Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

The Intent and Extent of Collective Threats: A Data-Driven


Conceptualization of Collective Threats and Their Relation to Political
Preferences
Dennis T. Kahn, Fredrik Björklund, and Gilad Hirschberger
Online First Publication, November 29, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000868

CITATION
Kahn, D. T., Björklund, F., & Hirschberger, G. (2021, November 29). The Intent and Extent of Collective Threats: A Data-
Driven Conceptualization of Collective Threats and Their Relation to Political Preferences. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000868
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
© 2021 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 0096-3445 https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000868

The Intent and Extent of Collective Threats: A Data-Driven


Conceptualization of Collective Threats and Their Relation to
Political Preferences

Dennis T. Kahn1, 2, Fredrik Björklund2, and Gilad Hirschberger1


1
Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, Reichman University
2
Department of Psychology, Lund University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The purpose of the present article is to systematically investigate how people perceive collective threat
and how such threat perceptions relate to political preferences. Existing threat taxonomies are mostly
derived from top-down analyses and little attempt has been made to examine bottom-up how people
spontaneously perceive threats. One area where this is of central importance is the relationship between
political preferences and threat perception. Prevailing theories in social psychology primarily study se-
curity and stability threats and conclude that conservatives are more sensitive to threats than liberals.
Other perspectives, however, have criticized this position and maintain that the relationship between
threat and political preferences depends on how both constructs are defined. To resolve this issue, we
carried out a systematic, data-driven investigation of how collective threats are perceived. In five prereg-
istered, data-driven studies, carried out on representative cross-cultural samples (aggregate N = 24,341),
we show that people tend to categorize collective threats along two dimensions—their intent (omission/
commission) and extent (local/global). We show that whereas liberals are more concerned than conser-
vatives by omission-based and global threats, conservatives are more concerned than liberals by com-
mission-based and local threats. These results suggest that collective threat is a multidimensional
construct and that political leftists and rightists do not necessarily differ in the extent to which they per-
ceive threats, but rather in the way they prioritize different threats facing society.
Keywords: collective threat, political preferences, data-driven research, multidimensional scaling, politi-
cal identity

Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000868.supp

The experience of threat is a primal and powerful motivator of threat only a concern for the political right, or do leftists and right-
human attitudes and behavior, but what exactly constitutes a ists differ only in what they consider to be threatening, and not in
threat? Although a seemingly simple concept, the term threat is the extent of feeling threatened? The influence of the experience
loaded with layers of meaning that are often not accounted for in of threat on human attitudes and behavior constitutes a fundamen-
the current literature (Feldman, 2013). Do individual threats differ tal question for psychological research, but the nature of the threat
from collective threats? Does threat perception always elicit fear- construct and its relation to political preferences remains unclear.
related motivations, or is the experience of threat more complex The purpose of the present article is to focus specifically on collec-
with multiple emotions at play that induce different motivations tive threats and systematically investigate how people perceive
and perceptions of justice or injustice (Lambert et al., 2019)? Is collective threat and how such threat perceptions relate to political
preferences.

Collective Threat
Dennis T. Kahn https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7194-8956
This research was supported in part by Fredrik Björklund’s grants from There are numerous theories of threat in social psychology, but
the Wenner-Gren Foundations in Stockholm (UPD2016-0077) and the few focus specifically on collective threat and even fewer make a
Crafoord Foundation (20190807) and the first authors grants from the Lars clear distinction between individual and collective threat. Thus,
Hierta foundation (FO2017-0278), the Helge Axs:son Johnson foundation making the case for collective threat as a unique construct is neither
(F18-0218) and the Magnus Bergvall foundation (2017-02270).
simple nor straightforward. Theories such as realistic conflict theory
All studies were preregistered, all analyses were preregistered unless
(Sherif et al., 1961), social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979,
otherwise noted, and all materials are accessible through the Open Science
Framework (https://osf.io/9cbvy/). 1986), system justification theory (Jost & Banaji, 1994), right-wing
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dennis T. authoritarianism (Altemeyer, 1998) and conservatism as motivated
Kahn, Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology, Reichman University, P.O. social cognition (Jost et al., 2003) are theories that focus primarily,
Box 167, Herzliya, 46150, Israel. Email: dennis.t.kahn@gmail.com but not exclusively, on collective threat without always defining

1
2 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

them as such. Other theories such as terror management theory (TMT; threats that other groups pose to worldviews, values, and beliefs;
e.g., Greenberg et al., 1997) or compensatory control theory (Kay and (d) the research that shows how different collective threats are
et al., 2008; Landau et al., 2015) focus primarily on individual level reliably associated with unique outcomes (Bai & Federico, 2020;
threat, but have used individual level threat (e.g., personal death) and Crawford, 2017; Hirschberger et al., 2016; Stephan & Stephan,
collective threat (e.g., 9/11) interchangeably (Landau et al., 2015) and 2000). We, thus, define collective threat as a state, event or phe-
do not make different predictions for these different levels of threat. nomenon that, if materialized, may have strong negative conse-
One of the first attempts to provide an integrative model of threat quences for the future of the group (broadly defined as any
is the meaning maintenance model (MMM; Proulx & Inzlicht, collective from the local group to humanity) and that elicits a
2012) that focuses on violations of meaning and their maintenance. motivation to take action to reduce said threat. Because politics is,
According to this model, most compensatory and defensive behav- to a large degree, an endeavor to address threats against society,
ior can be understood as an attempt to maintain or restore meaning. and because political ideologies provide different solutions to
It is not clear, however, that meaning is a broad enough umbrella dealing with collective threats, the study of the relationship
term to capture all compensatory behavior (Galinsky et al., 2012). between collective threat perception and political preferences is
In another effort to integrate the literature and define what threat is, potentially important both theoretically and practically.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Jonas and colleagues (2014) suggested that all threats entail an anx-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

iety-provoking discrepancy that is addressed with defensive behav- Collective Threat and Political Preferences
ior. This model is parsimonious and elegant, but it addresses only
the collective level when comparing individual and collective There is a long tradition in social psychology, from Adorno and
defenses. It does not, however, attempt to distinguish between indi- colleagues (1950/2019), through Altemeyer (1998) and Duckitt
vidual and collective threats. (2001), to Jost and colleagues (2003), of studying threats emanating
Whereas some models of threat attempt to integrate disparate from hostile intergroup relations. This research has concluded that
conceptualizations of threat into a unitary definition, others view political conservatives are more concerned about threats and display
threat as a multidimensional construct with different threats predict- a greater motivation to manage threats than do political liberals.
ing different outcomes. The sociofunctional threat-based approach Adorno and colleagues (1950/2019), in their seminal work The Au-
(Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005) and integrated threat theory (Stephan & thoritarian Personality, described political ideology as an expression
Stephan, 2000) distinguish between threats such as realistic threat, of basic personality needs. They explained how harsh and punitive
symbolic threat and safety threat. These different dimensions of parental strategies are later transformed into perceptions of out-
threat are mostly collective-level and are instrumental in revealing groups as hostile and threatening. Fear and the experience of threat
the distinctions between important social phenomena such as intol- are thought to be central in the development of a right-wing authori-
erance and prejudice and in delineating the unique prejudices of tarian worldview that “. . .see[s] the world as a dangerous place, as
left-wing and right-wing individuals (Crawford, 2014). society teeters on the brink of self-destruction from evil and vio-
There is only sparse literature that attempts to clearly differenti- lence” (Altemeyer, 1998, p. 87). This perception of the world as a
ate individual and collective levels of threat. In the aftermath of the dangerous and threatening place activates the motivational goal of
9/11 attacks, Huddy and Feldman (2011) suggested a distinction social control and security that is expressed in authoritarian and con-
between reactions to terrorism as an individual versus collective servative social attitudes and values (Duckitt, 2001). The ideology of
threat. They claim that Americans tended to overestimate their per- conservatism is, therefore, motivated by needs to manage uncertainty
sonal vulnerability to terrorism (Huddy et al., 2002), but these and threat, due to the promise of such ideologies to provide stability
assessments were independent of perceptions of threat to their and security (Jost et al., 2003, 2017). Consequently, conservatives
nation that were overall higher. The difference was not only in the (rightists) are considered more threat-sensitive than are liberals (left-
perceived magnitude of the threat, but in the behavioral consequen- ists) and display a greater motivation to reduce threat and uncertainty
ces of threat perception: Individual-level threats tended to promote (Altemeyer, 1998; Duckitt, 2001; Jost et al., 2003, 2008, 2017).1
self-protective behaviors, such as avoiding public places, whereas It is important to note that although authoritarianism is strongly
collective threat perceptions influenced policy preferences. predictive of conservative self-identification and social and politi-
In an effort to provide a theoretical framework to distinguish cal attitudes (Federico et al., 2017), authoritarianism is only one
between dimensions of threat, the multidimensional existential threat component of conservative ideology that may not always be pres-
model (MET; Hirschberger et al., 2016) differentiates between physi- ent (Wilson & Sibley, 2013). In the current research, we conceptu-
cal and symbolic threats at the individual and collective levels, with alize and measure political ideology on a scale ranging from very
different predictions for each level. According to this model, individ- liberal/leftist to very conservative/rightist. Using this scale to mea-
ual-level existential threats will lead to attempts at denial, as TMT sure political ideology enables comparison across a wide range of
predicts, because individual death cannot be averted. Groups, unlike political contexts (Inglehart & Klingemann, 1976; Jost et al.,
individuals, have the potential to exist indefinitely, and then collec- 2017).
tive level threat should induce a motivation to prevent or eliminate
the threat rather than deny it (Hirschberger & Ein-Dor, 2020). 1
We use the terms conservative/liberal and right/left interchangeably in
In the current research, our conceptualization of collective threat the current article. The terms conservative and liberal are primarily used in
relies on (a) the distinction between individual and collective the United States, whereas the terms right and left is used in other
threats offered by Huddy and Feldman (2011); (b) the MET model countries, notably in European countries, where the term liberal usually is
reserved for those who support a free-market economy and small
(Hirschberger et al., 2016); (c) the ideological conflict hypothesis government. We switch between these terminologies in order to stay true to
(Brandt et al., 2014; Crawford, 2014) that broadens the definition the terminology used in the theoretical perspective under discussion and
of collective threat to include not only physical threats but also the terms used in the political setting under discussion.
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 3

Criticism of the Conservatism–Threat Relationship terrorism, whereas the remaining studies focused mainly on xenopho-
bia and violent crime (Eadeh & Chang, 2020; Jost et al., 2017).
The conclusion that the need to manage uncertainty and threat Although terrorism and other threats against security and stability are
is associated exclusively with conservative ideology has been unquestionably important and pose a serious threat to society, the ab-
criticized in recent years, with several scholars pointing out that sence of threats against other societal phenomena (e.g., threats
the relationship between political preferences and perception of against social justice, threats against the environment, widespread vi-
threat depends on how threat and political preferences are defined ral contamination) precludes the possibility to account for a wider va-
and operationalized (e.g., Crawford, 2017; Eadeh & Chang, 2020; riety of phenomena in the description of collective threat. The
Elad-Strenger & Shahar, 2018; Hirschberger et al., 2016; Malka et decision to include only specific threats in a study is likely to greatly
al., 2014). For example, some research has shown that the relation- affect the results, and such decisions may be subject to theoretical or
ship between the need for security/certainty and conservatism ideological biases.
depends on whether political ideology is measured along a social From a theoretical perspective, perceptions of justice in particu-
or economic dimension (Malka et al., 2014; Federico & Malka, lar should be central to threat perceptions because the sense of
2018). This line of research further suggests that the different threat, especially when accompanied by feelings of anger, suggests
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dimensions of political ideology are weakly, and sometimes even


