Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516


www.elsevier.com/locate/jinsphys

Chromatic cues to trap the oriental fruit fly, Bactrocera dorsalis


Wen-Yen Wua, Yu-Po Chena, En-Cheng Yanga,b,
a
Department of Entomology, National Chung Hsing University, Taichung, Taiwan
b
Laboratory of Insect Neurobiology, Department of Entomology, National Taiwan University, Taipei 10617, Taiwan
Received 27 December 2005; received in revised form 5 February 2007; accepted 5 February 2007

Abstract

Various colors have been used as visual cues to trap insect pests. For example, yellow traps for monitoring and control of the oriental
fruit fly (Bactrocera dorsalis) have been in use for a very long time. However, the chromatic cue of using color traps has never been
meticulously investigated. In this study, the spectral sensitivities of the photoreceptors in the compound eyes of B. dorsalis were measured
intracellularly, and the theory of receptor quantum catch was applied to study the chromatic cue of fly attracting. Responses to five
wavelength categories with peak wavelengths of 370, 380, 490, and 510 nm, and one with dual peaks at 350 and 490 nm were recorded.
Based on spectral sensitivities, six colored papers were chosen to test the color preference of the fly, and an additional UV preference test
was done to confirm the effect of the UV stimuli. It was concluded that UV and green stimuli (spectra: 300–380 nm and 500–570 nm)
would enhance the attractiveness of a colored paper to the oriental fruit fly, and blue stimuli (380–500 nm) would diminish the
attractiveness.
r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Spectral sensitivity; Color preference; Oriental fruit fly; Bactrocera dorsalis

1. Introduction Liu, 1981), and numerous lure-and-kill traps with yellow


colored surfaces have been developed and applied in the
Several species of fruit flies (Tephritidae) are major and field, such as yellow sticky papers and the famous male-
widespread destructive pests in agriculture around the specific methyl eugenol-baited traps (Cornelius et al., 1999;
world (Christenson and Foote, 1960). Their oviposition Alyokhin et al., 2000; Chen and Dong, 2001). Both Vargas
causes serious damage to fruit crops, costing millions of et al. (1991) and Cornelius et al. (1999) demonstrated that
dollars every year. To control and monitor these pests, yellow fruit-mimicking spheres were an excellent device to
many studies have been devoted to trap-designs (Kat- lure the oriental fruit fly, and Alyokhin et al. (2000)
soyannos, 1989; Roessler, 1989; Heath et al., 1993). Since demonstrated that the visual cues of a yellow fruit-
host-finding is the indispensable stage for discovering a mimicking sphere trap could increase the attractiveness of
suitable place for oviposition, and since it is strongly hydrolyzed liquid protein odor. It has been shown that
influenced by both visual and olfactory cues (Prokopy and visual and olfactory cues can function separately and have
Owens, 1983; Prokey et al., 1990), the best strategy to a complementary effect to each other for attracting flies
attract these flies is by using these cues to develop effective (Jang and Light, 1991; Vargas et al., 1991; Cornelius et al.,
traps to lure them. 1999; Alyokhin et al., 2000; Corneluis et al., 2000a, b).
The oriental fruit fly, Bactrocera dorsalis, is the main Thus, to optimize lure-and-kill traps, these cues must be
fruit fly pest in the Pacific Rim (Haramoto and Bess, 1970; investigated carefully using experiments based on neu-
roethological aspects. However, most of these studies were
Corresponding author. Laboratory of Insect Neurobiology, Depart- done only on olfactory cues for tephritids (Light et al.,
ment of Entomology, National Taiwan University, Taipei 10617, Taiwan.
1992; Raptopoulos et al., 1995). Also, it should be noted
Tel.: +886 2 33669640; fax: +886 2 33652092. that using a color name as a reference to describe a fly’s
E-mail address: ecyang@ntu.edu.tw (E.-C. Yang). preference is not correct, since our color vision is quite

