Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jappdp

Testing TIK (Tuning in to Kids) with TEC (Test of Emotion Comprehension):


Does enhanced emotion socialization improve child
emotion understanding?
Rune Flaaten Bjørk a, *, Evalill Bølstad a, Francisco Pons a, Sophie Seychelle Havighurst b
a
University of Oslo, Norway
b
University of Melbourne, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study examined the predictive link between enhanced parental emotion-related socialization behaviors
Emotion socialization (ERSBs) and children’s emotional understanding. Parental ERSBs were enhanced using the Tuning in to Kids
Emotion understanding (TIK; Havighurst & Harley, 2007) parenting intervention, and children’s emotional understanding was directly
Emotion coaching
assessed using the Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC; Pons & Harris, 2000). 40 children in their final year of
Parenting intervention
kindergarten and one of their parents participated in the study. TIK led to positive changes in parental expressive
encouragement. All children showed statistically significant improvements in emotion understanding at six-
month follow-up. As expected, parental expressive encouragement positively predicted children’s development
of emotional understanding from baseline to follow-up. Thus, when parents encouraged children to talk about
emotions, children’s emotional understanding improved. Unexpectedly, emotion-focused responses (warm but
dismissive) negatively predicted children’s development of emotion understanding, Children’s emotional un­
derstanding did not improve as much when parents responded with comfort and distraction. Implications are
discussed.

Emotion understanding is the ability to recognize and predict emo­ focused parenting program appropriate for universal delivery, where the
tions in oneself and others (Denham, 1998; Harris, 1989; Saarni, 1999; main aim is to enhance parental ERSBs. Intervention designs can be used
Saarni & Harris, 1991), and is related to several developmental out­ to test developmental models examining a causal link between parental
comes in children (Bender, Pons, Harris, Esbjørn, & Reinholdt-Dunne, ERSBs and emotion understanding in children. Investigations of
2015; Cassidy, Werner, Rourke, Zubernis, & Balaraman, 2003; Den­ emotion-focused parenting programs provide emerging evidence of this
ham, 1986; Denham et al., 2003; Southam-Gerow & Kendall, 2000). A link (Havighurst et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015; Havighurst, Wil­
large body of research has linked emotion understanding in children to son, Harley, Prior, & Kehoe, 2010; Salmon, Dadds, Allen, & Hawes,
parent emotion-related socialization behaviors (ERSBs) – that is, the 2009; Van Bergen, Salmon, Dadds, & Allen, 2009; Wilson, Havighurst, &
way parents discuss emotions, model emotional expressions, and how Harley, 2012), but these studies did not investigate whether the positive
they respond to children’s emotions (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, changes in child outcomes were caused by positive changes in parental
1998). However, no previous studies have directly tested whether ERSBs. Other studies investigating the link between parental ERSBs and
enhanced ERSBs predict improvements in children’s emotional under­ emotion understanding in children have expanded on existing behav­
standing. Such information is needed to inform practice (i.e., emotion- ioral parenting interventions. Salmon, Dittman, Sanders, Burson, and
focused parenting interventions) and is crucial for understanding Hammington (2014), for example, compared an Emotion Enhanced
emotion socialization processes. Triple-P with the standard Triple-P parenting program. They found that
The current study uses an intervention design to test these associa­ parental ERSBs improved more in the emotion-enhanced condition post-
tions, applying the emotion-focused parenting program Tuning in to intervention and at a 4-month follow-up assessment, but there were no
Kids (TIK; Havighurst & Harley, 2007). TIK is a six-session emotion- differences between children’s emotional understanding in the two

* Corresponding author at: University of Oslo, Norway.


E-mail addresses: r.f.bjork@psykologi.uio.no (R.F. Bjørk), e.b.karevold@psykologi.uio.no (E. Bølstad), francisco.pons@psykologi.uio.no (F. Pons), sshavi@
unimelb.edu.au (S.S. Havighurst).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2021.101368
Received 18 July 2020; Received in revised form 19 November 2021; Accepted 22 November 2021
Available online 30 November 2021
0193-3973/© 2021 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

groups. It is therefore not evident that improving ERSBs in parents will (Pons et al., 2004). The TEC (Pons & Harris, 2000) measures the
lead to positive changes in children’s emotional understanding. developmental phases of emotion understanding and is, therefore, a
Simultaneously, most previous intervention studies on emotion so­ valuable instrument to apply in emotion socialization research.
cialization and emotion understanding have focused on emotion Most research on emotion socialization, however, has not applied a
recognition and causes of emotions (e.g., Denham & Kochanoff, 2002; developmental model of emotion understanding but has instead focused
Denham, Zoller, & Couchoud, 1994) that develop in the first phase of on external components of emotional understanding (e.g., Havighurst
emotion understanding from 3 to 4 years of age (Cutting & Dunn, 1999; et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015). A link between parental ERSBs and
Denham & Burton, 1996; Harris, 1989; Pons, Harris, & de Rosnay, children’s external emotion understanding has been established in
2004). Thus, there is an additional gap in the emotion socialization several studies using the Denham (1986) puppet task (for a review, see
literature concerning the later stages of emotion understanding (i.e., Denham et al., 2015). Denham’s (1986) puppet task is sometimes also
understanding mixed emotions or moral emotions), and it is largely referred to as the Emotion Skills Task, the Affective Knowledge Test, or
unknown how parental ERSBs affect the latter developmental phases of simply the puppet interview (Morgan, Izard, & King, 2010). The Coping
emotion understanding. with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES; Fabes, Eisenberg, &
Bernzweig, 1990) is often used as a measure of parental ERSBs in
A developmental model of emotion understanding conjunction with the puppet interview in the emotion socialization
literature. The CCNES is a widely used self-report questionnaire
Emotion understanding is one of the main constituents of emotional assessing parental ERSBs. It measures three types of supportive (parental
competence, in addition to emotional expressiveness and emotion expressive encouragement, emotion-focused, and problem-focused re­
regulation (Denham et al., 2003; Denham, Bassett, & Wyatt, 2015). actions) and three types of non-supportive (parental distress, mini­
Emotion understanding is positively related to children’s social adjust­ mizing and punitive reactions) ERSBs (for a review, see Johnson, Hawes,
ment, school achievement, academic success, and action coordination in Eisenberg, Kohlhoff, & Dudeney, 2017). In keeping with prior studies,
peer interaction (Bardack & Widen, 2019; Franco, Beja, Candeias, & we used the CCNES in the present study, but instead of the puppet
Santos, 2017; Viana, Zambrana, Karevold, & Pons, 2019). It is also interview, we used the TEC to measure developmental stages of emotion
positively related to the development of cognitive functioning, such as understanding. However, we still review prior research with the puppet
intelligence, working memory, and language development (Albanese, task as it provides a conceptual basis for the present investigation, as
De Stasio, Di Chiacchio, Fiorilli, & Pons, 2010; De Stasio, Fiorilli, & Di there is, to our knowledge, no studies on the impact on ERSB in­
Chiacchio, 2014; Morra, Parrella, & Camba, 2011), and negatively terventions on developmental stages of emotion understanding in
related to aggression, anxiety, depression, and behavior problems in children.
children (Bender et al., 2015; Schuberth et al., 2019; Southam-Gerow &
Kendall, 2002). Parental ERSBs predict emotion understanding
According to the developmental model of emotion understanding
(Pons et al., 2004), children’s emotional understanding develops Fabes, Poulin, Eisenberg, and Madden-Derdich (2002) conducted a
through a series of three partly overlapping phases during childhood: psychometric evaluation of the CCNES, using the puppet interview as an
the external, the mental, and the reflective phases (Harris, 1994; Harris, outcome measure to investigate the relationship between parental
Johnson, Hutton, Andrews, & Cooke, 1989; Kockeritz, Klinkhammer, & ERSBs and children’s external emotion understanding. They found that
von Salisch, 2010; Pons et al., 2004; Pons & Harris, 2005; Sprung, parental supportive ERSBs positively and non-supportive ERSBs nega­
Munch, Harris, Ebesutani, & Hofmann, 2015). The TEC was developed tively predicted child emotion understanding. In other words, when
to specifically assess these three phases of emotion understanding, parents comforted and distracted (i.e., emotion-focused reactions),
including three components for each phase, yielding a total of nine problem-solved (i.e., problem-focused reactions), and encouraged chil­
components: 1) recognition of emotion labels, 2) understanding external dren to share and express feelings (i.e., expressive encouragement),
causes of emotions, 3) understanding emotions that are based on desires children tended to have a better understanding of emotions. On the
4) comprehending belief-based emotions, 5) understanding that emo­ other hand, when parents responded with punitive responses (e.g.,
tions can be elicited by reminders, 6) understanding that emotions can yelling at the child), minimization (e.g., telling the child to stop crying),
be regulated 7) understanding hidden emotions, 8) comprehending or distress (e.g., losing their temper), children tended to perform more
mixed emotions, and 9) understanding that emotions can be affected by poorly on the puppet interview. Similarly, using the puppet interview
moral judgment (Pons & Harris, 2000). Components 1 to 3 develop in and an emotion labeling task, Denham et al. (1994) found that observed
the external phase of emotion understanding. In this phase, from 3 to 5 supportive parental ERSBs (i.e., explanations about and responsiveness
years of age, children develop the ability to recognize and label basic to emotions) positively predicted external components of emotion un­
emotional expressions such as happiness, sadness, anger, and fear derstanding in 3 year-olds (N = 47). This finding was replicated in a
(Cutting & Dunn, 1999; Denham, 1986). The mental phase from 4 to 7 larger study by Denham and Kochanoff (2002) using the CCNES and the
years of age corresponds to components 4–6): children acquire an un­ puppet interview. In a sample of 134 predominantly Caucasian, middle-
derstanding that beliefs, either false or true, can influence people’s income families of 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds, they found that supportive
emotions, that a reminder can elicit emotions, and that emotions can be parental ERSBs were related to better performance on the external
regulated by behavioral and psychological strategies (Band & Weisz, components of emotion understanding in children. Perlman, Camras,
1988; Harris et al., 1989; Pons et al., 2004). For example, children start and Pelphrey (2008) replicated these results using a computerized
to understand that sadness might follow from being reminded of a dead version of the puppet interview. In a study of 42 preschool children aged
pet, and that thought can regulate emotions. The reflective phase de­ 4 to 5 years, they found evidence of a significant association between
velops from around 4 to 8 years of age and corresponds to components non-supportive reactions and the external components of emotion un­
7–9 (Kårstad, Wichstrom, Reinfjell, Belsky, & Berg-Nielsen, 2015; Pons derstanding. In sum, several studies have found that supportive re­
et al., 2004). Children begin to understand that emotions are sometimes actions are related to improved child emotion understanding and that
hidden, mixed, or influenced by moral judgment (Brown & Dunn, 1996; non-supportive reactions are related to poorer emotion understanding
Gardner, Harris, Ohmoto, & Hamazaki, 1988; Lake, Lane, & Harris, in 3- to 5-year-old children (Denham et al., 1994; Denham et al., 2015;
1995). For example, children develop an understanding that feelings of Eisenberg et al., 1998; Fabes et al., 2002; Thompson et al., 2020).
guilt or shame may arise from getting caught stealing a cookie. In sum, The present study differs from prior studies on emotion socialization
research supports the notion that children’s emotional understanding and emotion understanding in that it uses the TEC to measure devel­
develops in partly overlapping phases during early to middle childhood opmental phases of emotion understanding. The TEC is advantageous in

