Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Augmentative and Alternative Communication, December 2009 VOL. 25 (4), pp.

274–286

The Use of Augmentative and Alternative Communication


Methods with Infants and Toddlers with Disabilities:
A Research Review
DIANE BRANSON* and MARYANN DEMCHAK

University of Nevada, Reno; Reno, Nevada, USA

This review sought to determine the evidence base of augmentative and alternative
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

communication (AAC) use with infants and toddlers with disabilities. The review identified
12 studies, involving 190 participants aged 36 months or younger. The majority of the studies
investigated unaided AAC methods (e.g., gestures or sign language), with 42% of the studies
also including aided AAC methods. Although all studies reported improvement in child
communication following AAC intervention, in-depth analyses of study methodology
indicated that only 7 out of 12 provided conclusive evidence. Implications for early
intervention AAC practice and suggestions for future research are proposed.

Keywords: Augmentative and alternative communication; Developmental disabilities;


Intervention; Research synthesis; Effectiveness
For personal use only.

INTRODUCTION forms ranging from natural gestures, manual


signs, and picture communication boards, to
Augmentative and alternative communication sophisticated voice output or speech generating
(AAC) interventions are methods and technol- devices.
ogy used to compensate for an individual’s Infants and toddlers with developmental delays
reduced communicative competence (Light, could fall into any of the three groups described
1989) and can be temporary or permanent by von Tetzchner and Martinsen (1992). It is
(American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa- important to focus on AAC use with infants and
tion, 1991). According to von Tetzchner and toddlers because there is evidence that a child’s
Martinsen (1992), individuals who might benefit early learning experiences during the first 3 years
from AAC fall into three groups: (a) the of life lay the foundation for later brain develop-
expressive language group, in which individuals ment (National Scientific Council on the Devel-
understand others’ spoken language but have oping Child, 2007). Interactions between a child
difficulty expressing themselves; (b) the suppor- and his or her caregiver provide those critical
tive language group, comprised of two sub- experiences (Sameroff & Fiese, 2000), but they
groups that include children who temporarily may be lacking or insufficient if the caregiver is
use AAC in order to facilitate understanding of unable to recognize and respond to the child’s
spoken language as well to express themselves subtle communication behaviors. Early access to
or children who speak but have difficulty being AAC methods can assist a child in using
understood; and (c) the alternative language intentional communication behaviors by making
group, in which AAC is a permanent means those behaviors recognizable to his or her
of receptive and expressive communication. caregiver who, in turn, can respond to and
AAC encompasses a variety of communication reinforce those early communication behaviors

*Corresponding author. University of Nevada, Reno; Reno, Nevada 89957, USA. Tel: þ1 775 772 3557. Fax: þ1 775 688 2984.
E-mail: dmbranson@aol.com

ISSN 0743-4618 print/ISSN 1477-3848 online Ó 2009 International Society for Augmentative and Alternative Communication
DOI: 10.3109/07434610903384529
AAC USE WITH INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH DISABILITIES 275

that lead to further development (Cress & children birth to 3 years of age and/or the child’s
Marvin, 2003). communication partner; (c) involved individuals
It is clear that AAC use with infants and with developmental disabilities with significant
toddlers can be a critical component in early communication delays (i.e., speech was not
intervention; however, the majority of AAC adequate to meet communication needs); and
research to date has focused on older individuals (d) included data on the implementation of either
(Charlop-Christy, Carpenter, Loc, LeBlanc, & unaided (no external device required, such as the
Kellet, 2002; Goldstein, 2002), which cannot be use of natural gestures) or aided (external aid of
seamlessly applied to children under the age of 3 some sort used, such as pictures or speech
years. Literature specifically addressing AAC use generating device) AAC methods because the
with infants and toddlers is limited and consists variable of interest was child AAC use.
primarily of position papers prepared by experts
in the field (e.g., Cress & Marvin, 2003; Romski &
Data Extraction
Sevcik, 2005), rather than empirical research.
Evidence-based practice (EBP) has been applied Both authors independently read and analyzed
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

to the field of AAC as a means of ensuring that each identified study using a 3-step process
research evidence is considered by clinical practi- described by Sigafoos et al. (2008). First, they
tioners who make practice-related decisions on a determined whether a study met all of the
daily basis (Raghavendra, 2000; Schlosser, 2003; inclusion criteria described above. Next, studies
Schlosser, Koul, & Costello, 2007; Schlosser & meeting the inclusion criteria were further ana-
Raghavendra, 2004; Schlosser, Wendt, Angerme- lyzed for methodological details based on cate-
ier, & Shetty, 2005). The purpose of this paper is gories used by Schlosser and Lee (2000): (a) study
to review existing research focusing on use of identification; (b) goal of the study; (c) number of
AAC with infants and toddlers. The review is participants; (d) participants’ age; (e) partici-
systematic rather than narrative, emphasizing the pants’ gender; (f) participants’ disability classifi-
rigor of the investigations and the resulting effect cation (e.g., developmental delay, speech and
For personal use only.

sizes, in order to evaluate the current evidence language delay, autism); (g) target of interven-
base for the use of AAC with infants and tion (i.e., child with disability, communica-
toddlers. tion partner, or both); (h) study design (e.g.,
alternating-treatments design, multiple-baseline
design, randomized control group); (i) setting
(e.g., home, childcare, clinic); (j) type of AAC; (k)
METHOD treatment integrity measure (e.g., 20% of video-
taped sessions were coded for adherence to
Search Strategy
treatment protocol); and (l) percentage of non-
Three techniques were used to locate appropriate overlapping data (PND) for single-subject experi-
articles: (a) computerized searches of abstract mental designs (Scruggs, Mastropieri, & Casto,
databases, (b) hand searches of pertinent journals, 1987; Scruggs, Mastropieri, Cook, & Escobar,
and (c) ancestral searches of references cited. 1986) or effect size for group designs. Appendix B
Computerized searches of PsychINFO, Educa- lists the operational definitions for the coding
tion Resources Information Center (ERIC), categories.
MEDLINE, Dissertation Abstracts and Cumula- The final step in reviewing the evidence was
tive Index of Nursing and Allied Health Litera- appraisal of certainty of evidence (Slavin, 1986).
tures (CINAHL) were implemented using the This was done by examining the methodological
following key words: ‘‘augmentative and alter- rigor of each included study as indicated by the
native communication,’’ ‘‘nonvocal,’’ ‘‘nonspeak- research design used and other methodological
ing,’’ and ‘‘early intervention.’’ Next, hand details suggested by Sigafoos and colleagues
searches or electronic searches of the tables of (2008), including: (a) a convincing demonstration
contents of 18 journals were performed (see of treatment effects claimed, (b) adequate inter-
Appendix A). Ancestral search, the use of other observer reliability data (i.e., 20% of the sessions
authors’ references in published articles and with 80% agreement or better), (c) operationally
books on the topic, was the final technique used defined dependent and independent variables, and
to locate pertinent studies (White, 1994). (d) study procedures described sufficiently to
allow for replication (i.e., measures of treatment
integrity provided). Studies were rated as either
Inclusion Criteria
conclusive (i.e., the design provided experimental
Studies met the following inclusion criteria: (a) control, the dependent variable was reliable,
conducted between 1982 and 2007; (b) involved and treatment integrity was solid, providing
276 D. BRANSON AND M. DEMCHAK

