ate-level language instruction, noted that inter-
perhaps more advanced students would benefit mediate-level instruction has often been ne-
differently from viewing videos. Moreover, their glected in FL research. They stressed that a review
studies focused on student retention of cultural of literature on intermediate-level FL instruction
information and not on how well students revealed studies that primarily describe course
grasped the meaning behind the cultural events. design, focus on particular language skills, or sug-
For the purposes of the current study, we chose gest teaching strategies. Research rarely had in-
to recast little “c” and big “C” culture, as discussed cluded empirical projects. Contrary to this trend,
in previous research, in light of the Standards for they designed a project with a total of 1,373 stu-
Foreign Language Learning (1996). The Standards dents studying intermediate-level French and
specifically focused on culture and provided ex- Spanish at 12 universities to determine student
plicit direction for teaching culture beyond its views on goals and activities. Regarding student
former status as a fifth language skill. Culture was viewpoints on goals, the findings reinforced those
placed at the core of the FL curriculum and of Martin and Laurie (1993); that is, students’
40 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
learning goals in intermediate courses more tion 1, concerning whether intermediate FL stu-
strongly related to linguistic than to cultural ob- dents would improve overall knowledge of French
jectives. Yet despite this favoritism toward speak- culture from a curriculum with a video compo-
ing, listening, reading, and writing objectives, nent, we hypothesized that students would learn a
Martin and Laurie also found that 87% of the significant amount of culture over the course of
intermediate-level students in their study listed the semester. We based this supposition on the
that the cultural reason of “learning more about findings of Herron, Corrie, Cole, and Dubreil
the French way of life is” (p. 198) very or some- (1999) and Herron et al. (2000) that beginning
what important as to why they enrolled in inter- students exposed to narrative video learned a sig-
mediate French. nificant amount of culture over the course of a
The finding that intermediate-level students semester. For Question 2, concerning the type of
want to learn about culture suggests a need for and culture more easily retained by the intermediate
the importance of research on how best to intro- students over a semester, we did not formulate a
duce culture at this level. With respect to tech- directional hypothesis. Even though previous re-
niques, Harlow and Muyskens (1994) also re- search had shown that students’ overall cultural
ported that when students were asked their views knowledge benefited from video viewing, differ-
on the most effective classroom and out-of-class ac- ences between the current study and previous re-
tivities for achieving their preferred goals, among search did not suggest a directional hypothesis
the top-ranked activities were films and video. The concerning which kind of culture students would
current research is responsive to these positive stu- remember better. Previous studies (Herron, Cor-
dent views about video by investigating how well it rie, Cole, & Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000)
actually imparts information about culture. had used episodic videos that presented a sto-
ryline, whereas the videos in the present study
SIGNIFICANCE OF THE CURRENT STUDY were journalistic in nature (i.e., consisting pri-
marily of interviews with native speakers) and per-
In light of the review of literature and building tained to unrelated cultural topics (e.g., social
on previous research, this study addressed the welfare programs, unemployment, higher educa-
following six research questions: tion, politics). This difference in genre could af-
fect the kind of cultural information remem-
1. Does intermediate French students’ overall
bered. Also, our participants displayed a more
knowledge of French culture improve over the
advanced level of proficiency than those in our
course of 1 semester in a curriculum with a jour-
earlier research, a factor that also could affect
nalistic video component?
long-term retention of type of culture.
2. Do intermediate French students retain
Questions 3, 4, and 5 dealt with short-term
more cultural practices or more cultural products
learning and understanding of cultural informa-
over the course of a semester in a curriculum with
tion. Omaggio Hadley (2001) suggested that the
a journalistic video component?
testing of listening comprehension should in-
3. Do AOs to the journalistic videos enhance
clude both short-answer responses and other
students’ ability to draw correct cultural infer-
types of responses that allow students to respond
ences in short-answer tests administered immedi-
more freely. Therefore, in the present research,
ately after video viewing?
our assessment of how much students remem-
4. Do AOs to the journalistic videos improve
bered or understood after viewing each video
students’ retention or comprehension of cultural
combined data for short-answer with data for free
information on tests of free recall administered
recall. Research Question 3 focused on whether
immediately after video viewing?
the students’ ability to make cultural inferences
5. Do intermediate students retain more infor-
on a short-answer test administered immediately
mation about cultural practices or cultural prod-
after viewing a video would be enhanced by an
ucts on tests of free recall administered
AO. We did not frame a hypothesis because of
immediately after video viewing?
discrepancies in previous research findings.
6. What are intermediate French students’ per-
Lively, Harper, and Williams (1998) had pre-
ceptions of how many cultural practices and cul-
viously argued that “opportunities for meaning-
tural products are presented in videos and of how
ful processing of the input texts can be effectively
much of that cultural information they learn?
realized only with the help of the teacher’s inter-
We began the research with certain expecta- vention” (p. 83). In support of this proposition,
tions in mind. Questions 1 and 2 dealt with long- Herron (1994) showed that beginning students’
term retention of cultural information. For Ques- listening comprehension of narrative video was
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 41
facilitated by the presence of an AO. However, journalistic rather than episodic in nature, (b) in
some earlier research revealed AOs to be helpful including participants at an intermediate level of
to students at the lowest levels of linguistic in- linguistic proficiency rather than at the begin-
struction only (Hudson, 1982; Mueller, 1980). ning stage, (c) in incorporating an assessment of
Questions 4 and 5 dealt with retention of cul- the effect of an AO on students’ cognitive ability
tural information on a free-recall test adminis- to make inferences and to recall information
tered immediately after the viewing of each video. about a foreign culture, and (d) in assessing what
For Question 4, concerning the benefits to free students remember culturally with short-answer
recall of introducing the videos with an AO, we and free-recall testing.
had no hypothesis, due to lack of related re-
search. Question 5 emerged from analyzing the METHODOLOGY
qualitative data collected on the free-recall tests
and from finding that what students remembered Participants
from each video could be categorized into cul-
tural practices and cultural products. In light of Fifty-one students enrolled in four sections of a
previous findings (Herron, Corrie, Cole, & 15-week, third-semester French course (FR201) at
Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000), we antici- a medium-sized, private, American university par-
pated that students would recall more practices ticipated in this study. The study took place dur-
than products on the free-recall portion of the ing the Fall 1999 semester. Four different teachers
postvideo viewing tests and so hypothesized. taught the four classes (groups) of FR201. One
Finally, for Question 6, concerning student teacher was a native speaker of the language; the
perceptions of culture presented and learned, we other three teachers were nonnative speakers of
posited that the intermediate students would fa- French. Chi-square analysis revealed no statisti-
vor cultural practices on both measures. We cally significant differences between groups by
based this supposition on the findings (Herron, gender, ␹2 (3, N ⫽ 51) ⫽ 0.99, p ⫽ .80, or by
Corrie, Cole, & Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., university classification (first-year students vs. up-
2000) that a large majority of beginning students per classmen), ␹2 (3, N ⫽ 51) ⫽ 4.34, p ⫽ .23.
believed that a vast number of cultural practices There were no differences between groups in the
had been presented and that they had learned a number of previous years of formal instruction in
comparable amount. By contrast, less than half of French, F(3, 46) ⫽ 1.96, p ⫽ .13. Table 1 presents
the students thought that a large number of cul- these measures by group.
tural products had been presented or learned.
The current project built on former research General Classroom Procedures
to provide a better understanding of the relation-
ship between a curriculum that includes a video Classes took place on the same days for all four
and the learning of culture in the FL classroom. sections (Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, and Fri-
It distinguished itself from former research in day). Each session lasted 50 minutes. The instruc-
several respects: (a) in its focus on video that is tional program for all four sections was Bravo!

