Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Neotrop Entomol (2012) 41:171–177

DOI 10.1007/s13744-012-0030-3

ECOLOGY, BEHAVIOR AND BIONOMICS

Color and Shape Discrimination in the Stingless Bee Scaptotrigona


mexicana Guérin (Hymenoptera, Apidae)
D SÁNCHEZ1, R VANDAME2
1
El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Tapachula, Chiapas, Mexico
2
El Colegio de la Frontera Sur, San Cristóbal de las Casas, Chiapas, Mexico

Keywords Abstract
Choice behavior, food sources, learning, To increase our understanding in bee vision ecology, we investigated the
Meliponini, visual stimuli color and shape discrimination performance of the stingless bee Scap-
Correspondence totrigona mexicana Guérin. Our main goal was to describe the choice
Daniel Sánchez, El Colegio de la Frontera behavior of experienced foragers over time, trying to understand to
Sur, Carretera Antiguo Aeropuerto Km 2.5, what extent color and shape stimuli (separately tested) aid them to
30700 Tapachula, Chiapas, Mexico;
dsanchez@ecosur.mx choose the rewarding option, in the presence of distracting, unreward-
ing stimuli. Single foragers were trained to collect sucrose solution from
Edited by Kleber Del Claro – UFU
a target plate. Afterwards, one distracting, unrewarding plate was
Received 6 September 2011 and accepted placed besides the target plate and eight choices were recorded. Our
9 February 2012 results showed that both color and shape stimuli assisted efficiently the
Published online 17 April 2012
trained foragers in locating the target plate. However, foragers chose
* Sociedade Entomológica do Brasil 2012 significantly more often the target plate in the color experiments than
in the shape experiments. In conclusion, in our experimental setup,
color was of better assistance to the foragers of S. mexicana than
shape to choose their rewards. This is the first study in which it is
demonstrated that the choice performance over time in a stingless
bee depends upon the characteristics of the resource, such as shape
and color.

Introduction Chittka 1999, Giurfa et al 1999, Horridge 2003, Menzel &


Giurfa 2006, Horridge 2007). Similar findings have been
Foragers of highly social bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Api- described in other highly social bees as well. In Bombus
nae) can quickly learn the visual and olfactory character- terrestris L., for example, foragers trained to different
istics of a resource, provided the calories obtained from artificial flowers preferred the morphs they previously vis-
the use of such resource are above a determined threshold ited (Gack 1981, Gumbert 2000). The capacity of the sting-
(Gould & Gould 1988, Chittka et al 1997, Beekman et al less bees Melipona rufiventris Lepeletier and Melipona
2003, Dyer & Chittka 2004a). Color, shape, and odor are quadrifasciata Lepeletier to discriminate shape, color, and
floral characteristics that convey information that bees can luminosity was evaluated and compared with that of A.
use to return to rewarding patches, avoiding the unprofit- mellifera, showing extensive similarities (Pessotti & Gomes
able ones (Collett 1992). Such characteristics are thus 1981). Foragers of the stingless bees, Tetragonisca angus-
thought to have co-evolved with the vision of bees, en- tula Latreille, Scaptotrigona mexicana Guérin-Méneville,
abling species-specific pollen transfer in plants and allowing Trigona fulviventris Guérin, Trigona fuscipennis Friese,
a more efficient food collection in bees in return (Chittka & and Trigona nigra Cresson (Villa & Weiss 1990, Slaa et al
Menzel 1992). 1998b), also directed the majority of their choices towards
In the case of the honeybee Apis mellifera L., a large the artificial flower they previously visited by using the
body of research has demonstrated its remarkable ability color of the food sources as a reference. The similarity in
to learn floral characteristics (Gould 1986, Lehrer et al 1995, the color receptors between stingless bees and honeybees
172 Sánchez & Vandame

