Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

HEALTH PROMOTION INTERNATIONAL Vol. 14, No.

2
© Oxford University Press 1999 Printed in Great Britain

Perceived sleep quality and its precursors


in adolescents
JORMA TYNJÄLÄ, LASSE KANNAS, ESKO LEVÄLAHTI and
RAILI VÄLIMAA
University of Jyväskylä, Department of Health Sciences, P.O. Box 35, FIN-40351 Jyväskylä, Finland

SUMMARY
This study investigated perceived sleep quality among 11-, quality. A health-promotive lifestyle (good sleep hygiene
13- and 15-year-old Finnish adolescents (n = 4187). and infrequent use of addictive substances) and good self-
Additionally, associations of selected behavioural, social perception also had significant correlation with good
and psychological factors with subjective sleep quality were perceived sleep quality. In 15-year-old girls, a good home
examined among 15-year-olds. This study is part of an in- atmosphere, good self-perception and health-promotive
ternational, WHO-co-ordinated survey of school children’s habits played an equally important role in associations with
health and lifestyle (the HBSC Study). In Finland, research subjective sleep quality. Physical activity in leisure time had
data represented the whole country. The data were collected minor but significant correlation with perceived sleep quality
during March–May 1994. Pupils responded anonymously among girls. In both sexes, perceived home atmosphere
to a standardized questionnaire during a class period. had a significant association with all the factors that had
About every 10th adolescent felt that their sleep quality was correlated significantly with subjective sleep quality. Results
at most satisfactory, about 30% of pupils had had dif- indicate that a good home atmosphere, a health-promotive
ficulties in falling asleep and almost every fifth adolescent lifestyle and good self-perception are important constituents
reported nocturnal awakenings every week. Thus, a large of good sleep among 15-year-olds. Good and refreshing sleep
proportion of pupils in every classroom has a weakened is one of the constituents for general well-being among
ability to concentrate on school work or other activities. adolescents. That is why these issues provide a challenge for
Among 15-year-old boys, a good home atmosphere was the health promotion and health education.
most important contributing factor to good perceived sleep

Key words: adolescents; health behaviour; health promotion; perceived sleep quality

INTRODUCTION

Sleep quality is an important indicator of chil- Few studies have sleep quality as the focus of
dren’s and adolescents’ health and well-being. major interest in normal adolescent populations
Poor perceived sleep quality is associated with (Price et al., 1978; Kirmil-Gray et al., 1984; Levy
poor reported health, poor physical fitness and et al., 1986; Hawkins and Shaw, 1992; Andrade
numerous psychosomatic symptoms. Sleep dif- et al., 1993; Manni et al., 1997; Vignau et al.,
ficulties and poor sleep quality may also be signs 1997). A need for a large-scale study on this issue
of stress factors in living environment or indicators has been expressed in earlier studies (Price et al.,
of a health-compromising lifestyle. (Price et al., 1978; Kirmil-Gray et al., 1984; Levy et al., 1986;
1978; Rimpelä and Rimpelä, 1983; Weydahl, Zammit, 1988; Lexcen and Hicks, 1993; Tynjälä
1991a; Mahon, 1994, 1995; King et al., 1996; and Kannas, 1993; see also Hyyppä, 1991). Study-
Pilcher et al., 1997; Vignau et al., 1997). ing perceived sleep quality in normal populations

155
156 J. Tynjälä et al.