conclusive evidence that the AAC implementa- findings of a study. Cohen’s d is a common
tion was effective); or inconclusive (i.e., the study statistic for determining effect size (Sprinthall,
did not use a recognized experimental design or 2003) of group designs, with effect size inter-
there were numerous methodological flaws that preted as small (.20), medium (.50), or large
reduced the certainty of the evidence presented in (.80) (Cohen, 1969).
the study regarding the effectiveness of the AAC The effectiveness measure for single-subject
intervention). The initial search yielded 42 papers data is the Percentage of Non-overlapping
or studies that focused on AAC use with infants Data (PND), which is calculated by identifying
and toddlers; 30 were excluded for the following the highest data point in baseline and deter-
reasons: (a) studies did not implement AAC mining the percentage of data points during
methods, such as narrative research reviews intervention that exceed this point (Scruggs
(Mirenda, 2003) and survey studies (Dugan, et al., 1986). Scruggs et al., (1987) recommend
Campbell, & Wilcox, 2006); (b) data were not that PND scores be interpreted as follows: (a)
collected on the AAC method implemented, but PND 5 50% reflect ineffective treatment, (b)
rather on the effect on spoken words (Yoder & PND 50%–70% reflect questionable effective-
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

Stone, 2006); or (c) the study did not include ness, (c) PND 70%–90% reflect fairly effec-
children with developmental disabilities (as per tive, and (d) PND 4 90% reflect a highly
the Drager, Light, Speltz, Fallon, & Jeffries (2003) effective treatment.
study of how typically developing 2 ½-year-olds
respond to different dynamic display AAC
technologies). Methodological rigor of the
included studies was of primary interest; how- RESULTS
ever, it should be clarified that lack of rigor was
not a factor in exclusion (a list of the excluded Seven single-subject studies involving a total of 32
studies is available upon request from the first participants and five group design studies invol-
author). ving a total of 158 participants met inclusion
For personal use only.

criteria. Table 1 provides a summary of the


dependent variable(s) and effect size, and an
Interrater Agreement
appraisal of the study’s certainty of evidence.
The authors independently screened these 42 Further information about the studies can found
papers using an inclusion/exclusion checklist. in Appendix C.
Interrater agreement for determining papers to
be included and excluded was 100% and resulted
Participants
in 12 studies meeting inclusion criteria. Subse-
quently, the authors independently coded each The studies involved a total of 190 participants
article included according to the operational who were 36 months of age or younger. The
definitions discussed earlier and presented in number of participants in each individual study
Appendix B. A point-by-point analysis of inter- varied from 1 to 58. It is important to note that
rater agreement showed over 98% agreement. some studies included other participants who
There was only one point of disagreement, in were not included in this review because the
which one author omitted a measure pertaining to participant age was over 36 months and/or AAC
treatment integrity. Subsequent review of the use was not targeted for the participant (Studies
original article and discussion of the omitted 1, 6, 7, 10).
measure resulted in rectifying the one disagree- Most of the participants (n ¼ 99, 52%) had
ment and a corrected interrater agreement result either unspecified developmental delays or one of
of 100%. a wide variety of identified etiologies (e.g.,
agenesis of the corpus callosum, Trisomy 8,
mitochondrial disorder), 32 had Down syndrome,
Measures of Effect Size and Percentage of
28 had multiple disabilities (e.g., cerebral palsy
Nonoverlapping Data
plus sensory impairments), 26 had an autism
Quantitative analysis of outcomes reported in spectrum disorder, and 5 had cerebral palsy. Two
the group design studies was based on reported of the studies also focused on the communication
effect size (ES). Effect size is a statistical partners of children with complex communication
measure that goes beyond answering the ques- needs: 4 mothers in one case study (Study 7)
tion of the probability of a difference in the and 25 caregivers in a pre-experimental study
means of two or more groups. Effect size gives (Study 2).
information about the size of the difference, Overall, participant ages ranged from 8 months
which can help determine the importance of the to 36 months; however, age information is not
AAC USE WITH INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH DISABILITIES 277

TABLE 1 Summary of Experimental Studies of AAC use in Early Intervention: Study Number, Authors, and Year of Publication;
Dependent Variable (DV); Effect Size Statistic (and its Interpretation shown in Parenthesis); Appraisal of the Study (see Appendix C
for Details Concerning Study Participants, Method, and Findings).

Study Dependent variable Effect size Appraisal

1. Anderson (2001) Requesting toys/foods using PECS PND 60 (Q) Conclusive


Requesting toys/food using signs PND 0 (I)
2. Chen et al. (2007) Unaided: Vocalizations and body movements; Effect size measure Inconclusive
caregiver use of cues; child use of intentional not possible
communication acts
3. DiCarlo et al. (2001) Intervals with sign PND 33 (I) Inconclusive
Frequency of sign PND 29 (I)
4. Fey et al. (2006) Rate of intentional communicative acts (i.e., Cohen’s d ¼ 0.40–0.68 Conclusive
requesting, commenting, and total rate of (medium)
intentional acts)
5. Iacono & Duncum (1995) Total number of words produced: sign only PND 0 (I) Conclusive
Total number of words produced: sign þ VOCA PND 66 (Q)
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

Number of different words: sign only PND 0 (I)


Number of different words: sign þ VOCA PND 83 (F)
6. Kouri (1988) Frequency of spontaneous signs (label, request, PND 42 (I) Conclusive
protest, play)
Frequency responsive signs (e.g., for ‘‘What do PND 60 (Q)
you want?’’)
Frequency of informal meaningful gestures or to PND 86 (F)
augment a linguistic form
7. Pennington & Child’s use of different communicative functions Effect size measure Inconclusive
McConachie (1999) and modes for expressive communication in not possible
conversational exchanges
8. Salmon et al. (1998) Initiations and responsive requests and comments PND measure not Inconclusive
possible
9. Stahmer & Ingersoll (2004) Various measures, including functional Effect size measure Inconclusive
For personal use only.

communication emphasizing requesting, not possible


labeling, and information sharing
10. Tait et al. (2004) Request ‘‘more’’ PND ¼ 96 (H) Conclusive
Choice making PND ¼ 70 (F)
Protest PND ¼ 94 (H)
Request ‘‘help’’ PND ¼ 100 (H)
11. Warren, Yoder, Gazdag, Experiment 1 Conclusive
Kim, & Jones (1993) Requests PND ¼ 92 (H)
Comments PND ¼ 100 (H)
Experiment 2
Requests PND ¼ 96 (H)
Comments PND ¼ 93 (H)
12. Yoder & Warren (1998) Rate of intentional communication acts Cohen’s d ¼ .5–.55 Conclusive
of requesting and focusing joint (Medium)
attention