explains why there is little difference in color discrimination Stimuli—colors


between those two taxa (Hertel & Ventura 1985, Chittka &
Wells 2004). However, at a finer scale, differences among Colors commonly used to study honeybee vision were
species are observed. For instance, the sensitivity of photo- employed: brown, red, pink, green, yellow, orange, blue,
receptors in the compound eye of M. quadrifasciata and A. and white. They were presented as 50-mm diameter×3-
mellifera is strikingly different at the electrophysiological mm thick circular plates made of ethyl vinyl acetate and
level, since M. quadrifasciata discriminates better in the colored from factory (PHASA industries, México). Previ-
bluish region, while the honeybee does it better at the ously to the experiments the plates were extensively
other wavelengths (Menzel et al 1989). Such difference washed with water, air dried, and covered with a thin
might be the reflection of their particular evolutionary layer of contact adhesive to allow us to remove any
history and the ecological context they live in, as hypoth- potential odor marks from the plates. Plates for color
esized for the visual and behavioral differences between experiments were disk-shaped since it is known that
honeybees and bumblebees (Dyer et al 2008). this form elicits a good discrimination response in A.
The discrimination process does not always develop into mellifera (Campan & Lehrer 2002).
a fixed, hardwired behavior. Apis mellifera foragers, for
example, discriminate between different artificial flowers Stimuli—shapes
only if a difference in caloric reward is obtained (Lamb &
Wells, 1995). Choice behavior can actually improve over Eight different forms were evaluated for the shape experi-
time, and foragers of A. mellifera will reach a plateau in as ments (cross, scalene, isosceles, bipetal, hexagon, octagon,
few as 13 visits (Giurfa 2004). On the other hand, it takes square, and circle). Making of the plates followed the same
much longer, up to 50 visits, to Bombus terrestris foragers procedure as with the color experiments, all with an aver-
to reach an accuracy of 70% (Dyer & Chittka 2004b). age size of approximately 20 cm2. They all were yellow,
Unfortunately, there is no similar data available for sting- because it is known that honeybee color discrimination is
less bees on this matter which would allow for a more good at this color (von Helversen 1972, Chittka & Wells
robust comparison with honeybees and bumblebees. 2004) and because photoreceptors of stingless bees have
Therefore, we investigated if the color and the shape of similar sensitivities to those from honeybees (Peitsch et al
the food source affect the choice behavior of the stingless 1992). Some shapes (square, triangles, and circle) were
Scaptotrigona mexicana, and if there is any improvement chosen because they have been traditionally used for these
over time in choosing for a particular stimulus. type of studies (Campan & Lehrer 2002), while the remain-
ing ones were used to explore their visual abilities with
disrupted (cross, bipetal) and intermediate forms from
square to circle (hexagon and octagon).
Material and Methods
Feeder
Study site and species
The “artificial flowers” (sources of stimuli) were placed on
Experiments were carried out in El Colegio de la Frontera the top of a feeder. The feeder was built on top of a 1-m
Sur in the city of Tapachula, Chiapas, Mexico, between 9 high tripod with a 15×15-cm square gray cardboard on top.
and 12 h from January–March 2005 and April–June 2011. Color or shape plates were placed on the cardboard to
Foragers from five healthy queenright colonies each with offer the bees the reward. The cardboard was positioned
2,000 to 3,000 individuals of S. mexicana housed in wood 45° relative to the ground and facing the colony under
boxes were evaluated. Scaptotrigona mexicana is a species evaluation.
of ecological and economic importance in the study region
given its abundance, its role as a pollinator of crops like Training phase
rambutan (Nephelium lappaceum) and its potential for
stingless beekeeping (Guzmán-Díaz et al 2004). It repre- A group of two to four foragers per repetition was trained
sents a good animal model to our investigation since it is from the nest entrance to a feeder located 5 m away from
very efficient at collecting resources in changing conditions the colony to collect 1.0 M sucrose droplets from the
(Sánchez et al 2008), in addition to its ability to choose the training plate. From this group only one forager was ran-
resource with high precision (Sánchez et al 2007). Earlier domly chosen for further testing. This focal bee was dis-
studies have hypothesized that S. mexicana foragers will tinctively marked on its thorax with water-based paint and
choose the stimuli they are trained to in higher frequencies left free to continue visiting the feeder; the remaining bees
over other alternatives (Sánchez et al 2007, 2008). were trapped with suction tubes, paint-marked on the
Color and Shape Discrimination in a Stingless Bee 173