will help us to determine if we need to be con- 1989; Weydahl, 1991a; Mahon, 1994, 1995;
cerned about sleep quality in younger age groups Pilcher et al., 1997; Vignau et al., 1997). Positive
(Price et al., 1978). self-esteem or self-concept is more typical of
Perceived sleep quality is described in many persons with good sleep quality (e.g. Price et al.,
studies as difficulty in falling asleep, sleep onset 1978; Healey et al., 1981; Kirmil-Gray et al., 1984;
latency, difficulty in maintaining sleep, awaken- see also Hyyppä et al., 1991).
ings at night, calmness of sleep, premature final Unhealthy behaviours tend to cluster (Lexcen
morning awakening, how refreshed a person and Hicks, 1993; Aarø et al., 1995), and therefore
feels after sleep and a person’s own view of sleep it is important to also study sleep quality from
quality (Monroe, 1967; Price et al., 1978; Healey the viewpoint of adolescents’ lifestyle. Studies
et al., 1981; Lugaresi et al., 1983; Kirmil-Gray have indicated that frequent smoking, heavy
et al., 1984; Levy et al., 1986; Strauch and Meier, use of alcohol and frequent use of coffee or caf-
1988; Andrade et al., 1993; Lexcen and Hicks, feined beverages are associated with poor
1993; Tynjälä et al., 1993; Tynjälä and Kannas, perceived sleep quality (e.g. Price et al., 1978;
1993; Åkerstedt et al., 1994; Saarenpää-Heikkilä Rimpelä and Rimpelä, 1983; Hallman et al., 1992;
et al., 1995). On the basis of these indicators, Rahkonen et al., 1992; Lexcen and Hicks,
persons are often grouped as, e.g. poor sleepers, 1993; Phillips and Danner, 1995; Vignau et al.,
occasional poor sleepers and good sleepers. In 1997). Physical activity has been found to have
these studies, the proportion of adolescents with positive effects and lack of physical activity
no sleep problems has varied between 40% and negative effects on subjective sleep quality (Price
70%, whereas about 10% have been categorized et al., 1978; Shapiro et al., 1984; see also Weydahl,
as poor sleepers with chronic and severe sleep 1991b). Sleeping habits, e.g. late and irregular
disturbances. bedtime, short sleeping time, large sleeping time
Investigations of the above-mentioned in- differences between the week and weekend, and
dicators of sleep quality among adolescents have long daytime naps are associated with poor
revealed that the majority of adolescents sleep perceived sleep quality (Kirmil-Gray et al., 1984;
well or very well (Andrade et al., 1993; Tynjälä, Carskadon, 1990; Ferber, 1990; see also Rimpelä
1993; Tynjälä and Kannas, 1993). Under 10% of and Rimpelä, 1983).
adolescents have reported difficulties in falling The first research task in the present study
asleep or nocturnal awakenings every or almost was to describe the self-reports of 11-, 13- and
every night (Morrison et al., 1992; Andrade et al., 15-year-old Finnish boys and girls on how well
1993; Tynjälä, 1993; Saarenpää-Heikkilä et al., they sleep (how often they have difficulties in
1995; see also Aro et al., 1987). Differences in the falling asleep, how often they wake up at night
indicators of subjective sleep quality between and how well in general they sleep). The second
age groups have been for the most part not sig- research task was to investigate what factors,
nificant (Tynjälä and Kannas, 1993; see also e.g. self-perception, social relationships, addictive
Strauch and Meier, 1988; Saarenpää-Heikkilä behaviour, sleep hygiene and physical activity,
et al., 1995). Moreover, sex differences have been are associated with perceived sleep quality in
rare or have not existed (Fischer and Wilson, 15-year-old Finns.
1987; Strauch and Meier, 1988; Hawkins and
Shaw, 1992; Tynjälä, 1993; Tynjälä and Kannas,
1993; Yarcheski and Mahon, 1994; Saarenpää- METHODS
Heikkilä et al., 1995). However, in some studies
girls have reported more sleep difficulties than This study is part of an international, WHO-
boys (e.g. Rimpelä and Rimpelä, 1983; Kirmil- co-ordinated survey of school children’s health
Gray et al., 1984; Aro et al., 1987; King et al., behaviour—the HBSC Study—which describes
1996; Vignau et al., 1997). the perceived health and lifestyle of 11-, 13- and
There are many studies that provide evidence 15-year-old adolescents from a health promotion
that poor sleep quality is associated, among other viewpoint.
things, with problems in social relationships, In Finland, the Institute for Educational
problems with handling troubles, problems at Research in Jyväskylä was responsible for the
school, disturbances in sleeping environment and sampling procedure. Samples were chosen from
psychological states, e.g. anxiety, depression, the Finnish school register by using a special
tension and worry (Price et al., 1978; Kahn et al., sampling program. The sample frame was the
Perceived sleep quality 157

number of Finnish-speaking pupils in each class from earlier studies (e.g. Tynjälä et al., 1997),
level. This computer program picked the schools these variables were thought to formulate the
using cluster sampling in such a way that the size following factors: sleep hygiene (bedtime during
of schools was taken into consideration (prob- school week, regularity of bedtime, sleep time
ability proportional to size of school—PPS). during school week); addictive behaviour (cigar-
Within the school, the class was randomly ettes per week, times being drunk, frequency of
selected. In only a few cases was more than one beer drinking); leisure time physical activity
class per school selected in the sample. The strata (frequency and time spent on physical activity
of schools were based on province division. The per week, activity in sport club); sedentary behav-
other basis of stratum was the division between iour (hours of watching TV, hours of watching
urban and rural communes. In summary, the VCR, number of evenings a week spent with
research data represented the Finnish-speaking friends); self-perception (index of self-esteem
part of the country well (94% of the whole popu- [Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.83; the index was a modi-
lation) in the 11-, 13- and 15-year age groups fication of Rosenberg, 1985; see also Lintunen
(Tynjälä and Törmäkangas, 1995). et al., 1995], feeling of self-confidence, feeling of
In Finland the age groups represent pupils in helplessness); index of problems at school (six
the 5th, 7th and 9th grades of the compulsory items, Cronbach’s Alpha was 0.68); social rela-
comprehensive school. Pupils responded anony- tionships (feelings of loneliness, perceived home
mously to a standardized questionnaire during a atmosphere, number of close friends, index of
class period with a teacher overseeing the pro- the perceived importance to family members and
cess. Data were collected in March–May 1994. close friends—six items with Cronbach’s Alpha
The final sample of the cleaned (Aarø et al., = 0.46); class and school atmosphere (encour-
1994) data consisted of 4187 pupils, 869 of which agement by teachers, how interested teachers
were 11-year-old boys (mean age was 11 years were in pupils, did pupils enjoy being together
and 9 months), 631 were 13-year-old boys (mean in the class, how friendly and helpful pupils felt
age 13 years and 10 months) and 576 were 15- the other pupils were in the class, frequency of
year-old boys (mean age 15 years and 9 months); being bullied, how safe pupils felt at school);
respective figures for girls were 845 (mean age 11 attitude toward school and school work (opinion
years and 9 months), 648 (mean age 13 years and about going to school, opinion of how good
9 months) and 618 (mean age 15 years and 9 he/she is in school, opinion of how well he/she
months). All 206 schools included in the sample knows what kind of things he/she is good at in
participated in the survey. Of all the pupils in the school).
sample, 93% responded to the questionnaire. Among 15-year-old boys and girls, factors’ cor-
Non-responses, 7% of the sample, consisted of relation structure was analysed using structural
pupils absent from school on the data collection equation models within the framework of LISREL
day. The data have been described in more detail (version 8.12a). The polychoric correlations
elsewhere (Tynjälä and Törmäkangas, 1995; King needed in LISREL were calculated using
et al., 1996). PRELIS [version 2.12a] (Jöreskog and Sörbom,
Perceived sleep quality was measured by 1993). These analyses were carried out separately
inquiring how well adolescents sleep in general, for boys and girls. The LISREL analyses were
how often they have difficulties in falling asleep performed using the generally weighted least
and how often they wake up during the night. squares (WLS) estimation method because the
The internal consistency of the sum index of these analyses were based on polychoric correlations.
variables was tested using Cronbach’s Alpha, In addition, the variables were not multinormally
and it was 0.66. Frequency distributions of these distributed and the data comprised mostly variables
variables were analysed by pupil’s sex and age with an ordinal scale. The analyses simultaneously
using cross-tabulations and Chi-square test. A included both the confirmatory factor analysis
more detailed description of these questions and and structural equation modelling. The equivalence
variables is presented in Appendix 1. of the regression coefficients was statistically tested
The associations between perceived sleep with nested models that are based on sequential
quality and 28 variables were investigated among Chi-square tests. If the Chi-square test was not
15-year-olds. These 28 variables were selected on significant, the regression coefficients were equal.
the basis of earlier studies. After investigating In the reverse case, the coefficients were not
the correlation structure and using information equal.
158 J. Tynjälä et al.
Table 1: General sleep quality by age and sex