TABLE 1
Sample Student Characteristics by Group

Characteristics Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Group 4


Gender
Number of Females 10 7 7 7
Number of Males 4 5 6 5
University Classification
% First-Year Students 64 50 85 50
% Second-Year Students 14 8 8 42
% Third-Year Students 25 8
% Fourth-Year Students 14 8 8
% Other 8 8
Years Experiencea
Mean 3.40 2.80 2.92 2.64
Standard Deviation 1.09 0.83 0.86 0.50
aOne semester of college French was treated as equivalent to 1 year of high school experience.
42 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
(Muyskens, Harlow, Vialet, & Brière, 1998), a dents watched the video, which lasted approxi-
book-based course supplemented by a videotape mately 6 minutes. Students then took the
and a workbook with aural and written activities. postvideo viewing test (10 minutes). Afterward,
The Bravo! textbook presents students with a mix instructors collected the tests and did not discuss
of texts, dialogues, grammar lessons, and exer- either the test or the video with students. For the
cises ranging from written grammar exercises to rest of the class, the teachers reviewed chapter
communicative activities. It reviews basics learned materials to prepare the students for the follow-
in previous French courses and invites students to ing day’s chapter test. The teachers did not dis-
build toward new concepts. Each chapter con- cuss any of the targeted videos until after the
tains cultural readings designed to explore a vari- administration of the posttest at the end of the
ety of subjects that include both cultural products semester.
and practices. These readings and related activi-
ties purportedly sharpen students’ cultural aware- Target Videos
ness (Instructor’s Guide. Bravo!, 1998).
An accompanying videotape, entirely in The study included eight target videos: videos
French, contains a series of 5-to-6-minute-long 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9 (listed in both the order
segments in a journalistic format consisting in which they appear in the Bravo! series and in
mainly of interviews with French-speaking people which they were taught). Each of the four FR201
from France, Canada, and the French Antilles. classes watched the same video on the same day.
Each of the 10 segments presents two topics. The The first of the eight videos was randomly as-
topics reflect the themes, aspects of culture, and signed to either the AO condition or the non-AO
functions of language presented in the text. The condition for two French classes. That video was
scenes are recorded on location and present spot then assigned to the opposite condition for the
interviews with a variety of native speakers using other two sections. Table 2 illustrates the counter-
authentic, contemporary language and with a balancing of treatment across groups. Assign-
wide range of accents. The tapes are not easy, but ment of videos to conditions alternated for the
they are appropriate and well suited for the inter- four classes. When summed across participants
mediate level. and videos, the results are counterbalanced: Each
In FR201, students completed 9 of the 10 class had a total of four videos in each condition.
Bravo! chapters. All classes followed the same se- This counterbalanced design also compensated
mester-long syllabus that allotted an average of 4 for the fact that random assignment of students
days per chapter. They saw the supplemental to the four classes was not possible as classes were
video on the day prior to the chapter test. Chap- preconstituted by the registrar.
ter tests came from the testbank included in the
Bravo! program. Teaching Procedure
On the video viewing days, class activities in all
four sections were similar. The class hour began All four sections followed the same procedure:
with a brief conversational warm-up, immediately The instructor showed the video of the week with-
followed by the study; that is, students were ex- out interruption, in class, on the same day. That
posed to an AO to the target video or not, de- viewing was the first time any of the students in
pending on experimental condition. Next, all stu- the four sections saw the new video. In two of the

