Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Reliability and Validity of

the Adult Reading History


Questionnaire
Dianne L. Lefly and Bruce F. Pennington

The reliability and validity of a revised version of Finucci’s (1982) Reading History Questionnaire was examined in two adult samples.
One sample included 84 adults from an ongoing study of familial dyslexia, and a second sample was composed of parents of 107 chil-
dren from a longitudinal study of reading development. Internal consistency was demonstrated by Cronbach’s alphas of .94 and .92 in
the two samples. Test-retest reliability was demonstrated by significant correlations (.87 and .84 in the two samples) over several years
between an earlier and revised form of the questionnaire. Validity was demonstrated via (a) correlations between the questionnaire score
and reading measures (rs .57-.70), (b) the results of a discriminant function analysis that used questionnaire scores to predict reading
=

disability diagnosis, and (c) the finding that the questionnaire had substantial incremental validity in predicting reading skill in a hier-
archical regression analysis that first entered IQ and SES. These results indicated that the questionnaire is both reliable and valid.

ne of the many factors that can lems. In 1905, Thomas noted that read- Scarborough (1989) reported an ex-
~ place a child at risk for reading ing disability, or dyslexia, tended to tensivelongitudinal study of 78 pre-
disabilities family history
is a &dquo;run in families&dquo; (p. 380). This observa- school children. She was interested in
of reading problems (Snow, Burns, & tion has been repeatedly confirmed what preschool differences might be
Griffin, 1998). Snow et al. identified (see Hallgren, 1950, for a review), and observed between children who later
three types of risk factors: the degree of familial risk has been became dyslexic and those who did
quantified. Across three studies by not. Because reading disability (RD)
1. Child-based risk factors, including Gilger, Pennington, and DeFries (1991), tends to run in families, she reasoned
severe cognitive deficiencies, lan- the median risk for a child of an af- that it would be important to include
guage and hearing impairments, fected parent was 9 times greater than children at high familial risk as well as
and attention-deficit / hyperactivity the population risk. Such a ratio, called those without such risk. Parents with
disorder; relative risk, quantifies the degree of and without a history of dyslexia were
2. Family-based risk factors, includ- familial risk of a disorder. Furthermore, recruited and tested. Thirty-four of the
ing family history of reading diffi- recent research has shown that this in- study children were from families in
culties, and home literacy environ- creased familial risk is due in part to ge- which a parent or sibling reported a
ments ; and netic influences (Cardon et al., 1994; history of dyslexia and had test re-
3. Neighborhood-, community-, and DeFries, Fulker, LaBuda, 1987; Grig-
& sults consistent with that history. The
school-based risk factors, including orenko et al., 1997). It would appear remaining 44 children in the study
ineffective schools and teaching that accurate information about par- were from families with no reading
methods. ental reading history would be valu- difficulty.
able in predicting a child’s likelihood of Children entered the study when
This article addresses the second type developing dyslexia. In fact, research they were 24 months of age. A variety
of risk factor and describes the reliabil- has shown that a significant portion of of early language and preliteracy tasks
ity and validity of an adult reading his- a child’s later reading outcome can be were administered over the preschool

tory questionnaire used to gather read- predicted from his or her parents’ read- period. The reading status of the study
ing history information. ing history (Elbro, Borstrom, & Peter- children was evaluated after they com-
Previous research has consistently sen, 1998 ; Gallagher, Frith, & Snowling, pleted the second grade. At that time,
documented the importance of family in press; Lefly, 1996; Scarborough, 54 of the 78 children were normal read-
history as a risk factor for reading prob- ers while 24 were
1989). reading well below
287

their expected levels. Scarborough volving 107 children. Of those 107 chil- and 5 of 42 (11.9%) normal-risk chil-
(1989) found that the best predictor of dren, 58 were from families in which at dren became dyslexic, yielding a rela-
second-grade reading ability was least one parent reported a signifi- tive risk of 3.1. The children with
family risk status: Sixty-eight percent cant of reading disability, and
history dyslexia differed from the children
(n 23) of the children from the high-
=
49 werefrom families with no history without dyslexia in letter naming, syl-
risk sample were reading significantly of reading disability. In that study, the lable deletion, initial phoneme dele-
below their expected level, whereas parents were not tested to determine tion, phoneme identification, digit
only 2% (n = 1) of the children from the whether their history was consistent span, pronunciation accuracy, distinct-
normal risk sample were below ex- with their reading status. Children be- ness of
phonological representations,
pected level, for a relative risk of 17, gan the study when they were 5 years and receptive vocabulary. Elbro et al.
which is higher than the median value of age and were tested four times be- reported that kindergarten measures
of 9 discussed earlier but still within tween kindergarten and the end of sec- accurately predicted the reading status
the range of previous estimates. In ond grade. The test battery included a of 84.4% of the sample. Their study ex-
addition, family history for dyslexia wide array of phonological processing tended the results of the other two
uniquely accounted for between 30% tasks, reading measures, spelling mea- family-risk studies by adding multiple
and 36% of the outcome variance in sures, and measures of cognitive abil- measures of phonological processing,