How well do you usually sleep? Boys Girls

11 years 13 years 15 years 11 years 13 years 15 years


% % % % % %

Very well 59 54 60 62 56 57
Quite well 33 35 33 32 37 34
Satisfactorily 7 10 5 6 5 6
Quite or very badly 1 1 2 1 2 3
Total 100 100 100 100 100 100
(n) (866) (630) (559) (841) (646) (609)
Between age groups, p value 0.049 NS
Between sex, p value NS 0.013 NS

NS = No statistical significant difference.

Table 2: Awakenings at night during the 6 months preceding the enquiry by age and sex

Frequency of nocturnal awakenings Boys Girls

11 years 13 years 15 years 11 years 13 years 15 years

About every night 4 3 2 3 2 3


More than once a week 7 6 4 5 6 5
About every week 10 8 8 11 10 10
About every month 19 22 18 18 19 20
Rarely or never 60 61 69 62 63 63
Total 100 100 100 100 100 100
(n) (860) (627) (573) (842) (646) (614)
Between age groups, p value 0.007 NS
Between sex, p value NS NS NS

NS = No statistical significant difference.

RESULTS week. There were no age group differences among


girls in nightly awakenings; among boys they
Perceived sleep quality in 11-, 13- and seemed to decrease with age (Table 2).
15-year-olds
Most adolescents slept well. About 60% felt
that they slept very well and every third rather Perceived sleep quality and its correlates
well. There were only slight sex or age group among 15-year-olds
differences in general sleep quality (Table 1). Structural equation models were used to describe
Every other adolescent had rarely or never associations of selected behavioural, social and
experienced difficulties in falling asleep during personal factors with perceived sleep quality.
the 6 months preceding the survey. However, The models include only those variables and
about 30% had had difficulties in initiating sleep factors that were accepted for the final statistical
at least once a week and 5% almost every night. models, which may be characterized more as of
There were no age group differences and only an inter-relationship type than explanatory type.
a small (p = 0.027) sex group difference in the The analyses were executed separately for boys
youngest age group where the problem was and girls. The factor structure is described in
slightly more typical among boys. Over 60% of Appendix 1. The polychoric correlation coefficients
the pupils did not report awakenings at night of the variables that were the basis for the final
during the 6 months preceding the survey, but models are presented in Appendices 2 and 3.
almost 20% had had nocturnal awakenings every Factors’ correlation structure used in the final
Perceived sleep quality 159

models is presented in Appendix 4. Only graph- sleep quality. Subjective sleep quality together
ically simplified versions of the structural equation with home atmosphere and leisure time physical
models are presented in this report: comprehen- activity explained 27% of the variance of self-
sive graphical presentations of the models are perception. Perceived sleep quality, home atmos-
available from the author. phere and self-perception explained 8% of the
variance of leisure time physical activity. In add-
ition, subjective sleep quality and home atmos-
A model for boys phere explained 29% of the variance of health
Among boys, home atmosphere, self-perception habits. Self-perception and leisure time physical
and health habits (second order factor composed activity also had a significant inter-relationship
of sleep hygiene factor and addictive behaviour with each other. Regarding the association of
factor) explained 22% of the variance in per- home atmosphere, self-perception, health habits
ceived sleep quality. Subjective sleep quality and leisure time physical activity with perceived
together with home atmosphere explained 11% sleep quality, the investigation of the equality
of the variance of self-perception and 14% of the of the regression coefficients revealed that the
variance of health habits. The second order factor, coefficients were not of equal importance (χ2 =
health habits, was formulated in the structural 9.290, d.f. = 3, p = 0.002). However, the first three
equation model, because the first order factors— were equally important (χ2 = 0.140, d.f. = 2, NS).
sleep hygiene and addictive behaviour—correlated It follows that leisure time physical activity
strongly with each other (r = 0.53). This cor- proved to be the least important in associations
relation was higher than their correlation with with subjective sleep quality (Figure 2).
the sleep quality factor (r = 0.24 and 0.22). Home The second order factor, health habits, was
atmosphere, self-perception and health habits formulated in the structural equation model,
were not equally important in associations with because the first order factors—sleep hygiene
subjective sleep quality (χ2 = 6.760, d.f. = 2, and addictive behaviour—correlated with each
p = 0.034), but self-perception and health habits other (r = 0.33). This correlation was higher than
were equally important (χ2 = 0.090, d.f. = 1, NS). the correlation between addictive behaviour
It follows that home atmosphere was the most factor and sleep quality factor (r = 0.25). The cor-
significant element concerning subjective sleep relation between sleep hygiene factor and sleep
quality. For the factor model with health habits quality factor was 0.36. For the factor model with
factor, but without structural equations, the χ2 health habits factor, but without structural
value was 59.096 (d.f. = 49, p = 0.153). Tests equations, the χ2 value was 109.421 (d.f. = 77,
for the equivalence of sleep hygiene factor and p = 0.009). Tests for the equivalence of the sleep
addictive behaviour factor in this second order hygiene factor and addictive behaviour factor were
factor were done against this model: these two performed against this model, and they revealed
factors were not equally important (χ2 = 14.162, that these two factors were equally important
d.f. = 1, p , 0.001—test of equivalence of in this second order factor (χ2 = 0.801, d.f. = 1,
residuals of the factors). Based on the previous NS—test of equivalence of residuals of the
Chi-square test, it follows that the sleep hygiene factors, Figure 2).
factor was more important than the addictive The reliability of the variables (R2) used in the
behaviour factor in this second order factor. model varied from 0.50 to 0.96. In a few cases,
Reliability of the variables (R2) used in the residuals of variables correlated positively with
model varied from 0.51 to 0.82. According to the each other. This indicates that the correlation
goodness-of-fit statistics of the model, the fit to structure of the variables was quite complicated
the data was good. The goodness of the statistical (see Appendix 3). According to the goodness-
model is presented in Figure 1. of-fit statistics of the model, the fit to the data
was not as good as in the boys’ model. The Chi-
square value was relatively high and its p value
A model for girls significant. However, the normed fit index (NFI)
Among girls, home atmosphere, self-perception, was very high (0.97) and the number of cases (n)
health habits (second order factor composed was greater than the critical number of cases
of sleep hygiene factor and addictive behaviour (CN). These indicated that the large sample size
factor) and leisure time physical activity ex- (n = 551) had had an effect on the Chi-square
plained 30% of the variance of perceived value and that conclusions about the goodness
160 J. Tynjälä et al.