TABLE 2
Counterbalancing of Treatments across Groups

AO Condition Non-AO Condition


(AO ⫹ Video) (Video Only)
Video 2 Group A and Group B Group C and Group D
Video 3 Group C and Group D Group A and Group B
Video 4 Group A and Group B Group C and Group D
Video 5 Group C and Group D Group A and Group B
Video 6 Group A and Group B Group C and Group D
Video 7 Group C and Group D Group A and Group B
Video 8 Group A and Group B Group C and Group D
Video 9 Group C and Group D Group A and Group B
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 43
four sections, the teachers provided no introduc- ple-choice pretest at the beginning of the semes-
tory remarks about the video. For these sections, ter, prior to watching the first of the target videos.
the students watched the video segment in its At the end of the semester, after viewing the last
entirety without taking notes. In the other two of the target videos, they took a test containing
sections, prior to presenting the video, the the same items. The teachers informed the stu-
teacher administered a four-sentence AO in the dents that neither the pretest nor the posttest
form of a transparency set on an overhead projec- counted toward their final grades in the class. Of
tor. The sentences were in French. The teacher the 37 items on the pretest/posttest, 15 related to
read aloud the four sentences once while the knowledge of cultural practices, 15 to knowledge
students listened and read silently. Then the stu- of cultural products, and 7 grammatical items
dents watched the video segment in its entirety served as distractors for the purpose of reducing
and took no notes. Sample AOs (with translation) students’ anticipation of being tested on culture.
are found below. The pretest/posttest was based on cultural infor-
mation contained in the target videos. Given that
Advance Organizer Video 2 the students were not exposed to any of the target
La vidéo 2 présente . . . videos prior to taking the pretest, they had to
1. comment offrir de payer l’addition en France. answer the questions solely on the basis of prior
2. différentes activités possibles pour une soirée knowledge. When they took the posttest, they had
entre amis. been exposed to the target videos and, therefore,
3. des informations sur l’histoire et la structure to the cultural information covered. Below are
de la Sorbonne. examples of pretest/posttest cultural items.
4. des conversations avec des étudiants qui don- Pretest/Posttest Items and Answers
nent leur opinion sur la vie académique et sociale
à la Sorbonne. (The correct answer is starred. Practices
(Video 2 presents . . . and products categories are indicated in
1. how to offer to pick up the check in parentheses.)
France. 1. What is distinctive about la Sorbonne?
2. possible evening activities to do with (products)
friends. a. It is the most expensive university in
3. information on the history and structure France.
of the Sorbonne. * b. It is the oldest university in France.
4. students giving their opinions about the c. It has the largest number of students
academic and social life at the Sorbonne.) for a French university.
d. Most public officials graduate from
Advance Organizer Video 4
there.
La vidéo 4 présente . . . 2. If one orders a fresh fruit juice drink in
1. des informations sur le classement des hôtels a French café, what might the waiter bring
en France. with it? (practices)
2. des informations sur l’origine et le fonctionne- a. nuts
ment du Club Med. b. an extra glass of water
3. des interviews avec des vacanciers du Club c. little cookies
Med. * d. a carafe of water
4. le récent changement d’atmosphère ou de vie
The design was an adaptation of a study by See-
au Club Med.
lye (1968) dealing with biculturation of U.S. citi-
(Video 4 presents . . .
zens in Latin America. For Seelye, test items had
1. information on hotel ratings in France.
been sorted into two major categories: (a) items
2. background information on Club Med.
that were associated with the ability to function in
3. interviews with Club Med vacationers.
a society (little “c”) and (b) items that measured
4. how life at Club Med has changed.)
knowledge not overtly associated with functioning
in the society (big “C”). Falling into the big “C”
Testing Procedures: Pretest and Posttest category was “erudite academic knowledge” (See-
lye, 1994, pp. 216–217). For the current study, we
In order to permit assessment of the main ef- operationally defined practices and products in
fect of video on cultural knowledge over time, all the following fashion. Questions regarding cul-
students in the four FR201 classes took a multi- tural practices pertained to the ability to function
44 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
in a society (i.e., communication in a cultural con- had 3 minutes to write everything that they re-
text and social patterns and conventions). Ques- membered from the video without further speci-
tions regarding cultural products forced the stu- fication of the elements on which they should
dent to react to situations dealing with social concentrate. The teachers distributed the
institutions (i.e., education, government, trans- postvideo viewing test immediately after the class
portation, religion), history, geography, national finished watching the video. They asked students
products, literature, and fine arts. to record their answers on the test sheets. All
Each pretest/posttest item consisted of a stem questions were in English, and students wrote
and four possible responses. Each test item was their answers in English so as not to confound
worth 0 or 1 point. Incorrect answers received 0 their cultural understanding with their skills in
points; correct answers, 1 point. A participant’s writing in the FL. Each class had a total of eight
pretest/posttest score was computed by adding postvideo viewing tests. We provide examples be-
the correct responses and dividing by the total low.
number of questions. The “practices” pretest/
Video 2 Postvideo Viewing Test
posttest scores and the “products” pretest/post-
test scores were calculated by the same process. 1. For French university students, Paris and
la Sorbonne are closely related in ways
Testing Procedures: Postvideo Viewing Tests (Short other than geographical location. Why is
Answer and Free Recall) this the case?
2. Some French university students find
In order to permit assessment of their ability to that being a student is financially easy, oth-
recall information and make inferences from the ers stress the difficulties. What is the basis
videos, students in all four classes took the same for this discrepancy?
comprehension test immediately after viewing
the new video for the week. This test had two (Students write what else they remember
distinct formats: short answer and free recall. about the video on the back of the answer
Short-answer items were designed and adminis- sheet.)
tered first to students to reveal their under- Video 3 Postvideo Viewing Test
standing of the meaning (perspectives) of cul-
tural knowledge in the videos. The free-recall 1. What does the Sunday meal in Guade-
portion that followed allowed students, with no loupe tell you about family life in the Fran-
guidelines, to inform researchers of what they cophone country?
retained from the videos, be it facts or their inter- 2. In France, more and more couples are
pretations of them. choosing to cohabitate rather than to
For the short-answer section, students an- marry. What sociological factors might en-
swered two questions involving inferences based courage them to do this?
on analysis of information in the video. No short- (Students write what else they remember
answer test item asked for information that had about the video on the back of the answer
