Schizotipy Review and Relationship With Schizophrenia

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia:


A systematic review
M.T. Nelson a,b,c,∗ , M.L. Seal b,d , C. Pantelis c , L.J. Phillips a
a
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
b
Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Royal Children’s Hospital, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
c
Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
d
Department of Paediatrics, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The personality dimension of schizotypy is well established, and schizotypal traits can be taken to rep-
Received 20 June 2012 resent a proneness toward developing psychosis. Yet, there are competing theories about the latent
Received in revised form structure of schizotypy. More specifically, there is controversy over the extent to which this propensity
26 December 2012
toward psychosis is present only in a small proportion of the population, or whether it is spread dimen-
Accepted 3 January 2013
sionally throughout the general community. On the basis of accumulating research findings the present
article argues for a fully dimensional model of schizotypy. It describes recent neurobiological, neuropsy-
Keywords:
chological, social and environmental evidence supporting the idea that schizotypy in healthy populations,
Schizotypy
Psychosis
and disorders on the schizophrenia spectrum are fundamentally linked. Directions for further research
Schizophrenia are also considered.
Schizoaffective disorder © 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Schizophreniform disorder
Schizotypal personality traits
Fully dimensional model

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 317
2. The quasi-dimensional approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 318
3. The fully dimensional approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 319
4. Genetic research: family, twin and adoption studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 320
5. Genetic research: molecular genetics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 320
6. Neuropsychological research: cognitive deficits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 321
7. Environmental and social research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 321
8. Biological research: neuroanatomical abnormalities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 322
9. Methodological considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 323
10. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 324
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 324

1. Introduction
and Broussard, 2009). These symptoms are usually subsumed
Schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder characterised by expe- under three main categories. They are; positive symptoms
riences such as hallucinations, delusions, personality changes, including hallucinations and delusions, disorganised symptoms
thought disorder, bizarre behaviour, impaired social interac- including thought disorder and bizarre behaviour; and negative
tion, and difficulties in carrying out daily activities (Compton symptoms including alogia, apathy, and amotivation. Although
these psychotic symptoms are most commonly associated with
schizophrenia, they are also characteristic of other psychotic disor-
∗ Corresponding author at: The Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The ders like schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder, and
University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia. Tel.: +61 421812989. delusional disorder. Symptoms of psychosis can also be seen in neu-
E-mail address: nelsonm@unimelb.edu.au (M.T. Nelson). rological disorders such as dementia, and in psychiatric disorders

0149-7634/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.004
318 M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327

that are not generally considered under the rubric of psychotic However, these models are often taken at face value, and evi-
disorders, for example severe depression. dence for them has not recently been critically scrutinised. The
Categorical diagnostic categories for mental health difficulties present review sought to resolve this by examining recent empirical
like schizophrenia are described in current nosological systems, taxometric, neuropsychological, environmental and biological evi-
such as the Diagnostic System of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; APA, dence pertaining to a potential dimensional relationship between
2000). They are useful in that they help to ensure disorders are schizophrenia and schizotypy in otherwise psychologically healthy
easily identifiable; they serve as a basis for treatment decision- populations. Some methodological issues prevalent in the area will
making; and they aid communication between clinicians (Livesley also be considered, as well as directions for future research.
and Jackson, 1992; Kraemer et al., 2004). For the sake of brevity, the present review includes findings
However, there are also a number of criticisms of the assump- pertaining to schizophrenia and schizotypal personality traits.
tion that psychotic disorders are truly categorical phenomena. For However reflecting the aforementioned difficulties, controversies
example, there is considerable hetereogenity within diagnostic and complexities inherent in conducting research into categori-
categories (Wing and Agrawal, 2003; Beck et al., 2009), and homo- cal psychiatric constructs, we do acknowledge that the findings
geneity between them. Furthermore, diagnoses do not necessarily herein can be also be common to other clinical phenomena; such
remain stable across individuals’ lifetimes, and it is often not until as schizoaffective disorder (e.g. Cheniaux et al., 2008), first-episode
a person has experienced multiple episodes and chronic disability psychosis (e.g. Mesholm-Gately et al., 2009), bipolar disorder
that a psychotic diagnosis becomes stable and clear-cut (McGorry and mania (e.g. Berrettini, 2000; Bramon and Sham, 2001); and
et al., 2009). Also, despite numerous studies into the physiologi- other personality constructs such as psychoticism (e.g. Mason and
cal correlates of schizophrenia and related disorders, no biological Claridge, 2006). It is for this reason, we prefer to use the terms
markers, or endophenotypes have yet been discovered (Beck et al., ‘schizotypy’ and ‘schizophrenia’ in a relatively broad sense.
2009). There is no test (biological or otherwise) that will unequiv-
ocally distinguish someone with psychosis, from someone who is
psychologically healthy (Wing and Agrawal, 2003; Wong and van 2. The quasi-dimensional approach
Tol, 2003; Beck et al., 2009). This is reflected in Heinrichs’ (2005)
review of biological studies, which describes a substantial over- The quasi-dimensional approach to schizotypy is based on a
lap between samples of people with schizophrenia, and samples of disease model of mental illness. It posits that schizotypy is a per-
people who are psychologically healthy. sonality organisation specific to a small group of individuals within
This overlap between clinical and non-clinical samples indicates the population (approximately 10%), who are labelled ‘schizotypes’
that categorical diagnoses such as that of schizophrenia, decided (Rado, 1953; Meehl, 1990; Lenzenweger, 1994; Beauchaine et al.,
upon by expert committee consensus rather than by nature, may 2008). They are compared to a larger group (approximately 90%)
obscure the true psychosis phenotype. This could reflect a mis- who are not at risk. As reviewed by Lenzenweger (2006), the model
representation of latent constructs, and may lead to erroneous can be attributed to Meehl’s (1962) theory; which described a
diagnoses, inappropriate treatment, and conflicting research find- specific genetic vulnerability towards developing psychosis. This
ings. Indeed, there is a growing consensus that dimensional views vulnerability was said to exist in the form of a genetic predispo-
of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders may be a more valid sition, manifesting as a neurointegrative defect called schizotaxia.
representation of the population distribution (van Os et al., 2009; According to Meehl (1962), schizotaxia is necessary but not suf-
Neuvo et al., 2012). ficient to cause schizophrenia. Rather than causing schizophrenia
For similar reasons, traditional (categorical) understandings of directly, it interacts with environmental influences throughout a
schizotypy have also met with theoretical criticism. Schizotypy person’s lifetime to determine the degree of decompensation they
describes a cluster of personality traits that include odd or bizarre experience (Lenzenweger, 2006). From this perspective, genetic
behaviour, strange speech, magical thinking, unusual perceptual vulnerability towards developing psychotic symptoms is consid-
experiences, and social anhedonia. There is some disagreement ered to be ‘taxonic’ (Korfine and Lenzenweger, 1995; Waller et al.,
regarding the underlying factor structure of schizotypy (Stefanis 2006). The approach is quasi-dimensional only because it refers to
et al., 2004; Mason and Claridge, 2006; Fonseca-Pedrero et al., levels of expression of a proposed disease process. Otherwise it is
2011). However, the prevailing understanding is that it is com- a discontinuous, categorical theory wherein any one individual is
prised of three identifiable factors, which broadly correspond to considered to either possess a genetic vulnerability, or they do not.
the positive, negative and disorganised dimensions of schizophre- Support for the quasi-dimensional approach can be found in
nia (Wuthrich and Bates, 2006; Fonseca-Pedrero et al., 2011). The studies that employ taxometric analyses (Waller and Meehl, 1998;
first factor is the ‘cognitive-perceptual factor’, which includes magi- Rawlings et al., 2008b). Taxometric analyses are a group of sta-
cal thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, ideas of reference and tistical procedures that are used to determine the underlying
paranoia (Raine, 1991, 2006). The second is the ‘interpersonal fac- structure of latent constructs. They are able to determine whether
tor’, which includes constricted affect, social anxiety, lack of close a given construct is categorical (taxonic), or dimensional. In 2008, a
personal relationships, and suspiciousness (Raine, 1991, 2006). The review of 19 published taxometric studies pertaining to schizotypy
final ‘disorganised factor’ includes odd behaviour and odd speech reported that fifteen supported a categorical model (Rawlings et al.,
(Raine, 1991, 2006). 2008b). Furthermore the taxonic base rate estimates in these stud-
Recent reviews summarising research into schizotypy have ies, ranging between 0.03 and 0.13, appeared to support Meehl’s
examined a range of topics associated with the construct. Examples (1990) postulation that 10% of the population are schizotypes
include unusual speech associated with schizotypy (Kiang, 2010); (Rawlings et al., 2008b).
the assessment of schizotypy (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., 2008); affec- Yet there are criticisms of the quasi-dimensional model, and
tive traits in schizophrenia and schizotypy (Horan et al., 2008); some of these focus particularly on the sampling methods used
schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia (Siever and when conducting taxometric analyses (Rawlings et al., 2008b). For
Davis, 2004); as well as cannabis use, psychosis and schizotypy instance, taxometric studies have often used either clinical samples
(Compton et al., 2007). Much of this research is based on the- with insufficient power for the purposes of taxometrics, or they
oretical assumptions arising from two competing models of the have used large student samples, which may not be representa-
distribution of schizotypy in the general population–called the tive of the general population (Rawlings et al., 2008b). Additionally,
quasi-dimensional and fully dimensional approaches respectively. studies often investigate only one aspect of schizotypy, using only
M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327 319

