Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

SHORT ARTICLE

Campaign Donations, Judicial Recusal, and Disclosure:


A Field Experiment
Jonathan S. Krasno, Binghamton University
Donald P. Green, Columbia University
Costas Panagopoulos, Northeastern University
Dane Thorley, Brigham Young University Law School
Michael Schwam-Baird, Grow Progress, Washington, DC

This article reports results from a field experiment exploring how judicial behavior is affected by complaints about
conflicts of interest. The conflicts of interest studied here arise in Wisconsin civil trial cases. Using public records, we
identify instances in which one party’s attorney contributed to the presiding judge’s previous election campaign. We
send a random subset of these judges a letter identifying the potential conflict and requesting recusal. We find that
highlighting the potential conflict and asking judges to recuse sharply increases the rate at which judges disclose this
relationship in court records but does not lead them to recuse. Furthermore, treated judges are no more likely to
disclose or recuse in subsequent cases that present a similar conflict of interest. This experiment, which is the first to
test possible remedies to judicial conflicts of interest, suggests that light touch interventions are insufficient to change
judges’ behavior.

A 2002 survey of 1,700 state trial court judges found


that 58% would prohibit judges from presiding over
cases in which one of the parties contributed to that
judge’s election campaign (Justice at Stake 2002). That sen-
timent has special significance because the majority of state
literature has considered the potential effects of these relation-
ships on case outcomes (e.g., Hazelton, Montgomery, and
Nyhan 2016), and legal scholars have written on require-
ments surrounding recusal (e.g., Flamm 2017; Marbes 2017),
systematic empirical research has seldom investigated the
judges are elected and, like other candidates, raise funds for conditions under which judges opt to remove themselves
their campaigns (American Judicature Society 2020). Because from cases involving donors or to disclose these financial ties
many campaign donors are lawyers or potential litigants, in court records.
judicial fund-raising means that judges are likely at some In this essay, we present the results of a randomized field
point to preside over cases involving donors. While a growing experiment that measures judges’ willingness to step aside

Jonathan S. Krasno (jkrasno@binghamton.edu) is professor of political science, Department of Political Science, Binghamton University, PO Box 6000,
Binghamton, NY 13902. Donald P. Green (dpg2110@columbia.edu) is professor of political science, Department of Political Science, Columbia University,
420 West 118th Street, New York, NY 10027. Costas Panagopoulos (costas@post.harvard.edu) is chair and professor of political science, Department of Political
Science, Northeastern University, 1135 Tremont St., Renaissance Park 958, Boston, MA 02115. Dane Thorley (thorleyd@law.byu.edu) is associate professor of
law, Brigham Young University Law School, 341 East Campus Drive, Provo, UT 84602. Michael Schwam-Baird (michael.schwambaird@gmail.com) is lead data
scientist at Grow Progress, 1015 15th St. NW, Washington, DC 20005.
This study was conducted in compliance with relevant laws and was approved by institutional review boards at Fordham University (i14-08-CP-009),
Binghamton University (373916), and Columbia University (AAAO6402). Support for this research was provided by Open Society Foundation and De-
mocracy Fund, neither of which bear any responsibility for the content of this article. Data and supporting materials necessary to reproduce the numerical
results in the article are available in the JOP Dataverse (https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/jop). An online appendix with supplementary material is
available at https://doi.org/10.1086/715069.

Published online July 23, 2021.


The Journal of Politics, volume 83, number 4, October 2021. q 2021 Southern Political Science Association. All rights reserved. Published by The University
of Chicago Press for the Southern Political Science Association. https://doi.org/10.1086/715069 000
000 / Campaign Donations, Judicial Recusal, and Disclosure Jonathan S. Krasno et al.