2
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

several ways. First, the TEC measures all nine components of emotion SES Brazilian preschoolers did not have difficulties recognizing negative
understanding and provides scale scores for external, mental, reflective, emotions but found it more difficult than high SES children to recognize
and total emotion understanding, whereas the puppet interview pri­ positive and neutral emotions. Kårstad et al. (2016) compared their
marily assesses external components of emotions (i.e., labeling of findings to high SES preschoolers in Norway and Italy, and low SES
emotions and understanding situational causes of emotions). Even preschoolers in Peru. High SES Brazilian preschoolers had almost the
though young children, such as those in the present sample’s age range, same level of emotion understanding as Norwegian and Italian pre­
are not expected to comprehend higher components of emotion under­ schoolers, and low SES Brazilian preschool children had a similar level
standing (i.e., the reflective and mental components), it is important to of emotion understanding as low SES children from Peru (Kårstad et al.,
also assess mental and reflective items as these components may develop 2016). The researchers concluded that the development of emotion
earlier in some cultures than in others (e.g., the understanding of hidden understanding was better explained by SES than culture, highlighting
emotions may develop as early as age 4 in Chinese children; Pons et al., the importance of including SES as a covariate in studies on emotion
2004; Tang et al., 2018). Secondly, the TEC is more standardized than understanding.
the puppet interview. The experimenter presents scenarios with draw­ Whether the Norwegian culture differs from other western cultures
ings on paper that are the same every time and reads aloud a stan­ in terms of emotion socialization practices is largely unknown (Bjørk
dardized verbal script with neutral intonation. Additional et al., 2020). One important factor that sets the Nordic countries apart
standardization is achieved when using the computerized version of the from other WEIRD (White, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Demo­
TEC (Haslinger, 2014). Third, the TEC does not require a verbal cratic) countries (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010), is the Nordic
response, which reduces the chances that results are confounded with Social-Democratic welfare system (the Nordic Model; Andersen et al.,
children’s language production. 2007). Through the welfare system, Nordic parents receive substantially
Only two previous studies have investigated the effects of supportive more economic and social support in their role as parents compared to
and non-supportive ERSBs on emotion understanding using the CCNES parents in the US, and families have universal access to child care
and the TEC (Bjørk, Havighurst, Pons, & Karevold, 2020; Chora, Mon­ (Zachrisson & Dearing, 2015). On the one hand, substantial economic
teiro, Ramos, & Amaral, 2019). In a sample of 75 Portuguese 3- to 6- support provided to families in the first year after birth enables parents
year-old children and their fathers, Chora et al. (2019) found a signifi­ to spend considerable time with their newborn babies, thereby poten­
cant negative association between fathers’ distress reactions and chil­ tially strengthening attachment bonds and supportive ERSBs in early
dren’s total TEC scores. In other words, when Portuguese fathers reacted childhood (Eisenberg et al., 1998; Godleski, Eiden, Shisler, & Livingston,
calmly in response to children’s negative emotions, children tended to 2020). On the other hand, universal access to childcare may encourage
have better emotion understanding. Relatedly, in a sample of 40, 5- to 6- parents to pursue professional careers in the early childhood years,
year-old Norwegian kindergarten children and their parents, Bjørk et al. reducing time spent with the family and therefore also reducing op­
(2020) found that parental expressive encouragement positively and portunities for socializing children into a world of emotions within the
significantly predicted children’s scores on the TEC. Parental distress family context (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007;
reactions did not predict emotion understanding in these Norwegian Thompson, 2013; Thompson et al., 2020). It is thus unclear how Nordic
families but when parents encouraged children to share difficult emo­ family-friendly policy schemes affect parental ERSBs and the develop­
tions, children tended to achieve higher scores on the TEC. ment of emotional understanding in children (Bjørk et al., 2020).
Whereas the puppet interview is typically used in Anglo-American A recent cross-cultural study of parental ERSBs in 12 cultures in 9
studies on emotion socialization (for a review, see Denham et al., countries (i.e., China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines,
2015), the TEC is often applied in cross-cultural and more culturally Sweden, Thailand, and the USA) found however that the mechanisms
diverse samples studying emotion understanding (Aznar & Tenenbaum, linking parent behavior to behavior problems in adolescents were
2013; Cavioni, Grazzani, Ornaghi, Pepe, & Pons, 2020; Chora et al., similar across cultures. These findings suggest that although parental
2019; Kårstad, 2016; Kårstad et al., 2016; Tang et al., 2018; Viana et al., ERSBs may vary across cultures (Cole & Tan, 2015; Raval & Walker,
2019; Vikan, Kårstad, & Dias, 2013). Thus, in the following section, 2019), the emotion socialization processes linking parental ERSBs to
cross-cultural influences on emotion understanding are discussed. adolescent behavior problems operate similarly across cultures. We
assumed that the same conclusion may generalize to children’s
Cultural influences on emotion understanding emotional understanding, though we did not explore discrete mecha­
nisms in the present investigation. As part of building a foundation for
Most cited research on emotion socialization comes from European such future works, the present study is the first to investigate the effects
American samples set in North America (Cole & Tan, 2015). Thus, there of the TIK intervention on parental ERSBs and children’s emotional
is a gap in emotion socialization literature where culturally diverse understanding in a Nordic cultural context. In the following sections, a
samples are seldom included (Raval & Walker, 2019). This is concerning broader introduction to the TIK intervention is given.
because the emotion understanding literature provides evidence of dif­
ferences in emotion understanding across cultures, suggesting that cul­ The TIK intervention
ture may affect children’s understanding of emotions. For example, a
cross-cultural study of Chinese preschool children (N = 65; age 4 to 6) TIK was developed to enhance emotion socialization skills in parents
applying the TEC, found that Chinese children developed an under­ (Havighurst & Harley, 2007), building on the seminal work of John
standing of emotions that followed a pattern similar to that found in Gottman and colleagues (Gottman & Declaire, 1997; Gottman, Katz, &
European American preschoolers (Tang et al., 2018). The Chinese pre­ Hooven, 1996, 1997). Gottman et al. (1996) found that an emotion-
schoolers, however, performed better at understanding hidden emotions coaching style of parenting was associated with better socio-emotional
compared to European children but were poorer at linking reminders to adjustment in children. Emotion coaching parents were 1) more aware
emotions. One possible explanation for this finding is that hiding emo­ of low-intensity emotions in themselves and their children, 2) viewed
tions are more common in the Chinese culture and children, therefore, their child’s negative emotions as an opportunity for intimacy and
develop this component earlier compared to Western European children. teaching, 3) validated their child’s emotions, 4) assisted the child
Results from another cross-cultural study applying the TEC suggested verbally in labeling emotions, and 5) helped their child to solve prob­
that socioeconomic status (SES) was confounded with culture. When lems or to set limits for the child’s behavior. Gottman et al. (1996) also
comparing TEC scores for high SES Brazilian preschoolers (n = 50) with identified a group of parents, whose style of parenting was associated
low SES Brazilian preschoolers (n = 50), Kårstad et al. (2016) found that with poorer child outcomes. These emotionally dismissive parents
SES affected the understanding of positive and neutral emotions. Low viewed negative emotions as potentially harmful and avoided talking