0022-1910/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jinsphys.2007.02.003
ARTICLE IN PRESS
510 W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516

different from that of an insect, and it is better to define Scientific Instrument Lab.), the microelectrode was ad-
the fly’s chromatic preference using the insect visible spec- justed and lowered vertically to insert into the retinula
trum. In this paper, we report the wavelength preference through the small window in the compound eye. After
of the oriental fruit fly based on the evidence from inserting the microelectrode, the fly was dark-adapted for
electrophysiological recordings complete with behavioral at least 10 min. The following manipulations were per-
demonstration. formed in the dark room to keep the fly dark-adapted. The
Since the host-finding behavior is closely related to mate- microelectrode was advanced meticulously, and a gentle
finding and oviposition behaviors in fruit flies (Prokopy tapping was performed when the tip of microelectrode was
and Owens, 1983), it can be assumed that the inherent going to penetrating through the cell membrane. Each time
wavelength preference is also closely related to host-finding the baseline potential shifted, a series of flash stimuli were
behavior. From a physiological perspective, the chromatic used to check if the tip of microelectrode was impaling the
cues of host-finding behavior can be taken as a different photoreceptor. If a depolarization response was detected,
proportion of stimulation for each type of photoreceptor. the Cardan arm perimeter would be adjusted to align with
Thus, selecting the chromatic stimuli according to the the visual axis of the recorded cell by obtaining the
spectral sensitivity of the photoreceptors for testing the maximum response. The recorded cell would be further
fly’s wavelength preference will yield more specific results verified as a photoreceptor that the cell had a depolarized
than studies referring to the host fruit of the fly only graded response waveform, which was composed of an
(Vargas et al., 1991; Cornelius et al., 1999). We tested the initial on-transient depolarizing peak and a sustained
fly’s preference for colored papers that were selected based plateau, to an on-axis saturated ‘‘white light’’ stimulus of
on the spectral sensitivities of a fly’s photoreceptors, and 200 ms duration. The signals measured by the microelec-
analyzed the results to reveal possible chromatic cues. trode were preamplified 10  by an amplifier (Neuropobe,
Model 1600, A-M Systems Inc.) and then sent via a 12-bit
2. Materials and methods multifunction data acquisition system (PCI-6024E,
National Instruments) to an IBM compatible PC.
2.1. Animals A Xenon-short arc lamp (XBO 1000W/HS/OFR,
OSRAM) was used as the stimulating light source. The
Experiments were carried out on both male and female polychromatic ‘‘white light’’ was guided to pass a quartz
adult oriental fruit flies, B. dorsalis, derived from a circular variable neutral-density wedge filter (Acton Re-
laboratory stock maintained at 2871 1C in a rearing room search Co.), which was capable of varying the intensity of
with 12:12 (L:D) photoperiod and fed with the peptone– the light over a range of approximately 3 log units, and
sugar mixture. then sent into the monochromator (SP-150-M with 150-
030-300 grating, Acton Research Co.). The separated
2.2. Spectral sensitivity measurement monochromatic light with half-band widths below 10 nm
was controlled by a magnetic shutter (SH-150, Acton
Before electrophysiological manipulation, the experi- Research Co.) to form the flash stimulation with 20 ms
mental fly was immobilized by placing it in a freezer with duration and was then guided by a UV-VIS fiber optic
chipped ice at 4 1C for 15–30 min. Then the head of the fly bundle (LG-455-020-3, Acton Research Co.) to project on
was mounted on a brass pedestal, with a beeswax/rosin the recorded eye. Since the terminal of the fiber optic
(3:1) mixture, so that the head and thorax were rigidly bundle with a diameter of approximately 1.5 mm was
secured, and the abdomen was free to perform ventilatory 230 mm away from the recorded eye, the terminal of the
movements. A small window was cut in the dorsal–lateral fiber optic bundle therefore subtended an angle less than
part of the left compound eye to expose the retinula for 0.371 at the eye.
inserting the recording microelectrode, and a silver wire The optic instruments and the multifunction data
was inserted into the thorax as the indifferent electrode. acquisition system were controlled by a program developed
The damaged surfaces were covered immediately with by LabVIEW software (ver. 6i, National Instruments) and
vaseline to prevent drying. The brass pedestal with the executed on the PC. Thus, it could produce equal quanta of
mounted animal was placed on a metal plate with the flux stimuli at 41 wavelengths from 300 to 700 nm, with
animal’s head at the center of a Cardan arm perimeter, 10 nm steps, and record the respective electrophysiological
which was mounted with an optical fiber-guided stimula- responses from the photoreceptor. The amplitude of the
ting light source. recorded responses were measured on-line and calculated
The microelectrode was made of microfilament alumi- as the spectral sensitivity by the same program. All the
nosilicate capillary glass (O.D. ¼ 1.0 mm, I.D. ¼ 0.68 mm, measured spectral sensitivity curves were calculated as the
AF100-68-10, Sutter Instrument Co.) pulled on a Flaming- mean of squared errors with the theoretical visual pigment
Brown microelectrode puller (P-97 Flaming/Brown Micro- absorption curves, which were derived from the nomogram
pipette Puller, Sutter Instrument Co.), and had a resistance by Ebrey and Honig (1977), at 41 peak wavelengths from
of 140–160 MO when filled with 1 M lithium chloride 300 to 700 nm. The lmax was determined at the wavelength
solution. Using a micromanipulator (MWS-32, Narishige with the minimum mean square error.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516 511