from cases in which such a potential conflict of interest is The American public and even judges themselves appear to
called to their attention.1 Conducted over a two-year period, agree with these conclusions; surveys show that 71% of Amer-
the study identified active civil cases in the Wisconsin Cir- icans and 46% of state judges believe campaign donations in-
cuit Court in which one of the attorneys had donated to the fluence judicial decisions (Justice at Stake 2004; Sample et al.
presiding judge’s previous campaign. A random selection of 2010).
those judges received a private letter highlighting the poten- Recusal as an outcome has received far less attention, even
tial conflict of interest and asking the judge to recuse. Because in the wake of Massey, with much of the empirical work
our experimental design involved the behavior of real judges simply documenting low rates of recusal. For example, Liptak
in actual civil cases, we were able to track an array of mean- and Roberts (2006) found that judges recused in 9 of 215 Ohio
ingful outcomes beyond recusal, including disclosure of these Supreme Court cases featuring donors, and Palmer (2010)
donations in the public case file. found that judges recused in none of the 425 Louisiana Su-
Recusal over campaign donations is at the center of policy preme Court cases featuring donors. Although some scholars
debate and emerging case law. In Caperton v. A. T. Massey have studied whether judges comply with mandatory recusal
Coal Co., 556 U.S. 868 (2009), the US Supreme Court found provisions (Miller and Curry 2013), legal scholars have pri-
that the failure of a justice of the West Virginia Supreme marily focused on whether and when certain legal regimes
Court to step aside in a case in which one of the parties had require recusal (e.g., Flamm 2017; Marbes 2017).
spent $3 million to help him win election violated the due The literature on recusal suggests a number of hypotheses
process clause. This ruling caused several states to adopt new about why campaign donations might trigger recusal, even
recusal requirements in the face of potential conflicts related when doing so is not explicitly mandated by law. These ex-
to campaign financing. These legal thresholds range from as planations fall into two broad categories: internal and ex-
low as $50 in Utah to as high as $1,500 in California, but ternal. By internal we mean that judges might perceive they
most states, including Wisconsin, provide no set threshold, have an ethical obligation to step aside when someone with
leaving recusal to judges’ discretion. whom they have a relationship appears in their courtroom.
We find that recusal and conflict-of-interest disclosure Recusal is accepted practice when family, friends, or business
rarely occur in the Wisconsin Circuit Courts, the state’s trial partners appear before a judge, in large part because of
courts. Judges in the treatment group, who received our letter concerns about a judge’s impartiality. In light of the survey
identifying a potential conflict and urging recusal, proved to evidence cited above, there is good reason to expect that
be only marginally more likely to recuse than judges in the many trial court judges believe that similar concerns arise if
control group. But while the treatment did not spur recusal, it they were to preside over a case involving a campaign donor.
did have a sizable effect on disclosure, as judges in the treat- External reasons, by contrast, involve how relationships
ment group were far more likely to acknowledge the donation may appear to others. Internal and external concerns may
in the trial record. Still, fewer than one-third of the judges in overlap, as judges may be committed to both preserving the
the treatment group ultimately disclosed the conflict of in- legitimacy of the judicial system and their own reputations.
terest after receiving the letter. These rates fall well short of Appearances may be especially important for judges who
what one would expect on the basis of surveys about recusal gain and retain their posts via elections; credible allegations
and disclosure, implying limited deference to such norms by of favoritism might be fatal to their careers, and recusal may
many elected judges. avoid jeopardizing their electoral prospects. When they do
preside over these cases, elected judges might be particularly
BACKGROUND, THEORY, AND EXPECTATIONS sensitive to any appearance of favorable treatment to their
The empirical literature on judicial campaign finance focuses donors on the grounds that it might hurt them at the ballot
mainly on the causal relationship between campaign contri- box (Gibson and Caldeira 2012, 2013; Nelson 2014), which
butions and judicial decision-making. These studies contend militates in favor of disclosing conflicts of interest.
that contributors achieve better outcomes than noncontrib- Whether a judge is responsive to her own internal voice
utors (Cann, Bonneau, and Boyea 2012; Hazelton et al. 2016). or concern that voters might learn and disapprove of her
actions in a case involving a campaign donor, both sets of
explanation point in the same direction: judges, all else equal,
1. This experiment was approved by the institutional review boards at should be much more likely to recuse in cases involving
three institutions: Columbia University, Fordham University, and Bing-
hamton University. The design and a preanalysis plan were preregistered
donors than in cases that do not. As we note below, however,
with Evidence in Governance and Politics. See app. D (apps. A–E available judges only recuse spontaneously in a small share of conflict-
online) for a discussion of the plan. of-interest cases. The question then is whether judges who
Volume 83 Number 4 October 2021 / 000