been included in the pretest or in the AO. The sheet.)
investigators constructed the short-answer por-
tion of the postvideo tests to assess students’ abil- A list of criteria was established for each short-
ity to reflect on cultural information in the video answer inference question. An answer received 0,
and to analyze it. This format allowed the investi- 1, or 2 points according to the level of adequacy
gators to assess the students’ understanding of in respect to the list of criteria. Each question
the relationship between cultural practices and received a separate score for the total number of
products on the one hand, and cultural perspec- points awarded over the total number of points
tives on the other. For example, for Video 2, the possible. For example, a participant who received
students listened to French students discuss life at 1 point for each question received a score of 1/2
La Sorbonne, including classes, jobs, and free (i.e., .50) for each question and 2/4 (.50) for the
time. On the postvideo viewing test, the students total postvideo test. A student who was awarded 2
were to explain financial constraints from the points for each question received a score of 4/4
perspective of a French student, not from their (i.e., 1.00) for the postvideo test. Each postvideo
own. After the students finished the two short-an- viewing test was scored blindly by two judges in
swer items stressing cultural understanding, order to establish an unbiased estimate of inter-
teachers in all four sections told them to turn the rater reliability. Both judges scored all postvideo
page to the free-recall test segment. The students viewing tests. The interjudge reliability for the
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 45
short-answer questions was 90%. Disagreements dent freely recalled by the total number of cultural
were identified and discussed, and the relevant utterances freely recalled by all students per video.
tests were re-scored accordingly. For example, for Video 2, students freely recalled
The material on the back of the postvideo view- and listed a total of 87 cultural utterances. The first
ing tests was scored along lines suggested by vari- student freely recalled two of those utterances
ous researchers (e.g., Bernhardt, 1983; Bern- and, therefore, received a score of 2/87 or .02 for
hardt & James, 1987) who recommended grading that video. That score was entered as an AO or
free recall in a somewhat objective fashion by non-AO score depending on whether the student
counting and awarding 1 point to any informa- had viewed the video in the AO or non-AO condi-
tion (in this case, cultural) that legitimately ap- tion. Next, the utterances of students were coded
peared in the video and was listed by students. as cultural practices or cultural products. A similar
This system has the advantage of giving students proportion was calculated. For example, of the
equal credit for every item they remembered as two utterances listed by the first student for Video
opposed to penalizing them for missing what the 2, one of the freely recalled items was categorized
teacher deemed important. under cultural products and one item under cul-
Prior to scoring the free-recall portion of the tural practices. The products score was computed
test, we determined that the students’ free re- as 1 divided by 87 (total cultural utterances freely
sponses for Video 8 did not contain any cultural recalled by all students for Video 2) or .01. The
information or indicate cultural understanding. practices score was computed in the same fashion
Consequently, we dropped Video 8 from the pool as 1 divided by 87 or .01. Final computations con-
of target videos available for scoring free recall. sisted of calculating mean scores for each student
As a result, for the free-recall measurement, 24 over all target videos in each condition (AO vs.
students were in the AO condition 3 times instead non-AO) and for each type of culture (practices vs.
of 4, and 23 students were in the non-AO condi- products).
tion 3 times instead of 4. By the same token, each
student had a set of two separate free-recall scores Testing Procedures: Cultural Questionnaire
for cultural practices and cultural products for
the same 7 videos. At the end of the semester, all students com-
Scoring the free-recall tests followed a specific pleted a cultural questionnaire. At the beginning
sequence. First, a designated investigator calcu- of the questionnaire, students found a definition
lated the total number of cultural utterances re- of cultural practices and cultural products that
called by the students per video. This initial clas- was designed to help them answer the questions.
sification of all utterances per video into This questionnaire included open-ended ques-
“cultural” or “not cultural” was cross-checked by tions asking for students’ comments about the
a second investigator. Interrater agreement was presentation of cultural information in Bravo! as
90%. Items that were not cultural were discarded well as comments about their acquisition of cul-
from further analysis. For example, investigators tural information from that same method. It also
did not count the response: “A family was inter- included items concerning their perceptions of
viewed in the video.” They did count the re- the learning of culture, as well as their percep-
sponse: “The video showed how close families in tions of how much culture they thought was pre-
Guadeloupe are.” Once these cultural elements sented in FR201 through Bravo! Finally, the stu-
were identified, the same designated investigator dents answered questions regarding whether they
categorized the elements into cultural practices had had previous experiences in France or pre-
and cultural products. This next classification was vious exposure to French (and, if so, under what
cross-checked by the second investigator men- circumstances).
tioned above, with a rate of agreement of 92%.
When disagreement occurred, the two re- RESULTS
searchers discussed and reached consensus con-
cerning how to count the particular utterance. Before analyzing data pertaining to the re-
Second, each student had two sets of two scores. search questions, we calculated item difficulties
One set represented how much a student freely re- for the pretest/posttest and the postvideo viewing
called in the AO versus non-AO condition. The tests. For the pretest/posttest, we calculated item
other set represented how much a student freely difficulty for questions regarding cultural prac-
recalled in terms of cultural practices and prod- tices and products separately. We ran t tests for
ucts. All individual scores were calculated by divid- independent samples in order to reveal possible
ing the number of cultural utterances that a stu- differences between mean difficulty scores for
46 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
the pretest/posttest items (15 items for each type and posttest performance, we computed Pearson
of culture). There was no significant difference product-moment correlations between pretest
between the mean difficulty for cultural practices and posttest performance for the total scores and
(M ⫽ .44, SD ⫽ 0.27) and cultural products (M ⫽ for each type of cultural information (practices
.51, SD ⫽ 0.21) items, t(29) ⫽ .88, p ⫽ .39. How- and products). For the total culture scores, there
ever, analysis of each item revealed certain items was a statistically significant correlation between
that had a difficulty higher than .90 or lower than pretest scores and posttest scores, r(40) ⫽ .40,
.10. We removed these items from the pre- p ⬍ .01. That is, students with higher scores on
test/posttest scores for all further analysis (i.e., the pretest tended to have higher scores on the
