Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

NEUROSCIENCE PERSPECTIVES

Development of Human Brain Functions


Mark H. Johnson

Aspects of postnatal human brain development are re- observed during infancy are the result of general mecha-
viewed. The development of brain function has commonly nisms of learning and plasticity. In this article, I will
been characterized in terms of the unfolding of a matura- outline these different viewpoints on typical human func-
tional sequence. In contrast, we argue that postnatal func- tional brain development and briefly discuss implications
tional brain development occurs through a dynamic process for our understanding of certain developmental disorders
of emerging patterns of interactions between different brain and the consequences of perinatal brain damage.
regions. Some of these processes may also be characteristic
of perceptual and motor skill learning in adults. Possible
implications of these views for our understanding of devel-
opmental disorders are raised. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54: The Structural Development of the Human
1312–1316 © 2003 Society of Biological Psychiatry Brain
Key Words: Brain development, cortex, infants, children While the developmental neuroanatomy of the mammalian
brain has been researched for decades, at this point
relatively few studies have focused on human postnatal
Introduction development. In general, human brain development ap-
pears to follow the same sequence of events observed in

U nderstanding the functional development of the hu-


man brain is critical for social, educational, and
clinical policies. Given its importance, it is surprising that
other primates but on a slower time schedule. One model
of genetic control predicts that the more delayed the
general time course of brain development in a species, the
until recently very little was known about the topic; larger the relative volume of the later developing struc-
however, with the advent of suitable methods for investi- tures (such as the cerebral cortex and particularly the
gating brain function in infants and children, this area of frontal cortex) (Clancy et al 2000). In accordance with this
neuroscience is now making rapid progress. A critical general prediction, the slowed rate of development in
focus of current research is the question of how the humans is associated with a relatively larger volume of
development of brain structure (neuroanatomy) relates to cortex and an especially large frontal cortex. An additional
the emerging motor, perceptual, and cognitive functions important benefit of a slowed time course of brain devel-
during childhood. Another issue central to this topic opment is that it potentially allows a prolonged postnatal
concerns the developmental origin of specialized struc- period during which interaction with the environment can
tures and processing within the brain. One perspective on contribute to the tuning and shaping of circuitry.
this second issue is that functional specialization of re- There are a number of ways to study postnatal neuro-
gions of the cerebral cortex arises mainly through intrinsic anatomical development in humans. Postmortem analyses
genetic and molecular mechanisms and that experience have been conducted, albeit usually on small sample sizes.
merely plays a role in the final fine-tuning. An alternative Developmental positron emission tomography (PET) and
view is that at least some aspects of human functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies are becoming
brain development involve a prolonged process of func- more common but are sometimes restricted to infants with
tional specialization that is heavily shaped by postnatal suspected clinical symptoms (for detailed review of tech-
experience. A parallel debate to that among developmental niques currently available, see Casey and de Haan 2002).
neuroscientists rages among developmental psychologists. One conclusion that can be drawn at this point is that
Some developmental psychologists argue that the human around the time of birth in humans most neurons have
infant is born with innate modules and core knowledge migrated to their appropriate locations within the cortex,
relevant to the physical and social world. In contrast, hippocampus, cerebellum, and other structures (although
others propose that many of the changes in behavior some neurogenesis continues into adulthood). Also at
birth, subcortical structures are clearly definable and
From Birkbeck College, University of London, London, United Kingdom.
closely resemble their adult forms. While the major
Address reprint requests to Mark H. Johnson, Centre for Brain and Cognitive landmarks (sulci, gyri) of the cerebral cortex are present, it
Development, School of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London,
Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom.
remains relatively immature at birth in terms of its
Received October 7, 2002; revised March 3, 2003; accepted April 15, 2003. interregional and intraregional connectivity.