2.3. Color preference test Experiments were conducted on sunny days from April
to June 2004 on National Chung Hsing University campus.
To test the color preference of the oriental fruit fly, 6 Tests were performed in a dodecagon maze (Fig. 2)
colored papers (black, white, green, yellow, orange and outdoors, with two sets of colored papers attached
red) were chosen according to the spectral sensitivities of symmetrically on the side walls in random orders and with
the photoreceptors. Reflectance spectra of these colored each colored paper separated with opaque black plastic
papers were obtained with a spectrometer (S2000, Ocean plating. To allow the flies to see all 6 colored papers in any
Optics Inc.) compared with a reflectance standard (Spec- horizontal direction, the tested flies restricted in a
tralon diffuse white standard, Labsphere, Inc.). The black transparent transport container were put at the center of
colored paper with almost no reflectance was chosen as the the maze before they were set free. After 3–5 min, the
test control. Yellow, orange and red colored papers were container was opened to set the files free and allowed them
chosen to test the effect of different stimuli on the to move and fly freely to lower any possible stress effect.
sensitivity peaks between 500 and 560 nm. The green and The number of flies in each inside-chamber was counted
white colored papers were chosen to test the effect of strong after 30 min. During the tests, the dodecagon maze with a
and no UV stimulus (Fig. 1). transparent top was placed in the shade of trees in order
to prevent any possible effects of polarization from
incident light.
Since it has been reported that the ERG of the fly may be
120 subject to age-related changes (Loew, 1975), and that
visual attractive cues for tephritid flies may differ between
100 sexes (Sivinski, 1990), the flies were tested separately for
different ages and sex with three replicates. Both male and
80 female flies from 1 to 20 days old from eclosion were tested.
Reflectance (%)

Each test was performed on at least 30 flies; no fly was


60 tested more than once.

40 2.4. UV preference test

20
According to the result of color preference test (see
below and Fig. 8), the white colored paper with a high UV
reflectance was less attractive to the oriental fruit fly than
0
the green colored paper and even less attractive than the
300 400 500 600 700
black colored paper, a further test was performed to
Wavelength (nm)
evaluate the affect of UV stimuli on the color preference.
Fig. 1. Reflectance spectra of colored papers used in the color preference Twelve white colored papers were attached onto the inside
test. walls of the dodecagon maze as the above description. On

Fig. 2. Sketch of the dodecagon maze with 12 inside-chambers used for simultaneously comparing the attractiveness of different colored papers to oriental
fruit flies. The numbers indicate the dimensions of the maze and inside-chambers in centimeters. The structures of the inside-chambers are identically, and
the right illustration indicates the dimension of one of the inside-chambers marked in dark-gray color.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
512 W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516

the top of the six chambers of the maze, UV-cut


transparent plastic plates servicing as UV cut-off filters 100
were applied to produce white colored paper without UV

Relative Sensitivity (%)


reflection below 380 nm, but the plates still had more than
80% transmission within 390–700 nm. The other six 75
chambers without the transparent plastic plates were
illuminated by normal light to the white colored papers,
50
and thus providing higher UV reflection from the papers.
All the six UV-cut chambers were separated by the normal
illuminated chambers. Therefore, the setup could provide
25
dual-choice of white colored papers with and without UV
illumination below 380 nm. The experimental procedure
followed the color preference test. Because the color
0
preference was not significantly different between the sexes 300 400 500 600 700
and the different ages (see color preference test result), the Wavelength (nm)
tests were performed with more than 25 males and 25
females with the age of 7 days old together at the same Fig. 3. The relative spectral sensitivity curve of the oriental fruit fly
time. Each test was performed with at least 50 flies; flies 370 nm photoreceptor (data averaged from three measurements of cell 15,
the scatter bar gives the standard error). The light gray dash line shows the
were tested only once. theoretical visual pigment absorption curve with the peak wavelength at
370 nm. The gray dot line shows the spectral sensitivity curve of the R7p
3. Results photoreceptor in the dipteran retinula (Hardie, 1986).