reading. Linguistic variables (sound ity. Lefly found that consistent statisti- and addressed the issue of the quality
and letter knowledge and discrete trial cally significant differences existed on of phonological representations with a
lexical retrieval) accounted for an addi- phonological processing variables (pho- measure designed to examine the dis-

tional 7% to 8% of the outcome vari- neme awareness, phoneme perception, tinctness of phonological represen-
ance. In contrast, a broad array of other verbal short-term memory, and lexical tations. This was the first family-risk
variables, including gender, age, SES, access) based on risk status alone from study to address the issue of phono-
preschool IQ, nonverbal measures, the prekindergarten period to the end logical representations with specific
early education, and exposure to read- of the second grade. Diagnosis of read- measures. Elbro et al. reported that the

ing and television, did not contribute ing disability was made near the end of measure of distinctness of phonologi-

significantly to reading outcome. A the study. Of the original high-risk cal representations contributed unique
combination of family history and sample, 23 (39.7%) of the children were variance beyond phoneme awareness
scores on the linguistic variables suc- diagnosed with RD and 35 (60.3%) in prediction of later reading outcome.
cessfully predicted 82% of reading out- were diagnosed without. In the low- These studies provide indirect evi-
comes. This finding documents the im- risk sample, 3 (6.1%) were diagnosed dence that adult self-reports of reading
portance of family history in the with RD and 46 (93.9%) were diag- difficulties are reliable and valid. There
development of reading disabilities. nosed without. Therefore, the relative is also direct evidence in the literature
In another ongoing, prospective risk was 6.5 in that sample. That study that such self-reports by adults are re-
study of preschool children at risk for was consistent with the others and liable and valid (Decker, Volger, & De-
developing dyslexia, Gallagher et al. clearly indicated the importance of Fries, 1989 ; Finucci, Whitehouse, Isaacs,
(in press) studied preschool children family history in the development of & Childs, 1984; Gilger, 1992; Schulte-
from 67 high-risk families and 37 low- explicit and implicit phonological pro- Korne, Deimel, & Remschmidt, 1997).
risk families. Because the participants cessing skills that are thought to be Decker et al. found that parents who
were preschoolers, the impact of fam- precursors to later reading skill. self-reported reading problems scored
ily risk on the precursors of reading was The most recent prospective study of significantly worse on an actual read-
assessed. These children were tested preschool children at high and normal ing than parents who did not re-
test
on a variety of speech and language familial risk for reading disability was port such problems. Finucci and her
measures that included more measures conducted by Elbro and his colleagues colleagues developed an adult reading
of phonological processing than did in Denmark (1998). In that study, par- history questionnaire to gather reading
Scarborough’s (1989) study. Gallagher ents were asked about their history of history information for which they re-
et al. found differences in knowledge reading problems, and then they were ported validity but no reliability data.
of nursery rhymes among 3- and 4- tested to verify their status. Forty-nine They found significant correlations be-
year-old children based on risk status high-risk children with at least one tween the overall score on the ques-
alone. The results of the Gallagher et al. parent with dyslexia and 42 normal- tionnaire and a reading test in two
study are consistent with Scarbor- risk Danish children entered the study groups: a family group (n 154) made
=

ough’s and extend the results of her in kindergarten, when they were 6 up of relatives of children who had
study by including more phonological years old. The children were followed been diagnosed with dyslexia, and a
processing measures. into second grade, at which time diag- control group (n =
90) of individuals
Lefly (1996) reported on a longitudi- noses were made. Of the children in whose socioeconomic status was simi-
nal reading development study in- the study, 18 of 49 (36.7%) high-risk lar to that of the family group. These
288

