Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/13629564

Consequences of self-handicapping: Effects on coping, academic


performance, and adjustment

Article  in  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology · July 1998


DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1619 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
167 3,496

3 authors, including:

Miron Zuckerman C. Raymond Knee


University of Rochester University of Houston
110 PUBLICATIONS   8,299 CITATIONS    69 PUBLICATIONS   3,947 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Religious cognition project View project

All content following this page was uploaded by C. Raymond Knee on 21 February 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1998, Vol. 74, No. 6, 1619-1628 0022-3514/98/$3.00

Consequences of Self-Handicapping: Effects on Coping, Academic


Performance, and Adjustment
Miron Zuckerman and Suzanne C. Kieffer C. Raymond Knee
University of Rochester University of Houston

Self-handicappers erect impediments to performance to protect their self-esteem. The impediments


may interfere with the ability to do well and, as such, may result in poor adjustment. Using a
longitudinaldesign, the present studies examined prospective effects of self-handicappingon coping,
academic performance, and several adjustment-relatedvariables (e.g., self-esteem). It was found
that, compared to low self-handicappers, high self-handicappers reported higher usage of coping
strategies implying withdrawal and negative focus. High self-handicappersperformed less well aca-
demically, an effect that was mediated in part by poor study habits. Finally, high self-handicapping
resulted in poorer adjustment over time, and poorer adjustment resulted in higher self-handicapping
over time. These relations are consistent with the idea of a vicious cycle in which self-handicapping
and poor adjustment reinforce one another.

Self-handicaps are obstacles to successful performance that quired self-handicaps (e.g., consuming alcohol) and the term
are constructed by a person to protect or enhance self-esteem self-reported handicapping to claimed self-handicaps (e.g., re-
(Berglas & Jones, 1978; Jones & Berglas, 1978). The obstacles porting high social anxiety). They also argued that behavioral
may interfere with performance but allow the person to discount self-handicaps are more costly in that they actually lower
responsibility for failure and augment credit for success (Kelley, chances for success but also are more convincing and less dis-
1971). If one fails, attribution to poor ability can be discounted putable than their self-reported counterparts. In support of this
because of the presence of another potential cause (the obsta- reasoning, Hirt, Deppe, and Gordon ( 1991 ) found that research
cle). If one succeeds, attribution to ability can be augmented participants predisposed to engage in self-handicapping pre-
because the good performance occurred despite the presence of ferred to use the less costly self-reported handicaps over the
the obstacle. behavioral self-handicaps when both were available.
Over 15 years of research have provided ample illustrations Aiming to study self-handicapping as a disposition, Jones and
of when and how self-handicapping occurs. The impetus for Rhodewalt ( 1982; see also Rhodewalt, 1990) developed the Self-
self-handicapping behavior is usually some threat to self-esteem Handicapping Scale (SHS). The SHS is a 25-item questionnaire
(Snyder & Smith, 1982). Behaviors fulfilling the function of that samples a large variety of self-handicapping behaviors (e.g.,
self-handicapping include drug and alcohol consumption (Ber- "I tend to put things off to the last minute" ). Internal reliability
glas & Jones, 1978; Higgins & Harris, 1988; Kolditz & Arkin, (Cronbach's alpha) of the scale was .79, and test-retest reliabil-
1982; Tucker, Vuchinich, & Sobell, 1981), decrease in practice ity over 1 month was .74 (see Rhodewalt, 1990, p. 75). Those
(Baumeister, Hamilton, & Tice, 1985; Rhodewalt, Saltzman, & scoring high on the SHS appear more likely to use both behav-
Wittmer, 1984), reduction in effort (Pyszczynski & Greenberg, ioral and self-reported handicaps when facing threats to self-
1983), and choice of debilitating performance settings (Rhode- esteem (Rhodewalt & Fairfield, 1989, as cited in Rhodewalt,
walt & Davison, 1986; Shepperd & Arkin, 1989). Other research 1990; Rhodewalt et al., 1984; Strube & Roemmele, 1985). For
has shown that self-handicapping also can be accomplished by example, Rhodewalt et al. found that intercollegiate swimmers
reports of test anxiety (Smith, Snyder, & Handelsman, 1982), and professional golfers with high SHS scores (compared to
social anxiety (Snyder, Smith, Augelli, & Ingram, 1985), bad those with low SHS scores) were more likely to withhold prac-
mood (Baumgardner, Lake, & Arkin, 1985), and traumatic life tice effort in the face of upcoming important competitions.
events (DeGree & Snyder, 1985).
In subsequent work, the SHS was abridged to form a 10-
The various forms of self-handicaps were classified by Arkin
item scale (Strube, 1985) and, alternatively, a 14-item scale
and Baumgardner (1985) into acquired obstacles (obstacles
(Rhodewalt, 1990). These two shorter versions are similar in
that actually lower the likelihood of success) and claimed obsta-
that all 10 items from the form recommended by Strube are
cles (obstacles that people claim to have). Leary and Shepperd
included in Rhodewalt's 14-item scale. (However, Strube short-
(1986) assigned the term behavioral self-handicapping to ac-
ened an earlier version of the SHS; consequently, his 10 items
appear in the 14-item scale in a slightly modified form.) To the
best of our knowledge, there have been no additional data on
Miron Zuckermanand Suzanne C. Kieffer, Departmentof Psychology,
the psychometric qualities of the various forms of the SHS. In
Universityof Rochester; C. Raymond Knee, Department of Psychology,
Universityof Houston. the present studies, we took a further look at the factor structure
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Miron of the SHS and examined the viability of a shorter version of
Zuckerrnan,Departmentof Psychology, Universityof Rochester, Roches- the scale.
ter, New York 14627. However, the primary goal of this investigation was to exam-

