Original Research: Patterns of Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Original Research

Patterns of Clinical Reasoning in


Physical Therapist Students
Sarah Gilliland, Susan Flannery Wainwright
S. Gilliland, PT, DPT, PhD, Department
of Physical Therapy, Crean College
Background and Purpose. Clinical reasoning is a complex, nonlinear problem-­ of Health and Behavioral Sciences,
solving process that is influenced by models of practice. The development of physical Chapman University, Irvine, CA 92618
therapists’ clinical reasoning abilities is a crucial yet underresearched aspect of entry-level (USA). Address all correspondence to
(professional) physical therapist education. Dr. Gilliland at: sgillila@chapman.edu.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


S.F. Wainwright, PT, PhD, Department
Objectives.  The purpose of this qualitative study was to examine the types of clinical of Physical Therapy, Jefferson School of
reasoning strategies physical therapist students engage in during a patient encounter. Health Professions, Thomas Jefferson
University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
Methods. A qualitative descriptive case study design involving within and across [Gilliland S, Wainwright SF. Patterns of
case analysis was used. Eight second-year, professional physical therapist students from clinical reasoning in physical therapist
2 different programs completed an evaluation and initial intervention for a standardized students. Phys Ther. 2017;97:499–511.]
­patient followed by a retrospective think-aloud interview to explicate their reasoning pro-
© 2017 American Physical Therapy
cesses. Participants’ clinical reasoning strategies were examined using a 2-stage qualitative
Association
method of thematic analysis. Published Ahead of Print:
March 24, 2017
Results.  Participants demonstrated consistent signs of development of physical ­therapy– Accepted: March 14, 2017
specific reasoning processes, yet varied in their approach to the case and use of reflec- Submitted: February 10, 2016
tion. Participants who gave greater attention to patient education and empowerment also
demonstrated greater use of reflection-in-action during the patient encounter. One nega-
tive case illustrates the variability in the rate at which students may develop these abilities.

Conclusions. Participants demonstrated development toward physical therapist–­


specific clinical reasoning, yet demonstrated qualitatively different approaches to the
­patient encounter. Multiple factors, including the use of reflection-in-action, may enable
students to develop greater flexibility in their reasoning processes.

Post a comment for this


article at:
https://academic.oup.com/ptj

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    499


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

C
linical reasoning is a complex physical therapy–specific characteristics soning with the patient while drawing
problem-framing, problem-solv- in their reasoning processes. on diverse knowledge sources through
ing, and decision-making pro- a seamless flow of social interaction
cess necessary for effective health care The process of hypothesis formation integrated with ­ assessment and treat-
practice. This highly context-dependent and evaluation is central to clinical rea- ment.14,30 Further, expert therapists
process requires interaction with the soning.22 During the diagnostic pro- give more attention to empowering, en-
patient, caregivers, and other health cess, health care practitioners develop gaging, and educating the patient than
care team members and is influenced hypotheses that guide data collected to their own skills and techniques.31,32
by models of practice.1 The process of during examination12 and the devel- These prior studies of clinical reasoning
clinical reasoning encompasses how a opment of treatment.23 A hypothesis is and expert practice in physical therapy
health care practitioner’s knowledge is any diagnostic idea2 that may identify have provided a framework for describ-
translated into patient care2—yet many pathology, an impairment, function- ing practice, yet have provided minimal

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


factors, including beliefs and models al deficit, or causes of and factors in- insight into how students develop these
of practice, influence what resources a fluencing the patient’s disability.9,10,23 capacities.
practitioner uses during rapid decision These hypotheses represent the way
making.3 Additionally, the iterative pro- practice-specific knowledge is organ- To date, there has been little work
cess of clinical reasoning requires clini- ized.24 A critical component of clinical addressing how to bridge what is
­
cians to make decisions and continually reasoning in orthopedic physical thera- known about expertise and expert
reassess actions taken in the face of un- py is the generation of comprehensive practice with professional education
certainty.1,2 hypotheses that address factors related practices for instruction and assess-
to the patient, the therapist, and the ment of clinical reasoning.33,34 Profes-
Importance of Clinical specific context.25 The hypotheses that sional physical therapist educators lack
clinicians develop during the patient consensus on what constitutes clinical
Reasoning Specific to examination and assessment represent reasoning and describe great variation
Physical Therapy their unfolding diagnostic process. in approaches to teaching it.35 To effec-
While studies have addressed the issue tively prepare professional students for
of diagnostic reasoning in medical stu- Patient cases are ambiguous by nature; autonomous practice,36,37 profession-
dents,4–6 3 key differences in physical thus clinical reasoning requires practi- al education programs must support
therapist practice suggest the need to tioners to develop a reasoning frame- students in developing their clinical
examine teaching strategies and the de- work when not all the facts are known.2 reasoning skills. One important step
velopment of clinical reasoning in stu- The lack of explicit structure in patient toward elucidating students’ develop-
dents specific to physical therapy. Stud- cases requires the clinician to determine ment of clinical ­reasoning is to examine
ies of medical reasoning have focused what to focus on prior to solving the how they ­engage in clinical reasoning.
on diagnostic reasoning to identify ac- problems presented.26 The approaches Building on the existing frameworks of
tive pathology (medical diagnosis).4–9 that therapists take to interacting with, clinical reasoning in experienced phys-
Within physical therapist practice, diag- examining, and assessing patients are ical therapists, this study examined the
nostic reasoning must not only identify shaped by the way the therapists frame patterns of reasoning strategies and
the active pathology but also identify the patient’s problems27 and are observ- assessments that second-year students
the reason for the problem and the con- able reasoning strategies. The types of demonstrated during a patient encoun-
sequences of the illness/disease pro- reasoning strategies used represent the ter. The ­primary research question for
cess.10–13 Second, physical therapists’ nature and scope of the specific health this study was: During an encounter
clinical reasoning includes an emphasis care practice.28 The reasoning strategies with a patient, what clinical decisions
on the analysis of movement10,13 that is that therapists engage in, alongside the do physical therapist students make,
central to experienced therapists’ clini- hypotheses they form, represent the and what clinical reasoning strategies
cal reasoning processes14 across varied scope and shape of the clinical deci- underlie their decisions?
physical therapist practice settings.15–18 sions they make. Analyses of the rea-
Third, due to the ongoing and interac-
tive nature of therapeutic work, phys-
soning strategies students draw on and Methods
the hypotheses they form can provide This qualitative, descriptive, multiple
ical therapists work collaboratively insight into their approaches to framing case study design involving within and
with the patient to determine ways to clinical problems. across case analyses38 allowed analysis
engage and motivate the patient in the
of the individual students’ reasoning,
treatment process.19,20 The interactive Expert physical therapists demonstrate patterns within the 2 Doctor of Physical
process of clinical reasoning includes not only efficient forward-reasoning Therapy (DPT) programs, and patterns
gaining an understanding of the pa- processes, but also a balance of ana- across all students.39,40 The participants’
tient’s context and perspective on the lytical problem solving with narrative clinical decisions and reasoning strate-
illness or injury.12,21 As physical ther- reasoning focused on the patient as gies were analyzed using the qualitative
apist students progress through their an individual.28,29 ­Expert therapists method of thematic analysis.41
education, they should develop these demonstrate ongoing collaborative rea-

