Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 17

4 Changing Behavior Using the Health

Belief Model and Protection


Motivation Theory
Sheina Orbell, Hina Zahid, and Caroline J. Henderson

Practical Summary

The health belief model and protection motivation theory are long-established models of
behavior change. They underpin many health communications, such as warnings on cigarette
packets, and are often the basis of commercial advertisements. According to these models,
behavior change follows from inducing a person to perceive threat if they do not act and
simultaneously providing information about an effective behavior they can easily enact in order
to address that threat (coping appraisal). Threat can be manipulated by increasing a sense of
vulnerability to a serious outcome. Coping appraisal involves providing information that
behavior will be effective in reducing threat and by illustrating that behavior, showing that it is
easy and not unpleasant to enact. If behavior change interventions manipulate threat appraisal
alone, they may backfire, because they create anxiety without a solution, and, in this case,
people will cope by discounting or denying the evidence or engage in wishful thinking.
4.1 Introduction Over time, cancer, cardiovascular disease, and
stroke became major sources of early mortality
(e.g., CDC, 2013). These changes set the scene
During the twentieth century, developed
for development of scientific interest in
countries witnessed a significant shift in major
understanding how best to promote behavior
causes of mortality from those predominantly
that would maximize the effectiveness of new
related to bacterial and viral infection, which
public health preventive measures such as
were exacerbated by living conditions such as
screening and modify behaviors such as
overcrowding and poor sanitation, to mortality
smoking, alcohol use, poor diet, and insufficient
that was attributable to what is now commonly
physical activity that are major causes of ill
referred to as
health.
“lifestyle”-related. Several factors contributed
Psychological science also underwent a
to this shift, including improvements in living
major paradigm shift toward the middle of
conditions; the development and introduction of
the twentieth century. During the early
new medical technologies such as screening for
decades of the twentieth century,
tuberculosis and vaccination programs for
psychology was dominated by behaviorism
diseases such as polio; and economic
and the view that people learn
development associated with declining working
stimulusresponse associations so that their
hours, jobs that were less physically
actions are controlled by external factors,
demanding, and greater disposable income.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
47 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

specifically contingent rewards or empirical evidence is evaluated, and the


punishments (Hull, 1943; Skinner, 1938). (enduring) use and efficacy of the models in
However, behaviorism was soon replaced informing intervention to modify behavior
is considered.
https://doi.org/10.1017/97811086773180.004 4.2 Overview of Theory and Evidence
by a view that people acquire mental
representations of their world and
expectations concerning the outcomes of 4.2.1 The Health Belief Model
their action. This shift from external to The health belief model is illustrated in Figure
internal cognitive control of behavior led to 4.1. The model focused on two aspects of
the development of a class of models people’s cognitive representations of health
referred to as expectancy value theories. behavior. Two key beliefs, perceived
The health belief model (Becker, Haefner,
susceptibility to illness and expected severity or
Kasl et al., 1977; Hochbaum, 1958;
impact of illness, comprise the mental
Rosenstock, 1966, 1974) and protection
representation of illness itself. Perceived
motivation theory (Rogers, 1975, 1983) are
benefits of action and perceived barriers/costs
two ofthe earliestsocial cognitiveaccounts
to action comprised the mental representation
ofpersonal behavior that were developed
of action. The model also proposed that cues to
primarily in the context of understanding
action serve to trigger behavior when
and changing health behavior. They
appropriate beliefs are held. Cues to action
established enduring theoretical principles
might include such things as a reminder letter
for behavior change that persist in some
or text message, a new symptom, or a television
more recent theoretical developments (see,
advertisement. An individual’s general health
e.g., Chapters 2, 7, and 10, this volume). In
motivation was also included in later versions
the following sections, the components and
of the model (e.g., Becker, Haefner, & Maiman,
structure of the health belief model and
1977). These six constructs together comprised
protection motivation theory are outlined,
the health belief model. No formal propositions

Perceived susceptibility
Perceived severity
Socio-demographic Health motivation
Action
variables Perceived costs or
barriers
Perceived benefits