1619
1620 ZUCKERMAN, KIEFFER, AND KNEE

ine the influence of self-handicapping, as measured by the SHS, the expense of problem-focused coping (successful perfor-
on well-being. The SHS can be viewed as a gauge of habitual mance). Stated differently, self-handicappers aim to maintain
excuse making, the excuse being either an actual obstacle to positive feelings about themselves at the cost of dealing effec-
goal achievement or an obstacle that the person merely claims tively with the situation. It follows that people high on the SHS
to have (Arkin & Baumgardner, 1985). Admittedly, this view will be more likely to report use of emotion-focused strategies,
is somewhat of a departure from the traditional definition of particularly those that inhibit movement toward actual goals (cf.
self-handicapping as strategies designed to protect self-esteem Carver, Scheier, & Pozo, 1992). For example, the high self-
in the face of ego threat. However, habitual self-handicapping handicapper may deny the existence of a stressful situation or
is consistent with Rhodewalt's (1990) interpretation of the SHS: disengage from it. Such coping mechanisms may be part and
" T h e chronic procrastinator and the 'habitually ill' hypochon- parcel of an overall effort to protect self-evaluation even while
driac have 'incorporated' those traits into their self-conceptions, neglecting actual situational demands.
and they will admit to them on the S H S " (p. 92). The relation between the SHS and academic performance was
If the high self-handicapper makes excuses by erecting imped- examined as a follow-up of Rhodewalt's (1990) results. It was
iments to performance, he or she cannot achieve as much as expected that after potential ability (as measured by Scholastic
the low self-handicapper. Interestingly, the impact of person- Aptitude Test [SAT] scores) was controlled for, high SHS stu-
initiated handicaps on performance has not been clearly estab- dents would earn lower grades than low SHS students. Grades
lished. Existing research has found either performance benefits depend in part on the quality of studying, and self-handicappers
(e.g., Rhodewalt & Davison, 1986; Rhodewalt et al., 1984, are expected to study less well as a way of protecting their
Study 2), no effects (e.g., Harris & Snyder, 1986; Rhodewalt self-esteem against poor performance. To examine whether the
et al., 1984, Study 1), or performance decrements (e.g., relation between the SHS and underachievement is in fact medi-
Springston & Chafe, 1987). A rationale for the performance ated by less efficient studying, we also measured the amount of
benefits of self-handicapping was proposed by Snyder (1990). time participants spent doing academic work and the efficiency
He argued that by lessening the impact of poor performance on of their study habits.
self-evaluation, self-handicapping allows the person to focus on
the task and not on potentially distracting self-relevant cues. Method
Although performance on a specific task may be helped by
self-handicapping, it is difficult to see how long-term achieve- Participants
ment and well-being would be affected. Given that the high Two hundred sixty-two undergraduate students (181 women and 81
self-handicapper is thought to employ a mixture of behavioral men) participated in partial fulfillment of an introductory course require-
excuses, ranging from deleterious health practices to inept at- ment. All participants filled out a battery of questionnaires near the
tempts at problem-solving, the cumulative effect of such tenden- beginning (Time 1) and again near the end (Time 2) of the fall semester.
cies should be anything but beneficial. Consistent with this anal- Approximately 2 months separated Time 1 and Time 2. Participants
ysis, Rhodewalt (1990) reported a substantial relation (r = attended the sessions in small groups.
- . 4 3 ) between high SHS scores and underachievement. ~ To the In addition to the participants listed above, 12 undergraduates filled
best of our knowledge, no one else has examined how self- out the questionnaires at Time 1 but not at Time 2. Reasons for failure
to attend the Time 2 sessions included participants' leaving early for the
handicapping, as a dispositional variable, influences achieve-
semester break, finishing the course requirement with other experiments,
ment or well-being. dropping the introductory psychology course, and our being unable to
We conducted two studies to investigate the relation between contact the participants despite repeated phone calls. The analyses re-
self-handicapping, as measured by the SHS, and several mea- ported below are based on data from the 262 undergraduates who partici-
sures of adjustment. In both studies, we used a longitudinal pated in both sessions.
design, allowing a test of the prospective effects of SHS scores
on all other variables. In the first study, we examined the relation Measures
between the SHS and measures of coping and academic achieve-
ment. In Study 2, we replicated and extended the findings from SHS. Participants filled out the 25-item SHS (Rhodewalt, 1990) at
Time 1 and Time 2.
Study 1.
Coping. At both Time 1 and Time 2, participants filled out a coping
inventory titled the Coping Operations Preference Enquiry (COPE;
Study 1 Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989) and two coping subscales added
to the COPE by the present authors. The COPE consists of a series of
Coping and academic achievement are natural choices for a four-item subscales, each measuring a specific coping strategy. Some
study on the consequences of self-handicapping. With respect
to coping, self-handicapping appears relevant to a central con-
struct in the coping l i t e r a t u r e I t h e distinction between prob- 1 Rhodewalt (1990) reported that underachievement was operation-
lem-focused and emotion-focused strategies (Lazarus & Folk- alized as a ratio between grade point average (GPA) and Scholastic
man, 1984). Although both types of coping strategies represent Aptitude Test (SAT) score. To create an interval scale, he further modi-
fied the ratio by the formula 1 + log (GPA/SAT x 1,000). Cohen and
attempts to manage stressful situations, problem-focused coping
Cohen (1983) noted that if the components of a ratio variable are not
is aimed at changing the cause of the distress whereas emotion- themselves ratio scales (having true zeros and equal intervals ), "correla-
focused coping is aimed at managing the affect associated with tions involving ratios should probably be avoided altogether, and an
the distress. If self-handicappers erect obstacles to performance alternative m e t h o d . . , should be chosen, such as partial correlations"
as a means of protecting self-esteem, they can be viewed as (p. 75). Thus, the results reported by Rhodewalt should be considered
engaging in emotion-focused coping (protecting self-esteem) at with a degree of caution.
CONSEQUENCES OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1621