Note. Percentage of Non-overlapping Data (PND) is interpreted as follows I ¼ ineffective (5 50); Q ¼ questionable (50–70); F ¼ fairly effective (70–90);
H ¼ highly effective (4 90) (Scruggs et al., 1986). PECS, Picture Exchange Communication System; PDD, Pervasive Developmental Disorders; VOCA,
Voice Output Communication Aid.

available for each individual participant because


Types of AAC
some studies reported age for groups rather than
for individual children (Studies 2, 3, 4, 9, and 12). The types of AAC used included manual signs,
Age specific data are available only for 24 (13 %) line drawings (from Board Builder,TM1 Picture
of the 190 participants (12 girls, 12 boys). They Communication SymbolsTM2 Blissymbols3, and
ranged from 16–36 months of age, with a mean MakatonTM4); photographs; voice output com-
age of 28.5 months. The majority of the munication aid (VOCA); gestures (e.g., head
individuals for whom specific ages were available nods, head shakes, hand claps, shoulder shrugs);
were 31 months of age or older (n ¼ 11; 46%), eye gaze shift from referent to communication
with the second largest group being 25–30 months partner; vocalizations; and body movements. Six
of age (n ¼ 6, 25%), followed by 21% in the 19–24 studies addressed only unaided AAC strategies
months old (n ¼ 5), and the fewest participants 18 (Studies 2, 4, 6, 8, 11, and 12). Two studies
months or less in age (n ¼ 2, 8%). focused on comparing an aided AAC method to
278 D. BRANSON AND M. DEMCHAK

an unaided AAC method (Studies 1 and 5); Study


Targeted skills
5’s recommendation was ultimately for multi-
modal AAC (i.e., signs plus VOCA). A single A variety of communication skills were targeted
study (Study 7) focused on only aided AAC. within the 12 studies: 4 (Studies 2, 4, 9, and 12)
Those studies that addressed aided AAC most targeted increasing intentional communication
often used line drawings (Studies 1, 7, 9, 10); acts; with 3 of those (Studies 2, 4, and 12)
while only two studies included the use of a focusing on prelinguistic, unaided AAC such as
VOCA (Studies 3 and 5), with one of those (Study vocalizations, body movements, and gestures;
3) reporting negligible use of the VOCA because and 1 (Study 9) focused on increasing partici-
it was present but not formally targeted. Only one pants’ use of symbolic communication through
study reported using photographs as an aided PECS or signs. Four other studies (Studies 5–8
AAC strategy (Study 10). The majority of the 190 targeted increasing spontaneous and responsive
participants (n ¼ 144, 76%) were involved in communicative acts in an interactive or ‘‘con-
studies that targeted unaided AAC (e.g., signs, versational’’ manner, and all focused on using
gestures, vocalizations, body movements, eye more symbolic AAC such as conventional
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

gaze shifts). gestures, signs, various line drawing symbols,


and VOCAs. Two studies (Studies 1 and 3)
targeted only requesting, with one of these
Intervention procedures
studies comparing the use of PECS to signs
A wide variety of intervention procedures were (Study 1) and the other (Study 3), evaluating
used across the studies, primarily in the form of the use of signs. The remaining two studies
intervention packages to teach specific targeted (Studies 10 and 11) targeted multiple functions
skills. In Studies 1, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 10, 11, and 12, the (i.e., requesting, choice making, protesting, and/
intervention targeted child behavior. Studies 2 or commenting).
and 7 indirectly targeted the children in that the
primary focus was on training caregivers and
Outcomes
For personal use only.

parents to encourage either more intentional


communicative acts (Study 2) or different func- Column 3 of Table 1 provides the effect size
tions and modes within conversational exchanges statistic for group designs and the percentage of
(Study 7). Study 9 was actually an evaluation of nonoverlapping data (PND) for single subject
an inclusive preschool program for children with designs, along with an interpretation of both
ASD. This study was included in the review effect size and PND. Two of the group design
because the authors included measures studies provided an effect size measure (Studies 4
pertaining to functional communication of the and 12), both of which were indicative of a
participants. medium effect. Six (Studies 1, 3, 5, 6, 10, and 11)
of the seven single subject designs presented data
in a manner that allowed PND to be calculated,
Design
while one single subject design study did not
A slight majority of the included studies used (Study 8). From these six studies, 19 PND values
some variation of a single subject design (n ¼ 7, were calculated due to studies with multiple
58%). Three of these studies used an alternating measures. Studies 10 and 11 were the only two
treatment design (Studies 1, 5, 8); three used a studies to yield PND values (7 PND values, 37%)
type of the multiple baseline design (Studies 3, 10, in the highly effective range (i.e., greater than 90).
11); and one used an ABAB withdrawal design Three PND values (16%) were classified in the
(Study 6). fairly effective range (i.e., 70–90%); 3 PND values
Group studies consisted of two pre-experi- (16%) were classified as being in the questionable
mental designs (Studies 2 and 9) to evaluate effectiveness range (i.e., 50–70%), and 6 of the 19
the effectiveness of specific curricula on chil- measures (31%) were classified as ineffective (i.e.,
dren’s communicative behaviors, and two ran- below 50). These values show that a slight
domized control group studies (Studies 4 and majority of interventions were determined to be
12) that evaluated the use of Prelinguistic highly or fairly effective (i.e., 10 PND values,
Milieu Treatment on children’s rate of inten- 53%).
tional communication. One study (Study 7)
investigated the effect of a semiscripted elicited
Appraisal of evidence
conversation intervention on a range of child
communicative functions using a case study The appraisal of evidence was determined to be
design. conclusive for seven of the studies (58%) (Studies
AAC USE WITH INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH DISABILITIES 279