face a conflict of interest recuse when urged to do so—a produce a list of “conflict-of-interest pairs” in which an at-
request that appeals to their sense of propriety but could also torney had donated at least $200 to a sitting judge assigned to
activate external motivations insofar as judges receiving our the case. We contracted with a vendor that serves Wisconsin’s
treatment might have feared additional publicity in the event law firms to do a weekly search for such pairs from newly
that our communications were publicly disclosed. It is also assigned civil cases, then double-checked that the lawyers
possible that the treatment letters induce recusals because involved were the donors on record. Some of the cases flagged
they remind or inform judges which attorneys donated to were inherently noncontentious (e.g., name changes and pa-
their campaign (i.e., judges will not recuse because of con- ternity acknowledgments), too fast moving to treat (e.g., re-
flicts of which they are unaware). No matter which causal straining orders and injunctions), or so uncontroversial that
pathway is activated by our communication, we expect ele- judges would be unlikely to take our letter treatments seri-
vated rates of recusal when an outside party notes the conflict ously (e.g., small claims). For the cases that remained after
of interest and requests that the judge step aside. this culling process, we used public information available
through the Wisconsin Circuit Court Access, an online search
EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN engine, to verify case details, check case status, and eventually
The Wisconsin Circuit Courts offer the mixture of institu- code the outcomes of interest. When necessary, we ordered
tional features necessary for our study: elected judges, attorney- hard copies of detailed case information through the circuit
specific campaign finance data, sufficient cases that feature court clerks.
pairs of donor-attorneys and judges, and a timely and search- Over the course of the experiment (October 2014–
able case docket. Federal judges are appointed, and appellate- December 2016), we identified 397 civil cases that featured
level state courts have a limited number of judges. Thirty-nine judge-attorney conflict-of-interest pairs, 270 of which were of
states elect trial-level judges, although fewer than 30 feature appropriate case types, had been recently assigned to the
pairs of candidates competing for a single position. Of these, judge, and had enough information to verify attorney iden-
only a handful make campaign finance data readily available, tities. None of these cases featured donor-attorneys on both
and even fewer have an online docket system that lends itself sides, and of the donor-attorneys in the sample, only four
to detailed and frequent searches. As we explain below, the lat- donated to more than one judge in our sample. In order to
ter was an essential element for ethical reasons. reduce spillover, we did not include otherwise eligible cases if
At the time of the study, Wisconsin’s 249 trial judges were the judge either had been assigned to the treatment condition
assigned to 69 circuit courts whose jurisdictions mostly fol- via a previous case (195 cases) or was presiding over a still-
low county boundaries. Circuit court judges are elected to active case that had been assigned to the control group within
staggered six-year terms in nonpartisan elections that occur the previous 5 months (14 cases). We also excluded any cases
each spring and generally hear both civil and criminal cases. in which the presiding judge would soon be up for reelection
As we explain below in our discussion of ethics, we restricted (1 case). These criteria resulted in an experimental pool of
our experiment to specific types of civil cases. We included all 60 cases. Each week, we randomly assigned the newly identified
active circuit judges who had raised campaign funds since experimental cases.3 Judges with more than one eligible case
2008 and were elected from 2009 to 2014. Because Wisconsin in a given week were dealt with separately, so that just one of
law exempts state candidates from filing campaign finance their cases was assigned to be treated (see app. B for a flow-
reports if their campaigns raised and spent less than $1,000, chart outlining the culling process by which experimental
the pool for this experiment is limited to the 113 winning cases were identified and assigned to treatments).
judges in the 2009–14 elections who were either required to Judges of cases in the treatment group received a letter (see
file reports with the state or who did so voluntarily. The av- app. A for an example) from the Judicial Integrity Project at
erage total contributions received by these 113 judges was Fordham University that reminded them of the donation(s)
$35,870, at least 16% of which came from attorneys.2 received from the attorney, including the amount donated,
In order to identify newly assigned cases involving attorney- and politely requested that they step aside “to maintain public
donors and judges, we matched campaign finance disclosure confidence” in the judicial system. At the advice of outside
data with information from the Wisconsin Bar Association to
3. The probability of assignment in a given week varied slightly be-
cause of rounding errors depending on the number of eligible cases or
2. This percentage almost certainly underestimates attorney donations. when a judge had multiple eligible cases in a given period, since we assigned
Any donor-attorneys who did not identify as attorneys in the disclosure no more than one treatment to each judge. Finally, we raised the probability
form or did not appear before the donee judge were not included when of assignment from .5 to .67 partway through the study to increase the
calculating the percentage. number of treated cases.
000 / Campaign Donations, Judicial Recusal, and Disclosure Jonathan S. Krasno et al.