© 2003 Society of Biological Psychiatry 0006-3223/03/$30.00


doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00426-8
Development of Human Brain Functions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1313
2003;54:1312–1316

The fourfold increase in the total volume of the brain from abilities of human infants and children has accrued as a result
birth to teenage years is not uniform. For example, while of behavioral testing (see Johnson and Mareschal 2001 for
there is a rapid increase in synaptogenesis around the time of recent review). While controversies remain, one theme
birth for all cortical areas studied, the timing of the most rapid that emerges from these studies is the active role played by
burst of synapse formation and the final peak density occurs the infant, toddler, or child in attending to novel and
at different ages in different areas (Huttenlocher and Dab- socially relevant stimuli and events. Thus, far from being
holkar 1997). In the visual cortex, there is a rapid burst of a passive learner or blank slate, the developing infant can
synapse formation at 3 to 4 months, and the maximum be viewed as an active participant in the latter stages of its
density of around 150% of adult level is reached between own brain development.
4 and 12 months. Synaptogenesis also starts at the same To date, much of the research attempting to relate brain
time within the prefrontal cortex, but the density increases to behavioral development in humans has been from a
much more slowly and does not reach its peak until well maturational viewpoint in which the goal is to relate the
after the first year. This differential time course of devel- maturation of particular regions of the brain, usually
opment of cortical regions is also observable in the living regions of cerebral cortex, to newly emerging sensory,
human brain using PET (Chugani et al 1987). During the motor, and cognitive functions (Figure 1A). Evidence
first weeks after birth, glucose uptake is highest in concerning the differential neuroanatomical development
sensorimotor cortex, thalamus, brainstem, and the cerebel- of cortical regions reviewed earlier is used to determine an
lar vermis. By 3 months, there are considerable rises in the age when a particular region is likely to become func-
parietal, temporal, and occipital cortices; basal ganglia; tional. Success in a new behavioral task at this age is then
and cerebellar cortex, while such rises are not found in the attributed to the maturation of a new brain region. By this
frontal and dorsolateral occipital cortex until approxi- view, functional brain development is the reverse of adult
mately 6 to 8 months. There is a continuing rise in overall neuropsychology, with the difference that specific brain
resting brain metabolism (glucose uptake) after the first regions are added in instead of being damaged. Despite the
year of life, with a peak approximately 150% above adult intuitive appeal of the maturational approach, it does not
levels achieved around 4 to 5 years of age for some successfully explain some aspects of human functional
cortical areas. As in other mammals, regressive events are brain development. For example, recent evidence suggests
also observed during human brain development. For ex- that some of the regions that are slowest to develop by
ample, PET studies reveal that while the overall level of neuroanatomical criteria show activity from shortly after
glucose uptake reaches a peak during early childhood birth (see below). Further, where functional activity has
which is much higher than that observed in adults, the been assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging
rates return to adult levels after about 9 years of age. A (fMRI) during a behavioral transition, multiple cortical
corresponding “rise and fall” in synaptic density (synapses and subcortical areas appear to change their response
per neuron) is observed with the density falling to adult pattern (Luna et al 2001), rather than one or two previ-
levels at different ages for different cortical regions during ously silent regions becoming active (mature).
later childhood. In contrast to the above approach (in which intrar-
In addition to the formation of dendritic trees and their egional connectivity matures), an alternative viewpoint,
associated synapses, most fibers become myelinated dur- interactive specialization, assumes that postnatal func-
ing postnatal development. Structural MRI images reveal tional brain development, at least within the cerebral
that the general appearance of brain structures is similar to cortex, involves a process of organizing patterns of inter-
that of adults by 2 years of age and that all major fiber regional interactions (Johnson 2000) (Figure 1B). Accord-
tracts can be observed by 3 years of age (Matsuzawa et al ing to this view, the response properties of a specific
2001). Increases in white matter extend through adoles- region are partly determined by its patterns of connectivity
cence into adulthood, particularly in frontal brain regions to other regions and their patterns of activity. During
(Giedd et al 1999). postnatal development, changes in the response properties
of cortical regions occur as they interact and compete with
each other to acquire their role in new computational
The Functional Development of the Human abilities. By this view, some cortical regions may begin
Brain with poorly defined functions and consequently are par-
Relating the evidence discussed above on the neuroanatomi- tially activated in a wide range of different contexts and
cal development of the brain to the remarkable changes in tasks. During development, activity-dependent interac-
motor, perceptual, and cognitive abilities during the first tions between regions sharpen up the functions of regions
decade or so of human life remains a challenging question. such that their activity becomes restricted to a narrower set
Considerable knowledge about the cognitive and perceptual of circumstances (e.g., a region originally activated by a
1314 BIOL PSYCHIATRY M.H. Johnson
2003;54:1312–1316