Cues to
Action
Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004

Figure 4.1 Schematic representation of the health belief model


Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 48

regarding the operationalization of constructs, were grounded in the need to explain and
nor combinatorial rules, were proposed. intervene to achieve improved health equality in
Typically, the constructs are viewed and society via uptake of preventive health services
operationalized as separate independent or modifications of personal behavior with
variables that additively predict behavior. implications for health (Rosenstock, 1974).
These features of the model may be viewed as Sociodemographic variables are not considered
weaknesses in the status of the model modifiable variables by psychologists, but it
(Harrison, Mullen, & Green, 1992) but may was hypothesized that identification of
also be seen as strengths in the context of a modifiable health beliefs that
developing understanding of beliefs associated distinguishbetweenindividualsevenofthesameba
with a wide range of different behaviors with ckground would inform evidence-based health
implications for health. For example, the education to achieve greater health equality
severity of a condition or illness might be (e.g., Orbell, Crombie, & Johnstone, 1996).
conceptualized in relation to a range of Currently, morbidity, mortality, and virtually all
expectancies regarding a wide range of health behaviors are persistently associated with
potentially relevant outcomes such as socioeconomic status, even in contexts where
disfigurement, change in personal or sexual health care interventions such as screening are
relations, inability to work, loss of income or publicly financed and free at point of delivery
loss of the ability to engage in desired leisure (e.g., Orbell et al., 2017). The health belief
activities, and model proposes that health beliefs mediate the
soon,aswellasearlydeath,andperceivedbenefits relationship of sociodemographic variables to
might correspondingly be related to potential health action. For example, social experience
prevention of these outcomes. Relatedly, engendered by social structural position might
barriers will be behavior-specific. For example, shape beliefs and subsequent
barriers to a behavior that requires appointment behavior(e.g.,doperceivedcosts/barriersloomlarg
keeping may include such things as time or erfor those with limited economic resources, or
distance or means of transport, whereas less daily experience of job discretion?) (Orbell
unpleasantness or lack of skill may be more et al., 1996; Salloway, Pletcher, & Collins,
relevant in another context such as making 1978). From a theoretical point of view, an
complex dietary changes. The generic important moderator hypothesis can also be
conceptualization of health beliefs permits derived from the model;
researchers to undertake effective pilot work in modificationofhealthbeliefswillmoderate(attenu
order to identify context-appropriate beliefs ate) the relationship of sociodemographic
about illness and barriers to action to measure variables to behavior, thereby narrowing
and to target in behavior change intervention. sociodemographic inequality.
A further important feature of the health
belief model concerns the relationship of health
beliefs to socioeconomic status and other
demographic indices such as gender, ethnicity,
and age. Figure
4.1showsthesevariablesontheleft-handportionof
themodel.Earlyaccountsofthehealthbeliefmodel

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
49 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

4.2.2 Protection Motivation Theory result in negative experiences, aka


“perceived barriers”), and self-efficacy
Protection motivation theory is represented
(beliefs in one’s competence and confidence
in Figure 4.2. The theory formalizes the
to perform the recommended action;
idea that protection motivation, that is,
Bandura, 1977) (see
motivation to take action to promote health
alsoChapters3and7,thisvolume).Threatappr
and prevent illness, or some other potential
aisal is proposed to enhance motivation to
threat, is derived from threat appraisal
act, in circumstances where response
combined with coping appraisal. Coping
efficacy is high, response costs are low, and
appraisal is viewed as the mental
self-efficacy is high.
representation of the “recommended”
Protection motivation theory also makes
behavioral response to threat.
theoretical predictions regarding the
Unlikethehealth belief model,the mental
possibility that an individual may not
representations of threat and coping
respond to threat by taking appropriate
appraisal are not proposed as direct
protective action to address it but, instead,
determinants of action but as direct
attempt to manage and reduce feelings of
determinantsofprotectionmotivation,usually

Cope with Threat


(e.g., Avoidance
Threat Appraisal Denial, Fatalism,
Perceived susceptibility Wishful thinking,
Perceived severity Hopelessness)
Fear

Coping Appraisal Protection Threat


Perceived response- motivation protective
efficacy (Intention to act behavior
Perceived response-cost to manage threat)
Perceived self-efficacy

Figure 4.2 Schematic representation of protection motivation theory


operationalized as intention. Intention, in threat. These alternate responses are often
turn, is a proximal determinant of action. referred to as “maladaptive coping
Threat appraisal comprises responses” (Rippetoe & Rogers, 1987),
perceivedsusceptibilityto, and perceived illustrated in Figure 4.2 by the box labeled
severityof,anoutcomeifactionisnottaken.Co “Cope with threat.” Where threat appraisal
ping appraisal comprises response efficacy is high, but response efficacy and self-
(beliefs that efficacy are low and response costs high,
therecommendedresponsewillbeeffectiveinr rather than embark on the goal of avoiding
educing threat, aka “perceived benefits”), threat, an individual may employ a range of
response costs (expectancies that strategies directed at discounting evidence
performing the recommended response will or denying threat or avoiding thinking about

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 50

threat in order to minimize unpleasant


emotions.
4.2.3 Empirical Evidence
Unsurprisingly, given their longevity and
continued use, the health belief model and
protection motivation theory have been subject
to multiple research syntheses and meta-
analyses. Janz and Becker’s (1984) review of
forty-six studies employing the health belief
model using a votecount procedure provided
strong support for the model components:
susceptibility (81 percent significant tests of
association), severity (65 percent), benefits (78
percent), and barriers (89 percent). Harrison,
Mullen, and Green (1992) and Carpenter (2010)
provided full meta-analyses that were capable
of not only establishing if relations were
significant but also estimating the size and
variability in the effects. Two metaanalytic
syntheses of protection motivation theory
(Floyd, Prentice-Dunn, & Rogers, 2000; Milne,
Sheeran, & Orbell, 2000) estimated the size and
variability of effects among the theory
constructs. These meta-analyses have tended to
focus on health behaviors but the models have
also been employed in occupational and
marketing contexts (see Sidebars 4.1 and 4.2).
Previous summaries of evidence have tended
to
relyontestsofassociationderivedfromprospective
designs, wherein beliefs and behavior are
measured

Sidebar 4.1 Protection motivation in an occupational setting

Safety is a major concern in many workplace settings and occupational injury contributes
substantially to absence from work. Lower back pain is a widespread occupational hazard
facing health care workers. Patient handling is often unpredictable, the human load bulky and
unstable, and often lifting is required in awkward and unplanned situations – factors that
contribute to manual handling risks
notfoundinotheroccupationalsettings.Inaddition,protectivebehaviorviatheuseof a hoist
requires the cooperation of coworkers. Rickett, Orbell, and Sheeran (2006)
investigatedtheroleofthreatandcopingappraisalspecifiedbyprotectionmotivation theory in

