When Will He Talk? An Evidence-Based Tutorial For Measuring Progress Toward Use of Spoken Words in Preverbal Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

AJSLP

Tutorial

When Will He Talk? An Evidence-Based


Tutorial for Measuring Progress Toward
Use of Spoken Words in Preverbal
Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder
Jena McDaniela and C. Melanie Schueleb

Purpose: Professionals face substantial challenges determining Conclusions: Communication samples can be used to
whether and when children with autism spectrum disorder show how a child performs within a therapeutic setting
(ASD) who are not yet using spoken words will use spoken during teaching (treatment data) and outside of the
language as their primary means of communication. This therapeutic setting (generalization probe data). Both types
tutorial provides speech-language pathologists with practical of data are critical for determining whether the child is
guidance on how to measure expressive language predictors exhibiting progress and which aspects of intervention are
for progress monitoring and making intervention decisions facilitating progress toward use of spoken words. These
for children with ASD who are preverbal. recommendations also balance the evidence for best
Method: This tutorial is a repackaging effort that seeks to practices for progress monitoring and the demands on
make the research accessible to clinicians wishing to implement clinicians’ time and effort. To encourage the measurement
evidence-based practice. of prelinguistic skills of children with ASD who are preverbal
Results: We describe intentional communication, consonant in clinical practice, we include (a) example data collection
inventory in communication acts, and responding to joint documents, (b) examples with hypothetical data and
attention as particularly valuable prelinguistic skills to measure. interpretation, and (c) guidance on communication sampling
We explain how and when to efficiently assess progress using procedures.
published assessments periodically and using brief (5-min) Supplemental Material: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.
communication samples for more frequent progress monitoring. 13557836

F
or typical language learners, the milestone of first et al., 2005). In fact, parents commonly report lack of spo-
words is expected around the first birthday (Oller ken words as an early concern prior to their child’s diagno-
et al., 1999). Typically, by 2 years of age, toddlers sis of ASD (Hess & Landa, 2012; Ozonoff et al., 2009).
have a vocabulary of several hundred words and, between During the early course of development, children with
2 and 3 years of age, children’s communication with multi- ASD vary widely in their spoken language skills (Ellis
word utterances blossoms. What is perhaps most remarkable Weismer et al., 2010; Luyster et al., 2008; Pickett et al., 2009).
is how seamless and effortless learning to talk is for most Some, albeit a small percentage, score within age expecta-
children. The same is not true for children with developmen- tions on early language measures (Ellis Weismer et al., 2010).
tal disabilities, including autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, many preschool children with ASD are not yet
For children with ASD, first words often do not appear at producing any words, and approximately 50%–70% are not
the anticipated time nor with great ease (Tager-Flusberg using spoken phrases (Ellis Weismer & Kover, 2015; Thurm
et al., 2015). Many of those children will learn to use spoken
words and phrases. However, approximately 25%–30% of
a
Life Span Institute, University of Kansas, Lawrence children with ASD are expected to remain minimally verbal
b
Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University and not use spoken language as their primary means of
Medical Center, Nashville, TN communication (Anderson et al., 2007; National Research
Correspondence to Jena McDaniel: jena.c.mcdaniel@ku.edu Council, 2001; Tager-Flusberg & Kasari, 2013; Tager-
Editor-in-Chief: Julie Barkmeier-Kraemer Flusberg et al., 2005; Wodka et al., 2013). Age alone is an
Editor: Danai Kasambira Fannin insufficient predictor for whether a child will talk. Pickett
Received July 11, 2020
Revision received September 28, 2020
Accepted October 26, 2020 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJSLP-20-00206 of publication.

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021 • Copyright © 2021 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1
Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
et al. (2009) concluded that, for children with ASD who are by explaining how to modify intervention based on assessment
not talking at age 5 years, the emergence of spoken words is data and by describing future research needs.
more likely before age 7 years than after. Yet, some children
with ASD reportedly started talking as late as 13 years of
age (Pickett et al., 2009; Williams, 1990). The Value of Measuring Prelinguistic Skills
A more robust ability to predict which children with Prelinguistic skills emerge prior to the onset of spo-
ASD will use spoken words would improve clinical deci- ken words and include the use of nonverbal means of com-
sions about prioritizing intervention goals, particularly goals munication. Prelinguistic behaviors include gestures (e.g.,
related to communication modes. Some children with ASD waving, reaching, and pointing), vocalizations (e.g., cooing
who are preverbal may benefit most from interventions with and canonical babbling), and eye gaze (e.g., looking back
a strong emphasis on spoken words. Others may benefit and forth between an object and another person and look-
from a strong emphasis on augmentative and alternative ing at something another person is pointing toward; Watt
communication (AAC). Complicating clinical decisions et al., 2006). Unlike spoken words, prelinguistic forms of
further, a child’s relative benefit from different communica- communication are not symbolic. Nonetheless, prelinguistic
tion modes may change as his or her skills change. Knowing communication is purposeful and conveys meaning. For ex-
the degree to which a child is exhibiting the necessary foun- ample, when a child reaches toward a container of bubbles
dational skills for spoken words provides critical informa- on a high shelf while looking back and forth between the
tion as to whether or not intervention strategies should be bubbles and an adult, she is likely requesting that the adult
adjusted. Examination of prelinguistic skills may signal that retrieve the bubbles for her. As another example, a child
a child is progressing toward spoken words and provide en- lifts his arms while looking at his caregiver in an apparent
couragement for continuing current intervention efforts. In request to be picked up. The child may add a vocalization,
contrast, if a child is not progressing toward spoken words if the initial gesture is ineffective. A child also may extend
as expected, the current intervention plan should be reevalu- her arm toward an adult to show a ball or other favorite
ated and adjusted as appropriate (e.g., change intervention toy in order for the adult to enjoy that item as she does. Thus,
strategies or intensity). These questions should be considered even without spoken words, children direct requests and
on an ongoing and frequent basis. Thus, frequent data col- comments about ongoing activities toward other people.
lection is needed for making clinical decisions (e.g., whether Importantly, prelinguistic skills predict expressive
the strategies, goals, approach, and/or intensity of interven- spoken language skills in children with and without ASD.
tion needs to be altered). For example, Watt et al. (2006) reported that joint attention
This tutorial applies empirical knowledge about pre- and consonant inventory in the second year of life predicted
dictors of expressive language growth in children with ASD age 3 years expressive language outcomes for children with
who are preverbal to evaluating change, including incremen- typical development. Morales et al. (2000) also found that
tal change, in clinical and educational settings. We use the responding to joint attention accounted for unique variance
term preverbal to convey that the children are using no or in later vocabulary development in children with typical de-
few words at this time and whose treatment goals include velopment. For children with ASD, a recent meta-analysis
acquisition of spoken language. A portion of these children revealed a strong association between vocalizations and
(approximately 25%–30%) are expected to remain minimally later expressive language (mean r = .50, 95% CI [0.23, 0.76];
verbal based on current evidence. Determining that a child McDaniel et al., 2018). Other prelinguistic skills, such as
is likely to remain minimally verbal requires examining their responding to joint attention (Paul et al., 2008; Thurm
prelinguistic skills and progress over time. Thus, the speech- et al., 2007; Yoder et al., 2015), intentional communication
language pathologist (SLP) does not need to know whether (Charman et al. 2005; Plumb & Wetherby, 2013; Yoder, 2006;
a child will use spoken words and phrases to implement the Yoder et al., 2015), and gestures (Luyster et al., 2008), also
empirically supported assessment approach described in this have predicted later expressive language outcomes in children
tutorial. Some children will show progress toward spoken with ASD. Describing a child’s prelinguistic skills provides
words, and intervention will be tailored to maximize spoken a rich picture of the child’s progress toward spoken words.
language gains. For other children, careful analysis of their
progress toward spoken words, even with changes to inter-
vention strategies, may lead to a reduced emphasis on spoken Particularly Valuable Prelinguistic Skills
words and increased emphasis on AAC. In both scenarios, for Predicting Expressive Language
the analysis of key prelinguistic skills when the child is not
using spoken words facilitates clinical decisions to improve Skills in Children With ASD
outcomes. Selecting prelinguistic skill predictors that yield the
In this tutorial, we describe the value of measuring most useful information increases progress monitoring effi-
prelinguistic skills and then describe three particularly valu- ciency and provides data for evidence-based, data-driven
able prelinguistic skills for predicting expressive language intervention planning. Replicated findings support the value
skills in children with ASD. We then describe how to assess of three prelinguistic skills for predicting expressive language
prelinguistic skills periodically and through frequent prog- growth in preverbal children with ASD: intentional commu-
ress monitoring to capture incremental change. We conclude nication, consonant inventory in communication acts (also