3.1. Spectral sensitivities of the compound eye


photoreceptors of the oriental fruit fly 100

Responses to five wavelength categories were measured


Relative Sensitivity (%)

from 37 cells in the dorsal–frontal part of the left 75


compound eye of the oriental fruit fly in dark adaptation.
The maximum intracellular response of the oriental fruit fly
photoreceptors was generally up to 70 mV. In some 50
recordings, however, such as in the 380 nm and the
510 nm photoreceptors, the recorded maximum response
could only reach about 25–40 mV. All recordings were 25
validated as intracellular by means of characteristics such
as the baseline drop during penetration of the cell
membrane (35 to 60 mV) and the transient-tonic 0
depolarized graded response waveform to a sufficient light 300 400 500 600 700
stimulus. The low maximum responses of these recordings Wavelength (nm)
could have been caused by the filtering effects of the special
histological superposition condition (Wu et al., 1985), or Fig. 4. The relative spectral sensitivity curve of the oriental fruit fly
380 nm photoreceptor (data averaged from two measurements of cell 23).
more likely the result of a microelectrode location distant
The light gray dash line shows the theoretical visual pigment absorption
from the cell body of the photoreceptor. According to their curve with the peak wavelength at 380 nm. The gray dot line shows the
lmax of the spectral sensitivity curve, these photoreceptors spectral sensitivity curve of the R7y photoreceptor in the dipteran retinula
were grouped and termed as 370, 380, 490, 510 nm and (Hardie, 1986).
dual-peak photoreceptors. Further details of these photo-
receptors are described as follows: photoreceptor in the UV range (Fig. 4), and formed a
370 nm photoreceptor: A single cell, with maximum plateau shoulder at 440–520 nm with sensitivity of
spectral sensitivity at 370 nm, was measured with three about 40% of the peak. The peak of the sensitivity curve
measurements, one bi-directional run (300–700 nm and was at 370 nm, but due to the fact that the averaged curve
700–300 nm) and another uni-directional run (from 300 to is best fitted with the theoretical visual pigment absorption
700 nm). The black solid line of Fig. 3 shows that the curve of 380 nm visual pigment (Fig. 4), this type is
sensitive range of this cell was mainly located in the UV therefore referred to as the 380 nm photoreceptor. The
range of 300–400 nm. The fitting procedure also demon- cell was measured successfully with two measurements
strated that the curve was best fitted by the theoretical (bi-directional run).
visual pigment absorption curve of 370 nm visual pigment. 490 nm photoreceptor: Three measurements were success-
380 nm photoreceptor: The spectral sensitivity curve of fully made on the 490 nm photoreceptor (one bi-directional
the 380 nm photoreceptor was similar to that of the 370 nm and another uni-directional run). This photoreceptor
ARTICLE IN PRESS
W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516 513

possesses a simple spectral sensitivity peak at 490 nm three repeats (one bi-directional and another uni-direc-
(Fig. 5), and the spectral sensitivity curve is best fitted with tional run).
the theoretical visual pigment absorption curve of 490 nm Dual-peak photoreceptor: These photoreceptors were the
visual pigment. photoreceptor most often encountered in 33 cells in the
510 nm photoreceptor: The shape of the spectral sensitiv- retinula. The spectral sensitivity curves of these cells were
ity curve of the 510 nm photoreceptor appeared to be like a composed of two major peaks at the UV (330–370 nm) and
‘‘screened’’ curve of the 490 nm photoreceptor (Fig. 6). the blue-green (430–510 nm) range. The band widths of the
Therefore, the spectral sensitivity peak was ‘‘shifted’’ to the peaks and the amplitude ratios between the peaks showed
range of 480–550 nm, and the curve was best fitted with the some varieties. Fig. 7 shows the spectral sensitivity curve
theoretical visual pigment absorption curve of 510 nm averaged from the measurement of 7 cells, the lmax was
visual pigment (Fig. 6). The cell was also measured with obtained at 350 and 490 nm by fitting with the theoretical
visual pigment absorption curve.