thorax and kept away from the setup to minimize social suitable for binary outputs (yes–no), such as the responses
facilitation and/or odor marking bias. of the foragers in the present study.
To test whether the frequency of choices to the target
plate departed from a random distribution, two datasets
Experimental setup—test phase
following a Poisson distribution were created and com-
pared against color and shape actual data using the same
Once at 5 m, and while the focal bee was inside the colony,
GEE approach as above.
the gray cardboard and the training plate were replaced
with identical, but new and clean, gray cardboard and
training plate (target plate from now on, with the same
Results
color or shape as the training plate). This was done in order
to minimize any possible effect of odor marking on choice
Color and shape discrimination
behavior during the tests. Simultaneously, one alternative
plate was positioned besides the target plate, within the
A total of 127 foragers were evaluated in the color experi-
cardboard. For the color experiments, the alternative plate
ments and 144 in the shape experiments. Each focal bee
had a color different to that of the target plate; for the
made eight choices within a time period of roughly 15 min.
shape experiments, the alternative plate had a different
Our data indicated that S. mexicana has the ability to dis-
shape to that of the target plate. Thus in the next visit to
criminate food sources by using color and shape cues, since
the setup the forager had to confront a new situation, with
the frequency of correct choices (the foragers choosing the
two possibilities (plates) to choose from: the target and the
target plate) departed significantly from random for both
alternative plates. These plates were presented without
color (Wald Chi-square0150.49, df01, P<0.001) and shape
any food droplet before the focal bee could make any
experiments (Wald Chi-square017.60, df01, P<0.001). How-
choice to (1) reduce any possible interference of the drop-
ever, they picked the target plate significantly more often in
let on choice behavior and (2) to avoid the forager from
the color experiments than in the shape experiments (Wald
associating the alternative plate with a reward. However,
Chi-square058.741, df01, P<0.001; Tables 1 and 2, Fig 1).
to keep the bees visiting the setup, a droplet was placed in
the target plate after the focal bee took off the chosen
Choice behavior over time
plate, either target or alternative. Moreover, to reduce any
potential micro-site learning, the cardboard was rotated,
When analyzing preferences for any of the stimuli, the
and the target and alternative plates exchanged positions
frequency with which the target feeder was chosen was
randomly after each choice.
not significantly different among the tested colors (Wald
Because we wanted to evaluate if our foragers could
Chi-square 04.76, df 07, P 00.689; Table 1) nor shapes
improve their choices over time, our focal bees had to
(Wald Chi-square05.39, df07, P00.613; Table 2). However,
choose between the target and the alternative plates eight
foragers seemed to change their precision over time in the
times. When the focal bee had completed its set of choices,
color experiments (Wald Chi-square021.77, df07, P00.003;
the feeder was removed, and the trapped bees were set
Fig 1), since the number of correct choices increased as time
free. In each repetition, a group of bees unfamiliar with our
passed (pairwise comparison taking visit 8 as comparison
setup, which was determined by the lack of paint-marks in
standard: Wald Chi-square>5, df01, P<0.05 for the first
its thorax, was subjected to training.
three visits; Wald Chi-square<2.5, df01, P>0.10 for the
remaining visits). Foragers seem to have fine-tuned their
Statistics precision until the fourth visit to the setup, from which a
plateau was reached. No change over time in the number of
Data on the choice behavior were analyzed with General- correct choices was detected in the shape experiments,
ized Estimating Equations (GEE) models considering a bina- though (Wald Chi-square08.34, df07, P00.304).
ry distribution with a log-link, and an autoregressive
working correlation matrix structure to account for the
dichotomous and time-correlated (repeated measure) na- Discussion
ture of the variable (Ballinger 2004). Parameters estimated
with GEE were tested against each other with the Wald’s Shape and color are important features that aid bee for-
Chi-square statistic. This approach allows for the analysis of agers to return to flowers (Avargues-Weber et al 2011).
longitudinal, autocorrelated data, such as choice behavior However, shape and color are not necessarily equally uti-
over time, in which the forager’s actual decision is influ- lized, depending upon the sensory capabilities of the spe-
enced by experience, i.e., by previous choices. It is highly cies (Campan & Lehrer 2002, Raine & Chittka 2005). In the
174 Sánchez & Vandame

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and proportion of choices of Scaptotrigona mexicana foragers to the target plate in the color experiments.

Number of repetitions is shown in brackets.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and proportion of choices of Scaptotrigona mexicana foragers to the target plate in the shape experiments.

Number of repetitions is shown in brackets.