3
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

about emotions. criteria were language skills (i.e., parents needed to be able to speak
The TIK intervention, therefore, aims to promote parental emotion English to participate in the intervention). No participants were
coaching, reduce parental emotional dismissiveness, and to encourage excluded based on language proficiency. One mother of twins responded
emotion talk in families (Havighurst & Kehoe, 2021). The program is twice to the questionnaires concerning her two children and one twin
structured and manualized. Every session consists of a warm-up exer­ was randomly removed from the sample (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013).
cise, home activity review, agenda-setting, and activities, as well as Thirty-five parents (89.7%) lived together with the other biological/
completing new home activities. Warm-up exercises include reflections adoptive parent; 32 children (82.1%) lived full-time with siblings; 34
on the week, emotional awareness, and mindfulness exercises. The main parents (87.2%) had a college or university degree of at least 4 years; 4
group activities are psychoeducation (e.g., learning basic concepts of parents (10.3%) had a college or university degree of less than 4 years;
emotion socialization in a normal, non-pathologizing language), and 1 parent (2.6%) had only completed high school. The sample rep­
reflective exercises, and roleplaying practicing emotion coaching skills. resented a well-educated higher SES population. Twenty-four parents
At first, roleplaying is scripted. As the program advances, group facili­ (61.5%) reported that they were”very well off” financially, and 15
tators encourage parents to engage in unscripted roleplaying. Home parents (38.5%) reported that they were “well off” financially. No par­
activities include practicing an emotion talk exercise with their children, ents reported that they were not well off financially.
practicing the five steps of emotion coaching, using an emotion diary, This study used a 2 × 2 intervention design as part of a larger Nor­
and completing other emotion-related activities involving their child. wegian Tuning in to Kids (N-TIK) pilot evaluation (Bolstad et al., 2021;
TIK RCT studies including samples of typically developing children ClinicalTrials: NCT04851704). The research team recruited participants
and children with behavior problems have found that parental ERSBs from 17 kindergartens within a 10 km radius of the University of Oslo
and child outcomes improved as a result of participating in TIK groups with a population of circa 1 million people (SSB, 2018). In Norway,
(Duncombe et al., 2016; Havighurst et al., 2010; Havighurst et al., 2013; kindergartens typically provide daycare in combination with preschool
Havighurst et al., 2015; Havighurst, Wilson, Harley, & Prior, 2009; elements. The curriculum is play-based and promotes a holistic
Karevold et al., 2021; Wilson et al., 2012). TIK intervention studies have approach to learning (Hansen & Alvestad, 2018). Participants were not
also found evidence of a positive effect on external emotion under­ randomized, but consecutively assigned to the intervention group or a
standing (assessed using the puppet interview; Denham, 1986) in chil­ wait-list control condition based on their kindergarten site. Control and
dren at six to ten-month follow-up (Havighurst et al., 2010; Havighurst intervention parents attended separate kindergartens. The mean number
et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015). For example, an RCT study of children from each kindergarten was 2.05 (median = 1; range = 1–6).
investigating the effect of TIK in 216 typically developing 4- to 5-year- Kindergarten staff (i.e., teachers and head of the department) circulated
old children and their parents, found improved ERSBs in parents and information letters about the study to parents of children attending the
emotion understanding in children at the six-month follow-up final year of kindergarten. Parents who expressed interest in partici­
(Havighurst et al., 2010). This finding has been replicated in later pating in the study were contacted by phone by a researcher who
studies (e.g., Havighurst et al., 2015) and suggests that children’s described the study and then sent them a plain language statement and
emotional understanding improves as a result of enhanced parental consent form via email or post. Children were assessed for emotion
ERSBs. understanding at the University of Oslo by the research team. Parents
completed online questionnaires on ERSBs while waiting. The assess­
Aims of the current study ment took approximately one and a half hours. The assessment was
repeated at 6-month follow-up (T2) for all participants. Control partic­
The main aim of this study was to investigate the link between ipants were offered the intervention after the 6-month follow-up
parental ERSBs and the developmental phases of emotion understand­ assessment was completed. The research study was approved by the
ing. Although previous TIK investigations have found evidence of an Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics, region:
increase in children’s emotional understanding as a result of participa­ south-east of Norway (REC: 2015/2383) and all parents gave written
tion in TIK groups (e.g., Havighurst et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015), and informed consent for their own as well as for their child’s partici­
these studies did not directly test whether the change in parenting pation in the study.
predicted change in children’s emotional understanding nor did they
differentiate between developmental levels thereof. We aimed to TIK parenting program delivery
examine whether improved parental ERSBs (as a result of the TIK
intervention) led to positive changes in external, mental, reflective, and TIK was originally developed in Australia and the parenting program
total emotional understanding (as measured using the TEC). More has never previously been delivered in Norway, and no experienced
importantly, we hypothesized that enhanced ERSBs would positively Norwegian facilitators were thus available. The parenting groups were
predict improvements in children’s emotional understanding from therefore conducted in English and facilitated by one of the program
baseline to follow-up. We used an intervention design to investigate the developers (last author) and a co-facilitator (second author). Both fa­
following research questions: 1) Did TIK lead to increased supportive cilitators were certified TIK trainers. Materials and scripted role-plays
ERSBs and decreased non-supportive ERSBs in our sample? 2) Did were translated to Norwegian. Norwegian translations were offered by
children’s emotional understanding increase more in the intervention the co-facilitator when participants had any language questions or
condition compared to the control group? 3) Did enhanced parental showed some misunderstanding. Fidelity was assessed by completing an
ERSBs positively predict the development of emotional understanding activity checklist after every session to ensure all components of the
from baseline to follow-up? intervention were delivered. All activities were completed in the inter­
vention groups.
Method
Measures
Participants & procedure
The coping with children’s negative emotions scale
Participants in this pilot study were a sample of 40 children (M age =
5.91 years, SD = 0.32, boys n = 21) recruited in their final year of Parental ERSBs were assessed using a Norwegian translation of the
kindergarten, as well as one of their parents (M age = 42.05, SD = 4.55, CCNES (Fabes et al., 1990). The questionnaire was translated to Nor­
fathers n = 10). All families with preschoolers in their final year of wegian by the first and last authors with back-translation and proof­
kindergarten were eligible to participate in the study. The only exclusion reading by bilingual scholars (Bjørk et al., 2020). The CCNES is a 72 item