strategies (e.g., active coping, planning) are clearly problem-focused, were conducted after we reversed relevant items such that higher
whereas others (e.g., denial, mental disengagement) are emotion-fo- scores always reflected higher self-handicapping. The results
cused. Two additional coping strategies, self-blame and self-focused indicated that 11 items could be deleted due to low i t e m - t o t a l
rumination, seemed relevant to our purpose but were not covered by correlations or low Ioadings on the first unrotated factor. The
the COPE. Self-blame, a tendency to criticize oneself for negative events,
analyses were then repeated for the remaining 14 items. The
appears in several classifications of coping, including the various ver-
varimax rotation yielded three factors with eigenvalues greater
sions of Folkman and Lazarus's (1980) The Ways of Coping Checklist
(see, e.g., Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Vitaliano, Russo, Carr, Maiuro, & than one. However, a major break occurred between the eigen-
Becker, 1985). Self-focused rumination is a potential response to nega- values of the first and second factors, 3.43 and 1.39, respectively.
tive events that has been implicated in depression (Lyubomirsky & This suggests that the 14 items largely represent a single con-
Nolen-Hoeksema, 1993; Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Fredrickson, struct. Corrected i t e m - t o t a l correlations for the 14 items ranged
1993; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1986). In the present study, we added from .26 to .58 (see Appendix B ) ; loadings on the first unrotated
to the COPE two four-item subscales that assessed these two other factor ranged from .34 to .71; internal reliability ( C r o n b a c h ' s
domains. Participants completed the COPE (including the two additional alpha) for the 14-item scale was .76.
subscales) under trait-like instructions that asked them to indicate what It is reassuring that the resulting 14-item SHS is extremely
they generally do under stress. Response choices ranged from 1 (I usually
similar to the one reported by Rhodewalt (1990). In fact, the
don't do this at all) to 4 (I usually do this a lot).
current 14-item version and his 14-item version of the SHS
Appendix A lists the coping strategies and a sample item from each
subscale. The Study 1 portion of Appendix A presents the internal reli- differ by a single item. The current version includes Item 23
abilities (Cronbach's alphas) of all subscales from Time 1 and Time 2. ( " I never let emotional problems in one part of m y life interfere
With the exception of mental disengagement, internal reliabilities were with things in m y l i f e " ; this item is reverse scored) but not
adequate, ranging from .62 to .90 at Time 1 and from .63 to .87 at Time Item 14 ( " S o m e d a y I might 'get it all together' " ) . Rhodewalt's
2. Cronbach's alphas for mental disengagement were .38 at Time 1 and version included Item 14 but not Item 23. In our data, internal
.44 at Time 2. Carver et al. (1989) also reported somewhat lower reliabil- reliability for Rhodewalt's 14-item version was .74. All subse-
ity for this subscale. The reason, according to those investigators, is that quent analyses are based on the current 14-item version of the
the items describe very different ways of disengaging (e.g., sleeping vs. SHS.
watching TV).
Missing from Appendix A are subscales of three additional coping
strategies: alcohol use-drug use, humor, and use of religion. Alcohol Coping
use-drug use and humor were developed after the publication of the
original COPE in 1989; neither was used consistently in relevant re- Scores for each coping scale from Time 2 were regressed
search (cf. Carver & Scheier, 1994, footnote 1 ). We considered use of onto baseline coping (the corresponding coping score from Time
religion to be irrelevant to the college population. All three subscales 1 ), sex, and SHS (all entered simultaneously in Step 1 ) and on
were included in Study 1 but not in Study 2. None of the three produced SHS x Sex (entered in Step 2). Here and in all subsequent
any results of interest, and they will not be discussed further. analyses, the number of significant interactions involving sex
Academic performance. Participants were asked to sign consent did not exceed what could be expected by chance; consequently,
forms allowing us to obtain from the registrar their SAT scores and
these interactions will not be discussed further. The left portion
grade point averages (GPAs) from both the fall and the spring semester.
of Table 1 presents significant SHS effects obtained in the cur-
This information was collected after the end of the academic year. In
addition, participants were asked at Time 2 to (a) list the courses they rent study. It can be seen that a higher level of SHS was associ-
were taking, (b) note the number of hours per week they were spending ated with increases in denial, behavioral disengagement, mental
on academic work (excluding the time spent in class), and (c) write a disengagement, self-blame, and self-focused rumination. It thus
short description of how they typically prepare for exams. Participants appears that self-handicapping is associated with emotion-
were divided into four groups, and each group's descriptions of exam focused strategies that orient persons away from their goals.
preparations were rated by 10 or It undergraduate judges. The judges
performed the task in partial fulfillment of an introductory course re-
quirement during the spring semester. For each description, the judges Academic Performance
rated on a 7-point scale how efficient the preparations were ( 1 = very GPAs from the fall and spring semester were substantially
inefficient, 7 = very efficient). The ratings were averaged across judges
related ( r = .69), and separate analyses of grades from each
within each of the four groups, yielding mean scores with reliabilities
(Cronbach's alphas) that ranged from .88 to .93. semester yielded almost identical results. We thus averaged the
On the basis of the descriptions of exam preparations, judges also two GPAs and examined the results in a series of regression
predicted the GPA that each participant would likely obtain. Because analyses. First, we regressed the mean GPA on Verbal SAT,
efficiency of exam preparation and predicted GPA were highly correlated Math SAT, sex, and SHS (Step 1) and on the SHS x Sex
(r = .94), only results concerning the former will be reported here. In interaction (Step 2). The results are presented in the left half
addition, the judges rated the difficulty of courses the participants were of Table 2. It can be seen that, after SAT scores were controlled
taking. However, this measure was not related to any other variable of for, SHS and GPAs were significantly related such that higher
interest and will not be discussed further. self-handicapping scores resulted in lower GPA over time.
We also e x a m i n e d whether the relation between self-handi-
capping and underachievement was mediated by study habits.
Results
Recall that participants provided estimates of the time spent per
Constructing a Short SHS week on academic work and that their descriptions of exam
preparations were rated on their degree of efficiency. As ex-
We conducted a factor analysis of the 25 SHS items and also pected, high SHS scores were related to less time spent on
e x a m i n e d the corrected i t e m - t o t a l correlations. These analyses academic work ( r = - . 16, p < .05) and to lower efficiency of
1622 ZUCKERMAN, KIEFFER, AND KNEE

Table 1
Predicting Coping Strategies From Self-Handicapping Scale (SHS) Scores and Self-Esteem

SHS SHS SHS


Without self-esteem Without self-esteem With self-esteem Self-esteem
(Study 1) (Study 2) (Study 2) (Study 2)
Coping strategy F(I, 256) pr F(1, 248) pr F(1,247) pr H I , 247) pr
Denial 17.00"** .25 10.06"* .18 7.77** .17 0.07 -.02
Behavioral disengagement 29.76"** .32 8.61"* .20 5.91" .15 1.04 -.06
Mental disengagement 11.15"* .20 10.65'* .16 6.63* .16 0.82 -.06
Self-blame 8.60"* .18 3.43t .12 0.66 .05 6.23" - . 16
Self-focused rumination 8.02** .17 13.92"** .23 11.59"** .21 0.08 -.02

t p < .10 (marginally significant). *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