Fig. 1: Factors associated with perceived sleep quality among 15-year-old boys: structural equation model.
Variables are presented in boxes, factors in ellipses; the R2 value above each factor describes how much of the
variance of that factor could be explained by the other factors (or variables); small arrows outside boxes or
ellipses describe residuals (actual numbers were intentionally dropped from the figure); the rest of the
abbreviations are described below the figure.

of the statistical model should not be made on About 30% of pupils had also had difficulties in
the basis of the Chi-square test. All the other falling asleep and almost every fifth adolescent
parameters of goodness of the model were reported nocturnal awakenings every week.
statistically adequate (Figure 2). These prevalences were relatively high. Accord-
ing to other studies (e.g. Tynjälä et al., 1997), at
least every third pupil was tired on almost every
DISCUSSION school morning or felt tired more often than
once a week. It may be concluded that a large
Åkerstedt et al. (1994) concluded in their study proportion of pupils in every classroom has a
that the index of sleep quality should include weakened ability to concentrate on school work
the following measures: sleep quality; calmness or other activities. Results on prevalences gained
of sleep; easiness of falling asleep; sleeping in this study are in line with earlier studies (e.g.
throughout the night; and refreshness after sleep. Andrade et al., 1993; Tynjälä, 1993; Saarenpää-
They also concluded that sleep quality is mainly Heikkilä et al., 1995), but prevalences cannot,
a question of sleep continuity. The questions however, be directly compared with each other
about sleep quality used in this study covered because of, e.g. differences in operationalizations
most of these criteria. Adolescents’ own percep- of sleep variables. Among 15-year-old boys, home
tions indicated that about every tenth person felt atmosphere seemed to be a very important
that their sleep quality was at most satisfactory. element in relation to perceived sleep quality.
Perceived sleep quality 161

Fig. 2: Factors associated with perceived sleep quality among 15-year-old girls: structural equation model.
Explanations: see caption for Figure 1.

Among girls of the same age, self-perception, 1994). The results in this study are in line with
health habits and home atmosphere were equally the finding that poor sleepers are less satisfied
important in associations with subjective sleep in their relationships with their parents (Healey
quality. et al., 1981). Adolescents’ complaints of poor sleep
may even be regarded as a possibly meaningful
and sensitive sign of severe family or personal
Interpretation of models disruption. Not only family relations but also
The association between self-esteem or self- parents’ health status reflects perceived sleep
image and sleep quality indicators has also been quality. If adolescents, especially girls, are worried
reported in many earlier studies (e.g. Price et al., about their parents’ health and well-being, it may
1978; Kirmil-Gray et al., 1984; Turtle et al., 1996). manifest itself as poor perceived sleep quality
In this study, the associations between home (Vignau et al., 1997).
atmosphere, pupils’ self-perception and perceived Physical activity was a significant factor only
sleep quality indicated that good relationships at among girls. This finding is not in concordance
home form an important ground for positive self- with the results obtained by Weydahl (1991a).
perception and health-promotive habits which This may result from differences in questions
all correlated with good subjective sleep quality. concerning subjective sleep quality and physical
The results support the idea that family support in activity. Among boys, health habits—composed
general has salutary effects on adolescents’ psycho- of sleep hygiene and addictive behaviour—and
logical well-being (Reicher, 1993; Vilhjalmsson, self-perception were equally important, but not
162 J. Tynjälä et al.