counsel, these letters were sent only to the presiding judges receive a treatment letter, its effects might also be felt by
via UPS next-day delivery with a signature required. Four of litigants or attorneys. Given the potentially high stakes of liti-
the judges who received our letter contacted the organization gation, these concerns were considered with utmost serious-
directly for more information and were satisfied with a brief ness. Beyond the normal institutional review board approval
statement emphasizing its goal of unbiased judicial decision- process (at each author’s home institution), we engaged in an
making along with the clarification that the notice they had extensive effort to identify and minimize risks from the outset
received was intended for them only. of this project, through discussion with scholars, consultation
Five outcomes were tracked for all assigned cases: appli- with legal advisors, and, importantly, examining a sample of
cations for recusal, recusals granted, judicial transfers, donor- case files from Wisconsin to learn more about the pace of lit-
attorney withdrawal, and judicial disclosure. Judges in Wis- igation in that state. These deliberations are discussed in detail
consin seeking recusal must submit an application to the in appendix C and helped inform our design in three ways.
chief judge in their district. We coded both applications and First, as we note above, we restricted our inquiry to civil
recusals; in practice, the two turn out to be identical. Because litigation, excluding criminal trials on the grounds that any
judges might also seek to address conflicts of interest through defendant’s potential loss of freedom was too weighty to ex-
formal channels other than recusal, we measure judicial trans- pose to experimental manipulation no matter its effect. This
fers (nonrecusal changes in judicial authority) and disclosure is not to deny the financial consequences of civil litigation,
(any docket activity or case document, including our treatment which may be profound. Our examination of trial records,
letter, in which the judge discloses the financial relationship however, suggested that civil litigation in Wisconsin generally
between herself and the attorney).4 The other outcome of in- proceeds at a relatively slow pace (the average length of time
terest we measure—whether the donor-attorney left the case— from case filing to decision for the 60 cases in our study was
reflects extrajudicial behavior related to the potential conflict of 345 days) and that litigants rarely make contact with the judge
interest. in any meaningful way such as a pretrial hearing or responsive
Given the frequency with which active attorneys donate pleadings within the first three weeks after a case is filed. This
to judicial campaigns (we identified over 600 Wisconsin led to our second decision to limit all treatments to newly
attorneys who had contributed), some may question whether assigned cases, ensuring that judges received missives about
judicial recusal is even feasible in this context. If donor- cases to which they and the parties had devoted minimal
attorneys routinely appear before judges, or if some attorneys resources. One might object that a litigant could be spending
donate to all judges, would the resulting potential conflicts be that time strategizing about meeting with a specific judge, but
too common for recusal to be practical? It turns out that con- this struck the experts we consulted as unrealistic, especially
flicts stemming from these contributions are relatively rare. As since few of the cases in the experiment involved claims large
we outline below, the total number of donor-judge conflicts enough to justify a lawyer’s time before any filings or meet-
we identified in Wisconsin trial courts over the course of our ings. As an extra precaution, we screened and excluded any
experiment was 398, less than 0.1% of civil cases filed, as cases in which docket activity had occurred in the first few
reported by the Wisconsin Court System (2017). The sorts of weeks. Finally, on the advice of attorneys we hired to examine
conflicts we highlight in this experiment are reasonably com- Wisconsin and federal law, we limit the experimental inter-
monplace over the course of a judges’ term, but it is not the case vention to private letters delivered only to the judges them-
that judges come across them so frequently that they can rea- selves. This constraint effectively prevented us from more
sonably adopt an informal “rule of necessity” precluding re- directly testing the impact of external pressures on recusal.
cusal on the grounds it would prevent the case from being This protocol simplified the logistics of the intervention, at
adjudicated. the cost of potentially weakening its effect, by lessening the
weight of external perceptions from judges’ calculations.
ETHICS Consideration of the ethics of this experiment would not
Field experiments have been criticized for their risks to par- be complete without recognizing that its subject itself is ju-
ticipants and society at large. In this case, while only judges dicial ethics. Campaign contributions by a litigant or lawyer
might easily be construed as an ongoing business relation-
ship, in the sense that they helped a judge attain her current
4. Note that we neglected to include disclosures as an outcome of position. Unlike legislative politics, where candidates typi-
interest in our preanalysis plan, despite having presented the research
design including disclosures at an academic conference before the plan
cally raise much more money from many more donors and
was submitted. See app. D for a more detailed discussion of this omission. the benefits of their policy making are usually widely shared,
The p-values reported below correct for multiple comparisons. elected judges (especially at the trial court level) typically
Volume 83 Number 4 October 2021 / 000