exam preparation (r = - . 2 1 , p < .01 ). The next questions to such effects on variables beyond coping and academic achieve-
be examined were (a) whether duration and efficiency correlate ment. In Study 2, accordingly, we examined the prospective
with GPA when SHS is controlled for and (b) whether the effects of SHS on self-esteem, affect, and the number of times
relation between SHS and GPA diminishes when duration and participants (all college students) visited the university health
efficiency are controlled for. Accordingly, the regression analysis service. We also again examined the prospective effects of SHS
of mean GPA was repeated with duration and efficiency as two on coping and academic performance but modified the coping
more predictors that were entered in Step 1. As can be seen in measure to increase the generalizability of the results.
the right-hand side of Table 2, higher scores on duration and A claim that self-handicapping influences adjustment also
efficiency were both significantly related to higher GPA after has to be defended against potential confounds. One immediate
controlling for SAT and SHS scores (as well as for each other). possibility is that self-handicapping effects reflect self-esteem
In addition, the magnitude of the relation between SHS and effects. Indeed, measures of the two constructs typically share
GPA became smaller (pr = - . 2 0 , p < .01, when duration and 20% of their variance. There is ample evidence, however, that
efficiency were not controlled for; pr = - . 1 2 , p = .07, when effects of self-handicapping are obtained even when self-esteem
duration and efficiency were controlled for). Clearly, at least is controlled for (see Rhodewalt, 1990, for a review). Still, it
some of the relation between SHS and GPA was mediated by seemed important to reexamine this issue in the present study as
the study habits that we chose to measure. some potential consequences of self-handicapping (e.g., lower
Overall, the results support the view that the high self-handi- academic achievement) seem natural consequences of lower
capper does not cope well and does not do well academically. self-esteem.
However, questions can be raised as to the validity, generalizabil- The relation between self-handicapping and adjustment also
ity, and implications of both effects. These questions were ad- raises the question of how self-handicapping itself is influenced
dressed in Study 2. by adjustment-related factors. Both self-esteem and affect ap-
pear relevant to this issue. Regarding self-esteem, Snyder and
Study 2 Higgins (1988) suggested that people low in self-esteem are
more uncertain about outcomes and, hence, more likely to self-
If we are to make claims about the effects of self-handicap- handicap. If so, one would expect that low self-esteem would
ping on general adjustment, it will be reassuring to demonstrate result over time in higher dispositional self-handicapping. How-
ever, if self-handicapping leads to lower general adjustment over
time (e.g., poorer academic performance), it may also lead to
Table 2 lower self-esteem. Results confirming prospective effects of
Predicting Grade Point Average From Self-Handicapping each construct on the other would be consistent with the idea
Scale (SHS) Scores With and Without Study Habits (Study 1) that low self-esteem and high self-handicapping create a vicious
cycle of reciprocal reinforcement. On the other hand, if the
Without study habits With study habits
results show prospective effects of self-esteem on self-handicap-
Predictor F(1,218) pr F(1, 216) pr ping but not vice versa, then the theoretical importance of self-
handicapping would be diminished considerably.
Verbal SAT 11.32"** .22 12.93"** .24 Regarding affect and mood, negative affect may induce a
Math SAT 12.62"* .23 15.51"** .26 person to engage in self-handicapping to guard against future
Sexa 2.46 - . 11 0.29 -.04
SHS 8.86** -.20 3.20t - . 12 mishaps and the resulting distress and anxiety. In fact, experi-
Duration -- -- 4.59* .14 menters often use noncontingent success, which supposedly
Efficiency -- -- 10.71 ** .22 arouses anxiety over performance, to elicit self-handicapping.
However, in the current formulation, negative affect due to fail-
Note. SAT = Scholastic Aptitude Test.
"Sex was coded as female = 0, male = 1. ure may also elicit self-handicapping as a means of preventing
t p < .10. (marginally significant). *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p a repetition of this experience. Self-handicapping, in turn, may
< .001. induce poor coping and lower achievement and, as such, may
CONSEQUENCES OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1623

also elicit additional negative affect. That is, to the extent that obtained in Study 1 for the trait-like version of the COPE would general-
self-handicapping guards against loss of self-esteem at the ex- ize to a situational or time-limited version. Accordingly, the enlarged
COPE was administered in a dispositional format at Time 1 (as it was
pense of effective coping, it eventually leads to additional disap-
administered in Study 1 ) and in a situational format at Time 2. Specifi-
pointment and further distress. Results confirming the reciprocal
cally, the participants were first asked at Time 2 to describe their most
relation between self-handicapping and negative affect would stressful experience of the semester and then to indicate their use of
again support the idea of a vicious cycle with higher self-handi- each COPE item with regard to that experience. Response choices for
capping and lower adjustment reinforcing each other. each item again ranged from 1 (1 didn't do this at all) to 4 (l did this
Finally, we also added to Study 2 a behavioral measure of a lot).
symptom r e p o r t i n g - - t h e n u m b e r of times participants visited Internal reliabilities for the COPE are presented in the Study 2 portion
the university health service for the purpose of treating an illness of Appendix A. It can be seen that the reliabilities of the subscales in
(as opposed to preventive care). Because individuals can use Study 2 were very similar to the values obtained in Study 1. Again,
s y m p t o m reporting to protect self-esteem (Mayerson & Rhode- except for mental disengagement, Cronbach's alphas for the COPE sub-
walt, 1988; Smith et al., 1982; Smith, Snyder, & Perkins, 1983), scales ranged from .60 to .90 at Time 1 and .68 to .91 at Time 2. As
before, mental disengagement yielded lower reliability (.43 and .36,
high self-handicappers may be more likely to complain about
respectively ).
sickness c o m p a r e d to low self-handicappers. To the extent that
Remaining measures. At both Time 1 and Time 2, participants filled
these symptoms are perceived as real (as they must be if they out the 25-item SHS (Rhodewalt, 1990) as well as measures of self-
are to serve as an excuse), the high self-handicapper may engage esteem and mood. Self-esteem was assessed by Rosenberg's 10-item
in more health care visits. The visits, in other words, are a Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1979). Mood was assessed by the Posi-
consequence of a continuous search for excuses that include tive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen,
health-related issues. 1988). The PANAS Negative Affect (NA) is a 10-item scale that mea-
sures the extent to which the person experiences negative feeling (e.g.,
irritable, hostile) and the PANAS Positive Affect (PA) is a 10-item scale
Method
that measures experience of positive feeling (e.g., enthusiastic, proud).
Participants At Time 1, participants completed the PANAS scales under long-term
instructions, which directed them to indicate how they feel in general;
The study was conducted a year after the completion of Study 1. Two at Time 2, participants indicated how they felt during the last 3 weeks.
hundred fifty-two undergraduate students (142 women and 110 men) In addition to these scales, participants were asked to sign consent forms
participated in partial fulfillment of an introductory course requirement. allowing us to obtain from the registrar their SAT scores and GPAs for
As in Study 1, participants filled out a battery of questionnaires near the fall and spring semesters and from the university health service the
the beginning and, 2 months later, near the end of the fall semester (Time number of visits for the purpose of treating an illness during the fall
1 and Time 2, respectively). Also as in Study 1, and for similar reasons, and spring semesters.
an additional number of undergraduates (n = 10) attended the Time 1
session but not the Time 2 session. The analyses below are based on
the 252 undergraduates who participated in both sessions. Results