100 3.2. Color preference of the oriental fruit fly

The preferred choice rates were obtained from three


Relative Sensitivity (%)

75 replicates of each age and sex. These rates were not


significantly related to either age or sex when analyzed with
a three-way ANOVA test, but they were significantly
50 related to the color of the colored papers (F ¼ 88.77;
df ¼ 5; Po0.0001; ANOVA Procedure of SASs). Choice
rates were also not significantly related to the mature status
25 of the flies (1–7 days old flies and older flies) when analyzed
with a two-way ANOVA test, but were significantly related
to the color of the colored papers (F ¼ 73.88; df ¼ 5;
0 Po0.0001; ANOVA Procedure of SASs). Therefore the
300 400 500 600 700 data from both sexes and all ages were combined for
Wavelength (nm) further analyses. Fig. 8 shows the mean choice rates for the
colored papers from 120 replicates (three replicates for 20
Fig. 5. The relative spectral sensitivity curve of the oriental fruit fly ages and two sexes). The green, yellow and orange colored
490 nm photoreceptor (data averaged from three measurements of cell 40,
papers, with an oriental fruit fly attracting rate of
the scatter bar gives the standard error). The light gray dash line shows the
theoretical visual pigment absorption curve with the peak wavelength at (Mean7SE) 0.2265270.00766, 0.2194170.00826, and
490 nm. The gray dot line shows the spectral sensitivity curve of the R8p 0.1913270.00704, respectively, were significantly more
photoreceptor in the dipteran retinula (Hardie, 1986). attractive than the red, black, or white colored papers

100
100
Relative Sensitivity (%)
Relative Sensitivity (%)

75
75

50
50

25 25

0 0
300 400 500 600 700 300 400 500 600 700
Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm)

Fig. 6. The relative spectral sensitivity curve of the oriental fruit fly Fig. 7. The relative spectral sensitivity curve of the oriental fruit fly dual-
510 nm photoreceptor (data averaged from six measurements of cell 14, peak photoreceptors (data averaged from seven cells, the scatter bar gives
the scatter bar gives the standard error). The light gray dash line shows the the standard error). The light gray dash lines show the theoretical visual
theoretical visual pigment absorption curve with the peak wavelength at pigment absorption curves with the peak wavelength at 350 and 490 nm.
510 nm. The gray dot line shows the spectral sensitivity curve of the R8y The gray dot line shows the spectral sensitivity curve of the R1–6
photoreceptor in the dipteran retinula (Hardie, 1986). photoreceptors in the dipteran retinula (Hardie, 1986).
ARTICLE IN PRESS
514 W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516

0.25 a 370 and 380 nm photoreceptors: The spectral sensitivity


a,b
curve of the 370 nm photoreceptor is similar to the curve of
b the R7p photoreceptor obtained from other flies (Fig. 2;
0.20
Horridge et al., 1975; Hardie, 1979, 1986; Hardie et al.,
c
1979). It is very likely that the sensitivity range of the
Attraction Rate