correlations were -.43 and -.25, respec- about learning letter names was added the original, and because Finucci’s
tively (both ps < .01 ). Hence, the poorer because research has shown that this is (1982) study provided no information
readers had higher self-report scores a specific area of difficulty for these in- about reliability. The results of our re-
than the better readers in both groups. dividuals. Another question directly liability and validity studies are re-
Recently, Schulte-K6rne et al. (1997) addressed difficulties in learning to spell ported next.
used Finucci’s (1982) questionnaire in a in elementary school. Another new
study to determine whether it would question asked about current reading Method

be advisable to substitute information speed in comparison to others of the


from a reading history questionnaire in same age and education level. We have ’

place of actual test data for adults. also observed that individuals with Participants ,
They used two types of discriminant dyslexia tend to put forth more effort Two independent samples used
were

analysis-linear discriminant analysis to achieve the same result as their in this study. One sample was made up
and Correlation and Regression Trees peers do, and have difficulty remem- of adults with and without dyslexia
(CART; Breiman, Friedman, Olshen, & bering complex instructions and direc- participating in a separate ongoing
Stone, 1984) analysis-to assess the tions. Consequently, items were added study of familial dyslexia (Pennington
consistency of classification rates be- to the original Finucci questionnaire et al., 1986; Smith, Kimberling, Pen-
tween actual psychometric test data that addressed these issues. nington, & Lubs, 1983). A second sam-
and the questionnaire. Overall, 71% of The original reading history ques- ple was made up of parents of children
the participants with RD and 98% of tionnaire (RHQ) was designed in a participating in the longitudinal study
those without were correctly classified forced choice format; that is, the re- of reading development (Lefly, 1996)
by the linear discriminant function spondent checked off a prescribed re- discussed earlier.
analysis, and 87% of the participants sponse to a series of questions, such as These two samples were ascertained
with RD and 91% of those without &dquo;Did you have trouble learning to read in different ways. The familial sam-
were correctly classified by the CART in elementary school?&dquo; (Yes, No, Don’t ple was a subsample from a large
analysis. Schulte-K6rne et al. con- remember) and &dquo;Did you ever require sample of families selected for a three-
cluded that, although it would not be tutoring or remedial reading class dur- generation history of reading disability
good practice to substitute the ques- ing your education?&dquo; (No, Little, Mod- who have been participants in our
tionnaire data for psychometric test erate, Great deal, Do not know). We felt ongoing research on the genetics of
data for diagnostic purposes, the con- that this format was not sensitive dyslexia. The total familial sample con-
cordance rate between objective test enough to the range of individual differ- sists of 359 individuals in 19 extended
measures and subjective self-report ences in adult reporters. We changed families. Of the 359 individuals in the
data was very high. the format to a Likert scale format in original sample, a subsample of 84
Unlike these studies of concurrent which the respondent could indicate were participants in the present study.
validity, Gilger’s (1992) study com- the degree of difficulty she or he had Sixty-nine of the study participants
pared adult self-report to actual read- with a particular area important to were adults when the original genetic

ing test scores obtained in childhood. reading. All responses were on the study began, and 15 were children who
Even with this time gap, he found same scale and allowed for ratings have since grown to adulthood. Over
moderate correlations (r s .32-.68) be-
=
along a continuum from 0 to 4 (e.g., a the years, we have gathered individ-
tween a retrospective adult self-report person who had no difficulty learning ual and family histories and IQ and
survey and childhood test scores in a to read might respond with a 0 to a achievement test data from the mem-
large sample of participants. question about difficulty when learn- bers of the family sample. In one of
Hence, previous research has shown ing to read; an individual who had ex- our mailings, we sent out our revised
thatparental reading history seems to treme difficulty could respond with adult reading history questionnaire
be an important factor in predicting a 4). This type of format also allows (ARHQ). The 84 individuals who com-
reading outcome for children, and that some control for socially appropriate pleted and returned the revised ques-
adult self-report information can be responding. Some respondents may be tionnaire constitute the familial sample
considered valid and reliable. Thus, a hesitant to say they had any difficulty in the reliability and validity studies
reliable and valid instrument for gath- learning to read or spell. These indi- reported in this article. Of those, 53
ering reading history would be very viduals might answer &dquo;No&dquo; to such a family members who completed the
useful. question in a forced-choice format but new questionnaire had filled out a
pre-
To this end, we modified the original would be less hesitant to indicate some vious version of the questionnaire an
Finucci (1982) questionnaire by adding level of difficulty on a Likert scale. average of 9.7 years ago. The longi-
more items derived from our clinical We felt it was important to evaluate tudinal sample was made up of par-
and research experience with adults the reliability and validity of the ques- ents of young children who completed
with dyslexia. For instance, a question tionnaire because we made changes to a 3-year longitudinal study of reading
289

development (N =107). All parents of diagnosed as &dquo;not reading disabled&dquo; Diagnostic Methods in the
children who participated in the study (NRD) had negative reading histories Family Sample ..