Shortening the SHS: A Replication


Measures
Factor analyses and corrected i t e m - t o t a l correlations of the
Coping. At both Time 1 and Time 2, participants completed an
25 SHS items produced results that were similar to those of
enlarged COPE measure that included all the subscales administered in
Study 1 plus three new ones. The reason for further enlarging the COPE Study 1. The 11 items deleted earlier were again deleted due to
was that this instrument lists problem-focused strategies (e.g., planning) low i t e m - t o t a l correlations or low loadings on the first unrotated
that are mostly viewed as adaptive and emotion-focused strategies (e.g., factor. A varimax rotation of the 14 remaining items produced
denial) that are mostly viewed as maladaptive. In Study 2, we wanted four factors with eigenvalues greater than one. There was again
to employ a more balanced coping inventory. We therefore added three a major break between the first and second factors (3.27 and
emotion-focused strategies that, at least on the surface, appear adaptive 1.62, respectively), suggesting the existence of a single con-
(see Appendix A for sample items), q~vo of these strategies--under- struct. Corrected i t e m - t o t a l correlations for the 14 items ranged
standing emotion and repairing emotion--were derived from the Trait from .22 to .49 (see Appendix B ); loadings on the first unrotated
Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai,
factor ranged from .29 to .66; internal reliability ( C r o n b a c h ' s
1995). The TMMS measures individual differences in attending to one's
alpha) for the 14-item scale was .74. Internal reliability for
feelings, clarity of feelings, and regulation of feelings. In the current
work, the strategy of understanding emotion represents both attention Rhodewalt's ( 1 9 9 0 ) 14-item version was .72. Once again, all
to and clarification of feelings, whereas the strategy of repairing emotion subsequent analyses are based on the current 14-item version
represents mood regulation when facing a negative event. The third of the SHS. (For both Study 1 and Study 2, analyses using
added strategy--expressing emotion--was a modification of an original Rhodewalt's 14-item version of the SHS or the full SHS yielded
subscale of the COPE that has since been deleted. That subscale, focus similar results.)
on and venting of emotion, included one item that represents the expres-
sion of emotion ( "I let my feelings out" ) and three items that represent
the experience and expression of negative affect (e.g., "I get upset and Coping
let my emotions out"). The subscale was eventually deleted from the
COPE because it was too close in content to the symptoms it was Scores from each coping scale were first examined in regres-
supposed to predict. In the current work, the subscale of expressing sion analyses identical to those conducted in Study 1. To test
emotion entails only the display of feelings; none of the items refer to whether self-handicapping effects are independent of those of
the experience or expression of negative affect. self-esteem, we then repeated the analyses with self-esteem
One purpose of the present study was to examine whether the results added as a predictor in Step 1 (along with coping baseline,
1624 ZUCKERMAN, KIEFFER, AND KNEE

SHS, and sex), and Self-Esteem x Sex and Self-Esteem × SHS the prospective effects of self-handicapping on self-esteem and
added as predictors in Step 2 (along with SHS x Sex). The affect and vice versa.
results are presented in the right portion of Table 1. We conducted two regression analyses to examine the pro-
Without self-esteem in the equation, a higher level of SHS spective effects of SHS and self-esteem on one another. In the
was again associated with increases in denial, behavioral disen- first analysis, self-esteem from Time 2 was regressed onto the
gagement, mental disengagement, self-blame (this relation only baseline (self-esteem from Time 1 ), sex, and SHS (Step 1 ) and
approached significance), and self-focused rumination. With then on the SHS x Sex interaction (Step 2). Results showed
self-esteem in the equation, SHS retained its significant effects that higher SHS scores at Time 1 led to a reduction in self-
on four coping strategies; self-blame was no longer influenced esteem over time, F ( 1 , 250) = 9.50, p < .01, pr = - . 1 9 . In
by SHS. Interestingly, self-blame was also the only strategy the second analysis, SHS from Time 2 was regressed onto the
influenced by self-esteem (see the right-hand column in Table baseline (SHS from Time 1 ), sex, and self-esteem (Step 1 ) and
1 ) and by a Self-Esteem x SHS interaction, F ( 1 , 2 4 5 ) = 5.76, on the Self-Esteem × Sex interaction (Step 2). The results
p < .05; however, this interaction could be easily obtained by showed that lower self-esteem scores at Time 1 led to an in-
chance alone. crease in self-handicapping over time, F ( 1 , 2 5 0 ) = 3.77, p <
SHS was not associated with any other coping strategy, in- .05, pr = -.12.
cluding the more "positive" emotion-focused strategies such To examine the prospective effects of self-handicapping and
as expressing, understanding, and repairing emotion, all Fs < affect on one another, we repeated the above analyses except
1. Once again, therefore, it appears that self-handicapping is that the PANAS NA and PANAS PA replaced self-esteem as
related to emotion-focused strategies that are also avoidance dependent variables, baselines, and predictors. The analysis with
oriented. the PANAS NA as the dependent variable showed that higher
SHS scores at Time 1 resulted in more negative affect over time,
A c a d e m i c Performance F ( 1 , 2 1 3 ) = 7.69, p < .01, pr = .19; this effect was maintained
even when self-esteem was added to the equation as another
As GPAs from fall and spring were again related (r = .62), predictor, F ( 1 , 212) = 5.76, p < .05, pr = .16. The analysis
they were averaged and examined in a regression analysis identi- with the PANAS PA as the dependent variable did not yield a
cal to the one conducted in Study 1. The analysis was then significant effect for SHS, either without or with self-esteem as
repeated with self-esteem added as a predictor in Step 1 (along a predictor, F ( 1 , 2 1 3 ) = 1.63, F ( 1 , 2 1 2 ) = 1.75, respectively;
with the two SAT scores, SHS, and sex) and with Self-Esteem however, the effect was in the expected direction (higher SHS
x Sex and Self-Esteem x SHS added as predictors in Step 2 resulting in lower positive affect). Self-esteem did not influence
(along with SHS x Sex). The results are presented in Table 3. either the PANAS N A scores, F < 1.15, or the PANAS PA
Without self-esteem in the equation, higher levels of SHS were scores, F < 1.
once again associated with lower GPA; this association re- The analysis with SHS scores as the dependent variable
mained significant even when self-esteem was added to the equa- showed that both the PANAS NA and the PANAS PA (entered
tion. Self-esteem and GPA were not related, and none of the together and thus controlling for each other's effect) influenced
two-way interactions were significant, p s > . 16. Overall, these self-handicapping. Higher PANAS NA scores and lower PANAS
results replicate those obtained in Study 1. PA scores at Time 1 resulted in higher SHS scores over time,
F ( 1 , 2 1 2 ) = 5.10, p < .05, pr = .15, and F ( 1 , 2 1 2 ) = 4.88,
Self-Esteem, Mood, and Visits to the Health Service p < .05, pr = - . 15, respectively. Thus, self-handicapping seems
to reinforce both low self-esteem and negative affect but, in
Zero-order correlations between self-handicapping, self-es-
turn, is also reinforced by both constructs (as well as by low
teem, and affect are presented in Table 4. It can be seen that,
positive affect).
for both Time 1 and Time 2, the SHS was negatively related to
Finally, the number of times participants went to the university
self-esteem and the PANAS PA and positively related to the
health service to receive treatment for an illness was examined
PANAS NA. More important from the current perspective are
in a regression analysis with sex and SHS scores as predictors
in Step 1 and SHS x Sex as a predictor in Step 2. Higher SHS
scores at Time 1 resulted in a higher number of visits during
Table 3 the fall semester, F ( 1 , 2 3 3 ) = 3.79, p = .05, pr = .13, and the
Predicting Grade Point Average From Self-Handicapping effect largely remained even when self-esteem was added as
Scale (SHS) Scores With and Without Self-Esteem (Study 2) another predictor, F ( 1 , 2 3 3 ) = 3.18, p = .07, pr = . 12. Self-
esteem and visits to the health service were not related, F < 1.
Without self-esteem With self-esteem Finally, there was no relation between SHS from Time 1 and
the number of visits to the health service during the spring
Predictor F(1, 219) pr F(1,218) pr semester.
Verbal SAT 17.53"* .27 16.68"* .27
Math SAT 8.46** .19 8.44* .19 General Discussion
Sexa 6.54* -.17 6.53* -.17
SHS 5.11" -.15 3.93* -.13 The present studies examined the relation of dispositional
Self-esteem -- -- 0.03 .01
self-handicapping with coping, academic performance, and sev-
a Sex was coded as female = 0, male = 1. eral measures of adjustment. The designs were longitudinal in
*p <.05. **p <.01. nature such that the relations that were obtained indicate pro-
CONSEQUENCES OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1625