Color and Shape Discrimination in a Stingless Bee 175

Fig 1 Proportion (±95% SE) of


focal bees of Scaptotrigona
mexicana choosing the target
plate over eight visits to the
setup in the test phase.

present study, we found that S. mexicana foragers consis- foragers in some degree. In bumblebees the detection of
tently used the training stimulus, shape or color, in their edges is also important to distinguish the flowers from the
orientation to the target plate. Their visual capabilities background (Lunau et al 2006). Shape discrimination is also
allowed them to distinguish between known, rewarding observed in honeybees; however, they seem be more
plates from novel, distracting alternatives. However, side discriminative than S. mexicana, since honeybees can differ-
by side, color stimuli were superior to shape stimuli in entiate among circle, triangle, square, and diamond-shaped
helping foragers to reach the rewarding plate, as revealed sources with an accuracy higher than 65% (Campan & Lehrer
by the higher proportion of choices to the target plate in 2002). Meanwhile, S. mexicana had a variable precision, as
the color experiments. Thus, shape is not as good as color low as 38% (scalene and circle-shaped sources) and as high as
at orienting foragers of S. mexicana to food sources. 75% (circle and isosceles-shaped sources). Overall, honey-
In a previous study that evaluated color choice perfor- bees (Gould & Gould 1988, Backhaus 1993, de Ibarra et al
mance in S. mexicana, the ability of foragers to discrimi- 2002) seem more similar to stingless bees than to bum-
nate between rewarding and unrewarding options, using blebees (Ney-Nifle et al 2001, Dyer & Chittka 2004a)
color cues, was evident (Villa & Weiss 1990). However, the regarding color and shape discrimination. Nonetheless,
effect of social facilitation was not considered, despite it is there exists the possibility that experimental differences
known that it largely influences the decision-making of evoke dissimilar behaviors and ultimately different results
approaching foragers (Slaa & Hughes 2009). Our experi- (Grüter et al 2011). Unfortunately, the amount of research
ments dealt with that and other experimental weaknesses, in stingless bee vision is insufficient to make robust com-
like statistical analysis, but interestingly we reached similar parisons with honeybees and bumblebees to account for
conclusions. In other stingless bee species, 77% of succes- such hypothesis.
sive visits were oriented to the previously visited flower
type, using color or odor as the differential cue; but when Choice behavior over time
using shape as the distinctive cue, foragers chose randomly
(Slaa et al 1998a). In our experiments S. mexicana behaved The first visit to the setup with the novel, distracting plate
similarly to those species in respect to color, but S. mex- initiates the discrimination process in the forager, and thus
icana choice behavior in the shape experiments departed potentially evokes the lowest discrimination performance.
significantly from random. In fact, shapes like stripes, dark We found that, on average, the proportion (±95% SE) of
centers, and peripheral dots, which are part of the display correct choices was 70% (±8) in the first visit in the color
of floral guides and stingless bees nest entrances, seem to experiments. In honeybees the discrimination of color
evoke a spontaneous preference in individuals of stingless ranges 60–100% in the first visit, depending upon the color
bees (Biesmeijer et al 2005). So it seems that shape-based distance between the training color and the distracting
signals are indeed detected and processed by stingless bee color (Dyer & Neumeyer 2005). Though we did not
176 Sánchez & Vandame