1, 4, 5, 6, 10, 11, and 12), including 5 single- with the PECS, and one compared manual signs
subject case designs (Studies 1, 5, 6, 10, and 11) alone with manual signs paired with a VOCA.
and 2 group designs (Studies 4 and 12). For the While these studies demonstrate that infants and
remaining 5 studies the appraisal of evidence toddlers can learn to use low-technology (with the
resulted in ratings of inconclusive. The reasons exception of the one study using a VOCA) AAC
for inconclusive ratings included pre-experimental methods to communicate a variety of early deve-
design (Study 2), case study (Study 7), and quasi- loping communication skills (e.g., requesting,
experimental design (Study 9), lack of treatment commenting, choice making, and protesting), the
integrity measures (Study 3 and 8), and lack of available research leaves a number of unanswered
baseline data (Study 8). questions about AAC use with infants and
Four studies appraised as providing conclusive toddlers.
evidence of a positive intervention effect were One of the unanswered questions of great inter-
linked to 10 of the highly or fairly effective PND est to clinicians and parents is the comparative
values (Studies 5, 6, 10, and 11). One study (Study effectiveness of various types of AAC methods for
1) appraised as conclusive, given sound design, children with specific disabilities. Only two of the
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

inter-observer agreement and treatment integrity studies compared AAC methods. Anderson
ratings, actually yielded PND values that were (2001), in her unpublished dissertation, compared
ineffective or questionable. The certainty of an aided and an unaided AAC method (PECS
evidence for Study 3 was inconclusive, with and sign language) with 5 children with autism
PND values calculated to be in the ineffective who were 27 and 36 months old, and found that
range. Two studies (Studies 5 and 6) were rated as (a) the children learned to use PECS faster than
being conclusive, with at least one PND value as sign language, and (b) more of the children who
fairly or highly effective; however, each study also used PECS were able to generalize this use to
had more than one PND value in the ineffective novel items compared to those who used sign
or questionable range. language. Iacono and Duncum (1995) compared
manual signs alone with manual signs plus a
For personal use only.

VOCA on the total number of words and the total


DISCUSSION number of different words produced by a 32-
month-old girl with Down syndrome. The child in
The purpose of this systematic review was to this study produced both more words and a larger
examine existing research focusing on AAC use variety of words in the sign plus VOCA condition
with infants and toddlers in order to identify the than in the sign alone condition.
evidence base and to recognize gaps in the While two studies alone do not provide
research literature. The reviewed studies reported adequate evidence to draw definitive conclusions
the following outcomes of AAC use with infants about the superiority of aided or unaided AAC,
and toddlers: (a) improved communication was they provide fodder for discussion of the poten-
reported for 97% of the 190 total participants, tial benefits of aided systems for young children.
but only 71% of those participants were enrolled The advantage shown by aided (PECS & VOCA)
in a study judged as providing conclusive over unaided AAC (gestures, eye gaze, sign
evidence; (b) communication partners were suc- language) demonstrated in the studies discussed
cessfully taught to create more communicative previously may be related to the relative trans-
opportunities for their child and to increase their parency and concreteness of the symbols used in
child’s use of intentional communication acts by each study. Pictures may be easier than manual
responding contingently; (c) a variety of AAC signs to learn for very young children for several
systems was used successfully with children 36 reasons. First, pictures more closely resemble
months of age and younger; and (d) children with their referents than manual signs, making them
various disabilities were taught to use AAC easier to understand. Second, pointing to or
methods to improve communication. reaching for a picture to make a request involves
The outcomes reported from the 12 included less physical effort and motor planning than
studies provide support for the use of AAC with executing a manual sign, and third, pictures are
infants and toddlers, but the certainty of evidence stationary reducing recall memory load (Miren-
was inconclusive for 5 out of the 12 studies. Of the da, 2003). Additional evidence-based studies
7 studies that were rated as providing conclusive comparing AAC methods with children with
evidence, four used unaided AAC (gestures or specific disabilities are needed before any conclu-
manual signs), one used unaided (signs) or aided sions can be drawn.
(pictures, graphic symbols) depending on the age Limited use of AAC with infants and toddlers
of the participant, one compared manual signs has been attributed to myths about the age at
280 D. BRANSON AND M. DEMCHAK

which AAC should begin with young children palsy, Down syndrome, intellectual disability
(Romski & Sevcik, 2005). It is difficult to predict not associated with Down syndrome, and
at a young age whether or not a particular child unspecified developmental delays), and across
will be able to use speech as a primary form of ages of children, beginning at 16 months of
communication. Even for children for whom age. Improvements were also noted across a
speech is unlikely to be adequate to support their range of intervention intensity and frequencies,
communication needs (e.g., children with signifi- from three sessions a week for 10 weeks
cant neuromotor impairments), clinicians are (Study 1) to two sessions a week for 8 months
hesitant to label that child as ‘‘nonspeaking’’ (Study 6).
prior to age 3 years old (Cress & Marvin, 2003),
and thus would not be reported as using AAC.
Summary and Implications for Practice
Cress and Marvin recommend that AAC be
introduced as soon as it is apparent that a child’s The current analysis contributes to the evidence
communicative signals are difficult to interpret. A that AAC methods can be used effectively with
common thread through all of the studies infants and toddlers with disabilities. Although
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

reviewed was the focus on improving intentional only seven studies provided conclusive evidence
communication acts in various forms. Chen, to this effect, it does appear that many different
Klein, and Haney’s (2007) study provided evi- types of AAC methods can be used to improve
dence that it is important to assist caregivers in a child’s intentional communication, including
identifying and responding to infants’ commu- (a) unaided methods such as signs and gestures
nicative signals at a very early age. These (Fey, Warren, Brady, Finestack, Bedin-Oja,
authors conducted a field study of the Promoting Fairchild, et al., 2006; DiCarlo, Stricklin,
Learning through Active Interaction (PLAI) Banajee, & Reid, 2001; Kouri, 1988; Tait
curriculum, and demonstrated that caregivers et al., 2004; Yoder & Warren, 1998); and (b)
could be taught to recognize and encourage aided methods that are nonelectronic (Ander-
infants’ preintentional communicative signals as son, 2001; Pennington & McConachie, 1999;
For personal use only.

early as 8 months of age. They suggest that it is Tait et al., 2004) or electronic technologies
better to strengthen infants’ communicative (Iacono & Duncum, 1995).
behaviors as soon as possible, rather than Although PECS was slightly more effective
waiting for failure. than sign language in one of the studies reviewed
None of the studies reviewed supported the idea (Anderson, 2001), a key finding that has implica-
of a minimum age requirement for introducing tions for both clinicians and parents is the idea
AAC; however, age did appear to influence the that a variety of AAC methods can be effective
choice of AAC system implemented. Studies that when caregivers respond consistently and contin-
included children under the age of 2 years old gently to their children’s communication at-
tended to use unaided (e.g., gestures, eye gaze, and tempts. If a toddler’s natural gestures are
sign language) rather than aided methods. Some difficult to interpret, a clinician should not
studies used unaided or aided AAC methods hesitate to introduce pictures or a VOCA in
depending on the children’s age level. For order to increase the caregiver’s ability to
example, Tait, Sigafoos, Woodyatts, O’Reilly, recognize and respond to the child’s communica-
and Lancioni (2004) targeted a presymbolic tive attempts. Furthermore, clinicians should be
communicative act with the youngest child (16 willing to try a variety of AAC methods –
months old) in their study. The goal of the including multi-modal AAC – with a young
intervention was for the child to use joint child before concluding that a particular child is
attention skills to request ‘‘more’’ (i.e., look at not ready for AAC (Beukelman & Mirenda,
the toy, then look at her mother, and then look 2005).
back at the toy). In contrast, the goals for the 26-
and 31-month-olds in the study were to point at a
Future Research Directions
picture to make a choice, and to produce signs for
‘‘help’’ and ‘‘more,’’ respectively. Most of the studies reported here were conducted
The conclusive evidence provided in the seven as part of university early intervention clinics.
studies indicates that AAC methods can be There is a need to move research from university
effective with infants and toddlers. All 135 clinics to homes and childcare centers where
participants in the conclusive studies demon- functional use of AAC methods during daily
strated an improvement in communication routines can be investigated. There is also a need
skills following the AAC intervention. Improve- to bridge the gap between research and practice in
ments in a variety of communication acts early intervention. Dugan et al. (2006) conducted
occurred across all disabilities (autism, cerebral a survey study of the assistive technology
AAC USE WITH INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH DISABILITIES 281