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that something is wrong with the world that needs to be rectified


negatively related (Malka, Lelkes, & Soto, 2017). The mixed evi- (Lambert et al., 2019). The sense of injustice and ensuing emo-
dence on the relationship between political ideology and physio- tions is a subjective perception that may vary as a function of po-
logical reactions to threat (Bakker et al., 2020; Hibbing et al.,
litical ideology. Thus, a terrorist attack may elicit moral outrage
2014; Oxley et al., 2008) casts further doubt on the simple rela-
and lead to greater support of hawkish policies as a form of retrib-
tionship that is proposed between conservatism and threat.
utive justice (Lambert et al., 2010). Threats to health care or to the
Research that conceptualizes threat as a multidimensional con-
environment, however, do not increase support for violence but
struct finds ideological differences in threat perception such that
rather motivate restorative justice motivations—greater support
right-wingers place greater emphasis on physical existential threats,
for health care and environmental policies (Eadeh & Chang,
whereas leftists place greater emphasis on symbolic existential threats
2020). The current research offers the opportunity to examine bot-
(Bai & Federico, 2020; Hirschberger et al., 2016). Along similar
tom-up whether justice considerations indeed guide people's clas-
lines, Crawford (2017) argued that whereas social conservatives are
sification of collective threats.
more influenced by physical threats, liberals and conservatives are
To move forward in the discussion on threat and resolve this im-
similarly influenced by meaning threats. He further shows that sym-
portant issue a rigorous, systematic and data-driven investigation
bolic threats, realistic threats, and safety threats are related to differ-
of the collective threat construct is needed to determine which
ent intergroup outcomes (Crawford, 2014). Indeed, the effect of
phenomena are perceived as collective threats, how these threats
threat on political preferences appears to be contingent on the type of
are organized and categorized, and whether the threat construct
threatening issue that is under study. According to the ideology-
has a unidimensional structure, as it is often operationalized in
affordance perspective on threat and political attitudes (Eadeh &
current research, or constitutes a multidimensional phenomenon
Chang, 2020; Lambert et al., 2019), different threats should enhance
comprising qualitatively different threats. To reduce risks and
the appeal of the political party that is perceived as better suited to
sampling biases (Fiedler, 2011), we propose that the best way to
address the specific threat. Research supports this position and indi-
approach this issue is to adopt a data-driven bottom-up approach
cates that threats to health care and the environment, for instance
that allows the “data to speak.” This enables the research to
(issues that liberals care about), produce increased support for com-
approach representative design (Brunswik, 1955, 1956), having an
ponents of liberal ideology (Eadeh & Chang, 2020).
unbiased and representative sample of stimuli.
Drawing from the scholarship on issue ownership (e.g., See-
berg, 2017), that suggests that issues that are important to political
parties are stable over time and across contexts, we expect to find The Present Research
cross-cultural similarities in the relationship between political ide- To examine how people spontaneously assess collective threats,
ology and threat perception. Namely, we posit that similarities it is important to ensure that the list of threats constitutes a repre-
among leftists and among rightist in threat perception are based on sentative sample of the phenomena people spontaneously perceive
similarities in the issues that political partisans deem important. as collective threats, and that the criteria on which collective
This issue/threat consistency is expected to hold cross-culturally. threats are assessed reflect the criteria that people spontaneously
use to assess threats. Previous research has used these guidelines
A Data-Driven Conceptualization of Collective Threat to clarify the content people spontaneously use to describe stereo-
types (Koch et al., 2016). Based on this research, the present
Most, if not all, would agree that political leftists and rightists alike
research examined four central questions:
are concerned about societal phenomena that they consider important
(e.g., Jost et al., 2017). Further, all political groups are strongly moti- 1. What phenomena do people perceive as collective
vated to reduce threats to issues that they care about. Collective threat threats?
is typically operationalized in current research as threat against secu-
rity and social stability. Indeed, a considerable part of the literature 2. How are these phenomena spontaneously categorized?
has focused its main attention on a single threat—terrorism (Eadeh &
Chang, 2020; Jost et al., 2017). In a recent meta-analysis on threat 3. What dimensions do people spontaneously use to com-
and political preferences, 45 out of 55 samples (82%) focused on pare collective threat?
4 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

4. In what way do the political left and right (or liberals and data quality control has been used in previous research (Imhoff
conservatives) differ in their concern about collective et al., 2020; Lamberty & Imhoff, 2019). In our preregistration of
threats? this study, we indicated that participants that did not recommend
using their responses would be excluded from the analyses. In this
The first step was to generate a list of phenomena that people per- study, no participants had to be excluded.
ceive as collective threats (Study 1). Following this, we examined
how people categorize collective threats, by asking them to assess Measures and Procedure
similarities between different threats, as well as to intuitively sort
Participants in the study were posed with the open-ended ques-
threats (Study 2). In Study 3, we examined how many dimensions
tion: “What are the three greatest threats against our society in
are needed to account for the perception of collective threat and
your view?” They were then asked to provide basic demographic
what the contents of these dimensions are. In Studies 3 through 5,
information. Political preferences were assessed on a scale ranging
we also examined the ways in which the political left and right dif-
from 1 (far left [U.K.]/very liberal [U.S.]) to 7 (far right [U.K.]/
fer in their concern about threat. Finally, in Study 5 we examined
very conservative [U.S.]).
whether the obtained patterns were universal, looking at representa-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tive samples from 22 countries. All studies are preregistered, all


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

analyses are preregistered unless otherwise noted, and all of the


Results
materials are accessible through the OSF (osf.io/9cbvy/). In accordance with the preregistration, we surveyed the list of
threats mentioned by the participants, combining threats that clearly
Study 1 referred to the same category. For example, terror, terrorism, terro-
rists, and terror attacks were combined into a single “terrorism”
The purpose of Study 1 was to examine what phenomena people
category. We then sorted the threats in each sample separately by
spontaneously perceive to be collective threats. A data-driven
prevalence, identifying the threats that, taken together, accounted
strategy was used, soliciting the phenomena that people mention
for two thirds (67%) of the threats mentioned in the sample. The
when asked what the greatest threats against society are. Partici-
remaining threats belonged to categories that each accounted for
pants were not limited by definitions, examples or instructions and
less than .7% of the mentioned threats. As per the preregistration,
were asked to provide spontaneous responses.2 The study was pre-
this cutoff point was chosen to create a list containing a clear ma-
registered on aspredicted.org (aspredicted.org/wn6fp.pdf).
jority of the mentioned threats, while still ensuring that the list did
not include rarely mentioned threats. We then proceeded to identify
Method
threats that were among the most commonly mentioned in both
Participants samples. We attempted to use the exact terms suggested by the par-
ticipants, to the extent possible. A number of adjustments were
Data were collected from two samples—one from the United made, however, for categories that referred to the same underlying
Kingdom and one from the United States. Each sample was repre- threat, but for which somewhat different terms were used in the
sentative of the general population in the ages 20 to 69 in terms of United Kingdom and the United States (see online supplemental
age and gender. Quotas for age and gender were based on the 2011 material for specific adjustments to threat categories). The final list
U.K. census (Office for National Statistics, 2011) and the 2017 U.S. of threats commonly mentioned in both samples, in order of preva-
Census (U.S. Census Bureau, 2017). The samples were taken from lence across the two samples, was the following:
the online Prolific platform (www.prolific.ac), and age and gender
quotas were based on Prolific’s prescreening.3 The U.K. sample 1. Terrorism
consisted of 359 participants (50% female, Mage = 43.33) and the
U.S. sample consisted of 309 participants (50% female, Mage = 2. Climate change
43.15). The U.K. sample was predominantly White/Caucasian
(93%), with 2% identifying as Black, 1% as East Asian, 2% as 3. War
South Asian, and 2% as as other. In the U.S. sample, 80% identi-
4. Poverty
fied as White/Caucasian, 6% as Black/African American, 3% as
East Asian, 3% as Latino/Hispanic, 1% as South Asian, and 7% as 5. Inequality
other. After completing the study, participants were given a quality
assurance question. The question read as follows: “For us as 6. Immigration
researchers, it is very important to get reliable answers to our ques-
tions. It may happen that some of our participants lack the energy 7. Racism
or attention necessary to provide such reliable and accurate
responses. Would you recommend that we use your response in our 8. Environmental damage
analyses of the results? Please note that your response to this ques-
2
tion does not in any way affect your payment for participating in The preregistration for this study, as referred to in the author note, is
for the U.K. sample. We carried out an exact replication with a U.S. sample
the study.” The participants could choose between the options:
for which we did not create a separate preregistration.
“Yes, I recommend that you use my responses in the analyses of 3
All of the research presented here with the exception of Study 5 was
the results”, “No, I do not recommend that you use my responses in collected on the Prolific platform. Research shows that Prolific produces
the analyses of the results” and “Other:______.” This method of higher quality data than other online data services (Palan & Schitter, 2018).
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 5

9. Crime well as a sorting task. The study was preregistered on aspredicted


.org (aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=bw7ng4f).
10. Nuclear weapons
Method
11. Government
Participants
12. Disease
A sample of 502 U.K. participants was collected through the
13. Pollution Prolific platform. As per the preregistration, Prolific's prescreening
questionnaire was used to create quotas for gender (50/50) and po-
14. Religion litical preferences (one third leftists, one third centrists, one third
rightists). This sample was predominantly white (93%), Black 1%,
15. Economic collapse
South Asian 1%, less than 1% of the sample identified as East
16. Extremism Asian or Latino/Hispanic, and 5% identified as other. As in Study
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1 and as indicated in the preregistration, a quality assurance ques-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

17. Overpopulation tion was posed to the participants, asking them if they recommend
using their responses (Imhoff et al., 2020). Eleven participants rec-
18. Greed ommended not to use their responses (2%, which is below com-
mon exclusion standards in online studies; Klein et al., 2014),
making the final sample size 491 (50% female, M age = 38.60).
Discussion
Measures
The results of Study 1 provided us with a list of phenomena that
participants perceived as collective threats. Doing so was an impor- Similarity Estimation. The 18 threats generated in Study 1
tant first step, because no systematic bottom-up examination of this were used as stimulus material in the study. The participants were
issue has been carried out in the past. We thereby approach the idea presented with a pair of threats and were asked “To what degree
of representative design (Brunswik, 1955, 1956), ensuring that the do you think these two threats belong to different or similar types
corpus of stimuli material represents what people spontaneously of threat?” Participants indicated their responses on a scale ranging
intend when thinking of the construct in question. It is notable that from 1 (very different types of threat) to 7 (very similar types of
from an ideology-affordance perspective (Lambert et al., 2019), threat). All possible combinations of the 18 threats were used,
some of the threats on this list represent issues that are perceived to resulting in 153 threat pairs. To limit the length of the question-
be “owned” by the political right, such as terrorism and crime (See- naire and reduce fatigue from answering a large number of similar
berg, 2017). These threats are often associated with greater retribu- questions, each participant was randomly shown half of the threat
tive justice motivations (e.g., counterterrorism measures). Other pairs (77 threat pairs). The order in which the threat pairs were
threats on the list, such as inequality and pollution, are thought to presented, as well as the internal order between the two threats,
be “owned” by the political left (Saad, 2007) and are associated were counterbalanced.
with restorative justice motivations (Eadeh & Chang, 2020). In the Sorting Task. Following the similarity estimation task, partic-
next studies, we determine whether participants indeed use justice- ipants were given a sorting task, in which they were shown a list
related criteria to organize collective threats. of all 18 threats and were asked to sort the different threats into
boxes in any way they liked, by dragging and dropping the threats
Study 2 into a certain box. Eighteen boxes were shown on the screen, in
case the participants wanted to place each threat in a separate box.
Study 1 addressed the first research question: what phenomena A threat could not be placed in more than one box. The instruc-
do people perceive as collective threats? In Study 2, we moved tions to the participants were “Please sort the threats in the list
forward with examining the second research question—how do below. You can divide them in any way you like, including putting
people categorize threats? The two most commonly used methods all of them in the same box, putting each of them in separate boxes
for examining how people classify and categorize objects are simi- or any number of boxes in between. It is possible to leave some of
larity estimations (e.g., Forgas, 1976; Koch, et al., 2016; Rosen- the boxes empty. The order of the different items within the box
berg et al., 1968) and object sorting (e.g., Canter et al., 1985; does not matter, nor does the order of the different boxes.” A
Ward, 1977; Ward & Russell, 1981). The advantage of both of screen shot of the sorting task is included in the online supplemen-
these approaches is that they allow each individual to base his or tal material. We also added a comprehension check that is avail-
her judgment on any dimensions that he or she chooses. These able in the online supplemental materials. Political preferences
techniques are therefore particularly suited for the investigation of were assessed on a scale from 1 (far left) to 7 (far right).
how people spontaneously categorize objects without precondi-
tions or theoretical assumptions. Similarity estimation entails esti- Results
mating how similar pairs of objects are, while object sorting
involves sorting a full set of objects into groups (see online supple- We analyzed the similarity estimations as well as the sorting
mental material for advantages and limitations of each method). task, using two different dimension reduction techniques, multidi-
To offset the advantages and disadvantages of the two methodolo- mensional scaling (MDS; Borg & Groenen, 2005) and principal
gies, participants in Study 2 carried out similarity estimations as component analysis (PCA; Jolliffe, 2002). The use of two different
6 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