were asked to fill out an adult reading and negative test results. The 15 indi-
history questionnaire when their chil- viduals who were children when they Unlike the diagnosis in Table 1, for the
dren began the longitudinal study. One were originally diagnosed were not in-
current study each member of the
of the variables of interest in the longi- cluded in the validity sample. This re- adult family sample was diagnosed as
tudinal duced the sample included in the RD or NRD based on test results alone.
study was parental reading
Previous history of reading difficulties
history, which was used to select fami- study from 77 to 62.
lies with a parent with reading prob- In the was not used as part of the diagnostic
longitudinal sample, we had
lems (the high-risk group) and without criteria for this study. In earlier studies
two completed ARHQs from 54 fathers
and 60 mothers in the longitudinal of this and other samples with
(the low-risk group). Three years later,
at the end of the longitudinal study, we sample. As explained earlier, families dyslexia, we have used two test-based
asked the parents to fill out another were classified as low or high risk adult diagnostic criteria to determine
who had RD. These criteria were as fol-
copy of the adult reading history ques- based on parental reading history. Ta-
tionnaire. ble 2 reveals no significant risk group lows :
The familial sample was used for differences for age or SES, but there
both reliability and validity analyses. were, of course, differences for the ini- 1. A reading quotient (RQ)-a score
The longitudinal sample was used tial RHQ score, as this was used to as- that compares observed reading
only for reliability analyses, as we did sign risk status. What is of interest here (Gray Oral Reading Test; Wieder-
not testthe parents in the longitudinal is that very similar group scores were holt & Bryant, 1984) and spelling
sample. Because the two samples were obtained on the second RHQ, suggest- (WRAT Spelling; Jastak & Wilkin-
ascertained in different ways, they pro- ing that there is longitudinal stability son, 1984) outcome to that pre-
vide a good test of how robust the reli- for this measure. dicted based on an individual’s
ability estimates are.

TABLE 1
Demographics of the FamilialI
and Longitudinal Samples Diagnostic Information for the Family Sample
In the familial sample, 84 adults re-
turned the ARHQ. Previous IQ and
achievement test results were available
for 77 of those returning the ARHQ.
Seven of the 84 were elderly family
members without previous test results,
so their ARHQ data were not included
in the validity portion of the study. Of
the 77 family members who had been Note. RD NRD
=
reading disability; = no reading disability.
previously tested and diagnosed, 15
were children and 62 were adults when

originally diagnosed. Table 1 presents TABLE 2


the original diagnostic classifications
for these individuals. The original di-
Mean Age, SES, and RHQ Scores for Longitudinal Sample
agnostic scheme incorporated both
reading history and test results. People
who were originally classified as RD
reported a positive history of RD and
had positive test results. &dquo;Compen-
sated dyslexics&dquo; were those who re-
ported a history of reading difficulty
but whose test results were not diag-
nostic of RD. Persons classified as
&dquo;questionable&dquo; were those who re-
ported negative reading histories but
whose test results were diagnostic of Note. SES = socioeconomic status; RHQ =
reading history questionnaire.
RD. Individuals who were originally ***p < .001.
290

overall ability (Finucci et al., 1984). Status (Hollingshead, 1975). The score in age, age2 (age at revised ARHQ), or
An RQ less than .80 is considered on this index is based the educa-
on IQ. As expected, significant differences
indicative of reading difficulty. tional and occupational levels of the were found between participants with

