Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

DOI 10.1007/s11065-012-9208-z

REVIEW

Language Development and Assessment


in the Preschool Period
Gina Conti-Ramsden & Kevin Durkin

Received: 1 March 2012 / Accepted: 29 May 2012 / Published online: 17 June 2012
# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Abstract Most young children make significant progress in component, such as vocabulary, is considered inadequate
learning language during the first 4 years of life. Delays or for determining whether preschool children have typical
differences in patterns of language acquisition are sensitive language or language impairment. Available evidence sup-
indicators of developmental problems. The dynamic, com- ports the inclusion of measures of phonological short-term
plex nature of language and the variability in the timing of memory in the assessment of the language abilities of pre-
its acquisition poses a number of challenges for the assess- school children. Further study of genetic, neurobiological
ment of young children. This paper summarises the key and early behavioural correlates of language impairments in
developmental milestones of language development in the preschool children is needed.
preschool years, providing a backdrop for understanding
difficulties with language learning. Children with specific Keywords Language development . Language assessment .
language impairment (SLI) are characterised illustrating the Preschool children . Specific Language Impairment (SLI) .
types of language difficulties they exhibit. Genetic evidence Phonological short term memory . Psychometric instruments
for language impairment suggests complex interactions
among multiple genes of small effect. There are few consis-
tent neurobiological abnormalities and currently there is no Introduction
identified neurobiological signature for language difficul-
ties. The assessment of young children’s language skills One of the landmarks that parents look forward to is their
thus focuses on the evaluation of their performances in baby’s first words. Although a great deal of social-
comparison to typically developing peers. Assessment of communicative development has occurred prior to this stage
language abilities in preschool children should involve an (Durkin 1995), the onset of use of the baby’s home language
evaluation of both expressive and receptive skills and signals an important advance in that child’s development.
should include an evaluation of more than one dimension Although it needs to be acknowledged that there is variation
of language. The use of a single measure of a language in the developmental timing of babies’ first words, most
parents will begin to worry about their child if she or he
G. Conti-Ramsden (*)
has not produced single words by 2 years of age.
Human Communication and Deafness, The University There are many reasons why children may not produce
of Manchester, their first words as expected. What is known is that delays or
Ellen Wilkinson Building, Oxford Road, differences in patterns of language acquisition are sensitive
Manchester M13 9PL, UK
indicators of developmental problems in preschool children.
e-mail: gina.conti-ramsden@manchester.ac.uk
Difficulties with language can signal a number of problems,
K. Durkin including hearing impairments, general learning disabilities
School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University and autism spectrum disorders. In addition, there are chil-
of Strathclyde,
dren who have difficulties with language, i.e. producing
Graham Hills Building, 40 George Street,
Glasgow G1 1QE, UK words to communicate and/or understanding what is said
e-mail: kevin.durkin@strath.ac.uk to them, whilst “everything else” appears to be normal. That
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 385