dimension reduction techniques allowed us to triangulate and exam- each dimension solution, there are content cells that correspond to
ine where the results from the different approaches converge. For threats on the high and low poles of each dimension. For example,
the purpose of the MDS and PCA, we created full matrices for each the threats that had low values on Dimension 1 in the two-dimension
of the threat pairs, resulting in 18 3 18 matrices. For the purpose of solution were terrrorism, racism, extremism, and war, whereas the
the similarity estimates, the lower part of the matrix contained the threats with high values on Dimension 1 in the two-dimension solu-
mean similarity rating when the threat in the row was presented first tion were climate change, pollution, environmental damage, overpop-
and the upper part of the matrix the similarity rating when the threat ulation, and disease.
in the column was presented first. For the sorting task, each cell of The results from the MDS and PCA analyses are presented in Ta-
the matrix contained the number of instances in which the different ble 2. The analysis indicated that one dimension was insufficient to
threats appeared in the same box. The upper and lower parts of the account for the results, both in terms of stress levels (..2 for the
matrix were thus identical for the sorting task. similarity estimates as well as for the sorting task) and in terms of
We then proceeded to run separate MDS analyses on the similar- explained variance (24% and 30% variance explained for similarity
ity estimates and the sorting data, using the PROXSCAL procedure assessments and the sorting task, respectively). The two- three- and
(Busing et al., 1997), treating the scale as ordinal, that is, not lim- four-dimensional solutions all had satisfactory stress levels (#.15
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ited by the assumption that all scale steps are of equal magnitude. for both measures) but provided a relatively limited amount of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Similarly, we carried out PCAs for the similarity estimates and the explained variance (between 39% and 60% variance explained; see
sorting data, using oblimin rotation, allowing for the scales not to Table 2). The results thus indicated that threat is a multidimensional
be orthogonal (Osborne et al., 2008). rather than unidimensional construct, but did not allow us to deter-
These analyses were carried out for one, two, three, and four mine whether two, three, or four dimensions are needed to accu-
dimensions to determine how many dimensions were sufficient to rately account for how people perceive threats.
account for the data. We used a number of criteria to reach this de-
cision. In MDS, the main criterion used is Kruskal’s S, or stress Discussion
(Kruskal, 1964), a measure of badness-of-fit. The stress value
should be as low as possible, with a cutoff point of ..2 usually The results from Study 2 provide the first data-driven examina-
indicating an unsatisfactory fit to the data (Kruskal & Wish, 1978). tion of how people categorize collective threats. These results are
A value of #.15 can be considered satisfactory and values #.10 as consistent with the notion that threat is a multidimensional, rather
good (Kruskal & Wish, 1978). The use of PCA further gave us an than unidimensional construct. We were, however, not able to adju-
indication of how much variance is explained by the component so- dicate with any certainty between the two-, three- and four-dimen-
lution. There are no firmly established cutoff points for explained sion solutions, nor were we able to describe the nature of the
variance, but a common requirement is that the component solution dimensions in question. For these purposes, we carried out Study 3.
can explain $60% of the variance (Hair et al., 2014).
Two other criteria are parsimony and interpretability. With regards Study 3
to parsimony, dimension solutions containing fewer dimensions were
We had, thus, generated a list of phenomena perceived as collective
preferred over solutions containing a larger number of dimensions.
For interpretability, we compared the different dimension solutions threats (Study 1) and examined how these threats are categorized (Study
2). The first preliminary step in Study 3 was to select content criteria
for consistency across the different data collection methods (similarity
that could be used to interpret the two-, three- and four-dimensional
estimates and sorting) and the different analysis strategies (MDS and
spaces found in Study 2 and generate a list of content dimensions on
PCA). Consistency was examined by identifying the extreme values
on each of the dimensions (upper and lower third of coordinates/com- which these threats are perceived to differ. On this basis, Study 3 sought
ponent loadings) and comparing across the four different solutions to (a) to disambiguate and interpret the dimensions; (b) adjudicate
(MDS for similarity estimates, PCA for similarity estimates, MDS for between the two-, three- and four-dimensional solutions; and (c) exam-
sorting, PCA for sorting). If a certain threat belonged to the upper ine the relationship between the dimensions of threat extracted from
third of the same dimension across at least three out of the four differ- this study and political identity. This study, therefore, allowed us to
ent solutions, it was said to belong to the upper third of said dimen- determine the number and nature of the dimensions that are needed to
sion (and vice versa for threats belonging to the lower third). For a account for the perception of collective threat. The study was preregis-
dimension solution to be consistent, we expected at least three threats tered on aspredicted.org (aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=ym2ku9).
to consistently belong to the same pole of a certain dimension across
the different analyses. The one-, two- and three-dimensional solutions Preliminary Step
satisfied our criteria of consistency in the sense that all dimensions Using an independent sample of 60 U.K. Prolific participants,
had at least three threats that belonged to the same pole of a certain we generated a list of 18 criteria that could be used to disambigu-
dimension. In the four-dimensional solution, Dimensions 3 and 4 only ate and interpret the different dimensions found in Study 2. This
had two threats consistently associated with the lower pole, making procedure enabled us to establish criteria that reflect what people
the four-dimension solution less interpretable. The full list of threats spontaneously use to categorize and describe collective threats (for
associated with the high and low poles of the different dimensions in full details of this procedure see the online supplemental material).
the two-, three- and four-dimension solutions can be found in Table 1. The resulting list contained the following themes:
Table 1 presents the dimension solution (i.e., the number of
the dimensions of threat) and the content of each dimension, summa- 1. Local/global consequences
rizing the specific threats associated with the extreme poles of each
dimension in the two-, three- and four-dimension solutions. Thus, for 2. Avoidability
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 7

Table 1
List of Threats Associated With the High and Low Poles of the Different Dimensions in the Two-, Three- and Four-Dimension Solutions
Solution
Dimension 1 3 4
1 Low
Terrorism Racism Extremism
Racism Religion Racism
Extremism Extremism Religion
Religion Terrorism Terrorism
War War War
High
Climate change Pollution Pollution
Pollution Overpopulation Environmental damage
Environmental damage Disease Climate change
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Overpopulation Environmental damage Disease


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Disease Climate change Overpopulation


Economic collapse Economic collapse
2 Low
Immigration Inequality Crime
Inequality Immigration Economic collapse
Poverty Poverty Immigration
Greed Crime Inequality
Economic collapse
High
Extremism War Environmental damage
Climate change Environmental damage Pollution
Environmental damage Pollution War
War
Terrorism
3 Low
Immigration Pollution
Religion Religion
Pollution
High
Economic collapse Bad government
Bad government Economic collapse
Greed Greed
4 Low
Economic collapse
Immigration
Poverty
High
Crime
Religion

3. Intentional/unintentional 12. Collective-level/individual-level consequences

4. Related to economy 13. Consequence of regular/irregular activity

5. Motivated by emotion 14. Malicious intent

6. Direct/indirect 15. Physical/symbolic

16. Clear perpetrator


7. Interhuman cruelty
17. Related to immigration
8. Targets environment
18. Long-term/short-term consequences
9. Affects some/affects all
It is notable that although many of these criteria are related to
10. Motivated by ideology the concept of justice (e.g., intentional, interhuman cruelty, mali-
cious intent), justice concerns were not explicitly cited as criteria
11. Related to power for categorizing collective threats. It seems, therefore, that if
8 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

Table 2 influence of the general concern about the threat from the assess-
Kruskal’s Stress and Explained Variance for the One-, Two-, ment of the threat on the different criteria. Each participant was
Three- and Four-Dimension Solutions asked to assess each of the 18 threats on seven randomly selected
criteria out of the 19 requiring each participant to make 126 sepa-
Analysis and method Kruskal’s S Explained variance
rate assessments. In addition, the order in which the 18 threats
One-dimension solution were presented was randomized. All participants assessed the
MDS similarity .31 threats on the three valence criteria. An example of the instruction
MDS sorting .22
PCA similarity 24% for the local/global criteria was as follows:
PCA sorting 30%
Two-dimension solution Threats can vary in the degree to which the consequences are felt on a
MDS similarity .15 global scale or on a local scale. For certain threats, the consequences
MDS sorting .13 would mainly be felt locally if the threat were to materialize, whereas
PCA similarity 39% for others, the consequences would be felt globally if the threat were
PCA sorting 46%
to materialize. Please rate the following 18 threats according to how
Three-dimension solution
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

MDS similarity .10 local or global the consequences would be.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

MDS sorting .07


PCA similarity 49% For each threat, participants were asked to assess the threat on the
PCA sorting 53% relevant criterion with a slider on a scale from 0–100, anchored at both
Four-dimension solution
MDS similarity .07 sides with short descriptions of the low and high poles of the criterion
MDS sorting .06 (e.g., local consequences/global consequences). Political preferences
PCA similarity 57% were assessed on a scale ranging from 1 (far left) to 7 (far right).
PCA sorting 60%
Note. MDS = multidimensional scaling; PCA = principal component Results
analysis.
Mean ratings of all threats on each of the 22 criteria (19 threat
criteria plus three valence criteria) were calculated and entered
justice concerns guide people's classification of collective threats,
into an 18 3 22 matrix. This matrix was used as input for a paral-
this does not occur at the explicit level.
lel analysis (Ledesma & Valero-Mora, 2007), using O’Connor’s
(2000) matrix. The parallel analysis indicated the existence of two
Method dimensions. We thus ran a PCA, limiting the number of compo-
Participants nents to two, using oblimin rotation. The first two components
accounted for 74% of the variance. The component solution indi-
We carried out power analyses using G*Power (Faul et al., cated that 11 out of the 22 criteria loaded ..4 on the first compo-
2009) to determine sample size for Study 3. According to a previ- nent and ,.4 on the second component, while 10 out of the 22
ous meta-analysis (Jost et al., 2017), the effect size of the relation- criteria loaded ..4 on the second component and ,.4 on the first
ship between political preferences and threat perceptions is small component. The only criterion that did not load ..4 on either com-
to medium (rs = .12–.31). The power analysis showed that a sam- ponent was “related to economy.” The full component solution
ple size between 79 (for r = .31) and 542 (for r = .12) was suffi- from the PCA can be found in Table 3.
cient to achieve a power of .80. Based on these analyses, we We then ran these analyses on each of the political groups sepa-
settled on a sample size of 300, which would be enough to detect rately to examine whether leftists and rightists perceive threats differ-
an effect size of .16 with a power of .80. Participants were ently or only show different levels of concern about threats (see online
recruited from the Prolific platform, creating quotas for gender, supplementary materials for full details of these analyses). The analy-
age and political preference among 300 U.K. participants who did ses indicated that threat perception was highly similar for leftists and
not take part in any of the earlier studies (91% White/Caucasian, rightists (i.e., both rightists and leftists agreed that climate change is a
2% Black, 1% East Asian, 2% South Asian, 4% other). In accord- global omission threat and that crime is a local commission threat).
ance with the preregistration, 10 participants who recommended The 16 criteria resulting from this analysis were considered the final
not to use their responses for the data analysis were excluded from list of criteria associated with the two dimensions. The 6 threats that
further statistical analyses, making the final sample size 290 (49% had the highest score on the first component were terrorism, war, ex-
female, M age = 42.96). tremism, religious fundamentalism, racism and nuclear weapons,
whereas the threats with the lowest scores on this component were dis-
Measures
ease, overpopulation, climate change, poverty, pollution and economic
The 18 threats generated in Study 1 and the 18 criteria generated instability. The threats with the highest score on the second component
in the preliminary analysis were used as stimuli in Study 3. We were climate change, nuclear weapons, environmental damage, pollu-
decided to divide the symbolic/physical criterion into two separate tion, war and overpopulation, whereas the threats with the lowest score
criteria, since the symbolic/physical division does not entail an on the second component were racism, immigration, inequality, crime,
unequivocal pair of opposites. We thus had a list of 19 criteria. In poverty, and religious fundamentalism.
addition, we also included three valence criteria (concern about The content description of the two dimensions was chosen based
threats, severity of threats, and importance of threats). The purpose on the criteria that loaded on the different dimensions in the PCA as
of including these criteria was to be able to tease apart the well as the specific threats associated with the high and low poles of
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 9