The RQ has been described in de- head of household for each family. The and without RD in years of education,
tail in
previous publications (Pen- Index provides for eight educational Hollingshead Index of Social Status,
nington, Lefly, Van Orden, Book- categories (from fewer than 8 years of RQ, and Discrim, and on all achieve-
man, & Smith, 1987; Pennington education to graduate and profes- ment measures.
et al., 1986); sional school) and seven occupational I! {’~ .
2. An age-discrepancy score categories (from professional and
(Discrim)-this is an age-
score upper management to unskilled work-
TABLE 3
adjusted discriminant function ers). The scores on the index can range
score. It is based on the Peabody from 11 to 77. Lower scores reflect The Adult Family Sample by Diagnosis
and Gender
Individual Achievement Test more education and higher-ranked oc-

(American Guidance Service 1970) cupations, while higher scores reflect


Reading Recognition, Reading less education and lower level occu-
Comprehension, and Spelling sub- pations. Although the Hollingshead
test scores, and was developed to scores for the two groups were signifi-

discriminate between school-aged cantly different from one another, both


children with and without school groups were in the range considered
reports of reading problems middle class. An examination of Ta- Note. RD =
reading disability; NRD = no reading
(Vogler & DeFries, 1983). ble 4 reveals no significant differences disability.
~’~ ,

These scores were significantly cor-


related with one another (r .77) in the
=

adult sample. The RQ is an IQ discrep- TABLE 4


ancy method of determining reading Descriptive Statistics and Mean Test Scores for Adult Family Sample
disability, whereas the Discrim is an
age-based discrepancy score not based
on IQ. It identifies those who are

having difficulty reading regardless of


IQ. In this study, a person was classi-
fied as RD if he or she met either IQ-
discrepant or age-discrepant criteria.
The agreement between the two types
of diagnostic criteria was 90% in the
adult sample. The IQ-discrepant cri-
terion identified four individuals as
having RD that the age-discrepant cri-
terion identified as NRD, and the age-
discrepant criterion identified two in-
dividuals as having RD that the
IQ-discrepant criteria identified as
NRD. Table 3 provides the details of
the family sample by diagnosis and
gender.
Table 4 presents demographic, IQ,
and achievement data scores for the
family sample adults in the validity
study. Two age variables are included
in the table: age at original question-
naire completion and age at second
questionnaire completion. Information
regarding the socioeconomic status of Note. RD reading disability; NRD no reading disability; SES socioeconomic status.
= = =

participant families was gathered aage at revised ARHQ.


using the Hollingshead Index of Social *p < .05. ***p < .001..
291

Scoring the ARHQ TABLE 5


Scores onthe ARHQ are computed by Means and Standard Deviations of Revised and Original RHQ in the Family Sample
dividing total number of points ob-
the
tained by an individual by the total
number of points possible on the ques-
tionnaire. Each item is in a Likert for-
mat, with scores ranging from 0 to 4.
The scale was constructed so that
lower scores indicate less difficulty.
The first 23 questionnaire items are used Note. RHQ =
reading history questionnaire; RD =
reading disability; NRD = no reading disability.
in the calculation of the score. There
are 92 possible points possible for these

23 items. of theoriginal and revised Adult Read- In Gilger’s (1992) study, one of the
Originally, 25 questions were related ing History Questionnaire scores by di- most interesting findings was the sig-
to an individual’s reading history; agnostic group. The means and stan- nificant correlation between childhood
dard deviations on both versions of the test results and adult reading history
however, 2 items had too many miss-
questionnaire were very similar, just as results. In the present study, we have a
ing data points to be useful and were
eliminated. One of these asked about was observed for the longitudinal sam- small sample (n 15) of individuals
=