2 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
known as diversity of key consonants used in communica- Intentional communication is important to capture
tion acts), and responding to joint attention (Charman because children with ASD who frequently engage in inten-
et al., 2005; Paul et al., 2008; Plumb & Wetherby, 2013; tional communication acts to express their wants, needs,
Thurm et al., 2007; Wetherby et al., 2007; Woynaroski et al., and thoughts and to direct others’ attention show an un-
2016; Yoder et al., 2015). Measuring these skills improves derlying desire to communicate (Bruner, 1974). Caregivers
the prediction of expressive language outcomes above and may respond to their children’s prelinguistic intentional com-
beyond other tested variables. Thus, investing time in evalu- munication acts with linguistic input that supports the child’s
ating these three predictors is expected to yield more “bang understanding of spoken words, a necessary skill for expres-
for your buck” than other predictors. sive language use (McDaniel et al., 2017; Yoder, 2006). One
Yoder et al. (2015) identified these three predictors example of such linguistic input is linguistic mapping, wherein
(as well as parent linguistic responses) as improving the pre- the adult describes the child’s action and/or underlying
diction of expressive language skills even when accounting message. For example, a child points to a ball and says,
for seven other empirically supported predictors (i.e., recep- “Uh,” and then the caregiver says, “That’s a ball,” as a lin-
tive language, attention during child-directed speech, motor guistic map (Yoder & Warren, 2002). Children who frequently
imitation, nonimitative oral motor functioning, object play, exhibit intentional communication acts may elicit more fre-
cognitive impairment level, and ASD severity). They evalu- quent and/or more complex linguistic input from caregivers
ated the predictors across 16 months (beginning at a mean that supports language development (Gros-Louis et al., 2006).
age of 34.7 months). Across a longer period of time, Charman They also may engage in more interactions that facilitate
et al. (2005) found that the rate of nonverbal intentional object knowledge. Such object knowledge is one cognitive
communication acts at age 2 years predicted language skills basis for learning early words (McDaniel et al., 2017).
at age 7 years. Similar to Yoder et al. (2015), Wetherby
et al. (2007) identified consonant inventory in communi- Consonant Inventory in Communication Acts
cation acts as a particularly valuable predictor of expressive
language skills in children with ASD at age 3 years. Siller Consonant inventory in communication acts (the
and Sigman (2008) identified responding to joint attention number of different consonants used communicatively) is a
as the strongest predictor of language development in children measure of phonological complexity. Use of more conso-
with ASD. Thus, replicated findings support using inten- nants within communication acts indicates more developed
tional communication, consonant inventory in communica- phonological skills than a relatively restricted consonant in-
tion acts, and responding to joint attention for monitoring ventory. Consonant inventory in communication acts also
spoken language development in children with ASD who includes a communicative component because only conso-
are not yet talking or just beginning to talk. nants within communication acts are counted. Thus, conso-
In the following sections, we first define intentional nants produced in vocalizations that do not appear to serve a
communication, consonant inventory in communication acts, communicative function (e.g., repetitive or self-stimulating
and responding to joint attention and describe how each of behaviors) are not counted. Children who use a wide variety
these predictors connects with expressive language. We then of consonants during communication acts may be attempt-
explain the development of these skills in children with typi- ing to say words they understand prior to those words being
cal development and children with ASD in Table 1. understood by listeners (Woynaroski et al., 2016). Those
children who use a wide variety of consonants during com-
munication acts also may be exhibiting oral motor skills
Intentional Communication shared with spoken language (McDaniel et al., 2017).

Intentional communication can be defined as a child’s


use of gestures, vocalizations, gaze, and/or spoken words to Responding to Joint Attention
convey meaning to another person (Wetherby et al., 1988). Responding to joint attention requires a child to shift
Gestures used by young children who are not yet talking or his or her gaze from a current focus to the referent of the
just beginning to talk can be classified as contact gestures adult’s bid for attention (Yoder et al., 2015). Adults’ bids
(e.g., reaching, showing, and giving objects), distal gestures can include pointing, spoken words, and/or eye gaze (Bottema-
(e.g., raising arms to be picked up and pointing at some- Beutel, 2016; Yoder et al., 2015). For example, a child may
thing at a distance to request or direct another person’s atten- be looking down at a book when his father points at a bird
tion), and representational gestures (e.g., waving, nodding outside the window while saying, “Look!” The child then
and shaking head, and blowing a kiss). When a child displays lifts his head and shifts his gaze to his father and follows his
intentional communication, which includes attention to an- father’s point to look at the bird. In contrast to initiating
other individual and an object or event, these behaviors joint attention, responding to joint attention can only hap-
or groups of behaviors are referred to as communication pen if and when a communication partner provides a bid to
acts. Communication acts may include multiple forms of the child. Thus, this skill necessitates that the child is moni-
communication, such as pointing to an object while looking toring another person’s communication and that monitoring
back and forth between an object and a person or vocalizing leads to a response. When children consistently respond to
while giving an object in apparent request for help. joint attention, they may attend to and benefit more from

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 3


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Table 1. Comparison of particularly valuable prelinguistic skills in children with typical development and children with autism spectrum disorder.

Children with typical Children with autism spectrum


Prelinguistic skill development (TD) disorder (ASD)

Intentional communication • Emerges around 8–9 months (R. L. Carpenter • May use forms of communication not
et al., 1983; M. Carpenter et al., 1998) observed in children with TD (e.g., another
• Examples include an infant raising her arms to person’s hand as a tool; Lord, 1995; Stone
request being picked up or handing a caregiver et al., 1997)
an object to request help • Lower rates of intentional communication
• Use of conventional communication (e.g., pointing (Wetherby et al., 2007) and smaller inventory
and nodding) and symbolic communication of conventional gestures than children with
(e.g., spoken words and manual signs) emerges TD and children with other developmental
around 12 months (Bates et al., 1975; Wetherby & disabilities (Charman et al., 2003; Watson
Prizant, 1989) et al., 2013)
• Combine multiple forms of communication to • Rate of nonverbal communication acts for
increase effectiveness (e.g., add vocalizations to 2-year-old children with ASD predicted
pointing when mother is not attending to child’s outcomes in multiple domains (e.g., social
object of interest; Wu & Gros-Louis, 2014) and communication skills) 5 years later
(Charman et al., 2005)
Consonant inventory in • Initially produce reflective, primitive sounds based • Smaller consonant inventories than children
communication acts on general state (e.g., comfortable, hungry, or tired) without ASD (Landa et al., 2013; Wetherby
and not intentional (Oller, 2000; Oller et al., 1999) et al., 2007)
• Vocalizations become intentional and more • Produce more nonspeech and atypical
complex in first year of life (Oller, 2000; Oller vocalizations (e.g., high-pitched squeals
et al., 1999) and low-pitched growls or glottal sounds),
• Produce precanonical babbling (e.g., marginal which tend to include few or no consonants,
babbling, squeals, and full vowels) around than children without ASD (Landa et al.,
3–8 months (Nathani et al., 2006; Oller, 2000; 2013; Plumb & Wetherby, 2013; Sheinkopf
Oller et al., 1999) et al., 2000; Wetherby et al., 2007)
• Produce canonical syllables (i.e., consonant–vowels
syllables with adultlike transitions between the
consonant and vowel, such as “buh” and “dah”)
around 5–10 months
• Use canonical syllables within intentional
communication, which emerges around
8–9 months (e.g., “buh-buh” while reaching
for a toy and looking toward an adult or
using “dadada” to gain an adult’s attention)
Responding to joint • Follow an adult’s point under certain conditions • Decreased responsivity to bids for joint
attention around 9 months (M. Carpenter et al., 1998; attention compared with children with TD
Lempers, 1979) (M. Carpenter et al., 1998, 2002; Chawarska
• Reliably respond to bids for joint attention around et al., 2012; Wetherby, 2006)
12–15 months (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984; • Difficulty following adults’ points, other
M. Carpenter et al., 1998) gestures, and eye gaze (Baron-Cohen, 1989;
• Ability to follow another individual’s point usually Baron-Cohen et al., 1997; M. Carpenter et al.,
precedes the ability to follow a person’s gaze to an 2002)
object (M. Carpenter et al., 1998; Lempers, 1979) • As a group, children with ASD followed the
same pattern of joint attention as children
with TD (e.g., sharing attention, then following
attention, and then directing others’ attention),
but 25% of them followed a different pattern
(e.g., directing others’ attention before following
bids for joint attention; M. Carpenter et al.,
2002)
• Some children with ASD appear to follow
gaze later than expected relative to other
joint attention skills (Paparella et al., 2011)