0.15 c 370 nm photoreceptor may be elicited by a single visual


pigment with its lmax at 370 nm. In contrast, the 380 nm
photoreceptor has a more complicated spectral sensitivity
0.10 d curve, in which a shoulder was observed from 440 to
520 nm. This curve is also similar to the R7y photoreceptor
of other dipteran flies (Fig. 3; Horridge et al., 1975; Hardie
0.05
et al., 1979; Hardie, 1986). According to Hardie (1986), the
mechanism that elicits these action spectra of the photo-
0.00 receptors may possibly be caused by the fact that the blue
Yellow Green Orange Red Black White sensitive rhodopsins (e.g. xanthopsin, lmax ¼ 430 nm) of
Color Paper the cell were greatly hindered by the blue screening pigment
(e.g. carotenoid). However, the cells possess a UV
Fig. 8. Attraction (Mean+SE) of different colored papers to the oriental
fruit fly. The same lowercase letters above the bars in (a–c) indicate no sensitizing pigment similar to the dual-peak photoreceptors
significant difference (F ¼ 73.61; df ¼ 5; Po0.0001; ANOVA Procedure and the absorbed energy of the unscreened sensitizing
and Tukey’s Studentized Range (HSD) Test of SASs). pigments can transfer to the blue sensitive rhodopsins and
cause the UV peak with the blue shoulder in the action
(Fig. 8). The white colored paper with the 0.079970.00504 spectra of these cells. Therefore, the 380 nm photoreceptor
oriental fruit fly attracting rate was significantly less has a complicated action spectrum, and the summation of
attractive to the oriental fruit fly than the red and black UV sensitizing pigment and the ‘‘hindered’’ blue sensitive
colored paper with an attracting rate of 0.1487470.00707 rhodopsins makes the spectral sensitivity curve best fit with
and 0.1341170.00578, respectively (Fig. 8; F ¼ 73.61; the 380 nm theoretical visual pigment absorption curve. In
df ¼ 5; Po0.0001; ANOVA Procedure of SASs). addition, the block effects of the blue screening pigments in
the 380 nm photoreceptor of the oriental fruit fly would be
less than those in the R7y photoreceptor of other dipteran
3.3. UV preference test
flies, as the 380 nm photoreceptor was more sensitive than
the R7y photoreceptor within the range between 420 and
The choice rates were obtained from 3 replicates, which
510 nm.
showed that the white colored papers with the UV
490 and 510 nm photoreceptors: The spectral sensitivity
illumination below 380 nm were significantly more attrac-
curves of 490 and 510 nm photoreceptors are similar to the
tive than the white colored papers without the UV
R8p and R8y photoreceptors of other dipteran flies (Figs. 4
illumination (Mean7SE: 0.4670.03055 vs. 0.246677
and 5; Hardie et al., 1979; Hardie, 1986). It is possible that
0.04807; F ¼ 13.17; df ¼ 1; Po0.05; ANOVA Procedure
the 490 and 510 nm photoreceptors of oriental fruit fly are
and Tukey’s Studentized Range (HSD) Test of SASs).
the R8 cells described by Wu et al. (1985). Because the
rhabdomere of the R8 cell is beneath the rhabdomere of the
4. Discussion R7 cell in the oriental fruit fly (Wu et al., 1985), the 370 and
380 nm photoreceptors may be considered as the R7 cells. If
4.1. Spectral sensitivity of the photoreceptors this is the case, the spectral reception of the 490 and 510 nm
photoreceptors should be filtered by the 370 and 380 nm
Dual-peak photoreceptor: The dual-peak photoreceptors photoreceptors, respectively. Because the spectral absorp-
have a similar action spectrum to the R1–6 photoreceptors tions of 490 and 370 nm photoreceptors are not over-
found in other dipteran flies (Fig. 7; Horridge and Mimura, lapping in their dominant peaks, the 490 nm photoreceptor
1975; Horridge et al., 1975; Hardie, 1979, 1986; Hardie et still keeps its spectral sensitivity in the blue-green range of
al., 1979). The antenna pigment hypothesis proposed by spectrum. On the other hand, because the spectral
Kirschfeld et al. (1977) suggested that the blue-green peak absorption of the 380 nm photoreceptor and the spectral
was from the intrinsic blue-green sensitive rhodopsins and sensitivity of the 510 nm photoreceptor overlapped, light
that the UV peak was due to the UV sensitizing pigments absorption by the UV sensitizing pigment and the blue-
transferring their absorbed energy to the rhodopsins. This green sensitive rhodopsin of the 510 nm photoreceptor is
would then make the action spectra of these photorecep- hindered by the 380 nm photoreceptor. Consequently, UV
tors become broadly distributed with two peak wave- sensitivity is diminished as a small sensitive peak, and the
lengths. Variation of the spectral sensitivity curves can be blue-green sensitivity is also altered, so that the spectral
assumed to be the result of self-absorption or electric sensitivity curve fits best with the 510 nm theoretical visual
coupling effects (Manzel, 1975). pigment absorption curve.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516 515