ple in Table 2, even though the test- who were diagnosed as RD or NRD in
difficulty learning foreign languages,
and the other asked about the fre- sample was much
retest interval in that childhood and completed ARHQs as
longer (9.7 years, on average). adults. The correlations between their
quency with which the individual read
to his or her children or grandchildren. In the longitudinal sample, the test- adult ARHQ scores and the diagnostic
Both of these questions are clinically was 3 years. The corre-
retest interval criteria that were calculated for them
lation between the two questionnaires as children were .75 ( p < .01 ) for the RQ
relevant, but many people left these
two questions unanswered, or indi- in the longitudinal sample was .81 and .88 ( p < .01) for the Discrim.
cated that they did not take foreign lan- (n 59) for mothers and .84 (n 52) for
= =
These correlations were somewhat
fathers. higher than those reported by Gilger
guage classes or did not have children
or grandchildren. A copy of the Adult
In sum, these results demonstrate (1992) and Finucci et al. (1984). The rea-
is in that the ARHQ had similarly high lev- son for these higher correlations is un-
Reading History Questionnaire els of both internal consistency and clear. It is possible that the diagnostic
the Appendix.
test-retest reliability in both samples. criteria were more sensitive than those
Because of the length of the test-retest used by the others, or that the longer
Results and Discussion interval, there is also evidence for con- questionnaire was responsible for the
siderable longitudinal stability of this higher correlations. In sum, the corre-
measure in adults. lations between the ARHQ and the di-
Reliability
Nonetheless, questionnaires can have agnostic criteria, whether collected in
In terms of internal consistency, the items with a high degree of internal childhood or in adulthood, support the
questionnaire was evaluated in both
consistency and can show test-retest validity of the ARHQ.
samples independently, to ensure that In addition to the correlational anal-
the results were not driven by the
reliability without being valid. To as-
sess validity, we correlated the ARHQ ysis, two other types of analyses were
highly selected familial sample. The scores with actual measures of read- performed to address the validity of
Cronbach’s alpha for the 23-item earlier from the familial the ARHQ: a discriminant function
ing gathered
ARHQ was .94 in the familial sample sample. and a regression analysis. The discrimi-
and .92 in the longitudinal sample, in- nant function analyses were performed
dicating that the items were highly in- to answer two different questions:
tercorrelated with one another and that Validity
this result was not sample dependent. The validity study involved partici-
1. What score on the ARHQ is the
So, the questionnaire clearly demon- pants from the family sample, as de-
strates internal scribed earlier. The ARHQ was highly best cutoff between individuals
consistency.
To evaluate test-retest reliability, we correlated with the test-based diagnos- with dyslexia and those without?
correlated the scores of the earlier and tic criteria (RQ: r = .57; Discrim: r = .70) 2. What differences exist between
later forms of the questionnaire in both in the adult validity sample. In con- those diagnosed by IQ discrepancy
samples. The correlation between the trast, no significant correlations existed criteria and those diagnosed by
two questionnaires in the family sam- between either version of the ARHQ age discrepancy criteria on the
ple was .87. Table 5 presents the means and age or IQ. ARHQ cutoff score?
292

In the discriminant analysis, the as the SES measure because it incorpo- reading scores after SES and IQ were

ARHQ score was used to predict RD rated both education and occupational accounted for.
and NRD diagnoses based on test cri- information. The Discrim diagnostic The study presented here is consis-
teria. The results of this analysis re- score was chosen as the reading mea- tent with, and extends, previous re-

vealed that a cutoff score of .40 resulted sure because it is an age-adjusted mea- search in the However, there are
area.

in a sensitivity of 81.8%, a specificity of sure of reading skill that does not ad- some limitations to this study. First, the

77.5%, and an overall correct classifica- just for IQ. The Hollingshead score was ARHQ does not require participants to
tion rate of 79.0% (see Table 6). entered first, followed by IQ and, fi- specify any type of specific cause for
As can be seen, only two thirds nally, the ARHQ score. The results of parental reading difficulties (e.g., brain
(18 / 27) of those classified as RD by the this analysis are found in Table 7. injury, lack of schooling, hearing im-
ARHQ met test criteria for RD. This Together SES, IQ, and ARHQ ex- pairment, etc.). This would be a good
overprediction arose in part from the plained 51% of the variance in the follow-up question to add to the ques-
compensated adults who were diag- reading scores, more than half of tionnaire. The answer would help dis-
nostically NRD while having higher which was accounted for by the ARHQ criminate individuals whose reading
scores on the ARHQ, representing a after the effects of SES and IQ had been difficulties were based on a specific,
history of reading problems. Four of taken into account. These results pro- identifiable etiology, such as head in-
the nine participants who were diag- vide evidence of incremental validity jury, from the others.
nosed as NRD by tests but classified as for the ARHQ. Another limitation to this study is
RD by the discriminant analysis were that the individuals in the study were
compensated adults. The remaining primarily middle class, with above-
compensated adults had history scores average IQ scores. Any generalizations
that were low enough for them to be ,