Table 3 intent (e.g., terrorism, war, extremism). The second dimension was
Component Solution from the Study 3 PCA termed local/global, with threats that are low on this dimension per-
ceived as local threats (e.g., racism, immigration, inequality),
Criteria Component 1 Component 2 whereas threats that are high on this dimension were perceived as
Malicious intent .98 global threats that affect all of humanity (e.g., climate change, nu-
Threats against symbolic values and .96 clear weapons, environmental damage). These dimensions could also
principles
Clear perpetrator .96
be said to capture the intent and extent of collective threat—the
Intentional .96 degree to which the threat is perceived as the result of intentional
Interhuman cruelty .94 action and the extent of the threat in question.
Ideologically motivated .92 We were further interested in comparing the coordinates of the dif-
Direct (active) .91
ferent threats on the two dimensions across Studies 2 and 3, including
Emotional motivation .91
Related to power .85 two different methods of measurement and analyzed using two differ-
Consequence of regular/irregular actions .76 ent dimension reduction techniques. For each threat, we calculated
Avoidable .48 standardized scores for the MDS and PCA of the similarity assessment
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Related to economy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and sorting task (Study 2) as well as the PCA of the criteria assessment
Affects all .91
Severe .87 (Study 3), resulting in five standardized scores for each threat on each
Global .87 of the two dimensions. The placement of the threats in the two-dimen-
Collective-level consequence .87 sional space was strikingly similar across the different studies, different
Targets environment .84 measurements, and different statistical approaches, as indicated by
Long-term consequences .84
Concerning .76 intercorrelations for placement of the threats on the same dimension
Threats against something tangible or .75 across the different scores between .43 and .98, with 14 out of 20 cor-
physical relations having intercorrelations of ..70. To graph the different
Related to immigration .72 threats in a two-dimensional space, we calculated average placement
Important .65
on the two dimensions for the different threats in Studies 2 and 3.
Note. Component loadings of ,.4 are not displayed in the table. PCA = These composite placements on the different dimensions had high reli-
principal component analysis. ability—Dimension 1 a = .98, Dimension 2 a = .90—again attesting
to the high degree of consistency regardless of measurement and statis-
the different dimensions. We term the first component omission/com- tical approach.
mission with threats low on this dimension perceived as the result of Figure 1 shows the placement of each of the threats in the 2-
indirect, unintentional passivity without malicious intent (e.g., dis- dimensional space, based on this composite placement. Property-fit-
ease, overpopulation, climate change), whereas threats high on this ting analyses was further carried out to compare the suitability of
dimension were perceived as direct, intentional action with malicious the two dimensions for interpreting the threats’ one-dimensional,

Figure 1
Placement of Collective Threats on the Intent and Extent Dimensions Based on Average Placement
on the Composite, Combining Results From Studies 2 and 3

High
War
Nuclear weapons
Environmental damage
Climate change
Pollution
Terrorism

Extremism

Extent
Bad government Religious
Overpopulation
fundamentalism
Racism
Economic collapse
Disease
Greed Crime
Poverty

Immigration
Low Inequality

Low High

Intent
10 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

two-dimensional, three-dimensional, and four-dimensional similar- Table 4


ity space. Details from these analyses can be found in the online Regression Coefficients (b Values) for the Relationship Between
supplemental material (please note that this research was conducted Political Preference and Concern About Threat, Controlling for
before the COVID-19 pandemic. It is likely that today disease Age and Gender, Organized by Rating on the Omission/
would be rated higher in extent). Commission and Local/Global Dimensions
To examine the relationship between political preferences
and concern about threats, we calculated a composite score for Omission/ Political Preference 3
commission Threat Concern
overall concern about threat that includes the three valance crite- Threat M rating b [95% CI]
ria: concern about threats, severity of threat, and importance of
Disease 24.91 .01** [ .12, .10]
threat for each of the 18 threats (reliability scores ranged from a = Overpopulation 32.14 .17** [ .05, .28]
.78 to a = .91 for the different threats). The higher the score on Climate change 38.46 .24** [ .36, .13]
this threat composite, the more the participant was concerned Pollution 41.66 .23** [ .34, .12]
about the threat in question. We carried out linear regressions Poverty 42.98 .34** [ .45, .23]
Environmental damage 43.36 .25** [ .36, .13]
between political preferences and concern about each of the 18
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Economic collapse 44.17 .03** [ .15, .08]


threats separately, controlling for age and gender. We sorted the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Immigration 44.69 .43** [ .32, .53]


threats according to their mean rating on the omission/commission Inequality 56.05 .47** [ .57, .36]
and local/global dimensions and present the regression coefficients Bad government 57.47 .22** [ .33, .11]
Greed 67.01 .28** [ .39, .17]
among political preferences and the different threats (see Table 4).
Crime 67.16 .14* [ .03, .25]
Looking at the omission/commission dimension, concern about Nuclear weapons 71.55 .09 [ .20, .02]
three out of the five threats (climate change, pollution, poverty) Racism 74.50 .29** [ .40, .19]
associated with the omission pole were significantly related to Religious fundamentalism 75.85 .15* * [ .04, .26]
left-wing political preferences, whereas concern about three out of War 79.89 .06 [ .17, .05]
Extremism 80.07 .10† [ .02, .21]
the five threats (terrorism, extremism, religious fundamentalism) Terrorism 83.99 .21** [ .10, .32]
associated with the commission pole were significantly related to Immigration 42.33 .43** [ .32, .53]
right-wing political preferences. There were, however, exceptions Racism 44.11 .29** [ .40, .19]
to this pattern. Concern about overpopulation, a threat rated as Inequality 46.50 .47** [ .57, .36]
Crime 47.01 .14* [ .03, .25]
omission-based, was associated with right-wing political preferen-
Religious fundamentalism 48.09 .15* *[ .04, .26]
ces and concern about racism, a threat rated as commission-based, Poverty 50.07 .34** [ .45, .23]
was associated with left-wing political preferences. Turning our Greed 50.74 .28** [ .39, .17]
attention to the local/global dimension, concern about three of Extremism 52.68 .10† [ .02, .21]
the five threats (immigration, crime, religious fundamentalism) Disease 57.05 .01** [ .12, .10]
Bad government 58.14 .22** [ .33, .11]
associated with the local pole were significantly related to right- Terrorism 59.23 .21** [ .10, .32]
wing political preferences, whereas concern about three out of the Economic collapse 63.34 .03** [ .15, .08]
five threats (climate change, environmental damage, pollution) Overpopulation 66.74 .17** [ .05, .28]
associated with the global pole were significantly related to left- War 71.14 .06 [ .17, .05]
Nuclear weapons 78.62 .09 [ .20, .02]
wing political preferences. There were, however, exceptions to
Pollution 78.89 .23** [ .34, .12]
this pattern as well. Concern about racism and inequality were Environmental damage 80.70 .25** [ .36, .13]
rated as local but were nonetheless associated with left-wing polit- Climate change 83.89 .24** [ .36, .13]
ical preferences.
Note. Political Preference 3 Threat Concern refers to the regression
To examine whether the observed pattern constituted a significant coefficient predicting the concern about threat composite using political
linear trend, we carried out multilevel modeling analyses, treating the preferences as the predictor, controlling for age and gender. CI = confi-
different threats as within-subjects clusters of analysis. These analy- dence interval.
ses were not preregistered. Using a multilevel modeling strategy *p , .05. ** p , .001.
allowed us to obtain an overall view of the relationship between
political preferences and concern about threat, taking into account which the threat was perceived as global (b = .40, SE = .03, t =
how each of these threats was rated on the two dimensions. In 14.30, p , .001, LLCI =.35, ULCI = .46) were related to concern
carrying out the MLM-analyses, we followed Heck et al.’s (2013) about threats. Importantly, we were interested in whether the rela-
recommendations. In the first step, we entered only type of threat as tionship between political preferences and concern about threat
a within-subjects predictor and concern about threat as an outcome depended on the degree to which the threat was omission/commis-
variable. This analysis showed that the level of concern differed
sion-based and/or local/global. In order to examine this, in the
depending on the type of threat, t(5003) = 183.04, p , .001, justify-
third and fourth steps, we entered interaction terms between politi-
ing the use of a multilevel model. In the second step, we entered indi-
vidual-level (right-wing) political preferences, as well as group-level cal preference and omission/commission (third step) and political
rating on the omission/commission and local/global dimensions. Age preference and local/global (fourth step). The analyses revealed
and gender were also included as controls. The analysis showed that significant interactions between political preferences and omis-
left-wing political preferences (b = –1.54, SE = .24, t = –6.44, p , sion/commission (b = .03, SE = .01, t = 2.53, p = .011, LLCI =.01,
.001, LLCI = –2.01, ULCI = –1.07), overall degree to which the ULCI = .06) as well as between political preferences and local/
threat was perceived as commission-based (b = .16, SE = .02, t = global (b = –.06, SE = .02, t = –3.15, p = .002, LLCI = –.09,
7.70, p , .001, LLCI =.12, ULCI = .20), and overall degree to ULCI = –.02). The more commission-based and global the
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 11

threat, the stronger the relationship between right-wing political Study 4


preferences and concern about the threat. Conversely, the more
omission-based and global the threat, the stronger the relationship The main purpose of Study 4 was to examine ideological differ-
between left-wing political preferences and concern about the ences in the allocation of resources to issues identified as threats
and nonthreat-related societal issues. These issues were further
threat.
rated on the local/global and omission/commission dimensions to
examine whether conservatives and liberals would differ in their
Discussion
resource allocation to threats varying on different dimensions.
Study 3 showed that people perceive threats along two main Using a resource allocation task enabled us to test our hypotheses
dimensions—omission/commission and local/global—in other using a different method and a different list of threats. The study
words, the intent and extent of collective threat. The omission/ was preregistered on aspredicted.org (aspredicted.org/blind.php
commission dimension refers to the degree to which the threat in ?x=tz4y8i).
question is the result of indirect, unintentional passivity without
malicious intent (omission), or direct, intentional action with mali- Method
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cious intent (commission). The local/global dimension refers to the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

degree to which the threat is a local phenomenon affecting parts of Participants


the population (local), or a global phenomenon affecting the entire The sample consisted of 401 U.S. participants, representative of
population (global). This finding constitutes an important theoreti- the U.S. population in terms of age, gender and political preferen-
cal advancement in the study of threat perception as it reveals ces. Participants were recruited from the Prolific platform. In ac-
dimensions of threat that were previously unaccounted for using a cordance with the preregistration, seven participants (1.75%) who
data-driven and unconditional examination of the perception of recommended not to use their responses for the data analysis were
collective threats. Moreover, although participants did not explic- excluded from further statistical analyses. We also identified six
itly use justice concerns to classify threats, Figure 1 suggests that instances of participants with identical Prolific ID’s, providing
justice-related perceptions may have implicitly affected threat per- clear indication that the same individual completed the question-
ceptions. Specifically, threats on the left-side of Figure 1 tend to naire twice. Although not preregistered, we excluded the second
reflect social justice violations (poverty, environmental damage) of these sets of responses for each such individual. Including the
that require restorative justice actions. Threats on the right side of duplicate participants does not affect the results or levels of signifi-
the figure reflect threats to the physical integrity of the collective cance in any meaningful way. The final sample size was thus 388
(war, terrorism) that often generate retributive justice motivations (49% female, M age = 43.27). Most participants identified as
(Lambert et al., 2010). White/Caucasian (79%), with 5% identifying as Black, 2% as East
The results from the study further show that the clarification of Asian, 6% as Latino/Hispanic, and 8% as other.
the dimensions along which threats are perceived and categorized
helps to clarify the relationship between political preferences and Measures and Procedure
concern about threat. First, when considering a wide range of
Resource Allocation Task. We were interested in contrasting
threats generated by participants themselves and thus relatively
allocation of resources to deal with threat-related issues with allo-
impervious to researcher bias in item selection, leftists were more
cation of resources to deal with nonthreat-related issues. We also
concerned by threats than rightists. This is an important result,
examined allocation of resources to threats that differ on the omis-
since it runs counter to predictions made by many theories in
sion/commission and local/global dimensions. Participants were
the field (Altemeyer, 1998; Duckitt, 2001; Jost et al., 2003, 2008,
given the following instructions:
2017).
We do not however draw the conclusion that leftists are gener-
Below you will find a list of issues. Your task is to indicate how much
ally more concerned about threats than rightists. Rather, the con- resources (money, time, labor. etc.) you think should be devoted to deal-
clusion that we believe to be the most appropriate is that the ing with each of these issues. Please indicate in percentages how much
relationship between political preference and concern about threat resources you think should be devoted to each of these issues in the box
depends on the threat under consideration. More specifically, the next to the text describing the issue. The total percentage should amount
more the threat was perceived as commission-based and local, the to 100%. You can divide the resources any way you like. You can fill
more concern about the threat was associated with right-wing po- out all the boxes or leave some of the boxes empty, as you like.
litical preferences. The more omission-based and global the threat,
the more concern about the threat was associated with left-wing They were then presented with the list of issues, next to each of
political preferences. Thus, political leftists and rightists do not which was a box in which they could enter a number. To aid the
necessarily differ in their overall concern about threats, but in the participants, a box appeared at the bottom indicating the sum of
specific threats that they are concerned about. It is however the values they had entered so far. The participants were able to
important to note that despite these clear overall patterns in the revise and change the percentages entered in each box as many
data, there were notable exceptions. For example, concern about times as they wished until the sum of values reached 100%.
racism, a threat rated as local as well as commission-based, Creating a New List of Threats. In Study 1, we asked partici-
was nonetheless associated with left-wing political preferences, pants to list threats that they consider the greatest threats to society. It
whereas concern about overpopulation, a threat rated as omission- is possible that the threats people think of as the greatest threats to so-
based by our participants, was associated with right-wing political ciety are not the threats that people think about often. In that case,
preferences. our study of collective threats may be limited to threats that are
12 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

severe but rare and exert little effect on everyday life. To address this threats according to their mean rating on the omission/commission
limitation, we used a data-driven strategy again to solicit the collec- and local/global dimensions and present the regression coefficients
tive threats that people think about often. Using an independent rep- between political preferences and the different threats and non-
resentative U.S. sample (N = 300) on the Prolific platform, a new list threat-related issues (see Table 5). Conservative political preferen-
of threats was generated that was near identical to the list generated ces were in this study associated with allocation of resources to
in Study 1. This new list of threats showed the same pattern of rela- threats over nonthreat-related issues. This relationship, however,
tionships with political ideology as found in Study 3. For full details depended heavily on the type of threat. Whereas conservatives
of this analytic step, see Study 1 in the online supplemental material. allocated more resources to commission threats (violent protests,
We used this list of threats in the current study. crime, and terrorism), liberals allocated more resources to omis-
Assessing Intent and Extent of Threats sion threats (COVID-19 and poverty). Conservatives further allo-
cated more resources to local threats (i.e., violent protests and
To assess the threats on the omission/commission and local/ crime), and although liberals generally allocated more resources
global dimensions, participants in this study were given a general to global threats (i.e., the COVID-19 pandemic, poverty), terror-
definition of the different dimensions and were then asked to rate
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ism—which in the study was perceived as a global threat—was