language input from adults than when they do not respond may also develop greater expressive language skills compared
to joint attention or respond inconsistently. For example, with children with ASD with lower joint attention skills.
children with ASD who tend to respond consistently to bids
for joint attention may benefit more from linguistic map-
ping, follow-in comments, and expansions compared with
children who less consistently respond to bids for joint atten-
How to Assess Prelinguistic Skills
tion (McDaniel et al., 2017; Siller & Sigman, 2008). As a Monitoring progress is a critical component of pro-
result, children with ASD with greater joint attention skills viding evidence-based intervention. SLPs must determine

4 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
the degree to which a child is responding to an intervention inventory in communication acts, and/or responding to
to determine whether and how services need to be adjusted. joint attention. See Table 2. These assessments look very
Data collection for clinical decisions needs to be proactive different than traditional speech-language assessments that
and frequent. Assessment does not just occur during the use books of picture stimuli and ask children to respond to
child’s first appointment or during his or her eligibility eval- questions and directions. Published assessments that focus
uation. Furthermore, the type of assessment data collected on early communication skills provide a framework for
during therapy sessions for progress monitoring differs from observing skills that may be particularly helpful to profes-
that collected during an initial or annual evaluation to de- sionals with less experience assessing early communication
termine eligibility and general areas of strength and need. skills. The framework helps SLPs identify salient features
The selected published assessments for evaluating prelin- of early communication that may be underappreciated or
guistic skills described below are more appropriate for ini- hard to notice without training.
tial or annual assessments than progress monitoring due to Several limitations of these published assessments
the time required to administer and score. For progress should be acknowledged. Most notably, they require sub-
monitoring, we recommend using brief, frequent commu- stantial time and expertise to score and interpret. The ad-
nication samples during therapy sessions to provide data ministration times are comparable to or shorter than common
needed for clinical decisions. Such progress monitoring helps language assessments, but scoring video-recorded sessions
SLPs determine the degree to which a child is responding to for specific behaviors can be quite time-consuming. There-
an intervention by assessing performance within and outside fore, even though some of the published assessments can
of a therapeutic context (Olswang & Bain, 1994). capture subtle changes in prelinguistic skills, they were not
designed to be administered frequently (e.g., weekly). Be-
cause some of these published assessments are not readily
Selected Published Assessments for Evaluating available in a user-friendly form (e.g., Early Social Commu-
Prelinguistic Skills Periodically nication Scales; Mundy et al., 2003), SLPs may have to de-
Several published assessments use specific tasks or vote substantial time to understanding the administration
activities to assess intentional communication, consonant procedures and gathering all of the supplies. Despite these

Table 2. Selected published communication assessments for children with ASD who are preverbal.

Assessment Description Developmental level Skill area(s) assessed

Communication • Norm-referenced assessment 6–24 months • Twenty types of behaviors are


and Symbolic of communicative competence scored including rates of intentional
Behavior Scales • The 30-min behavior sample communication, consonants used in
Developmental includes communication communication acts, and responding
Profile Behavior temptations, book sharing, to joint attention
Sample (Wetherby symbolic and constructive • Social, speech, and symbolic composite
& Prizant, 2002) play probes, and a language scores are provided
comprehension probe
Communication • A scripted assessment with a Birth to word combinations • Scale encompasses preintentional
Complexity series of 12 play-based activities (words, signs, and/or (e.g., environmental awareness
Scale (Brady • 12-point scale that describes the augmentative and and attending to object or person),
et al., 2012) early expressive communication alternative communication intentional nonsymbolic (e.g., gestures
skills of individuals who primarily [AAC] symbols) and vocalizations), and beginning
communicate with presymbolic symbolic communication (e.g., words,
or early symbolic means signs, or AAC symbols)
• Approximately 20–30 min to
administer
Communication • An expressive communication Birth to 24 months • Inventories how an individual
Matrix (Rowland, measure designed for individuals communicates for four functions
2004) with complex communication (i.e., refuse, obtain, social interaction,
needs (e.g., difficulty producing and information exchange) and at
natural speech) seven stages of communication
• Based on a series of questions and development, including preintentional,
observations, a graphical display intentional, presymbolic, and symbolic
of the individual’s communication communication
skills is generated
• Approximately 10–60 min to complete
Early Social • A 15- to 25-min structured observation 8–30 months • Focuses on the child’s ability to initiate
Communication designed to measure nonverbal and respond to three types of behavior:
Scales (Mundy communication skills joint attention, behavioral requests,
et al., 2003) • The examiner presents nine tasks, and social interaction
such as object spectacle, social
interaction, gaze following, and
turn-taking tasks