Table 1. Balance across Treatment Groups

Weighted Mean
p-Value (Two-Tailed
Pretreatment Covariate Treatment Group Control Group Weighted t-Test)

Judge gender (male) .665 .651 .913


Attorney gender (male) .791 .673 .310
Case type (divorce) .225 .210 .894
Attorney side (plaintiff ) .631 .730 .432
Donation amount $350.5 $333.2 .746
Attorney’s contributions as share
of total a judge received .033 .041 .675

depend on a small core of financial supporters, and the tential conflict cases (just 1 recusal out of 32 cases), never
decisions they make have immediate consequences for the disclosed the financial relationship on the court record, and
donors who appear in their courtrooms. For further dis- only transferred from four cases. Similarly, the donor-
cussion of ethical considerations and their design implica- attorneys in these cases withdrew at low rates. While the low
tions, see appendix C. base rate of recusal is consistent with previous empirical stud-
ies, the absence of disclosure is striking given the emphasis on
RESULTS transparency in Wisconsin’s Code of Judicial Conduct on this
Over the course of the experiment, we assigned 60 circuit subject (see Wisconsin Court System 2014). It might be sup-
court cases featuring donor-recipient pairs to the control and posed that recusal is a more drastic response to an appearance
treatment conditions. These cases featured 35 judges, had an of a conflict of interest and therefore less common, while dis-
average attorney donation of $331, and covered a spectrum closure is simpler and inoculates judges from allegations aris-
of case types. We treated 28 cases with letters that called ing after the fact. Yet, disclosure is as rare as recusal.
attention to donations ranging from $200 to $1,000. We as- Compared to the control group, judges recuse themselves
signed 32 cases to the control condition. Using weighted t-tests from a potential-conflict case at slightly higher rates when
(see table 1), the distribution of assignment to treatment and they receive a letter requesting recusal (1 recusal out of
control is balanced on judge gender, attorney gender, attorney 28 cases in the experimental group; regression estimates the
side, case type, and donation amount (total amount and pro- average treatment effect to be 1.8 percentage points after
portion of all contributions).5 adjusting for covariates), but this difference is much smaller
Our main results are presented in table 2.6 We calculate the than we initially expected and falls well short of statistical
estimated average treatment effects of the recusal letters on significance at the .05 level.7 Judicial transfers proved to be
four outcomes of interest (we combine applications for re- equally likely in both groups, and attorneys were estimated to
cusal and recusals granted, which are identical in our data set) be 5 percentage points more likely to withdraw in the treat-
and an overall behavioral measure (Any Action) using multi- ment group, a small and statistically insignificant difference.
variate regressions that account for a number of pretreatment While the low rate of recusal among treated judges might
covariates. We also use block-level inverse probability weights suggest that our intervention letters were overlooked or ig-
to account for varying probabilities of assignment. One-tailed nored, the high rate of formal disclosure of the donation
p-values are calculated using randomization inference. We relationship in the court record is strong evidence that the
account for multiple comparisons using a Šidák correction. judges took note of the treatments. Judges who received let-
One striking feature of the results is the very low base rate ters asking them to recuse were 33 percentage points more
of recusal in the control group. In the absence of our inter- likely to disclose than judges who did not receive letters, a
vention, judges almost never recused themselves from po- result that is statistically significant at the p ! :0001 level
(and remains significant at p ! :05 after a Šidák correction).
5. Weights reflect the fact that assignment probabilities varied by block. It is possible that the number of judges entering the
Accordingly, we weight by the inverse of the probability that each judge was conflict into the record reflected or generated cross-judge
assigned to her experimental group.
6. Appendix E reports (planned) standard comparison-of-means tests
and covariate subgroup analyses. 7. See app. D for a more detailed discussion of power calculations.
000 / Campaign Donations, Judicial Recusal, and Disclosure Jonathan S. Krasno et al.

