Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

The Analysis of Verbal Behavior 1982, 1, 1-3

Skinner's Elementary Verbal Relations:


Some New Categories
Jack Michael
Western Michigan University
In Verbal Behavior (1957) B. F Skinner identified and named five elementary verbal relations: mand,
tact, intraverbal, textual and echoic. Because of their etymological commitment to visual and
auditory stimuli respectively, the last two categories do not function well as general categories.
Adding two more general categories, codic and duplic, to the first three results in a set of five
mutually exclusive and collectively exhaustive groupings. Textual behavior and other relations
involving point-to-point correspondence but no formal similarity fall into the codic category. Echoic
behavior and other relations with formal similarity fall into the duplic category. This arrangement
results in useful category names for all elementary forms and prevents potentially confusing
extensions, such as referring to Braille reading as textual behavior, or sign imitation as echoic
behavior.

In Verbal Behavior (1957) Skinner identified MAND


and named five types of functional relations When the response form (topography) is
between controlling variables and verbal controlled by a current unlearned or learned
responses. These are the mand, tact, intra-
verbal, textual and echoic relations. In the motivational variable (an unconditioned or
section on transcription (pp. 69-71) he almost conditioned establishing operation) such as
named two more, which can be usefully deprivation or the warning stimulus in an
referred to as copying a text and taking dicta- avoidance situation, the relation is called a
tion (see paragraph 2 and 3 of page 70)\ Skin- mand. Said another way, the response form
ner's general analysis of verbal behavior has is most closely related historically to what
greatly facilitated our ability to talk effectively has previously functioned as reinforcement
about human behavior, and these elemen- for responses of that form. The response can
tary behavioral units are an essential aspect consist of speaking, writing, signing (as with
of this analysis. the sign language of the deaf), finger spell-
In teaching from Verbal Behavior I have ing, sending Morse code, etc. Skinner dassi-
found it convenient to add two more special fies mands as requests, commands, entreat-
terms to the list of elementary relations. This ies (on the basis of how the listener is rein-
addition does not identify new or previously forced), and as mands for nonverbal action
overlooked relations, but rather provides versus mands for verbal action (which the
names for implied categories, and thus a latter are called questions), plus some other
place for several forms of verbal behavior that associated types (pp. 38-41). Another way of
were not previously classifiable. The sug- dassifying mands is to say that one can
gested change also makes the basic cate- mand objects, actions, attention, and more
gories more nearly collectively exhaustive. complex events as when one asks for infor-
The new terms are codic and duplic, which mation or instruction, or says "Thank you"
like echoic, textual, and intraverbal function as because an increase in the listener's future
adjectives preceding behavior or relation, and favorable behavior would be effective as a
like these others can occur alone when form of reinforcement. In common-sense
behavior is understood. The basic arrange- terms, in the mand what is said (signed,
ment is shown below in a form that is con- written, etc.) is determined by what the
venient for instructional purposes. speaker wants.
I The audience relation was also treated as an elemen-
tary verbal relation, but differs from the tact only in the TACT
size of the repertoire controlled, and in the fact that the
nonverbal stimulus usually consists of the collection of In the tact relation the response form is
stimuli involving the listener. controlled primarily by an immediately prior
1
2 JACK MICHAEL
nonverbal stimulus (an object, action, rela- point correspondence between signs and
tion, property, etc.). As with all of the words (although the situation is somewhat
elementary verbal relations except the mand, complicated by the existence of initialized
the effect of the establishing operation on the signs-signs that incorporate some aspect of
response form is minimized by the fact that finger spelling). The sign for cat, for exam-
the reinforcement for the tact is usually ple, consists of stroking imaginary facial
generalized conditioned reinforcement vibrissae. This clearly has no point-to-point
(Skinner, 1957, pp. 52-55). The response can correspondence with either the spoken or
consist of speaking, writing, signing, finger the written cat. The finger spelled cat, of
spelling, sending Morse code, etc. It might course, has point-to-point correspondence
seem reasonable to substitute some term with both spoken and written cat, but not
such as naming or describing for the tact rela- with the sign for cat.
tion, but as Skinner insists (1957, p. 82), there
are good reasons for avoiding such a CODIC BEHAVIOR
substitute. A useful contrast between mand
and tact is that ".. . .the mand permits the The codic relation is characterized by three
listener to infer something about the speaker defining features: (1) The response form is
regardless of the external circumstances, controlled by a verbal stimulus, (2) with
while the tact permits him to infer something which it has point-to-point correspondence,
about the circumstances regardless of the but (3) where there is NO formal similarity
condition of the speaker" (Skinner, 1957, p. between stimulus and response product.
83). In terms of group coordination, the Formal similarity is Skinner's term for the