500    Physical Therapy  Volume 97  Number 5 May 2017


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

Table 1. during the encounter was video- and


Preliminary program differences audio-recorded. The primary researcher
took notes on the participant’s actions
University A University B
during the patient encounter to guide
Course First term includes course on profes- First term is entirely foundational the post-encounter interview. The du-
Sequencing sional interactions science
ration of the patient encounters ranged
Separate clinical courses for exami- Clinical courses address examination from 20 to 40 minutes, with an average
nation and intervention (in separate and intervention in same course
time of 28 minutes, similar to initial as-
terms)
sessments in many clinics.44 The stand-
Timing of Clinical Preclinical experiences are in integrat- Preclinical experiences are 2-week
ardized patient did not provide any
Experiences ed onsite clinic experiences offsite
feedback to the participant.
Readings on expert practice included

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


Clinical experiences concentrated Clinical experiences interspersed over Immediately following the patient en-
during third year (Terminal second and third years (Integrated
counter, the primary researcher in-
­experiences) experiences)
terviewed the participant regarding
Students have had 6 weeks of clinical Students have had two 2-week ­reasoning processes during the patient
experience ­preclinical experiences and
12–16 weeks of clinical experience
encounter (see eAppendix 2, available
at academic.oup.com/ptj, for interview
guide and guide development). Review
of the patient session video was used
Participants and Contexts lected from each program to participate to prompt discussion of participants’
Students were recruited from 2 pro- in this study. thought processes underlying actions
fessional physical therapist education taken during the encounter. All inter-
programs. These education programs Participants views were audio-recorded and tran-
were selected based on differences in The participants at both programs had scribed verbatim.
their overall program structures. Both similar demographics. No participant
programs use traditional curricula42 had prior experience as a physical Analyses
but d ­iffer in the sequencing of their therapist assistant or athletic trainer.
­ Transcripts from the patient encounter
courses, the types of preclinical expe- All participants had prior experiences were annotated to indicate the partici-
riences included, and the scheduling of as physical therapy aides and/or vol- pant’s and the patient’s actions along-
students’ full-time clinical experiences. unteers. Most also had personal experi- side the verbal exchange. The first stage
University A uses primarily terminal ence as a patient in physical therapy or of coding used structural coding45 to
clinical experiences, while University with a family member as a patient. Av- identify the participant’s actions dur-
B uses integrated clinical experienc- erage age at University A (mean age, 27 ing the patient encounter, hypotheses
es. At the time of this study, students years) was slightly higher than at Uni- formed, and interventions selected
from University A had participated in versity B (mean age, 25.25 years). Par- based on the elements of physical ther-
6 weeks of full-time off-campus clinical ticipants expressed interest in a variety apist examination.46 Each participant’s
affiliations and one semester part-time of physical therapist practice settings, statements of diagnostic ideas, con-
at an onsite clinic, whereas students at but the spectrum of practice areas was tributing factors, and judgments were
University B had participated in 16–20 evident across both programs.39,40 The coded as hypotheses.2,24 Each statement
weeks of full-time, off-site clinical expe- demographic information of the partici- coded as a hypothesis could be coded
riences. Table 1 summarizes the prelim- pants is summarized in eTable 2 (availa- in 2 categories—for example, a state-
inary differences and eTable 1 (availa- ble at academic.oup.com/ptj). ment identifying muscular weakness
ble at academic.oup.com/ptj) presents
as a cause of the patient’s injury could
the entire curriculum at each program, Data Collection be coded as both an impairment and
highlighting the different timing of clin- Participants completed the standard- a contributing factor. Hypotheses repre-
ical experiences. ized patient (“the patient”) encounter,43 sent the clinician’s synthesis and inter-
which entailed a physical therapist pretation of clinical data.24 Hypotheses
Participant Selection ­examination, assessment, and interven- and clinical reasoning strategies repre-
To best identify differences due to the tion. Prior to meeting the patient, each sent the clinician’s knowledge structure
influences of program structure rather participant was presented with instruc- and organization during the patient en-
than clinical experiences, students were tions and given the patient’s referral counter.24 Physical therapist clinicians
selected from the final term of their ­information (see eAppendix 1, available generate hypotheses related to diagno-
second year in the 3-year doctoral pro- at academic.oup.com/ptj). If the par- sis and management.47 The categoriza-
grams. A random sample of 4 student ticipant had not completed the assess- tion of the hypotheses generated using
volunteers representative of gender dis- ment in 35 minutes, the participant was the coding scheme described by Jones
tribution within each program was se- ­instructed to proceed to the treatment et al24 can be used to relate the par-
phase. Participant–patient interaction ticular clinical reasoning and decision

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    501


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

making to the broader International the use of multiple codes was achieved. r­eflection-on-action during their work
Classification of Functioning, Disabil- The higher level of agreement on the with the standardized p
­ atient.
ity and Health (ICF) framework. Cod- reasoning strategies (see below) that
ing categories for the hypotheses were built on the identification of the hypoth- Examination Process
derived from Jones et al’s24 hypothe- eses demonstrates the overall level of During the standardized patient en-
sis categories, with additional codes agreement in the analysis. Further, each counter, students demonstrated many
emergent from the data (see eTable 3, participant confirmed his or her actions similarities across programs. Consist-
available at ­academic.oup.com/ptj, for during the post-encounter interview. ent with elements of the examination
­definitions of Hypothesis codes). Partic- process,46 all students began the pa-
ipant’s selected treatment interventions The second stage of coding examined tient encounter with an interview and
were categorized based on the physical the relationship of the hypotheses transitioned to examination/tests and
therapist interventions described in the participants formed, examination data measures aimed at identifying the pa-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


APTA Guide to Physical Therapist Prac- collected, and actions taken to identify tient’s pathology and biomechanical or
tice46 and the dimensions of the ICF.48 their reasoning strategies. Reasoning structural links to the pathology. These
strategies represent the range of clin- biomechanically focused examination
­
All stages of analysis, including the pre- ical decisions and actions that physical tests included assessing posture and
liminary coding frames, were informed therapists make across practice fields.28 active range of motion of the spine,
by the existing literature in the field.49 The participants’ reasoning strategies palpating the painful region, and con-
Within each coding category, addition- were coded based on the strategies de- ducting special tests aimed at identi-
al emergent codes were added using fined by Edwards et al.28 (See eTable fying affected tissues. All participants
an iterative process during the initial 4, available at academic.oup.com/ptj, sought information about the patient’s
data coding. As this work was part of for strategy code definitions.) Again, a description of chief complaint, goals
a doctoral dissertation, the original and random sample of the data was cod- for therapy, and details of the patient’s
revised coding frames were reviewed ed by the primary researcher and sec- pain. Most participants (3 of 4 at each
by the primary researcher’s disserta- ond coder trained on the coding sys- program) elicited information about
tion committee prior to final coding of tem, achieving 90% agreement (kappa the patient’s employment, recreational
the data. Further, subsets of the tran- 0.88). ­During the retrospective think- interests, and past and current medical
scripts were coded by secondary cod- aloud, each participant’s explanations history.
ers (trained research assistants) to es- were analyzed for instances of reflec-
tablish the reliability of the coding.50 tion-in-action and reflection-on-ac- The students from the 2 programs dif-
Finally, the final coding of the data was tion.51,52 Also, any reasoning errors the fered in how they responded to the pa-
reviewed by the committee to establish participant made were classified based tient’s disclosure of her type II diabetes.
consensus on the application of the on the nature of erroneous conclusions At University A, participants inquired if
codes. The primary researcher main- drawn.53 In this second stage, the rela- she took medication and then asked no
tained a log of the coding and analysis tionship between the treatment inter- further questions when they learned that
process (including initial impressions ventions to examination data collect- she did not. At University B, upon learn-
from the data collection sessions) to ed and the participant’s stated overall ing that the patient did not take medi-
document the evolution of the final goals for the patient was also analyzed. cation for the diabetes, the participants
analysis. asked further follow-up questions re-
Results garding her management of the diabetes.
To enhance the credibility and con- The following section describes the
sistency of these findings, a random students’ clinical actions and reasoning Hypotheses
subsample of the data was coded by a processes during the patient encoun- Students formed hypotheses about the
second coder trained on the coding sys- ter. The section begins by describing patient’s condition throughout their ex-
tem. The primary investigator and the their actions during the examination amination process. The hypotheses the
second coder achieved 97% agreement process and the types of hypotheses participants formed focused primari-
(kappa 0.964) for coding of clinical ac- they generated. The relationships be- ly on identifying the patient’s ­affected
tions and 72% agreement (kappa 0.69) tween the students’ examination pro- body structure. Figure 1 displays the
for coding of hypotheses. Discrepancies cesses and hypotheses generated are hypotheses participants named most
between coders when coding hypothe- presented in terms of their reasoning frequently, and eTable 5 (available
ses occurred due to the use of co-occur- strategies, reasoning patterns, and rea- at ­ academic.oup.com/ptj) provides
rences of the codes. Almost all of the soning e ­rrors. The section continues ­example quotations. For example, Han-
discrepancies arose when one coder with explanation of the relationship be- nah (from University B) hypothesized
had applied only one code (usually a tween the students’ reasoning process- about the relationship of the patient’s
“contributing factors” code) and the es and the goals and interventions they tight hamstrings to her back pain: “She
other coder had applied 2 (the “con- selected for the p ­atient. Finally, this was really tight. . . . For somebody
tributing factors” and the type of factor, section presents evidence of the par- that tight, anytime you bend would be
such as “impairment”). Following dis- ticipants’ use of r­eflection-in-action and strenuous if you don’t have that give
cussion between coders, consensus on