practices of multidisciplinary early intervention References


providers across the United States. The results of American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1991).
their survey indicated that providers did not select Report: Augmentative and alternative communication.
AAC or other assistive technology options for ASHA, 33, 9–12.
children until a child was older than 24 months, Anderson, A. (2001). Augmentative communication and
autism: A comparison of sign language and the Picture
and that even then, interveners tended to select a Exchange Communication System. Unpublished doctoral
low-tech option (55.2%) over a high-tech com- dissertation. University of California, San Diego.
munication device (8.4%). It is important to Beukelman, D., & Mirenda (2005). Augmentative and
encourage both providers and parents of infants alternative communication: Supporting children and adults
with disabilities to explore the use of AAC with complex communication needs (3rd edn). Baltimore,
MD: Paul H. Brookes.
methods as early as possible. In the studies Charlop-Christy, M., Carpenter, M., Loc, L., LeBlanc, L., &
reviewed in this paper, children as young as 15 Kellet, K. (2002). Using the Picture Exchange Commu-
months old were taught to use sign language nication System (PECS) with children with autism:
(DiCarlo et al., 2001) and pictures (Tait et al., Assessment of PECS acquisition, speech, social-commu-
2004), and children as young as 30-months old nicative behavior and problem behavior. Journal of
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 213–231.


were taught to use a VOCA (Iacono & Duncum, Chen, D., Klein, D., & Haney, M. (2007). Promoting
1995). interactions with infants who have complex
Further research on AAC use with infants multiple disabilities: Development and field-testing of
and toddlers is needed. Promising research the PLAI curriculum. Infants & Young Children: An
directions include parent-implemented augmen- Interdisciplinary Journal of Special Care Practices, 20,
149–162.
ted language experiences using speech generat- Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for behavioral
ing communication devices (Romski, Sevcik, analysis. New York: Academic Press.
Adamson, Smith, & Cheslock, 2006) and the Cress, C., & Marvin, C. (2003). Common questions about
use of individualized visual scenes displayed on AAC services in early intervention. Augmentative and
a speech generating device (Light & Drager, Alternative Communication, 19, 254–272.
DiCarlo, C., Stricklin, S., Banajee, M., & Reid, D. (2001).
2007) This type of research, investigating the Effects of manual signing on communicative ver-
For personal use only.

effect of parents modeling AAC use during balizations by toddlers with and without disabilities in
family routines, may help parents and early inclusive classrooms. The Journal of the Association for
intervention providers embed AAC into mean- Persons with Severe Handicaps, 26, 120–126.
ingful activities for infants and toddlers with Drager, K. D. R., Light, J. C., Speltz, J. C., Fallon, K. A., &
Jeffries, L. Z. (2003). The performance of typically
disabilities in order to facilitate early learning developing 2 1/2-year-olds on dynamic display AAC
experiences that can promote a child’s further technologies with different system layouts and language
development. organizations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 46, 298–312.
Dugan, L., Campbell, P., & Wilcox, M. (2006). Making
Acknowledgements decisions about assistive technology with infants and
toddlers. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 26,
Preliminary results of this paper were reported at 25–32.
the International Division of Early Childhood Fey, M., Warren, S., Brady, N., Finestack, L., Bedin-Oja, S.,
(DEC) of the Council of Exceptional Children Fairchild, M., Sokol, S., & Yoder, P. (2006). Early effects
conference held in Minneapolis, MN October 28, of Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching
for children with developmental delays and their parents.
2008. Journal of Speech, Language, Hearing Research, 49, 526–
547.
Declaration of interest: The authors report no Goldstein, H. (2002). Communication intervention for
conflicts of interest. The authors alone are respon- children with autism: A review of treatment efficacy.
sible for the content and writing of this paper. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 32, 373–
396.
Iacono, T., & Duncum, J. (1995). Comparison of sign alone
and in combination with an electronic communicataion
Notes device in early language intervention: Case study.
Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 11, 249–
1 Board Builder (a precursor to Boardmaker) is a registered 259.
trademark of Dynavox Mayer-Johnson, 2100 Wharton Kouri, T. (1988). Effects of simultaneous communication in
St., Suite 400, Pittsburgh, PA 15203, USA. a child-directed treatment approach with preschoolers
2 Picture Communication Symbols (PCS) is a registered with severe disabilities. Augmentative and Alternative
trademark of Dynavox Mayer-Johnson, 2100 Wharton Communication, 4, 222–232.
St., Suite 400, Pittsburgh, PA 15203, USA. Light, J. (1989). Toward a definition of communicative
3 Blissymbols are available from Blissymbolics Communi- competence by individuals using augmentative and
cation International, Suite 104, 1630 Lawrence Ave. West, alternative communication systems. Augmentative and
Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Alternative Communication, 5, 137–144.
4 Makaton is a registered trademark of The Makaton Light, J., & Drager, K. (2007). AAC technologies for young
Charity, Manor House, 46 London Road, Surrey, UK. children with complex communication needs: State of the
282 D. BRANSON AND M. DEMCHAK

science and future research directions. Augmentative and Slavin, R. (1986). Best-evidence synthesis: An alternative to
Alternative Communication, 23, 204–216. meta-analysis and traditional reviews. Educational Re-
Mirenda, P. (2003). Toward functional augmentative and searcher, 15, 5–11.
alternative communication for students with autism: Sprinthall, R. (2003). Basic statistical analysis (7th edn).
Manual signs, graphic symbols, and voice output com- Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
munication aids. Language, Speech and Hearing Services Tait, K., Sigafoos, J., Woodyatts, G., O’Reilly, M., &
in Schools, 34, 203–216. Lancioni, G. (2004). Evaluating parent use of
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child functional communication training to replace and en-
(2007). The timing and quality of early experiences hance prelinguistic behaviours in six children with
combine to shape brain architecture: Working Paper #5. developmental and physical disabilities. Disability and
Retrieved 12 February 2009 from: http://www.develop Rehabilitation: An International, Multidisciplinary Jour-
ingchild.net nal, 26, 1241–1254.
Pennington, L., & McConachie, H. (1999). Mother-child von Tetzchner, S., & Martinsen, H. (1992). Introduction to
interaction revisited: Communication with non-speaking symbolic and augmentative communication. San Diego:
physically disabled children. International Journal of Singular.
Language and Communication Disorders, 34, 391–416. White, H. (1994). Scientific communication and literature
Raghavendra, P. (2000). Evidence-based practice: Where retrieval. In H. Cooper & L. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook
are we in AAC? Paper presented at the 9th biennial of research synthesis (pp. 42–53). New York: Russell Sage
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

conference of the International Society for Augmen- Foundation.