4
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

self-report scale assessing parental reactions to children’s negative effect sizes were detectable (Ellis, 2010). Missing scores were imputed
emotions. Parents are asked to rate on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (very using Expectation Maximation (Heck, Thomas, & Tabata, 2014).
unlikely) to 7 (very likely) how likely it is that they will react in a certain Normality was found to be acceptable for most variables, except for the
way to their child’s negative emotions in a variety of parenting sce­ CCNES Distress Reactions and Minimization Reactions scales and the
narios. The parent is presented with 12 scenarios with 6 possible re­ TEC scales. Bootstrapping is robust to violations of non-normality
actions in each scenario, that corresponded to one of the 6 subscales in (Erceg-Hurn & Mirosevich, 2008; Field, 2013), and was thus applied
the CCNES: Minimization Reactions, Distress Reactions, Punitive Re­ in all analyses. First, descriptive statistics and correlations were esti­
actions, Expressive Encouragement, Emotion-Focused Reactions, or mated. Secondly, multilevel analyses were conducted to test the effect of
Problem-Focused Reactions. One example of a scenario is: “If my child time (i.e., maturation) and condition (intervention or control) by time
loses some prized possession and reacts with tears, I would…” Possible on parental ERSBs and child emotion understanding (Heck et al., 2014).
responses on this item are: “tell my child that he/she is over-reacting” Third, parental ERSBs that changed from baseline to follow-up were
(Minimization Reaction); “get upset with him/her for being so careless added as predictors in the multilevel models.
and then crying about it” (Distress Reaction); “tell him/her that’s what
happens when you’re not careful” (Punitive Reaction); “tell him/her it’s Results
OK to cry when you feel unhappy” (Expressive Encouragement);
“distract my child by talking about happy things” (Emotion-Focused Preliminary analyses
Reaction); “help my child think of places he/she hasn’t looked yet”
(Problem-Focused Reaction). The average Cronbach’s alpha for the Descriptive statistics and correlations are presented in Table 1. The
CCNES scales was 0.77, ranging from 0.65 to 0.92, indicating moderate parental non-supportive subscales (i.e., minimizing, punitive, and
to good reliability. distress reactions) were positively correlated, and there was a significant
positive correlation between the baseline (T1) and follow-up (T2) as­
The test of emotion comprehension sessments. Parental emotion and problem-focused reactions (i.e., both
supportive ERSBs) were correlated at baseline and follow-up. At base­
The Norwegian version of the TEC is a direct assessment measure line, parental expressive encouragement was positively correlated with
used to capture child emotion understanding (Kårstad et al., 2015; Pons emotion understanding (i.e., TEC emotion and TEC external), but not at
& Harris, 2000). The TEC measures nine components of emotion un­ follow-up. The TEC subscales were significantly correlated with each
derstanding: 1) emotion recognition, 2) external cause, 3) desire, 4) other at baseline assessment, and from baseline to follow-up, with the
belief, 5) reminder, 6) regulation, 7) hidden, 8) mixed, and 9) morally exception of the TEC reflective scale where there was no correlation
based emotions. The TEC external scale is comprised of components 1–3, between baseline and follow-up assessments. Higher parental education
the TEC mental scale includes components 4–6, and the TEC reflective was associated with higher scores on TEC external at follow-up.
scale is comprised of components 7–9. The TEC emotion scale is a total
score including all nine components. The experimenter presented Main analyses
participating children with a picture book, read the corresponding
stories aloud, and asked children to indicate the emotion of the pro­ Multilevel analyses were conducted to investigate improvements in
tagonist in each story by pointing to one of the four faces presented ERSBs and emotion understanding from baseline to follow-up (Heck
under each cartoon scenario. The faces exhibited two pleasant emotions et al., 2014). Estimates of fixed effects as a function of time and time by
(happy and all right) and two unpleasant emotions (either scared, angry, condition are presented in Table 2. Improved parental ERSBs were
or sad). The experimenter reported the emotion the child identified added as predictors in the multilevel models to investigate whether
through pointing. The TEC was thus very clear to code and left little changes in parental ERSBs predicted the change in emotion under­
room for ambiguity. Success on one component yielded 1 point. As each standing. All models included the following predictors: kindergarten,
TEC scale (i.e., external, mental, and reflective) measured three com­ SES, time, condition, and time*condition (interaction effect). Effect sizes
ponents, children achieve a minimum score of 0 and a maximum score of were calculated using the independent-groups pretest-posttest design
3 on each TEC scale, and a maximum score of 9 on the TEC emotion (IGPP) formula for multilevel analyses (Feingold, 2009). Goodness of fit
(total) scale. For example, a score of 3 on the TEC external scale in­ was evaluated by comparing the chi-square change (− 2LL; Log Likeli­
dicates that the child has passed tests of emotion recognition (compo­ hood) and df change to the critical values of the chi-square distribution
nent 1), causes of emotions (component 2) and desire-based emotions (Field, 2013).
(component 3). The Cronbach’s α coefficient for the TEC emotion (total) The analysis followed the steps in multilevel analysis suggested by
scale was estimated as 0.68 at baseline and 0.53 at follow-up. Acceptable Heck et al. (2014). Steps one to five were conducted for every outcome
levels of internal consistency have been found in other studies on larger variable. As in hierarchical regression, predictors were added incre­
samples (α = 0.72; n = 406; Franco et al., 2017; α = 0.79; n = 1000; mentally to the multi-level models, and the chi-square change indicated
Albanese & Molina, 2008). Lower reliability estimates are typically re­ whether adding covariates to the model significantly improved model fit
ported for tests like the TEC (i.e., α = 0.32; n = 201; Garrett-Peters, (Field, 2013).
Castro, & Halberstadt, 2017). The factor structure of the TEC is well- To investigate whether TIK led to improved ERSBs (i.e., research
established in other studies and the TEC shows good test-retest reli­ question 1), we first constructed a model including only the outcome
ability (Cavioni et al., 2020; Ornaghi, Grazzani, Cherubin, Conte, & variable (i.e., a null model) for each outcome measure (Heck et al.,
Piralli, 2015; Pons et al., 2004). 2014). Second, we constructed models including random intercepts for
kindergarten sites (i.e., random intercept model). None of the random
Data analysis intercept models converged and kindergarten site was excluded from
further analyses. Third, we added SES (i.e., parental education and self-
Data analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS version 27. Data were reported family economy) as fixed effects. Fourth, we added time as a
screened prior to analysis according to the guidelines of Tabachnick and dummy coded covariate (0 = baseline, 1 = follow-up) in the multilevel
Fidell (2013). G*Power 3.1.9.7 was used to estimate sensitivity for models. Time significantly increased model fit for emotion-focused re­
detection of level 1 effects in a repeated measures within-between mixed sponses, χ 2(1) = 12.28, p < .01. Fifth, a dummy coded condition variable
designs, given a sample of 39 participants and a desired power of 0.80 (0 = control, 1 = intervention) was added to the models including the
(Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007; Howell, 2013). The power interaction effects (time*condition). This significantly improved model
analysis estimated an f value of 0.23, suggesting that medium to large fit in the parental expressive encouragement model, χ 2(2) = 13.63, p <

5
R.F. Bjørk et al.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics and correlations.
Measure Parental Emotion Socialization Behaviors Child Emotion Understanding SES

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21

1. T1 Minimizing Reactions 2.10 .74


2. T1 Punitive Reactions 1.83 .56 .57**
3. T1 Distress Reactions 2.61 .74 .46** .53**
4. T1 Expressive
Encouragement 5.30 .75 − .27 − .28 − .22
5. T1 Emotion Focused
Reactions 5.79 .65 .19 .15 .29 − .13
6. T1 Problem Focused
Reactions 6.03 .43 − .01 − .06 .00 .28 .41**
7. T2 Minimizing Reactions 1.91 .58 .76** .66** .46** − .27 .16 − .11
8. T2 Punitive Reactions 1.65 .47 .51** .88** .53** − .31 .15 − .05 .71**
9. T2 Distress Reactions 2.47 .65 .37* .31 .72** − .22 .05 − .17 .53** .41**
6

1. T2 Expressive
Encouragement 5.56 .90 .01 − .13 .03 .22 − .09 .18 − .18 − .08 − .01
11. T2 Emotion Focused
Reactions 5.25 .82 .16 .29 .20 − .22 .64** .31 .22 .36 − .04 − .08
12. T2 Problem Focused
Reactions 5.87 .63 − .04 .02 .03 .05 .38* .58** .02 .11 − .13 .22 .64**
13. T1 TEC External 2.59 .64 − .03 − .19 − .26 .38* − .30 .03 − .07 − .19 − .07 .09 − .13 .13
14. T1 TEC Mental 1.31 .98 − .03 .12 .03 .24 − .09 .01 .11 .23 .03 − .02 .10 .09 .25
15. T1 TEC Reflective 1.56 .91 .02 − .29 − .06 .18 − .14 − .20 − .06 − .24 − .02 − .25 − .18 − .27 .32* .18
16. T1 TEC Emotion 5.46 1.79 − .02 − .15 − .11 .35* − .23 − .08 .01 − .06 − .02 − .10 − .08 − .04 .66** .73** .72**
17. T2 TEC External 2.92 .27 .12 − .01 − .13 .02 − .14 .05 .10 .10 .20 − .02 .05 .10 .43** .30 .31 .47**

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368


18. T2 TEC Mental 1.82 .72 − .02 .07 − .15 .28 − .26 − .16 − .01 .04 .03 .08 .01 − .13 .36* .42* .17 .44** .47**
19. T2 TEC Reflective 2.08 .90 − .40* − .21 − .23 .10 − .19 − .13 − .21 − .09 − .19 − .06 − .26 − .15 .06 .45** .36* .45** .15 .15
2. T2 TEC Emotion 6.84 1.35 − .24 − .10 − .27 .20 − .35 − .18 − .10 − .02 − .03 .02 − .17 − .15 .30 .58** .36* .60** .55** .73** .75**
21. Parental Education 4.85 .43 .12 .13 .03 − .15 .31 .30 .12 .15 .13 − .16 .22 .10 .05 .30 − .11 .13 .34* .09 .11 .20
22. Financial status 4.59 .55 − .06 − .04 − .01 − .16 − .09 − .10 .12 .01 .18 − .16 − .10 − .18 .03 − .05 − .16 − .10 − .05 .00 .09 .06 .17

T1 = baseline assessment. T2 = follow-up assessment. n = 39. *p < .05. **p < .01.
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

Table 2
Multi-level Mixed Effects Modeling: Intervention Outcomes.
Baseline Follow-up Time (both conditions) Interaction (Time X Condition)

Measures Condition Mean SD Mean SD b Bias SE p d b Bias SE p d


Control 2.08 .81 2.00 .69 − .08 .01 .24 .744 .27 − .21 − .02 .31 .506 .31
Minimizing reactions
Intervention 2.11 .68 1.84 .48
Control 1.92 .69 1.68 .56 − .23 − .01 .19 .249 .33 .09 .02 .23 .738 .13
Punitive reactions
Intervention 1.76 .45 1.63 .39
Control 2.46 .81 2.42 .72 − .04 − .01 .26 .885 .21 − .19 .00 .32 .557 .34
Distress reactions
Intervention 2.75 .65 2.53 .60
Control 5.43 .81 5.14 1.03 − .31 .02 .29 .300 .31 1.06** − .03 .36 .003 1.01
Expressive encouragement
Intervention 5.25 .73 6.05 .55
Control 5.83 .64 5.47 .69 − .37* .00 .18 .042 .74 − .41 .01 .32 .206 .32
Emotion-focused reactions
Intervention 5.77 .69 5.04 .90
Control 6.07 .42 5.97 .60 − .10 .00 .17 .550 .33 − .17 .00 .24 .452 .13
Problem-focused reactions
Intervention 6.15 .45 5.90 .70
Control 2.58 .61 2.94 .23 .36* .00 .08 .026 .73 − .07 .00 .22 .775 .01
External emotion understanding
Intervention 2.60 .68 2.90 .31
Control 1.53 .90 1.89 .66 .35 − .02 .27 .189 .63 .27 .02 .38 .491 1.39
Mental emotion understanding
Intervention 1.10 1.02 1.75 .79
Control 1.63 .90 2.00 .94 .34 .01 .31 .284 .60 .28 − .01 .43 .527 .33
Reflective emotion understanding
Intervention 1.50 .96 2.15 .88
Control 5.74 1.41 6.89 1.37 1.12* .00 .46 .019 .90 .42 .03 .70 .556 1.14
Total emotion understanding
Intervention 5.20 2.09 6.80 1.36

Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions and child emotion understanding pre-and post-intervention. SES was included as a covariate.
*
p < .05.