502    Physical Therapy  Volume 97  Number 5 May 2017


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

terns that contribute to and are affected


by the pathology. Students also frequent-
ly exhibited diagnosis of movement im-
pairments, reasoning about procedures
(identifying possible interventions and
strategies for implementing the interven-
tions), and diagnosis of c­ausal factors
(see Figure 2 and e ­ Table 6, available at
academic.oup.com/ptj, for examples).
Two participants (Lisa from University B
and Bethany from University A) demon-
strated greater reasoning focused on

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


identifying the patient’s personal needs
and impact of the pathology. Bethany ex-
plained her reasoning for asking the pa-
tient about her goals for therapy: “Just to
know where she is heading to and make
sure we’re on the same page. Obviously
I want her to get better, I want [her] to
Figure 1. not have pain. I want her to know there
Most common hypotheses generated by participants. are a million things we want patients to
do, but we can only prioritize so much.”

through your hips.” Three students ously been identified. For ­ example, Overall Reasoning Patterns
demonstrated a pattern of generating
­ following negative findings on neu- The organization of the students’ rea-
hypotheses ­ focused on understanding rological testing, Kelly stated: “Proba- soning strategies determined their over-
the patient’s behavioral characteristics bly can rule out nerve at that point all reasoning patterns. Four primary
in addition to identifying the pathology. that’s causing her pain.” This statement patterns of reasoning emerged: follow-
For example, Lisa (from University B) was coded as “ruling out” and “struc- ing protocol, the hypothetico-deduc-
hypothesized about the patient’s will- ture.” For example, Hannah explained tive process, reasoning about pain, and
ingness to move following observation her testing of the patient’s hamstring analysis of patient behavioral patterns.
of a forward bend: “So I wanted to see length: “She had a lot of tightness in Figure 3 presents the overall distribu-
how willing she was to move, for one her hamstring. She couldn’t do [forward tion of reasoning patterns.
thing. . . . Very cautious with bending flexion] with her knees straight, so I’m
forward.” The pattern of identifying be- thinking okay well, you’re supposed to Protocol.  Six of the 8 participants
havioral characteristics was unique to be moving your hips but you’re getting initiated their patient encounter by
these students (from University B) and a lot of from your back so that could creating an ­examination form based on
not present universally in the partici- be contributing to some of your pain.” their memory of forms they had used
pants. Participants, however, rarely dis- Hannah’s statement was dual coded as in classes or clinical experiences. Mason
cussed the impact of the pathology on “impairment” and “contributing factor.” (from University A) explained the notes
the patient’s life (participation) or the Students also frequently linked a struc- he had written p ­rior to meeting the
patient’s perspective on her condition. ture (such as a specific muscle) to a patient:
pathological process (such as a strain).
Statements coded as hypotheses could This pattern of linking an anatomical I was jotting things down because
be coded in 2 categories (code co-oc- structure to a pathological process (for those little notes, they make sense
currences) if the statement was repre- example, identifying a paraspinal mus- to me, and that’s what I would
sentative of 2 categories. The hypothesis cle strain) was also evident in the stu- use to go back to write my ini-
code co-occurrences further illustrate dents’ final assessments of the patient. tial evaluation to document. I can
the elements of diagnosis in the stu- go back and sort of like when we
dents’ problem-solving processes. Ta- Reasoning Strategies were taught to go through a typi-
ble 2 summarizes the most common The students’ reasoning strategies were cal evaluation exam, an eval, and
co-occurrences. Each count in Table 2 identified based on the relationships we needed to hit these points, so
indicates an occurrence of a statement between their examination data collec- I’m just kind of making a writ-
that was coded in both identified cat- tion and hypothesis generation. The ten note as to what the points
egories. Following from their focus on most common reasoning strategy28 was are for documentation purposes,
identifying the affected structure, the Diagnostic Reasoning, demonstrating
­ but also if I go back, say I do my
most common co-occurrence involved a focus on diagnosis of the primary exam, and I realized that I forgot
ruling out a structure that had previ- pathology as well as movement pat-
­ to ask her something, I could
look over there to see if I wrote it

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    503


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

Table 2.
Code co-occurrences of hypothesis codes
Contributing Activity Ability/ Impairment Pathology/ Medical
Ruling Out Structure
Factors Restriction (body function) Diagnosis
Ruling Out 3 1 10 9 22
Contributing
3 9 18 6 8
Factors
Activity Ability/
1 9 1
Restriction
Impairment
10 18 6 7
(body function)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