tative and Alternative Communication, Washington, DC. Yoder, P., & Stone, W. (2006). A randomized comparison of
Romski, M., & Sevcik, R. (2005). Augmentative commu- the effect of two prelinguistic communication interven-
nication and early intervention: Myths and realities. tions on the acquistion of spoken communication in
Infants & Young Children: An Interdisciplinary Journal preschoolers with ASD. Journal of Speech, Language,
of Special Care Practices, 18, 174–185. Hearing Research, 49, 698–711.
Romski, M., Sevcik, R., Adamson, L., Smith, A., & Yoder, P., & Warren, S. (1998). Maternal responsivity
Cheslock, M. (2006). Toddlers, parent-implemented predicts the prelinguistic communication intervention
augmented language interventions,and communication de- that facilitates generalized intentional communication.
velopment. Paper presented at the 12th biennial conference Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 41,
of the International Society for Augmentative and Alter- 1207–1219.
native Communication, Dusseldorf, Germany.
Sameroff, A., & Fiese, B. (2000). Transactional regulation:
For personal use only.

The developmental ecology of early intervention. In J.


Shonkoff & S. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook of early child-
hood intervention (2nd edn, pp. 135–159). Cambridge: APPENDIX A
Cambridge University Press. Hand-Searched Journals
Schlosser, R. (2003). Roles of speech output in augmentative
and alternative communication: Narrative review. Aug- American Association for the Education of the
mentative & Alternative Communication, 19, 5–27. Severely/Profoundly Handicapped
Schlosser, R., Koul, R., & Costello, J. (2007). Asking
well-built questions for evidence-based practice in aug- American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology
mentative and alternative communication. Journal of Assistive Technology
Communication Disorders, 40, 225–238. Augmentative and Alternative Communication
Schlosser, R., & Lee, D. (2000). Promoting generalization Exceptional Children
and maintenance in augmentative and alternative com- Exceptionality
munication: A meta-analysis of 20 years of effectiveness
research. Augmentative & Alternative Communication, 16, Focus on Autism and other Developmental Dis-
208–226. orders
Schlosser, R., & Raghavendra, P. (2004). Evidence-based Infants & Young Children: An Interdisciplinary
practice in augmentative and alternative communication. Journal of Special Care Practices
Augmentative & Alternative Communication, 20, 1–21. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
Schlosser, R., Wendt, O., Angermeier, K., & Shetty, M. (2005).
Searching for evidence in augmentative and alternative Handicaps
communication: Navigating a scattered literature. Aug- Journal of the Association for the Severely
mentative and Alternative Communication, 21, 233–255. Handicapped
Scruggs, T., Mastropieri, M., & Casto, G. (1987). The Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
quantitative analysis of single subject research methodol- Journal of Communication Disorders
ogy: Methodology and validation. Remedial and Special
Education, 8, 24–33. Journal of Early Intervention
Scruggs, T., Mastropieri, M., Cook, S., & Escobar, C. Journal of Special Education Technology
(1986). Early intervention for children with conduct Journal of Speech, Language, Hearing Research
disorders: A quantitative synthesis of single-subject Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the
research. Behavioral Disorders, Volume 8. 260–270. Schools
Sigafoos, J., Didden, R., Schlosser, R., Green, V., O’Reilly,
M., & Lancioni, G. (2008). A review of intervention Research and Practice for Persons with Severe
studies on teaching AAC to individuals who are deaf and Disabilities
blind. Journal of Physical Disabilities, 20, 71–99. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education
AAC USE WITH INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH DISABILITIES 283

APPENDIX B
Operational Definitions for Coding the Studies

Coding category Operational definition

Study identification Record the names of the authors of the study and the year it was conducted or published.
Goal of the study List the AAC outcome for the study.
Participants Record the following for each study: (a) number of participants, (b) each participant’s age, (c) each
participant’s gender, (d) each participant’s disability classification.
Target of intervention Record the primary target of the intervention: (a) child, (b) communication partner (parent, teacher, peer).
Intervention design For group designs: (a) randomized pre-test/post-test control group, (b) one group pretest/post-test. For
single-subject designs: (a) single-participant, alternating treatment design; (b) single-participant, multiple
baseline designs (across behaviours or settings); (c) single-participant withdrawal design; (d) multiple
participant, multiple baseline.
Setting Record where the intervention occurred: (a) home, (b) childcare, (c) clinic, (d) pre-school.
AAC system Record all types of AAC system(s) used in the intervention according to the following categories: (a)
unaided AAC (i.e., communication system that only uses the communicator’s body, such as manual
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

signs); (b) aided AAC systems without voice output (e.g., communication board, PECS); (c) aided AAC
systems with voice output (i.e., electronic AAC systems that produce either digitized or synthetic
speech).
Treatment fidelity Record measures used to ensure treatment fidelity: (a) documentation of training for treatment providers,
(b) documentation of observation of treatment sessions by trained observers.
Interrater reliability Record measures used to ensure reliability of coding treatment data.
Effect of intervention Calculate the effect of AAC intervention using data provided by the study. For single-subject experimental
designs, report the percentage of nonoverlapping data (PND; Scruggs et al., 1987). Calculate the PND
by dividing the total number of data points in the intervention that do not overlap the data points in
baseline by the total number of data points in the intervention and multiply by 100. For studies with
multiple participants, calculate the mean
For personal use only.