as important as home atmosphere in associations resemble each other but the weights of different
with subjective sleep quality. Sleep hygiene and elements are not the same.
addictive behaviour correlated strongly with The question of missing data arises when using
each other among boys, but this correlation was many variables in the analyses. This was the case
also significant in girls. This association has been in the structural equation models in 15-year-olds.
found in earlier studies in Finland and elsewhere A comparison of respondents who were included
among adolescents (e.g. Rimpelä and Rimpelä, in the models (no missing data in any of the
1983; Tynjälä et al., 1993, 1997). variables in the model) against those respondents
One purpose of this study was to investigate who had missing data in one or more variables
how latent behavioural and other factors are (they were automatically excluded from the
associated with subjective sleep quality. In many models) revealed only one difference between
earlier studies, the focus has been on the re- the groups: the ‘excluded’ group—17% of boys
lationships between single manifest (observable) and 10% of girls—went to bed later than the
variables and perceived sleep quality, therefore group included in the models. No other significant
direct comparisons with earlier studies cannot differences were found. It may be concluded that
be made. However, it can be concluded that the the results of the structural equation models
results of this study support earlier studies, e.g. represent the whole 15-year-old study group
frequent use of addictive substances (drinking adequately well.
and smoking in this study) had a negative effect
on perceived sleep quality (e.g. Lexcen and
Hicks, 1993; Phillips and Danner, 1995; Vignau Future research challenges
et al., 1997). This seems to be typical in adolescent In future research, the role of family relation-
years, but these associations have been weak or ships, social support in the family, the health status
did not exist in the adult population (Palmer of parents and siblings and their associations
et al., 1980; Urponen et al., 1988; Vuori et al., 1988). with adolescents’ sleep and subjective health in
This study focused on the associations of general should be covered in more detail, not
selected behavioural, social and psychological only in cross-sectional studies but also in follow-
factors with adolescents’ perceived sleep quality. up studies (e.g. Välimaa et al., 1993; Vilhjalmsson,
Exclusion of many relevant indicators of subject- 1994; Vignau et al., 1997). In this study, only one
ive health was done intentionally to emphasize question measured family relationships, but even
the main interest, subjective sleep quality. Initially, that proved to be of great importance concerning
the variables in the models were selected on the adolescents’ subjective sleep quality. Good
basis of earlier studies. During the formulation family relationships probably have a ‘protective’
of structural equation models, many originally effect in relation to adolescents’ health and in a
significant variables—on the basis of initial cor- broader sense to their well-being (see Nestmann
relations with the indicators of subjective sleep and Hurrelmann, 1994). Aro (1988) indicated
quality variables—were dropped from the models. in her study that children from both divorced
Exclusions of these variables were made on a and discordant families reported more distress
statistical basis: if the variable was no longer symptoms, more abundant use and in boys more
significant in the model it was dropped out. As a delinquent acts than children from intact families.
result, the models describe the associations Also, severe illness of a family member was highly
rather efficiently. The home atmosphere variable associated with a higher number of symptoms,
in the statistical models could be described as and with the greatest increase in symptoms in
a natural ‘background’ factor because it is an both sexes. The effect of life events involving
important ground for all later developments in illness or loss was greater in boys than girls (Aro,
children and adolescents. Home atmosphere also 1988; see also Rutter, 1985). The results gained in
seemed to have a great impact on adolescents’ this study emphasize the importance of family
self-perception and health habits in general. relationships to adolescents’ health and well-
Manni et al. (1997) found in their study that being. An interesting future topic would be to
gender was an important factor in associations investigate which matters constitute a good night’s
with chronic poor sleep. There were more sleep from a wider perspective (Zammit, 1988).
females than males among poor sleepers in their Few studies have investigated the association
study. However, in this study models were between sleep variables and perceived health
compiled separately for both sexes. The models status in adolescence. The results of these studies
Perceived sleep quality 163

have been somewhat contradictory. On one ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS


hand, studies have suggested that sleep quality
correlates more strongly than sleep quantity with This study was financially supported by the Yrjö
health (Pilcher et al., 1997). On the other hand, in Jahnsson foundation and the Juho Vainio
some studies sleep disturbance is associated with foundation. The authors wish to thank Phyllis
perceived health status only in middle adolescence Wacker, PhD and Ms Kirsi Talvitie for correcting
(15–17-year-olds), but not in the early (12–14- the language.
year-olds) or late phase of adolescence (18–21-
year-olds) [Yarcheski and Mahon, 1994; Mahon, Address for correspondence:
Jorma Tynjälä
1995]. Thus, further research is required to clarify University of Jyväskylä
the association between sleep and perceived Department of Health Sciences
health in adolescence. P.O. Box 35
According to Mahon (1995), better subjective FIN-40351 Jyväskylä
measures of sleep for adolescents are needed: Finland
an effort should be made to develop reliable and
valid measures of the experience of sleep during
adolescent development. Nurses and other health REFERENCES
care professionals should routinely collect data
on adolescents’ alcohol and drug use because Aarø, L. E., Wold, B. and Smith, C. (1994) Health Behaviour
they have been known or thought to influence in School-aged Children. WHO Cross-National Survey
(HBSC). Research Protocol for the 1993–94 Study.
sleep (e.g. Lexcen and Hicks, 1993; Phillips and Research Center for Health Promotion, University of
Danner, 1995; Tynjälä et al., 1997). Health care Bergen.
professionals should create a knowledge base Aarø, L. E., Laberg, J. C. and Wold, B. (1995) Health
regarding the extent to which sleep contributes behaviours among adolescents: towards a hypothesis of
two dimensions. Health Education Research, 10, 83–93.
to the health and well-being of adolescents Åkerstedt, T., Hume, K., Minors, D. and Waterhouse, J.
(Mahon, 1995). (1994) The subjective meaning of good sleep, an intra-
individual approach using the Karolinska Sleep Diary.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 287–296.
Some practical considerations Andrade, M. M. M., Benedito-Silva, A. A., Domenice, S.,
Arnhold, I. J. P. and Menna-Barreto, L. (1993) Sleep
What kind of practical considerations should be characteristics of adolescents: a longitudinal study.
made in school health education, school health Journal of Adolescent Health, 14, 401–406.
care staff training and parent counselling? Aro, H., Paronen, O. and Aro, S. (1987) Psychosomatic
Mahon (1995) suggests the following issues to be symptoms among 14–16-year-old Finnish adolescents.
Social Psychiatry, 22, 171–176.
discussed to promote good sleep in adolescence: Aro, H. (1988) Stress, development and psychosomatic
pupils should sleep in quiet, comfortable and symptoms in adolescence. A study of 14 to 16-year-old
well-ventilated rooms. They should avoid exces- school children. Acta Universitatis Tamperensis, Ser. A,
sive expenditure of energy in the evening hours Vol. 242. University of Tampere, Tampere (Academic
immediately preceding sleep, and if needed, pupils dissertation).
Carskadon, M. A. (1990) Patterns of sleep and sleepiness in
should be instructed in the use of relaxation adolescents. Pediatrician, 17, 5–12.
techniques. Ferber, R. (1990) Sleep schedule-dependent causes of
Poor sleep quality, poor sleep hygiene, fre- insomnia and sleepiness in middle childhood and
quent use of alcohol, regular smoking and poor adolescents. Pediatrician, 17, 13–20.
Fischer, B. E. and Wilson, A. E. (1987) Selected sleep
self-perception identified in this study provide an disturbances in school children reported by parents:
important challenge to health promotion among prevalence, interrelationships, behavioral correlates and
school children. Although adolescents’ sleep parental attributions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 64,
quality is clearly related to home atmosphere, 1147–1157.
the role of the school is also important. In order Hallman, M., Kannas, L. and Tynjälä, J. (1992) Smoking,
drinking and healthiness of living. A study of smoking
to deal with these sleep-related problems that and drinking among 11–16-year-old Finns. Reports of the
affect children’s health, issues about sleep and National Board of Health, Finland, 61/1992, Helsinki,
rest need to be included in the health education Finland (in Finnish).
curriculum in schools. Intensive cooperation Hawkins, J. and Shaw, P. (1992) Self-reported sleep quality
in college students: a repeated measures approach. Sleep,
and a good functioning network between school 15, 545–549.
and home will also be needed to foster dialogue Healey, E. S., Kales, A., Monroe, L. J., Bixler, E. O.,
between teachers, school nurses and parents. Chamberlin, K. and Soldatos, C. R. (1981) Onset of
164 J. Tynjälä et al.
insomnia: role of life-stress events. Psychosomatic Pilcher, J. J., Ginter, D. R. and Sadowsky, B. (1997) Sleep
Medicine, 43, 439–451. quality versus sleep quantity: relationships between sleep
Hyyppä, M. T. (1991) Promoting good sleep. Health and measures of health, well-being and sleepiness in
Promotion International, 6, 103–110. college students. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42,
Hyyppä, M. T., Kronholm, E. and Matlar, C.-E. (1991) 583–596.
Mental well-being of good sleepers in a random population Price, T., Coates, T. J., Thoresen, C. E. and Grinstead, O. A.
sample. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 64, 25–34. (1978) Prevalence and correlates of poor sleep among
Jöreskog, K. G. and Sörbom, D. (1993) LISREL 8 and adolescents. American Journal of Diseases of Children,
PRELIS 2 Documentation. Scientific Software Inter- 132, 583–586.
national, Chicago. Rahkonen, O., Ahlström, S. and Karvonen, S. (1992)
Kahn, A., Merckt, C., Rebuffat, E., Mozin, M. J., Sottiaux, Drinking, health and a strenuous lifestyle among young
M., Blum, D. and Hennart, P. (1989) Sleep problems in people. Alkoholipolitiikka, 57, 169–178 (in Finnish with
healthy preadolescents. Pediatrics, 84, 542–546. an English abstract, p. 178).
King, A., Wold, B., Tudor-Smith, C. and Harel, Y. (1996) Reicher, H. (1993) Family and peer relations and social-
The Health of Youth: A Cross-National Survey. WHO emotional problems in adolescence. Studia Psychologica,
Regional Publications, European Series No. 69. 35, 403–408.
Copenhagen, Report 69, Canada. Rimpelä, M. and Rimpelä, A. (1983) Sleeping habits and
Kirmil-Gray, K., Eagleston, J. R., Gibson, E. and Thoresen, disturbances. In Rimpelä, M., Rimpelä, A., Ahlström, S.,