Pathology/ Medical
9 6 6 16
Diagnosis
Structure 22 8 1 7 16

down—or if I did forget to ask, I This process included using the location treatment seems to be working,
can ask it next time. and description of the patient’s pain to so a couple different reasons.
develop hypotheses about the primary
A seventh participant, Sophia (from pathology. Hannah (from University B) Three of the participants from University
University B), did not create a form, demonstrated the biomedical approach B demonstrated a behavioral a­ pproach
but during the interview she referred to reasoning about pain as she to reasoning about pain in addition to
to information and structure from pri- explained her use of the ­patient’s pain the biomedical approach. These 3 stu-
or examination forms as part of what description in guiding her ­thinking: dents formed assessments about the
guided her examination process. Draw- patient’s behavioral responses to the
ing on the structure of these examina- Because different structures cause pain and the patient’s perspective on
tion forms helped participants organize different types of pain, I would the pain, in addition to their biomedical
their examination process and make like to know which structure is analysis of the location and description
sure they addressed the information most likely causing her pain and of her pain. Lisa (from University B)
they had learned was important during her describing what it feels like interpreted the patient’s ratings of the
a patient evaluation. Most participants can help differentiate. . . . Achy, pain as an indication of how the patient
expressed that they were afraid they I thought it could be muscle or reacts and perceives her injury.
might forget to elicit important infor- joint but then the sharp made me
mation from the patient if they did not think, okay, there might be some So the visual-analog scale obvi-
write themselves the examination sheet involvement with the joint. May- ously is very subjective, it’s hard
as a reminder. be a fracture or even just nerve to compare one person to anoth-
involvement if it’s like any other er but really for me it just gives
Hypothetico-Deductive Process.  All symptoms associated with it, so me a good idea of how this per-
but one participant demonstrated use I wanted to ask more about that. son reacts to pain. What their
of the established reasoning pattern, the idea of pain is. So at rest she gave
hypothetico-deductive process, through Further, 6 participants used the patient’s it a 1 out of 10 and at best a 1 out
their identification of multiple primary ratings of her pain to determine the lev- of 10. That means it is bothering
hypotheses with follow-up testing to el of severity of the injury. Finally, 5 par- her all the time, which is good
rule in or out selected hypotheses. 5,7 ticipants also used the patient’s ratings to know, which is still kind of in
The students used this process not of her pain to set goals for treatment. that inflammatory phase, but it’s a
only to identify the patient’s primary Mason (from University A) explained pretty low level, not too bad and
pathology (medical diagnosis) but also that the patient’s pain ratings could then it’s getting to a 7 or 8 out of
in a physical therapy–specific pattern help him determine if his treatment had 10 at the end of the day and that’s
of identifying the impairments that been effective: a big jump and I am a little bit
contributed to the patient’s pathology. more inclined to believe her.
I kind of have to have a range of
Reasoning About Pain.  Participants a pain scale and pain is some- These different approaches to reason-
demonstrated 2 distinct patterns of thing I can document over time, ing about pain demonstrate that even
reasoning about pain that have been like patients, if I see that her pain though all the participants collected
identified in the literature. First, all is going down over time, that similar data from the patient, their rea-
participants demonstrated a biomedical is another objective measure I sons for collecting those data and their
approach to reasoning about pain.54 could use to be like: alright, the interpretations differ.

504    Physical Therapy  Volume 97  Number 5 May 2017


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

manage her diabetes as evidence that


she would be likely to follow through
on a home exercise program.

So that made me want to espe-


cially ask what type of exercise
is she doing. But she’s going to
make those kinds of changes in
her life that probably, her com-
pliance is going to be a little bit
better than someone who is not
mindful of exercise or their diet.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


Reasoning Errors
The participants in this study demon-
strated 2 primary patterns of reasoning
errors during their encounter with the
patient: failing to generate a key hy-
pothesis and hanging on to a hypoth-
Figure 2. esis in the face of conflicting findings.
Reasoning strategies used. Six participants demonstrated a failure
to generate key ideas or hypotheses in
their evaluation of both the patient’s
primary pathology and comorbidities
due to jumping prematurely to one idea
and never generating alternative hy-
potheses. This pattern is consistent with
Croskerry’s55 description of Confirma-
tion Bias and Premature Closure. As a
result, these participants failed to ap-
propriately assess the patient’s current
condition and impact of her comorbidi-
ties. Four participants (3 from University
B and 1 from University A) maintained
a hypothesis of muscle strain despite
gathering data that suggested other
reasons for the patient’s pain. Finally,
participants demonstrated different un-
derstandings of the process of making
a diagnosis of sacroiliac joint dysfunc-
tion. Participants from the 2 programs
differed on which factors they gave the
most weight to during their assessment:
Figure 3. provocation tests or pelvic alignment.
Reasoning patterns.
The participants’ discussion of their
decision-making process following the
patient encounter indicated that these
differences were representative of their
Behavioral Analysis.  Two of the 3 overall behavioral patterns that was
learning of the necessary and sufficient
participants (from University B) who not present in the other participants’
conditions for ruling in a sacroiliac joint
demonstrated a behavioral analysis work with the patient. For example,
dysfunction.
approach to reasoning about pain Lisa explained that the patient’s current
also reasoned about the patient’s approach to managing her back pain
overall behavioral responses. Their provided insight into the patient’s Negative Case Example
analysis of the patient’s immediate behavioral profile and how she would One participant demonstrated reason-
and current management of back respond to a treatment program. Sophia ing patterns that diverged from the con-
pain and management of diabetes similarly explained how she interpreted sistent use of protocol and hypotheti-
demonstrated a focus on the patient’s the patient’s use of diet and exercise to co-deductive processes evident in the
other students’ work. As a negative case

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    505


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

barely move anymore. That’s the


worst thing you could have done
for yourself.

Sophia’s attention to patient education


paralleled her attention to the patient’s
behavioral responses throughout the
encounter.

Six of the 8 participants developed goals


that followed from examination data
collected and hypotheses formed, and

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


selected interventions based on the
goals and examination data selected.
Two participants, however, demon-
strated disconnections between their
examination process and intervention
selection. Kelly (from University A),
for example, focused her examination
Figure 4. on identifying the primary patholo-
Participants’ use of reflection. gy and did not include any functional
movement or strength assessments. Her
goals, however, addressed participation,
and she described interventions focused
example,38 Bethany (from University ment. Bethany described her reason- on strengthening and functional activi-
A) relied on trial and error throughout ing for prioritizing pain management ties. The most common disconnection
her interactions with the patient. When in her treatment program. between examination data and goals/
she conducted a test that elicited the interventions was the inclusion of func-
patient’s pain, Bethany was unable to Decrease pain because pain is so tional activity goals and interventions
form any assessment from that test, as limiting. Pain limits her from do- without an assessment of those move-
she didn’t feel that she had been able to ing anything. So she says sitting ments. Four participants had the patient
conduct the test as she had learned in is better, which is good but she perform an active forward flexion range
class. She also was unable to determine works so much and for her to re- of motion (a measure of impairment)
follow-up tests to clarify the results of turn to work like, say, 8 hours, I and indicated that this constituted their
the test. Bethany further carried out nu- think pain management is a big functional movement assessment.
merous manual muscle tests for the pur- part. Her active movement and
pose of “documentation.” She stopped everything, I think she can get Reflection
three-quarters of the way through the by—I think pain is the culprit, and Students’ responses during the post-en-
examination and asked if a clinical in- I want her to rest too, but if she’s counter interview indicated their use
structor was available to assist her. not able to, then . . . I would really of both reflection-in-action and reflec-
love to see if the e-stim helps her. tion-on-action52 during the patient en-
Goals, Interventions, and counter. This use of reflection shaped
Relations to Reasoning Processes The students from University B, on the their clinical decisions through their
The relationship between participants’ other hand, prioritized patient educa- assessment of their in-the-moment de-
interventions, goals, and examination tion and self-management. Sophia ex- cision making as well as their ability to
data revealed both strengths and lim- plained her reasoning for prioritizing draw on prior experiences to inform
itations of the reasoning processes patient education. their decisions. Figure 4 depicts stu-
across participants. The PT Clinical dents’ overall use of reflection-in-action
Performance Instrument (CPI)56 (item Definitely her patient education. and reflection-on-action.
12 for Plan of Care) and the litera- So that she has follow-through
ture in clinical decision making indi- when doing the activities. Telling Reflection-In-Action.  S t u d e n t s
cate that the interventions a therapist her why this is beneficial. And demonstrated reflection in action
­selects should be guided by the exam- then also, as well with what pa- through their assessment and
ination data and evaluations.57 Differ- tient education goes, continuing questioning of their findings and
ences were seen in what the students to move instead of stopping al- decisions during the patient session.
from the 2 programs prioritized in together. I’ve seen patients who Some used reflection to reevaluate
their treatment plans. Three of the 4 hurt their back and then they conclusions they had drawn from prior
participants from University A placed stop moving and it’s five months tests or to consider tests they needed
the highest priority on pain manage- down the road and they can