4.2. Color preference of the oriental fruit fly chromatic cues of the color preference obtained by
applying the fundamental physiological information are
Previous studies on another tephritid pest, the apple helpful in studying the visual ecology of the oriental fruit
maggot fly (Rhagoletis pomonella), demonstrated that the fly, and provide a useful tool for developing pest control
intensity contrast of dark host against bright background methods. While various yellow traps have been widely used
plays a more important role in the fly’s host detection than in field trapping, most of the ‘‘yellow colors’’ were
host colour (Agee, 1985; Owens and Prokopy, 1986). determined by human perception without precisely quanti-
Contrarily, a bright host (e.g. colored with yellow or white) fying the action spectrum of the pest and concerning the
is more attractive to the oriental fruit fly when tested with negative effect. Our results suggest that a trap with
different-color spheres (Vargas et al., 1991), suggesting that sufficient reflection within 300–380 nm and 500–570 nm
different strategies are used for seeking host targets but without reflection within 380–500 nm will be more
between the two fruit fly species. effective in attracting the oriental fruit fly.
When comparing the reflectance spectra of the colored
papers and their attractiveness for the oriental fruit fly Acknowledgment
(Figs. 1 and 8), it is evident that several spectral cues are
involved with the attractiveness. The green colored paper We thank the anonymous reviewers for insightful
that showed the higher attractiveness among the colored corrections and improvements. We are also most grateful
papers has a broader and higher reflectance, indicating that to Dr. Kuang-Hui Lu and his lab for their generous fly
chromatic stimulation of the spectra within 500–570 nm stocks. This work was supported by a grant (94 AS-13.2.1-
contains crucial cues for attracting the oriental fruit fly. BQ-BG) from the BAPHIQ and a grant (NSC 91-2313-B-
This is also supported by the fact that orange colored paper 005-117) from National Science Council, Taiwan.
with a reflectance spectrum slightly higher than that of red
colored paper, in the range of 470–630 nm, has a
References
significantly stronger attractiveness to the flies than the
red colored paper. In addition, the white colored paper Agee, H.R., 1985. Spectral response of the compound eye of the wild and
used in our experiment has the broadest reflectance laboratory-reared apple maggot fly, Rhagoletis pomonella. Journal of
spectrum but the lowest attractiveness to the flies, even Agricultural Entomology 2, 147–154.
less than the black colored paper, and the green colored Alyokhin, A.V., Messing, R.H., Duan, J.J., 2000. Visual and olfactory
paper has a similar attractive effect as the yellow colored stimuli and fruit maturity affect trap captures of oriental fruit flies
(Diptera: Tephritidae). Journal of Economic Entomology 93, 644–649.
paper, which has a much higher reflectance within Chen, C.C., Dong, Y.J., 2001. Evaluation of trapping effectiveness
500–570 nm than the green colored paper. Thus, it is of the improved McPhail trap for oriental fruit fly (Bactrocera dorsalis)
reasonable to hypothesize that there is a negative cue in the (Diptera: Tephritidae). Formosan Entomologist 21, 65–75 (in
spectrum to reduce the attractiveness. Chinese).
Christenson, L.C., Foote, R.H., 1960. Biology of fruit flies. Annual
The UV-sensitive peaks seen in the recorded spectral
Review of Entomology 5, 171–192.
sensitivity curves of the fly initially suggested that negative Cornelius, M.L., Duan, J.J., Messing, R.H., 1999. Visual stimuli and the
cues might be present in this spectral region. However, the response of female oriental fruit flies (Diptera: Tephritidae) to fruit-
preference test indicated that UV reflection enhanced mimicking traps. Journal of Economic Entomology 92, 121–129.
attractiveness, as did the ‘‘green’’ reflection (500–570 nm). Corneluis, M.L., Duan, J.J., Messing, R.H., 2000a. Volatile host fruit
The UV-positive effect could be attributed to the UV odors as attractants for the oriental fruit fly (Diptera: Tephritidae).
Journal of Economic Entomology 93, 93–100.
sensitivity of the dual-peak photoreceptors as well as the Corneluis, M.L., Neregel, L., Duan, J.J., Messing, R.H., 2000b.
370 nm and the 380 nm photoreceptors. A final possibility Responses of female oriental fruit flies (Diptera: Tephritidae) to
for the location of the negative cue could be within the protein and host fruit odors in field cage and open field tests.
visual sensitive range of 380–500 nm, for which no Environmental Entomology 29, 14–19.
Ebrey, T.G., Honig, B., 1977. New wavelength dependent visual pigment
corresponding photoreceptor was recorded in this study.
nomograms. Vision Research 17, 147–151.
This negative effect could be caused by opponent mechan- Haramoto, F.H., Bess, H.A., 1970. Recent studies on the abundance of
isms, or related to an undiscovered blue photoreceptor the oriental and Mediterreanean fruit flies and the status of their
(sensitivity range between 400 and 500 nm), as suggested by parasites. Proceeding of the Hawaiian Entomological Society 20,
a previous ERG study on the oriental fruit fly (Wu, 1989) 551–566.
and the evidence shown in other dipteran flies (Hardie, Hardie, R.C., 1979. Electrophysiological analysis of fly retina. I:
comparative properties of R1-6 and R7 and 8. Journal of Comparative
1986). The green-positive effect could be attributed to the Physiology A 129, 19–33.
green sensitivity of the 510 nm photoreceptor, or the Hardie, R.C., 1986. The photoreceptor array of the dipteran retina.
related opponent mechanisms as mentioned above. Since Trends in Neurosciences 9, 419–423.
the action spectra of the photoreceptors overlapped each Hardie, R.C., Franceschini, N., Mclntyre, P.D., 1979. Electrophysiologi-
other, genetic mutants such as photoreceptor-defective cal analysis of fly retina II. Spectral and polarisation sensitivity in R7
and R8. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 133, 23–39.
strains or RNAi knock down of individual photopigments Heath, R.R., Epsky, N.D., Landolt, P.J., Sivinski, J., 1993. Development
would be required to confirm which photoreceptors were of attractants for monitoring Caribbean fruit flies (Diptera: Tephri-
actually involved with color preference. Nevertheless, the tidae). Florida Entomologist 76, 233–244.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
516 W.-Y. Wu et al. / Journal of Insect Physiology 53 (2007) 509–516