.01, but not in any other models. The results are presented in Table 2.
Table 3
Parental expressive encouragement was the only ERSB that changed
Multi-level Mixed Effects Modeling: Predicting emotion understanding.
significantly as a function of time by condition (Cohen’s d = 1.01),
corresponding to a large effect size (Ellis, 2010). Parental emotion- Model Bootstrapped estimates of fixed effects
focused reactions changed significantly in both the intervention and b Bias SE p
control groups from baseline to follow-up (Cohen’s d = 0.74), showing a TEC external modela
medium to large effect size (Ellis, 2010). Parental education .08 .01 .08 .275
To investigate whether children’s emotional understanding Financial status − .03 .00 .06 .649
increased more in the intervention condition compared to controls (i.e., Time .36* .01 .16 .026
Condition .05 .01 .19 .809
research question 2), we repeated the multilevel analysis using
Time*Condition − .31 .00 .21 .168
emotional understanding as the outcome measure. Time significantly Expressive encouragement .14* .00 .05 .013
increased model fit for all models, TEC external χ 2(1) = 8.29, p < .01, Emotion-focused reactions − .11* .00 .05 .052
TEC mental χ 2(1) = 6.25, p < .05, TEC reflective χ 2(1) = 5.44, p <. 05, TEC mental modelb
and TEC emotion χ 2(1) = 12.88, p < .01. External and total emotion Parental education .21** .00 .10 .012
understanding also increased significantly in both groups from baseline Family economy − .09 .00 .10 .369
to six-months follow-up with medium to large effect sizes. Children’s Time .37 − .04 .28 .197
Condition − .38 − .02 .28 .203
emotional understanding increased more in the intervention condition
Time*Condition − .04 .03 .38 .924
but did not significantly change as a function of time by condition. The Expressive encouragement .19* .00 .10 .054
mean baseline scores on external emotion understanding (2.94 in the Emotion-focused reactions − .11 − .01 .10 .246
control group and 2.90 in the intervention group) came close to the TEC reflective modelc
maximum score on TEC external (3.00) post-intervention, indicating a Parental education .02 .00 .12 .844
possible ceiling effect. Family economy − .13 .00 .12 .285
Finally, to investigate whether the change in parental ERSBs pre­ Time .24 .01 .32 .450
Condition − .12 .01 .29 .676
dicted change in emotion understanding (i.e., research question 3),
Time*Condition .16 .01 .46 .731
parental ERSBs that changed from baseline to follow-up (i.e., expressive Expressive encouragement .00 .00 .12 .964
encouragement and emotion-focused reactions) were added as cova­ Emotion-focused reactions − .21* .00 .10 .032
riates in the multilevel TEC models. Results are presented in Table 3.
TEC emotion modeld
Model fit significantly improved when parental expressive encourage­ Parental education .33 .01 .23 .115
ment and emotion-focused reactions were added in the sixth step of the Financial status − .23 .00 .20 .252
TEC external, χ 2(2) = 8.95, p < .05, and TEC emotion models, χ 2(2) = Time 1.04* .02 .46 .036
Condition − .44 − .02 .52 .422
9.05, p < .05. Model fit did not significantly improve in the mental,
Time*Condition − .28 .00 .69 .696
χ 2(2) = 4.62, and reflective models, χ 2(2) = 3.50. Expressive encouragement .36* .01 .18 .051
Time remained a significant positive predictor of external and total Emotion-focused reactions − .44** .00 .15 .003
emotion understanding when enhanced parental ERSBs were added to a
χ 2(2) = 8.95, p < .05.
the multilevel models. Expressive encouragement was a significant b
χ2(2) = 4.62, p > .05.
positive predictor of children’s development of emotional understand­ c
χ2(2) = 3.50, p > .05.
ing, and the emotion-focused responses scale was a significant negative d
χ 2(2) = 9.05, p < .05. *p < .05 **p < .01.
predictor of the development of children’s external and total emotional
understanding.

7
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

Discussion cooperation, dialogue, consensus and humility over masculine values


such as competitiveness, gloating over success and a winner-takes-it-all
The present study tested the assumption from emotion socialization mentality (Hofstede, 2011). Although cooperation, dialogue and
theory that parental ERSBs aid in the development of emotion under­ consensus are positive values, on the dark side of femininity, Nordic
standing in children (Eisenberg et al., 1998). Previous intervention culture discourages people from taking pride in success, bragging about
research has suggested that emotion-focused parenting programs, such wealth, or being the center of attention. This anti-masculine tendency is
as TIK, lead to increased emotional understanding in children (e.g., referred to as the “Janteloven” (Bromgard et al., 2014).
Havighurst et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015; Havighurst & Kehoe, The law of “Jante” (“Janteloven”) is a Nordic literary construct
2021; Wilson et al., 2012), but no previous studies have directly tested consisting of ten commandments, stating in sum that “you shall not
whether changes in parental ERSBs positively predicted improvements believe that you are better than other people” (Bromgard et al., 2014;
in the development of children’s emotion understanding. In the present Cappelen & Dahlberg, 2018; Trotter, 2015). In Nordic societies, overt
study, we tested this predictive link using an intervention design and emotional displays are problematic because the actor becomes the
advanced statistical methods. The main aim was to examine whether center of attention, threatening the equalitarian values of the “Jantelo­
improved parental ERSBs (as a result of the TIK intervention) positively ven”. Strong emotional expressions (i.e., crying or screaming) implies
predicted changes in external, mental, reflective, and total emotional that the actor is putting his or her feelings above those of others.
understanding in a sample of 4- to 5-year-old children (as measured Therefore, when a Nordic child trips and falls, parents hastefully and
using the TEC). We found that parental ERSBs improved as a result of the chairfully exclaim “Up again!” as if nothing just happened (Wallace &
intervention and that emotional understanding improved over time in Kjøs, 2012). The parental response may be warm and sensitive, solving
both intervention and control children. Emotional understanding the child’s immediate problems in a culturally appropriate manner (i.e.,
improved more in the intervention group than in the control group, but getting the child up on her feet again without attracting attention), but
this difference was not statistically significant at six months follow-up. does not take advantage of the opportunity to learn new emotion
In short, emotion understanding improved in both groups as a result regulation skills from the emotional experience.
of development over time, and parents’ emotion-focused reactions Although Norwegian parents may be culturally motivated to
decreased in both groups. Parental expressive encouragement increased discourage expressions of emotions (Bourrelle, 2017), they may become
as a result of the TIK intervention. More importantly, when parents more inclined to do so when they learn that children down-regulate
became more encouraging towards children, there was an increase in more rapidly using emotion coaching rather than invalidating strate­
children’s emotional understanding. In other words, when parents gies. Parents participating in the present study may have embraced the
encouraged their children to talk about negative emotions (e.g., emotion coaching strategy because they were motivated to raise well-
encouraging children to express sadness or fear when falling off a bike), regulated, socially and culturally competent children while staying
children’s showed stronger improvements in external and total within the “Jante” mentality and avoiding mutual discomfort associated
emotional understanding during the 6-month-period from baseline to with strong emotional expressions. This can explain the increase in
follow-up compared to children whose parents were not as encouraging. expressive encouragement. The primary motivation for parents partici­
Unexpectedly, parental emotion-focused reactions were negatively pating in the study may not primarily have been to encourage children
related to improvements in children’s emotional understanding. In these to express emotions, but to help children down-regulate emotions. In
families, when parents responded to children’s negative emotions with short, TIK might not only have taught parents good parenting skills but
distraction and comfort (e.g., helping the child to think happy thoughts may also have helped parents learn how to raise emotionally healthy
when falling off a bike), children’s emotional understanding did not children within the specific cultural context. The cultural context does,
improve as much. Our findings are discussed in detail in the following however, not explain why we did not find a similar effect on non-
sections. supportive strategies.
Previous TIK studies have found a reduction in non-supportive ERSBs
Did TIK Lead to Increased Supportive ERSBs and Decreased Non-supportive as a result of the intervention (e.g., Havighurst et al., 2015). We,
ERSBs? therefore, also expected to find a reduction in non-supportive ERSBs (i.
The TIK parenting intervention is based on Gottman et al.’s (1996) e., minimizing, punitive, and distress reactions) as a result of the inter­
original research, and the main aim is to increase supportive (i.e., vention. The effect sizes were small, however, and we did not have
emotion coaching) and reduce non-supportive (i.e., dismissive) styles of sufficient statistical power to detect small effects. Previous studies have
parenting (Havighurst & Harley, 2007). Numerous TIK studies over the found an effect of the TIK program on emotion understanding (e.g.,
last 20 years have found that the TIK program partly or completely Havighurst et al., 2015), but, as further discussed in the next section, we
achieves these goals (Havighurst et al., 2009; Havighurst et al., 2010; did not find evidence of such a direct effect of the intervention on
Havighurst et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015; Wilson et al., 2012). In children’s understanding of emotions.
TIK groups, parents learn to encourage the expression of emotions in
children (e.g., “all emotions are acceptable, although not all behaviors”), Did children’s emotional understanding increase more in the intervention
and previous TIK studies have found an effect on expressive encour­ group?
agement (e.g., Wilson et al., 2012). The current study replicated this Children’s emotional understanding increased from baseline to
finding, showing a large effect size. The large positive effect on parental follow-up regardless of whether their parents had participated in the TIK
expressive encouragement suggests a willingness for Norwegian parents intervention condition or in the control condition. There was a ceiling
to talk about negative emotions when encouraged and helped to do so. effect on children’s external emotional understanding, leaving little
How do we interpret this finding? It goes against the cultural ste­ room for additional improvements in the intervention group. Children’s
reotype suggesting that Norwegians usually do not express emotions mental and reflective emotional understanding increased more in the
(Bourrelle, 2017). An explanation can be found in the “Janteloven” and intervention group compared to controls, but this difference was not
the feminine values typical of Nordic societies (Bromgard, Trafimow, & significant.
Linn, 2014; Cappelen & Dahlberg, 2018; Hofstede, 2011; Hofstede- According to emotion socialization theory (Eisenberg et al., 1998),
Insigths, 2021; Trotter, 2015). supportive parental ERSBs improve children’s emotion understanding,
Norway is ranked as the second most feminine society (only sur­ regulation, and expression (i.e., emotional competence; Denham, 2015).
passed by Sweden) on Hofstede Insights’ records of more than 50 soci­ We did not find any statistically significant direct effects of the TIK
eties assessed using Hofstede’s (2011) 6-D model of cultural dimensions intervention on children’s emotional understanding even though many
(Hofstede-Insigths, 2021). Feminine societies typically value previous studies have found such an effect (Havighurst et al., 2010;