one taxometric procedure (Rawlings et al., 2008b). Lastly and most


importantly, Rawlings et al. (2008b) have argued that evidence
from taxometric analyses which support both a categorical view-
point and a base rate in the range of 10%, may actually arise as an
artefact of positively skewed sample distributions (Rawlings et al.,
2008b). Their own taxometric study attempted to address these
criticisms by using more than one taxometric procedure, a large
diverse adult sample, multiple measures of schizotypy, and by tak-
ing skew into account. Its findings supported a dimensional rather
than a categorical latent structure (Rawlings et al., 2008b).
Another criticism of the quasi-dimensional approach has been
highlighted by research showing that the anomalous perceptual
experiences associated with schizotypy are much more prevalent
in the general population than the 10% estimate proposed by Meehl
(1990). For example, Verdoux et al. (1998) asked 790 individuals
Fig. 1. A diagrammatic representation of the proposed model of schizotypy
from primary care services to fill in a self-report measure com-
(Claridge and Beech, 1995).
prising items assessing experiences that resembled hallucinations
and delusions. Examples include: “Do you ever feel as if there
is a conspiracy against you?”, “Do you ever feel as if people are clinical and non-clinical psychosis populations (Linscott and van
looking oddly at you?”, and “Do you ever think that people can Os, 2010). Further evidence for the dimensional approach is based
communicate telepathically?”. For participants with no history of on analyses supporting the three factor structure of schizotypy
psychiatric disorder (n = 462), individual item endorsement varied (cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal and disorganised), which is
from between 5% and 70%, with 46.9% of people believing they analogous to the three factor structure of psychosis (positive, neg-
could communicate telepathically, 25.5% believing they had been ative and disorganised; Liddle, 1987; Rossi and Daneluzzo, 2002;
persecuted in some way, and 4.8% believing that they could hear Wuthrich and Bates, 2006). There is also evidence that individ-
voices conversing (Verdoux et al., 1998). Another study investi- uals with psychotic disorders tend to score highly on measures of
gated the prevalence of psychotic-like experiences in 2441 young schizotypy (Lenzenweger, 1994; Camisa et al., 2005).
people (aged 18–23) in Australia (Scott et al., 2008). Individual item However, it should be remembered that not all people with high
endorsement in this study ranged from between 5.5.% (‘do you ever levels of schizotypy are necessarily dysfunctional. For example, it
feel as if you are a robot or zombie without a will of your own?’) has been shown that individuals scoring highly on measures of
and 77% (‘do you ever feel as if some people are not what they seem schizotypy can (and do) function well in terms of subjective well-
to be?). Indeed, one estimate from a longitudinal study looking at being (Goulding, 2004). Here, subjective wellbeing can be defined
mental health in the Dutch adult general population indicated that as a self-rated sense that a person has control over their own life, can
the incidence of positive subclinical psychotic experiences in oth- balance and cope with both positive and negative life events, and
erwise healthy cohorts is up to 100 times greater than traditional can maintain stability (Goulding, 2004). Many people who score
estimates such as Meehl’s would suggest (Hanssen et al., 2005). highly on measures of schizotypy also exhibit adaptive strengths
This high prevalence of anomalous experiences in the general pop- such as creativity, as evidenced by increased involvement in cre-
ulation blurs the line between what may be considered ‘normal’ ative activities including painting and drawing, writing, music,
and ‘abnormal.’ It indicates that schizotypy may not be a discrete, gardening and web design (Batey and Furnham, 2008; Rawlings
bounded entity affecting only a small proportion of the population, and Locarnini, 2008; Nelson and Rawlings, 2010).
as would be consistent with a quasi-dimensional model. Therefore, similar to the quasi-dimensional approach, the fully
dimensional approach does not assume schizotypal traits are suffi-
cient in and of themselves, to indicate risk for psychopathology
3. The fully dimensional approach (Rawlings et al., 2008a). Rather, it is only when high levels of
schizotypy are combined with other aetiological risk factors that
A more recent model of the potential relationship between an individual may be considered at risk for schizophrenia and
schizotypal personality and schizophrenia is the fully dimensional other psychotic disorders. According to this perspective, unless
approach, which is outlined by Claridge and colleagues (Claridge high schizotypy is combined with other risk factors, it is considered
and Beech, 1995; Claridge and Davis, 2003; Rawlings et al., 2008a). neutral in regards to psychopathology (Rawlings et al., 2008a).
The fully dimensional approach posits that schizotypy represents The fully dimensional approach is also consistent with what is
‘natural central nervous system variations’, which in their extreme, termed the ‘continuum hypothesis’ of psychosis (Allardyce et al.,
manifest as vulnerability to mental illness (Rawlings et al., 2008a, 2007). This is the current predominant view, that psychosis is sim-
p. 1669). The main contention advocated by the fully dimensional ilar to chronic physical problems like diabetes and heart disease.
approach is that the latent structure of schizotypy is on a contin- It is considered to have multifactorial aetiology, wherein multi-
uum applying to all members of the population. It is considered to ple genes interact both with each other and with the environment
range from low schizotypy and psychological health, to extremely to determine outcome (Allardyce et al., 2007; The International
high schizotypy and potential dysfunction in the form of psychosis Schizophrenia Consortium, 2009). These different combinations of
(Fig. 1). genes and environmental risk factors result in a range of different
As the fully dimensional approach describes schizotypy as con- phenotypic expressions lying on a continuum from normal through
tinuous throughout the general population, it can account for the to clinical psychosis.
high prevalence of anomalous experiences reported by researchers Consequently by adopting the fully dimensional theory,
such as van Os and his colleagues (Verdoux et al., 1998; Johns and research into schizotypy becomes important because it directly
van Os, 2001; van Os et al., 2009). In this way, the fully dimensional aids investigation into the aetiology of schizophrenia and other
approach appears superior to the quasi-dimensional approach. It psychotic disorders. Furthermore, it does so whilst avoiding con-
is also consistent with the majority of current theories pertain- founds often associated with psychosis research, such as illness
ing to schizophrenia, which tend to describe continuity between chronicity, medication, and hospitalization. From this perspective,
320 M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327

it is important to assess not only similarities between various levels et al., 2000). Also in a more recent study, 263 relatives of people
of schizotypy and schizophrenia, but also dissimilarities (Fanous with psychosis completed three measures of schizotypy – including
and Kendler, 2004). For example, it has been proposed that pos- two self-report measures and a semi-structured interview (Mata
sible biological endophenotypes common to the two constructs et al., 2003). In this investigation, it was the positive symptoms
may represent underlying genetic factors, while findings specific of psychosis, as reported by the patients, which were related to
to schizophrenia may represent environmental factors (Cannon schizotypy in their relatives. A cluster of symptoms reported by
et al., 2002; Fanous and Kendler, 2004). Further empirical evidence the patients (delusions, hallucinations and thought disturbance)
that psychotic symptoms and schizotypal personality features in was positively correlated with outcomes from all three measures of
psychologically healthy people lie on a common continuum can schizotypy in relatives, including both positive and negative traits.
be gleaned from genetic, neuropsychological, environmental, and Two other family studies have reported mixed findings as
biological research. to heritability between schizotypy and schizophrenia. Kendler
et al. (1996) found no differences in levels of schizotypy between
relatives of patients with psychotic illnesses, and relatives of con-
4. Genetic research: family, twin and adoption studies trols. Furthermore, Appels et al. (2004) reported that parents
of schizophrenia patients actually had lower scores on the SPQ
A first line of convincing evidence that schizotypy lies on a Cognitive-Perceptual Factor than control participants. However,
continuum with schizophrenia comes from genetic research. Both the authors of both of these studies account for their somewhat
Kraeplin (1919/1971) and Bleuler (1911/1950) first observed that anomalous findings by highlighting the problems of conducting
relatives of individuals with schizophrenia appeared notably more research into schizotypy using self-report measures (Kendler et al.,
odd and eccentric than people in the general population. Since then, 1996; Appels et al., 2004). Appels et al. (2004) also reasoned that
much behavioural genetic research throughout the 20th and early including both parents (mothers and fathers) of schizophrenia
21st centuries has indicated that schizotypal traits and psychosis patients in their analysis may have resulted in a loss of power. It
show some level of genetic inheritance (Kety et al., 1975; Baron is more likely that only one parent passes on a genetic risk of psy-
et al., 1983; Kendler et al., 1996; Mata et al., 2003). chosis, not necessarily both parents (Appels et al., 2004). Indeed,
For example, it has been clearly established from family, twin, Appels et al. (2004) then conducted an alternative investigation,
and adoption studies that schizophrenia is heritable (McCue et al., wherein parents of an individual with schizophrenia who reported
1983; Cannon et al., 1998; McClellan et al., 2007). Similarly, there a family history of schizophrenia spectrum disorder in earlier gen-
is growing evidence that heightened schizotypy can also be expe- erations (i.e. their own parents or earlier) reported higher levels of
rienced by multiple generations of the one family. For instance, both positive and negative schizotypal traits than parents with no
Hanssen et al. (2006) gave three measures of schizotypy to 272 family history.
individuals from 82 families. All three measures showed family- Further to the above family-based research, there have been
specific variation for both positive and negative traits. However, some investigations that have used a twin design to assess a pos-
only one measure showed family-specific variation for the nega- sible genetic relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia.
tive trait once variation due to the positive trait was controlled for, Self-reported experiences of perceptual aberration and social anhe-
indicating that the positive dimension of schizotypy (anomalous donia in particular were found to be at least partly inherited in
experiences) specifically varied with family membership. Interest- a study of 98 monozygotic and 59 same sex dizygotic twin pairs
ingly, these results were obtained in families that were not selected (MacDonald et al., 2001). Another study with 105 monzygotic and
on the basis of a pre-existing diagnosis of any psychiatric disorder, 90 dizygotic twins described common family specific variation
providing evidence for dimensional variation within the general between psychosis and schizotypy as measured by the Oxford-
population as well as heritability (Hanssen et al., 2006). Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE; Mason
Further evidence of the dimensional and heritable nature of et al., 1995; Jang et al., 2005).
schizotypy within psychologically healthy populations comes from
Kendler et al. (1991) who administered four measures of schizotypy
(including self-report and interview) to 29 monozygotic and dizy- 5. Genetic research: molecular genetics
gotic twin pairs. Distributions of schizotypy in this sample were
normal, indicating dimensionality rather than taxonicity, which In summary, considerable evidence from family, adoption, and
would have been represented by a bimodal distribution (Kendler twin studies indicates that psychotic disorders and schizotypy in
et al., 1991). Comparison of correlations between monozygotic and healthy populations share common genetic underpinnings. Yet
dizygotic pairs in this sample also indicated that both positive although these family, twin and adoption studies are informative
and negative schizotypal traits showed family-specific variation in terms of describing heritability, they cannot identify specific
(Kendler et al., 1991). chromosomal regions giving rise to any one phenotype (Fanous
It is acknowledged, of course, that evidence showing schizotypy et al., 2007). Around 25 years ago, Gottesman, McGuffin and Farmer
runs in families, and separate evidence showing that schzophrenia (1987) remarked that the results of behavioural genetic studies
runs in families does not mean that the two phenomena are nec- gave only clues to the genetic basis of schizophrenia, and that
essarily associated. Yet, there is also research indicating that both molecular genetics would provide clear answers. However despite
schizotypy and schizophrenia occur in the same families, which years of molecular genetic research, no single gene, or even a clear
suggests they may be heritable together (Kety et al., 1975; Baron number of possible genes have been identified for schizophrenia,
et al., 1983; Kendler et al., 1989, 1996; Mata et al., 2003). nor for psychosis more broadly (Allardyce et al., 2007; Tandon et al.,
One example is a study by Kremen et al. (1998) wherein 44 2008), despite large samples in recent studies (Ripke et al., 2011).
controls and 40 biological relatives of people with schizophrenia Indeed, there are no genes proposed to be related to schizophre-
completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ; Raine, nia that are not controversial or subject to conflicting findings.
1991). Relatives had higher scores on the Cognitive-Perceptual For instance, in a recent review and meta-analysis of the genetic
(positive) Factor than controls. Similar results using the same mea- research into schizophrenia, it was estimated that there have
sure were reported in a smaller study of 13 participants with a been in excess of 1000 genetic association studies exhibiting
family history of psychosis, 38 participants with a family history largely inconsistent results (Allen et al., 2008). This review found
of substance use disorder, and 51 control participants (Yaralian that across 118 different meta-analyses, there were nominally
M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327 321