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
51 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

explaining consistent hoist usage by health care workers. They developed scales to assess
threat appraisal via perceived susceptibility to back problems (the
likelihoodofacquiringbackproblemsasaconsequenceofmovingpatients),perceived severity (the
extent to which back problems would interfere with career and social roles), and fear of back
problems. Coping appraisal comprised response efficacy (the
extenttowhichusingahoistwouldreducethechancesofacquiringorworseningback problems),
response costs of using the hoist (such as the extent to which hoist use was perceived as time-
consuming or would make the health care worker difficult to work with), response costs of
not using the hoist (such as annoying my boss or colleagues), and self-efficacy (control over,
competency, and ease of hoist use). Findings showed that threat appraisal variables were
unrelated to the intention to use a hoist, and actual hoist use, during the following six weeks,
whereas coping appraisal variables were important predictors, including self-efficacy (r =
0.43), response costs (r = −0.41), response benefits (r = 0.23), and social costs of not using a
hoist (r = 0.35). The study pointed to the importance of workplace culture in ensuring safety
equipment was available and staff competent in its use and to the promotion of shared
beliefs regarding the appropriateness and normative expectation of hoist use.

Sidebar 4.2 Protection motivation in advertising to promote purchasing behavior

Adverts that aim to create new markets or increase product sales often combine elements
that seek to enhance threat, followed by a “recommended response” to buy and usea
product, consistent with protection motivation theory. These adverts typically exploit social
anxieties. This form of advertising is common in relation to dental hygiene products such as
mouthwash, home germ-cleansing products, and products related to home or family
security such as life insurance. For example, the manufacturers of a mouthwash (Listerine TM)
created an advert that self-evidently changed behavior by creating a remarkable new
multimillion-dollar market for a previously unknown product in the 1950s. 1 One of these
adverts showed an

attractiveyoungwomanwhowas“atrisk”(susceptibility)offailingtoattractaman and being left


lonely and without companionship (undesired consequence: severity) because of her bad
breath. The advert went on to introduce a coping response (bottle of mouthwash) that
was portrayed as effective (in curing halitosis and attracting men: response efficacy) and
easy to use (illustrated by images of placing the bottle conveniently by the sink and a
demonstration of its use: self-efficacy).
Modern advertising often follows a similar format by first arousing or making salient the
threat that might arise from neglecting funeral insurance, or allowing children to come
into contact with unclean surfaces, or failing to get the boiler serviced, followed by
product information that emphasizes elements of coping
appraisaldesignedtoenhanceperceptionsthattheproductiseasytoobtainanduse and highly
effective in reducing threat. The purpose of adverts is to create novel behavior and

1 See “Listerine Mouthwash to the Rescue – 1950s,” Online video clip. www.youtube.com/watch? v=_39gkDAbMaI.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 52

increase sales so they often employ effective behavior change techniques such as
modeling to illustrate the perceived ease of use and efficacy of
theproduct.Greateruseofthesecopingappraisaltechniquesininterventionsmight enhance
behavior change in many domains.
at different points in time and measurement of consequence of new experience and knowledge.
beliefs precedes measurement of behavior. Prospective studies, in which the measurement
Many early tests employed what of beliefs precedes the measurement of
epidemiologists might call “case-control behavior, are likely to provide more reliable
studies,” or retrospective designs, in which estimates of the relative importance of beliefs in
people enacting a particular behavior are promoting motivation and behavior change.
compared with those not enacting a previous
behavior. Such approaches may have value in
epidemiology through identifying the location
of an outbreak of disease by answering key
questions (e.g., “Have people with salmonella
poisoning been in a recent location more than
people who do not have salmonella
poisoning?”). However, when applied to the
measurement of beliefs, that is, comparing the
current beliefs of people who have or have not
performed a behavior in the past, they are
fraught with theoretical and empirical
limitation. For example, a person who has
undergone screening for cancer and received a
normal result, or who always uses a condom
during sexual intercourse, may perceive low
personal susceptibility to illness, even if
perceived susceptibility to illness was originally
a motivating force for action (Weinstein &
Nicolich, 1993).
The effect of perceived susceptibility on
behavior might also be attenuated if an
individual responds to threat by engaging in a
threat management coping response such as
denial of risk or fatalism. In such cases, a
measure of perceived susceptibility might
reflect the outcome of this cognitive process.
Relatedly, a person who has performed a given
behavior repeatedly and found it straightforward
and easy will probably perceive few barriers
and high self-efficacy, based on that experience.
Second, beliefs may change over time, as a

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
53 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

A summary of evidence derived from 1995; Hodgkins & Orbell, 1998). For example,
prospective studies of the health belief model Ben-Ahron and colleagues observed that low-
and protection motivation theory is provided in perceived severity and self-efficacy predicted
Table 4.1. Taken together, the table shows that avoidance coping and lowresponse efficacy and
all variables specified by the health belief model self-efficacy predicted religious faith coping in
and protection motivation theory show small relation to binge drinking. Abraham and
consistent relationships in the theoretically colleagues showed that low response and self-
predicted direction with future behavior. Given efficacy were associated with wishful thinking
that barriers may be equated to response costs in relation to condom use to prevent
Table 4.1 Summary of prospective relations between constructs specified by the health belief model and
protection motivation theory and behavior
Meta-analysis