Horridge, G.A., Mimura, K., 1975. Fly photoreceptors I. Physical Owens, E.D., Prokopy, R.J., 1986. Relationship between reflectance
separation of two visual pigments in Calliphora retinula cells 1–6. spectra of host plant surfaces and visual detection of host fruit by
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 190, 211–224. Rhagoletis pomonella flies. Physiological Entomology 11, 297–307.
Horridge, G.A., Mimura, K., Tsukahara, Y., 1975. Fly photoreceptors II. Prokopy, R.J., Owens, E.D., 1983. Visual detection of plants by
Spectral and polarized light sensitivity in the drone fly Eristalis. herbivorous insects. Annual Review of Entomology 28, 337–364.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 190, 225–237. Prokey, R.J., Green, T.A., Vargas, R.I., 1990. Dacus dorsalis flies can
Jang, E.B., Light, D.M., 1991. Behavioral responses of female oriental learn to find and accept host fruit. Journal of Insect Behavior 3,
fruit flies to the odor of papayas at three ripeness stages in a laboratory 663–672.
flight tunnel (Diptera: Tephritidae). Journal of Insect Behavior 4, Raptopoulos, D., Haniotakis, G., Koutsaftikis, A., Kelly, D., Mavraga-
751–762. nis, V., 1995. Biological activity of chemicals identified from extracts
Katsoyannos, B.I., 1989. Response to shape, size and color. In: Robinson, and volatiles of male Rhagoletis cerasi. Journal of Chemical Ecology
A.S., Hooper, G. (Eds.), World Crop Pests, Vol. 3A, Fruit Flies, their 21, 1287–1297.
Biology, Natural Enemies and Control. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. Roessler, Y., 1989. Insecticidal bait and cover sprays. In: Robinson, A.S.,
307–324. Hooper, G. (Eds.), World Crop Pests, Vol. 3B, Fruit Flies, their
Kirschfeld, K., Franceschini, N., Minke, B., 1977. Evidence for a Biology, Natural Enemies and Control. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp.
sensitizing pigment in fly photoreceptors. Nature 269, 386–390. 329–336.
Light, D.M., Jang, E.B., Flath, R.A., 1992. Electroantennogram responses Sivinski, J., 1990. Colored spherical traps for capture of Caribbean fruit
of the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata, to the volatile fly, Anastrepha suspensa. Florida Entomologist 73, 123–128.
constituents of nectarines. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata Vargas, R.I., Stark, J.D., Prokopy, R.J., 1991. Response of oriental fruit
63, 13–26. fly (Diptera: Tephritidae) and associated parasitoids (Hymenoptera:
Liu, Y.C., 1981. A review on studies of the oriental fruit fly, Dacus dorsalis Braconidae) to different-color spheres. Journal of Economic Entomol-
Hendel in Taiwan. Bulletin of the Society of Entomology 16, 9–26 (in ogy 84, 1503–1507.
Chinese). Wu, C.Y., 1989. Receptors in insect II. Electroretinogram of the
Loew, E.R., 1975. CO2-induced changes in the ERG of the fly Sarcophaga compound eye in the oriental fruit fly (Dacus dorsalis Hendel). Bulletin
bullata. A component analysis. Journal of Insect Physiology 21, of the Institute of Zoology, Academia Sinica 28, 7–13.
181–197. Wu, C.Y., Chang, C.S., Tung, L.C., Lin, J.T., 1985. Receptors in insect I.
Manzel, R., 1975. Colour receptors in insects. In: Horridge, G.A. (Ed.), The fine structure of the compound eye of the oriental fruit fly Dacus
The Compound Eye and Vision of Insects. Oxford University Press, dorsalis Hendel. Bulletin of the Institute of Zoology, Academia Sinica
London, pp. 121–153. 24, 27–38.

You might also like