significant effects in 24 variants of 16 different genes – namely, functioning in both schizotypy and schizophrenia together, on the
APOE, COMT, DAO, DRD1, DRD2, DRD4, DTNBP1, GABRB2, GRIN2B, basis of a fully dimensional model (Cochrane et al., 2012).
HP, IL1B, MTHFR, PLXNA2, SLC6A4, TP53, and TPH1 (Allen et al., Cochrane et al. (2012) conducted two separate studies. In the
2008). first, it was reported that in 99 psychologically healthy people,
To date there has been limited molecular genetic research into the Interpersonal (negative) Factor of the SPQ was related to
a potential relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia reduced verbal fluency, and the Disorganised Factor was related
specifically. In a linkage study, Fanous et al. (2007) demonstrated to reduced negative priming. In the second study, corresponding
that a number of genes thought to be related to schizophre- symptom measures from the Scales for the Assessment of Posi-
nia in patients, were also associated with levels of schizotypy in tive and Negative Symptoms (SAPS and SANS; Andreasen, 1983,
non-psychotic relatives. Two other studies have indicated a sig- 1984) showed similar relationships with verbal fluency and nega-
nificant level of association between schizotypy and the COMT tive priming in 20 participants with a diagnosis of schizophrenia.
gene (Avramopoulos et al., 2002), and between schizotypy and the Again, similar to outcomes of research focused on psychosis, rela-
DTMBP1, NRG1 and DAAO genes (Stefanis et al., 2007). However, tive cognitive deficits associated with schizotypy appear to relate
reflecting the conflictual findings in regards to molecular genetic to negative (interpersonal) and disorganised traits rather than
research and psychosis, another investigation found no clear rela- positive (cognitive-perceptual) traits (Chen et al., 1997; Park and
tionship between schizotypy and the COMT gene (Ma et al., 2007). McTigue, 1997; Moritz et al., 1999).
As reviewed by Fanous and Kendler (2004), the following con- One point of departure in neurocognitive findings between
clusions can be drawn from the genetic research on schizotypy schizotypy and schizophrenia is that effect sizes appear to be larger
and schizophrenia. First, behavioural genetic research has shown in schizophrenia research compared to schizotypy research. Specif-
that both schizophrenia and schizotypy are heritable, and that ically, significant reductions in cognitive variables in people with
schizotypy and psychotic disorders aggregate in families together. schizophrenia are often reported alongside medium to large effect
Furthermore, heritability can be broken down into specific traits, sizes (e.g. Reichenberg and Harvey, 2007; Simonsen et al., 2011);
whereby negative psychotic symptoms tend to predict negative whilst effect sizes in studies of schizotypy are often small (e.g. Chen
schizotypal traits in relatives, and in particular, positive psychotic et al., 1997; Noguchi et al., 2008). This may indicate that cogni-
symptoms tend to predict positive schizotypal traits (Fanous et al., tive decline is more prominent for people with schizophrenia, and
2001; Fanous and Kendler, 2004). Yet despite the optimism sur- indeed may be a significant defining feature of clinical psychosis
rounding early molecular genetic research into the genetic causes of (Bora et al., 2010).
both schizotypy and schizophrenia (Gottesman et al., 1987; Tandon
et al., 2008), findings in this area are still unclear. Further research
is needed before it can be confidently established that there are
shared genetic abnormalities between schizotypy and schizophre- 7. Environmental and social research
nia, and even more before we can break these down into specific
traits. This is reflective of the state of genetic research into psychi- Further support for a continuum model of schizotypy and
atric disorders more broadly (Meyer-Lindenberg and Weinberger, schizophrenia can be gleaned from research examining environ-
2006; Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Coordinating Committee et al., mental and social correlates of the two constructs. Identified
2009), and particularly the difficulties of finding genetic common- environmental and social risk factors for schizophrenia are wide-
alities in inherently heterogeneous populations. ranging. They include antenatal and birth complications such as
maternal infections (Stober et al., 1997; Brown, 2006; Xiao et al.,
2009), nutritional deficiency (St Clair et al., 2005; Markham and
6. Neuropsychological research: cognitive deficits Koenig, 2011), maternal stress during pregnancy (Markham and
Koenig, 2011), foetal hypoxia (Cannon et al., 2000) and older
In another relevant vein of research, it has been consistently parental age at conception (Malaspina et al., 2001; Petersen et al.,
shown that schizophrenia is associated with cognitive deficits 2011). They also include childhood trauma (Lardinois et al., 2011),
(Tandon et al., 2009). These include generalised cognitive dys- migration and minority group membership (Morgan et al., 2010),
function (Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998; Fioravanti et al., 2005; urbanicity (Kelly et al., 2010), cannabis use (Richardson, 2010),
Reichenberg and Harvey, 2007; Simonsen et al., 2011), as well as parental separation (including death; Morgan et al., 2007), adverse
deficits in verbal IQ (Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998; Reichenberg child rearing (Willinger et al., 2002), and childhood infection
and Harvey, 2007), working memory (Wood et al., 2003; Haenschel (Rantakallio et al., 1997).
and Linden, 2011; Simonsen et al., 2011), episodic memory Similar environmental and social risk factors have been impli-
(Reichenberg and Harvey, 2007), executive functioning (Heinrichs cated in the development of anomalous experiences associated
and Zakzanis, 1998; Fioravanti et al., 2005; Reichenberg and with schizotypy. Zammit et al. (2009) report in their longitudinal
Harvey, 2007), verbal fluency (Simonsen et al., 2011), processing study of 6356 children, that ‘PLIKS’ (‘psychotic-like experiences’)
speed (Simonsen et al., 2011), and attention (Heinrichs and were positively associated with pregnancy and birth complica-
Zakzanis, 1998; Fioravanti et al., 2005), amongst others (Heinrichs tions. These included maternal infection, maternal diabetes, and
and Zakzanis, 1998; Fioravanti et al., 2005; Reichenberg, 2010). need for resuscitation following birth (Zammit et al., 2009). Addi-
These neurocognitive deficits are more often correlated with neg- tionally, positive anomalous experiences have been associated
ative and disorganised symptoms than with positive symptoms with childhood trauma (Steel et al., 2009; Lovatt et al., 2010),
(Basso et al., 1998; Pantelis et al., 2001; Galderisi et al., 2009; urbanicity (Scott et al., 2009), and ethnicity and minority group
Lindsberg et al., 2009). membership (Sharpley and Peters, 1999; Johns et al., 2002; Morgan
Heightened schizotypy has also been associated with subtle et al., 2009). Parental separation has been associated with symp-
cognitive deficits in psychologically healthy people (Moritz et al., toms of schizotypal personality disorder (SPD), which is said to
1999; Noguchi et al., 2008). Specifically, inverse associations have lie on the continuum between schizotypy in healthy popula-
been found between schizotypy and verbal IQ (Noguchi et al., tions and full blown psychotic disorders (see Fig. 1; Anglin et al.,
2008), attention (Chen et al., 1997), and working memory (Park 2008). However there does not appear to be any research to date
and McTigue, 1997; Schmidt-Hansen and Honey, 2009). Further- addressing parental separation and schizotypy in healthy popula-
more, at least one paper has explicitly evaluated neurocognitive tions.
322 M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327