measure the color distance, we found similar results to that Campan R, Lehrer M (2002) Discrimination of closed shapes by two
of the honeybee (Table 1). When evaluating choice behav- species of bee, Apis mellifera and Megachile rotundata. J Exp Biol
205:559–572
ior over time, beyond the first visit, honeybees performed Chittka L, Menzel R (1992) The evolutionary adaptation of flower
quite similarly to S. mexicana, but honeybees learned more colours and the insect pollinators’ colour vision. J Comp Physiol A
slowly than foragers of S. mexicana, requiring over ten Sens Neural Behav Physiol 171:171–181
visits to achieve a similar discrimination performance Chittka L (1999) Learning and adaptation. Q Rev Biol 74:326–327
Chittka L, Gumbert A, Kunze J (1997) Foraging dynamics of bumble
(Giurfa 2004). We observed that the proportion of correct bees: correlates of movements within and between plant species.
choices reached a plateau in the fourth visit in the color Behav Ecol 8:239–249
experiments. Bumblebees will require more visits to Chittka L, Wells H (2004) Color vision in bees: mechanisms, ecology
achieve a similar color discrimination performance of that and evolution, p. 165–191. In: Prete FR (ed) Complex worlds from
simpler nervous systems. MIT Press, Boston, p 456
of S. mexicana or of honeybees (Dyer et al 2008, Heinrich Collett TS (1992) Landmark learning and guidance in insects. Philos
et al 1977). Our results demonstrate that S. mexicana Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 337:295–303
rapidly reaches a high color discrimination performance, de Ibarra NH, Giurfa M, Vorobyev M (2002) Discrimination of
faster than honeybees and outperforming bumblebees. On coloured patterns by honeybees through chromatic and achro-
matic cues. J Comp Physiol A Sens Neural Behav Physiol 188:503–
the other hand, the proportion of choices directed to the 512
target plate in the first visit in the shape experiments was Dyer AG, Chittka L (2004a) Biological significance of distinguishing
56% (±8), and improved very slowly over time. Color is between similar colours in spectrally variable illumination: bumble-
thus decisively a more important cue than shape in S. bees (Bombus terrestris) as a case study. J Comp Physiol A Sens
Neural Behav Physiol 190:105–114
mexicana, which is also the case in honeybees and Dyer AG, Chittka L (2004b) Fine colour discrimination requires differ-
bumblebees. ential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften 91:224–
This is the first study that evaluates how the choice 227
performance of a stingless bee, based on shape and color Dyer AG, Neumeyer C (2005) Simultaneous and successive colour
discrimination in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J Comp Physiol A
discrimination, changes over time. Our results can help to Sens Neural Behav Physiol 191:547–557
understand the ecological factors that have modulated the Dyer AG, Spaethe J, Prack S (2008) Comparative psychophysics of
vision of S. mexicana, but these conclusions would be of bumblebee and honeybee colour discrimination and object detec-
limited value. Thus, more stingless bee species should be tion. J Comp Physiol A Sens Neural Behav Physiol 194:617–627
Gack C (1981) The effect of imitation stamens in flowers in pollination
studied for a thorough understanding of the evolution of experiments with Bombus terrestris. Zool Jahrb Abt Syst Okol Geogr
bee vision and of how modern eye structures affect behav- Tiere 108:229–246
ior and ultimately contribute in niche partitioning of this Giurfa M (2004) Conditioning procedure and color discrimination in
important taxon of tropical pollinators. the honeybee Apis mellifera. Naturwissenschaften 91:228–231
Giurfa M, Hammer M, Stach S, Stollhoff N, Muller-Deisig N, Mizyrycki
C (1999) Pattern learning by honeybees: a condition procedure and
recognition strategy. Anim Behav 57:315–324
Acknowledgments We appreciate the valuable assistance of Manuel Gould JL (1986) The biology of learning. Ann Rev Psychol 37:163–192
de Jesús Hernández and Omar Arguello Nájera during the experiments. Gould JL, Gould CG (1988) The honey bee. Scientific American Library,
We would like to thank James Nieh and two anonymous reviewers for New York
their suggestions on this manuscript. This study was possible thanks to Grüter C, Moore H, Firmin N, Helantera H, Ratnieks FL (2011) Flower
the support of FORDECYT-CONACYT agreement no. 116306, and to the constancy in honey bee workers (Apis mellifera) depends on eco-
Mexican-European FONCICYT 94293 grant “MUTUAL—Mutualisms with logically realistic rewards. J Exp Biol 214:1397–1402
bees in tropical landscapes: risks and rescue for biodiversity and crop Gumbert A (2000) Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus terrestris):
production”. innate preferences and generalization after learning. Behav Ecol
Sociobiol 48:36–43
Guzmán-Díaz MÁ, Rincón-Rabanales M, Vandame R (2004) Manejo y
conservación de abejas nativas sin aguijón (Apidae: Meliponini). El
References Colegio de la Frontera Sur, Tapachula, p 40
Heinrich B, Mudge PR, Deringis PG (1977) Laboratory analysis of
flower constancy in foraging bumblebees: Bombus ternarius and
Avargues-Weber A, Deisig N, Giurfa M (2011) Visual cognition in social B. terricola. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:247–265
insects. Annu Rev Entomol 56:423–443 Hertel H, Ventura DF (1985) Spectral sensitivity of photoreceptors in
Backhaus W (1993) Color vision and color choice behavior of the the compound eye of stingless tropical bees. J Insect Physiol
honey bee. Apidologie 24:309–331 31:931–935
Ballinger GA (2004) Using generalized estimating equations for longi- Horridge A (2003) Visual discrimination by the honeybee (Apis melli-
tudinal data analysis. ORM 7:127–150 fera): the position of the common centre as the cue. Physiol
Beekman M, Oldroyd BP, Myerscough MR (2003) Sticking to their Entomol 28:132–143
choice—honey bee subfamilies abandon declining food sources at a Horridge A (2007) The preferences of the honeybee (Apis mellifera)
slow but uniform rate. Ecol Entomol 28:233–236 for different visual cues during the learning process. J Insect Physiol
Biesmeijer J, Giurfa M, Koedam D, Potts S, Joel D, Dafni A (2005) 53:877–889
Convergent evolution: floral guides, stingless bee nest entrances, Lamb JM, Wells H (1995) Honey bee (Apis mellifera) use of flower
and insectivorous pitchers. Naturwissenschaften 92:444–450 form in making foraging choices. J Kans Entomol Soc 68:388–398
Color and Shape Discrimination in a Stingless Bee 177