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

each of the threats on the dimensions. For the local/global dimen- associated with conservative political preferences (see Table 5).
sion, participants were told: As in Study 3, we carried out multilevel modeling analyses to
assess whether the relationship between political preference and
Threats can to a different degree be local and have short-term conse-
allocation of resources to address a threat differed depending on
quences affecting parts of the population, or global with long-term con-
sequences affecting the entire population. Please indicate whether the the intent and extent of the threat. The different threats were
following threats would be local with short-term consequences affecting treated as within-subjects clusters of analysis. These analyses were
part of the population or global with long-term consequences affecting not preregistered. In the first step, we entered only type of threat as
the entire population. They were then asked to rate the different threats a within-subjects predictor and percentage of resources devoted to
on a scale ranging from 1 (entirely local) to 5 (entirely global). dealing with the threat as an outcome variable. This analysis
showed that the resource distribution differed depending on the
For the omission/commission dimension, participants were type of threat, t(1,566) = 49.16, p , .001. In the second step, we
given the following instruction: entered individual-level (right-wing) political preferences, as well
as individual-level rating on the omission/commission and local/
Threats can, to a different degree, be the result of a person or a group
global dimensions. Age and gender were also included as controls.
doing something intentionally to hurt society. While some threats de-
velop on their own and are the result of failure to take action, other The analysis showed that right-wing political preferences (b = .77,
threats come about because someone or some group intentionally is SE = .10, t = 7.41, p , .001, LLCI =.56, ULCI = .970), rating of
taking action to hurt society. Please indicate whether the following the threat as omission-based (b = –.68, SE = .14, t = -4.88, p ,
threats would be the result of failure to take action, or intentional .001, LLCI =-.95, ULCI = –.40) and rating of the threat as global
action carried out by someone or some group trying to hurt society. (b = 1.05, SE = .16, t = 6.46, p , .001, LLCI =.73, ULCI = 1.37)
were related to distribution of resources to the threat. In the third
They were then asked to rate each of the threats on a scale rang- and fourth steps, we entered interaction terms between political
ing from 1 (entirely failure to take action) to 5 (entirely intentional preference and omission/commission (third step) and political
action). Finally, participants were asked to indicate their age, preference and local/global (fourth step). The analyses revealed
gender and political preferences on a scale ranging from 1 (very significant interactions between political preferences and omis-
liberal) to 7 (very conservative).
sion/commission (b = .22, SE = .08, t = 2.87, p = .004, LLCI =.07,
ULCI = .36) and between political preferences and local/global
Results (b = –.50, SE = .09, t = –5.53, p , .001, LLCI = –.67, ULCI =
Three of the threats (terrorism, crime, and violent protests/riots) –.32). The more commission-based and local the threat, the more
were clearly perceived as threats of commission, whereas two of allocation of resources to address the threat was associated with
the threats (poverty and the COVID-19 pandemic) were clearly right-wing political preferences; the more omission-based and
seen as threats of omission. Further, whereas three threats were per- global the threat, the more allocation of resources to address the
ceived as global (the COVID-19 pandemic, terrorism, and poverty), threat was associated with left-wing political preferences.
two threats were perceived as local (crime and violent protests; see
Table 5). Mean composites were calculated in accordance with this Discussion
division to capture local and global threats and omission and com-
mission threats. Mean composites based on the division generated The main purpose of Study 4 was to examine differences in
in the preliminary analyses (see the online supplemental material) resource allocation to threats among liberals and conservatives. The
were calculated to capture threats and nonthreat-related issues. results from the study showed that while conservatives allocated
We carried out linear regressions, controlling for age and gen- more resources to reduce threat, this pattern depended primarily on
der, to examine the relationship between political preferences and the type of threat. Whereas conservatives allocated resources to
relative allocation of resources to threat-related versus nonthreat- local and commission threats, liberals allocated resources to global
related issues, omission versus commission threats and local ver- and omission threats. These results corroborate the findings of
sus global threats as well as the specific issues. We sorted the Study 3 using a different method and an independent list of threats.
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 13

Table 5
b Coefficients for the Relationship Between Political Preference and Resource Allocation, Controlling for Age and Gender, Organized
by Rating on the Omission/Commission and Local/Global Dimensions
Omission/commission Local/global Political Preference 3 % Resource
Issue M % resource M rating M rating b [95% CI]
Threat 10.58 .20** [.10, .29]
Omission 14.44 2.23 .25** [ .35, .16]
Commission 8.00 3.58 .42** [.33, .51]
Local 8.37 2.74 .40** [ .31, .49]
Global 12.05 3.79 .14** [ .24, .04]
Poverty 11.34 2.17 3.54 .20** [ .30, .10]
COVID-19 17.55 2.30 4.07 .14** [ .24, .04]
Violent protests 6.80 3.20 2.55 .33** [.23, .42]
Crime 9.94 3.40 2.93 .32** [ .22, .41]
Terrorism 7.27 4.13 3.74 .26** [ .16, .36]
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Nonthreat 11.78 .20** [ .29, .10]


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Healthcare 17.40 .16** [ .26, .06]


LGBTQ rights 4.56 .43** [ .52, .34]
Education 14.72 .11* [ .20, .01]
Family and children 10.43 .15** [ .05, .25]
Note. Political Preference 3 % Resource represents the regression coefficient predicting the percentage of resources allocated to a certain threat using
political preferences as the predictor, controlling for age and gender. CI = confidence interval.
* p , .05. ** p , .001.

Study 5 you think that ____ is a major threat, a minor threat or not a threat to
(survey country)?” The threats that were listed included the follow-
Study 5 was carried out to examine on a global scale whether the ing: global climate change, the condition of the global economy, the
political left and right differ in their concern about threat and more Islamic militant group known as ISIS, cyberattacks from other coun-
specifically to examine whether the political left is concerned about tries, and large number of refugees leaving countries such as Iraq
omission-based global threats whereas the political right is con- and Syria. Participants were finally asked to what degree China,
cerned about commission-based local threats. We were further inter- the United States and Russia constituted threats, but the results
ested in examining whether these patterns were context-dependent regarding these country-based threats were not analyzed, since these
or are cross-culturally valid. Because Studies 1 through 4 were con- items referred to the origin of the threat rather than its’ nature.
ducted on predominantly White samples, Study 5 provided an op-
portunity to test our predictions on diverse samples worldwide. This Preliminary Analyses
study was based on an analysis of the data from Pew Research
To map the threats from the Pew Research Center’s Global
Center’s (2017) global attitudes and trends survey from 2017.
Trends and Attitudes Survey on the local/global and omission/
commission dimensions, we conducted a preliminary study. This
Method study also provided an opportunity to verify the two-dimensional
Participants model of collective threat and the relationship between political
preferences and threat, using a different list of threats, generated
The survey was carried out in 38 countries in large (N = 1000) by independent researchers. An independent representative sample
representative samples—altogether 41,953 participants. In 22 of of U.S. participants recruited on the Prolific platform (N = 303)
these countries (Argentina, Australia, Brazil, Canada, Chile, was used in this preliminary study (see Study 2 in online supple-
Colombia, France, Germany, Greece, Hungary, Israel, Italy, Mex- mentary materials). The results of this preliminary study provide
ico, Netherlands, Peru, Poland, South Korea, Spain, Sweden, the basis for our analyses in Study 5.
United Kingdom, United States, and Venezuela; aggregate N =
22,529), participants were asked to indicate their political prefer- Results
ences on a left–right scale.4 The participants from these 22 coun-
tries constituted the study’s sample. We carried out multilevel modeling analyses, treating the different
threats as within-subjects clusters of analysis and the different coun-
Measures tries as between-subjects clusters of analysis. In the first step, we
entered only type of threat as a within-subjects predictor, country as
Political preferences were measured on a scale ranging from 0
a between-subjects predictor and concern about threat as an outcome
(left) to 6 (right). In the United States, participants were asked to
variable. This analysis showed that the overall proportion of variance
indicate their political orientation on a scale from 1 (very conserva-
tive) to 5 (very liberal). The U.S. scale was reversed and recoded to 4
align with the other countries (1 = 6, 2 = 4.5, 3 = 3, 4 = 1.5, 5 = 0). Data about race/ethnicity were available only for Brazil, Israel, and the
United States. In Brazil, 40.68% identified as White, 14.98% as Black, and
Participants were asked to rate to what extent different phenom- 36.86% as Mulato/Parda; in Israel, 66% identified as Jewish, 33.97% as
ena constituted a threat. They were asked, “I’d like your opinion Arab; in the United States, 71.8% identified as non-Hispanic White, 8.05%
about some possible international concerns for (survey country). Do as non-Hispanic Black, 10.73% as Hispanic, and 9.43% as other.
14 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

in concern about threats that lies between countries (the intraclass Commission interaction (but not a significant Political Preferences
correlation) was 18.88%. In other words, the overall level to which 3 Local/Global) interaction in South Korea and significant Political
the different phenomena were perceived as great threats differed Preferences 3 Local/Global interactions (but not significant Politi-
from country to country (Wald Z = 52.90, p , .001), justifying carry- cal Preferences 3 Omission/Commission interactions) in Brazil,
ing out a multilevel model. In the second step, we entered individual- Chile and France. These patterns were not observed in Colombia,
level (right-wing) political preferences, as well as average ratings of Italy, Mexico, Peru, and Venezuela. We include the interaction
the threats from the preliminary analyses (see the online supplemen- graphs for all the different countries in the online supplemental ma-
tal material) on the omission/commission and local/global dimen- terial. The obtained patterns were thus the strongest in countries in
sions. Age and gender were also included as controls. The analysis North America, Western and Southern Europe, and Australia,
showed that rating of the threat as commission-based (b = .18, SE = although they were less in effect in Latin American countries.
.004, t = 50.93, p , .001, LLCI = .18, ULCI = .19) and rating of the
threat as global (b = .26, SE = .004, t = 69.46, p , .001, LLCI = .25, Discussion
ULCI = .27) were related to concern about threats, whereas political
preferences were unrelated to concern about threat (b = –.004, SE = The results of Study 5 corroborate Studies 3 and 4 and indicate
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.003, t = –1.17, p = .241). In the third and fourth steps, we entered that more concern about commission-based threats was associated
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