APPENDIX C
Study 2: Chen, Klein, & Haney (2007)
Summary of Experimental Studies of AAC use in
Early Intervention Participants. 25 infants (8–33 months) with multi-
ple disabilities and sensory impairments, and their
Study 1: Anderson (2001)
caregivers.
Participants. 5 participants (23–35 months): 1 boy Type of AAC. Unaided (Vocalizations and body
(35 months) with PDD; 4 with Autism (2 girls: 34 movements).
and 23 months; 2 boys: 27 and 31 months). Dependent variables and effect size. Caregiver use
Type of AAC. Signs and PECS using pictures of cues; child use of intentional communication
from the program Board Builder. acts; effect size measure not possible based on
Dependent variables and effect size. Requesting design.
toys and foods using PECS: PND 60 (Q); Method. Five curriculum modules developed and
Requesting toys and food using signs: PND 0 (I) field-tested with two cohorts of early interven-
Method. Compared effectiveness of PECS and tionists who then implemented them with
manual signs. Treatment sessions, (3 times a families. First cohort: four half-day training
week, 90 min each, about 10 weeks) alternated sessions (observing video segments; role plays;
between PECS and sign conditions. Simultaneous progress reports; practice explaining concepts
modeling of vocal label with AAC system and demonstrating strategies; and coaching).
targeted for that session. Child-directed strategies Replication cohort: two separate 1-day training
used. Learning trial occurred when the child sessions. Implementation of the curriculum with
initiated a communicative attempt. families ranged from 6 to 21 months (M ¼ 13.8
Findings. All children mastered more items in months).
PECS than in sign condition; rate of acquisition Findings. Total number of cues and events cued
faster in PECS for majority of children. higher following use of PLAI curriculum. 19 of
Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Alternating the 24 caregivers reported an increase in child’s
treatment, multiple baseline probe design ensures communication initiations.
experimental control, inter-observer agreement Design and appraisal. Inconclusive: Pre-experi-
over 80%, and treatment integrity measures and mental design used; child outcome data limited to
data presented. anecdotal reports by caregivers.
284 D. BRANSON AND M. DEMCHAK

the setting as needed, therapists waited for,


Study 3: DiCarlo, Stricklin, Banajee, & Reid
prompted, and responded to nonverbal commu-
(2001)
nicative efforts consistent with perceived intent.
Participants. 12 children with disabilities, 12 Children averaged a total of 80 PMT sessions
children without disabilities (15 to 36 months). over 6 months.
Disabilities included autism, Down syndrome, Findings. Children in RE/PMT group produced
and cerebral palsy. significantly more intentional communicative acts
Type of AAC. Manual signs to request items (free than children in the no-treatment group.
play, art). VOCA present, but not dependent Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Randomized
variable. control group treatment design provides experi-
Dependent variables and effect size. Intervals with mental control, detailed treatment integrity and
sign: PND 33 (I); frequency of sign: PND 29 (I). interrater observer procedures and data.
Method. Children in both groups attended 2 half-
day sessions per week. Treatment occurred over
Study 5: Iacono and Duncum (1995)
25 sessions. Baseline and treatment conditions
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

included use of environmental arrangements to Participants. 1 girl (32 months), Down syndrome.
facilitate communicative attempts and teacher Type of AAC. Signs and VOCA. Assessing
modeling communicative behaviors (vocal and spontaneous and responsive communicative acts.
VOCA models). During treatment the teacher Dependent variables and effect size. Total number
also used simultaneous vocal and sign models of of words produced, sign only: PND 0 (I). Total
key concepts and words. number of words produced, sign þ VOCA: PND
Findings. Small increase in frequency of sign use 66 (Q). Number of different words, sign only:
during signing program; use of verbalizations did PND 0 (I); # of different words, sign þ VOCA:
not decrease for either group of children during PND 83 (F).
signing program. Method. The effectiveness of sign and sign plus an
Design and appraisal. Inconclusive: Multiple electronic communication aid was compared
For personal use only.

baseline design adequate, but treatment integrity using a child-directed approach. Six 30-min
measures missing. treatment sessions alternated between pretend
cooking and dressing up activities using scripts.
Researcher modeled target vocabulary according
Study 4: Fey et al. (2006)
to the treatment condition: in the sign condition all
Participants. 51 children (24–33 months); 25 models were provided using simultaneous produc-
received RE/PMT, 26 in control group. All tion of sign and speech. In the sign þDynavox
identified as having mild to moderate mental condition, both sign þ speech and sign þ Dynavox
retardation; 13 within each group with a diag- productions were randomly presented.
nosis of Down syndrome. Other diagnoses: Findings. Child produced more spontaneous/
Trisomy 8, Mitochondrial disorder, Angelman responsive productions during the sign þ Dyna-
syndrome, Fragile X, cerebrovascular accident at Vox condition than in the sign-only condition.
birth; 17 had developmental delays of unknown Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Alternating
origin. treatment design provided evidence that the
Type of AAC. Gestures, vocalization, gaze shifts sign þ VOCA condition was more effective;
from referent to communication partner. inter-observer reliability 80% or better; treatment
Dependent variables and effect size. Rate of integrity data reported.
intentional communicative acts (i.e., requesting,
commenting, and total rate of intentional acts):
Study 6: Kouri (1998)
Cohen’s d ¼ 0.40–0.68 (medium effect).
Method. Children received both responsive edu- Participants. 2 children (34–36 months), 1 boy (36
cation (RE) and prelinguistic milieu teaching months) with Autism; 1 girl (34 months) with
(PMT) over 6 months. RE: Parents taught in Down syndrome.
eight 1-hr individual sessions by a Hanen-certified Type of AAC. Signs, informal gestures (e.g., head
therapist who used videos and observation to nod, hand claps).
recognize and respond to real or possible child Dependent variable and effect size. Frequency
communicative intents. Parents also read an spontaneous signs (label, request, protest, play):
accompanying book and were assigned tasks to PND 42 (I); frequency responsive signs (e.g., to
do with their child. PMT: Sessions were 4 days the question ‘‘What do you want?’’): PND 60 (Q);
per week for 20 min each (average of 3.32 Frequency informal meaningful gestures or to
weekly). Within on-going routines and arranging augment a linguistic form: PND 86 (F).
AAC USE WITH INFANTS AND TODDLERS WITH DISABILITIES 285

Method. Children received 8 months of interven- Type of AAC. Gestures (i.e., point, nod, shrug),
tion, with both individual and group treatment sign, or vocalization.
sessions occurring 2 times a week. Treatment Dependent variables and effect size. Initiations and
sessions were child-directed. The clinician fol- responsive requests and comments: PND measure
lowed the child’s attentional lead, modeled a sign- not possible.
plus-spoken word whenever the child made eye Method. Explicit prompts: In random order,
contact, and modified the environment to en- trainer used six situations (e.g., bubbles, wind-
courage communication. No response require- up toy, desired item in container) designed to
ments were placed on the child. The only stimulate communication and a multi-level sys-
difference between baseline and treatment ses- tem of prompts (joint attention and expectant
sions was the child-directed focus and the use of a wait; removal of object or stopping activi-
simultaneous model. ty þ verbal prompt; verbal prompt þ visual cue;
Findings. Higher levels of interaction and sign use verbal prompt and visual cue þ physical prompt)
occurred in the sign-plus-speech intervention to elicit intentional requesting and commenting.
phase than during the withdrawal phase. Minimal prompts: As above but no explicit
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Intervention prompting. Trainer reacted to communicative


design (ABAB) provided experimental control; behaviors by commenting and expanding on each
treatment protocol followed; inter-observer relia- act. Sessions were 20–30 min. Number of sessions
bility data over 80%. for each child was between two and six.
Findings. Compared to minimal prompting, all
children used more intentional communication
Study 7: Pennington and McConachie (1999)
under explicit prompt condition with more
Participants. 4 children (32–36 months) and their responsive acts than initiations.
mothers: 1 boy (32 months); 3 girls (34, 35, and 36 Design and appraisal. Inconclusive: Alternating
months) with cerebral palsy. treatment design adequate, but baseline data not
Type of AAC. PCS, Blissymbols, and Makaton reported for any children; no graphed data pro-
For personal use only.

signs. vided; treatment integrity measures not reported.