With respect to cannabis use, a large study involving 1049 stu- imaging (fMRI), diffusion weighted imaging (DWI), magnetic res-
dents showed that high levels of cannabis use were associated with onance spectroscopy (MRS), single-photon computed emission
both positive and negative ‘non-clinical psychotic experiences’, as tomography (SPECT), and positron emission tomography (PET).
measured by the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences There are a multitude of reviews and meta-analyses summarising
(CAPE) questionnaire (Skinner et al., 2011). Furthermore, the ear- the field of neuroimaging and schizophrenia (Ward et al., 1996;
lier students began their cannabis use, the more likely they were Shenton et al., 2001; Pantelis et al., 2005; Gur et al., 2007; Keshavan
to report positive experiences (Stefanis et al., 2002; Skinner et al., et al., 2008; Fornito et al., 2009; Wood et al., 2011), therefore only
2011). This study along with numerous others, echoes psychosis main findings are reported here.
research and demonstrates a positive association between schizo- Briefly, structural MRI studies have shown reduced whole-brain
typy and cannabis use (Barkus and Lewis, 2008; Compton et al., volume and complimentary enlargement of lateral ventricles and
2009; Esterberg et al., 2009; Cohen et al., 2011). increases in cerebrospinal fluid (Gur et al., 2007; Keshavan et al.,
In addition, unlike the neurocognitive findings reported above, 2008). Affected cortical regions include but are not limited to,
it is worth noting that effect sizes for these environmental findings medial temporal lobe areas, most notably the hippocampus and
appear on the whole to be of similar magnitude for both schizo- amygdala (Velakoulis et al., 2006; Gur et al., 2007; Keshavan et al.,
typy (Morgan et al., 2010) and psychosis (Morgan et al., 2009). 2008). DWI studies report deficits in white matter connectivity
This may be because most studies investigating environmental across the whole brain, corpus callosum, cingulum bundle, and
variables in relation to schizotypy have used measurement tools superior longitudinal fasciculus, amongst others (Kubicki et al.,
that assess for positive traits specifically (such as the Psychotic- 2007; Kyriakopoulos et al., 2008; Seal et al., 2008; Zalesky et al.,
Like Experiences Structured Interview and Psychosis Screening 2011). MRS research seems to have focused on findings of reduc-
Questionnaire; Bebbington and Nayani, 1995; Horwood et al., tions in N-acetyl aspartate metabolites (Gur et al., 2007). PET
2008; Morgan et al., 2009; Zammit et al., 2009), rather than all and SPECT research has examined the role of over activation of
aspects of schizotypy (as is assessed via the SPQ and O-LIFE). This dopamine receptors (particularly D2 receptors) in schizophrenia
might have increased power, as effects did not get ‘washed out’ (Gur et al., 2007; Keshavan et al., 2008). Lastly, fMRI research has
when averaging across qualitatively different aspects of schizo- explored decreases in activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cor-
typy. tex, especially when participants are presented with challenging
cognitive tasks (Berman and Meyer-Lindenberg, 2004; Keshavan
et al., 2008). This tendency is reinforced by outcomes of PET
8. Biological research: neuroanatomical abnormalities and SPECT research focusing on the same area (Keshavan et al.,
2008).
Research has indicated that both schizophrenia and schizotypy Although there is much neuroimaging research into schizotypal
have been associated with a number of similar neuroanatomical personality disorder (Dickey et al., 2002; Hazlett et al., 2012), a very
abnormalities. Importantly, several of the key neuroanatomical limited number of neuroimaging studies have investigated neu-
findings relating to schizophrenia have also been replicated in roanatomical correlates of schizotypy in psychologically healthy
schizotypy research. For instance, neurological soft signs are sub- people. One MRI study reported that schizotypy was associated
tle deficits in areas such as motor coordination, laterality and with reduced brain volume in prefrontal areas (Raine et al., 1992).
sensory-perceptual performance (Obiols et al., 1999). Unlike hard In another investigation, Modinos et al. (2010) gave a self-report
neurological signs, they do not indicate any specific neurological measure of schizotypy to six hundred university students. Thirty-
disease (Obiols et al., 1999). Higher rates of neurological soft signs eight of the highest and lowest scorers subsequently underwent
have been identified in people with psychotic disorders compared MRI scans. Differences between groups included significantly larger
to controls (Leask et al., 2002), and they have also been shown volumes in the whole brain, medial posterior congulate cortex and
to correlate with schizotypy in non-clinical samples (Bollini et al., precuneus for the high schizotypy group (Modinos et al., 2010). This
2007; Kaczorowski et al., 2009; Chan et al., 2010). In two studies, was the reverse of what is normally seen in psychosis research,
this relationship with schizotypy appeared to be driven by associa- in which there are reductions in volume (Keshavan et al., 2008).
tions with interpersonal (negative) and disorganised factors rather However, the authors concluded that the alterations were consis-
than by associations with the cognitive-perceptual (positive) factor tent with a continuum model. They cite previous research findings
(Bollini et al., 2007; Kaczorowski et al., 2009). However, two stud- of increases in volume in SPD and first-episode psychosis sam-
ies have also found associations between neurological soft signs ples (Modinos et al., 2010), as opposed to reductions in volume in
and positive aspects of schizotypy (Barkus et al., 2006; Chan et al., chronic schizophrenia samples (Keshavan et al., 2008). Increases
2010). in grey matter in the middle, superior temporal, angular, and
Furthermore, people with schizophrenia also have higher levels orbitofrontal gyri have also been associated with PLEs (‘psychotic-
of the subtle variations in bone structure, cartilage and soft tis- like experiences’) in a young adolescent sample (Jacobson et al.,
sue known as minor physical anomalies (McGrath et al., 2002). 2010).
These minor physical anomalies have also been significantly In another adolescent sample (aged 12–20), Lagioia et al. (2010)
associated with levels of overall schizotypy, as well as cognitive- examined brain networks relating to schizotypy using fMRI. They
perceptual and interpersonal factors specifically (Bollini et al., report a discontinuity between their findings and those investi-
2007). gating psychosis. However, their study was limited by its small
In addition, there are wide reports of eye tracking dysfunctions sample size of only 39 across a relatively large developmental age
in relation to schizophrenia (Levy et al., 2010), such as impairments range. Then, in our recent DWI study we identified negative corre-
in smooth pursuit and antisaccade eye movements (Lipton et al., lations between measures of white matter structure and positive
1983). Like neurological soft signs and minor physical anomalies, schizotypal traits in seven white matter tracts within frontal and
findings of occulomotor deficits have also been replicated in high temporal areas (Nelson et al., 2011). This supports the findings
schizotypy groups (O’Driscoll et al., 1998). of Volpe et al. (2008), which showed that ‘psychotic personality
More recently, developments in neuroimaging technologies traits’ as measured by the Schizophrenia, Paranoia and Psycho-
have identified brain structural, metabolic and functional abnor- pathic Deviate subscales of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
malities in relation to schizophrenia. These technologies include Inventory (Greene, 2000), were associated with changes in white
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional magnetic resonance matter structure in the corpus callosum, right arcuate fasciculus,
M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327 323

and frontoparietal fibres (Volpe et al., 2008). Currently, there are Another reason that it is difficult to compare studies in this area,
no known investigations of the neurobiological basis of schizotypy is that although there has been much investigation into under-
using MRS, PET, or SPECT imaging modalities. lying structure of schizotypy itself (Stefanis et al., 2004; Mason
In summary, despite the relative lack of research into a relation- and Claridge, 2006; Wuthrich and Bates, 2006); further research
ship between schizotypy and schizophrenia using neuroimaging is needed to determine the concurrent, discriminant and con-
modalities, other biological findings such as those of oculomotor struct validity of schizotypy and psychosis-related phenomena
deficits, MPAs and neurological soft signs indicate there may be together. For instance, terms such as positive and negative schizo-
some biological similarity between people who score highly on typy, schizotypal personality, ‘PLIKS’ and ‘PLEs’ (’psychotic-like
measures of schizotypy and people with schizophrenia. experiences’; e.g. Zammit et al., 2009; Kelleher and Cannon, 2011),
subclinical psychotic experiences (e.g. van Os et al., 2009), anoma-
lous experiences (e.g. Brett et al., 2007), cognitive-perceptual,
9. Methodological considerations interpersonal and disorganised personality traits (e.g. Raine, 1991,
2006), SPD-proneness (Chan et al., 2010), and non-clinical dimen-
The above paucity in neuroimaging research is a case in point, sions of psychosis (Skinner et al., 2011) are used interchangeably.
in that further investigation into the relationship between schizo- It is not clear whether they are measuring the same experiences,
typy and schizophrenia is still needed to resolve mixed findings, or how they relate to one another. We have assumed that terms
particularly in relation to neurobiology and molecular genetics. like ‘PLEs’, ‘positive schizotypy,’ and ‘subclinical psychotic expe-
Large scale longitudinal and cross-sectional designs are required riences’ all refer to the same phenomena. That is, the anomalous
that apply the same measures to people in the general popu- experiences of positive schizotypy which are represented on the
lation, to people with SPD, people at high risk for psychosis, cognitive-perceptual factor of the SPQ. We have similarly assumed
first episode psychosis and to people with schizophrenia. Again that ‘negative schizotypy’, and the interpersonal factor of the SPQ
reflecting the complexity of the field, and reflecting the bur- are the same construct. Furthermore, we have assumed that the
geoning unitary psychosis view, which posits that schizophrenia terms ‘schizotypy’, schizotypal personality’, ‘SPD-proneness’, and
and bipolar disorder as two representations of a single con- ‘non-clinical dimensions of psychosis’ are all subsumed under the
struct, these investigations could be extended to other psychotic umbrella of schizotypy.
disorders and affective psychoses. Indeed, as was pointed out There is certainly evidence that ‘psychotic’ traits and ‘schizoty-
by one reviewer, there is some evidence (to be built upon) pal’ traits are convergent constructs (Bentall et al., 1989; Claridge
that the unitary psychosis view is also reflected in dimensional et al., 1996; Stefanis et al., 2002; Kwapil et al., 2008). For instance,
schizotypy research; for example via the four factor overlapping Claridge et al. (1996) assessed the convergence of schizotypal
scales of the O-LIFE (Mason et al., 1995; Mason and Claridge, traits and subclinical hallucinations and delusions in a community
2006). sample of 1095 participants. Factor analysis from this study sug-
Continuum research of this type is difficult currently, with many gested that subclinical hallucinations and delusions do map onto
measures designed only for one level of severity on a proposed the cognitive-perceptual factor of schizotypy. However, more up-
schizotypy and psychosis dimension. For example, there are meas- to-date investigation exploring and comparing the psychometric
ures designed to assess the positive symptoms of psychosis, but properties of a range of measures pertaining to schizotypy, anoma-
not the anomalous experiences associated with schizotypy, and lous experiences and psychotic symptoms would greatly assist in
vice versa. Problems of floor and ceiling effects could be avoided further development of the field.
by designing measurement tools that can account for variation If the above terms are found to be referring the same con-
across the entire continuum. One example of this is the Appraisals structs, in our own research and in future research we prefer to
of Anomalous Experiences Interview (AANEX; Brett et al., 2007). use the following terminology. The term ‘anomalous experiences’
Although lengthy, the AANEX is able to measure experiences ran- is preferred when referring to PLEs, PLIKs and so forth. The term
ging from the anomalous, psychotic-like experiences of schizotypy, ‘anomalous experiences’ distances these types of phenomena from
to the florid hallucinations and delusions of psychosis. psychotic symptoms, thus avoiding the risk of pathologizing expe-
Another important methodological issue is that thus far, schizo- riences that are considered to lie within the range of normal human
typy research has predominantly relied on self-report measures experience. ‘Positive schizotypy’ is preferred when referring to
such as the SPQ and O-LIFE. These questionnaires are short and the tendency to have anomalous experiences, ‘negative schizo-
easy to administer, however they can be subject to the various typy’ is preferred when referring to non-clinical experiences similar
confounds associated with self-report measures, such as social to negative symptoms of psychosis, and ‘disorganised schizotypy’
desirability bias (Moritz et al., 1999; Shean et al., 2007). Therefore is preferred when referring to non-clinical experiences similar to
there is a need for wider use of psychometrically sound measure- disorganised symptoms of psychosis. We prefer ‘schizotypy’ as
ments such as structured and semi-structured interviews. In this the umbrella term subsuming all of the above. Use of the term
way, the AANEX also meets another need in relation to future ‘schizotypy’ rather than terms such as ‘non-clinical dimensions of
research on schizotypy. psychosis’ and ‘SPD-proneness’ avoids the loss of its status as a per-
A related issue is the widespread use of statistical techniques sonality trait, and again avoids pathologizing normal experiences.
imposing artificial categorical boundaries on what are otherwise Finally, it could well be that all of these methodological issues
dimensional scales. We refer here to the use of median splits and mentioned above are contributing to small effect sizes and type II
similar techniques that dichotomize, or trichotomize samples that error in categorical research comparing groups. As has been seen,
have been administered self-report measures such as the SPQ. They some investigations into schizotypy and anomalous experiences
are used to extract, for example, ‘high schizotypy’ and ‘low schizo- often report much smaller effects sizes than those seen relation to
typy’ groups to be compared in subsequent statistical analyses. psychosis. While others – for example those environmental investi-
However, they result in a substantial loss of statistical power, and gations focusing specifically on anomalous experiences rather than
can therefore lead to mixed and contradictory findings. Interview schizotypy as a whole – report findings of similar magnitude to psy-
measures such as the AANEX, that allow wide variability across the chosis research. It is not yet possible to conclude whether or not
continuum will avoid floor and ceiling effects, increase power, and smaller effects sizes are reflective of low power and extraneous
allow investigators to compare ‘apples with apples’ when looking at variables, or whether they reflects a ‘true’ reduction in magnitude
similarities and differences between schizotypy and schizophrenia. of effect.
324 M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327