Table 2. Estimated Average Treatment Effects: Multivariate Regression

Weighted Mean

Outcome Control Group Treatment Group Estimated ATE* SE p-Value (One-Tailed)

Recusals granted .031 .037 .018 .053 .319


Judicial transfers .154 .093 2.011 .087 .558
Judicial disclosure of conflict .000 .324 .334 .091 .000
Attorney withdrawal .092 .111 .047 .079 .285
Any action** .216 .398 .260 .115 .020
N 32 28

Note. Šidák correction in our model accounts for five outcomes and results in a level of .0102. Coefficients and p-values in bold remain significant at p ! :05
despite the Šidák correction.
* Based on a regression model with the following pretreatment covariates: judge gender, donor-attorney gender, donor-attorney side (plaintiff or re-
spondent), case type (binary—divorce case or not), and the total donation amount given to the judge by the donor-attorney.
** Collapses outcomes of interest and is coded 1 to denote occurrence of at least one of the outcomes, 0 otherwise. This outcome was not included in our
preanalysis plan but was suggested by a reviewer.

spillover effects of some kind. Although it might be the case One explanation for the results we observe is that, not-
that judges discussed the letters with one another, it does not withstanding their sense that campaign donations ought to be
seem to be the case that this prompted recusals in the control grounds for other judges to recuse, judges may believe they
group. It may be that treated judges decided among them- are capable of acting impartially in their own cases (Marbes
selves to ignore the requests for recusal, although in some 2017). Another possibility is that the intervention we employ
ways that communication would redefine the treatment to be is too circumscribed to inspire action. By restricting ourselves
a combination of receiving the letter and discussing it with to private communications, we effectively test only one of
colleagues, a combination that could have changed behavior the two sets of reasons why judges might decide to recuse
but evidently proved ineffective. themselves, essentially an appeal to their own sense of pro-
Because the project extended over several years and judges priety. A question for future research is whether an appeal
were treated no more than once, we were able to observe to external considerations, such as a threat by a prominent
treated judges in 195 subsequent cases in which a lawyer con- watchdog organization to call public attention to the appear-
tributed to their campaign. We found no instances of dis- ance of impropriety, would spur more recusals. For the time
closure, even among those judges who previously disclosed a being, our experiment contributes a stubborn fact to the lit-
conflict. Notably, this included 107 cases involving the same erature on recusal: a private appeal to judicial ethics fails to
conflict-of-interest pair of judge and donor. Disclosure is not impel many judges to recuse themselves from cases litigated by
a behavior adopted or sustained by judges in the absence of their campaign donors.
monitoring by an outside entity.
REFERENCES
American Judicature Society. 2020. “Methods of Judicial Selection.” Na-
CONCLUSION tional Center for State Courts. http://judicialselection.us/judicial_selection
To our knowledge, this study represents the first randomized /methods/selection_of_judges.cfm?state (accessed May 18, 2020).
field experiment using judicial officials as subjects. As such, Cann, Damon M., Chris W. Bonneau, and Brent D. Boyea. 2012. “Cam-
it provides new insights into judicial impartiality and the paign Contributions and Judicial Decisions in Partisan and Nonpar-
tisan Elections.” In Kevin T. McGuire, ed., New Directions in Judicial
sensitivity of elected judges to the appearance of conflicts of
Politics. New York: Routledge, 38–42.
interest. Although the intervention we describe had no ap- Flamm, Richard E. 2017. Judicial Disqualification: Recusal and Disquali-
preciable effect on recusal, the normative and theoretical im- fication of Judges. Berkeley: Banks & Jordan.
plications of this null finding are far reaching. The private Gibson, James L., and Gregory A. Caldeira. 2012. “Campaign Support,
communications designed to highlight potential conflicts of in- Conflicts of Interest, and Judicial Impartiality: Can Recusals Rescue
the Legitimacy of the Courts?” Journal of Politics 74 (1): 18–34.
terest were insufficient to impel judges to step aside, although Gibson, James L., and Gregory A. Caldeira. 2013. “Judicial Impartiality,
they did move a substantial percentage of treated judges to dis- Campaign Contributions, and Recusals: Results from a National Sur-
close this relationship in the record. vey.” Journal of Empirical Legal Studies 10 (1): 76–103.
Volume 83 Number 4 October 2021 / 000