Construct Harrison et al. (1992) Carpenter (2010) Milne et al. (2000)

Susceptibility 0.19 0.05 0.12


Severity 0.13 0.15 0.07
Benefits 0.10 0.27

Barriers −0.16 −0.30

Response Efficacy 0.09

Response Costs −0.25

Self-Efficacy 0.22

Protection Motivation 0.44


(Intention)
Note. Coefficients are bias-corrected averaged correlation coefficients.
and benefits to response efficacy, itmay also HIVinfection. Hodgkins and Orbell observed
beinferred that thesevariables appear to be more that high response costs predicted avoidance of
strongly associated with future behavior than breast self-examination. These studies provide
threat appraisal variables. Selfefficacy also evidence consistent with the theoretical
emerged as an important variable not included prediction that, in the absence of high coping
in the health belief model. The authors of all appraisal, perceived threat may prompt
meta-analytic synthesis do, however, note that maladaptive coping responses.
there is large unexplained heterogeneity in These reviews of evidence are derived only
effects across studies. In addition, the from studies conducted in the health behavior
metaanalyses did not include consideration domain. However, protection motivation theory,
ofmaladaptive responses to threat predicted by in particular, has also guided research in
protection motivation theory. A few studies occupational settings (e.g., Melamed et al.,
have examined these predictions (e.g., Abraham 1996; Rickett, Orbell, & Sheeran, 2006) (see
et al., 1994; BenAhron, White, & Phillips, Sidebar 4.1) and more recently in relation to

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 54

cybersecurity (e.g., Ashley et al., 2016; Hanus communications(leafletsorbooklets)


& Wu, 2016). The meta-analyses provide (e.g.,Strecher et al., 1994), video presentations,
valuable estimates of the size of the relationship or verbal communications viatelephone(e.g.,
between cognitive representations (beliefs) and Anderson etal., 2011), or in person (e.g., Tola et
behavior. However, it should be borne in mind al., 2016). Modern approaches might also
that these syntheses provide summaries of the involve text or e-communications (see Chapter
strength of the relation between single variables 29, this volume). Some interventions employ
and the behavior in question. They do not health professionals and intensive education
provide an assessment of the extent to which the programs lasting over several sessions. For
individual example, Tolaet al.(2016) report ahealth belief
variables,incombination,explainvarianceinfutur model intervention in Ethiopia to improve
e behavior. Future research may consider adherence to treatment in patients with
combining meta-analysis with techniques to test tuberculosis. The intervention involved seven
the combined effects of the theory variables in sessions of health education delivered by health
predicting behavior (e.g., Cheung & Hong, professionals that included psychological
2017; Hagger et al., 2017). Researchers should distress counseling to facilitate the processing
also note the possibility of ceiling effects, a of information and material to address
class of response invariance that occurs when perceived susceptibility and response efficacy,
all participants in a study endorse a belief to an methods to overcome barriers to adherence and
almost identical extent (e.g., the belief that to enhance self-efficacy. The intervention
cancer is a serious disease). resulted in a substantial increase in adherence
Invariance will render a variable statistically that
incapable of explaining variance in behavior, wassupportedbyachangeintargetedhealthbeliefs.
underlining the importance of considering the Interventions designed to demonstrate that a
wording of relevant items carefully. change in beliefs results in a change in behavior
should follow baseline assessment of beliefs
specified by the theory, followed by
4.3 How Have the Theories Been reassessment of the beliefs to determine if the
Used to Change Behavior? manipulation has been successful in changing
beliefs (see Chapters 19 and 22, this volume).
The health belief model and protection Final assessment of behavior at an appropriate
motivation theory have both been tested in point in time to observe behavior change should
interventions to change behavior. However, follow. Good practice to test theory would also
health belief model interventions have more establish that the changes in beliefs engendered
often been employed in public health or clinical by the education mediate the impact of the
contexts, whereas protection motivation theory intervention on behavior change.
has been subject to more laboratory Recommended elements of an intervention to
experimental tests (Milne, Orbell, & Sheeran, change beliefs are presented in Appendix 4.1
2002). Interventions should aim to modify (supplemental materials).
constructs specified by the theory. Typically,
health belief model and protection motivation
theory interventions take the form of written

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
55 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