506    Physical Therapy  Volume 97  Number 5 May 2017


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

to revisit. Hannah (from University B) Experience in novice and experienced lems, suggesting different approaches to
explained: clinicians. The students demonstrated framing the clinical problems.27
reflection on specific action as they
I wanted to see if maybe I missed reevaluated decisions they had made Development of Physical
something at the beginning. And during the immediate patient session. Therapy-Specific Reasoning
based on the way that she was The students from University B who had All of the students demonstrated 2 of
bending, I wanted to see if she more clinical experience demonstrated 3 key characteristics of established
had any anterior tilt because be- greater use of reflection on professional diagnostic patterns in physical thera-
­
fore I was just kind of looking at if experience as they drew on specific py: a focus on movement and the in-
things are even between the sides prior examples of patient experiences to tegration of a biomechanical analysis
but now I wanted to see if, okay, guide their decision making throughout of factors contributing to injury.24 The
if something is contributing to the the interview and examination and to students demonstrated less explicit at-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


way she’s moving that’s limiting it. inform their selection of interventions. tention, however, to the impact of the
patient’s injury on her level of participa-
Other participants actively debated the Overall, students’ use of reflection ena- tion and quality of life, elements of the
merits of following clinical wisdom bled them to draw on prior patient ex- evaluation process highlighted in the
over what they had read in research as periences and adapt their examination PT CPI (item 10).56
they made decisions during their work process to the specific current patient.
with the patient. Lisa (from University Students’ use of reflection-on-action Seven participants demonstrated a fo-
B) explained: demonstrated their learning from prior cus on movement analysis through
experiences (reflection on professional their examination process, hypotheses,
The fall, I especially wanted to experiences) and their potential to learn reasoning strategies, and interventions.
see, sometimes with a fall onto from their experiences with the current The students’ attention to movement
one side or the other you can patient (reflection on specific action). is likely influenced by the coursework
cause a little bit of a jarring with in biomechanics, exercise science,
that SI [sacroiliac], and her pain, Discussion and motor control in both programs
the way she was pointing to her This study has described students’ clin- (see eTable 1). The students’ atten-
pain, I wanted to see if there was ical decisions and reasoning processes tion to movement parallels the focus
any malalignment there. I know during their encounter with a standard- on movement in the reasoning of ex-
that the research is all over the ized patient. These analyses contribute pert and novice physical therapists.15,18
place with SI stuff, but I’ve seen to our understanding of the develop- The students, however, demonstrated
enough patients feel better after mental patterns in physical therapist stu- 2 different approaches to their reason-
you do a mobilization or an MET dents’ clinical reasoning. The hypotheses ing about movement, suggesting that
[muscle energy technique] and the students developed represent their some participants held different foci
I think it’s worth looking at in organization of knowledge specific to for their assessment processes. Four
terms of pain relief. their practice.24 The types of reasoning students focused exclusively on move-
strategies the students engaged further ment at the impairment level, such as
The participants who demonstrated re- represent their development of a PT’s identifying limitations in a forward
flection-in-action demonstrated a great- specific scope of practice.28 The analysis bend or hip abduction strength. The
er ability to adapt their examination of the students’ hypotheses, reasoning students’ focus on impairments over
and evaluation process to the unfolding strategies, and reasoning patterns has functional assessments, however, sug-
findings. Peter (from University B) and provided insights into their foci during gests gaps in their understanding of the
Mason and Kelly (from University A) a patient encounter. All of the students importance of functional movement in
demonstrated the least reflection-in-ac- were at the same stage of their profes- physical therapy assessment and inter-
tion and demonstrated the most “line- sional education, yet they demonstrated vention.59,60 Three students, however,
ar” rule-driven approach to reasoning, qualitatively different foci in their clini- demonstrated greater attention to the
which is typically evident in novices.31,58 cal reasoning and decision making. The patient’s movement patterns and be-
following section discusses the relation- haviors. These 3 students’ attention to
Reflection-On-Action.  Students ship of the students’ hypothesis genera- movement patterns suggests progress
demonstrated reflection-on-action as tion and reasoning processes to their de- toward the development of movement
they reassessed their immediate actions velopment of physical therapy–specific scripts identified in expert therapists.15
during the patient assessment and drew reasoning. First, the students’ reasoning The variability in the students’ perspec-
on prior experiences with patients processes suggest their progress toward tive on movement (biomechanical or
from classes or clinical affiliations. physical therapy–specific reasoning as behavioral) within and across programs
These patterns parallel Wainwright et well as limitations in their development. suggests that factors beyond the explic-
al’s51 findings of Reflection on Specific Second, the students demonstrated qual- it curriculum may contribute to their
Action and Reflection on Professional itatively different approaches to identi- approaches to reasoning about move-
fying and addressing the patient’s prob- ment. The absence of an organized

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    507


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

reasoning process in the negative case novice ­physical therapists.30,31 A higher explained that measured i­ncreases in
exemplar illustrates the variability in reliance on protocols was noted in first- range of motion would be his primary
development of c­ linical reasoning pro- year DPT students in a prior study.53 indication that he had been effective in
cess evident in professional ­education. The students in this current study used treatment. “Did she get better post-test?
protocols to organize their initial rea- So, if I’m doing an intervention wheth-
The participant’s focus on developing soning but demonstrated flexibility to er it’s to gain range of motion, post-as-
hypotheses identifying affected ana- diverge from the protocol as the case sessment would be my best gauge.”
tomical structures and movement im- unfolded. The students’ use of protocols The students’ attention to movement
pairments contributing to the patient’s may help the students in developing the and impairments as contributing factors
current condition (as demonstrated in routines necessary for a well-organized suggests that the students are develop-
the hypothesis counts in Figure 1) in- clinical reasoning process.2 The two ing the analytical or technical aspects
dicates development toward a physi- patterns of reasoning about pain (bio- of the ­physical therapist ­diagnostic pro-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