10. Conclusions Anglin, D.M., Cohen, P.R., Chen, H., 2008. Duration of early maternal separation
and prediction of schizotypy symptoms from early adolescence to midlife.
Schizophrenia Research 103 (1–3), 145–150.
As has been demonstrated, in the last five years there has Appels, M.C.M., Sitskoorn, M.M., Vollema, M.G., Kahn, R.S., 2004. Elevated levels of
been an explosion of research consistently demonstrating simi- schizotypal features in parents of patients with a family history of schizophrenia
larities between schizotypy and schizophrenia. Indeed, it would spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30 (4), 781–789.
Avramopoulos, D., Stefanis, N.C., Hantoumi, I., Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Ste-
seem that for many findings in relation to schizophrenia, there is fanis, C.N., 2002. Higher scores of self-reported schizotypy in healthy young
a corresponding finding in relation to schizotypy – although not males carrying the COMT high activity allele. Molecular Psychiatry 7 (7),
always of the same magnitude of effect. This is especially true for 706–711.
Barkus, E., Lewis, G., 2008. Schizotypy and psychosis-like experiences from recre-
environmental and neurocognitive findings, but is somewhat less
ational cannabis in a non-clinical sample. Psychological Medicine 38 (9),
true for biological and molecular genetic findings – where there 1267–1276.
is a relative paucity of research. The investigations reviewed in Barkus, E., Stirling, J., Hopkins, R., Lewis, S., 2006. The presence of neurological soft
signs along the psychosis proneness continuum. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32 (3),
the present article that report parallel, albeit attenuated, results
573–577.
between schizotypy and schizophrenia are consistent with a fully Baron, J.M., Gruen, R., Asnis, L., Kane, J., 1983. Familial relatedness of schizophre-
dimensional model, wherein varying levels of schizotypal person- nia and schizotypal states. The American Journal of Psychiatry 140 (111),
ality traits throughout the general population lie on a continuum 1437–1442.
Basso, M.R., Nasrallah, H.A., Olson, S.C., Bornstein, R.A., 1998. Neuropsychological
with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. correlates of negative, disorganized and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia.
It would appear that it is no longer a question of whether or not Schizophrenia Research 31 (2–3), 99–111.
the two constructs are related. A more pertinent question would be Batey, M., Furnham, A., 2008. The relationship between measures of creativity and
schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences 45 (8), 816–821.
what determines whether or not an individual makes the hypothet- Beauchaine, T.P., Lenzenweger, M.F., Waller, N.G., 2008. Schizotypy, taxometrics, and
ical transition from high schizotypy to full-blown psychosis? This disconfirming theories in soft science: comment on Rawlings, Williams, Haslam,
transition could happen in a number of different ways. One possible and Claridge. Personality and Individual Differences 44 (8), 1652–1662.
Bebbington, P., Nayani, T., 1995. The psychosis screening questionnaire. Interna-
mechanism might be that as an individual moves along the contin- tional Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 5 (1), 11–19.
uum, their symptoms become linearly more severe. For example, in Beck, A.T., Rector, N.A., Stolar, N., Grant, P., 2009. Schizophrenia: Cognitive Theory,
relation to cognitive deficits, it might be that impairments become Research, and Therapy. The Guilford Press, New York.
Bentall, R.P., Claridge, G., Slade, P.D., 1989. The multidimensional nature of schizo-
more numerous, pervasive and severe along an increasing schizo-
typal traits: a factor analytic study with normal subjects. Clinical Psychology 28
typy and psychosis scale. (4), 363–375.
However, as a fully dimensional model of schizotypy would sug- Berman, K., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., 2004. Functional brain imaging studies in
schizophrenia. In: Charney, D.S., Nestler, E.J. (Eds.), Neurobiology of Mental Ill-
gest, it is more likely that the effects of various genetic, biological
ness. , second ed. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 311–323.
and environmental risk factors combine with high schizotypy in Berrettini, W.H., 2000. Are schizophrenic and bipolar disorders related? A review of
various cumulative, nonlinear and perhaps even idiopathic ways family and molecular studies. Biological Psychiatry 48 (6), 531–538.
to determine outcome. Although outside the scope of this article, Bleuler, E., 1911/1950. Dementia Praecox, or the Group of Schizophrenias (J. Zinkin,
Trans.). International Universities Press, NY.
valuable intersections could be made here with the burgeoning lit- Bollini, A.M., Compton, M.T., Esterberg, M.L., Rutland, J., Chien, V.H., Walker, E.F.,
erature on ultra high-risk for psychosis groups, which identify the 2007. Associations between schizotypal features and indicators of neurological
anomalous psychotic experiences associated with schizotypy as a and morphological abnormalities. Schizophrenia Research 92 (1), 32–40.
Bora, E., Yucel, M., Pantelis, C., 2010. Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia and
risk factor for the development of full-blown psychosis along with a affective psychoses: implications for DSM-V criteria and beyond. Schizophrenia
number of other risk factors such as depression, reduced attention, Bulletin 36 (1), 36–42.
and family history of psychosis (Yung et al., 2004). Bramon, E., Sham, P.C., 2001. The common genetic liability between schizophrenia
and bipolar disorder: a review. Current Psychiatry Reports 3 (4), 332–337.
We propose that the relationship between schizotypy and Brett, C.M.C., Peters, E.P., Johns, L.C., Tabraham, P., Valmaggia, L.R., McGuire, P.K.,
psychosis can be viewed as similar to the relationship between neu- 2007. Appraisals of anomalous experiences interview (AANEX): a multidi-
roticism and anxiety disorders. High levels of neuroticism are often mensional measure of psychological responses to anomalies associated with
psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry 191 (Suppl. 51), s23–s30.
found in people with anxiety disorders, however high neuroticism
Brown, A.S., 2006. Prenatal infection as a risk factor for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
is not sufficient in and of itself to indicate the presence of an anx- Bulletin 32 (2), 200–202.
iety disorder. The same can be said of schizotypy and psychosis in Camisa, K.M., Brockbrader, M.A., Lysaker, P., Rae, L.L., Brenner, C.A., O’Donnell, B.F.,
2005. Personality traits in schizophrenia and related personality disorders. Psy-
that high levels of schizotypy may be associated with psychosis,
chiatry Research 133 (1), 23–33.
and they may be associated with similar variables to psychosis, but Cannon, T.D., Kaprio, J., Lonnqvist, J., Huttunen, M., Koskenvuo, M., 1998. The genetic
they do not necessarily indicate dysfunction. Just as neuroticism epidemiology of schizophrenia in a Finnish twin cohort: a population-based
on its own is insufficient to indicate a specific anxiety disorder, modelling study. Archives of General Psychiatry 55 (1), 67–74.
Cannon, T.D., Rosso, I.M., Hollister, J.M., Bearden, C.E., Sanchez, L.E., Hadley, T., 2000.
the value of schizotypy is not found in its use as a screening mea- A prospective cohort study of genetic and perinatal influences in the etiology of
sure for specific psychotic disorders. Rather, the value of schizotypy schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26 (2), 351–366.
lies in what it can tell us about normal human experience, and Cannon, T.D., van Erp, T.G.M., Glahn, D.C., 2002. Elucidating continuities and dis-
continuities between schizotypy and schizophrenia in the nervous system.
the possible pathways from psychological health to psychological Schizophrenia Research 54 (1–2), 151–156.
dysfunction. Chan, R.C.K., Wang, Y., Zhao, Q., Yan, C., Xu, T., Gong, Q.-Y., Manschreck, T.C., 2010.
Neurological soft signs in individuals with schizotypal personality features. Aus-
tralian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 44 (9), 800–804.
Chen, W.J., Hsiao, C.K., Lin, C.C.H., 1997. Schizotypy in community samples: the
References three-factor structure and correlation with sustained attention. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology 106 (4), 649–654.
Allardyce, J., Suppes, T., van Os, J., 2007. Dimensions and the positive psychosis phe- Cheniaux, E., Landeira-Fernandez, J., Telles, L.L., Lessa, J.L., Dias, A., Duncan, T., Ver-
notype. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 16 (Suppl. 1), siani, M., 2008. Does schizoaffective disorder really exist? A systematic review of
s34–s40. the studies that compared schizoaffective disorder with schizophrenia or mood
Allen, N.C., Bagade, S., McQueen, M.B., Ioannidis, J.P., Kavvoura, F.K., Khoury, M.J., disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders 106 (3), 209–217.
Tanzi, R.E., Bertram, L., 2008. Systematic meta-analyses and field synopsis of Claridge, G., Beech, T., 1995. Fully and quasi-dimensional constructions of schizo-
genetic association studies in schizophrenia: the SzGene database. Nature: typy. In: Raine, A., Lencz, T., Mednick, S.A. (Eds.), Schizotypal Personality.
Genetics 40 (7), 827–834. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 192–216.
American Psychiatric Association, 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Claridge, G., Davis, C., 2003. Personality and Psychological Disorders. Arnold, London.
Disorders, fourth ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, text rev. Claridge, G., McGreery, C., Mason, O., 1996. The factor structure of ‘schizotypal’ traits:
Andreasen, N.C., 1983. Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. University a large replication study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 35 (Pt 1), 103–115.
of Iowa, Iowa City, IA. Cochrane, M., Petch, I., Pickering, A., 2012. Aspects of cognitive functioning in schizo-
Andreasen, N.C., 1984. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. University of typy and schizophrenia: evidence of a continuum model. Psychiatry Research
Iowa, Iowa City, IA. 196 (2–3), 230–234.
M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327 325