mand permits the speaker to alter the envi- case where the controlling stimulus and the
ronment through someone else's behavior, response product are (1) in the same sense
and the tact permits the listener to react to mode (both are visual, or both are auditory,
the behavior of others " . . . rather than directly or both are tactile, etc.) and (2) resemble each
to things and events" (Skinner, 1957, p. 432). other in the physical sense of resemblance.
Note that codic is meant to suggest the kind
INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR of relation seen in a formal code, where one
stimulus is said to stand for another stimulus
Here the response form is controlled by (1) that it does not resemble. Textual behavior and
a verbal stimulus (the product of someone's taking dictation are special types of codic
verbal behavior-but this is not a simple con- behavior. In the textual relation the stimulus
cept, since the same behavior may have ver- is visual (written or printed words) and the
bal and nonverbal products) with which (2) response consists of speaking. In common-
the response does not have point-to-point cor- sense terms textual behavior is reading out
respondence. Point-to-point correspondence loud (without the implication that the reader
between stimulus and response (or between understands-can react in any other way
stimulus and response product) is in effect to-what is being read). In taking dictation
when subdivisions or parts of the stimulus the stimulus is auditory (the result of some-
control subdivisions or parts of the response one's vocal behavior) and the response con-
(or response product). In intraverbal behav- sists of writing what is heard. There is at pre-
ior the parts of the stimulus are not related sent no commonly used form of codic
in any special way to the parts of the behavior involving signs although such a
response. An example of intraverbal behav- system was developed some time ago and is
ior is a tendency to say swamp as a result of available in dictionary form (Stokoe,
hearing someone say alligator. The response Casterline, & Croneberg, 1965). The relation
can be speaking, writing, signing, etc. and between finger spelling and vocal or written
the verbal stimulus can be the result of some- stimuli may be codic (as well as intraverbal).
one's vocal, writing, signing, etc. behavior. Likewise, Braille reading out loud is codic
Note that for American Sign Language, also behavior as is writing in Braille what one
called Ameslan, or just Sign (which is not hears spoken.
equivalent to finger spelling) vocal or writ-
ten responses to signs, or signing responses DUPLIC BEHAVIOR
to vocal or written words are intraverbal
behavior. There is generally no point-to- The two defining features of the duplic
SKINNER'S ELEMENTARY VERBAL RELATIONS 3
relation are (1) that the response form is con- tions because no technical term is available,
trolled by a verbal stimulus, and (2) the as when one refers to Braille reading as tex-
response product has formal similarity with tual behavior. Braille reading is clearly a form
the controlling stimulus. (Sometimes the of codic behavior, and would be expected to
necessity for point-to-point correspondence share functional properties with other
between stimulus and response is cited as a members of the same category, but to call it,
third requirement, but formal similarity and all other forms of codic behavior involv-
between stimulus and response product ing a vocal response textual is potentially con-
always implies point-to-point correspond- fusing. A similar undesirable extension oc-
ence between stimulus and response so this curs when sign imitation is called echoic
third requirement needn't be listed.) The behavior, but is avoided by identifying it as
response can be speaking, writing, signing, duplic, or more specifically sign duplic
etc. Duplic implies duplicates or copies. behavior. Where a special term is needed
Echoic behavior and copying a text are special because of frequent use it is better to make
types of duplic behavior. In the echoic rela- one up that is more etymologically correct,
tion (echoing what one hears) the stimulus such as Vargas' (1982) "mimetic."
is auditory and the response is speaking. In This terminological refinement should be
copying a text (copying what one sees in considered an instance of the general effort
written form) the stimulus is visual and the to eliminate ambiguity from technical and
response is writing. Imitating someone's scientific language, an effort that is often
signs is also duplic behavior, as is finger initiated and possibly most keenly appre-
spelling what one sees someone finger spell. ciated by those who spend most of their time
The general importance of the basic distinc- teaching others to use that language.
tion between codic and duplic behavior is
dealt with in Verbal Behavior in several places, REFERENCES
but especially pages 67-68. Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discrimina-
tive and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of
SUMMARY the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149-155.
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-
With the five basic category names it now Century-Crofts.
becomes possible to classify all elementary Stokoe, W. C., Casterline, D. C., & Croneberg, C. G.
forms of verbal behavior in terms of impor- (1965). A dictionary of American Sign Language on
tant defining properties, as well as to immed- linguistic principles. Washington: Gallaudet College
Press.
iately classify any new form that develops. Vargas, E. A. (1982). Intraverbal behavior: The codic,
The two new terms also make it unnecessary duplic, and sequelic subtypes. The Analysis of Verbal
to extend existing categories to novel condi- Behavior, 1, 5-7.

You might also like