cal therapy-specific diagnostic process medical and behavioral) demonstrated cess, yet giving limited attention to the
of identifying movement factors that by the students in this study are also psychosocial components of the pro-
contribute to the injury.11,61-64 First-year consistent with patterns of reasoning cess. This limited attention suggests that
DPT students in a prior study devel- about pain by musculoskeletal physical these students may have only a limited
oped hypotheses almost exclusive- therapists.54 The limited number of stu- understanding of patient-centered care.
ly focused on identifying anatomical dents who demonstrated a consistent Further research should investigate the
structures, influenced by their recent pattern of reasoning about the patient’s relationships between students’ under-
basic sciences courses.53 The trend of behavioral responses and psychoso- standings of patient-centered care and
identifying both anatomical structures cial experiences (as noted in Figure 3) their approaches to the patient encoun-
and movement impairments in the sec- points to a limitation in the students’ ter.
ond-year students in the current study development of attention to the pa-
demonstrates progress toward phys- tient’s experience of the process.65,66 Different Approaches to the
ical therapy–specific clinical reason- Patient Encounter
ing.24 The students’ continued attention The participants demonstrated less at- The differences observed in students’
to identifying anatomical structures tention to the impact of the patient’s in- approaches to the patient encounter il-
may be influenced by their academic jury on her life function. Physical ther- lustrate Schon’s theory that real world
coursework or their clinical instructors’ apists must address the consequences problem solving involves first framing
approaches to clinical reasoning. Fur- of the patient’s disease process in ad- the problem, then solving it.52 The 2
ther, the participant’s identification of dition to the pathology itself,11 and this primary approaches to the patient en-
movement impairments as contributing process includes understanding how counter were the biomedical approach
­factors (as demonstrated by the co-oc- the effects on physical function impact and the behavioral approach. Students
currences of impairments and contrib- a patient’s ability to carry out his/her demonstrating the biomedical approach
uting factors noted in Table 2) enabled life roles. Only 3 students, however, focused their examination and hypoth-
them to develop interventions based on developed multiple hypotheses about esis development around identifying
the hypotheses they had formed.61 the impact of the pathology on the the patient’s primary pathology, and
patient’s life and the impact of the pa- treatment plans focused on the biome-
The students’ reasoning strategies and tient’s personal characteristics on her chanical and impairment levels. Three
reasoning patterns further demon- function and prognosis (demonstrated students (from University B) demon-
strate their progression toward physical by the low percentage of hypotheses strated a greater focus on identifying
­therapy–specific reasoning. Two of the developed about patient characteristics patient behavioral characteristics that
most common reasoning strategies em- in Figure 1). Further, as demonstrated impact movement and treatment, and
ployed were the diagnosis of movement in Figure 3, only 2 students engaged in included patient education and activi-
impairments and causal factors (see patterns of reasoning concerning the ty modification in their treatment plans
­Figure 2): key elements of the ­physical patient’s behavioral presentation. The in addition to a biomedical analysis.
therapist responsibility to address remaining students focused their ex- These students’ educational approach
movement patterns (pathokinesiology) amination, assessment, and treatment to patient treatment suggests that they
that contribute to a patient’s health on identifying the patient’s health con- may be developing some of the char-
condition.64 The student quotations in dition (pathology) and biomechanical acteristics of more expert practitioners,
eTable 6 provide examples of the stu- problems. This biomedical focus of even at this early stage of their educa-
dents’ movement-specific analysis of the encounter is evident in the most tion.31,32 Programmatic differences may
the patient’s impairments and causal common reasoning strategies (Diag- contribute to the differences observed
factors. The most common patterns in nosis of Primary Pathology and Diag- in how they learned to interpret and act
the students’ overall reasoning (use of nosis of Movement Impairments in on the data they collected, yet further
protocols and the hypothetico-deduc- Figure 2) as well as 5 of the students’ study is necessary to draw definitive
tive process as i­llustrated in Figure 3) use of Biomedical Reasoning about conclusions.
are also consistent with the work of Pain ­(Figure  3). For ­example, Mason

508    Physical Therapy  Volume 97  Number 5 May 2017


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

Although all participants collected simi- clinical reasoning has also been noted Further analysis alongside established
lar data during their examinations, their in the nursing education literature.67 measures such as the Health Scienc-
interpretation and use of those data While these comments indicate limita- es Reasoning Test70 of Study Processes
suggest different approaches to framing tions in this student’s own capacities for Questionnaire71 (assessing deep ver-
the clinical problem. For example, all clinical reasoning, they also indicate her sus superficial approaches to learning)
students asked the patient to rate her awareness of her limitations. Fostering could enhance our understanding of the
levels of pain. Six students used this in- a deep approach to learning may sup- cognitive and dispositional factors that
formation as an indication of the sever- port students’ development of reflective underlie students’ development of clini-
ity of the patient’s injury and a measure capacities, as a deep approach to learn- cal reasoning.72
for progress. Two students, on the oth- ing requires ongoing self-evaluation
er hand, used the pain ratings to gain to achieve a true understanding of the Finally, the qualitatively different
insight into the patient’s ­ perceptions ­material.68 This deep approach to learn- app­
­ roaches to clinical reasoning

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


of and behavioral responses to the in- ing is necessary overall for clinical rea- demons­ trated by the students in this
jury as demonstrated through their soning, as surface approaches to learn- study suggest that individual-level
reasoning patterns concerning pain ing are not compatible with the skills factors may also underlie these differ-
(demonstrated in Figure 3). The stu- needed for clinical reasoning.69 ences.73 ­ Further examination of the
dents’ variations in approach to the individual students’ backgrounds (prior
patient problem suggest that they may Limitations and Future educational, personal, and professional
be operating from different models of experiences) and their perspectives on
practice (most from a biomedical model Directions physical therapist practice may shed
and 2 from a biopsychosocial model).27 This study has contributed to our under-
more light on individual-level factors
Further research should investigate stu- standing of physical therapist students’
that influence students’ engagement
dents’ conceptualizations of practice in development of clinical reasoning skills
in reflection and patient-centered care.
relation to their clinical reasoning and by examining the varieties of patterns
The exploration of students’ different
decision making. of clinical reasoning in s­tudents from
approaches to clinical reasoning could
2 different professional education pro-
also be expanded by replication of this
grams. This study has several limita-
Managing Uncertainty with study but using multiple standardized
tions that should be addressed in future
Reflection patient encounters representing dif-
research. First, program-level factors
reflection-in-action and reflection-on-­ that influence the students’ reasoning ferent patient cases within and across
action influenced the students’ process- processes could be better examined in physical therapy disciplines. The use
es through the evaluation process. Each studies repeating the methods of this of multiple patient cases could provide
participant demonstrated use of reflec- study that include more participants greater confirmation of student-specif-
tion-on-action at least once during the and more programs. The data from ic patterns of reasoning versus context
patient encounter, as demonstrated in this current study suggest that students specificity in response to the patient
Figure  4. This use of reflection-on-ac- from University B (with greater clinical case. As with any qualitative research,
tion has been noted in prior studies of experience at the time of the study) en- there are inherent limitations in the in-
students and novices.51,67 The use of gaged in more reflection-in-action and terpretive nature of qualitative coding.74
reflection-in-action was observed more patient education; however, more in- Considering the limitations of the cod-
frequently in some participants than depth analyses of the programs’ curric- ing process, multiple methods to en-
previously reported in the literature.51,67 ula, culture, and andragogy are neces- sure trustworthiness were employed in
Six of the students demonstrated at least sary to draw any definitive conclusions this study, including use of established
one occurrence of reflection-in-action. about program-level factors. Future methods and coding frames,49,50 trian-
Overall, students’ reflection-in-action studies should include greater breadth gulation,38,75,76 and reliability coding.50
took 2 primary forms. Four students of program-related data sources, such
used reflection to reassess their actions, as a review of course syllabi, observa- This study has begun the process of
reevaluate (or reexamine) certain tests tion of classes, and interviews with fac- describing the variations in develop-
or examinations, and shift course dur- ulty and administration. ment of clinical reasoning in physical
ing the examination. These students therapist students. The findings from
demonstrated greater flexibility in their this study indicate that students are
Second, this study did not relate the
progress through the case and were engaging in qualitatively different ap-
students’ clinical reasoning processes
able to adapt their tests and measures to proaches to clinical problem framing
to their broader academic or cognitive
the unfolding situation. 51,52 One student and problem solving through the types
abilities. Future studies could increase
who demonstrated the least knowledge of hypotheses they develop and reason-
our understanding of the relationship
regarding the case reflected on her own ing strategies they engage. Differences
between students’ academic and cogni-
limitations and indicated desire for ex- were evident both within and between
tive abilities and dispositions through
ternal guidance from a clinical instruc- programs. While the findings from this
further analysis of the relationships
tor during the examination and the in- study are only preliminary, they suggest
between students’ patterns of clinical
terview. This use of reflection-in-action that both individual- and program-­level
reasoning and their academic and clin-
to question one’s confidence during factors may contribute to differences
ical performance within their program.