Cohen, A.S., Buckner, J.D., Najolia, G.M., Stewart, D.W., 2011. Cannabis and Johns, L.C., van Os, J., 2001. The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general
psychometrically-defined schizotypy: use, problems and treatment consider- population. Clinical Psychology Review 21 (8), 1125–1141.
ations. Journal of Psychiatric Research 45 (4), 548–554. Kaczorowski, J.A., Barrantes-Vidal, N., Kwapil, T.R., 2009. Neurological soft signs
Compton, M.T., Broussard, B., 2009. The Episode of Psychosis: A Guide for Patients in psychometrically identified schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research 115 (2–3),
and their Families. Oxford University Press, Oxford. 293–302.
Compton, M.T., Chien, V.H., Bollini, A.M., 2009. Associations between past alcohol, Kelleher, I., Cannon, M., 2011. Psychotic-like experiences in the general population:
cannabis, and cocaine use and current schizotypy among first-degree relatives of characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychological Medicine 41 (1),
patients with schizophrenia and non-psychiatric controls. Psychiatric Quarterly 1–6.
80 (3), 143–154. Kelly, B.D., O’Callaghan, E., Waddington, J.L., Feeney, L., Browne, S., Scully, P.J.,
Compton, M.T., Goulding, S.M., Walker, E.F., 2007. Cannabis use, first-episode psy- Clarke, M., Quinn, J.F., McTigue, O., Morgan, M.G., Kinsella, A., Larkin, C., 2010.
chosis, and schizotypy: a summary and synthesis of recent literature. Current Schizophrenia and the city: a review of literature and prospective study of psy-
Psychology Review 3, 161–171. chosis and urbanicity in Ireland. Schizophrenia Research 116 (1), 75–89.
Dickey, C.C., McCarley, R., Shenton, M.E., 2002. The brain in schizotypal person- Kendler, K.S., Lieberman, J.A., Walsh, D., 1989. The structured interview for schizo-
ality disorder: a review of structural MRI and CT findings. Harvard Review of typy (SIS): a preliminary report. Schizophrenia Bulletin 15 (4), 559–571.
Psychiatry 10, 1–15. Kendler, K.S., Ochs, A.L., Gorman, A.M., Hewitt, J.K., Ross, D.E., Mirsky, A.F., 1991. The
Esterberg, M.L., Goulding, S.M., McClure-Tone, E.B., Compton, M.T., 2009. Schizotypy structure of schizotypy: a pilot multitrait twin study. Psychiatry Research 36 (1),
and nicotine, alcohol, and cannabis use in a non-psychiatric sample. Addictive 19–36.
Behaviours 34 (4), 374–379. Kendler, K.S., Thacker, L., Walsh, D., 1996. Self-report measures of schizotypy as
Fanous, A., Gardner, C., Walsh, D., Kendler, K.S., 2001. Relationship between pos- indices of familial vulnerability to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 22 (3),
itive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia and schizotypal symptoms in 511–521.
nonpsychotic relatives. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 669–673. Keshavan, M.S., Tandon, R., Boutros, N.N., Nasrallah, H.A., 2008. Schizophrenia, ‘just
Fanous, A.H., Kendler, K.S., 2004. The genetic relationship of personality to major the facts’. What we know in 2008. Part 3: neurobiology. Schizophrenia Research
depression and schizophrenia. Neurotoxicity Research 6 (1), 43–50. 106 (2–3), 89–107.
Fanous, A.H., Neale, M.C., Gardner, C.O., Webb, B.T., Straub, R.E., O’Neill, F.A., Walsh, Kety, S.S., Rosenthal, D., Wender, P.H., Shulsinger, F., Jacobson, B., 1975. Mental illness
D., Riley, B.P., Kendler, K.S., 2007. Significant correlation in linkage signals from in the biological and adoptive families of adopted individuals who have become
genome-wide scans of schizophrenia and schizotypy. Molecular Psychiatry 12 schizophrenic: a preliminary report based on psychiatric interviews. In: Fieve,
(10), 958–965. R., Brills, H. (Eds.), Genetic Research in Psychiatry. The Johns Hopkins University
Fioravanti, M., Carlone, O., Vitale, B., Cinti, M., Clare, L., 2005. A meta-analysis of Press, Baltimore, MD.
cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Kiang, M., 2010. Schizotypy and language: a review. Journal of Neurolinguistics 28,
Review 15 (2), 73–95. 193–203.
Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paino, M., Lemos-Giraldez, S., Garcia-Cueto, E., Campillo- Korfine, L., Lenzenweger, M.F., 1995. The taxonicity of schizotypy: a replication.
Alvarez, A., Villazon-Garcia, U., Muhiz, J., 2008. Schizotypy assessment: state Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104 (1), 26–31.
of the art and future prospects. International Journal of Clinical and Health Kraemer, H.C., Noda, A., O’Hara, R., 2004. Categorical versus dimensional approaches
Psychology 8 (2), 557–593. to diagnosis: methodological challenges. Journal of Psychiatric Research 38 (1),
Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paino, M., Lemos-Giraldez, S., Sierra-Baigrie, S., Muniz, J., 2011. 17–25.
Measurement invariance of the schizotypal personality questionnaire-brief Kraeplin, E., 1919/1971. Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia (R. M. Barclay, Trans.).
across gender and age. Psychiatry Research 190 (2–3), 309–315. Robert E. Krieger Publishing Company, Huntington, NY.
Fornito, A., Yucel, M., Patti, J., Wood, S.J., Pantelis, C., 2009. Mapping grey matter Kremen, W.S., Faraone, S.V., Toomey, R., Seidman, L.J., Tsuang, M.T., 1998. Sex differ-
reductions in schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation analysis of ences in self-reported schizotypal traits in relatives of schizophrenic probands.
voxel-based morphometry studies. Schizophrenia Research 108 (1–3), 104–113. Schizophrenia Research 34 (1–2), 27–37.
Galderisi, S., Davidson, M., Kahn, R.S., Mucci, A., Boter, H., Gheorghe, M.D., Kubicki, M., McCarley, R., Westin, C.-F., Park, H.-J., Maier, S., Kikinis, R., Jolesz, F.A.,
Rybakowski, J., Libiger, J., Dolifus, S., Lopez-Ibor, J.J., Peuskens, J., Hranov, L.G., Shenton, M.E., 2007. A review of diffusion tensor images in schizophrenia. Jour-
Fleischhacker, W., 2009. Correlates of cognitive impairment in first episode nal of Psychiatric Research 41 (1–2), 15–30.
schizophrenia: the EUFEST study. Schizophrenia Research 115 (2–3), 104–114. Kwapil, T.R., Barrantes-Vidal, N., Silvia, P.J., 2008. The dimensional structure of
Gottesman, I.I., McGuffin, P., Farmer, A.E., 1987. Clinical genetics as clues to the ‘real’ the Wisconsin Schizotypy Scales: factor identification and construct validity.
genetics of schizophrenia (a decade of modest gains while playing for time). Schizophrenia Bulletin 34 (3), 444–457.
Schizophrenia Bulletin 13 (1), 23–47. Kyriakopoulos, M., Bargiotas, T., Barker, G.J., Frangou, S., 2008. Diffusion tensor imag-
Goulding, A., 2004. Schizotypy models in relation to subjective health and para- ing in schizophrenia. European Psychiatry 23 (4), 255–273.
normal beliefs and experiences. Personality and Individual Differences 37 (1), Lagioia, A., van de Ville, D., Debbane, M., Lazeyras, F., Eliez, S., 2010. Adolescent
157–167. resting state networks and their associations with schizotypal trait expression.
Greene, R.L., 2000. The MMPI-2: An interpretive manual, 2nd ed. Allyn & Bacon, Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 4, 35.
Needham Heights, MA, US. Lardinois, M., Lataster, T., Mengelers, R., van Os, J., Myin-Germeys, I., 2011. Child-
Gur, R.E., Keshavan, M.S., Lawrie, M., 2007. Deconstructing psychosis with human hood trauma and increased stress sensitivity in psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica
brain imaging. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33 (4), 921–931. Scandinavica 123 (1), 28–35.
Haenschel, C., Linden, D., 2011. Exploring intermediate phenotypes with EEG: work- Leask, S.J., Done, D.J., Crow, T.J., 2002. Adult psychosis, common childhood infec-
ing memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Behavioural Brain Research 216 (2), tions and neurological soft signs in a national birth cohort. The British Journal
481–495. of Psychiatry 181, 387–392.
Hanssen, M., Bak, M., Bijl, R., Vollebergh, W., van Os, J., 2005. The incidence and Lenzenweger, M.F., 1994. Psychometric high-risk paradigm, perceptual aberrations,
outcome of subclinical psychotic experiences in the general population. British and schizotypy: an update. Schizophrenia Bulletin 20 (1), 121–135.
Journal of Psychiatry 44 (pt 2), 181–191. Lenzenweger, M.F., 2006. Schizotypy: an organizing framework for schizophrenia
Hanssen, M., Krabbendam, L., Vollema, M., Delespaul, P., van Os, J., 2006. Evidence research. Current Directions in Psychological Science 15 (4), 162–166.
for instrument and family-specific variation of subclinical psychosis dimensions Levy, D.L., Sereno, A.B., Gooding, D.C., O’Driscoll, G.A., 2010. Eye tracking dysfunc-
in the general population. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115 (1), 5–14. tion in schizophrenia: characterization and pathophysiology. In: Swerdlow,
Hazlett, E.A., Goldstein, K.E., Kolaitis, J.C., 2012. A review of structural MRI and dif- N.R. (Ed.), Behavioral Neurobiology of Schizophrenia and its Treatment. Cur-
fusion tensor imaging in schizotypal personality disorder. Current Psychiatry rent Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. Springer-Verlag Publishing, New York,
Reports 14 (1), 70–78. pp. 311–347.
Heinrichs, R.W., 2005. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. American Psychol- Liddle, P.F., 1987. The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia: a re-examination of the
ogy 60 (3), 229–242. positive negative dichotomy. British Journal of Psychology 151, 145–151.
Heinrichs, R.W., Zakzanis, K., 1998. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quan- Lindsberg, J., Poutiainen, E., Kalska, H., 2009. Clarifying the diversity of first-episode
titative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12 (3), 426–445. psychosis: neuropsychological correlates of clinical symptoms. Nordic Journal
Horan, W.P., Blanchard, J.J., Clark, L.A., Green, M.F., 2008. Affective traits in of Psychiatry 63 (6), 493–500.
schizophrenia and schizotypy. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34 (5), 856–874. Linscott, R.J., van Os, J., 2010. Categorical versus continuum models in psychosis:
Horwood, J., Salvi, G., Thomas, K., Duffy, L., Gunnell, D., Hollis, C., Lewis, G., Menezes, evidence for discontinuous subpopulations underlying a psychometric contin-
P., Thompson, A., Wolke, D., Zammit, S., Harrison, G., 2008. IQ and non-clinical uum. Implications for DSM-V, DSM-VI, and DSM-VII. Annual Review of Clinical
psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Psychology 6, 391–419.
British Journal of Psychiatry 193 (3), 185–191. Lipton, R.B., Levy, D.L., Holzman, P.S., Levin, S., 1983. Eye movement dysfunctions in
Jacobson, S., Kelleher, I., Harley, M., Murtagh, A., Clarke, M., Blanchard, M., Connolly, psychiatric patients: a review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 9 (1), 13–32.
C., O’Hanion, E., Garavan, H., Cannon, M., 2010. Structural and functional brain Livesley, W.J., Jackson, D.N., 1992. Guidelines for developing, evaluating, and revising
correlates of subclinical psychotic symptoms in 11–13 year old schoolchildren. the classification of personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease
NeuroImage 49 (2), 1875–1885. 180 (10), 609–618.
Jang, K.L., Woodward, T.S., Lang, D., Honer, W.G., Livesley, J., 2005. The genetic and Lovatt, A., Mason, O., Brett, C., Peters, E., 2010. Psychotic-like experiences, appraisals
environmental basis of the relationship between schizotypy and personality: a and trauma. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 198 (11), 813–819.
twin study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 193 (3), 153–159. Ma, X., Sun, J., Yao, J., Wang, Q., Hu, X., Deng, W., Sun, X., Liu, X., Murray, R.M., Collier,
Johns, L.C., Nazroo, J.Y., Bebbington, P., Kuipers, E., 2002. Occurrence of hallucinatory D.A., Li, T., 2007. A quantitative association study between schizotypal traits and
experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. The British Journal COMPT, PRODH and BDNF genes in a healthy Chinese population. Psychiatry
of Psychiatry 180, 174–178. Research 153 (1), 7–15.
326 M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327