“everything else” has traditionally been defined to include evolutionarily adapted nervous systems that enable them to
adequate input from the senses: normal hearing and normal/ attend to and perceive the acoustic and temporal character-
corrected vision. It also includes an adequate biological istics of spoken language. Soon after birth, infants can
basis to develop language (they have no obvious signs of discriminate all the sound contrasts languages of the world
brain damage) and an adequate basis for learning, i.e., their use, even those which are not present in their home language
nonverbal abilities as measured by IQ are similar to those of (Kuhl and Meltzoff 1997; McMurray and Aslin 2005).
their peers of the same age. A desire to engage socially is Exposure to talk in the first year of life provides the neces-
also important: such children seek to interact socially with sary input for the infant to develop sensitivity to the speech
adults and peers and as such are not like children with sounds of her or his home language or, in the case of
autism who are not as socially engaged. These children are multilingual families, languages (Krentz and Corina 2008).
usually referred to as children with specific language im- During this period, speech sounds gradually emerge, fol-
pairment (Bishop 1997; Leonard 1998). lowed by babbling, with the infant developing long sequen-
Given the importance of language to human behaviour, it ces of sounds reflecting the intonation contour of the adult
is not surprising to find that language difficulties are a risk language (Locke 1989; Majorano and D’Odorico 2011;
factor for associated difficulties in other aspects of child- Papousek and Papousek 1989; Saaristo-Helin et al. 2011).
ren’s lives. For example, research indicates developmental Infants begin to use communicative gestures, such as point-
interactions between language impairments and difficulties ing, showing and offering interesting objects to others
acquiring literacy skills, the development of memory skills (Behne et al. 2012; Fusaro et al. 2012; Tomasello et al.
and more general nonverbal abilities throughout middle 2007). During this period, infants also display the begin-
childhood, adolescence and beyond (Conti-Ramsden and nings of language comprehension. They begin to respond to
Durkin 2007; St Clair et al. 2010). Still more broadly, their own name (Tincoff and Jusczyk 1999) and to associate
children with language difficulties are at risk of less suc- words with objects (Bernhardt et al. 2007; Gogate and
cessful developmental and educational outcomes. There is Hollich 2010; Masur et al. 2012), particularly within specif-
evidence that children growing up with language impair- ic contexts, for example, when the adult says the word
ment experience greater difficulties in social interaction than “duck” at bathtime whilst playing with a toy duck. In
do typical children and adolescents (Durkin and Conti- typically developing children, language comprehension usu-
Ramsden 2007). These children are more vulnerable to ally precedes language production.
academic failure, social exclusion, behavioural and emo- The above “prelinguistic” period ends with the child
tional difficulties, and to being bullied (Conti-Ramsden et uttering her or his first word, usually around the first birth-
al. 2009; St Clair et al. 2011). day (Fenson et al. 1994; Majorano and D’Odorico 2011;
Within this context, the neuropsychological assessment Saaristo-Helin et al. 2011). These first words can vary in
of preschool children’s language skills is of crucial impor- how close they are to the adult form. During the first months
tance. It enables us to identify accurately difficulties and after their first word, children add an average of 10 words
guide appropriate management of interventions and support per month until they accumulate around 50 words. After that
for children and their families – ideally, from an early age. In there appears to be a “word spurt” and children’s vocabu-
this review, we summarise briefly the key developmental laries increase at a much faster rate of over 30 words per
milestones of language development in the preschool years month (Benedict 1979; Goldfield and Reznick 1990),
as a backdrop for understanding difficulties with language though this can occur somewhat later in children with less
learning, including what specific language impairment favourable circumstances, such as significantly pre-term
entails. We then examine the neurobiological evidence for birth (Kern 2007a). Around their second birthday, children
language development and language impairments, conclud- show evidence of grammatical knowledge by combining
ing that there is currently no identified neurobiological words together in multiword speech (Anisfeld et al. 1998;
signature for language difficulties. This is followed by a Schipke and Kauschke 2011; Thordardottir et al. 2002),
discussion of issues relevant to the assessment of language though delays in expressive language are found in toddlers
abilities in preschool children. We include considerations of who were born pre-term (Barre et al. 2011) or have suffered
context, content and commonly used instruments, as well as maltreatment (Culp et al. 1991; Eigsti and Cicchetti 2004).
limitations of particular approaches. Typically developing toddlers begin to put words together in
utterances like “more juice”, “hurt nose” and “daddy car”
(Braine 1976). These first word combinations tend to be
Language Development in the Preschool Years missing function words and bound morphemes that mark
information such as possession (possessive +‘s), number
Learning language is one of the key developmental tasks (plural +s) or tense (+ ing, + ed). Next, children begin to
facing children in the preschool years. Human infants have produce longer utterances, combining three or more words
386 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