Figure 1. A hypothetical illustration of


three accounts of the neural basis of an
advance in behavioral abilities in infants.
(A) Represents a maturational view in
which maturation of one region (in this
case the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) en-
ables new behavioral abilities to appear.
(B) Illustrates an interactive specialization
view in which the onset of a new behav-
ioral ability is due to changes in the inter-
actions between regions that were already
partially active. By this view, several re-
gions adjust their functionality together to
enable new computations. (C) Shows a
third perspective, skill learning, in which
certain regions become active during the
acquisition of a range of new skills
throughout the life span. The activation of
some of these skill acquisition regions
declines as expertise is developed. Re-
printed with permission from Johnson
(2001) by Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
© 2001 Macmillan Magazines Ltd.

wide variety of visual objects may come to confine its structures and pathways and that there are dynamic
response to upright human faces). The onset of new changes in the cortical processing of stimuli during devel-
behavioral competencies during infancy will therefore be opment. Experiments with scalp recorded electrical poten-
associated with changes in activity over several regions tials have suggested that there is increasing spatial local-
and not just by the onset of activity in one or more ization of selective processing with age or experience of a
additional region(s). In further contrast to the maturational stimulus class. For example, for word recognition, differ-
approach, this view predicts that during infancy, patterns ences between known words and control stimuli are initially
of cortical activation during behavioral tasks may be more found over large areas, but this difference narrows to the
extensive than those observed in adults. Additionally, leads over the left temporal lobe only when vocabulary
within broad constraints, successful behavior in the same reaches around 200 words, irrespective of maturational age
tasks can be supported by different patterns of cortical (Neville et al 1992). In parallel with changes in the
activation in infants and adults. patterns of regional activation are changes in the “tuning”
Recent evidence does, in fact, indicate that the same of individual regions. For example, when event-related
behavior in infants and adults can be mediated by different potentials (ERPs) are recorded during passive exposure to
Development of Human Brain Functions BIOL PSYCHIATRY 1315
2003;54:1312–1316