item number 1, products; item number 28, prod- posttest, and the students who did less well on the
ucts; item number 3, practices; item number 17, pretest continued to do less well on the posttest.
practices; and item number 23, practices). There was also a statistically significant correla-
We also assessed item difficulty on the tion between practices pretest and posttest scores,
postvideo viewing tests measuring cultural under- r(40) ⫽ .35, p ⬍ .05, but a nonsignificant correla-
standing. No item had a difficulty higher than .90 tion between products pretest and posttest scores,
or lower than .10. Therefore, we retained all r(40) ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .08.
items on the postvideo viewing tests. To assess the effectiveness of an AO on stu-
To assess whether students processed cultural dents’ ability to make inferences about culture
information (and therefore whether changes in (Research Question 3), we employed a t test for
overall cultural knowledge over the course of the paired samples for the 47 participants for whom
semester occurred) and to assess possible differ- we had complete data. A paired t test takes advan-
ences in long-term learning of cultural practices tage of the fact that each student performed in
and products, we subjected pertinent data to a 2 both the AO and non-AO conditions. The total
(pre, post) ⫻ 2 (type of culture) repeated mea- AO mean score (M ⫽ .49, SD ⫽ 0.16) was not
sures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Data for 9 of significantly different from the total non-AO
the original 51 participants were removed from mean score (M ⫽ .52, SD ⫽ 0.16), t(46) ⫽ 1.25,
the analysis because they had missed either the p ⫽ .22, effect size ⫽ 0.27.
pretest or the posttest. Regarding the effect of AOs on students’ ability
With respect to the main effect of video on to recall freely cultural information from the vid-
cultural knowledge over time, posttest scores (re- eos (Research Question 4), we again used a t test
gardless of culture type) were statistically signifi- for paired samples to assess differences in mean
cantly higher than pretest scores, F (1, 41) ⫽ proportion correct scores between the AO and
40.04, p ⬍ .001. The effect size (standardized the non-AO conditions. For the free-recall por-
difference between the means) for this analysis tion of the postvideo viewing tests, the total AO
was 0.50. Table 3 shows the pretest and posttest mean scores (M ⫽ .02, SD ⫽ 0.02) were not sig-
means overall and by type of culture. Regarding nificantly different from the total non-AO mean
the main effect of culture type, no statistically scores (M ⫽ .02, SD ⫽ 0.02), t(46) ⫽ ⫺1.3, p ⫽
significant difference (effect size ⫽ 0.07) be- .20, effect size ⫽ 0.
tween practices and products scores (regardless For Research Question 5 concerning the kind
of pretest or posttest) emerged. The Culture ⫻ of culture freely recalled after viewing each video,
Time interaction effect was not statistically signifi- we conducted a t test for paired samples. Mean
cant (effect size ⫽ 0.17). scores for cultural practices were significantly
To explore further the relation between pretest higher than for cultural products, t(46) ⫽ 4.53,
p ⬍ .001, effect size ⫽ 1.29. Table 4 presents the
mean scores for practices and products for all
TABLE 3 subjects on the free-recall portion of the
French 201 Pretest and Posttest Means (and postvideo viewing tests.
Standard Deviations) Overall and by Type of To determine the association between per-
Culture formance on the free-recall portion of the
postvideo viewing tests and the posttest, we used
Pretest Posttest
Pearson product-moment correlations. The
Practices .45 (0.15) .61 (0.16) .53 (0.13) postvideo free-recall scores were significantly
Products .46 (0.14) .56 (0.16) .51 (0.12) correlated with all performance measures on the
Total .46 (0.11) .59 (0.13)*** posttest. In other words, students who per-
***Posttest scores higher than pretest scores, formed well on the free-recall portion of the
p ⬍ .001. postvideo viewing tests (regardless of condition
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 47
TABLE 4 .41, p ⬍ .01. Background in French was also posi-
Free Recall Mean Proportion Correct (and tively associated with “practices” scores, r(42) ⫽
Standard Deviations) by Type of Culture for .42, p ⬍ .01. Given this correlational finding, we
Postvideo Viewing Tests
ran a 2 (pre, post) ⫻ 2 (type of culture) ⫻ 2
Type of Culture (background group) repeated measures ANOVA
with the between-subjects background variable
Practices Products
defined as either 2 to 3 years French background
Postvideo (n ⫽ 32) or 4 to 5 years background (n ⫽ 10).
Viewing Tests .02 (0.01)*** .01 (0.01) There was a statistically significant interaction be-
***p ⬍ .001. tween background and the pretest/posttest mea-
sures, F(1, 40) ⫽ 4.19, p ⬍ .05, effect size ⫽ 0.63,
with the total culture mean score for the 4- and
or type of culture) also tended to perform well 5-year background group increasing from 0.47
on the posttest (overall, for practices and prod- (SD ⫽ 0.11) on the pretest to 0.67 (SD ⫽ 0.19) on
ucts; see Table 5). the posttest and the scores for the 2- and 3-year
Regarding student perceptions (Research group increasing to a lesser degree, from 0.46
Question 6), 45 of the 51 students completed the (SD ⫽ 0.16) to 0.56 (SD ⫽ 0.15). The overall
cultural questionnaire. Of these students, 67% pretest/posttest main effect remained statistically
thought that a lot or a vast number of cultural significant, F(1, 40) ⫽ 44.46, p ⬍ .001, effect size
practices appeared in the videos, and 13% ⫽ 0.53. There also was a nonsignificant trend for
thought that a lot or a vast number of cultural a Pretest/Posttest ⫻ Type of Culture interaction,
products appeared. Moreover, whereas 31% of the F(1, 40) ⫽ 2.88, p ⫽ .09, effect size ⫽ 0.47. The
students believed that they learned a lot or a vast cell means appear in Table 3. In partialling out
amount about cultural practices throughout the the variability attributable to background, we ob-
semester, only 9% of the students believed that served a trend for the cultural practices scores to
they learned a lot or a vast amount about cultural increase more than the products scores overall.
products. To test differences between type of cul- Finally, statistically significant positive correla-
ture presented and type of culture learned, we tions between students’ background in French
conducted t tests for paired samples. Students be- and all but one of the performance measures on
lieved that more cultural practices (M ⫽ 3.67, SD the free-recall portion of the postvideo viewing
⫽ 0.83) were presented than cultural products (M tests surfaced. Students who had more French
⫽ 2.73, SD ⫽ 0.75), t(44) ⫽ 5.83, p ⬍ .001, effect prior to the beginning of the semester were able to
size ⫽ 0.98. Likewise, they believed that they recall more cultural information regardless of
learned more about cultural practices (M ⫽ 3.07, whether they were in the AO or the non-AO condi-
SD ⫽ 0.86) than cultural products (M ⫽ 2.24, SD ⫽ tions. Students with more background in French
0.98), t(44) ⫽ 4.55, p ⬍ .001, effect size ⫽ 1.07. were also able to recall more cultural practices and
Part of the information collected from students more cultural products information than students
pertained to previous exposure to French, pre- who had less French (see Table 6).
vious travel to France, and previous exposure to a No significant correlations were found be-
foreign culture. Prior French instruction was tween amount of French background and the
positively associated with posttest scores, r(42) ⫽ pretest scores. Nor were there significant correla-