4.4 Evidence Base for the Use of the be behavior- and context-specific. The
Theories in Changing Behavior assumption that “off-the-shelf” measures, or
pre-validated measures similar to those
available for constructs such as anxiety or
depression, represent superior measures is very
4.4.1 Evaluation of Behavior Change
unhelpful in the context of establishing the
Interventions Based on the
specific cognitive change brought about by an
Health Belief Model and
intervention targeting specific social cognition
Protection Motivation Theory
variables (see Chapter 19, this volume). For
The first reported intervention based on the example, Jones et al. (2013) note the use of a
health belief model was published in 1970 wide range of intervention evaluation measures
(Haefner & Kirscht, 1970). The study that are validated but irrelevant to establishing
concerned check-up visits to the doctor with an the veridicality of manipulation of health belief
impressive eight-month follow-up. Just one model variables. Further limitations of the
systematic review of interventions employing extant evidence base relate to inadequate
the health belief model has been published to transparency and reporting of intervention
date (Jones et al., 2013) and there is, to date, no content in research reports and articles,
review of interventions based on protection practices that stymie evaluationand
motivation theory, although Milne et al. (2002) replicationefforts (see also Chapter 23, this
reviewed the impact of experimental volume), and inadequate follow-up, in terms of
manipulations on changing cognitive both time lag and the objective measurement of
representations. Jones and colleagues reviewed behavioral outcomes.
eighteen studies, including sixteen randomized
controlled trials, in contexts such as alcoholism,
sleep apnea, asthma, and diabetes. The authors
4.4.2 Theoretical and Empirical
concluded that 77 percent of interventions Relationship to Fear Appeals
applying the health belief model resulted in Perceived “threat” is a fundamental variable in
significant changes in behavior but noted that both the health belief model and protection
few studies provided evaluation of the motivation theory and in both theories
mediation hypothesis discussed in section 4.2.1. comprises perceptions of susceptibility to an
While there is considerable support for the adverse outcome and perceived severity of that
idea that behavior change interventions based outcome. Both accounts are premised on the
on the health belief model and protection idea that motivation follows from awareness
motivation theory are capable of changing that there is a threat to be addressed (see also
behavior, it is perhaps timely, given that Chapter 5, this volume). Response to threat is a
severity, benefits, and barriers are threat- function of beliefs about the likelihood of a
specific, to consider reviews of studies that response being effective, and beliefs that the
address behavior change in particular contexts response is easy to perform, and will not result
in those contexts alone rather than attempt to in unpleasant consequences that undermine the
aggregate them across contexts. As noted in positive consequences or benefits. Thus threat
Section 4.2.1, the measurement (and appropriate arousal might be seen as fundamental to
manipulation) of relevant constructs will always interventions to promote behavior change. A

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 56

useful summary term for this idea is “fear components might undermine attempts to
appeal.” This section considers current evidence change behavior. The effect of manipulating
regarding the importance of “appealing to fear.” efficacy under conditions of low threat was
The history of fear appeals is a long one and nonsignificant, whereas the effect of
the importance of threat in fear appeals is a manipulating efficacy under conditions of high
contentious and much debated issue. In recent threat was highly significant and large (d =
years, several meta-analyses have tackled the 0.71). This analysis provides important
question of when appealing to fear may or may clarification of behavior change processes not
not effect behavior change (Kok et al., 2018; available either from meta-analyses of bivariate
Peters, Ruiter, & Kok, 2013; Tannenbaum et al., relations between constructs in prospective
2015; Witte & studies or from existing reviews of behavior
Allen, 2000). Peters and colleagues conducted a change interventions employing protection
review of studies that had manipulated both motivation theory and the health belief model.
threat (via perceived susceptibility or severity Although the analysis is based on the small
or a combination thereof) and efficacy (via number of rigorous tests available, findings
response or self-efficacy or a combination indicate that behavior change interventions
thereof). Their review adopted very stringent should not target threat appraisals unless either
inclusion criteria, requiring that threat and the population is already high in self-efficacy
coping efficacy be manipulated in a 2 × 2 and response efficacy (low barriers, high
experimental design (low vs. high or present vs. benefits) or the intervention also includes
absent), and assessed a “real” behavioral elements that will substantially increase
outcome (e.g., studies where behavior involved response and self-efficacy. For example, if an
requesting more information were excluded). intervention seeks to address accessibility to a
Surprisingly, given that no date restrictions service by providing the service in the worksite
were applied to their search criteria, just six without loss of income, manipulating threat
studies were eligible for inclusion. They perceptions may be adequate. More often,
encompassed a range of behaviors such as however, personal self-efficacy needs to be
taking roundworm medication, earthquake addressed. Findings are consistent with recent
preparation, tetanus vaccination, truancy, and meta-analyses with admittedly less stringent
condom use. inclusion criteria that also propose that
The key hypothesis was that there would be a interventions that promote threat are unlikely to
significant interaction between threat and be effective in isolation. For example, in a
efficacy manipulations, and this was supported meta-analysis of interventions to promote
in the analysis. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) from an condom use to protect against sexually
analysis of simple effects of the manipulations transmitted disease, there was minimal evidence
are presented in Appendix 4.2 (supplemental that threat-inducing arguments were effective
materials). Threat manipulations significantly (Albarracín et al., 2005). Related to these
increased behavioronly when efficacy was findings, Sheeran et al. (2014) suggest that
high.Infact, the effect of manipulating threat in interventions that increased perceptions of
combination with low efficacy was negative, susceptibility have larger effects on behavior
suggesting that this combination of intervention