8
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

Havighurst et al., 2013; Havighurst et al., 2015). encourage their children to talk about emotions, children’s emotional
The most salient and perhaps also most important explanation for the vocabulary and their understanding of emotions improve.
lack of effects on emotional understanding may be the instrument used The unexpected, negative effect of parental emotion-focused re­
to assess it (i.e., the TEC). At baseline most children participating in the actions was also interesting, and warrants replication. When parents in
study had already achieved a maximum score of 3 on the TEC external our sample reacted with comfort and distraction in the face of negative
scale, leaving no or little room for gains attributable to the intervention emotions, children’s emotional understanding improved significantly
(in addition to maturation effects). The ceiling effect in 4- to 5-year-olds less from baseline to follow-up. How can this be? An explanation for this
in the current study is interesting because it suggests that the Denham finding is that the emotion-focused reactions CCNES scale, rather than
(1996) puppet interview (which is widely used in emotion socialization assessing emotion coaching responses (as suggested by its name), mea­
literature) may be a better fit in detecting changes as a result of emotion- sures an emotionally dismissive style of parenting (Bjørk et al., 2020).
focused parenting interventions for children in this age group. It also According to Gottman et al. (1996), dismissive parents are warm and
explains why previous TIK studies found a direct effect on children’s supportive in that they want their children to feel better (e.g., encour­
emotional understanding (Havighurst et al., 2010; Havighurst et al., aging children to think happy thoughts when they are sad), but
2013; Havighurst et al., 2015), while we did not. dismissive in that they fail to acknowledge the importance of the un­
An additional or alternative explanation for the lack of direct effects derlying negative emotions (e.g., silver lining their negative emotional
on children’s emotional understanding is the duration of time from experience). Gottman et al. (1996) found that warm and supportive, but
baseline to follow-up assessments. Salmon et al. (2014) suggested that also dismissive parents tended to have children with poorer emotional
emotion-focused interventions take longer to exert an effect on child competence. Thus, when parents did not label and validate emotions (e.
outcomes compared to behaviorally focused interventions. They found, g., did not verbally and emotionally address the child’s sadness and fear
however, an effect of their emotion-cause talk intervention at two when he/she fell of a bike, but instead tried to help the child forget about
months follow-up, and other TIK studies have found significant effects of the incident), children tended to have a poor understanding of emotions.
the intervention on external components of emotion understanding at Even though children can’t articulate their emotions adequately at a
six to ten-month follow-up (Havighurst et al., 2010; Havighurst et al., young age, the experience of being dismissed can still generate consid­
2013; Havighurst et al., 2015). A six-month period from baseline to erable additional negative affect that would require regulation. Over
follow-up should, therefore, be long enough for the intervention to exert time, this chain of events could make it harder for children to learn to
an effect on children’s emotional understanding, leaving the lack of regulate emotions, thus contributing to the lower improvements in
direct effect in the present study best explained by the ceiling effect in emotional competence over time. In our data, it appears that a dismis­
the external components. sive style of responding to negative emotions leads to fewer improve­
Although we did not find a direct effect of the intervention on chil­ ments in children’s emotional understanding. Given the dismissive
dren’s emotional understanding, we did find an indirect effect of the nature of the emotion-focused reactions CCNES items (Bjørk et al.,
intervention via one parental ERSB. Expressive encouragement (which 2020), our findings thus support Gottman et al.’s (1996) research find­
increased as a result of the intervention) predicted improvements in ings, and are in line with Morris et al.’s (2007) tripartite model and Katz,
children’s emotional understanding. These findings are discussed in the Maliken, and Stettler’s (2012) Parental Meta-Emotion Philosophy
next section. (PMEP) theoretical framework suggesting a direct link between parental
emotion coaching and children’s emotional competence. In sum, our
Did enhanced parental ERSBs positively predict improvements in emotional findings thus support emotion socialization theory, while also high­
understanding? lighting an avenue for empirical confirmation concerning the precise
To test the assumption from emotion socialization theory (Eisenberg, mechanisms of effect, specifically in terms of different effects of
1998) that parental ERSBs aid the development of emotional under­ dismissive/supportive and emotion coaching/supportive ERSBs on child
standing in children, we included improved supportive ERSBs as pre­ outcomes.
dictors in the statistical models. This was done by adding parental ERSBs
that changed from baseline to follow-up (i.e., expressive encouragement Strengths and limitations
and emotion-focused reactions) as predictors in the multilevel TEC As is true in all investigations, this study possessed crucial strengths
models. As expected, expressive encouragement positively and signifi­ and limitations. Regarding strengths, we used a well-established, evi­
cantly predicted external and total emotion understanding, controlling dence-based emotion-focused parenting program (TIK) to manipulate
for the effect of time (i.e., maturation from baseline to follow-up), parental ERSBs in an experimental design. Second, the study was further
condition and SES. We also expected, based on emotion socialization strengthened by the use of a comprehensive, standardized, objective
theory, that supportive ERSBs would improve emotion understanding in measure to assess all nine components of emotion understanding in
children. This was demonstrated for expressive encouragement in both children (The TEC; Pons & Harris, 2000). The TEC was easy to admin­
models. Unexpectedly, we found that parental emotion-focused re­ ister and provided estimates of mental and reflective phases of emotion
sponses negatively predicted children’s total score on emotion under­ understanding, in addition to external components. Lastly, advanced
standing, which seems counterintuitive. We discuss each of these novel statistical methods were used to test the predictive link between
findings in turn below. enhanced parental ERSBs and improved emotional understanding in
In the context of this experimental investigation, this study provided children.
novel support for the assumption that when parents encourage their This pilot study had also had a number of limitations. The small
children to share their negative emotional experiences, this shift in sample size resulted in limited power and we were only able to detect
parental attitudes leads to the development of better emotional under­ moderate to large effect size. Further, the sample was homogenous,
standing in children. We assumed from previous research that this consisting of mainly highly educated middle-class families who were
would be the case (Havighurst et al., 2010; Havighurst et al., 2013; recruited voluntarily from a large geographical area, implying a strong
Havighurst et al., 2015), but to date, scholars in emotion socialization potential for selection bias. Additionally, while the use of the TEC was an
have not tested this assumption through modeling change in ERSBs. In advantage relative to the puppet interview used in prior studies, in the
practical and clinical terms, this finding suggests that counselors and TIK context of this study, its psychometrics were somewhat disappointing. In
group facilitators can teach parents to encourage their children to talk particular, the reliability estimates on the TEC at follow-up were
about negative emotional experiences, which again leads to their better somewhat low, reducing the chances of detecting an effect of the
emotional understanding. As described by emotion socialization theory intervention. Ceiling effects on the external scale left limited room for
(Eisenberg et al., 1998), when parents talk about emotions and change. The TEC external scale could have profited from including more