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    509


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

in the development of physical thera- References 17 McGinnis PQ, Hack LM, Nixon-Cave
pists’ reasoning. This preliminary exam- K, Michlovitz SL. Factors that influ-
1 Higgs J, Jones MA. Clinical decision ence the clinical decision making of
ination of students’ clinical reasoning making and multiple problem spaces. physical therapists in choosing a bal-
In: Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Chris- ance assessment approach. Phys Ther.
provides an initial step in linking the tensen N, eds. Clinical reasoning in the 2009;89(3):233–247.
theories of clinical reasoning in experi- health professions. 3rd ed. Amsterdam:
enced therapists to the developmental Elsevier; 2008:3–17. 18 May S, Greasley A, Reeve S, Withers S.
Expert therapists use specific clinical
needs of professional students. Further 2 Barrows HS, Feltovich PJ. The clin- reasoning processes in the assessment
ical reasoning process. Med Educ. and management of patients with shoul-
studies should investigate the impact of 1987;21(2):86–91. der pain: a qualitative study. Aust J Phys-
programmatic factors (such as timing of 3 Schoenfeld AH. How we think: A theory iother. 2008;54(4):261–266.
clinical experiences) on students’ devel- of goal-oriented decision making and 19 Mattingly C. What is clinical reasoning.
opment of clinical reasoning abilities. its educational applications. New York: Am J Occup Ther. 1991;45(11):979–986.
Routledge; 2010.
20 Wainwright SF, McGinnis PQ. Factors
4 Coderre S, Jenkins D, McLaughlin K.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


Author Contributions and Qualitative differences in knowledge
that influence the clinical decision-mak-
ing of rehabilitation professionals in
Acknowledgments structure are associated with diagnos- long-term care settings. J Allied Health.
tic performance in medical students. 2009;38(3):143–151.
Concept/idea/research design: S. Gilliland, Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract.
2009;14(5):677–684. 21 Jensen GM. Learning what matters
S.F. Wainwright most. 2011 McMillan Lecture. Phys Ther.
Writing: S. Gilliland, S.F. Wainwright 5 Norman G. Research in clinical reason- 2011;91(11):1674–1689.
ing: Past history and current trends. Med
Data collection: S. Gilliland Educ. 2005;39(4):418–427. 22 Holdar U, Wallin L, Heiwe S. Why do we
Data analysis: S. Gilliland do as we do? Factors influencing clinical
6 Patel VL, Groen GJ. Developmental ac-
Fund procurement: S. Gilliland counts of the transition from medical
reasoning and decision-making among
The authors thank Judith Sandholtz, PhD physiotherapists in an acute setting.
student to doctor: Some problems and Physiother Res Int. 2013;18(4):220–229.
(School of Education, University of Califor- suggestions. Med Educ. 1991;25(6):527–
nia, Irvine), for her advising throughout the 535. 23 Rothstein JM, Echternach JL. Hypothe-
sis-oriented algorithm for clinicians. A
study design and analysis process. 7 Elstein AS, Shulman LS, Sprafka SA. method for evaluation and treatment
Medical Problem Solving: An Analysis planning. Phys Ther. 1986;66(9):1388–
Ethics Approval of Clinical Reasoning. Cambridge, MA: 1394.
Harvard University Press; 1978.
This study was approved through the inter- 24 Jones MA, Jensen GM, Edwards I. Clin-
8 Bordage G, Grant J, Marsden P. Quan- ical reasoning in physiotherapy. In:
nal review boards at University of Califor- titative assessment of diagnostic ability. Higgs J, Jones MA, Loftus S, Christensen
nia, Irvine, Chapman University, Irvine, and Med Educ. 1990;24(5):413–425. N, eds. Clinical reasoning in the health
California State University, Long Beach. All 9 Patel VL, Groen GJ. Knowledge based professions. 3rd ed. Amsterdam: Elsevi-
participants signed informed consent prior solution strategies in medical reasoning. er; 2008:245–256.
Cogn Sci. 1986;10:91–116. 25 Yeung E, Woods N, Dubrowski A, Hodg-
to participation in the study.
10 Rothstein JM, Echternach JL, Riddle DL. es B, Carnahan H. Establishing assess-
Funding Support The Hypothesis-Oriented Algorithm for ment criteria for clinical reasoning in
Clinicians II (HOAC II): A guide for patient orthopedic manual physical therapy: a
This work was supported, in part, by the management. Phys Ther. 2003;83(5):455– consensus-building study. J Man Manip
470. Ther. 2015;23(1):27–36.
2014 Adopt a Doc Scholarship of the
­Education Section of the American Physical 11 Jette AM. Diagnosis and classification by 26 Schon DA. Educating the reflective prac-
physical therapists: A special communi- titioner. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass;
Therapy Association and by a 2014–2015 cation. Phys Ther. 1989;69(11):967–969. 1987.
Promotion of Doctoral Studies (PODS) II
12 Christensen N, Black L, Jensen GM. 27 Trede F, Higgs J. Clinical reasoning and
scholarship through the Foundation for Physiotherapy clinical placements and models of practice. In: Higgs J, Jones
Physical Therapy. learning to reason. In: Higgs J, Sheehan MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, eds. Clini-
D, Baldly Currens J, Let’s W, Jensen GM, cal reasoning in the health professions.
Disclosures eds. Realising exemplary practice-based 3rd ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2008:31–
education. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers; 41.
The authors declare that they have no con- 2013:135–142. 28 Edwards I, Jones M, Carr J, Braun-
flicts of interest. At the time of this study, 13 Jones MA. Clinical reasoning in manu- ack-Mayer A, Jensen GM. Clinical rea-
one author (S.G.) was adjunct faculty at al therapy. Phys Ther. 1992;72(12):875– soning strategies in physical therapy.
one of the programs (Chapman University); 884. Phys Ther. 2004;84(4):312–330; discus-
sion 331-315.
however, the students in this study were not 14 Jensen GM, Gwyer J, Shepard KF, Hack
enrolled in her class at the time of this study, LM. Expert practice in physical therapy. 29 Edwards I, Jones M, Higgs J, Trede F,
Phys Ther. 2000;80(1):28–43; discussion Jensen GM. What is collaborative rea-
and she has not taught them in any course 44-52. soning? Adv Physiother. 2004;6:70–83.
work directly related to the task involved in
15 Embrey DG, Guthrie MR, White OR, 30 Jensen GM, Shepard KF, Hack LM. The
the study. Dietz J. Clinical decision making by novice versus the experienced clinician:
experienced and inexperienced pedi- insights into the work of the physical
Dr Gilliland is a certified strength and condi- atric physical therapists for children therapist. Phys Ther. 1990;70(5):314–
tioning specialist. This work served as partial with diplegic cerebral palsy. Phys Ther. 323.
fulfillment of Dr Gilliland’s doctoral disserta- 1996;76(1):20–33. 31 Jensen GM, Shepard KF, Gwyer J, Hack
tion work. 16 Riolo L. Skill differences in novice LM. Attribute dimensions that distin-
and expert clinicians in neurologic guish master and novice physical ther-
DOI: 10.1093/ptj/pzx028 physical therapy. Neurology Report. apy clinicians in orthopedic settings.
1996;20(1):60–63. Phys Ther. 1992;72(10):711–722.