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
57 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

change if they also increase response efficacy Niederdeppe & Kemp, 2018; Peters &
and selfefficacy (d = 0.52). ShootsReinhard, 2018; Peters et al., 2018;
Behaviorchangestudiesneedtoemployeffectiv Roberto, Mongeau, & Liu, 2018; White &
e methods to target these constructs and Albarracín, 2018), those communications
demonstrate, focused primarily on behavior change (that
inpilotworkwithsimilarpopulations,thattheinterv leads to economic gains by
ention has powerful effects on these constructs sellingproducts)tendtoemploythreatasameansto
prior to implementation (see Appendix 4.2). introduce clear visual and narrative
Further work is also warranted that employs 2 representations of response efficacy, and self-
(threat manipulation/intervention) × 2 efficacy, including behavioral modeling (see
(selfefficacy/response efficacy manipulation), or Sidebar 4.2). However, it should also be noted
even 2 (severity manipulation) × 2 that a good deal of behavior change in health
(susceptibility manipulation) × 2 (response contexts concerns changing existing behavioral
efficacy manipulation) × 2 (self-efficacy habits that require specific skills and techniques
manipulation), designs with sufficiently large (see Chapter 13, this volume).
samples to examine the effects of manipulations
of severity, susceptibility, response efficacy, and
4.4.3 Relationship of Beliefs to
self-efficacy independently on behavior and
Sociodemographic Variables
their interactive effects. Protection motivation
theory provides a theoretical rationale for The health belief model (Figure 4.1) proposed
people making “threat control” or emotion that beliefs mediate the effects of social
control responses to health communications (see structural variables such as socioeconomic
Figure 4.2). Peters and colleagues’ (2013) status, income, and education and demographic
observation that threat combined with low variables such as gender, age, and ethnicity on
efficacy had a negative effect on behavior is behavior. Relatively few studies have formally
consistent with the prediction from protection examined this mediation hypothesis (e.g.,
motivation theory that people will engage in Orbell et al., 1996). More recently, Orbell et al.
denial, refutation of evidence, (2017) provided a unique evaluation of the
oremotionmanagementresponseswhentherecom ability of health beliefs to mediate the effects of
mended coping response is difficult or both socioeconomic status and ethnicity on
psychologically costly (Rippetoe & Rogers, objectively observed colorectal screening
1987). Behavior change interventions that behavior. Findings provided strong support for
manipulate threat alone are not indicated by the role of perceived costs (barriers) and self-
current evidence and may do more harm than efficacy as mediator variables. As noted in
good. Whereas health communications such as Section 4.2.1, the identification of modifiable
graphic threat images on cigarette packets tend cognitive representations not only permits
to neglect the importance of intervention to change behavior but such
selfefficacyofrecommendedactiononbehaviorcha intervention should also moderate (attenuate)
nge (interested readers might like to consider the relationship of sociodemographic variables
recent debate of this issue: Borland, 2018; on future behavior where such prior
Brewer, Hall, & Noar, 2018; Kok et al., 2018; relationships between sociodemographic
Malouff, 2018;

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 58

variables and behavior have been established manipulations, evidence that the manipulations
(see also Chapter 27, this volume). impact on coping efficacy, and long-term
This hypothesis remains largely untested. follow-up of behavior.
Some recent research does suggest one
possible route by which manipulation of
perceived benefits (response efficacy) and
References
perceived barriers (response costs) might Abraham, C. S., Sheeran, P., Abrams, D., & Spears,
modify sociodemographic variability. R. (1994). Exploring teenagers’ adaptive and
maladaptive thinking in relation to the threat of
Manipulations in which the benefits of action
HIVinfection. Psychology and Health, 9, 253–
are framed as more immediate and the costs
272.
more distal have been shown to increase
https://doi.org/10.1080/08870449408407485
motivation for preventive health behavior across Albarracín, D., Gillete,J.C., Earl, A.N., Glasman,L.R.,
multiple domains (Orbell, Perugini, & Rakow, & Durantini, M. R. (2005). A test of major
2004; Orbell & Hagger, 2006; Orbell & assumptions about behaviour change:
Kyriakai, 2008; see also Whitaker et al., 2011). A comprehensive look at the effects of passive
A recent study showed that these manipulations and active HIV interventions since the beginning
modified the motivation of people with of the epidemic. Psychological Bulletin, 131,
Pakistani ethnicity to take up screening (Zahid, 856–897.
2019). People of Pakistani ethnicity may share a https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.856
form of predeterministic fatalism that results in Anderson, R. T., Camacho, F., Iaconi, A. I.,
Tegeler, C. H., & Balkrishnan, R. (2011).
low perceived efficacy to modify the long-term
Enhancing the effectiveness of community
life course. Manipulating response efficacy as
stroke risk screening: A randomised controlled
an immediate benefit served to promote
trial. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular
motivation for screening. Further research is Diseases,
required that investigates the moderation
20, 330–335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
hypothesis. .jstrokecerebrovasdis.2010.02.002
Ashley, D. N., Boothe, L. G., & Pearson, M. R.
(2016). Risky electronic communication
4.5 Conclusions behaviors and cyberbullying victimisation: An
application of Protection Motivation Theory.
The health belief model and protection Computers in
motivation theory provided an important Human Behavior, 60, 508–513. https://doi.org/10
delineation of key variables in motivating .1016/j.chb.2016.02.010
behavior change, particularly in the context of Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Towards a
health threat, and were two of the earliest unifying theory of behavioral change.
expectancy value models of behavior change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
https://doi.org/10.1037
Variables specified by the models predict
/0033-295X.84.2.191
behavior prospectively and many interventions
Becker, M. H., Haefner, D. P., Kasl, S. V., Kirscht, J.
guided by these models show effects on
P., Maiman, L. A., & Rosenstock, I. M. (1977).
behavior change. Use of the models to change Selected psychosocial models and correlates of
behavior should ensure adequate consideration individual health-related behaviors. Medical
of coping efficacy, publication of content of Care,

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
59 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