Summary should be made to a similar group. In


classified as NRD according to the dis- the future, it would be important to ob-
criminant criteria. The reliability of the ARHQ was tain data from a wider
sample to test
It is important for those making use demonstrated by its internal consis- whether these findings extend to a
of this questionnaire to be aware that tency in both study samples (family broader population. This limitation
there arecompensated adults who re- sample: .94, longitudinal sample: .92), means that caution should be exercised

port significant reading history but


a and by the high correlations between
are not diagnosably RD. Their positive two versions the ARHQ (family sam-

reading history may place their chil- ple : .87, longitudinal sample: .84), even TABLE 6
dren at risk for RD although they are across a long test-retest interval. The Classification Based on
not themselves currently RD. validity of the ARHQ was demon- Discriminant Analysis
The results of the discriminant strated by the high correlation between
analysis did not vary much as a result the ARHQ and diagnostic measures
of IQ age discrepancy diag-
versus for adults (r s = .57-.70) and by the high
noses. Follow-up analyses revealed correlations for those study partici-
that a .40 cutoff resulted in a sensitivity pants who were originally diagnosed
of 83.3% and a specificity of 76.3% for as children and later completed the

adults diagnosed with IQ-discrepant ARHQ as adults (rs = .75-.88). Consis-


criteria. A sensitivity of 76.2% and spe- tent with these correlations, the ARHQ
cificity of 70.0% was found for adults performed reasonably well in a dis- Note. 79.0% of original grouped cases correctly
diagnosed based on the age-discrepant criminant function analysis and ac- classified. RD = reading disabled; NRD = no
criterion. The questionnaire and the .40 counted for considerable variance in reading disability. :

cutoff appeared to classify a slightly


higher percentage of IQ-discrepant in-
dividuals with dyslexia, but the differ-
TABLE 7
ence was quite small.
Regression Summary Model Summary
The third and final validity test in-
volved a hierarchical regression analy-
sis. It was performed to address the
issue of incremental validity. Specifi-
cally, it tested whether the ARHQ
added significant predictive informa-
tion to demographic information such
as SES and IQ. In this sample, the

Hollingshead Index Score was chosen Note. SES = socioeconomic status; ARHQ = adult reading history questionnaire.
293

in interpreting the results if the ques- Breiman, L., Friedman, J. H., Olshen, R. A., genetic study. Acta Psychiatrica et Neuro-
tionnaire is used in clinical or research & Stone, C. J. (1984). Classification and re- logica Supplement, 65, 1-287.
settings where the population is mark- gression trees. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index
International Group. of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni-
edly different from the ones used here. Cardon, L. R., Smith, S. D., Fulker, D. W.,
Obviously, not every child who has a versity.
Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Wide
parent with a positive history of read- DeFries, J. C. (1994). Quantitative trait lo-
ing difficulty will develop reading dif- range achievement test-Revised. Wiliming-
cus for reading disability on chromosome
ton, DE: Jastak Assessment Systems.
ficulty. As stated earlier, the likelihood 6. Science, 266, 276-279.
of developing RD is considerably Decker, S. N., Vogler, G. P., & DeFries, J. C. Lefly, D. L. (1996). Phonological processing skills
and risk status. Unpublished doctoral dis-
greater in families with a history of (1989). Validity of self-reported reading sertation, University of Denver, Denver.
RD, but research shows that at least disability by parents of reading-disabled
roughly half of the children in such and control children. Reading and Writing, Pennington, B. F., Gilger, J. W., Olson, R. K.,
families will not develop RD. On the ,1327-331. (1992). The external valid-
& DeFries, J. C.