TABLE 5
Pearson Product-Moment Correlations (n ⫽ 41) between Student Performance on All Measures of the
Free-Recall Portion of the Postvideo Viewing Tests and Performance on the Posttest

Posttest Posttest Posttest


Practices Products Total
Free Recall in the AO Condition .52*** .32* .52***
Free Recall in the Non-AO Condition .34* .29a .40**
Free Recall of Practices Information .49*** .28b .48**
Free Recall of Products Information .41** .44** .54***
ap⫽ .06, statistically nonsignificant positive trend.
bp ⫽ .08, statistically nonsignificant positive trend.
*p ⬍ .01. **p ⬍ .01. ***p ⬍ .001.
48 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
TABLE 6
Pearson Product-Moment Correlations between Student Background and Performance Measures

Performance Measures Student Background


Postvideo Viewing Tests (n ⫽ 46)
Free Recall in the AO Condition .34*
Free Recall in the Non-AO Condition .39**
Free Recall of Practices Information .37*
Free Recall of Products Information .36*
Postvideo Viewing Tests (n ⫽ 49)
Inferential Items in AO Condition .35*
Posttest (n ⫽ 44)
Practices .42**
Products .23a
Total .41**
ap ⫽ .14, statistically nonsignificant positive trend.

*p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01.

tions found between previous travel to France knowledge over the course of a semester in a
and test scores, or between previous exposure to curriculum with an accompanying journalistic-
a foreign culture and test scores. style video. At the onset of the semester, students
took a pretest on cultural information to which
DISCUSSION they would be exposed via the video segments
throughout the semester. At the completion of
Before drawing conclusions from this study, we the study, students took the same test (posttest)
stress that any implications should be interpreted and scored significantly higher than on the pre-
cautiously in light of the following constraints. test. These findings support the initial hypothesis
First, to isolate the effect of the technological that the viewing of the short video segments that
media on cultural knowledge, we had to limit as accompany the text, Bravo! would help their cul-
much as possible all teacher-related factors. We tural knowledge over the semester. These results
did not take into account teachers’ views regard- were also consistent with previous findings (Her-
ing the goals of FL education, their perspective ron, Corrie, Cole, & Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al.,
on the teaching of culture, or the degree of pri- 2000) that beginning students of French im-
oritization of culture in the classroom. With re- proved their cultural knowledge significantly
spect to the students, although we did investigate when exposed to a curriculum containing narra-
the effect of video on their cognitive skills, we tive video.
made no attempt to measure their motivation, It is important to note that we do not make the
anxiety, or behavior when using video technology claim that the video segments were solely respon-
as a tool for learning about French culture. Fur- sible for this improvement, because the structure
thermore, we did not consider the impact of po- of the study does not permit us to evaluate sepa-
tential cultural conflicts between students and rately the curriculum, teacher techniques, and
teachers (different nationality, generation, gen- the viewing of the videos. The findings suggest,
der, etc., or the fact that teachers are teaching however, that after a semester of exposure to an
another culture), which may constitute a likely intermediate French program that included
occurrence. Keeping the above limitations in video, students improved their factual knowledge
mind, we suggest the following conclusions in of French and Francophone culture.
response to the research questions. The fact that students in the present study im-
proved in overall French cultural knowledge
Question 1: Does intermediate French students’ could be at least partially explained by turning to
overall knowledge of French culture improve over the the correlational results between previous back-
course of 1 semester in a curriculum with a ground in French and pretest/posttest perfor-
journalistic video component? mance. We had found no correlation between
background in French and the pretest scores, but
Results of the current study indicate that inter- there was a significantly positive correlation be-
mediate-level college French students do im- tween background in French and posttest scores.
prove their French and Francophone cultural This finding implies that how much French the
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 49
students knew did not affect pretest scores but tapes can also be effective for building cultural
did affect posttest scores. The increase in scores, knowledge. Our research design did not allow for
however, was associated with the level of French teacher mediation or intervention to aid student
background. comprehension during the video viewing. This
The analysis of variance that included the back- constraint, however, did not appear to affect the
ground measure revealed that the gains in overall retention of cultural information, particularly for
knowledge of culture were greater for the stu- the students who had more background in
dents with more French background. Further- French. When preparing authentic videotexts,
more, ad hoc comments by students to teachers teachers should remember that explanation dur-
during the study revealed their perceptions of the ing the video could benefit the retention of cul-
difficulty of the videotexts. They expressed con- tural information, especially for the students with
cern that the videos were “very hard to under- the least amount of previous French.
stand.” It could be that the significant difference
in favor of an improvement in cultural informa- Question 2: Do intermediate French students retain
tion between pretest and posttest scores might be more cultural practices or more cultural products over
related to the students’ French instructional the course of a semester in a curriculum with a
background at the onset of the study. If indeed journalistic video component?
the videos were inherently difficult, it makes
sense that comprehension would favor the stu- With respect to the distinction between prac-
dents with more French in their backgrounds, as tices and products scores on the pretest/posttest,
our correlational and ANOVA tests indicated. the magnitude of the gains in cultural practices
Our hypothesis was reinforced by the finding scores was not different from the products gains
that student linguistic background was also posi- over the course of the semester. Also, practices
tively correlated with the ability to recall freely scores on the pretest and on the posttest were not
cultural information. Furthermore, student per- significantly higher than products pretest and
formance on the free-recall portion of the posttest scores. These results are not consistent
postvideo viewing tests was positively correlated with previous related research (Herron, Corrie,
with the performance on the posttest. This rela- Cole, & Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000) re-
tionship suggests that the more background in vealing that beginning students had statistically
French students had, the more cultural informa- significantly higher little “c” (practices) scores
tion they were able to recall from the video on a than big “C” (products) scores on the pretest and
short-term level (free-recall test) and the more on the posttest and that for FR101 students there
information they learned on the long-term level were greater gains in little “c” than in big “C”
(posttest). Given that student background was knowledge over the course of the semester.
not significantly correlated with performance on It is possible that the current research revealed
the pretest, it did not help the students with un- no such differences because of the nature of the
known cultural information. However, back- authentic videotext. The journalistic video seg-
ground did appear to help them retain new cul- ments of Bravo! do not present students with an
tural information once they were exposed to it in adventure as did the French in Action (1994) nar-
the target videos. rative videos used in earlier studies (Herron, Cor-
These and previous findings encourage teach- rie, Cole, & Dubreil, 1999; Herron et al., 2000).
ers to enhance the cultural aspect of the first- and Sometimes the video segments in the current re-
second-year FL college programs by using a cur- search presented French people around the
riculum that includes an authentic video compo- world talking about culture rather than demon-
nent. This message is especially important for strating it. This presentational mode, although
teachers in light of Harlow and Muyskens’ (1994) appropriate for the intermediate level, might not
findings that using video in the classroom was emphasize behavioral representations. In the cur-
given a high priority by intermediate-level stu- rent study, the students’ retention of cultural
dents and a rather low priority by instructors. We practices could have been negatively affected by
concur with their suggestion that instructors capi- the video’s possible lack of consistent behavioral
talize on students’ interest in video as a worth- representation of everyday cultural acts. This
while activity in spite of the time and expense omission could explain why students performed
needed to implement it effectively. Not only can equally well on practices and products items on
a video component build listening comprehen- the pretest/posttest.
sion skills as shown in previous research (Rubin, A further reason as to why we found no signifi-
1990; Secules et al., 1992), but the use of video- cant difference between kind of culture learned
50 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
relates to course expectations at the intermediate items correctly. Most important, student per-
level. Martin and Laurie (1993) found that when formance did not appear to be overly handi-
intermediate-level students were asked to priori- capped by short-answer items that required them
tize their course goals, they rated equally well to reflect on the perspectives underlying the cul-
learning about both popular culture defined as tural practices and products they viewed, which
“the French way of life” and hegemonal culture entails a higher level of thinking than knowledge
defined as “major aspects of French culture” (pp. of cultural facts. Correlational analysis disclosed
189–198). In their study, 86% of the students a positive association between student back-
indicated the importance of knowing some- ground and performance on the inferential items
thing/more about French life and customs, and in the AO condition of the postvideo viewing
84% of the students indicated the importance of tests. This result seems to indicate that, when
knowing something/more about some of the ma- prompted by an AO, students who had more
jor aspects of French culture. Martin and Laurie’s background in French performed better on the
practically identical response in students’ cultural short-answer, inferential portion of the postvideo
learning goals suggests a possible reason why stu- viewing tests than students with less French in
dents in the current research performed equally their background. This finding also suggests that
well on cultural practices and products over the students found the target videos difficult and that
course of the semester. the students with more French benefited from
We should note here that when a measure of the information given in the AOs.
French background was included in the type of
Culture ⫻ Pretest/Posttest model, a trend ap- Question 4: Do AOs to the journalistic videos improve
peared for cultural practices scores to increase students’ retention or comprehension of cultural
more than products scores overall. That is, when information on tests of free recall administered
French instructional background was taken into immediately after video viewing?
account, differences for the means in Table 3
were significant. This finding suggests that in- The administration of an AO prior to video
structional background relates to students’ reten- viewing did not enhance the intermediate FL stu-
tion of cultural practices. It could be that the dents’ free recall of culture embedded in the
students’ previous French programs emphasized videos. This finding was consistent with the result
cultural practices more than cultural products, above indicating that the AO did not improve
but given the available sample information, it is student performance on the more convergent,
not feasible to assess this possibility in any precise short-answer items of the postvideo viewing tests.
way. Future research that would more precisely This result could be an indication that AOs do
define the nature of students’ previous French not appear to enhance cultural proficiency for
background could help explain this trend. the intermediate-level FL student. Another possi-
ble interpretation would be that whereas the AO
Question 3: Do AOs to the journalistic videos treatment in this study (descriptive statements
enhance students’ ability to draw correct cultural summarizing the video’s topics) does not seem to
inferences in short-answer tests administered enhance cultural reflection, a different AO treat-
immediately after video viewing? ment could benefit comprehension.