15, 27–46. https://doi.org/10.1097/ /j.1559-1816.2000.tb02323.x


00005650-197705001-00005 Haefner, D. P., & Kirscht, J. P. (1970). Motivational
Becker, M. H., Haefner, D. P., & Maiman, L. A. and behavioral effects of modifying health
(1977). The health belief model in the prediction beliefs.
of dietary compliance: A field experiment. Public Health Reports, 85, 478–484.
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 18, 348– Hagger, M. S., Koch, S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., &
366. https://doi.org Orbell, S. (2017). The common-sense model
/10.2307/2955344 of self-regulation: Meta-analysis and test of a
Ben-Ahron, V., White, D., & Phillips, K. (1995). process model. Psychological Bulletin, 143,
Encouraging drinking at safe limits on single 1117–1154.
occasions: The potential contribution of https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000118
protection motivation theory. Alcohol and Hanus, B., & Wu, Y. A. (2016). Impact of users’
Alcoholism, 30, 633–639. https://doi.org/10.1093 security awareness on desktop security behavior:
/oxfordjournals.alcalc.a045774 A protection motivation theory perspective.
Borland, R. (2018). Misinterpreting theory and Information Systems Management, 33, 2–16.
ignoring evidence: Fear appeals can actually https://doi.org/10.1080/10580530.2015.1117842
work: A comment on Kok et al. (2018). Health Harrison, J. A., Mullen, P. D., & Green, L. W. (1992).
Psychology Review, 12, 126–128. https://doi.org A meta-analysis of studies of the health belief
/10.1080/17437199.2018.1445545 model with adults. Health Education Research,
Brewer, N. T., Hall, M. G., & Noar, S. M. (2018). 7, 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/7.1.107
Pictorial cigarette pack warnings increase Hochbaum, G. M. (1958). Public Participation in
quitting: A comment on Kok et al. Health Medical Screening Programs: A
Psychology Review, 12, 129–132. https://doi.org Sociopsychological Study. Public Health Service
/10.1080/17437199.2018.1445544 Publication 572. Washington, DC: US
Carpenter, C. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of the Government Printing Office.
effectiveness of health belief model variables in Hodgkins, S. & Orbell, S. (1998) Does protection-
predicting behavior. Health Communication, 25, motivation theory predict behavior? A
661–669. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236 longitudinal test and exploration of the role of
.2010.521906 previous behavior. Psychology and Health, 13,
CDC (Centers for Disease Control). (2013). Health 237–250. https://doi.org/10.1080
Behaviors of Adults: United States, 2008–2010. /08870449808406749 Hull, C. L. (1943).
Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Principles of Behaviour: An Introduction to
Human Services. Behaviour Theory. Oxford:
Cheung, M. W. L., & Hong, R. Y. (2017). Appleton-Century.
Applications of meta-analytic structural equation Janz, N., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief
modeling in health psychology: Examples, model: A decade later. Health Education
issues, and recommendations. Health Psychology Quarterly, 11, 1–47. https://doi.org/10.1177
Review, 11, /109019818401100101
265–279. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199 Jones, C. J., Smith, H., & Llewellyn, C. (2013).
.2017.1343678 Evaluating the effectiveness of interventions in
Floyd, D. L., Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. improving adherence: A systematic review.
(2000). A meta-analysis of research on Health Psychology Review, 8, 253–269.
protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Kok, G., Peters, G.-J. Y., Kessels, L. T. E., Hoor, G.
Social Psychology, 30, 407–429. A., & Ruiter, R. A. C. (2018). Ignoring theory
https://doi.org/10.1111 and misinterpreting evidence: The false belief in