MacDonald, A.W.I., Pogue-Geile, M.F., Debski, T.T., Manuck, S., 2001. Genetic A., Sklar, P., Sullivan, P.F., 2009. Genomwide association studies: history, ratio-
and environmental influences on schizotypy: a community-based twin study. nale, and prospects for psychiatric disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry
Schizophrenia Bulletin 27 (1), 47–58. 166 (5), 540–556.
Malaspina, D., Harlap, S., Fennig, S., Heiman, D., Nahon, D., Feldman, D., Susser, E.S., Rado, S., 1953. Dynamics and classification of disordered behaviour. The American
2001. Advancing paternal age and the risk of schizophrenia. Archives of General Journal of Psychiatry 110 (6), 406–416.
Psychiatry 58 (4), 361–367. Raine, A., 1991. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based
Markham, J.A., Koenig, J.I., 2011. Prenatal stress: role in psychotic and depressive on DSM-II-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin 17 (4), 555–564.
diseases. Psychopharmacology 214 (1), 89–106. Raine, A., 2006. Schizotypal personality: neurodevelopmental and psychosocial tra-
Mason, O., Claridge, G., 2006. The Oxford-Liverpool inventory of feelings and experi- jectories. Annual Reviews of Clinical Psychology 2, 291–326.
ences: further description and extended norms. Schizophrenia Research 82 (2), Raine, A., Sheard, C., Reynolds, G.P., Lencz, T., 1992. Pre-frontal structural and func-
203–211. tional deficits associated with individual differences in schizotypal personality.
Mason, O., Claridge, G., Jackson, M., 1995. New scales for the assessment of schizo- Schizophrenia Research 7 (3), 237–247.
typy. Personality and Individual Differences 18 (1), 7–13. Rantakallio, P., Jones, P., Moring, J., von Wendt, L., 1997. Association between central
Mata, I., Gilvarry, C.M., Jones, P.B., Lewis, S.W., Murray, R.M., Sham, P.C., 2003. Schizo- nervous system infections during childhood and adult onset schizophrenia and
typal personality traits in nonpsychotic relatives are associated with positive other psychoses: a 28-year follow-up. International Journal of Epidemiology 26
symptoms in psychotic probands. Schizophrenia Bulletin 29 (2), 273–283. (4), 837–843.
McClellan, J., Susser, E., King, M.C., 2007. Schizophrenia: a common disease caused Rawlings, D., Locarnini, A., 2008. Dimensional schizotypy, autism, and unusual word
by multiple rare alleles. British Journal of Psychiatry 190, 194–199. associations in artists and scientists. Journal of Research in Personality 42 (2),
McCue, M., Gottesman, I.I., Rao, D.C., 1983. The transmission of schizophrenia under 465–471.
a multifactorial threshold model. American Journal of Human Genetics 35 (6), Rawlings, D., Williams, B., Haslam, N., Claridge, G., 2008a. Is schizotypy taxonic?
1161–1178. Response to Beauchaine, Lenzenweger, and Waller. Personality and Individual
McGorry, P.D., Allot, K., Jackson, H.J., 2009. Diagnosis and the staging model of psy- Differences 44 (8), 1663–1672.
chosis. In: Jackson, H.J., McGorry, P.D. (Eds.), The Recognition and Management Rawlings, D., Williams, B., Haslam, N., Claridge, G., 2008b. Taxometric analysis sup-
of Early Psychosis. Cambridge University Press, Melbourne, pp. 17–28. ports a dimensional latent structure for schizotypy. Personality and Individual
McGrath, J., El-Saadi, O., Grim, V., Cardy, S., Chapple, B., Chant, D., Lieberman, D., Differences 44 (8), 1640–1651.
Mowry, B., 2002. Minor physical anomalies and quantitative measures of the Reichenberg, A., 2010. The assessment of neuropsychological functioning in
head and face in patients with psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry 59 (5), schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12 (3), 383–392.
458–464. Reichenberg, A., Harvey, P.D., 2007. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophre-
Meehl, P.E., 1962. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. American Psychology 17 nia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychological
(12), 827–838. Bulletin 133 (5), 833–858.
Meehl, P.E., 1990. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and Richardson, T.H., 2010. Cannabis use and mental health: a review of recent epidemi-
schizophrenia. Journal of Personality Disorders 4 (1), 1–99. ological research. International Journal of Pharmacology 6 (8), 796–807.
Mesholm-Gately, R.I., Guiliano, A.J., Goff, K.P., Faraone, S.V., Seidman, L.J., 2009. Ripke, S., Sanders, A.R., Kendler, K.S., Levinson, D.F., Sklar, P., Holmans, P.A., Lin,
Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neu- D.-Y., Duan, J., Orphoff, R., Andreassen, O.A., Scolnick, E., Cichon, S., St Clair,
ropsychology 23 (3), 315–336. D., Corvin, A., Gurling, H., Werge, T., Rujescu, D., Blackwood, D.H.R., Pato, C.N.,
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Weinberger, D.R., 2006. Intermediate phenotypes and genetic Malhotra, A.K., Purcell, S., Dudbridge, F., Neale, B.M., Rossin, L., Visscher, P.M.,
mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 7 (10), Posthuma, D., Ruderfer, D.M., Fanous, A., Stefansson, H., Steinberg, S., Mowry,
818–827. B., Golimbet, V., De Hert, M., Jonsson, E.G., Bitter, I., Pietilainen, O.P.H., Collier,
Modinos, G., Mechelli, A., Ormel, J., Groenwold, A., Aleman, A., McGuire, P.K., 2010. D.A., Tosato, S., Agartz, I., Albus, M., Alexander, M., Amdur, R.L., Amin, F., Bass,
Schizotypy and brain structure: a voxel-based morphometry study. Psycholog- N., Bergen, S.E., Black, D.W., Borglum, A.D., Brown, M.A., Bruggeman, R., Buc-
ical Medicine 40 (9), 1423–1431. cola, N.G., Byerley, W.F., Cahn, W., Cantor, R.M., Carr, V., Catts, S.V., Choudhury,
Morgan, C., Charalambides, M., Hutchinson, G., Murray, R.M., 2010. Migration, K., Cloninger, C.R., Cormican, P., Craddock, N., Danoy, P.A., Datta, S., de Haan,
ethnicity and psychosis: toward a sociodevelopmental model. Schizophrenia L., Demontis, D., Dikeos, D., Djurovic, S., Donnelly, P., Donohoe, G., Duong, L.,
Bulletin 36 (4), 655–664. Dwyer, S., Fink-Jensen, A., Freedman, R., Freimer, N.B., Friedl, M., Georgieva, L.,
Morgan, C., Fisher, H., Hutchinson, G., Kirkbride, J., Craig, T.K., Morgan, K., Dazzan, P., Giegling, I., Gill, M., Glenthoj, B., Godard, S., Hamshere, M., Hansen, M., Hansen,
Boydell, J., Doody, G.A., Jones, P.B., Murray, R.M., Leff, J., Fearon, P., 2009. Ethnic- T., Hartmann, A.M., Henskens, F.A., Hougaard, D.M., Hultman, C.M., Ingason, A.,
ity, social disadvantage and psychotic-like experiences in a healthy population Jablensky, A.V., Jakobsen, K.D., Jay, M., Gesche, Kahn, R.S., Keller, M.C., Kenis,
based sample. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 119 (3), 226–235. G., Kenny, E., Kim, Y., Kirov, G.K., Konnerth, H., Konte, B., Krabbendam, L., Kra-
Morgan, C., Kirkbride, J., Leff, J., Craig, T., Hutchinson, G., McKenzie, K., Morgan, K., sucki, R., Lasseter, V.K., Laurent, C., Lawrence, J., Lencz, T., Lerer, F.B., Liang, K.-Y.,
Dazzan, P., Doody, G.A., Jones, P., Murray, R., Fearon, P., 2007. Parental separation, Lichtenstein, P., Lieberman, J.A., Linszen, D.H., Lonnqvist, J., Loughland, C.M.,
loss and psychosis in different ethnic groups: a case-control study. Psychological Maclean, A.W., Maher, B.S., Maier, W., Mallet, J., Malloy, P., Mattheisen, M., Mat-
Medicine 37 (4), 495–503. tingsdal, M., McGhee, K.A., McGrath, J.J., McIntosh, A., McLean, D.E., McQuillin,
Moritz, S., Andresen, B., Naber, D., Krausz, M., Probstein, E., 1999. Neuropsycholo- A., Melle, I., Michie, P.T., Milanova, V., Morris, D.W., Mors, O., Mortensen, P.B.,
gical correlates of schizotypal disorganisation. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 4 (4), Moskvina, V., Muglia, P., Myin-Germeys, I., Nertney, D.A., Nesadt, G., Nielsen, J.,
343–349. Nikolov, I., Nordentoft, M., Norton, N., Nothen, M.M., O’Dushlaine, C.T., Olincy,
Nelson, B., Rawlings, D., 2010. Relating schizotypy and personality to the pheno- A., Olsen, L., O’Neill, F.A., Orntoft, T.F., Owen, M.J., Pantelis, C., Papdimitriou, G.,
menology of creativity. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36 (2), 388–399. Pato, M.T., Peltonen, L., Petursson, H., Pickard, B., Pimm, J., Pulver, A.E., Puri, V.,
Nelson, M.T., Seal, M.L., Phillips, L.J., Merritt, A.H., Wilson, R., Pantelis, C., 2011. An Quested, D., Quinn, E.M., Rasmussen, H.B., Rethelyi, J.M., Ribble, R., Rietschel, M.,
investigation of the relationship between cortical connectivity and schizotypy Riley, B.P., Shi, J., Sigurdsson, E., Silverman, J.M., Spencer, C.C.A., Stefansson, K.,
in the general population. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 199 (5), Strange, A., Strengman, E., Stroup, T.S., Suvisaari, J., Terenius, L., Thirumalai, S.,
348–353. Thygesen, J.H., Timm, S., Toncheva, D., van den Oord, E., van Os, J., van Winkel,
Neuvo, R., Chatterji, S., Verdes, E., Naidoo, N., Arango, C., Ayuso-Mateos, J.L., 2012. The R., Veldink, J., Walsh, D., Wang, A.G., Wiersma, D., Wildenauer, D.B., Williams,
continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: a cross-national H.J., Williams, N.M., Wormley, B., Zammit, S., Sullivan, P.F., C.O.D.M., Daly, M.J.,
study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38 (3), 475–485. Gejman, P.V., The Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study
Noguchi, H., Hori, H., Kunugi, H., 2008. Schizotypal traits and cognitive function in (GWAS) Consortium, 2011. Genome-wide association study identifies five new
healthy adults. Psychiatry Research 161 (2), 162–169. schizophrenia loci. Nature Genetics 43 (10), 969–976.
O’Driscoll, G.A., Lenzenweger, M.F., Holzman, P.S., 1998. Antisaccades and smooth Rossi, A., Daneluzzo, E., 2002. Schizotypal dimensions in normals and schizophrenic
pursuit eye tracking and schizotypy. Archives of General Psychiatry 55 (9), patients: a comparison with other clinical samples. Schizophrenia Bulletin 54
837–843. (1–2), 67–75.
Obiols, J.E., Serrano, F., Caparros, B., Subira, S., Barrantes, N., 1999. Neurological soft Schmidt-Hansen, M., Honey, R.C., 2009. Working memory and multidimensional
signs in adolescents with poor performance on the continuous performance test: schizotypy: dissociable influences of the different dimensions. Cognitive Neu-
markers of liability for schizophrenia spectrum disorders? Psychiatry Research ropsychiatry 26 (7), 655–670.
86 (3), 217–228. Scott, J., Martin, G., Bor, W., Sawyer, M., Clark, J., McGrath, J., 2009. The prevalence and
Pantelis, C., Stuart, G.W., Nelson, H.E., Robbins, T.W., Barnes, T.R.E., 2001. Spatial correlates of hallucinations in Australian adolescents: results from a national
working memory deficits in schizophrenia: relationship with tardive dyskinesia survey. Schizophrenia Research 107 (2–3), 179–185.
and negative symptoms. The American Journal of Psychiatry 158 (8), 1276–1285. Scott, J., Welham, J.L., Martin, G., Bor, W., Najman, J.M., O’Callaghan, M., Williams, G.,
Pantelis, C., Yucel, M., Wood, S.J., Velakoulis, D., Sun, D., Berger, G., Stuart, G.W., Aird, R., McGrath, J., 2008. Demographic correlates of psychotic-like experiences
Yung, A., Phillips, L., McGorry, P.D., 2005. Structural brain imaging evidence in young Australian adults. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 118 (3), 230–237.
for multiple pathological processes at different stages of brain development in Seal, M.L., Yucel, M., Fornito, A., Wood, S.J., Harrison, B.J., Walterfang, M., Pell, G.S.,
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 31 (3), 672–696. Pantelis, C., 2008. Abnormal white matter microstructure in schizophrenia: a
Park, S., McTigue, K., 1997. Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal voxelwise analysis of axial and radial diffusivity. Schizophrenia Research 101
personality. Schizophrenia Research 26 (2–3), 213–220. (1), 106–110.
Petersen, L., Mortensen, P.B., Pederson, C.B., 2011. Paternal age at birth of first child Sharpley, M.S., Peters, E.R., 1999. Ethnicity, class and schizotypy. Social Psychiatry
and risk of schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry 168 (1), 82–88. and Epidemiology 34 (10), 507–512.
Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Coordinating Committee, Cichon, S., Craddock, N., Shean, G., Bell, E., Cameron, C., 2007. Recognition of nonverbal affect and schizotypy.
Daly, M., Faraone, S.V., Gejman, P.V., Kelsoe, J., Lehner, T., Levenson, D.F., Moran, The Journal of Psychology 141 (3), 281–291.
M.T. Nelson et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 317–327 327