Regarding short-term retention, the admini- Question 5: Do intermediate students retain more
stration of an AO prior to video viewing did not information about cultural practices or cultural
reveal a significant difference in cultural under- products on tests of free recall administered
standing between the AO and non-AO condi- immediately after video viewing?
tions. This finding is consistent with previous re-
search indicating that AOs are most helpful to Students recalled significantly more about cul-
beginning FL students (Hudson, 1982; Mueller, tural practices than cultural products on the
1980). However, a close look at the mean propor- open-ended postvideo viewing tests. The issue of
tion of correct responses on the short-answer in- why student free recall favored practices is not a
ferential tests (49% correct in the AO condition question directly answered by this research. It
and 52% correct in the non-AO condition) could be that aspects of daily life are simply easier
points to strong performance overall on test for students to remember than cultural products.
items measuring students’ understanding of cul- Or perhaps there might have been more in-
tural perspectives. On the average, students an- stances of cultural practices than cultural prod-
swered approximately 1 of every 2 inferential ucts embedded in the journalistic videos, which,
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 51
of course, would have favored the higher free items on the free-recall portion of the postvideo
recall of practices. Although we had no reason to viewing tests. Although the reason for this differ-
think that this imbalance of practices over prod- ence in recall remains elusive, when asked for
ucts was the case, in future research a detailed personal opinions or when given freedom of re-
content analysis of each video segment prior to its sponse, these students appeared to be drawn to
use might be warranted. Even without an exten- anthropological culture. Our results suggest that
sive analysis of the content of the source video, it teachers should remember this preference and
appears that the cultural practices in the video include cultural practices in culture activities.
segments were the subjects of conversation rather In conclusion, we would like to suggest direc-
than demonstrated actions. However, even with tions for further research. Future studies could
the relative absence of “culture in action,” when measure students’ motivation, anxiety, or behav-
asked to recall freely the most salient aspects of ior when using video to learn about culture. We
the videos, the students still mentioned cultural invite studies that will extend beyond factual un-
behaviors and values significantly more often derstanding in the directions of cross-cultural
than cultural achievements and institutions. sensitivity and subjectivity. Future investigations
One possibility for why the students recalled could explore the effects of video on the cultural
significantly more information about cultural proficiency of advanced FL students, especially
practices than cultural products on the free-recall with the current study’s finding that previous
tests but failed to perform significantly better on background in French can positively relate to cul-
cultural practices at pretest or posttest is worthy tural gain. The updated Standards (1999) empha-
of exploration. This difference in results could size that all three aspects of culture are inter-
find some basis in the notion that the free-recall woven and inseparable. In the current study, we
responses mirrored what students deemed as chose to delineate between practices, products,
memorable, in this case daily patterns of living, and perspectives, but we invite studies to explore
whereas the multiple-choice items on the pre- how they are interconnected. Researchers should
test/posttest were indications of what the investi- examine the advantages of interactive video deliv-
gators thought students should remember. When ered digitally to students via the Internet as com-
given a choice, students, in their responses, fa- pared with teacher-controlled, classroom video
vored elements belonging to the area of cultural presentations.
practices. This interpretation has support in the With culture constituting a core element of FL
study by Martin and Laurie (1993) in which they education and with language and culture en-
asked intermediate-level students to prioritize twined, we need to translate these principles into
reasons for choosing to study French. They deter- practice. This study highlighted one aspect of
mined that overall, nearly 87% of the students culture (knowledge), one learning approach
studied were motivated to enroll by the desire to (video viewing), and one type of classroom activ-
study the French way of life whereas 42% of stu- ity (an AO). We hope it will foster a chain of
dents enrolled to learn about a particular aspect research on how best to teach culture at all levels.
of French culture, such as art or history. A tenta-
tive conclusion is that what originally motivates
students about cultural study might be an indica- REFERENCES
tion of what they will freely recall most effectively
from authentic video. Adair-Hauck, B., Willingham-McLain, L., & Youngs, B.
E. (2000). Evaluating the integration of technol-
Question 6: What are intermediate French students’ ogy and second language learning. CALICO Jour-
perceptions of how many cultural practices and nal, 17, 269–306.
cultural products are presented in videos and of how American Council on the Teaching of Foreign Lan-
guages. (1986). Proficiency Guidelines. Yonkers, NY:
much of that cultural information they learn?
Author.
Analysis of the data indicated that the students American Council on the Teaching of Foreign Lan-
guages. (1998). Proficiency Guidelines for K–12
believed that more cultural practices than prod-
Learners. Yonkers, NY: Author.
ucts had been presented and that they had
Bernhardt, E. B. (1983). Testing foreign language read-
learned more about cultural practices than cul- ing comprehension: The immediate recall proto-
tural products. These findings from the student col. Die Unterrichtspraxis, 16, 27–33.
opinion poll in favor of practices are in line with Bernhardt, E. B., & James, C. J. (1987). The teaching
the results above that students recalled signifi- and testing of comprehension in foreign language
cantly more cultural practices than products learning. In D. W. Birckbichler (Ed.), Proficiency,
52 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
policy, and professionalism in foreign language educa- tors in L2 reading performance. Language Learn-
tion (pp. 65–81). Lincolnwood, IL: National Text- ing, 32, 1–31.
book Company. Just, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of
Brooks, N. D. (1968). Language and language learning: comprehension: Individual differences in working
Theory and practice. New York: Harcourt Brace memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.
Jovanovich. Kitajima, R., & Lyman-Hagar, M. A. (1998). Theory-
Bush, M. D. (2000). Digital versatile disc (DVD): The driven use of digital video in foreign language
new medium for interactive video. CALICO Jour- instruction. CALICO Journal, 16, 37–48.
nal, 17, 453–474. Kramsch, C. (1993). Context and culture in language teach-
Capretz, P. (1994). French in action. New Haven, CT: Yale ing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
University Press. Kramsch, C. (1998). Language and culture. Oxford: Ox-
Chun, D. M., & Plass, J. L. (2000). Networked multimedia ford University Press.
environments for second language acquisition. In Lafford, P. A., & Lafford, B. A. (1997). Learning lan-
M. Warschauer & R. Kern (Eds.), Network-based lan- guages and culture with internet technologies. In
guage teaching, concepts and practice (pp. 151–190). M. D. Bush & R. M. Terry (Eds.), Technology-en-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. hanced language learning (pp. 215–262). Lin-
Chung, J. (1999). The effects of using video texts sup- colnwood, IL: National Textbook Company.
ported with advance organizers and captions on Lange, D. L. (1999). Planning for using the new na-
Chinese college students’ listening comprehen- tional cultural standards. In J. Phillips & R. M.
sion: An empirical study. Foreign Language Annals, Terry (Eds.), Foreign language standards: Linking
32, 295–308. research, theories and practices (pp. 57–120). Lin-
Hanley, J., Herron, C., & Cole, S. P. (1995). Using video colnwood, IL: National Textbook Company.
as an advance organizer to a written passage in the Lange, D. L. (2000). Implication of theory and research
FLES classroom. Modern Language Journal, 79, for the development of principles for teaching
57–66. and learning culture in second language class-
Harlow, L. L., & Muyskens, J. A. (1994). Priorities for rooms. In R. M. Paige, D. L. Lange, & Y. A. Yer-
intermediate-level language instruction. Modern shova (Eds.), Culture as the core: Integrating culture
Language Journal, 78, 141–154. into the language curriculum (pp. 115–184). Minnea-
Herron, C. (1994). An investigation of the effectiveness polis, MN: Center for Advanced Research on Lan-
of using an advance organizer to introduce video guage Acquisition.
in the foreign language classroom. Modern Lan- Lange, D. L., & Klee, C. A. (2000). Interdisciplinary
guage Journal, 78, 190–198. perspectives on culture learning in the second
Herron, C., Cole, S. P., York, H., & Linden, P. (1998). A language curriculum. Introduction. In D. L.
comparison study of students’ retention of foreign Lange, C. A. Klee, R. M. Paige, & Y. A. Yershova
language video: Declarative versus interrogative (Eds.), Culture as the core: Interdisciplinary perspec-
advance organizer. Modern Language Journal, 82, tives on culture learning in the language classroom (pp.
237–247. i–viii). Minneapolis, MN: Center for Advanced Re-
Herron, C., Corrie, C., Cole, S. P., & Dubreil, S. (1999). search on Language Acquisition.
The effectiveness of a video-based curriculum in Lively, M. G., Harper, J., & Williams, J. K. (1998). Medi-
teaching culture. Modern Language Journal, 83, ating language with teacher talk: Bringing speech
518–533. to ideas. In J. Harper, M. G. Lively, & M. Williams
Herron, C., Corrie, C., Cole, S. P., & Henderson, P. (Eds.), The coming of age of the profession (pp.
(1999). Do prequestioning techniques facilitate 81–96). Boston: Heinle.
comprehension of French video? French Review, Martin, A. L., & Laurie, I. (1993). Student views about
72, 1076–1090. the contributions of literary and cultural content
Herron, C., Dubreil, S., Cole, S. P., & Corrie, C. (2000). to language learning at intermediate level. Foreign
Using instructional video to teach culture to be- Language Annals, 26, 188–207.
ginning foreign language students. CALICO Jour- Martinez-Gibson, E. A. (1998). A study on cultural
nal, 17, 395–429. awareness through commercials and writing. For-
Herron, C., & Hanley, J. (1992). Using video to intro- eign Language Annals, 31, 115–139.
duce children to a foreign culture. Foreign Lan- Maxim, H. H. (2000). Integrating language learning
guage Annals, 25, 419–426. and cultural inquiry in the beginning foreign lan-
Herron, C., Hanley, J., & Cole, S. P. (1995). A compari- guage classroom. ADFL Bulletin, 32, 12–17.
son study of two advance organizers for introduc- Mueller, G. A. (1980). Visual contextual cues and listen-
ing beginning foreign language students to video. ing comprehension: An experiment. Modern Lan-
Modern Language Journal, 79, 387–395. guage Journal, 64, 335–340.
Hinkel, E. (Ed.). (1999). Culture in second language teach- Muyskens, J. A. (Ed.). (1994). New ways of learning and
ing and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- teaching: Focus on technology and foreign language edu-
sity Press. cation. Boston: Heinle.
Hudson, T. (1982). Effects of induced schemata on the Muyskens, J. A., Harlow, L. L., Vialet, M., & Brière, J.-F.
“short circuit” in L2 reading: Non decoding fac- (1998). Bravo! (3rd ed.). Boston: Heinle.
Carol Herron, Sébastien Dubreil, Cathleen Corrie, and Steven P. Cole 53
Muyskens, J. A., Harlow, L. L., Vialet, M., & Brière, J.-F. effect of video context on foreign language learn-
(1998). Instructor’s Guide. Bravo! (3rd ed.). Boston: ing. Modern Language Journal, 76, 480–490.
Heinle. Seelye, H. N. (1968). Measuring the ability to function
Omaggio Hadley, A. (2001). Teaching languages in context cross-culturally. In H. N. Seelye (Ed.), A handbook
(3rd ed.). Boston: Heinle. on Latin America for teachers (pp. 334–343). Spring-
Paige, R. M., Jorstad, H. L., Siaya, L., Klein, F., & Colby, field, IL: Office of Education. (ERIC Document
J. (2000). Culture learning in language education: Reproduction Service No. ED 027 797h)
A review of the literature. In R. M. Paige, D. L. Seelye, H. N. (1984/1994). Teaching culture. Lin-
Lange, & Y. A. Yershova (Eds.), Culture as the core: colnwood, IL: National Textbook Company.
Interdisciplinary perspectives on culture learning in the Shrum, J. L., & Glisan, E. W. (2000). Teacher’s handbook.
language curriculum (pp. 47–113). Minneapolis, Boston: Heinle.
MN: Center for Advanced Research on Language Standards for Foreign Language Learning: Preparing for the
Acquisition. 21st Century. (1996). Yonkers, NY: Standards for
Rubin, J. (1990). Improving foreign language listening Foreign Language Learning Project.
comprehension. In J. E. Alatis (Ed.), Georgetown Standards for Foreign Language Learning in the 21st Century.
University Round Table (pp. 309–316). Washington, (1999). Yonkers, NY: National Standards in For-
DC: Georgetown University Press. eign Language Education Project.
Secules, T., Herron, C., & Tomasello, M. (1992). The