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 5


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
limitations, the published assessments can yield useful infor- to know how a child performs within a therapeutic setting
mation when used with children displaying early communi- when teaching is occurring (i.e., treatment data) as well as
cation skills. Additionally, they can provide a framework outside of the therapeutic setting (i.e., generalization probe
for communication samples. The manuals often include help- data; Olswang & Bain, 1994). Seeing progress within a
ful descriptions of key behaviors, such as definitions and treatment session provides encouraging data; however,
examples of joint attention and intentional communication. ultimately, the child needs to be able to use those skills
in everyday environments with a variety of communication
partners. We consider (a) what to measure, (b) how to mea-
Frequent Progress Monitoring of Prelinguistic Skills
sure, and (c) when to measure to make intervention deci-
to Capture Incremental Change sions for children with ASD who are preverbal.
Progress monitoring measures must be relatively
brief in time to administer and score so that they can be What to Measure
administered repeatedly across short time intervals. In addi- For what to measure, we focus on intentional com-
tion, progress monitoring measures must capture change at munication, consonant inventory in communication acts,
relatively small increments (not just large changes) to docu- and responding to joint attention. As we have already dis-
ment change over these short time intervals. It is essential cussed, these variables are particularly predictive of expres-
to capture not only the achievement of a goal (i.e., large sive language skills for children with ASD who are not yet
change) but also any incremental changes so as to inform talking.
intervention planning on a week-to-week basis. For exam- Supplemental Material S1 includes blank example
ple, let us assume a long-term goal of spoken words for a data collection documents for monitoring intentional com-
child with ASD who is 3 years old and not yet talking. In- munication, consonant inventory in communication acts,
tervention focused on spoken language may yield many and responding to joint attention in communication sam-
changes that are evidence of moving toward the goal long ples. These documents can be adapted by SLPs for specific
before the goal of spoken words, or even the subgoal of first children. They do not need to be used in their current form.
words, is actually achieved. A progress monitoring measure Instead, SLPs are encouraged to modify the number of tar-
that captures change only at the level of number of spoken get trials, communication sample context, format, level of
words will not be adequate to individualize intervention. prompting provided, and other features to meet the prog-
Selecting and administering appropriate progress ress monitoring needs of the children they serve. The details
monitoring assessments can be difficult for an SLP work- of the progress monitoring procedures also should continue
ing with young children with ASD. Children with ASD to evolve as the evidence base for intervention for children
may have difficulty understanding assessment tasks, and with ASD develops.
social impairments may interfere with test performance Intentional communication. Intentional communica-
(Barokova & Tager-Flusberg, 2018; Tager-Flusberg, 2000). tion may be quantified in different ways, including the total
Furthermore, methods for progress monitoring prelinguis- number of intentional communication acts or the number of
tic skills are fewer than for progress monitoring skills be- communication acts with a certain feature (e.g., total types
yond the preverbal stage (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2009). of functions and forms) within a specified period of time (e.g.,
One progress monitoring tool that is designed for the 5 min). Examining a child’s repertoire of functions and forms
prelinguistic stage is the Early Communication Indicator may highlight specific areas of need for intervention. Be-
(ECI; Greenwood et al., 2019). It is one of the Individual cause intentional communication includes prelinguistic as
Growth and Development Indicators developed to assess well as linguistic communication acts, measures of inten-
progress in children birth to 3 years of age (Greenwood et al., tional communication are appropriate for children at the pre-
2019, 2011, 2006; Luze et al., 2001; Priest et al., 2001). The linguistic level and those beginning to use linguistic forms,
ECI assesses total communication, gestures, vocalizations, such as spoken words or signs. SLPs can weight higher level
single words, and multiple words during a 6-min play-based forms of intentional communication with more points to
assessment with a standardized set of toys (i.e., house or show growth over time. For example, children receive
farm set). The ECI could be used to measure intentional 1 point for nonsymbolic intentional communication acts,
communication. However, it is not designed to be scored 2 points for communication acts with a single spoken (or
in sufficient detail to collect data on the consonant inven- signed) word, and 3 points for communication acts with
tory in communication acts and does not include specific spoken (or signed) phrases for the weighted frequency of
opportunities for responding to joint attention. Thus, we intentional communication (also known as weighted triadic
describe how to use data from therapeutic activities and communication; Greenwood et al., 2006; Yoder et al.,
communication sampling to collect the necessary data for 2009). The example data collection form for intentional
evidence-based clinical decisions in place of or in addition communication in Supplemental Material S1 divides the
to published assessments. observed communication acts into nonsymbolic, single
We apply the framework described by Olswang and spoken words/signs, and spoken/signed phrases to permit
Bain (1994) to discuss the details of data collection for mak- differential weighting, if desired. Alternatively, one could
ing intervention decisions (i.e., progress monitoring) for chil- focus on nonsymbolic communication with separate boxes for
dren with ASD who are preverbal. It often is very helpful acts that use vocal communication, gestural communication,

6 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
or both. Such a system would specify how a child is using to which he or she is using sounds communicatively may
nonsymbolic communication and whether over time he or be informative. Progress may be seen first in a reduction of
she is more frequently combining modes (i.e., vocalization atypical vocalizations (e.g., high-pitch squeals and low-pitch
plus gesture) to communicate more effectively. growls) or an increase in using vowels within communica-
For children who are not producing intentional com- tion acts. This approach to documenting progress focuses
munication acts, consider marking intentional behaviors on what the child is doing, rather than what he or she is not
that they demonstrate, which an adult can interpret. Inten- doing. Observing what the child is doing enables the SLP
tional behaviors are actions that the child does on purpose to make week-to-week decisions about whether and how
but that are not yet directed toward another individual. For the focus and/or approach of intervention needs to change
example, the child may smile and/or vocalize when he or in light of the incremental progress and long-term goals.
she sees a favorite toy character without looking between For example, a child may exhibit increasing use of vowels,
the character and a person. Thus, the child produces an but not consonants, within communication acts. A logical
intentional behavior(s) that suggest he or she would like focus for intervention would be building from that commu-
the character but does not direct the behaviors to another nicative intent and providing many opportunities for the
person. Even though the act is not intentional communica- child to use consonants within meaningful contexts to elicit
tion, it includes several skills foundational to intentional communicative use of consonants. Possible strategies include
communication (e.g., apparent interest in object/activity, vo- recasts, follow-in comments, and linguistic mapping from
calizing, and displaying appropriate facial expressions). As the adult to provide rich linguistic input.
another example, the child may push away a food in appar- Responding to joint attention. Children must be pro-
ent protest while only looking at the food. The child signals vided with opportunities to demonstrate their responses to
that he or she does not want the food but does not direct bids for joint attention because responding to joint atten-
that message toward a communication partner. In contrast, tion inherently follows another individual’s action. Sufficient
when the child hands food back to his communication part- opportunities are required for stable estimates. The exam-
ner, that action would be classified as intentional commu- iner also must attend to the type(s) of cues he or she pro-
nication. Intentional behaviors, such as those described in vided (e.g., pointing toward versus looking toward the
these examples, may become the child’s means of intentional object of interest). The example data collection form for
communication especially with targeted intervention. Moni- responding to joint attention in Supplemental Material S1
toring the frequency and diversity of intentional behavior provides spaces to note the type of joint attention bids
can indicate forms and/or functions of intentional commu- provided, accuracy for each trial, and overall accuracy for
nication most likely to develop with continued support. the session.
Consonant inventory in communication acts. The di- For children who currently do not respond to bids
versity of key consonants used in communication acts is an for joint attention independently, documenting the type and
empirically supported way to measure consonant inventory amount of support necessary to elicit a response can capture
in communication acts (Wetherby et al., 2007; Woynaroski progress even though the child is not demonstrating the
et al., 2016; Yoder et al., 2015). To calculate the diversity skill independently. SLPs can adjust the level of support
of key consonants used in communication acts, count the to identify the child’s current skill level and then increase
number of specific “key” consonants (i.e., /m/, /n/, /b/ or /p/, difficulty over time. For example, saying “Look!” while
/d/ or /t/, /g/ or /k/, /j/, /w/, /l/, /s/, and /ʃ/) that the child uses looking toward and pointing to a picture on the wall
communicatively. These consonants were identified as “key” provides more support than saying, “Wow!” when look-
because most appear relatively early in typical development ing at an exciting toy because the first includes a pointing
and listeners can reliably identify them. Because voiced– gesture.
voiceless cognates (i.e., /b/ vs. /p/, /d/ vs. /t/, and /g/ vs. /k/)
are hard to distinguish reliably, children receive only 1 point How to Measure
for each pair regardless of whether they use one or both of Communication samples address multiple weaknesses
the consonants. Thus, the maximum score is 10. The conso- of published assessments (e.g., long administration and/or
nants may be produced in nonword vocalizations or spoken scoring time and limited flexibility). They provide an excel-
words, as long they are in communication acts. Whether lent opportunity to collect treatment data and generalization
the consonant was used in a communication act is the focus probe data regarding prelinguistic skills of interest in clini-
of the analysis, not whether it was produced in a word cal and educational settings (Barokova & Tager-Flusberg,
(McDaniel et al., 2019). Consonants produced outside of 2018; Kasari et al., 2013; Tager-Flusberg et al., 2009). In-
communication acts are not counted. An SLP can use a tentional communication, consonant inventory in commu-
form similar to the blank example data collection form for nication acts, and responding to joint attention can be (but
consonant inventory in communication acts in Supplemen- do not have to be) measured within a single communica-
tal Material S1 to circle the consonants observed within a tion sample. We use the term communication sampling rather
communication sample. than language sampling to emphasize the assessment of pre-
For children who are not producing consonants, re- linguistic skills rather than spoken words. Nonetheless, most
cording the types of sounds the child is producing (in addi- language sampling principles and recommendations apply.
tion to continuing to listen for consonants) and the degree Communication sampling typically involves video-recording