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 60

fear appeals. Health Psychology Review, 12, .25.4.537


111–125. Orbell, S., & Kyriakai, M. (2008). Temporal framing
https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2017.1415767. and persuasion to adopt preventive health
Malouff, J. M. (2018). What constitutes evidence that behavior: Moderating effects of individual
fear appeals have positive effects on health differences in consideration of future
behaviour? Commentary on Kok, Peters, consequences on sunscreen use. Health
Kessels, ten Hoor, and Ruiter (2018). Health Psychology, 27, 770–779.
Psychology https://doi.org/10.1037
Review, 12, 133–135. https://doi.org/10.1080 /0278-6133.27.6.770
/17437199.2018.1445541 Orbell, S., Perugini, M., & Rakow, T. (2004).
Melamed, S., Rabinowitz, S., Feiner, M., & Individual differences in sensitivity to health
Weisberg, E. (1996). Usefulness of protection communications: Consideration of future
motivation theory in explaining hearing consequences. Health
protection device use among male industrial Psychology, 23, 388–396.
workers. Health https://doi.org/10.1037/
Psychology, 15, 209–215. https://doi.org/10.1037 0278-6133.23.4.388
//0278-6133.15.3.209 Orbell, S., Sczepura, A., Gumber, A., Weller, D., &
Milne, S., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2002). Hagger, M. S. (2017). South Asian ethnicity,
Combining motivational and volitional socio-economic status and psychological
interventions to promote exercise participation: mediators of faecal occult colorectal screening
Protection motivation theory and implementation participation: A test of a process model. Health
intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology, Psychology, 36, 1161–1172. https://doi.org/10
7, 163–184. .1037/hea0000525
https://doi.org/10.1348/135910702169420 Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C., & Kok, G. (2013).
Milne, S., Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Prediction Threatening communication: A critical re-
and intervention in health-related behavior: A analysis and a revised meta-analytic test of fear
meta-analytic review of protection motivation appeal theory. Health Psychology Review, 7, S8–
theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, S31.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2012.703527
106–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816
.2000.tb02308.x Peters, G.-J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C., ten Hoor, G. A.,
Niederdeppe, J., & Kemp, D. (2018). Ignoring theory Kessels, L. T. E., & Kok, G. (2018). Towards
and evidence: Commentary on Kok et al. (2018). consensus on fear appeals: A rejoinder to the
Health Psychology Review, 12, 136–139. https:// commentaries on Kok, Peters, Kessels, ten Hoor, and
doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.1445543 Ruiter (2018). Health Psychology Review, 12,
Orbell, S., Crombie, I., & Johnston, G. (1996) Social 151–156. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199
cognition and social structure in the prediction .2018.1454846
of cervical screening uptake. British Journal of Peters, E., & Shoots-Reinhard, B. (2018). Don’t
Health Psychology, 1, 35–50. https://doi.org/10 throw the baby out with the bath water:
.1111/j.2044-8287.1996.tb00490.x Commentary on Kok, Peters, Kessels, ten Hoor,
Orbell, S., & Hagger, M. S. (2006). Temporal framing and Ruiter (2018). Health Psychology Review,
and the decision to take part in Type 2 diabetes 12, 140–143. https://
screening: Effects of individual differences in doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2018.1445542
consideration of future consequences on Rickett, B., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2006).
persuasion. Health Psychology, Socialcognitive determinants of joist usage
25, 537–548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133 among health care workers. Journal of

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
61 SHEINA ORBELL, HINA ZAHID, AND CAROLINE J. HENDERSON

Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 182–196. review of the experimental evidence.


https://doi.org Psychological Bulletin,
/10.1037/1076-8998.11.2.182 140, 511–543. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033065
Rippetoe, P. A., & Rogers, R. W. (1987). Effects of Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behaviour of Organisms:
components of protection motivation theory on An
adaptive and maladaptive coping with a health Experimental Analysis. New York:
threat. Journal of Personality and Social AppletonCentury.
Psychology, 52, 596–604. Strecher, V. J., Kreuter, M., DenBoer, D. J., Kobrin,
https://doi.org/10.1037 S., Hospers, H. J., & Skinner, C. S. (1994). The
/0022-3514.52.3.596 effects of computer tailored smoking cessation
Roberto, A. J., Mongeau, P. A., & Liu, Y. (2018). A messages in family practice settings. Journal of
(re) defining moment for fear appeals: A Family Practice, 39, 262–270.
comment on Kok et al. (2018). Health Tannenbaum, M. B., Hepler, J., Zimmerman, R. S. et
Psychology Review, 12, al. (2015). Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of
144–146. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199 fear appeal effectiveness and theories.
.2018.1445546 Psychological Bulletin, 141, 1178–1204.
Rogers, R. W. (1975). A protection motivation theory https://doi.org/10.1037 /a0039729.
of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of Tola, H. H., Shojaeizadeh, D., Tol, A. et al. (2016).
Psychology, 91, 93–114. https://doi.org/10.1080 Psychological and educational intervention to
/00223980.1975.9915803 improve tuberculosis treatment adherence in
Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological Ethiopia based on health belief model: A cluster
processes in fear appeals and attitude change: randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE, 11,
A revised theory of protection motivation. In e0155147. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal
J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.). Social .pone.0155147.
Psychophysiology: A Source Book (pp. 153– Weinstein, N. D., & Nicolich, M. (1993). Correct and
176). incorrect interpretations of correlations between
New York: Guilford Press. risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Health
Rosenstock, I. M. (1966). Why people use health Psychology, 12, 235–245.
services. Millbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, 44, https://doi.org/10.1037
94–124. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0009 //0278-6133.12.3.235
.2005.00425.x Whitaker, K. L., Good, A., Miles, A., Robb, K., &
Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the Wardle, J. (2011). Socioeconomic inequalities in
health belief model. Health Education colorectal cancer screening uptake: Does time
Monographs, 2, 328–335. perspective play a role? Health Psychology, 30,
https://doi.org/10.1177 702–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023941
/109019817400200403 White, B. X., & Albarracín, D. (2018).
Salloway, J. C., Pletcher, W. R., & Collins, J. J. Investigating belief falsehood. Fear appeals do
(1978). Sociological and social psychological change behaviour in experimental laboratory
models of compliance with prescribed regimen: studies. A commentary on Kok et al. (2018).
In search of a synthesis. Sociological Health
Symposium, 23, 100–121. Psychology Review, 12, 147–150. https://doi.org
Sheeran, P., Harris, E., & Epton, T. (2014). Does /10.1080/17437199.2018.1448292
heightening risk appraisals change people’s Witte, K., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of
intentions and behaviour? A meta-analytic fear appeals: Implications for effective public

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004
Changing Behavior Using the Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory 62

health campaigns. Health Education and


Behavior, 27,
591–615. https://doi.org/10.1177/10901981000
2700506
Zahid, H. (2019). Cultural influences on
representation, coping and prevention of illness.
Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Essex.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Durham University Library, on 27 Sep 2020 at 05:19:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available
at https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108677318.004

You might also like