faces, the resulting component that is sensitive to upright interactive specialization view would encourage the view
human faces (the “n170”) in adults is much more broadly that the onset of a new behavior such as this will entail
tuned in its response in infants. Specifically, in adults, the changing patterns of interactions between multiple brain
n170 shows a different amplitude and latency to human regions (Luna et al 2001). By a skill learning view,
upright faces than to animal or inverted faces. In infants, parallels can be drawn with research on adults. Work on
the equivalent ERP component responds similarly to complex motor skill learning tasks in adult primates shows
upright and inverted human faces (de Haan et al 2002). that parts of prefrontal cortex are often activated during
This evidence for dynamic changes in cortical processing the early stages of motor skill acquisition, but this activa-
during infancy is consistent with a process in which tion recedes to more posterior regions as expertise is
interregional interactions help to shape intraregional con- acquired (Miller 2000). Consistent with this latter view,
nectivity, such that several regions together come to Csibra et al (2001) examined the cortical activity associ-
support particular perceptual and cognitive functions. ated with the planning of eye movements in 4-month-old
A third perspective on human functional brain develop- infants. They observed eye movement related potentials
ment (termed skill learning) involves the proposal that the over frontal sites but not over the more posterior (parietal)
brain regions active in infants during the onset of new sites where they are normally observed in adults. Converg-
perceptual or behavioral abilities are similar, or identical ing results are obtained when eye movement tasks are
to, those involved in complex skill acquisition in adults studied in infants with perinatal focal damage to the
(Figure 1C). With regard to perceptual expertise, Gauthier cortex. Infants with damage to the frontal quadrants of the
et al (1999) have shown that extensive training of adults brain show long-lasting deficits in visual orienting tasks,
with artificial objects (called “greebles”) eventually results but infants with the more posterior damage, which causes
in activation of a cortical region previously associated deficits in adults, do not (Craft and Shatz 1994).
with face processing, the fusiform face area. This suggests
that the region is normally activated by faces in adults, not
because it is prespecified for faces, but due to our Conclusions and Implications for Atypical
extensive expertise with that class of stimulus. Further, it Development
encourages parallels with the development of face pro- The three perspectives on human functional brain devel-
cessing skills in infants (Gauthier and Nelson 2001). The opment discussed above have different implications for
extent to which parallels can be drawn between adult our understanding of developmental disorders and the
expertise and infant development remains unclear. Future effects of brain damage over the first years of life. By the
experiments will need to trace in more detail changes in maturational view, genetic disorders could potentially lead
the patterns of cortical activation during training in adults to focal cortical damage and consequently to selective
and development in infants. cognitive, motor, or perceptual disorders. Symptoms of
The differences between the three viewpoints on func- these disorders would first become evident at the normal
tional brain development described above can be illus- age of maturation of the regions concerned; however,
trated by consideration of the prefrontal cortex. By neu- recent reviews have concluded that there are few, if any,
roanatomical criteria, this region of the brain is the slowest human developmental abnormalities of genetic origin that
part of the brain to develop, with detectable changes still only affect one or two specific regions of cortex (Johnson
occurring into the teenage years (Giedd et al 1999). et al 2002). Rather, structural and functional neuroimaging
According to some, maturation within the frontal lobes is usually reveal subtle but widespread differences in the
related to the onset of the ability to successfully reach for brains of groups with developmental disorders. Relatedly,
desirable objects toward the end of the first year. Infants claims of domain-specific cognitive deficits in syndromes
younger than 9 months often fail to accurately retrieve a such as autism and Williams syndrome have been chal-
hidden object after a short delay period if the object’s lenged and replaced with hypotheses about different styles
location is changed from one where it was previously or modes of processing (Karmiloff-Smith 1998). Finally, it
successfully retrieved. Young infants tend to perseverate is difficult to explain reports of recovery of functions
by reaching to the hiding location where the object was following early brain damage by the maturational view
found on the immediately preceding trial. This error is without recourse to additional special mechanisms of
similar to those made by human adults with frontal lesions plasticity.
and monkeys with lesions to the dorsolateral prefrontal According to the interactive specialization view, devel-
cortex, leading to the proposal that the maturation of this opmental disorders of genetic etiology will often involve
region in human infants allows them to retain information disruption of the typical biases and interactions between
over space and time and to inhibit prepotent responses regions that give rise to adult patterns of cortical func-
(Diamond 2001). In contrast to this maturational view, the tional specialization. By this view, subtle symptoms
1316 BIOL PSYCHIATRY M.H. Johnson
2003;54:1312–1316