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 7


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
a child in a context that offers opportunities to communi- opportunities for the child to request and comment while
cate and identifying and analyzing specific behaviors by building Mr. Potato Head to facilitate intentional commu-
reviewing the video recording. If only a limited number of nication acts. The SLP can record the number of inten-
behaviors are of interest, live communication sampling may tional communication acts observed during the sample, which
be possible. However, recording samples is preferred to per- includes support from the therapeutic strategies (e.g., with-
mit multiple viewings of subtle cues or details, which may holding objects, offering choices, follow-in comments, and
not have been of interest initially and to share with other linguistic mapping). See the Appendix for an example. The
team members. In this section, we describe how communica- example document divides the intentional communication
tion samples can be used to collect generalization probe acts into nonsymbolic acts, single words, and phrases to
data as well as treatment data. capture key changes in the onset of spoken words. The
For generalization probe data, we are interested in use of nonsymbolic communication acts is expected to pre-
how the child performs outside of a therapeutic context. When cede spoken words and to increase across the prelinguistic
using communication samples for generalization probe data, period. We also record the activity and supports on the
it is advantageous to keep many of the features of the sam- document to provide context for interpreting the results.
ple constant across repeated measures. For example, for Similarly, SLPs can use the example documents for
each sample, use the same communication partner, same consonant inventory in communication acts and respond-
room, same length (e.g., 5 min), and the same set of toys ing to joint attention. They can record the data live during
for the communication samples, ones that are not used in a therapeutic activity (which is the communication sample)
therapy. This consistency not only reduces the number of or watch the brief video recording after the session. If a re-
factors that may influence the child’s performance day to cording is used, it would be possible to document multiple
day but also makes the sampling procedure more routine prelinguistic skills (i.e., intentional communication, conso-
and less cognitively demanding for the SLP. The generaliza- nant inventory in communication acts, and responding to
tion probe data can be gathered from the treating SLP inter- joint attention) from a single sample. SLPs can weigh these
acting with the child without using all of the supports that options based on the degree to which it is possible to re-
are typical in treatment. The SLP sets up the environment cord data live, the logistics of recording sessions, and the
to support engagement and communication. Engagement frequency with which data need to be collected, which we
is supported by following the child’s lead and joining in consider in the next section.
play at the child’s demonstrated level of play. Communica-
tion is supported by talking about topics related to the When to Measure
child’s focus of attention, monitoring utterance length and For when to measure treatment data, we recommend
complexity (while maintaining grammatical utterances), collecting some data during every treatment session. Such
and avoiding directives (except when needed for safety). data will not necessarily cover every goal in every session.
This interaction style minimizes modeling and prompting Nonetheless, the data should be related to the child’s long-
of communication and language, which differentiates it term goal. For children with ASD who are preverbal, asses-
from interaction styles often used in therapeutic activities. sing intentional communication, consonant inventory in
Consequently, this communication sample is likely to re- communication acts, or responding to joint attention would
semble those used during evaluation sessions more than a all relate to the long-term goal of use of spoken words. For
typical therapy session. example, if the SLP sees a child 2 times per week, the SLP
The sample data collection pages (see Supplemental might collect treatment data for each of two of the three
Material S1) include spaces to record the context of the subgoals during one activity per session. That plan would
sample (e.g., activity, communication partner, and setting). permit treatment data from one or two sessions per week
If an SLP uses the same interaction style for all generaliza- to inform planning for the next week.
tion probe communication samples, a shorthand phrase, For generalization probe data, we recommend starting
such as “communication sampling procedure,” can be used by collecting weekly 5-min communication samples with
to describe the support. See the intentional communication children with ASD who are preverbal. This frequency and
page in the Appendix for an example. Generalization data length balance the available empirical evidence and the
also can be collected during communication samples with current demands on clinicians’ time and effort. For many
a peer or teacher who is unlikely to use therapeutic strategies variables, we do not have empirical support for the exact
during the sample. This communication sample could take amount of time needed to collect a stable estimate that re-
place in the therapy room, classroom, cafeteria, playground, sults in individuals being similarly ranked across testing occa-
or other common location. The data collection documents in sions separated by short periods of time (e.g., 2 or 4 weeks).
Supplemental Material S1 can still be used. They include In contrast, an unstable measure fluctuates between adminis-
spaces to write the context of the sample. trations and is influenced by extraneous factors beyond the
For treatment data, we are interested in how the child skill being assessed (e.g., fatigue, attention, and social en-
performs with therapeutic support. Thus, the communica- gagement). Samples increase in stability as they increase in
tion sampling context is the therapeutic context rather than length and as the child’s level of skills increases (McDaniel,
using the interaction style described for collecting gen- in press; Sandbank & Yoder, 2014; Yoder et al., 2018).
eralization probe data. For example, an SLP may provide When a child is just beginning to show a new skill, more

8 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
variability is observed. Relevant to assessing prelinguistic For data interpretation and intervention planning, it
skills, we found that consonant inventory in communica- is important to distinguish between predictors of expressive
tion acts was sufficiently stable with only two 6-min ECI language growth and malleable factors in children with ASD
(Greenwood et al., 2019, 2011) samples for young children who are preverbal. Being a predictor of expressive language
with ASD (13–30 months of age; McDaniel, Yoder, et al., does not in and of itself indicate that the predictor should
2019). Twelve months later, only one 6-min ECI was needed be targeted. For example, chronological age often correlates
for this same sample of children with ASD. with expressive language; however, we cannot target the
Based on the available empirical evidence and known child’s chronological age as an intervention target. None-
demands on SLPs’ time, we anticipate that this level of theless, if a predictor is a malleable factor with theoretical
treatment data (i.e., each session) and generalization probe and empirical support that its relation with expressive lan-
data (i.e., weekly) will be sensitive to change but not overly guage is or could be causal, it can be considered for an in-
burdensome for collection and interpretation. The data are tervention target. Recognizing the underlying rationale for
likely to provide the SLP with sufficient information to read- why these predictors might affect language growth and the
ily describe the child’s progress and current skills, including expected patterns of development in children with typical
the information needed to update annual Individualized Fam- development and children with ASD can guide interven-
ily Service Plans (before age 3;0 [years;months]) or Individ- tion planning decisions. The three predictors we describe in
ualized Education Programs (after age 3;0). For students this article differ in the state of the evidence. Intentional
receiving services from a third-party payer, assessments may communication presents with more supportive evidence than
be required to demonstrate the need for continued services, consonant inventory in communication acts and responding
in addition to the treatment and generalization probe data. to joint attention. Nonetheless, the latter two still present
The published assessments described in the prior section and with supportive theoretical and empirical evidence. When
Table 2 may fulfill that need. Nonetheless, some children implementing the progress monitoring procedures described
are going to require more frequent and/or longer samples above, the SLP will be able to determine the degree to
for adequate stability to show incremental change. In such which these variables are changing and make informed clini-
cases, consider increasing the length of the sample or com- cal decisions regarding changes in intervention approaches
bining samples from multiple sessions in an effort to reduce and focuses.
variability from one observation to the next (e.g., Wetherby
et al., 1988; Yoder et al., 2018). For example, consider col- Intentional Communication
lecting a 5-min sample 3 times per week, adding the results A number of intervention strategies including, but not
together, and examining the results on a week-to-week basis limited to, Hanen More Than Words (Carter et al., 2011),
rather than session-by-session basis to make patterns and Parent-Mediated Communication-Focused Treatment in
progress more salient. This viewpoint is analogous to exam- Children With Autism (Green et al., 2010), Picture Ex-
ining the stock market over a period of weeks or months change Communication System (Yoder & Stone, 2006),
rather than day to day. Also consider confirming that the Pivotal Response Treatment (Hardan et al., 2015), and
features of the sample context are as consistent as possi- Responsive Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching
ble to minimize other potentially influential factors (e.g., (Yoder & Stone, 2006) have demonstrated success in im-
communication partner, activity/set of toys, time of day, proving language skills of children with ASD. Thus, inten-
and level of fatigue). For example, the SLP may discover tional communication can be addressed via multiple avenues.
that a child shows greater success in the morning relative Targeting intentional communication necessarily involves
to the afternoon. Collecting the sample at the same time attending to how the child uses a variety of forms of com-
of day would be expected to reduce the observed variabil- munication. Intentional communication includes nonverbal
ity. Another approach to gathering more data would be forms of communication (e.g., gestures, vocalizations, and
to ask the child’s caregivers or teachers to provide input. eye gaze) as well as signs and aided forms of AAC. Chil-
The SLP could ask the caregivers or teachers about how dren with ASD have demonstrated benefit from AAC for
the child is communicating with them or using different developing intentional communication and other language
consonant sounds, for example. skills (Ganz et al., 2012; Kasari et al., 2014). An SLP may
consider introducing AAC strategies and track the degree
to which the child uses those strategies for intentional com-
Modifying Intervention Based munication and whether increased use of spoken language
is used simultaneously. For example, when introduced to a
on Assessment Data speech-generating device (SGD), a child may initially show
The progress monitoring data enables SLPs to evalu- increased requesting with the SGD, followed by increases
ate week-to-week progress to adjust intervention strategies in vocalizations. This pattern would support continued use
as needed to maximize outcomes. By keeping track of the of the SGD to augment spoken communication. In con-
child’s level of success with different levels of support, the trast, another child who is also introduced to an SGD may
SLP can interpret which supports are helpful, which are in- initially show increased requesting with the SGD but no in-
effective, and which can be faded because the child exhibits creases in vocalizations. This pattern would support in-
the skills in at least some contexts without support. creased emphasis on the use of the SGD and potentially