Hazelton, Morgan L. W., Jacob M. Montgomery, and Brendan Nyhan. 2016. Miller, Banks, and Brett Curry. 2013. “The Effect of Per Se Recusal Rules
“Does Public Financing Affect Judicial Behavior? Evidence from the North on Donor Behavior in Judicial Elections.” Justice System Journal 34 (2):
Carolina Supreme Court.” American Politics Research 44 (4): 587–617. 125–51.
Justice at Stake. 2002. “State Judges Frequency Questionnaire.” Brennan Nelson, Michael J. 2014. “Responsive Justice?” Journal of Law and Courts
Center for Justice. http://www.justiceatstake.org/media/cms/JASJudges 2 (1): 117–52.
SurveyResults_EA8838C0504A5.pdf (accessed September 5, 2017). Palmer, Vernon Valentine. 2010. “The Recusal of American Judges in the
Justice at Stake. 2004. “March 2004 Survey Highlights: Americans Speak Post-Caperton Era: An Empirical Assessment of the Risk of Actual
Out on Judicial Elections.” Brennan Center for Justice. http://www Bias in Decisions Involving Campaign Contributors.” Global Jurist 10
.justiceatstake.org/media/cms/ZogbyPollFactSheet_54663DAB970C6 (3): 4.
.pdf (accessed September 5, 2017). Sample, James, Adam Skaggs, Jonathon Blitzer, and Linda Casey. 2010.
Liptak, Adam, and Janet Roberts. 2006. “Campaign Cash Mirrors a High The New Politics of Judicial Elections, 2000–2009: Decade of Change.
Court’s Rulings.” New York Times, October 1. https://www.nytimes Ed. Charles Hall. Washington, DC: Justice at Stake Campaign.
.com/2006/10/01/us/01judges.html. Wisconsin Court System. 2014. Wisconsin Supreme Court Rules: Chapter 60,
Marbes, Melinda A. 2017. “Reforming Recusal Rules: Reassessing the Pre- Code of Judicial Conduct. https://www.wicourts.gov/supreme/sc_rules.jsp.
sumption of Judicial Impartiality in Light of the Realities of Judging and Wisconsin Court System. 2017. Caseload Summary by Responsible Court
Changing the Substance of Disqualification Standards to Eliminate Cog- Official, 2014–2017. https://www.wicourts.gov/publications/statistics
nitive Errors.” St. Mary’s Journal on Legal Malpractice and Ethics 7:238–303. /circuit/circuitstats.htm. (accessed September 5, 2017).

You might also like