other hand, just because a parent does DeFries, J. C., Fulker, D W., & LaBuda,
of
ity age- versus IQ-discrepancy defini-
tions of reading disability: Lessons from
not report a history of reading diffi-
M. C. (1987). Evidence for a genetic aeti- a twin study. Journal of Learning Disabili-
ology in reading disability of twins. Na-
culty does not mean the child will not ture, 329, 537-539. ties, 25, 562-573.
develop RD. Some cases of RD are non- Elbro, C., Borstrom, I., & Petersen, D. K. Pennington, B. F., Lefly, D. L., Van Orden,
familial. Moreover, the ARHQ does not G. C., Bookman, M. O., & Smith, S. D.
(1998). Predicting dyslexia from kinder-
perfectly detect every familial case. Thus, garten: The importance of distinctness of (1987). Is phonology bypassed in normal
clinicians and researchers should use of lexical or dyslexic development? Annals of Dys-
phonological representations
this questionnaire as a screening in- items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), lexia, 37, 62-89.
strument and not as a diagnostic tool. 36-60. Pennington, B. F., McCabe, L. L., Smith,
With these caveats in mind, it appears Finucci, J. M., Isaacs, S. D., Whitehouse, S. D., Lefly, D. L., Bookman, M. O., Kim-
that the ARHQ is a reliable and valid C. C., & Childs, B. (1982). Empirical vali- berling, W. J., & Lubs, H. A. (1986).
instrument for gathering information dation of reading and spelling quotients. Spelling errors in adults with a form of fa-
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol- milial dyslexia. Child Development. Aug,
about parental reading history.
ogy, 24
, 733-744. (4), 1001-1013.
57
Finucci, J. M., Whitehouse, C. C., Isaacs, Scarborough, H. S. (1989). Prediction of
ABOUT THE AUTHORS S. D., & Childs, B. (1984). Derivation and reading disability from familial and indi-
Dianne L. Lefly, PhD, is a past research asso- validation of a quantitative definition of vidual differences. Journal of Educational
ciate at the University of Denver and currently specific reading disability for adults. De- Psychology, 81 (1 ), 101-108.
is research manager for assessment in the Den- velopmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Schulte-K&ouml;rne, G., Deimel, W., & Rem-
ver public schools. Bruce F. Pennington, PhD, ,26
143-153. schmidt, H. (1997). Can self-report data
holds the John Evans professorship in psychol- Gallagher, A. M., Frith, U., & Snowling, M. J. on deficits in reading and spelling predict
ogy at the University of Denver and is director (in press). Language processing skills in
spelling disability as defined by psycho-
of the Developmental Neuropsychology Center. pre-schoolers at risk of developmental metric tests? Reading and Writing, 9,
He is the recipient of both a Research Scientist dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology, Psy- 55-63.
Award and a MERIT award from the National chiatry, and Allied Disciplines.
Institutes of Health. Address: Dianne L. Lefly, Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington,
Gilger, J. W. (1992). Using self-report and B. F., & Lubs, H. A. (1983). Specific read-
900 Grant St., Denver, CO 80203.
parental report survey data to assess past ing disability: Identification of an inher-
and present academic achievement of
ited form through linkage analysis. Sci-
AUTHORS’ NOTE adults and children. Journal of Applied De-
ence,219, 1345-1347.
This research was supported by NIMH grants velopmental Psychology, 13, 235-256. Snow, C., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.).
No. MH38820 (MERIT Award), MH00419 Gilger, J. W., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, in
J. C. (1991). Risk for reading disability as (1998). Preventing reading difficulties
(Research Scientist Award), an NICHD Learn- young children. Washington, DC: National
a function of parental history in three
ing Disabilities Research Center Grant Academy Press.
(HD27802), and Mental Retardation Research family studies. Reading and Writing, 3,
205-217. Thomas, C. J. (1905). Congenital ’word-
Center Grant NICHD (HD04024), as well as blindness’ and its treatment. Ophthalmo-
Grigorenko, E. L.,
Wood, B., Meyer, M. S.,
F.
grants from the March of Dimes Foundation scope, 3, 380-385.
and the Orton Dyslexia Society. Hart, L. A., Speed, W. C., Shuster, A., &
Pauls, D. L. (1997). Susceptibility loci for Vogler, G. P., & DeFries, J. C. (1983). Linear-
distinct components of developmental ity of offspring-parent regression for
REFERENCES
dyslexia on chromosomes 6 and 15. Amer- general cognitive ability. Behavior Genet-
American Guidance Service. (1970). Pea- ican Journal of Human Genetics, 60, 27-39. ics, 13, 355-360.
body individual achievement test. Circle Hallgren, B. (1950). Specific dyslexia (con- Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (1984). Gray
Pines, MN: Author. genital word blindness): A clinical and oral reading test. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
294

APPENDIX
... Adult Reading History Questionnaire

PLEASE NOTE: This reading history questionnaire applies to you, and not to your children.
Please circle the number of the response that most nearly describes your attitude or experience for each of the following ques-
tions or statements. If you think your response would be between numbers, place an &dquo;X&dquo; where you think it should be.
296

Scoring the RHQ: ....

Scores on the RHQ are calculated by totaling the points an individual scores on the first 23 items (the other 3 questions are for
informational purposes only) on the questionnaire, and dividing by 92. This should yield a percentage score for that person.
Generally, scores greater than .30 are considered to be indicative of a positive history of RD.

You might also like