interaction terms between political preference and omission/commis- with right-wing political preferences, and more concern about omis-
sion (third step) and political preference and local/global (fourth sion-based threats was associated with left-wing political preferen-
step). The analyses revealed significant interactions between political ces. Study 5 demonstrates that this result is robust and is even
preferences and omission/commission (b = .05, SE = .003, t = 17.87, obtained in response to threats generated by independent research-
p , .001, LLCI =.05, ULCI = .06) as well as between political pref- ers, as well as when including participants from a wide range of
erences and local/global (b = –.09, SE = .003, t = –29.55, p , .001, countries in most parts of the world. Results from Studies 3, 4, and
LLCI = –.10, ULCI = –.08). The more commission-based and local 5 established that more concern about local threats is associated
the threat, the more concern about the threat was associated with with right-wing political preferences whereas more concern about
right-wing political preferences and the more omission-based and global threats is associated with left-wing political preferences.
global the threat, the more concern about the threat was associated Taken together, our studies indicate that the more omission-based
with left-wing political preferences. and global the threat, the more concern about the threat is associ-
In the fifth step of the analysis, we were mainly interested in ated with left-wing political preferences, whereas the more commis-
whether the interaction between Political Preferences 3 Omission/ sion-based and local the threat, the more concern about the threat is
Commission and the interaction between Political Preferences 3 associated with right-wing political preferences. The results of
Local/Global varied between the different countries. In this step, Study 5 corroborated the findings of the previous studies across rep-
we allowed the slopes for the interactions between Political Prefer- resentative samples from 22 countries located in five different con-
ences 3 Omission/Commission and Political Preferences 3 tinents. It seems that the overarching ideological difference in
Local/Global to randomly vary across the different countries. The concern about threat is not as clear and robust as much of the litera-
analysis indicated that the interaction between Political Preferen- ture suggests and depends on the types of threat examined in each
ces 3 Omission/Commission (Wald Z = 2.64, p = .008) and Politi- specific study. When differences in the meaning of different threats
cal Preferences 3 Local/Global (Wald Z = 3.11, p = .002) had are not considered, such general ideological differences seem mean-
small but significant variations across the different countries. ingless. The cross-cultural samples further allowed us to examine
To probe these differences between countries, we carried out two differences across countries and whether the obtained effects were
multilevel modeling analyses separately in each of the 22 countries stronger in some countries than others. The analyses showed that
in the sample. In the first of these analyses, political preference, the patterns mainly stemmed from liberal Western democracies in
omission/commission and the interaction between the two were North America, Europe, and Australia and we thus limit our conclu-
entered as fixed effects, controlling for age and gender. In the sec- sions regarding the link between political preference and concern
ond analysis, political preference, local/global and the interaction about threat to such countries at present.
between the two were entered as fixed effects along with the control
variables. Dawson’s (Dawson & Richter, 2006; Dawson, 2014) General Discussion
Excel templates for plotting two-way interactions were used in
order to visualize and interpret the interaction effects in the different The results from five preregistered studies reveal how collective
countries. With regard to the omission/commission dimension, the threats are perceived cross-culturally and how these perceptions
results clearly confirmed the overall pattern that the more commis- are related to political preferences. Specifically, this series of stud-
sion-based the threat, the more concern about the threat was associ- ies clarifies the kind of phenomena that people perceive as collec-
ated with right-wing political preferences. We visualize this pattern tive threat and demonstrate that people tend to categorize
across all countries in Figure 2. Further, for the local/global dimen- collective threats according to their extent (local/global) and intent
sion, the more global the threat, the more concern about the threat (omission/commission). The results further show that whereas po-
was associated with left-wing political preferences. This pattern litical rightists (or conservatives) are concerned about commission
across all different countries is presented in Figure 3. Interactions threats and local threats more than are leftists (or liberals), leftists
along these patterns could clearly be seen in the United States, are concerned about omission and global threats to a greater
United Kingdom, Argentina, Australia, Canada, Germany, Greece, degree than rightists. We arrive at this conclusion employing a
Hungary, Israel, the Netherlands, Poland, Spain, and Sweden. There systematic bottom-up investigation: after compiling a list of phe-
was further a significant Political Preferences 3 Omission/ nomena that participants spontaneously perceived as collective
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 15

Figure 2
Simple Slopes Plot of the Interaction Between Political Preferences and Omission/
Commission Across Countries
0.2
0.18
0.16
0.14
Threat concern

0.12
Omission
0.1
Commission
0.08
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

0.06
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

0.04
0.02
0
Left Right

Note. The terms left and omission refer to one standard deviation below the mean on political
preferences and omission/commission, whereas right and commission refer to one standard
deviation above the mean on political preferences and omission/commission. Error bars repre-
sent 95% confidence intervals for the interaction term.

threats (Study 1), we showed that threat is a multidimensional, result of indirect, unintentional passivity without malicious intent
rather than unidimensional construct (Study 2). We further gener- (omission), or direct, intentional action with malicious intent
ated a list of criteria which participants used to disambiguate and (commission). The local/global dimension refers to the degree to
interpret the different dimensions and found that people perceive which the threat is a local phenomenon affecting parts of the popu-
collective threat along two main dimensions—omission/commis- lation (local), or a global phenomenon that affect populations
sion and local/global (Study 3). The omission/commission dimen- beyond the immediate group (global). These findings were repli-
sion refers to the degree to which the threat in question is the cated in separate samples in which we collected new lists of

Figure 3
Simple Slopes Plot of the Interaction Between Political Preferences and Local/
Global Across Countries
0.3

0.25

0.2
Threat concern

0.15
Local
Global
0.1

0.05

0
Left Right
-0.05

Note. The terms left and local refer to one standard deviation below the mean on political
preferences and local/global, whereas right and global refer to one standard deviation above
the mean on political preferences and local/global. Error bars represent 95% confidence
intervals for the interaction term.
16 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

threats (Study 4) and on lists of collective threats, generated by in- Another possible way to understand differences between liberals
dependent researchers (Study 5). The results from the studies fur- and conservatives in threat perception is to expand the anxiety-cen-
ther show that whereas leftists are more concerned about tric approach to threat that is currently dominant in the literature.
omission-based and global threats political rightists are more con- This approach may inadvertently lead to a greater focus on conserv-
cerned about commission-based local threats (Studies 3 through ative threat perception. An alternative would be to consider other
5). This pattern was validated across representative samples from precursors of threat perception. For instance, many threats represent
22 countries located in five different continents (Study 5). justice violations and are likely to trigger anger or moral outrage
These results have substantial theoretical implications since (Eadeh & Chang, 2020; Lambert, Eadeh, & Hanson, 2019). The
they provide the basis for a unique conceptualization of collective current study provides a basis to continue studying justice motiva-
threat built entirely on lay perceptions, with few preconditions and tions that may underlie threat perception. On the one hand, partici-
assumptions. As such, the two-dimensional model of collective pants in our studies did not explicitly use justice criteria when
threat provides an opportunity to reexamine the relationship classifying threats (Study 3). On the other hand, our mapping of
between collective threat and political preference while dramati- collective threats (see Figure 1) and analyses of ideological differ-
cally reducing the risk of bias. The results produced an unequivo- ences in threat perception suggest that justice motivations may have
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cal picture with regards to the number of dimensions along which


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

played an important, albeit implicit, role in threat classification.


threats differ and the content of these dimensions. The dimensions Whereas liberals/leftists focused on social justice violations such as
in question (omission/commission and local/global) were obtained poverty and racism, conservatives/rightists were concerned with
from two independent assessments of lay perceptions of threat and physical harm to the group that is often associated with retributive
lists of threats compiled by other independent researchers. These justice (Lambert et al., 2019). Overall, the results of the current
findings allow for a mapping of societal threats into different research provide a basis for expanding our understanding of what
types, or categories, of threat and enable us to examine how these underlies threat perception and what precipitates ideological differ-
dimensions derived from a bottom-up analysis relate to current ences in threat perception. Our conclusion that threat is not just a
conceptualizations of threat in the literature. right-wing issue is also in line with recent research that finds that
ideological rigidity and ideological extremism are not only charac-
Implications for the Study of Political Preference and teristic of the right and are found among liberals and conservatives
Threat alike (Zmigrod, 2020).
In this article, we demonstrated the importance of the two- Why Do Liberals and Conservatives Differ in Their
dimensional model of threat for the study of the motivational bases Concern About Different Threats?
of political preferences. According to the theory of conservatism
as motivated social cognition, conservatives are more sensitive to Our interpretation of the results regarding the relationship
threat and more motivated to reduce threat and uncertainty than between omission-commission threats and political preference is
liberals (Jost et al., 2003). This general conclusion regarding the that while conservatives are concerned about threats that threaten
role of threat, however, is based almost entirely on research opera- the stability of the status quo (i.e., commission threats), liberals are
tionalizing threat as security and stability threats (Eadeh & Chang, concerned about threats that emanate from the status quo—threats
2020; Jost et al., 2017). The current research indicates that the that will occur because of passivity and the failure to make neces-
conclusion reached by this theoretical perspective is overly broad. sary changes and reforms (i.e., omission threats). The degree to
We show that the perception of collective threat extends beyond which one endorses change or stability in society lies at the heart of
threats to security and stability to include phenomena that are the the conservative-liberal, or right-left, divide (Burke, 1790/1973; Jost
consequence of unintentional passivity—threats that result from et al., 2003; Jost, Federico, & Napier, 2009; Paine, 1791/1986).
exaggerated stability, a failure to make necessary changes in the Whereas conservative ideology emphasizes collective stability and
status quo. We reason that any comprehensive conclusion regard- values associated with conservation, tradition and security, liberal
ing the relationship between the perception of threat and political ideology tends to emphasize values associated with openness to
preferences, should consider a wider range of collective threats, change (e.g., Piurko et al., 2011). Indeed, the ideology of conserva-
including omission threats as well as commission threats and local tism historically grew out of resistance to chaos, disorder and anar-
threats as well as global threats. Notably, in our studies, we find chy (e.g., Burke, 1790/1973), while the historical roots of liberal
that while conservatives tend to be more concerned by threats that (e.g., Locke, 1690/1976; Paine, 1791/1986) as well as socialist (e.g.,
are the result of intentional activity with malicious intent and have Marx, 1867/1974; Rousseau, 1762/1968) ideology can be traced to
local consequences, they are in fact less concerned than liberals by attempts to bring about a new social order, by means of collective
threats that are the result of unintentional passivity and have global reform or revolution. While the danger in omission threats lies in
consequences. What differentiates, therefore, between conserva- passivity and the failure to implement necessary changes, the danger
tives and liberals is not the degree to which they are concerned by in commission threats lies in the carrying out of an action that threat-
collective threat overall, but rather which collective threats they ens security and stability. To use a naval metaphor, if the danger in
are more concerned about. It is notable that these ideological dif- omission threats lies in not changing a catastrophic course, the dan-
ferences are specific to concern about threat and not to threat per- ger in commission threats lies in someone hijacking the ship, steer-
ception. Both rightists and leftists perceive threats in a similar way ing it into dangerous waters. Therefore, conservatives' safeguarding
on omission/commission and local/global dimensions (Study 3). of collective stability and security may underlie their greater empha-
The difference between political partisans, therefore, pertains to sis on commission threats, whereas liberals' openness to collective
their level of concern about collective threats. change may underlie their greater emphasis on omission threats.
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 17

Our interpretation of left/right differences in emphases on local of these models fit well with our data-driven strategy. The results
versus global threat rests on ideological differences in ingroup of this bottom-up analysis may provide the necessary fine-tuning
identification. Patriotism and belongingness to the national group lacking in top-down models.
tends to be associated with conservatism and right-wing political
preferences. In contrast, leftists tend to have a stronger degree of Applied Importance
identification with humanity as a whole (McFarland et al., 2012;
Waytz et al., 2016). It could be that because local threats often Society is becoming ever more divided and polarized along po-
threaten the national group and global threats threaten all of litical partisan lines, with each side being convinced of the virtue
humanity, rightists tend to emphasize local threats, whereas leftists of their own perception of reality and the folly of their opponents’
tend to emphasize global threats. No measures of ingroup identifi- (Finkel et al., 2020). One of the main points of contention regards
cation nor attitudes toward stability versus change were included which collective threats are the most pressing and urgent to deal
in the present studies however, so we were unable to test these with. Each side in ideological disputes tends to be convinced that
interpretations in the present research. We believe that the the other side is unaware, or chooses to ignore, the most pressing
endeavor to uncover the reasons and mediating variables that can threats against society. In order to decrease such polarization and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

explain why conservatives and liberals differ in their concern deal effectively with the threats and challenges facing society,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