should be evident from birth, but these will become Craft S, Schatz J (1994): The effects of bifrontal stroke during
compounded through the infant’s abnormal interactions childhood on visual attention: Evidence from children with
sickle cell anemia. Dev Neuropsychol 10:285–297.
with its environment. While there are currently few
behavioral studies of developmental disorders during in- Csibra G, Tucker LA, Johnson MH (2001): Differential frontal
cortex activation before anticipatory and reactive saccades in
fancy, those that do exist suggest discontinuities in the infants. Infancy 2:159 –174.
patterns of behavioral and cognitive deficits through post- de Haan M, Pascalis O, Johnson MH (2002): Specialization of
natal development. For example, in one such disorder, neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human
Williams syndrome, adults present with behavioral deficits infants. J Cogn Neurosci 14:199 –209.
in number tasks but show surprising proficiency in some Diamond A (2001): A model system for studying the role of
aspects of language. A question recently investigated is dopamine in the prefrontal cortex during early development in
whether this pattern of specific deficits is also observed in humans: Early and continuously treated phenylketonuria. In:
Williams syndrome infants, as would be expected if they Nelson CA, Luciana M, editors. Handbook of Developmental
Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 433– 472.
have a damaged innate module for numbers. Standard
Gauthier I, Nelson CA (2001): The development of face exper-
infant paradigms for assessing number and object naming tise. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:219 –224.
skills were used with toddlers with Williams syndrome
Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC
(Paterson et al 1999). The toddlers did not show the same (1999): Activation of the middle fusiform “face area” in-
behavioral profile as observed in adults with the syn- creases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat
drome, indicating that the profile of behavioral deficits in Neurosci 2:568 –573.
developmental disorders can change during ontogeny and Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Lui H,
that it is not appropriate to characterize such deficits in Zijdenbos A, et al (1999): Brain development during child-
terms of damage to domain-specific modules. From the hood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neu-
rosci 2:861–863.
interactive specialization perspective, it may also be easier
to understand recovery of function following early damage Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar JC (1997): Developmental anatomy
of prefrontal cortex. In: NA Krasnegor, GR Lyon, PS Gold-
since the same mechanisms that ensure specialization man-Rakic, editors, Development of the Prefrontal Cortex:
during the typical developmental trajectory could ensure a Evolution, Neurobiology, and Behavior. Baltimore, MD: Paul
different pattern of specialization. H. Brookes, 69 – 84.
Further assessment of the three perspectives on human Johnson MH (2000): Functional brain development in infants:
functional brain development presented in this article will Elements of an interactive specialization framework. Child
require more improved methods for noninvasive func- Dev 71:75–81.
tional imaging methods, such as near infrared spectros- Johnson MH (2001): Functional brain development in humans.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2:475–483.
copy (NIRS)/optical imaging, and more detailed compu-
tational models that generate predictions about both Johnson MH, Halit H, Grice S, Karmiloff-Smith A (2002):
Neuroimaging of typical and atypical development: A per-
neuroanatomy and behavior (Casey and de Haan 2002). spective from multiple levels of analysis. Dev Psychopathol
Whatever the outcome of these investigations, a better 14:521–536.
understanding of functional brain development in human Johnson MH, Mareschal D (2001): Cognitive and perceptual devel-
infants and children may have profound consequences for opment during infancy. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:213–218.
educational, clinical, and social policies. Karmiloff-Smith A (1998): Development itself is the key to
understanding developmental disorders. Trends Cogn Sci
2:389 –398.
Luna B, Thulborn KR, Munoz DP, Merriam EP, Garver KE,
The author acknowledges financial support from the United Kingdom Minshew NJ, et al (2001): Maturation of widely distributed
Medical Research Council (Programme Grant G9715587) and Birkbeck brain function subserves cognitive development. Neuroimage
College. 13:786 –793.
Matsuzawa J, Matsui M, Konishi T, Noguchi K, Gur RC, Bilker
W, et al (2001): Age-related volumetric changes of brain gray
References and white matter in healthy infants and children. Cereb
Casey BJ, de Haan M (2002): Imaging techniques and their Cortex 11:335–342.
application in developmental science. Dev Science 5(Special Miller EK (2000): The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control.
Issue):265–396. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:59 –65.
Chugani HT, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC (1987): Positron emis- Neville HJ, Mills D, Lawson D (1992): Fractionating language:
sion tomography study of human brain functional develop- Different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods.
ment. Ann Neurol 22:487–497. Cereb Cortex 2:244 –258.
Clancy B, Darlington RB, Finlay BL (2000): The course of Paterson SJ, Brown JH, Gsodl MK, Johnson MH, Karmiloff-
human events: Predicting the timing of primate neural devel- Smith A (1999): Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders.
opment. Dev Science 3:57–66. Science 286:2355–2358.

You might also like