Table 4
Correlations Between Self-Handicapping, Self-Esteem, and Affect (Study 2)
1 2 3 4

Scale Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2 Time 1 Time 2

1. SHS -- -- -.43 -.41 .50 .44 -.46 -.33


2. Self-Esteem -- -- -.49 -.38 .35 .38
3. PANAS NA -- -- -.37 -.31
4. PANAS PA -- --

Note. SHS = Self-Handicapping Scale; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; PA = Positive
Affect; NA = Negative Affect. All correlations were significant, ps < .01.

spective effects of self-handicapping on the variable of interest. (Mdn = .34) in Study 1 and from .04 to .36 (Mdn = .27) in
It was found that high self-handicappers reported higher usage Study 2. To explain the apparent paradox, Scheier et al. ( 1989,
of coping strategies associated with withdrawal and negative footnote 9) suggested that focusing on negative aspects of one's
self-focus, performed less well academically, and scored lower experience may lead to more active attempts to suppress it. In
on several adjustment-related variables. Most of the findings the same vein, people who self-handicap may do so precisely
were replicated across two studies and, therefore, appear robust. because they are prone to dwell on the negative aspects of their
All the analyses were based on a short 14-item form of the experience.
SHS that was identified in both studies and is also similar to Turning now to academic achievement, it was found that after
Rhodewalt's (1990) 14-item version. Nevertheless, we believe SAT scores were controlled for, higher self-handicappers earned
that future investigators should administer the full 25-item SHS lower grades than did low self-handicappers. Although our mea-
and conduct their own analyses to confirm the viability of the sures of coping were based on self-reports, GPA is a perfor-
present short form of the scale. mance variable and as such provides hard evidence that the
To assess coping, we used either a dispositional (Study 1 ) or practice of self-handicapping puts the person at risk. That is,
a situational (Study 2) measure. In both cases, high SHS scores self-handicappers may indeed accrue attributional benefits but,
were related to an increase in emotion-focused strategies that at least in the current studies, they paid for such benefits with
imply withdrawal (denial, mental disengagement, and behav- poor academic performance.
ioral disengagement) and negative focus (rumination). The rela- The relation between SHS and GPA was mediated in part by
tion of SHS with a fifth strategy, self-blame, disappeared when study habits. Specifically, it was shown that (a) high SHS scores
self-esteem was held constant. It is important to note that SHS were related to shorter and less efficient exam preparations, (b)
was not related to emotion-focused strategies that imply a more shorter and less efficient exam preparation were each related to
positive focus (such as positive reinterpretation and repairing lower GPAs after SHS scores were controlled for, and (c) the
emotion). It thus appears that self-handicapping results in a relation between SHS and GPA was weaker when duration and
dysfunctional coping style. It should be acknowledged that a efficiency of exam preparations were held constant. Because
contextual approach to coping suggests that no coping strategy we used self-reports to assess study habits, correspondence of
is universally good or bad and that coping outcomes depend on our measures with actual behavior is suspect. For example, it
the particular circumstances that are involved. However, even is possible that high self-handicappers do not work less before
Lazarus (1993), a strong proponent of this view, admitted that exams but report having done so to protect self-esteem. Because
some coping strategies "might more often be better or worse both self-reported duration and efficiency of exam preparation
than others" (p. 234). In empirical studies, denial and disen- were related to actual grades, it may be argued that they must
gagement have been associated with poorer health outcomes reflect some actual exam preparation. On the other hand, it can
(Aldwin & Revenson, 1987) and with lower optimism and also be argued that those who anticipate lower grades report
higher anxiety (Carver et al., 1989). As previously noted, self- worse study habits as a self-handicapping technique. However,
focused rumination has been linked with depression (Nolen- if actual study habits do not account for the relation between
Hoeksema et al., 1993). self-handicapping and lower grades, it is not clear what does.
The reader may wonder about the relation of SHS with self- Stated differently, it is more parsimonious to infer that high
focused rumination. After all, self-handicapping is designed to self-handicappers prepared a behavioral excuse (they actually
protect the person from the negative implications that rumina- studied less well) that resulted in a lower GPA than to infer
tion entails. Accordingly, self-handicapping style might be ex- that high self-handicappers only said they studied less well and
pected to correlate negatively with this strategy. However, a that something else accounted for their lower GPA.
common finding in the coping literature is that a tendency to Higher SHS scores were also related to a decrease in self-
dwell on one's negative experience is often related to denial esteem, an increase in negative affect, and a higher number of
and disengagement (e.g., Carver et al., 1989; Folkman & Laza- visits to the university health service. Lower self-esteem, higher
rus, 1985; Scheier et al., 1989). Indeed, in the current data, the negative affect, and lower positive affect, in turn, resulted over
correlations of the three withdrawal strategies (denial, behav- time in higher levels of self-handicapping. Both low self-esteem
ioral disengagement, and mental disengagement) with self-fo- and negative affect indicate poor adjustment. Low self-esteem
cused rumination were all positive, ranging from .23 to .43 is a correlate of helplessness and depression; higher levels of
1626 ZUCKERMAN, KIEFFER, AND KNEE