Lehrer M, Horridge GA, Zhang SW, Gadagkar R (1995) Shape vision in Sánchez D, Kraus F, Hernández M, Vandame R (2007) Experience
bees: innate preference for flower-like patterns. Philos Trans R Soc but not distance influences the recruitment precision in the
Lond B Biol Sci 347:123–137 stingless bee Scaptotrigona mexicana. Naturwissenschaften
Lunau K, Fieselmann G, Heuschen B, van de Loo A (2006) Visual 94:567–573
targeting of components of floral colour patterns in flower-naive Sánchez D, Nieh JC, Vandame R (2008) Experience-based interpreta-
bumblebees (Bombus terrestris; Apidae). Naturwissenschaften tion of visual and chemical information at food sources in Scapto-
93:325–328 trigona mexicana (Apidae, Meliponini). Anim Behav 76:407–414
Menzel R, Giurfa M (2006) Dimensions of cognition in an insect, the Slaa EJ, Cevaal A, Sommeijer MJ (1998a) Floral constancy in Trigona
honeybee. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 5:24–40 stingless bees foraging on artificial flower patches: a comparative
Menzel R, Ventura DF, Werner A, Joaquim LCM, Backhaus W (1989) study. J Apic Res 37:191–198
Spectral sensitivity of single photoreceptors and color vision in the Slaa EJ, Ruiz B, Salas R, Zeiss M, Sommeijer MJ (1998b) Foraging strat-
stingless bee, Melipona quadrifasciata. J Comp Physiol A Neuro- egies in stingless bees: flower constancy versus optimal foraging?
ethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 166:151–164 Sommeijer MJ, Francke PJ (eds) Proceedings of the Section Experi-
Ney-Nifle M, Keasar T, Shmida A (2001) Location and color learning in mental and Applied Entomology of the Netherlands Entomological
bumblebees in a two-phase conditioning experiment. J Insect Society (N.E.V.). Netherlands Entomological Society, Plantage Mid-
Behav 14:697–711 denlaan 64, 1018 DH Amsterdam 414, Netherlands, pp 185–190
Peitsch D, Fietz A, Hertel H, de Souza J, Ventura DF, Menzel R (1992) Slaa EJ, Hughes WO (2009) Local enhancement, local inhibition,
The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their eavesdropping, and the parasitism of social insect communication.
receptor-based colour vision. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens In: Jarau S, Hrncir M (eds) Food exploitation by social insects:
Neural Behav Physiol 170:23–40 ecological, behavioral, and theoretical approaches. CRC, New York,
Pessotti I, Gomes M (1981) Aprendizagem em abelhas. III. Discrim- pp 147–164
inação com três tipos de estímulos visuais. Rev Bras Biol 41:667– Villa JD, Weiss MR (1990) Observations on the use of visual and
672 olfactory cues by Trigona spp. foragers. Apidologie 21:541–546
Raine NE, Chittka L (2005) Comparison of flower constancy and von Helversen O (1972) Zur spektralen Unterschiedsempfindlichkeit
foraging performance in three bumblebee species (Hymenoptera: der Honigbiene. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav
Apidae: Bombus). Entomol Gen 28:81–89 Physiol 80:439–472

You might also like