Shenton, M.E., Dickey, C.C., Frumin, M., McCarley, R.W., 2001. A review of MRI find- Verdoux, H., Maurice-Tison, S., Gay, B., van Os, J., Salamon, R., Bourgeois, M.L., 1998.
ings in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 49 (1–2), 1–52. A survey of delusional ideation in primary-care patients. Psychological Medicine
Siever, L.J., Davis, K.L., 2004. The pathophysiology of schizophrenia disorders: per- 28 (1), 127–134.
spectives from the spectrum. The American Journal of Psychiatry 161., 398–413. Volpe, U., Federspiel, A., Mucci, A., Dierks, T., Frank, A., Wahlund, L.-O., Galderisi, S.,
Simonsen, C., Sundet, K., Vaskinn, A., Birkenaes, A.B., Engh, J.A., Faerden, A., Jons- Maj, M., 2008. Cerebral connectivity and psychotic personality traits: a diffusion
dottir, H., Ringen, P.A., Opjordsmoen, S., Melle, I., Friis, S., Andreasen, O.A., 2011. tensor imaging study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuro-
Neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar and schizophrenia spectrum disorders science 258 (5), 292–299.
depends on history of psychosis rather than diagnostic group. Schizophrenia Waller, N.G., Meehl, P.E., 1998. Multivariate Taxometric Procedures: Distinguishing
Bulletin 37 (1), 73–83. Types from Continua. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Skinner, R., Conlon, L., Gibbons, D., McDonald, C., 2011. Cannabis use and non-clinical Waller, N.G., Yonce, L.J., Grove, W.M., Faust, D., Lenzenweger, M.F., 2006. A Paul
dimensions of psychosis in university students presenting to primary care. Acta Meehl Reader: Essays on the Practice of Scientific Psychology (Multivariate
Psychiatrica Scandinavica 123 (1), 21–27. Applications Series). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey.
St Clair, D., Xu, M., Wang, P., Yu, Y., Fang, Y., Zhang, F., Zheng, X., Gu, N., Feng, G., Sham, Ward, K.E., Friedman, L., Wise, A., Schultz, S.C., 1996. Meta-analysis of brain and
P., He, L., 2005. Rates of adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to the cranial size in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 22 (3), 197–213.
Chinese famine of 1959–1961. Journal of the American Medical Association 294 Willinger, U., Heiden, A.M., Meszaros, K., Formann, A.K., Aschauer, H.N., 2002. Mater-
(5), 557–562. nal bonding behavior in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder, considering
Steel, C., Marzillier, S., Fearon, P., Ruddle, A., 2009. Childhood abuse and schizotypal premorbid personality traits. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry
personality. Social Psychiatry and Epidemiology 44 (11), 917–923. 36 (5), 663–668.
Stefanis, N.C., Hanssen, M., Smirnis, N.K., Avramopoulos, D.A., Evdokimidis, I.K., Ste- Wing, J.K., Agrawal, N., 2003. Concepts and classification of schizophrenia. In: Hirsch,
fanis, C.N., Verdoux, H., van Os, J., 2002. Evidence that three dimensions of S.R., Weinberger, D.R. (Eds.), Schizophrenia. , second ed. Blackwell, Maiden, MA,
psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychological Medicine pp. 3–14.
32 (2), 347–358. Wong, A.H., van Tol, H.H., 2003. Schizophrenia: from phenomenology to neurobio-
Stefanis, N.C., Smyrnis, N., Avramopoulos, D., Evdokimidis, I., Ntzoufras, I., Stefanis, logy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 27 (3), 269–306.
C.N., 2004. Factorial composition of self-rated schizotypal traits among young Wood, S.J., Pantelis, C., Proffitt, T., Phillips, L.J., Stuart, G.W., Buchanan, J.A., Mahony,
males undergoing military training. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30 (2), 335–350. K., Brewer, W., Smith, D.J., McGorry, P.D., 2003. Spatial working memory ability
Stefanis, N.C., Trikalinos, T.A., Avramopoulos, D., Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Ntzani, is a marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychological Medicine 33, 1239–1247.
E.E., Ioannidis, J.P., Stefanis, C.N., 2007. Impact of schizophrenia candidate genes Wood, S.J., Yung, A.R., McGorry, P.D., Pantelis, C., 2011. Neuroimaging and treatment
on schizotypy and cognitive endophenotypes at the population level. Biological evidence for clinical staging in psychotic disorders: from the at-risk mental state
Psychiatry 62 (7), 784–792. to chronic schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 70 (7), 619–625.
Stober, G., Kocher, I., Franzek, E., Beckmann, H., 1997. First-trimester maternal ges- Wuthrich, V.M., Bates, T.C., 2006. Confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor
tational infection and cycloid psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 96 (5), structure of the schizotypal personality questionnaire and chapman schizotypy
319–324. scales. Journal of Personality Assessment 87 (3), 292–304.
Tandon, R., Keshavan, M.S., Nasrallah, H.A., 2008. Schizophrenia, ‘just the facts’: what Xiao, J., Buka, S.L., Cannon, T.D., Susuki, Y., Viscidi, R.P., Torrey, E.F., Yolken, R.H., 2009.
we know in 2008. 2. Epidemiology and etiology. Schizophrenia Research 102 Serological pattern consistent with infection with type I toxoplasma gondii in
(1–3), 1–18. mothers and risk of psychosis among adult offspring. Microbes and Infection 11
Tandon, R., Nasrallah, H.A., Keshavan, M.S., 2009. Schizophrenia, ‘just the facts’ 4. (3), 1011–1018.
Clinical features and conceptualisation. Schizophrenia Research 110 (1–3), 1–23. Yaralian, P.S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Hooley, J.M., Bihrie, S.E., Ventura, J., 2000. Elevated
The International Schizophrenia Consortium, 2009. Common polygenetic varia- levels of cognitive-perceptual deficits in individuals with a family history of
tion contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460 (6), schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research 46 (1), 57–63.
748–752. Yung, A.R., Phillips, L.J., Yuen, H.P., McGorry, P.D., 2004. Risk factors for psychosis in
van Os, J., Linscott, R.J., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., Krabbendam, L., 2009. A an ultra high-risk group: psychopathology and clinical features. Schizophrenia
systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for Research 67 (2), 131–142.
a psychosis proneness–persistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder. Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., Seal, M.L., Cocchi, L., Westin, C.F., Bullmore, E.T., Egan, G.F.,
Psychological Medicine 39 (2), 179–195. Pantelis, C., 2011. Disrupted axonal fiber connectivity in schizoprenia. Biological
Velakoulis, D., Wood, S.J., Wong, M.T., McGorry, P.D., Yung, A., Phillips, L.J., Psychiatry 69 (1), 80–89.
Smith, D., Brewer, W., Proffitt, T., Desmond, P., Pantelis, C., 2006. Hippocam- Zammit, S., Odd, D., Horwood, J., Thompson, A., Thomas, K., Menezes, P., Gunnell, D.,
pal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: a Hollis, C., Wolke, D., Lewis, G., Harrison, G., 2009. Investigating whether adverse
magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psy- prenatal and perinatal events are associated with non-clinical psychotic symp-
chosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals. Achives of General Psychiatry 63 (2), toms at age 12 years in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Psychological Medicine 39 (9),
139–149. 1457–1467.

You might also like