negative affect (and, to a lesser extent, lower levels of positive the results is not as plausible as the "actual obstacles"
affect) have been related to measures of distress, depression, interpretation.
and anxiety (Watson et al., 1988). Thus, the reciprocal relations First, one consequence of self-handicapping--lower G P A - -
between these adjustment-related variables and self-handicap- cannot be easily seen as a claimed obstacle only; and, as noted
ping are consistent with the hypothesis that these constructs earlier, if high self-handicappers merely claimed bad study hab-
reinforce each other, thus forming a vicious circle: Self-handi- its, it is not clear why they obtained lower grades. Second, most
capping results in lower adjustment which, in turn, induces fur- of the coping strategies related to high self-handicapping can
ther increases in self-handicapping. Of course, a complete test hardly serve as claimed obstacles, particularly as measured in
of this hypothesis requires that participants be followed for a Study 2. For example, high self-handicappers in Study 2 re-
longer period of time than a single semester. It would also be ported responding to a stressful event with denial and behavioral
interesting to examine whether experimental mood induction disengagement; such reactions may help the individual to deal
influences the actual use of behavioral self-handicaps. The hy- with the stress, but they cannot serve as an excuse for an event
pothesis is that people are more likely to self-handicap under a that has already happened. Third, visits to the health service as
bad mood than under a good mood. a means for special consideration from instructors is actually
The relation between SHS and frequency of visits to the health not a self-handicapping technique. The person asking for special
service further supports the notion of a negative relation between consideration is trying to get a better grade rather than fend off
self-handicapping and well-being. However, although number an ego threat; and a higher grade on the basis of a false claim
of health visits is a behavioral measure, its relation with self- can be construed as better performance at some cost to self-
handicapping is open to various interpretations. One obvious esteem, almost the inverse of a self-handicapping technique.
possibility is that the high self-handicapper who frequently visits The most parsimonious explanation of the findings, therefore,
the health service is the "habitually" ill hypochondriac that is that the self-reports used in the current studies serve as indica-
Rhodewalt (1990) hypothesized is likely to have a high score tors of actual behavior. We conclude that self-handicapping re-
on the SHS. An alternative is that high self-handicappers go to sults in poor adjustment over time and that poor adjustment, in
the health service to obtain an official excuse (a physician's turn, elicits more self-handicapping.
report) and a justification for the special consideration they
request from their instructors. Either way, health visits that are 2 These issues were raised by two anonymous reviewers.
not related to the person's actual health appear dysfunctional.
Still, without additional information it is difficult to identify the
exact implications of the finding, and the difficulty is exacer- References
bated by a lack of relation between the SHS and health visits
Aldwin, C. M., & Revenson, T. A. (1987). Does coping help? A reexam-
in the spring semester.
ination of the relation between coping and mental health. Journal of
Taken together, the results illustrate the nature and conse- Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 337-348.
quences of self-handicappers' habitual self-defeating behavior. Arkin, R. M., & Baumgardner, A. H. ( 1985 ). Self-handicapping. In J. G.
As noted earlier, this view of self-handicapping is somewhat Harvey & G. W. Weary (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applica-
different from its usual characterization as a strategic response tions (pp. 169-202). San Diego: Academic Press.
to ego threat. In the present studies we did not identify ego- Baumeister, R. E, Hamilton, J. C., & Tice, D. M. (1985). Public versus
threatening antecedents of self-handicapping behaviors but private expectancy of success: Confidence booster or performance
pressure? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1447-
rather examined these behaviors as a way of life.
1457.
Still, it is necessary to acknowledge some of the ambiguity Baumgardner, A.H., Lake, E.A., & Arkin, R.M. (1985). Claiming
inherent in the findings. Because most of our measures were mood as a self-handicap: The influence of spoiled and unspoiled public
based on self-reports, it is not clear whether we identified what identities. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 349-357.
high self-handicappers actually do or what they merely claim Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping
to do. As argued earlier, one interpretation of the relation be- strategy in response to noncontingent success. Journal of Personality
tween SHS and inefficient study habits is that high self-handi- and Social Psychology, 36, 405-417.
cappers do not study less but only claim to. A similar question Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. E (1994). Situational coping and coping
dispositions in a stressful transaction. Journal of Personality and
can be raised about the relation of the SHS with self-reports of
Social Psychology, 66, 184-195.
avoidant coping. It is not clear, for example, whether high self-
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. E, & Pozo, C. (1992). Conceptualizing the
handicappers actually engage in more denial or merely claim process of coping with health problems. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.),
to. Even a more behavioral measure such as health care visits Hostility, coping, and health (pp. 167-199 ). Washington, DC: Ameri-
is not free from ambiguity. We noted that high self-handicappers can Psychological Association.
are more likely to seek health care because they habitually culti- Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. E, & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing cop-
vate symptoms or perhaps as a means of getting special consid- ing strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal ofPersonali~
eration from instructors. 2 and Social Psychology, 56, 267-283.
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation
Naturally, self-handicapping would be seen as less of a threat
analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
to adjustment if it results in claimed obstacles to success (claims DeGree, C.E., & Snyder, C.R. (1985). Adler's psychology (of use)
of inefficient study habits, avoidant coping, and bad health) today: Personal history of traumatic life events as a self-handicapping
but not in actual or acquired obstacles to success (e.g., actual strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; 48, 1512-
inefficient study habits; cf. Arkin & Baumgardner, 1985). We 1519.
believe, however, that the "claimed obstacles" interpretation of Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-
CONSEQUENCES OF SELF-HANDICAPPING 1627