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 9


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
other forms of AAC to support intentional communication children with ASD exhibited greater improvement in respond-
(Schlosser et al., 2007). In addition to considering the fre- ing to joint attention after a joint attention intervention that
quency and forms (e.g., use of vocalizations, signs, and combined developmental and applied behavioral analysis
AAC) of communication, functions of communication should strategies than did children randomly assigned to the sym-
be considered as well when selecting more specific interven- bolic play group or the control group.
tion targets.

Consonant Inventory in Communication Acts Future Research Needs


Consonant inventory in communication acts helps Continued research is needed to predict more accurately
maintain the focus of intervention on the communicative the communication and language outcomes of children with
use of consonants and the complexity of vocal communica- ASD who are preverbal, to develop and refine valid and re-
tion rather than on nonspeech oral motor skills. Nonspeech liable measures of prelinguistic and early language skills in
oral motor exercises should be avoided due to a lack of children with ASD for use in clinical practice, and to de-
supportive evidence, even if used in an attempt to expand velop interventions that maximize spoken language outcomes
a child’s consonant inventory (McCauley et al., 2009). Al- for children with ASD. Measures for clinical practice must
though limited information is known about the specific consider feasibility including the amount of training, adminis-
means through which children with ASD expand their con- tration time, and scoring time required. These measures may
sonant inventory in communication acts, intentional com- use a combination of direct observation, caregiver and/or
munication and parent linguistic responses may facilitate its teacher report, and automated (computer) analyses for vocal
growth through receptive vocabulary (Woynaroski et al., analysis for example (LENA Research Foundation, 2015).
2016). Increases in intentional communication and parent Related to intervention development, more evidence about
linguistic responses may first result in receptive vocabulary the rate and type of progress expected during specific inter-
growth and then expansion of the child’s consonant inven- ventions will improve SLPs’ and related professionals’ abili-
tory in communication acts. Theoretically, once children ties to determine whether an individual child is making
understand words, they begin to try to say those words in expected progress or whether his or her progress is slower
communication acts, even if they are not identifiable words than expected and intervention changes need to be consid-
to the listener (Woynaroski et al., 2016). Additional evidence ered. As mentioned above, we also need additional evidence
shows that expanded consonant use is at least partially re- of the degree to which interventions directly targeting con-
sponsible for the increase in intentional communication sonant use improve expressive language skills in children
leading to increased expressive language skills (McDaniel with ASD who are preverbal. Achieving these goals could
et al., 2017). Thus, seeing change in consonant use in com- improve dramatically the long-term outcomes of children
munication acts is encouraging for progress toward use of with ASD who are preverbal and support them and their
spoken words. However, we do not yet know the degree to families across the life span.
which directly targeting consonant use improves expressive
language skills in children with ASD who are preverbal. If
progress is not observed in the child’s use of consonants in Conclusions
communication acts despite data-driven adjustments to the In this tutorial, we have discussed the identification,
intervention approach, the SLP may consider shifting to- measurement, and application of three particularly strong
ward initiating or emphasizing AAC strategies (e.g., SGD, predictors of expressive language growth in preverbal chil-
picture symbols, or signs) to support expressive language dren with ASD (i.e., intentional communication, consonant
skills. The child’s consonant inventory in communication inventory in communicative acts, and responding to joint
acts should still be monitored across that time to know attention). Changing the questions posed for progress mon-
whether a shift toward spoken language is warranted as itoring for children with ASD who are preverbal could in-
the child develops further. fluence intervention practices positively. By changing the
question from “How many words does he use?” to questions
Responding to Joint Attention about predictors of expressive language growth, such as
To document incremental progress in responding to “Which sounds is he using communicatively?” and “How
joint attention, careful observation of the cues provided (e.g., is she expressing her wants and needs?” the response changes
proximal point, distal point, or eye gaze with or without a from “Zero,” to descriptions of what the child with ASD can
verbalization such as “Look!”) and the child’s response (e.g., do. Focusing on skills rather than deficits is not only more
looking to speaker’s hand or looking to the object of inter- encouraging but also more informative for intervention. Eval-
est) is needed. Because the cues from the communicative uating predictors of expressive language growth could eluci-
partner can dramatically change the communicative difficulty date small but meaningful changes in communication skills
level, the specific cues provided should be noted during inter- and, when needed, provide the impetus for changing the
vention. Improvements in responding to joint attention fol- course of intervention. Observing these meaningful changes
lowing developmental and behavioral interventions have or the lack of these meaningful changes is particularly sig-
been reported (Kasari et al., 2006, 2010; White et al., 2011). nificant for SLPs serving children with ASD who are prever-
For example, Kasari et al. (2006) reported that preschool bal and their families. Currently, there is limited guidance