C. E. (1984) Sleep disturbance in adolescents: sleep quality, Honkala, E., Kannas, L., Laakso, L., Paronen, O., Rajala,
sleep habits, beliefs about sleep, and daytime functioning. M. and Telama, R. (eds) Health Habits among Finnish
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 13, 375–384. Youth. The Juvenile Health Habits Study 1977–1979.
Levy, D., Gray-Donald, K., Leech, J., Zvagulis, I. and Pless, Publications of the National Board of Health, Helsinki,
B. (1986) Sleep patterns and problems in adolescents. No. 4, pp. 71–81 (in Finnish with an English summary,
Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 7, 386–389. p. 213).
Lexcen, F. J. and Hicks, R. A. (1993) Does cigarette smoking Rosenberg, M. (1985) Self-concept and psychological well-
increase sleep problems? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, being in adolescence. In Leathy, R. L. (ed.) The Develop-
16–18. ment of the Self. Academic Press, Orlando, pp. 205–246.
Lintunen, T., Leskinen, E., Oinonen, M., Salinto, M. and Rutter, M. (1985) Resilience in the face of adversity.
Rahkila, P. (1995) Change, reliability, and stability in self- Protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disorders.
perceptions in early adolescence: a four-year follow-up British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 598–611.
study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, Saarenpää-Heikkilä, O. A., Rintahaka, P. J., Laippala, P. J.
18, 351–364. and Koivikko, M. J. (1995) Sleep habits and disorders
Lugaresi, E., Cirignotta, F., Zucconi, M., Mondini, S., Lenzi, in Finnish schoolchildren. Journal of Sleep Research, 4,
P. L. and Coggagna, G. (1983) Good and poor sleepers: 173–182.
an epidemiological survey of the San Marino Population. Shapiro, C. M., Warren, P. M., Trinder, J., Paxton, S. J.,
In Guilleminault, C. and Lugaresi, E. (eds) Sleep/Wake Oswald, I., Flenley, D. C. and Catterall, J. R. (1984) Fitness
Disorders: Natural History, Epidemiology and Long- facilitates sleep. European Journal of Applied Physiology,
Term Evolution. Raven Press, New York, pp. 1–12. 53, 1–4.
Mahon, N. E. (1994) Loneliness and sleep during Strauch, I. and Meier, B. (1988) Sleep need in adolescents:
adolescence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 227–231. a longitudinal approach. Sleep, 11, 378–386.
Mahon, N. E. (1995) The contributions of sleep to perceived Turtle, J., Jones, A. and Hickman, M. (1996) Young people
health status during adolescence. Public Health Nursing, and health: the health behaviour of school-aged children.
12, 127–133. A report on the 1995 survey. August, BMRP International,
Manni, R., Ratti, M. T., Marchioni, E., Castelnovo, G., London.
Murelli, R., Sartori, I., Galimberti, C. A. and Tartara, A. Tynjälä, J. (1993) Sleeping habits, sleep difficulties and
(1997) Poor sleep in adolescents: a study of 869 17-year- educational expectations among Finnish school children.
old Italian secondary school students. Journal of Sleep In Shemeikka, S. and Nissinen, A. (eds) The Yearbook of
Research, 6, 44–49. the Health Education Research 1992. Publications of the
Monroe, L. J. (1967) Psychological and physiological National Board of Health, Helsinki, No. 1, pp. 99–113
differences between good and poor sleepers. Journal of (in Finnish with an English summary, p. 109).
Abnormal Psychology, 72, 255–264. Tynjälä, J. and Kannas, L. (1993) Sleeping habits of Finnish
Morrison, D. N., McGee, R. and Stanton, W. R. (1992) Sleep school children by sociodemographic background. Health
problems in adolescence. Journal of American Academy Promotion International, 8, 281–289.
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 94–99. Tynjälä, J., Kannas, L. and Välimaa, R. (1993) How young
Nestmann, F. and Hurrelmann, K. (1994) Child and Europeans sleep. Health Education Research, 7, 69–80.
adolescent research as a challenge and opportunity for Tynjälä, J. and Törmäkangas, K. (1995) The 1994 data of a
social support theory, measurement, and intervention: WHO-co-ordinated survey of school children’s health
and vice versa. In Nestmann, F. and Hurrelman, K. (eds) behaviour. In Kannas, L. (ed.) Perceived Health, Well-being
Social Networks and Social Support in Childhood and and School Satisfaction Among School Children. National
Adolescence. Walter de Gruyter, New York, pp. 1–22. Board of Education, Helsinki, pp. 193–197 (in Finnish).
Palmer, C. D., Harrison, G. A. and Hiorns, R. W. (1980) Tynjälä, J., Kannas, L. and Levälahti, E. (1997) Perceived
Association between smoking and drinking and sleep tiredness among adolescents and its association with
duration. Annals of Human Biology, 7, 103–107. sleep habits and use of psychoactive substances. Journal
Phillips, B. A. and Danner, F. J. (1995) Cigarette smoking of Sleep Research, 6, 189–198.
and sleep disturbance. Archives of International Urponen, H., Vuori, I., Hasan, J. and Partinen, M.
Medicine, 7, 734–737. (1988) Sleeping habits, sleep quality and sleep disorders
Perceived sleep quality 165
in urban working-aged people. Sosiaalilääketieteellinen Vuori, I., Urponen, H., Hasan, J. and Partinen, M. (1988)
Aikakauslehti, 25, 338–351 (in Finnish with an English Epidemiology of exercise effects on sleep. Acta Physio-
abstract, p. 351). logica Scandinavica, 133 (Supplement 574), 3–7.
Välimaa, R., Kannas, L. and Pötsönen, R. (1993) Social Weydahl, A. (1991a) Sex differences in the effect of
support, adolescents and perceived health. In Shemeikka, exercise upon sleep. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72,
S. and Nissinen, A. (eds) The Yearbook of the Health 455–465.
Education Research 1992. Publications of the National Weydahl, A. (1991b) Sleep-quality among girls with differ-
Board of Health, Helsinki, No. 1, pp. 63–80 (in Finnish ent involvement in competitive sports during fall
with an English summary, pp. 79–80). in the arctic circle. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73,
Vignau, J., Bailly, D., Duhamel, A., Vervaecke, P., Beuscart, 883–892.
R. and Collinet, C. (1997) Epidemiologic study of sleep Yarcheski, A. and Mahon, N. E. (1994) A study of sleep
quality and troubles in French secondary school during adolescence. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 9,
adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 21, 343–350. 357–367.
Vilhjalmsson, R. (1994) Effects of social support on self- Zammit, G. K. (1988) Subjective ratings of the character-
assessed health in adolescence. Journal of Youth and istics and sequelae of good and poor sleep in normals.
Adolescence, 23, 437–452. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 123–130.