about omission and commission threats is an important topic for what is needed is increased awareness and understanding of the
future research. processes involved in the experience of threat and the effect such
threat experiences have on attitudes, behavior, and decisions. An
Why Omission/Commission and Local/Global? increased awareness and understanding of one’s own and others’
The presented studies do not provide an answer to why threats perception of collective threats may enable policymakers to con-
were primarily organized in terms of omission/commission and sider the full domain of threats to society and be aware of their
local/global. It is however interesting to note the parallel to a long own ideological blind spots. Because society ultimately needs to
tradition in attribution theory (Darley & Shultz, 1990; Fincham & be aware of both omission and commission threats that may be
Roberts, 1985; Heider, 1958; Shaver, 1985; Shultz et al., 1981; local or global for it to survive and thrive, the results of the current
Weiner, 1995) and the cognitive development tradition of moral research also provides empirical support for the claim that ideolog-
psychology (e.g., Baron & Ritov, 2004; Hebble, 1971; Shultz et al., ical diversity is important (Duarte et al., 2015). Societies that con-
1986; Yuill & Perner, 1988; Zelazo et al., 1996), which have both sist of both liberals and conservatives may be better equipped to
contrasted the relative weight given to consequences versus inten- detect and deal with the full array of collective threats.
tions of negative actions. In moral judgment, people tend to base
their judgment on the scope of the negative consequences and the Limitations
degree to which the agent had malevolent intentions (e.g., Cush-
The results from these five studies provide validity for the rela-
man, 2008). Viewed from this perspective, it is conceivable that
tionship between collective threat and political preferences across
participants in our studies assessed threats in a similar way, compar-
representative samples from 22 countries. Whereas the first four
ing them in terms of degree of intentionality (omission/commission)
studies were conducted among samples that are predominantly
and the extent of the negative consequences (local/global).
White and from industrialized nations ((WEIRD: White, Educated,
Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) samples; Jones, 2010),
Implications for Threat Taxonomies Study 5 enabled us to test our hypotheses on more diverse popula-
The obtained results also have important implications for exist- tions. However, one should note that even Study 5 did not sample
ing threat taxonomies. Integrated threat theory (Stephan & Ste- populations in some regions of the world such as Africa and most
phan, 2000; Stephan & Renfro, 2002), the multidimensional of the Middle East and this limits the generalizability of our con-
existential threat model (Hirschberger et al., 2016) and the com- clusions. Study 5 delineated the boundaries and limitations of the
pensatory political behavior model (Crawford, 2017) all differenti- relationship between political preferences and threat perceptions
ate between physical and symbolic threats (or realistic/symbolic and clarified the degree of universality of the findings. Our results
threats, physical/meaning threats). indicate that while the difference between political leftists and
The current research indicates that these theories come close to rightists is generalizable across a wide range of Western democra-
the subjective perception people tend to have of collective threats. cies, these differences may be limited to this cultural-political
Physical threats tend to be commission threats, typically of a local sphere. This finding is in line with earlier results that have indi-
nature. That is, threats with immediate, local consequences that cated that the left–right division is uncommon outside of Western
are the result of intentional activity, intending to harm others. cultures (Malka et al., 2017).
These include, notably, threats such as terrorism, war, crime, and To keep the focus on the complexities involved in the percep-
other acts of violence. Symbolic threats that are related to world- tion of collective threat, we used a relatively simple measurement
views, values and beliefs are often threats of omission with global of political preference in the present studies, asking people to char-
implications (e.g., threats to democracy and human rights), but not acterize their political preferences on a unidimensional left–right,
always. Threats to religious beliefs and values, for instance, are or liberal–conservative, scale. We do however acknowledge that
also symbolic in nature but may be local threats that are perceived there are additional potential dimensions on which political prefer-
as threats of commission (e.g., opposition to a religious practice). ence may vary (e.g., Carmines et al., 2012; Feldman & Johnston,
The current research provides a more nuanced understanding of 2014). Notably, political preferences are commonly differentiated
models of threat that appear in the literature and shows that some along cultural/social and economic political dimensions (Treier &
18 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

Hillygus, 2009) and research has shown that these dimensions are COVID-19. Indeed, research shows that conservatives are consider-
weakly, sometimes even negatively, related (Malka et al., 2017). ably less concerned about COVID-19 than liberals (Cova & Diaz,
The present studies thus take a first step in examining the relation- 2020; Conway, Woodard, & Zubrod, 2021; Conway, Woodard, &
ship between political preferences and threat, using a fully data- Zubrod, Chan, 2021). These different responses correspond with our
driven approach. Future studies could look at more complex con- contention that liberals are more attuned to global threats of omission
ceptualizations of political preferences. such as threats to the environment, to democratic institutions, and to
Because the reported studies are correlational rather than experi- public health.
mental, we cannot draw conclusions about the causal relationship Assessment of the threats facing society determines to a large
between perception of threat and political preference. Future stud- extent how we face the challenges of the future. Understanding
ies could examine the causal relationship by raising the salience of how we perceive and assess collective threats and how this informs
omission versus commission threats to see whether doing so political preferences is therefore a central question for social psy-
affects political preferences in the expected way. Further, whereas chological research. Using a data-driven strategy, we show that the
the creation of the lists of threats used in the studies was mostly perception of threats includes a wide range of phenomena that tend
data-driven, we do not claim that these lists constitute exhaustive to be organized around the degree to which it is perceived as a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

lists of all phenomena people perceive as collective threats. In


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

result of intentional action and its local or global reach. This con-
fact, we were surprised that in lay perceptions of threat (Studies 1 ceptualization of collective threats allowed us to revisit the relation-
and 4), and expert perceptions of threat (Study 5), threats to de- ship between threat and political preferences and, in contrast to
mocracy, for instance, were missing. We also thought that justice- many theories in the field, we show that what differentiates between
related motivations would play a major role in threat classification conservatives and liberals is not their degree of sensitivity to
and found evidence for this only at the implicit, but not the threats, but rather which threats they are sensitive to.
explicit, level. We believe that there would be differences in the
phenomena mentioned across different cultural settings and across Context
different time periods. We also recognize that contextual factors
such as the emergence of a particular threat (e.g., COVID-19) Prevailing perspectives in political psychology claim that it is
could change threat perceptions. We do suggest, however, that mainly political rightists, or conservatives, that are motivated by
although the specific phenomena experienced as collective threat the experience of uncertainty and threat. At least to some degree,
may vary, the main dimensions along which they are perceived this seems to resonate well with real world experience. In times of
(i.e., local/global and omission/commission) will remain stable. war or waves of terror attacks, people seem to gravitate toward
Finally, whereas the relevance of affect to threat seems almost more rightist and conservative attitudes. But something seemed to
obvious, the vast majority of the research in the threat literature be missing in this perspective. Looking at statements made by po-
has failed to account for the role of affective experience in the per- litical leaders from the entire political spectrum, everyone seems
ception of threat (Lambert et al., 2019). The current research is no to perceive significant threats against society, regardless of politi-
exception to this rule. The results of our studies, however, hint at cal orientation. The question then rather seems to be—what are
the possibility that justice-related concerns may underlie ideologi- rightists and leftists threatened by? In order to provide an unbiased
cal differences in threat perception. These results pave the way for answer to this question, we decided that the best approach would
future research that may wish to better understand the relationship be to carry out a data-driven study, wherein participants them-
between collective threats, justice concerns, and affective experi- selves decide which phenomena are defined as collective threat
ences. For instance, because anger is an action-oriented emotion and how these phenomena should be assessed.
that is associated with the motivation to rectify injustices (Lambert
et al., 2010), future studies on the role of anger in addressing com-
mission and omission threats that are either global or local may References
further our understanding of the processes underlying ideological Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brenswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N.
differences in threat perception. (2019). The authoritarian personality. Verso Books. (Original work
published 1950)
Concluding Thoughts Altemeyer, R. A. (1998). The other. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology: Authoritarian personality (Vol. 30,
Although most of the research reported here took place before the pp. 47–91). Academic Press.
outbreak of COVID-19 – a global viral pandemic that poses a serious Bai, H., & Federico, C. M. (2020). Collective existential threat mediates
worldwide health threat, the model derived from this research offers a White population decline’s effect on defensive reactions. Group Proc-
new perspective and sheds unique light on the type of threat COVID- esses & Intergroup Relations, 23(3), 361–377. https://doi.org/10.1177/
19 poses, and on how people perceive such threats. COVID-19 is per- 1368430219839763
ceived as a global threat of omission—there is no intentional perpetra- Bakker, B. N., Schumacher, G., Gothreau, C., & Arceneaux, K. (2020).
Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological responses to
tor and to offset the potential catastrophic consequences of the threat
threats. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(6), 613–621. https://doi.org/10
substantial preemptive measures must be taken. The various reactions
.1038/s41562-020-0823-z
of politicians to the threat also seem to fit our model quite well. Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (2004). Omission bias, individual differences, and
Whereas liberals tend to emphasize the necessity of public health care normality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
systems that can provide care for all in such times, many conservative 94(2), 74–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.03.003
politicians and pundits focus on the global economic consequences of Borg, I., & Groenen, P. (2005). Modern multidimensional scaling: Theory
the extreme reactions governments have taken in response to and applications. Springer-Verlag.
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 19

Brandt, M. J., Reyna, C., Chambers, J. R., Crawford, J. T., & Wetherell, G. Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of ideol-
(2014). The ideological-conflict hypothesis: Intolerance among both lib- ogy and prejudice. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental
erals and conservatives. Current Directions in Psychological Science, social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 41–113). Academic Press. https://doi
23(1), 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413510932 .org/10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80004-6
Brunswik, E. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory in a Eadeh, F. R., & Chang, K. K. (2020). Can threat increase support for liber-
functional psychology. Psychological Review, 62(3), 193–217. https:// alism? New insights into the relationship between threat and political
doi.org/10.1037/h0047470 attitudes. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(1), 88–96.
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psycho- https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550618815919
logical experiments. University of California Press. https://doi.org/10 Elad-Strenger, J., & Shahar, G. (2018). Revisiting the effects of societal
.1525/9780520350519 threat perceptions on conflict-related political positions: A three-wave
Burke, E. (1973). Reflections on the revolution in France. Anchor Press. study. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 62(8), 1753–1783. https://doi
(Original work published 1790) .org/10.1177/0022002717703684
Busing, F. M. T. A., Commandeur, J. J., Heiser, W. J., Bandilla, W., & Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical
Faulbaum, F. (1997). PROXSCAL: A multidimensional scaling pro- power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression
gram for individual differences scaling with constraints. In W. Badilla &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi


F. Faulbaum (Eds.), Softstat 97: Advances in statistical software
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
(pp. 67–73). Heidelberg.
Federico, C. M., Fisher, E. L., & Deason, G. (2017). The authoritarian left
Canter, D., Brown, J., & Groat, L. (1985). A multiple sorting procedure for
withdraws from politics: Ideological asymmetry in the relationship
studying conceptual systems. In M. Brenner, J. Brown, & D. Canter
between authoritarianism and political engagement. The Journal of Poli-
(Eds.), The research interview: Uses and approaches (pp. 79–114). Aca-
tics, 79(3), 1010–1023. https://doi.org/10.1086/692126
demic Press.
Federico, C. M., & Malka, A. (2018). The contingent, contextual nature of
Carmines, E. G., Ensley, M. J., & Wagner, M. W. (2012). Political ideol-
ogy in American politics: One, two or none? Forum, 10(3), 1–18. the relationship between needs for security and certainty and political
https://doi.org/10.1515/1540-8884.1526 preferences: Evidence and implications. Advances in Political Psychol-
Conway, L. G., III, Woodard, S. R., & Zubrod, A. (2021) Social psycho- ogy, 39(1), 3–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12477
logical measurements of COVID-19: Coronavirus perceived threat, Feldman, S. (2013). Comments on: Authoritarianism in social context: the
Government responses, impacts and Experiences Questionnaires role of threat. International Journal of Psychology, 48(1), 55–59.
[Unpublished manuscript]. University of Montana (Conway III & https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.742196
Zubrod), Keene State College. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/z2x9a Feldman, S., & Johnston, C. (2014). Understanding the determinants of po-
Conway, L. G., III, Woodard, S. R., Zubrod, A., & Chan, L. (2021). Why litical ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychol-
are conservatives less concerned about the coronavirus (COVID-19) ogy, 35(3), 337–358. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12055
than liberals? Comparing political, experiential, and partisan messaging Fiedler, K. (2011). Voodoo correlations are everywhere—Not only in
explanations. Personality and Individual Differences, 183, 111–124. social neurosciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2),
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111124 163–171. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611400237
Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to Fincham, F. D., & Roberts, C. (1985). Intervening causation and the mitiga-
different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice”. tion of responsibility for harm doing. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 770–789. https:// chology, 21(2), 178–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(85)90014-9
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.770 Finkel, E. J., Bail, C. A., Cikara, M., Ditto, P. H., Iyengar, S., Klar, S., &
Cova, F., & Diaz, R. (2020). Individual differences and reactions to the co- Druckman, J. N. (2020). Political sectarianism in America. Science,
ronavirus outbreak. https://osf.io/497s2/ 370(6516), 533–536. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abe1715
Crawford, J. (2017). Are conservatives more sensitive to threat than liber- Forgas, J. P. (1976). The perception of social episodes: Categorical and
als? It depends on how we define threat and conservatism. Social Cogni- dimensional representations in two different social milieus. Journal of
tion, 35(4), 354–373. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2017.35.4.354 Personality and Social Psychology, 34(2), 199–209. https://doi.org/10
Crawford, J. T. (2014). Ideological symmetries and asymmetries in politi- .1037/0022-3514.34.2.199
cal intolerance and prejudice toward political activist groups. Journal of Galinsky, A. D., Whitson, J. A., Huang, L., & Rucker, D. D. (2012). Not
Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 284–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
so fluid and not so meaningful: Toward an appreciation of content-spe-
.jesp.2014.08.002
cific compensation. Psychological Inquiry, 23(4), 339–345. https://doi
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of
.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.730978
causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2),
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management
353–380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006
theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments
Darley, J. M., & Shultz, T. R. (1990). Moral rules—Their content and ac-
and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi-
quisition. Annual Review of Psychology, 41(1), 525–556. https://doi.org/
mental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61–139). Academic Press.
10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002521
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60016-7
Dawson, J. F. (2014). Moderation in management research: What, why,
when, and how. Journal of Business and Psychology, 29(1), 1–19. Hair, J. F., Jr., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2014). Multi-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-013-9308-7 variate data analysis. Pearson.
Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in Hebble, P. W. (1971). The development of elementary school children’s
moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope judgment of intent. Child Development, 42(4), 1203–1215. https://doi
difference test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 917–926. https:// .org/10.2307/1127804
doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.917 Heck, R. H., Thomas, S. L., & Tabata, L. N. (2013). Multilevel and longi-
Duarte, J. L., Crawford, J. T., Stern, C., Haidt, J., Jussim, L., & Tetlock, tudinal modeling with IBM SPSS. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/
P. E. (2015). Political diversity will improve social psychological sci- 9780203701249
ence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e130. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. Wiley.
S0140525X14000430 https://doi.org/10.1037/10628-000
20 KAHN, BJÖRKLUND, AND HIRSCHBERGER

Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., & Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativ- Koch, A., Imhoff, R., Dotsch, R., Unkelbach, C., & Alves, H. (2016). The
ity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain ABC of stereotypes about groups: Agency/socioeconomic success, con-
Sciences, 37(3), 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13001192 servative-progressive beliefs, and communion. Journal of Personality
Hirschberger, G., & Ein-Dor, T. (2020). A temporal account of collective and Social Psychology, 110(5), 675–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/
victimization as existential threat: Reconsidering adaptive and maladap- pspa0000046
tive responses. In J. R. Vollhardt (Ed.), The social psychology of collec- Kruskal, J. B. (1964). Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of
tive victimhood (pp. 100–120). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/ fit to a nonmetric hypothesis. Psychometrika, 29(1), 1–27. https://doi
10.1093/oso/9780190875190.003.0005 .org/10.1007/BF02289565
Hirschberger, G., Ein-Dor, T., Leidner, B., & Saguy, T. (2016). How is Kruskal, J. B., & Wish, M. (1978). Multidimensional scaling. SAGE.
existential threat related to intergroup conflict? Introducing the multidi- https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412985130
mensional existential threat (MET) model. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Lambert, A. J., Eadeh, F. R., & Hanson, E. J. (2019). Anger and its conse-
1877. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01877 quences for judgment and behavior: Recent developments in social and
Huddy, L., & Feldman, S. (2011). Americans respond politically to 9/11: political psychology. In J. M. Olson (Ed.), Advances in experimental
Understanding the impact of the terrorist attacks and their aftermath. Amer- social psychology (Vol. 59, pp. 103–173). Academic Press. https://doi
ican Psychologist, 66(6), 455–467. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024894
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2018.12.001
Huddy, L., Khatib, N., & Capelos, T. (2002). Trends: Reactions to the ter-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Lambert, A. J., Scherer, L. D., Schott, J. P., Olson, K. R., Andrews, R. K.,
rorist attacks of September 11. Public Opinion Quarterly, 66(3),
O'Brien, T. C., & Zisser, A. R. (2010). Rally effects, threat, and attitude
418–450. https://doi.org/10.1086/341395
change: An integrative approach to understanding the role of emotion.
Imhoff, R., Dieterle, L., & Lamberty, P., (2020). Resolving the puzzle of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(6), 886–903. https://
conspiracy worldview and political activism: Belief in secret plots
doi.org/10.1037/a0019086
decreases normative but increases nonnormative political engagement.
Lamberty, P., & Imhoff, R. (2019). From sperm to fatherhood: An experi-
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(1), 71–79. https://doi
.org/10.1177/1948550619896491 mental approach to determinants of paternal responsibility. Archives of
Inglehart, R., & Klingemann, H. D. (1976). Party identification, ideologi- Sexual Behavior, 48(7), 2217–2228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-019
cal preference and the left–right dimension among Western mass pub- -01480-5
lics. In I. Crewe, D. Farlie, & I. Budge (Ed.), Party identification and Landau, M. J., Kay, A. C., & Whitson, J. A. (2015). Compensatory control
beyond, representations of voting and party competition (pp. 234–273). and the appeal of a structured world. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3),
Wiley. 694–722. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038703
Jolliffe, I. (2002). Principal component analysis. Springer. Ledesma, R. D., & Valero-Mora, P. (2007). Determining the number of
Jonas, E., McGregor, I., Klackl, J., Agroskin, D., Fritsche, I., Holbrook, factors to retain in EFA: An easy-to-use computer program for carrying
C., & Quirin, M. (2014). Threat and defense: From anxiety to approach. out parallel analysis. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation,
In J. M. Olson & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Advances in experimental social 12(2), 1–11.
psychology (Vol. 49, pp. 219–286). Academic Press. Locke, J. (1976). The second treatise of government (J. W. Gough, Ed.).
Jones, D. (2010). Psychology. A WEIRD view of human nature skews psy- Basil Blackwell. (Original work published 1690)
chologists’ studies. Science, 328(5986), 1627–1627. https://doi.org/10 Malka, A., Lelkes, Y., & Soto, C. J. (2017). Are cultural and economic
.1126/science.328.5986.1627 conservatism positively correlated? A large-scale cross-national test.
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justi- British Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 1–25. https://doi.org/10
fication and the production of false consciousness. British Journal of .1017/S0007123417000072
Social Psychology, 33(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309 Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M., & Lelkes, Y. (2014). Do needs for se-
.1994.tb01008.x curity and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 106(6), 1031–1051. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036170
60, 307–337. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163600 Marx, K. (1974). Capital (Vol. I). Lawrence & Wishart. (Original work
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political published 1867)
conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). All humanity is my
129(3), 339–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
ingroup: A measure and studies of identification with all humanity.
Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: Its resurgence
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 830–853. https://
in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspectives on Psycho-
doi.org/10.1037/a0028724
logical Science, 3(2), 126–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916
O’Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the
.2008.00070.x
number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test.
Jost, J. T., Stern, C., Rule, N. O., & Sterling, J. (2017). The politics of fear:
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32, 396–402.
Is there an ideological asymmetry in existential motivation? Social Cog-
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200807
nition, 35(4), 324–353. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2017.35.4.324
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., & Laurin, K. (2008). Office for National Statistics. (2011). 2011 Census data. https://www.ons
God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism .gov.uk/census/2011census/2011censusdata
for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Osborne, J. W., Costello, A. B., & Kellow, J. T. (2008). Best practices in
Psychology, 95(1), 18–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.18 exploratory factor analysis. Best Practices in Quantitative Methods,
Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B., Jr., Bahník, Š., 10(7), 86–99. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412995627.d8
Bernstein, M. J., Bocian, K., Brandt, M. J., Brooks, B., Brumbaugh, C. Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing, M. V., Miller, J. L.,
C., Cemalcilar, Z., Chandler, J., Cheong, W., Davis, W. E., Devos, T., Scalora, M., Hatemi, P. K., & Hibbing, J. R. (2008). Political attitudes
Eisner, M., Frankowska, N., Furrow, D., Galliani, E. M., . . . Nosek, vary with physiological traits. Science, 321(5896), 1667–1670. https://
B. A. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability: A “many labs” rep- doi.org/10.1126/science.1157627
lication project. Social Psychology, 45(3), 142–152. https://doi.org/10 Paine, T. (1986). Rights of man. Knowledge Products. (Original work pub-
.1027/1864-9335/a000178 lished 1791)
COLLECTIVE THREAT AND POLITICAL PREFERENCES 21

Palan, S., & Schitter, C. (2018). Prolific—A subject pool for online experi- Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup con-
ments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 17, 22–27. flict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2017.12.004 intergroup relations. Monterey (pp. 33–47). Brooks/Cole.
Pew Research Center. (2017). Global attitudes and trends, February Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup
16–May 8, 2017. https://www.pewresearch.org/global/ behaviour. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of inter-
Piurko, Y., Schwartz, S. H., & Davidov, E. (2011). Basic personal values group relations (pp. 7–24). Nelson-Hall.
and the meaning of left-right political orientations in 20 countries. Polit- Treier, S., & Hillygus, D. S. (2009). The nature of political ideology in the
ical Psychology, 32(4), 537–561. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221 contemporary electorate. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(4), 679–703.
.2011.00828.x https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfp067
Proulx, T., & Inzlicht, M. (2012). The five “A” s of meaning maintenance: U.S. Census Bureau. (2017). National population by characteristics:
Finding meaning in the theories of sense-making. Psychological Inquiry, 2010–2017. https://www.census.gov/data/tables/2017/demo/popest/nation
23(4), 317–335. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.702372 -detail.html
Rosenberg, S., Nelson, C., & Vivekananthan, P. S. (1968). A multidimen- Ward, L. M. (1977). Multidimensional scaling of the molar physical envi-
sional approach to the structure of personality impressions. Journal of ronment. The Journal of Multivariate Behavioral Research, 12(1),
Personality and Social Psychology, 9(4), 283–294. https://doi.org/10 23–42. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr1201_2
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.1037/h0026086 Ward, L. M., & Russell, I. A. (1981). Cognitive set and the perception of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Rousseau, J. J. (1968). The social contract. Penguin. (Original work pub- place. Environment and Behavior 13(5), 610–632. https://doi.org/10
lished 1762) .1177/0013916581135005
Saad, L. (2007, December 7). Democratic party winning on issues. https:// Waytz, A., Iyer, R., Young, L., & Graham, J. (2016). Ideological differences
news.gallup.com/poll/103102/democratic-party-winning-issues.aspx in the expanse of empathy. In P. Valdesolo & J. Graham (Eds.), Social
Seeberg, H. B. (2017). How stable is political parties’ issue ownership? A psychology of political polarization (pp. 61–77). Routledge/Taylor &
cross-time, cross-national analysis. Political Studies, 65(2), 475–492. Francis Group.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0032321716650224 Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory
Shaver, K. G. (1985). The attribution of blame: Causality, responsibility, of social conduct. Guilford Press.
and blameworthiness. Springer-Verlag. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1 Wilson, M. S., & Sibley, C. G. (2013). Social dominance orientation and
-4612-5094-4 right-wing authoritarianism: Additive and interactive effects on political
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W., & Sherif, C. W. (1961). conservatism. Political Psychology, 34(2), 277–284. https://doi.org/10
Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The robbers cave experiment. The .1111/j.1467-9221.2012.00929.x
University Book Exchange. Yuill, N., & Perner, J. (1988). Intentionality and knowledge in childrens’
Shultz, T. R., Schleifer, M., & Altman, I. (1981). Judgments of causation, judgments of actors responsibility and recipients’ emotional reaction.
responsibility, and punishment in cases of harm-doing. Canadian Journal Developmental Psychology, 24(3), 358–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/
of Behavioural Science, 13(3), 238–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0081183 0012-1649.24.3.358
Shultz, T. R., Wright, K., & Schleifer, M. (1986). Assignment of moral Zelazo, P. D., Helwig, C. C., & Lau, A. (1996). Intention, act, and outcome
responsibility and punishment. Child Development, 57(1), 177–184. in behavioral prediction and moral judgment. Child Development, 67(5),
https://doi.org/10.2307/1130649 2478–2492. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131635
Stephan, W. G., & Renfro, C. L. (2002). The role of threat in intergroup Zmigrod, L. (2020). The role of cognitive rigidity in political ideologies:
relations. In D. M. Mackie & E. R. Smith (Eds.), From prejudice to Theory, evidence, and future directions. Current Opinion in Behavioral
inter-group emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. Sciences, 34, 34–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.10.016
191–207). Psychology Press.
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (2000). An integrated threat theory of Received May 4, 2020
prejudice. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination Revision received July 3, 2021
(pp. 23–45). Lawrence Erlbaum. Accepted July 7, 2021 n

You might also like