10 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
for decisions regarding how to measure expressive language doi.org/10.2307/1166214
predictors for progress monitoring and making intervention Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Inter-
decisions for children with ASD who are preverbal. By using relations among social-cognitive skills in young children with
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2),
the clinical procedures described in this tutorial, SLPs can
91–106. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014836521114
be more confident in their decisions to continue with a cur- Carpenter, R. L., Mastergeorge, A. M., & Coggins, T. E. (1983).
rent intervention approach or to change course. These deci- The acquisition of communicative intentions in infants eight to
sions enable children with ASD who are preverbal to receive fifteen months of age. Language and Speech, 26(2), 101–116.
high-quality, effective intervention services that are tailored https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098302600201
to their specific needs and growth over time. Carter, A. S., Messinger, D. S., Stone, W. L., Celimli, S., Nahmias,
A. S., & Yoder, P. J. (2011). A randomized controlled trial of
Hanen’s “More Than Words” in toddlers with early autism
Acknowledgments symptoms. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
52(7), 741–752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02395.x
All authors read and approved the final manuscript. This re- Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring
search was supported by a U.S. Department of Education Prepa- early language development in preschool children with autism
ration of Leadership Personnel grant (H325D140087; PI: Schuele) spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative De-
and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Develop- velopment Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language,
ment (U54HD090216; PI: Colombo Kansas Intellectual and De- 30(1), 213–236. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005482
velopmental Disabilities Research Center). Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J.-A., &
Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with
autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at
References 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time.
Anderson, D. K., Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(5), 500–513.
Thurm, A., Welch, K., & Pickles, A. (2007). Patterns of growth https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00377.x
in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disor- Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context modulates
der. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(4), 594–604. attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. The Journal
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.594 of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(8), 903–913. https://
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02538.x
people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interac- Ellis Weismer, S., & Kover, S. T. (2015). Preschool language vari-
tion. Child Development, 55(4), 1278–1289. https://doi.org/10.2307/ ation, growth, and predictors in children on the autism spec-
1129997 trum. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(12),
Barokova, M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2018). Commentary: Measur- 1327–1337. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12406
ing language change through natural language samples. Journal Ellis Weismer, S., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010). Early language
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, 2287–2306. https:// patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to tod-
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3628-4 dlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Devel-
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative opmental Disorders, 40(10), 1259–1273. https://doi.org/10.1007/
pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, s10803-010-0983-1
7(2), 113–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1989.tb00793.x Ganz, J. B., Earles-Vollrath, T. L., Heath, A. K., Parker, R. I.,
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do chil- Rispoli, M. J., & Duran, J. B. (2012). A meta-analysis of single
dren with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to case research studies on aided augmentative and alternative
crack the code of language? Child Development, 68(1), 48–57. communication systems with individuals with autism spectrum
https://doi.org/10.2307/1131924 disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(1),
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of 60–74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1212-2
performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Be- Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V.,
havior and Development, 21(3), 205–226. https://www.jstor.org/ Howlin, P., Le Couteur, A., Leadbitter, K., Hudry, K., Byford,
stable/23084619 S., Barrett, B., Temple, K., Macdonald, W., Pickles, A., & the
Bottema-Beutel, K. (2016). Associations between joint attention PACT Consortium. (2010). Parent-Mediated Communication-
and language in autism spectrum disorder and typical de- Focused Treatment in Children With Autism (PACT): A ran-
velopment: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. domised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160.
Autism Research, 9(10), 1021–1035. https://doi.org/10.1002/ https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60587-9
aur.1624 Greenwood, C. R., Buzhardt, J., Walker, D., Jia, F., & Carta, J. J.
Brady, N. C., Fleming, K., Thiemann-Bourque, K., Olswang, L., (2019). Criterion validity of the Early Communication Indicator
Dowden, P., Saunders, M. D., & Marquis, J. (2012). Develop- for infants and toddlers. Assessment for Effective Intervention,
ment of the Communication Complexity Scale. American Jour- 45(4), 298–310. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534508418824154
nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(1), 16–28. https://doi. Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., & McConnell, S. (2011). Advances
org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0099) in measurement for universal screening and individual progress
Bruner, J. S. (1974). From communication to language—A psy- monitoring of young children. Journal of Early Intervention,
chological perspective. Cognition, 3(3), 255–287. https://doi. 33(4), 254–267. https://doi.org/10.1177/1053815111428467
org/10.1016/0010-0277(74)90012-2 Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., Walker, D., Hughes, K., &
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, Weathers, M. (2006). Preliminary investigations of the application
C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative of the Early Communication Indicator (ECI) for infants and tod-
competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the dlers. Journal of Early Intervention, 28(3), 178–196. https://doi.org/
Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), i–174. https:// 10.1177/105381510602800306

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 11


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., Goldstein, M. H., & King, A. P. expressive language in children with autism spectrum disorder.
(2006). Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelin- Research in Developmental Disabilities, 72, 202–213. https://
guistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.11.010
30(6), 509–516. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071914 McDaniel, J., Woynaroski, T., Keceli-Kaysili, B., Watson, L., &
Hardan, A. Y., Gengoux, G. W., Berquist, K. L., Libove, R. A., Yoder, P. J. (2019). Vocal communication with canonical syl-
Ardel, C. M., Phillips, J., Frazier, T. W., & Minjarez, M. B. lables predicts later expressive language skills in preschool-aged
(2015). A randomized controlled trial of pivotal response treat- children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech,
ment group for parents of children with autism. The Journal of Language, and Hearing Research, 62(10), 3826–3833. https://
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(8), 884–892. https://doi. doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-19-0162
org/10.1111/jcpp.12354 McDaniel, J., Yoder, P., Estes, A., & Rogers, S. J. (in press).
Hess, C. R., & Landa, R. J. (2012). Predictive and concurrent va- Stability of vocal variables measured during the Early Com-
lidity of parent concern about young children at risk for autism. munication Indicator for children with autism spectrum dis-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 575–584. order. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1282-1 Disabilities.
Kasari, C., Brady, N., Lord, C., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2013). McDaniel, J., Yoder, P. J., & Watson, L. R. (2017). A path model
Assessing the minimally verbal school-aged child with autism of expressive vocabulary skills in initially preverbal preschool
spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 6(6), 479–493. https://doi. children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
org/10.1002/aur.1334 Developmental Disorders, 47(4), 947–960. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Kasari, C., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2006). Joint attention s10803-016-3016-x
and symbolic play in young children with autism: A random- Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Yale, M., Messinger, D.,
ized controlled intervention study. The Journal of Child Psychol- Neal, R., & Schwartz, H. (2000). Responding to joint attention
ogy and Psychiatry, 47(6), 611–620. https://doi.org/10.1111/ across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language
j.1469-7610.2005.01567.x acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(3),
Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A. C., Wong, C., Kwon, S., & Locke, J. (2010). 283–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(99)00040-4
Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., &
intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and De- Seibert, J. (2003). Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS).
velopmental Disorders, 40(9), 1045–1056. https://doi.org/10.1007/ University of Miami.
s10803-010-0955-5 Nathani, S., Ertmer, D. J., & Stark, R. E. (2006). Assessing vocal
Kasari, C., Kaiser, A., Goods, K., Nietfeld, J., Mathy, P., Landa, R., development in infants and toddlers. Clinical Linguistics & Pho-
Murphy, S., & Almirall, D. (2014). Communication interven- netics, 20(5), 351–369. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200500211451
tions for minimally verbal children with autism: A sequential National Research Council. (2001). Educating children with autism.
multiple assignment randomized trial. Journal of the American National Academies Press.
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(6), 635–646. Oller, D. K. (2000). The emergence of the speech capacity. Erlbaum.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.01.019 https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410602565
Landa, R. J., Gross, A. L., Stuart, E. A., & Faherty, A. (2013). Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R., & Schwartz, H. K. (1999).
Developmental trajectories in children with and without autism Precursors to speech in infancy: The prediction of speech and
spectrum disorders: The first 3 years. Child Development, 84(2), language disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32(4),
429–442. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01870.x 223–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(99)00013-1
Lempers, J. D. (1979). Young children’s production and compre- Olswang, L. B., & Bain, B. (1994). Data collection: Monitoring
hension of nonverbal deictic behaviors. The Journal of Genetic children’s treatment progress. American Journal of Speech-
Psychology, 135(1), 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325. Language Pathology, 3(3), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-
1979.10533420 0360.0303.55
LENA Research Foundation. (2015). LENA Pro. http://www. Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Steinfeld, M. B., Hill, M. M., Cook, I.,
lenafoundation.org/lena-pro Hutman, T., Macari, S., Rogers, S. J., & Sigman, M. (2009).
Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year-olds referred for possible How early do parent concerns predict later autism diagnosis?
autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(8), Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30(5),
1365–1382. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01669.x 367–375. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ba0fcf
Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. Paparella, T., Goods, K. S., Freeman, S., & Kasari, C. (2011). The
(2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with emergence of nonverbal joint attention and requesting skills in
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental young children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders,
Disorders, 38(8), 1426–1438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007- 44(6), 569–583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.08.002
0510-1 Paul, R., Chawarska, K., Cicchetti, D., & Volkmar, F. (2008). Lan-
Luze, G. J., Linebarger, D. L., Greenwood, C. R., Carta, J. J., guage outcomes of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: A
Walker, D., Leitschuh, C., & Atwater, J. B. (2001). Developing two year follow-up. Autism Research, 1(2), 97–107. https://doi.
a general outcome measure of growth in the expressive com- org/10.1002/aur.12
munication of infants and toddlers. School Psychology Review, Pickett, E., Pullara, O., O’Grady, J., & Gordon, B. (2009).
30(3), 383–406. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2001.12086122 Speech acquisition in older nonverbal individuals with autism:
McCauley, R. J., Strand, E., Lof, G. L., Schooling, T., & Frymark, T. A review of features, methods, and prognosis. Cognitive and
(2009). Evidence-based systematic review: Effects of nonspeech Behavioral Neurology, 22(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1097/
oral motor exercises on speech. American Journal of Speech- WNN.0b013e318190d185
Language Pathology, 18(4), 343–360. https://doi.org/10.1044/ Plumb, A. M., & Wetherby, A. M. (2013). Vocalization development
1058-0360(2009/09-0006) in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech,
McDaniel, J., D’Ambrose Slaboch, K., & Yoder, P. J. (2018). A Language, and Hearing Research, 56(2), 721–734. https://doi.
meta-analysis of the association between vocalizations and org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0104)