APPENDIX 1. FACTOR STRUCTURES USED IN THE FINAL MODELS AMONG 15-YEAR-OLDS

Factor (F), variable (V) Questions and their alternatives in parentheses

Perceived sleep quality (F) How well do you usually sleep? (very well, quite well, satisfactorily, quite badly,
very badly).
How often do you have difficulties in falling asleep? (almost daily, more than once a
week, about once a week, about once a month, rarely or never).
How often do you wake up during the night? (almost daily, more than once a week,
about once a week, about once a month, rarely or never).
Home atmosphere (V, F)a What kind of atmosphere do you have in your home? (very good, quite good, neither
good nor bad, quite bad, very bad).
Sleep hygiene (F) Do you usually go to bed at a certain time? (very regularly, quite regularly, quite
irregularly, very irregularly).
When do you usually go to bed if you have to go to school the next morning?
(alternatives ranged from no later than 9 pm to 2 am or later in half hour intervals).
Self-perception (F) Do you feel helpless? (always, often, sometimes, rarely, never).
Do you feel confident in yourself? (always, often, sometimes, rarely, never).
Index of self-esteem was formulated from the following statements: I feel that I am as
capable and skilful as any other person; I think that I have plenty of good qualities;
I often feel that I am a failure; I can manage things as well as any other person;
I feel that I am all right; I am satisfied with myself; I sometimes feel totally useless;
I sometimes think I am no good at all (strongly agree, partly agree, partly disagree,
strongly disagree).
Addictive behaviour (F) How many cigarettes do you usually smoke in a week?
Have you ever had so much alcohol that you were really drunk? (no, never; yes, once;
yes, two–three times; yes, four–10 times; yes, more than 10 times).
How often do you drink beer? (every day, every week, every month, less than once a
month, never).
Leisure time physical activity (F) Outside school hours: How often do you usually exercise in your free time so much
that you get out of breath or sweat? (every day, four–six times a week, two–three
times a week, once a week, once a month, less than once a month, never).
Outside school hours: How many hours do you usually exercise in your free time
so much that you get out of breath or sweat? (None, about 30 min, about 1 h, about
2–3 h, about 4–6 h, 7 h or more).
Are you member of a sports club? (no; yes, but I don’t participate in exercises;
yes, and I am training in a sports club). This variable was dichotomized afterwards:
the first two categories were combined.

a
Home atmosphere factor was composed of home atmosphere variable.
166 J. Tynjälä et al.
APPENDIX 2. POLYCHORIC CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS AMONG 15-YEAR-OLD BOYS
(n = 484)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12)

(1) Wake up during night → seldom 1.00


(2) Difficulty in falling asleep → seldom 0.55 1.00
(3) How well do you sleep in general → well 0.59 0.53 1.00
(4) Bedtime during school week → early 0.02 0.18 0.00 1.00
(5) Regularity of bedtime → regular 0.12 0.07 0.19 0.46 1.00
(6) Cigarettes a week → none 0.06 0.17 0.05 0.38 0.24 1.00
(7) Times being drunk → never 0.08 0.19 0.06 0.33 0.29 0.71 1.00
(8) Drinking beer → never 0.08 0.17 0.06 0.35 0.26 0.66 0.78 1.00
(9) Feeling of helplessness → never 0.28 0.20 0.17 –0.07 0.01 –0.10 –0.04 –0.09 1.00
(10) Feeling of self-confidence → always 0.23 0.11 0.17 –0.08 0.11 –0.10 –0.04 –0.09 0.58 1.00
(11) Self-esteem → good 0.12 0.17 0.17 0.06 0.16 0.12 0.10 0.04 0.49 0.54 1.00
(12) Home atmosphere → good 0.37 0.25 0.28 0.08 0.27 0.11 0.21 0.15 0.22 0.27 0.24 1.00

APPENDIX 3. POLYCHORIC CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS AMONG 15-YEAR-OLD GIRLS


(n = 551)

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) (15)

(1) Wake up during night → seldom 1.00


(2) Difficulty in falling asleep → seldom 0.53 1.00
(3) How well do you sleep in
general → well 0.62 0.65 1.00
(4) Bedtime during school week → early 0.04 0.18 0.06 1.00
(5) Regularity of bedtime → regular 0.16 0.37 0.30 0.59 1.00
(6) Cigarettes a week → none 0.17 0.20 0.18 0.21 0.28 1.00
(7) Times being drunk → never 0.09 0.15 0.06 0.19 0.19 0.66 1.00
(8) Drinking beer → never –0.03 0.13 0.05 0.09 0.22 0.59 0.75 1.00
(9) Physical activity (times) →
almost daily 0.03 0.09 0.16 0.02 –0.02 0.08 0.04 0.05 1.00
(10) Physical activity (h) →
many hours a week 0.18 0.10 0.16 0.04 0.06 0.21 0.05 0.05 0.81 1.00
(11) Activity in sport club → active 0.25 0.05 0.19 –0.04 –0.07 0.17 –0.04 0.00 0.59 0.71 1.00
(12) Feeling of helplessness → never 0.27 0.32 0.31 –0.05 0.06 0.01 –0.04 –0.06 0.05 0.09 0.05 1.00
(13) Feeling of self-confidence → always 0.29 0.29 0.30 –0.02 0.02 0.05 –0.02 –0.05 0.12 0.16 0.01 0.53 1.00
(14) Self-esteem → good 0.30 0.28 0.29 0.05 0.13 0.21 0.05 0.00 0.14 0.24 0.17 0.57 0.64 1.00
(15) Home atmosphere → good 0.26 0.29 0.43 0.10 0.19 0.21 0.24 0.22 0.19 0.17 0.06 0.17 0.29 0.30 1.00

APPENDIX 4. CORRELATIONS OF FACTORS AND HOME ATMOSPHERE VARIABLE IN THE


FINAL MODELS. BOYS’ CORRELATIONS IN THE UPPER TRIANGLE AND GIRLS’
CORRELATIONS IN THE LOWER TRIANGLE

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

(1) Perceived sleep quality → good 1.000 — 0.316 0.404 0.308


(2) Physical activity → active 0.252 1.000 — — —
(3) Self-perception → good 0.472 0.258 1.000 0.262 0.062
(4) Home atmosphere → good 0.384 0.208 0.420 1.000 0.337
(5) Health habits → healthy 0.519 0.139 0.119 0.435 1.000

You might also like