510    Physical Therapy  Volume 97  Number 5 May 2017


Clinical Reasoning in Physical Therapist Students

32 Resnik L, Jensen GM. Using clinical out- 47 Doody C, McAteer M. Clinical reasoning 63 Delitto A, Snyder-Mackler L. The di-
comes to explore the theory of expert of expert and novice physiotherapists in agnostic process: examples in ortho-
practice in physical therapy. Phys Ther. an outpatient orthopaedic setting. Phys- pedic physical therapy. Phys Ther.
2003;83(12):1090–1106. iotherapy. 2002;88(5):258–268. 1995;75(3):203–211.
33 Norman G. Research in medical educa- 48 World Health Organization. Towards a 64 Sahrmann SA. The human movement
tion: Three decades of progress. Bmj. common language for functioning, dis- system: our professional identity. Phys
2002;324(7353):1560–1562. ability and health. 2002; http://www. Ther. 2014;94(7):1034–1042.
who.int/classifications/icf/training/icf-
34 Furze J, Gale JR, Black L, Cochran TM, 65 Ferreira PH, Ferreira ML, Maher CG,
beginnersguide.pdf Accessed November
Jensen GM. Clinical reasoning: Devel- Refshauge KM, Latimer J, Adams RD.
20, 2010.
opment of a grading rubric for student The therapeutic alliance between cli-
assessment. Journal of Physical Therapy 49 Mays N, Pope C. Rigour and qualitative nicians and patients predicts outcome
Education. 2015;29(3):34–45. research. BMJ. 1995;311(6997):109–112. in chronic low back pain. Phys Ther.
35 Black LL, Christensen N, Furze J, Huhn 50 Shenton AK. Strategies for ensuring 2013;93(4):470–478.
K, Wainwright SF, Vendrely A. Taking trustworthiness in qualitative research 66 Fuentes J, Armijo-Olivo S, Funabashi
our pulse: Clinical reasoning in the projects. Education for Information. M, et al. Enhanced therapeutic alliance

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ptj/article/97/5/499/3089730 by guest on 26 October 2021


classroom and clinic. Paper presented 2004;22:63–75. modulates pain intensity and muscle
at: Combined Sections Meeting of the pain sensitivity in patients with chron-
51 Wainwright SF, Shepard KF, Harman
American Physical Therapy Association; ic low back pain: an experimental con-
LB, Stephens J. Novice and experienced
February, 2015; Indianapolis, IN. trolled study. Phys Ther. 2014;94(4):477–
physical therapist clinicians: A compar-
36 Brookfield S. Clinical reasoning and ge- ison of how reflection is used to inform 489.
neric thinking skills. In: Higgs J, Jones the clinical decision-making process. 67 Burbach B, Barnason S, Thompson SA.
MA, Loftus S, Christensen N, eds. Clini- Phys Ther. 2010;90(1):75–88. Using “Think Aloud” to capture clinical
cal reasoning in the health professions. reasoning during patient simulation. Int.
52 Schon DA. The reflective practitioner:
Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2008:65–75. J. Nurs. Educ. Scholarsh. 2015;12(1):1–7.
How professionals think in action. New
37 Simmons B. Clinical reasoning: Concept York: Basic Books, Inc; 1983. 68 Newble DI, Entwistle NJ. Learning
analysis. J Adv Nurs. 2010;66(5):1151– styles and approaches: implications
53 Gilliland SJ. Clinical reasoning in first-
1158. for medical education. Med Educ.
and third-year physical therapist stu-
38 Patton MQ. Qualitative reserach & dents. J Phys Ther Educ. 2014;28(3):64– 1986;20(3):162–175.
evaluation methods. 3rd ed. Thousand 80. 69 Jensen GM, Paschal KA. Habits of mind:
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc; 2002. Student transition toward virtuous prac-
54 Smart K, Doody C. The clinical reason-
39 Baxter P, Jack S. Qualitative case study ing of pain by experienced musculo- tice. J Phys Ther Educ. 2000;14(3):42–47.
methodology: Study design and imple- skeletal physiotherapists. Man Ther. 70 Insight Assessment. Health Sciences Rea-
mentation for the novice reseracher. 2007;12(1):40–49. soning Test. 2013; http://www.insigh-
Qual Rep. 2008;13(4):544–559. tassessment.com/Products/Products-
55 Croskerry P. The importance of cog-
40 Merriam SB. Qualitative research and nitive errors in diagnosis and strat- Summary/Critical-Thinking-Skills-Tests/
case study applications in education. San egies to minimize them. Acad Med. Health-Sciences-Reasoning-Test-HSRT.
Francisco: Josey-Bass Publishers; 1998. 2003;78(8):775–780. Accessed February 16, 2015.
41 Charmaz K, Belgrave LL. Qualitative in- 56 American Physical Therapy Association. 71 Biggs J. The revised two-factor Study
terviewing and grounded theory anal- Physical Therapist Clinical Performance Process Questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. Brit-
ysis. In: Gubrium JF, Holstein JA, Mar- Instrument. Alexandria, VA: American ish Journal of Educational Psychology.
vasti AB, McKinner KD, eds. The SAGE Physical Therapy Association; 2006. 2001;71:133–149.
handbook of interivew research: The 57 Riddle DL, Rothstein JM, Echternach JL. 72 Huhn K, Deutsch JE. Development
complexity of the craft. 2nd ed. Thou- and assessment of a web-based patient
Application of the HOAC II: an episode
sand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.; simulation program. J Phys Ther Educ.
of care for a patient with low back pain.
2012:347–366. 2011;25(1):5–10.
Phys Ther. 2003;83(5):471–485.
42 Commission on Accreditation in Physi- 73 Dall’alba G. Understanding medi-
58 Conger MM, Mezza I. Fostering crit-
cal Therapy Education. Evaluative cri- cal practice: different outcomes of a
ical thinking in nursing students in
teria for PT programs. Alexandria, VA: pre-medical program. Adv Health Sci
the clinical setting. Nurse educator.
American Physical Therapy Association; Educ Theory Pract. 2002;7(3):163–177.
1996;21(3):11–15.
2014.
59 Willy RW, Davis IS. The effect of a 74 Flick U. An introduction to qualitative
43 Ladyshewsky R, Baker R, Jones MA, Nel- research. London: Sage; 2009.
hip-strengthening program on mechan-
son L. Evaluating clinical performance
in physical therapy with simulated pa- ics during running and during a sin- 75 Lincoln Y, Guba E. Naturalist inquiry.
gle-leg squat. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; 1985.
tients. Journal of Physical Therapy Edu- 2011;41(9):625–632.
cation. 2000;14(1):31–37. 76 Merriam SB. Qualitative research: A
60 de Bruin ED, Murer K. Effect of addi- guide to design and implementation.
44 Roberts LC, Whittle CT, Cleland J, Wald tional functional exercises on balance in San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2009.
M. Measuring verbal communication in elderly people. Clinical rehabilitation.
initial physical therapy encounters. Phys 2007;21(2):112–121. 77 Sandholtz JH. Preservice Teachers’ con-
Ther. 2013;93(4):479–491. ceptions of effective and ineffective
61 Sahrmann SA. Diagnosis by the physical teaching practices. Teacher Education
45 Saldana JM. The coding manual for therapist–a prerequisite for treatment. Quarterly. 2011;38(3):27–47.
qualitative researchers. London: Sage A special communication. Phys Ther.
Publications Ltd; 2009. 1988;68(11):1703–1706.
46 American Physical Therapy Association, 62 Guccione AA. Physical therapy diag-
ed. Guide to Physical Therapist Practice. nosis and the relationship between
2nd ed. Alexandria, VA: American Physi- impairments and function. Phys Ther.
cal Therapy Association; 2003. 1991;71(7):499–503; discussion 503-494.

May 2017 Volume 97  Number 5  Physical Therapy    511

You might also like