Dependent Variable and Effect Size. Child’s use of
different communicative functions and modes for
Study 9: Stahmer and Ingersoll (2004)
expressive communication in conversational ex-
changes: Effect size measure not possible based on Participants. 20 children (22–31 months) with
design. autism spectrum disorders (ASD).
Method. Conversation Situation: 10-min mother- Type of AAC. PECS, sign language.
child interaction with pretend play toys shown to Dependent variables and effect size. Evaluating the
elicit a full range of communicative functions. effectiveness of an inclusive preschool program
Mothers asked to ‘‘play and talk as you usually using various measures (functional communica-
do.’’ Script Situation: the researcher played with tion emphasizing requesting, labeling, and shar-
the child using the same toys, but prompted the ing information); effect size measure not possible
children to take an initiating role and attempted based on study design.
to elicit a full range of communicative functions. Method. Focus was on evaluating an inclusive
Findings. Children produced wider range of preschool program for children with ASD.
communicative functions in the semiscripted Children were enrolled in the program 9.5 months
elicitation conversation with clinicians than in on average. Evidenced-based teaching techniques
conversation with their mothers. The 4 children (e.g., discrete trial training, pivotal response
under 3 years old did not use their AAC method training and incidental teaching) used according
in either situation. to individual child’s need. Both signs and PECS
Design and appraisal. Inconclusive: Case study introduced initially; use of one system intensified
design not adequate to provide evidence that once child demonstrated a preference.
communicative script alone accounted for chil- Findings. Significant increase in functional com-
dren’s use of a greater range of communicative munication skills. Upon completion, 90% of
functions. children used a functional communication system
independently (PECS, signs and/or spoken lan-
guage). Children who used PECS or sign combi-
Study 8: Salmon, Rowan, and Mitchell (1998)
nations also acquired spoken words.
Participants. 3 children (17–30 months): 2 girls Design and appraisal. Inconclusive: The pre-
(24 and 17 months) with Down syndrome; 1 girl experimental design used does not provide
(30 months) with agenesis of the corpus callosum. conclusive evidence that children’s increase in
286 D. BRANSON AND M. DEMCHAK

functional communication skills resulted from 25 min each for 60 sessions. Experiment 2:
participating in the inclusive preschool program. Replicated and extended use of the milieu
approach across teachers, setting and materials
to investigate generalization. Training: 4 days a
Study 10: Tait, Sigafoos, Woodyatts, O’Reilly, and
week (25 min per session) for between 37 and 61
Lancioni (2004)
sessions, due to the multiple baseline design.
Participants. 4 children (16–31 months): 1 boy (23 Findings. Experiment 1: Requests and comments
months) with cerebral palsy; 1 girl (16 months) both increased during intervention. Experiment 2:
and 2 boys (26 & 31 months) with cerebral palsy Requesting increased dramatically for all sub-
and other diagnoses (i.e., sensory impairments jects.
and/or epilepsy). Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Multiple base-
Type of AAC. Manual signs, photos, eye gaze line design; targeted behavior only increased
(shift between referent and partner, fix on object), during intervention; interobserver rating data
and graphic symbol for ‘‘yes.’’ averaged above 80%.
Dependent variables and effect size. Request
Augment Altern Commun Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Lakehead University on 03/15/15

‘‘more’’: PND ¼ 96 (H); choice making:


Study 12: Yoder and Warren (1998)
PND ¼ 70 (F); protest: PND ¼ 94 (H); request
‘‘help’’: PND ¼ 100 (H). Participants. 58 children (17–36 months). Devel-
Method. Mothers received 30-min training on opmental disabilities; etiologies varied with 39
how to acknowledge, prompt, and react to having no identifiable etiology or diagnosis.
targeted behaviors. They followed a written plan Type of AAC. Gestures.
and received feedback on child progress and on Dependent variables and effect size. Rate of
implementation of plan (graphed data; reviewing intentional communication acts of requesting
videos). Each received 27 10-min sessions across and focusing joint attention: Cohen’s d ¼ .5–.55
situations (toy play, mealtime, social interaction)/ (Medium effect).
communicative functions, with 2 to 5 monthly Method. Prelinguistic milieu teaching (PMT):
For personal use only.

follow-up sessions. Sessions (20 min) 4 times per week for 6 months.
Findings. Probes across communicative functions Focus on establishing play routines with turn-
demonstrated higher use of target replacement taking around themes (e.g., peek-a-boo); adult
behaviors following intervention. withheld turn, prompted child as needed to
Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Multiple base- produce targeted behavior to request, then moved
line design demonstrated targeted behaviors only to increasing child’s need to draw trainers’
increased following intervention; inter-observer attention to child’s focus of interest and attention.
ratings were over 80%; treatment integrity data Findings. Children in PMT group used more
reported. frequent intentional communication than children
in control group (trainer only responded to
children’s communication without any demands
Study 11: Warren, Yoder, Gazdag, Kim, and Jones
placed on child, no imitation of child’s motor or
(1993)
vocal acts).
Participants. Experiment 1: 1 boy (20) with Down Design and appraisal. Conclusive: Randomized
syndrome. Experiment 2: 1 boy (23 months) with Control Group Treatment design demonstrated
Down syndrome; 1 girl (29 months) and 2 boys PMT increases the rate of intentional commu-
(26 months, 30 months) with various etiologies. nication acts; treatment integrity procedures in
Type of AAC. Experiment 1: Gestures, signs. place; inter-observer ratings over 80%.
Experiment 2: Gestures, signs.
Dependent variables and effect size. Experiment 1; Note. Percentage of Non-overlapping Data
Requests: PND ¼ 92 (H); comments: PND ¼ 100 (PND) is interpreted as follows I ¼ ineffective
(H). Experiment 2; Requests PND ¼ 96 (H) (5 50); Q ¼ questionable (50–70); F ¼ fairly effec-
Comments: PND ¼ 93 (H). tive (70–90); H ¼ highly effective (4 90) (Scruggs
Method. Experiment 1: Prelinguistic requesting et al., 1986). PECS, Picture Exchange Communica-
was targeted using modified milieu teaching tion System; PDD, Pervasive Developmental Dis-
approach; training occurred 4 days a week for orders; VOCA, Voice Output Communication Aid.

You might also like