aged community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, Rhodewalt, E, Saltzman, A. T., & Wittmer, J. (1984). Self-handicapping
219-239. among competitive athletes: The role of practice in self-esteem protec-
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. ( 1985 ). If it changes it must be a process: tion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 5, 197-209.
A study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college exami- Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self New York: Basic Books.
nation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150-170. Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, L., & Palfai, T. (1995).
Harris, R. N., & Snyder, C. R. (1986). The role of uncertain self-esteem Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelli-
in self-handicapping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, gence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In J. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emo-
51, 451-458. tion, disclosure, health (pp. 125-154). Washington, DC: American
Higgins, R. L., & Harris, R. N. (1988). Strategic "alcohol" use: Drink- Psychological Association.
ing to self-handicap. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, Scheier, M. F., Matthew, K. A., Owens, J. E, Magovern, G. J., Sr., Lefeb-
191-202. vre, R. C., Abbott, R. A., & Carver, C. S. (1989). Dispositional opti-
Hirt, E. R., Deppe, R. K., & Gordon, L. J. (1991). Self-reported versus mism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The benefi-
behavioral self-handicapping: Empirical evidence for a theoretical cial effects on physical and psychological well-being. Journal of Per-
distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 981- sonality and Social Psychology, 57, 1024-1040.
991. Shepperd, J. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1989). Determinants of self-handicap-
Jones, E. E., & Berglas, S. (1978). Control of attributions about the ping: Task importance and effects of pre-existing handicaps on self-
self through self-handicapping strategies: The appeal of alcohol and generated handicaps. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15,
the role of underachievement. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- 101-112.
letin, 4, 200-206. Smith, T. W., Snyder, C. R., & Handelsman, M. M. (1982). On the self-
Jones, E.E., & Rhodewalt, E (1982). The Self-Handicapping Scale. serving function of an academic wooden leg: Test anxiety as a self-
(Available from Frederick Rhodewalt, Department of Psychology, Uni- handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
versity of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112.) 42, 314-321.
Kelley, H.H. (1971). Causal schemata and the attribution process. Smith, T. W., Snyder, C. R., & Perkins, S. (1983). On the self-serving
Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. function of hypochondriacal complaints: Physical symptoms as self-
Kolditz, T. A., & Arkin, R. M. (1982). An impression management inter- handicapping strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
pretation of the self-handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality ogy, 44, 787-797.
and Social Psychology, 43, 492-502. Snyder, C. R. (1990). Self-handicapping processes and sequelae: On the
Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Coping theory and research: Past, present, and taking of a psychological dive. In R. Higgins, C.R., Snyder, & S.
future. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 234-247. Berglas (Eds.), Self-handicapping: The paradox that isn't (pp. 107-
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. 150). New York: Plenum.
New York: Springer. Snyder, C. R., & Higgins, R. L. (1988). Excuses: Their affective role
Leary, M. R., & Shepperd, J. A. (1986). Behavioral self-handicaps ver- in the negotiation of reality. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 23-35.
sus self-reported self-handicaps: A conceptual note. Journal of Per- Snyder, C. R., & Smith, T. W. (1982). Symptoms as self-handicapping
sonality and Social Psychology, 51, 1265-1268. strategies: The virtues of old wine in a new bottle. In G. Weary &
Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating H. L. Mirels (Eds.), Integrations of clinical and social psychology
properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social (pp. 104-127). New York: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 65, 339-349. Snyder, C.R., Smith, T. W., Aagelli, R.W., & Ingram, R.E. (1985).
Mayerson, N. H., & Rhodewalt, E (1988). The role of self-protective On the self-serving function of social anxiety: Shyness as a self-
attributions in the experience of pain. Journal of Social and Clinical handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 6, 203-218. 48, 970-980.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B.L. (1993). Re- Springston, E J., & Chafe, P. M. ( 1987, June). Impressions of fictional
sponse styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal protagonist exhibiting self-handicapping behaviors. Paper presented
of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20-28. at the meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Vancouver,
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1983). Determinants of reduction in British Columbia, Canada.
intended effort as a strategy for coping with anticipated failure. Jour- Strube, M. J. (1985). An analysis of the Self-Handicapping Scale. Basic
nal of Research in Personality, 17, 412-422. and Applied Social Psychology, 7, 211-224.
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1986). Evidence for a depressive self- Strube, M.J., & Roemmele, L.A. (1985). Self-enhancement, self-
focusing style. Journal of Research in Personality, 20, 95-106. assessment, and self-evaluative task choice. Journal of Personality
Rhodewalt, E (1990). Self-handicappers: Individual differences in the and Social Psychology, 49, 981-993.
preference for anticipatory self-protective acts. In R. Higgins, C. R. Tucker, J. A., Vuchinich, R. W., & Sobell, M. ( 1981 ). Alcohol consump-
Snyder, & S. Berglas (Eds.), Self-handicapping: The paradox that tion as a self-handicapping strategy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
isn't (pp. 69-106). New York: Plenum. 90, 220-230.
Rhodewatt, E, & Davison, J. (1986). Self-handicapping and subsequent Vitaliano, P. P., Russo, J., Carr, J. E., Maiuro, R. D., & Becker, J. (1985).
performance: Role of outcome valence and attributional certainty. The Ways of Coping Checklist: Revision and psychometric properties.
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 7, 307-323. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 20, 3-26.
Rhodewalt, E, & Fairfield, M. L. (1989). Claimed self-handicaps and Watson, D., Clark, L.A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
the self-handicapper: The effect of reduction in intended effort on validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
performance. Unpublished manuscript. University of Utah, Salt Lake PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
City. 1063-1070.

(Appendixes follow)
1628 ZUCKERMAN, KIEFFER, AND KNEE

Appendix A

S a m p l e I t e m s a n d I n t e r n a l R e l i a b i l i t i e s o f C o p i n g S u b s c a l e s f o r S t u d i e s 1 a n d 2 at T i m e 1 a n d T i m e 2

Study l Study 2

Time Time Time Time


Coping subscale Sample item 1 2 1 2

COPE subscales
Active coping I concentrate my efforts on doing something about it. .70 .69 .67 .72
Planning I try to come up with a strategy about what to do. .85 .79 .83 .83
Suppression of competing activities I keep myself from getting distracted by other thoughts or activities. .64 .63 .61 .68
Restraint coping I restrain myself from doing anything too quickly. .63 .70 .60 .74
Instrumental support-seeking I try to get advice from someone about what to do. .82 .83 .82 .75
Emotional support-seeking I discuss my feelings with someone. .90 .87 .90 .91
Positive reinterpretation I look for something good in what is happening .70 .73 .73 .77
Acceptance I accept the reality of the fact that it happened. .62 .69 .68 .77
Denial I refuse to believe that it has happened. .70 .78 .73 .77
Behavioral disengagement I just give up trying to reach my goal. .65 .74 .68 .71
Mental disengagement I turn to work or other substitute activities to think about it less. .38 .44 .43 .36

Subscales added to Studies 1 and 2


Self-blame I blame myself. .74 .78 .71 .86
Self-focused rumination I relive the problem by dwelling on it all the time. .86 .83 .80 .87

Subscales added to Study 2


Expressing emotion I take time to express my emotions. .90 .90
Understanding emotion I work on understanding my feelings. .86 .85
Repairing emotion I try to be optimistic in spite of what happened. .86 .78

Appendix B

P s y c h o m e t r i c s o f the 1 4 - I t e m S e l f - H a n d i c a p p i n g S c a l e F r o m S t u d y 1 a n d S t u d y 2

Corrected i t e m - t o t a l Loadings on the first


correlations unrotated factor

Study Study Study Study


Item 1 2 1 2

1 .27 .32 .36 .42


2 .41 .26 .53 .34
4 .44 .43 .57 .59
5 .37 .36 .50 .45
8 .37 .35 .49 .47
9 .36 .28 .48 .39
11 .35 .22 .49 .29
15 .31 .34 .43 .46
16 .43 .49 .56 .66
18 .26 .34 .34 .45
19 .36 .34 .48 .47
21 .30 .38 .42 .51
23 .32 .32 .44 .48
25 .58 .47 .71 .63

Received October 28, 1996


Revision received July 9, 1997
Accepted July 11, 1997 •

View publication stats

You might also like