12 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Priest, J. S., McConnell, S. R., Walker, D., Carta, J. J., Kaminski, Watt, N., Wetherby, A., & Shumway, S. (2006). Prelinguistic pre-
R. A., Mcevoy, M. A., Good, R. H., Greenwood, C., III, & dictors of language outcome at 3 years of age. Journal of Speech,
Shinn, M. R. (2001). General growth outcomes for young chil- Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1224–1237. https://doi.
dren: Developing a foundation for continuous progress measure- org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/088)
ment. Journal of Early Intervention, 24(3), 163–180. https://doi. Wetherby, A. M. (2006). Understanding and measuring social
org/10.1177/10538151010240030101 communication in children with autism spectrum disorders. In
Rowland, C. (2004). Communication matrix. Oregon Health & Sci- T. Charman & W. Stone (Eds.), Early social-communicative in
ence University,, Design to Learn Projects (Original work pub- autism spectrum disorders: Relevance to early diagnosis, identi-
lished 1990). fication and intervention (pp. 3–34). Guilford.
Sandbank, M., & Yoder, P. J. (2014). Measuring representative Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. (2002). Communication and Symbolic
communication in young children with developmental delay. Behavior Scales Developmental Profile–First Normed Edition.
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 34(3), 133–141. Brookes.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121414528052 Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & Walker, V. G.
Schlosser, R. W., Sigafoos, J., Luiselli, J. K., Angermeier, K., (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal chil-
Harasymowyz, U., Schooley, K., & Belfiore, P. J. (2007). Ef- dren from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of
fects of synthetic speech output on requesting and natural speech Speech and Hearing Research, 31(2), 240–252. https://doi.org/
production in children with autism: A preliminary study. Re- 10.1044/jshr.3102.240
search in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(2), 139–163. https://doi. Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (1989). The expression of com-
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2006.10.001 municative intent: Assessment guidelines. Seminars in Speech and
Sheinkopf, S. J., Mundy, P., Oller, D. K., & Steffens, M. (2000). Language, 10(1), 77–91. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0028-1082491
Vocal atypicalities of preverbal autistic children. Journal of Wetherby, A. M., Watt, N., Morgan, L., & Shumway, S. (2007).
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4), 345–354. https:// Social communication profiles of children with autism spec-
doi.org/10.1023/A:1005531501155 trum disorders late in the second year of life. Journal of Autism
Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 960–975. https://doi.org/
the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors 10.1007/s10803-006-0237-4
and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental White, P. J., O’Reilly, M., Streusand, W., Levine, A., Sigafoos, J.,
Psychology, 44(6), 1691–1704. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013771 Lancioni, G., Fragale, C., Pierce, N., & Aguilar, J. (2011). Best
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, practices for teaching joint attention: A systematic review of
S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in two- and three-year- the intervention literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Dis-
old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental orders, 5(4), 1283–1295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.
Disorders, 27(6), 677–696. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025854816091 02.003
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). The challenge of studying language de- Williams, T. (1990). Language acquisition in autistic children: A
velopment in autism. In L. Menn & N. Bernstein Ratner (Eds.), research note. The European Journal of Psychiatry, 4(3),
Methods for studying language production (pp. 313–332). Erlbaum. 173–179.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Kasari, C. (2013). Minimally verbal school- Wodka, E. L., Mathy, P., & Kalb, L. (2013). Predictors of phrase
aged children with autism spectrum disorder: The neglected and fluent speech in children with autism and severe language
end of the spectrum. Autism Research, 6(6), 468–478. https:// delay. Pediatrics, 131(4), e1128–e1134. https://doi.org/10.1542/
doi.org/10.1002/aur.1329 peds.2012-2221
Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. E. (2005). Language and Woynaroski, T., Watson, L., Gardner, E., Newsom, C. R., Keceli-
communication in autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & Kaysili, B., & Yoder, P. J. (2016). Early predictors of growth
D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive develop- in diversity of key consonants used in communication in ini-
mental disorder (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 335–364). Wiley. https:// tially preverbal children with autism spectrum disorder. Jour-
doi.org/10.1002/9780470939345.ch12 nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 1013–1024.
Tager-Flusberg, H., Rogers, S., Cooper, J., Landa, R., Lord, C., https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2647-7
Paul, R., Rice, M., Stoel-Gammon, C., Wetherby, A., & Yoder, Wu, Z., & Gros-Louis, J. (2014). Infants’ prelinguistic communi-
P. J. (2009). Defining spoken language benchmarks and select- cative acts and maternal responses: Relations to linguistic devel-
ing measures of expressive language development for young opment. First Language, 34(1), 72–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, 0142723714521925
Language, and Hearing Research, 52(3), 643–652. https:// Yoder, P. J. (2006). Predicting lexical density growth rate in young
doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0136) children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of
Thurm, A., Lord, C., Lee, L.-C., & Newschaffer, C. (2007). Predic- Speech-Language Pathology, 15(4), 378–388. https://doi.org/
tors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism 10.1044/1058-0360(2006/035)
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- Yoder, P. J., Lloyd, B. P., & Symons, F. J. (2018). Observational
orders, 37(9), 1721–1734. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006- measurement of behavior (2nd ed.). Brookes.
0300-1 Yoder, P. J., & Stone, W. L. (2006). A randomized comparison of
Thurm, A., Manwaring, S. S., Swineford, L., & Farmer, C. (2015). the effect of two prelinguistic communication interventions on
Longitudinal study of symptom severity and language in minimally the acquisition of spoken communication in preschoolers with
verbal children with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology ASD. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(4),
and Psychiatry, 56(1), 97–104. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12285 698–711. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/051)
Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., Baranek, G. T., Dykstra, J. R., & Yoder, P. J., Stone, W. L., Walden, T., & Malesa, E. (2009). Pre-
Wilson, K. P. (2013). Communicative gesture use in infants dicting social impairment and ASD diagnosis in younger sib-
with and without autism: A retrospective home video study. lings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(1), 25–39. and Developmental Disorders, 39(10), 1381–1391. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0145) 10.1007/s10803-009-0753-0

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 13


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (2002). Effects of prelinguistic milieu Yoder, P. J., Watson, L., & Lambert, W. (2015). Value-added pre-
teaching and parent responsivity education on dyads involving dictors of expressive and receptive language growth in initially
children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech, Lan- nonverbal preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Jour-
guage, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1158–1174. https://doi. nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(5), 1254–1270.
org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/094) https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2286-4

14 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Appendix ( p.1 of 4)
Data Collection Documents With Hypothetical Data

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 15


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Appendix ( p. 2 of 4)
Data Collection Documents With Hypothetical Data

16 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Appendix ( p. 3 of 4)
Data Collection Documents With Hypothetical Data

McDaniel & Schuele: When Will He Talk? 17


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Appendix ( p. 4 of 4)
Data Collection Documents With Hypothetical Data

18 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 30 • 1–18 • January 2021

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Universidad Del Museo Social Argentino on 01/28/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions

You might also like