9
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

items, such as emotion recognition of complex emotions such as shame Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. (2013). Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children’s
understanding of emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(670). https://doi.org/10.3389/
and surprise. The mental and reflective scales would also have profited
fpsyg.2013.00670
from more items (e.g., additional vignettes portraying belief-based or Band, E. B., & Weisz, J. R. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: Children’s
mixed emotions or emotion regulation). A second set of limitations perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Developmental Psychology, 24(2),
concerns potential language barriers: while we sought to prevent this 247–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.2.247
Bardack, S. R., & Widen, S. C. (2019). Emotion understanding and regulation:
issue through deliberate study inclusion criteria regarding English lan­ Implications for positive school adjustment. In V. LoBue, K. Pérez-Edgar, & K. A. Buss
guage fluency, this still may have impeded the delivery of TIK in this (Eds.), Handbook of emotional development (pp. 479–491). Switzerland, AG: Springer
study, in that TIK materials were translated into Norwegian, but group Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-17332-6_19.
Bender, P. K., Pons, F., Harris, P. L., Esbjørn, B. H., & Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L. (2015).
sessions were conducted in English. This was necessary as this was the Emotion understanding in clinically anxious children: A preliminary investigation.
first time TIK groups were conducted in Norway, and experienced Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1916). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01916
Norwegian TIK facilitators were not available. Thus, future studies on Bjørk, R. F., Havighurst, S. S., Pons, F., & Karevold, E. B. (2020). Pathways to behavior
problems in Norwegian kindergarten children: The role of parent emotion
TIK in Norway should use trained Norwegian-speaking group facilita­ socialization and child emotion understanding. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology.
tors. There was also a misfit between the emotion-focused reactions https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12652 (Advance online publication).
CCNES scale and the emotion-coaching concept in the present study and Bourrelle, J. S. (2017). The social guidebook to Norway: An illustrated introduction. Mondå.
Bromgard, G., Trafimow, D., & Linn, C. (2014). Janteloven and the expression of pride in
more integrative conceptualizations of parental support and emotion Norway and the United States. The Journal of Social Psychology, 154(5), 375–378.
coaching responses can be included in future research (e.g., Jespersen, https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2014.914884
Hardy, & Morris, 2021). Lastly, while reliance on self-report measures of Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion understanding from three to six
years. Child Development, 67(3), 789–802. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
parental ERSBs is a common limitation in studies on emotion socializ­
8624.1996.tb01764.x
ation, for future studies, researchers should strive to augment these re­ Cappelen, C., & Dahlberg, S. (2018). The law of Jante and generalized trust. Acta
ports with validated observational assessment methodologies. Sociologica, 61(4), 419–440. https://doi.org/10.1177/0001699317717319
Cassidy, K. W., Werner, R. S., Rourke, M., Zubernis, L. S., & Balaraman, G. (2003). The
relationship between psychological understanding and positive social behaviors.
Conclusions Social Development, 12(2), 198–221. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00229
Cavioni, V., Grazzani, I., Ornaghi, V., Pepe, A., & Pons, F. (2020). Assessing the factor
Our findings provide support for emotion socialization theory and structure and measurement invariance of the test of emotion comprehension (TEC):
A large cross-sectional study with children aged 3-10 years. Journal of Cognition and
extend the existing empirical evidence in the field by showing that Development, 21(3), 406–424. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2020.1741365
parental expressive encouragement positively, and emotion-focused Denham, S. A., & Burton, R. (1996). A social-emotional intervention for at-risk 4-year-
responses negatively predicted the development of emotion under­ olds. Journal of School Psychology, 34(3), 225–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-
4405%2896%2900013-1
standing in children. When parents encouraged their children to share Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. (2015). The socialization of emotional
difficult emotional experiences, children tended to have better competence. In J. E. Grusec, & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory
emotional understanding. When parents responded with comfort and and research (2nd ed., pp. 590–613). The Guilford Press.
Chora, M., Monteiro, L., Ramos, M., & Amaral, R. (2019). Um olhar sobre o papel do pai
distraction, however, children’s emotional understanding did not in­ na compreensão emocional das crianças: Os estilos parentais e práticas de
crease as much. Thus, this research clearly demonstrates a link between socialização das emoções negativas [A look at the father’s role in children’s
parental expressive encouragement and the development of emotion emotional understanding: Parenting styles and socialization practices of negative
emotions]. Psicologia, 33(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.17575/rpsicol.v33i1.1372
understanding in children.
Cole, P. M., & Tan, P. Z. (2015). Emotion socialization from a cultural perspective. In
J. E. Grusec, & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research
Author contributions (2nd ed., pp. 499–519). The Guilford Press.
Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and
family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70
Rune Flaaten Bjørk. Main and corresponding author, participation in (4), 853–865. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00061
the study implementation and data collection, responsible for the study De Stasio, S., Fiorilli, C., & Di Chiacchio, C. (2014). Effects of verbal ability and fluid
data analyses and interpretations, and of the writing of the manuscript. intelligence on children’s emotion understanding. International Journal of Psychology,
49(5), 409–414. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12032
Evalill Bølstad Karevold. Coauthor. PI of the overall project. Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers:
Responsible for the overall project conception, implementation and data Contextual validation. Child Development, 57(1), 194–201. https://doi.org/10.2307/
collection, participation in the study data collection, analyses and 1130651
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. Guilford Press.
interpretation and in the writing of the manuscript. Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Wyatt, T. (2015). The socialization of emotional
Francisco Pons. Coauthor, participation in the study data interpre­ competence. In J. E. Grusec, & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory
tation and in the writing of the manuscript. and research (2nd ed., pp. 590–613). The Guilford Press.
Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., &
Sophie Seychelle Havighurst. Senior author. Participation in the Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social
study planning, implementation and data collection, analyses and competence? Child Development, 74(1), 238–256. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-
interpretation, and on the writing of the manuscript. 8624.00533
Denham, S. A., & Burton, R. (1996). A social-emotional intervention for at-risk 4-year-
olds. Journal of School Psychology, 34(3), 225–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-
Acknowledgements 4405%2896%2900013-1
Denham, S. A., & Kochanoff, A. T. (2002). Parental contributions to preschoolers’
The present study was funded by the Department of Psychology, understanding of emotion. Marriage & Family Review, 34(3–4), 311–343. https://doi.
org/10.1300/J002v34n03_06
University of Oslo. Denham, S. A., Zoller, D., & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers’
emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 928–936. https://doi.org/
References 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.928
Duncombe, M. E., Havighurst, S. S., Kehoe, C. E., Holland, K. A., Frankling, E. J., &
Stargatt, R. (2016). Comparing an emotion- and a behavior-focused parenting
Albanese, O., De Stasio, S., Di Chiacchio, C., Fiorilli, C., & Pons, F. (2010). Emotion program as part of a multsystemic intervention for child conduct problems. Journal
comprehension: The impact of nonverbal intelligence. The Journal of Genetic of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 45(3), 320–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Psychology, 171(2), 101–115. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221320903548084 15374416.2014.963855
Albanese, O., & Molina, P. (2008). The development of emotion comprehension and its Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion.
assessment. The Italian standardization of the test of emotion comprehension (TEC). Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241–273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1
Unicopli. Ellis, P. D. (2010). The essential guide to effect sizes: Statistical power, meta-analysis, and the
Andersen, T. M., Holmström, B., Honkapohja, S., Korkman, S., Tson, S. H., & interpretation of research results. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/
Vartiainen, J. (2007). The Nordic model. Embracing globalization and sharing risks. 10.1017/CBO9780511761676
Taloustieto Oy. https://www.etla.fi/wp-content/uploads/2012/09/B232.pdf. Erceg-Hurn, D. M., & Mirosevich, V. M. (2008). Modern robust statistical methods: An
easy way to maximize the accuracy and power of your research. American
Psychologist, 63(7), 591–601. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.591