Questions about Submissions Frequently Asked


by International Authors
The MLJ is pleased to have a wide international readership and an increased number of international
submissions. Potential authors often ask the following questions.

1. I am not a native speaker of English. Should I have a native speaker review and correct my paper?

All authors, especially those whose native language is not English, are encouraged to have native-
English-speaking colleagues read their papers and suggest adjustments for language and style.

2. What types of EFL studies are likely to be published in the MLJ ?

The MLJ considers studies or essays dealing with second or foreign language learning. Therefore,
papers that concern teaching and learning English abroad fall within the parameters of the MLJ.
However, the MLJ is most interested in articles with a strong theoretical base that address issues of
concern to more than one language and more than one locale. Prospective authors should look at back
copies of the Journal and at the examples available online. Alternative publication outlets might be one
of the TESOL publications.

3. Should I send the MLJ editor an email message with an abstract of the article prior to submitting the
paper?

This practice is not recommended. An author should view the sample issues online, or better yet, read
the Journal, in order to assess whether the topic and development of the paper fits the parameters of the
Journal. With regard to the quality of the paper, the editor cannot tell from an abstract whether the
paper will be accepted for publication.

4. Can I submit the same paper to two journals at the same time?

Dual submission is not permitted by the MLJ. The MLJ does not, except with special arrangements
such as exchange articles, publish papers that have appeared in print elsewhere. For submitted
manuscripts, the time waiting for a decision is relatively short (2 to 3 months). Reviewers invest
substantially in papers they read. If the reviewers accept a paper, they expect the paper to be available
for publication.

You might also like