10
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Bernzweig, J. (1990). Coping with children’s negative Johnson, A. M., Hawes, D. J., Eisenberg, N., Kohlhoff, J., & Dudeney, J. (2017). Emotion
emotions scale (CCNES): Description and scoring. Arizona State University. socialization and child conduct problems: A comprehensive review and meta-
Fabes, R. A., Poulin, R. E., Eisenberg, N., & Madden-Derdich, D. A. (2002). The coping analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 54, 65–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
with Children’s negative emotions scale (CCNES): Psychometric properties and cpr.2017.04.001
relations with children’s emotional competence. Marriage & Family Review, 34(3–4), Karevold, E., Tamnes, C. K., Havighurst, S. S., Nygaard, E., Bjørk, R. F., Stavrinou, M. L.,
285–310. https://doi.org/10.1300/J002v34n03_05 & Espeseth, T. (2021). A pilot study of a parent emotion socialization intervention:
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical Impact on parent behavior, child self-regulation, and adjustment. Frontiers in
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Psychology, 4552. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.730278
Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146 Kårstad, S. B. (2016). Young children’s emotion understanding: The impact of parent and
Feingold, A. (2009). Effect sizes for growth-modeling analysis for controlled clinical child factors, socioeconomic status, and culture [doctoral dissertation, Norwegian
trials in the same metric as for classical analysis. Psychological Methods, 14(1), 43–53. University of Science and Technology]. https://doi.org/10.5539/jedp.v6n2p113
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014699 Kårstad, S. B., Vikan, A., Berg-Nielsen, T.-S., Moreira, L., de Abreu, E. L., & Rique, J.
Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (4th ed.). Sage. (2016). Young brazilian children’s emotion understanding: A comparison within and
Franco, M. D. G., Beja, M. J., Candeias, A., & Santos, N. (2017). Emotion understanding, across cultures. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 6(2), 113–124.
social competence and school achievement in children from primary school in https://doi.org/10.5539/jedp.v6n2p113
Portugal. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1376). https://doi.org/10.3389/ Kårstad, S. B., Wichstrom, L., Reinfjell, T., Belsky, J., & Berg-Nielsen, T. S. (2015). What
fpsyg.2017.01376 enhances the development of emotion understanding in young children? A
Gardner, D., Harris, P. L., Ohmoto, M., & Hamazaki, T. (1988). Japanese children’s longitudinal study of interpersonal predictors. British Journal of Developmental
understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. International Psychology, 33(3), 340–354. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12095
Journal of Behavioral Development, 11(2), 203–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Katz, L. F., Maliken, A. C., & Stettler, N. M. (2012). Parental Meta-emotion philosophy: A
016502548801100204 review of research and theoretical framework. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4),
Garrett-Peters, P. T., Castro, V. L., & Halberstadt, A. G. (2017). Parents’ beliefs about 417–422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00244.x
children’s emotions, children’s emotion understanding, and classroom adjustment in Kockeritz, M., Klinkhammer, J., & von Salisch, M. (2010). The development of emotional
middle childhood. Social Development, 26(3), 575–590. https://doi.org/10.1111/ understanding and behavorial self-regulation in kindergarten children from german
sode.12222 and immigrant families. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 59(7),
Godleski, S. A., Eiden, R. D., Shisler, S., & Livingston, J. A. (2020). Parent socialization of 529–544.
emotion in a high-risk sample. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 489–502. https:// Lake, N., Lane, S., & Harris, P. L. (1995). The expectation of guilt and resistance to
doi.org/10.1037/dev0000793 temptation. Early Development and Parenting, 4(2), 63–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Gottman, J. M., & Declaire, J. (1997). Raising an emotionally intelligent child: The heart of edp.2430040203
parenting. Simon & Schuster. Morgan, J. K., Izard, C. E., & King, K. A. (2010). Construct validity of the emotion
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and matching task: Preliminary evidence for convergent and criterion validity of a new
the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of emotion knowledge measure for young children. Social Development, 19(1), 52–70.
Family Psychology, 10(3), 243–268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00529.x
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How families communicate Morra, S., Parrella, I., & Camba, R. (2011). The role of working memory in the
emotionally. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. development of emotion comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Hansen, J. E., & Alvestad, M. (2018). Educational language practices described by 29(4), 744–764. https://doi.org/10.1348/2044-835x.002006
preschool teachers in Norwegian kindergartens. European Early Childhood Education Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of
Research Journal, 26(1), 128–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/ the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16
1350293X.2018.1412052 (2), 361–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Ornaghi, V., Grazzani, I., Cherubin, E., Conte, E., & Piralli, F. (2015). ‘Let’s talk about
Basil Blackwell. emotions!’. The effect of conversational training on preschoolers’ emotion
Harris, P. L. (1994). The child’s understanding of emotion: Developmental change and comprehension and prosocial orientation. Social Development, 24(1), 166–183.
the family environment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(1), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12091
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01131.x Perlman, S. B., Camras, L. A., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2008). Physiology and functioning:
Harris, P. L., Johnson, C. N., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., & Cooke, T. (1989). Young Parents’ vagal tone, emotion socialization, and children’s emotion knowledge.
children’s theory of mind and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 3(4), 379–400. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(4), 308–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938908412713 j.jecp.2008.03.007
Haslinger, J. (2014). Computerized test of emotion comprehension. University of Vienna. Pons, F., & Harris, P. L. (2000). Test of emotion comprehension: TEC. Oxford University
Havighurst, S. S., Duncombe, M., Frankling, E., Holland, K., Kehoe, C., & Stargatt, R. Press.
(2015). An emotion-focused early intervention for children with emerging conduct Pons, F., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Longitudinal change and longitudinal stability of
problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(4), 749–760. https://doi.org/ individual differences in children’s emotion understanding. Cognition and Emotion,
10.1007/s10802-014-9944-z 19(8), 1158–1174. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500282108
Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2007). Tuning in to kids: Emotionally intelligent parenting Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and
program manual. University of Melbourne. 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of
Havighurst, S. S., & Kehoe, C. E. (2021). Tuning in to kids: An emotion coaching Developmental Psychology, 1(2), 127–152. https://doi.org/10.1080/
approach to working with parents. In C. A. Essau, D. J. Hawes, & J. L. Allen (Eds.), 17405620344000022
Family-based intervention for child and adolescent mental health: A core competencies Raval, V. V., & Walker, B. L. (2019). Unpacking ‘culture’: Caregiver socialization of
approach (pp. 269–283). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/ emotion and child functioning in diverse families. Developmental Review, 51,
9781108682053.021. 146–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.11.001
Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Kehoe, C., Efron, D., & Prior, M. R. (2013). Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. Guilford Press.
“Tuning in to kids”: Reducing young children’s behavior problems using an emotion Saarni, C., & Harris, P. L. (1991). Children’s understanding of emotion. Cambridge
coaching parenting program. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 44(2), University Press.
247–264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-012-0322-1 Salmon, K., Dadds, M. R., Allen, J., & Hawes, D. J. (2009). Can emotional language skills
Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., & Prior, M. R. (2009). Tuning in to kids: An be taught during parent training for conduct problem children? Child Psychiatry and
emotion-focused parenting program-initial findings from a community trial. Journal Human Development, 40(4), 485–498. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0139-8
of Community Psychology, 37(8), 1008–1023. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.20345 Salmon, K., Dittman, C., Sanders, M. R., Burson, R., & Hammington, J. (2014). Does
Havighurst, S. S., Wilson, K. R., Harley, A. E., Prior, M. R., & Kehoe, C. (2010). Tuning in adding an emotion component enhance the triple P-positive parenting program?
to kids: Improving emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children - Journal of Family Psychology, 28(2), 244–252. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035997
findings from a community trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(12), Schuberth, D. A., Zheng, Y., Pasalich, D. S., McMahon, R. J., Kamboukos, D., Dawson-
1342–1350. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02303.x McClure, S., & Brotman, L. M. (2019). The role of emotion understanding in the
Heck, R. H., Thomas, S. L., & Tabata, L. N. (2014). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling development of aggression and callous-unemotional features across early childhood.
with IBM SPSS (2nd ed.). Routledge. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(4), 619–631. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? s10802-018-0468-9
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). A preliminary study of the emotion
S0140525X0999152X understanding of youths referred for treatment of anxiety disorders. Journal of
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context. Online Clinical Child Psychology, 29(3), 319–327. https://doi.org/10.1207/
Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1), 2307–0919 https://scholarworks.gvsu.edu/ s15374424jccp2903_3
orpc/vol2/iss1/8/. Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation and understanding:
Hofstede-Insigths. (2021). Country comparison. Retrieved November 18 from https Implications for child psychopathology and therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 22
://www.hofstede-insights.com/country-comparison/. (2), 189–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00087-3
Howell, D. C. (2013). Statistical methods for psychology (8th ed.). Wadsworth Cengage Sprung, M., Munch, H. M., Harris, P. L., Ebesutani, C., & Hofmann, S. G. (2015).
Learning. Children’s emotion understanding: A meta-analysis of training studies. Developmental
Jespersen, J. E., Hardy, N. R., & Morris, A. S. (2021). Parent and peer emotion Review, 37(9), 41–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.05.001
responsivity styles: An extension of Gottman’s emotion socialization parenting SSB. (2018). Over 1 million innbyggere i Oslo tettsted [Over 1 million inhabitants in Oslo
typologies. Children, 8(5), 319. https://www.mdpi.com/2227-9067/8/5/319. and suburbs]. Retrieved 10.22 from https://www.ssb.no/befolkning/artikler-og
-publikasjoner/over-1-million-innbyggere-i-oslo-tettsted.

11
R.F. Bjørk et al. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 78 (2022) 101368

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Pearson emotion knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10(3), 162–187. https://
Education. doi.org/10.1080/15248370903155825
Tang, Y., Harris, P. L., Pons, F., Zou, H., Zhang, W., & Xu, Q. (2018). The understanding Viana, K. M. P., Zambrana, I. M., Karevold, E. B., & Pons, F. (2019). Emotions in motion:
of emotion among young Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Impact of emotion understanding on children’s peer action coordination. Cognition
Development, 42(5), 512–517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025417741366 and Emotion, 34(4), 831–838. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1669535
Thompson, R. A. (2013). Socialization of emotion and emotion regulation in the family. Vikan, A., Kårstad, S. B., & Dias, M. (2013). Young Brazilian and Norwegian children’s
In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 173–186). Guilford concepts of strategies and goals for emotion regulation. Journal of Early Childhood
Publications. Research, 11(1), 63–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X12456981
Thompson, S. F., Zalewski, M., Kiff, C. J., Moran, L., Cortes, R., & Lengua, L. J. (2020). An Wallace, P., & Kjøs, P. (2012). Opp igjen!: om å reise seg etter en smell [Up agian!: how to
empirical test of the model of socialization of emotion: Maternal and child stand after a fall]. Vigmostad & Bjørke.
contributors to preschoolers’ emotion knowledge and adjustment. Developmental Wilson, K. R., Havighurst, S. S., & Harley, A. E. (2012). Tuning in to kids: An
Psychology, 56(3), 418–430. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000860 effectiveness trial of a parenting program targeting emotion socialization of
Trotter, S. R. (2015). Breaking the law of Jante. Myth and Nation, 23(1), 1–24. preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 26(1), 56–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/
https://www.gla.ac.uk/media/Media_404385_smxx.pdf. a0026480
Van Bergen, P., Salmon, K., Dadds, M. R., & Allen, J. (2009). The effects of mother Zachrisson, H. D., & Dearing, E. (2015). Family income dynamics, early childhood
training in emotion-rich, elaborative reminiscing on children’s shared recall and education and care, and early child behavior problems in Norway. Child Development,
86(2), 425–440. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12306

12

You might also like