usually in simple, active declarative utterances such as “I Difficulties with Language Learning
want my book”. They begin to include function words such
as articles (a, the), auxiliary verbs (am, is, are, has, have), Preschool children who develop specific language impair-
and pronouns (him, her). Questions (marked in English by ment (SLI) are usually characterized by having language
change in word order rather than intonation, for example, difficulties from the outset of the language-learning process.
“Are you playing?”) and negative sentences that go beyond Instead of reaching developmental language milestones on
the use of “can’t” and “don’t” which appear to function as schedule (first words around a child’s first birthday, word
general negative markers (for example “I won’t go”) come combinations around the child’s second birthday), children
later (Tyack and Ingram 1977). Production of multiclause with SLI are slow from the beginning. It is a hallmark of SLI
utterances marks the later phases of language development that these children are late talkers: they are late in acquiring
in young children (Kirjavainen et al. 2009). By the end of their first words and in putting together their first word
the preschool period, and before most typically developing combinations. It is not the case that children with SLI start
children attend school at 5 years of age, the aforementioned developing language normally and then stop and become
achievements in oral language growth have been completed delayed or lose what they have learned. Occurrence of
(Hoff 2009). “language loss” in infancy is reported in some children with
During this period, typically developing children are also autism spectrum disorders (ASD) but not in children with
becoming more proficient in the interactional dimensions of SLI. This appears to be a distinguishing feature between the
language use, mastering the subtle cues that signal turn- two disorders (Pickles et al. 2009) and hence can be partic-
taking in conversations (Rutter and Durkin 1987), discover- ularly useful for the differential diagnosis between SLI and
ing how to find common ground and sustain discourse with ASD in the preschool period. In the preschool and early
others (O’Neill et al. 2009; Short-Meyerson 2010), and how childhood period, difficulties with the sound system of the
to repair conversational misunderstandings (Laakso and language, i.e., phonology, can co-occur with SLI but are not
Soininen 2010). They develop skills in the production and considered to be a hallmark of the disorder. By middle
understanding of longer stretches of language (narratives childhood, problems with sound production are usually
and stories; Griffin et al. 2004) and become increasingly resolved or less evident (unless there is oral-facial motor
able to use language to discuss future events (Atance and difficulty/apraxia) and most children with SLI are
O’Neill 2005). In short, language is more than a system of intelligible.
rules to be mastered (itself a remarkable achievement) but, Interestingly, children with SLI can also have difficulties
concomitantly, a vital means of engaging with the social in understanding what is said to them. For example, follow-
world and organizing one’s experiences within it. ing instructions such as “bring me the big ball” (from
This brief summary of language development has to be amongst an array of balls). Some researchers have identified
qualified by the very important fact that there are substantial sub-types of SLI based on how impaired their comprehen-
individual differences in the timing of the course of lan- sion abilities may be (Conti-Ramsden and Botting 1999a;
guage acquisition within the population of typically devel- Rapin 1996), as difficulties with comprehension are a key
oping children (Fernald and Marchman 2011; Hoff 2010; indicator, or red flag for risk and persistence of language
Locke 1989). Much variability is observed in the timing of difficulties (Bishop et al. 2012; Bishop and Edmundson
children’s language gains. 1987; Conti-Ramsden and Durkin 2011; Nippold and
Research has long documented the existence of “late Schwarz 2002). Children with expressive SLI have more
bloomers”. These are children who may have only a few difficulty with talking (producing words, also referred to as
words by age 2 but a year later by age 3 have caught up with expressive language) than with understanding what is said
their peers in expressive language skills (Fischel et al. 1989; to them (comprehending language, also referred to as recep-
Thal et al. 1991; Wake et al. 2011). There are also consistent tive language). Although difficulties with talking attract the
findings from studies in Australia (Zubrick et al. 2007), the most attention to listeners and can occur in isolation, the
United Kingdom (Roulstone et al. 2002) and the United majority of children with SLI present with difficulties in
States of America (Bates et al. 1995) that approximately both talking and understanding. These children are referred
one fifth (19 %) of typical infants are not combining to as having expressive-receptive SLI or mixed SLI. It is
words by their second birthday at 24 months of age. much more rare to see children who have problems under-
However, the majority of these children catch up with standing what is said to them but can talk relatively normal-
their peers by 3 to 4 years of age. It appears that the ly (except in the case of children with autism, and some
variation in early childhood language development is in- individuals with Williams Syndrome, who manifest relative-
deed great. This makes the assessment of language and ly fluent use of social talk in the context of relatively poor
the identification of language difficulties in the preschool social understanding and pragmatic impairments). In addi-
years challenging. tion, there are some children with SLI who appear to have
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 387

difficulties with social understanding but do not present with twin studies, where identical twins have a much higher con-
the full cluster of deficits related to autism. These children cordance for SLI than non-identical twins (Bishop 2002).
are referred to as having pragmatic language impairment There is research support for the heritability of two clinical
(Bishop 1998; Bishop and Norbury 2002; Botting and markers for SLI: phonological short-term memory difficulties
Conti-Ramsden 2003). SLI is recognized by the ICD-10 as indexed by poor performance on nonword repetition tasks
(World Health Organization 1992), the DSM-IV-TR (American and difficulties with grammatical morphology, in particular
Psychiatric Association 2000) and the clinical research defini- poor performance in tasks involving formation of the past
tion of SLI (Tomblin et al. 1996). Table 1 presents each of these tense in English (Bishop 2006; Falcaro et al. 2008; Newbury
definitions of SLI. et al. 2009). However, no candidate gene sites of large effect
SLI is much more prevalent than other developmental have been found for SLI. The evidence to date indicates that
difficulties that have received more public attention, for patterns of inheritance appear to be complex involving inter-
example, autism. SLI is a common disorder: although esti- actions among multiple genes of small effect (Bishop 2006).
mates vary between 3 % and 7 % depending on definitions Within this context, there are currently no specific genetic
and statistical criteria employed (Silva 1987; Tomblin et al. markers for SLI that enable identification of the disorder via
1997), this is a much larger proportion compared to around genetic screening (Misyak and Christiansen 2011).
one percent for autism (Baird et al. 2006). More males Until recently, SLI was thought to be a disorder of child-
appear to be more affected than females. In referred samples hood. Follow-up studies into later childhood and adoles-
(i.e., children drawn to the attention of educational and cence have revealed that although some appear to “grow
clinical practitioners), the sex ratio of affected males: out of it”, for a significant proportion of children this disor-
females is around 3 or 4:1 (Conti-Ramsden and Botting der is persistent (approximately 3 % of adolescents experi-
1999b; Robinson 1991). Although it is often the case that ence SLI, Mckinley and Larson 1989). Those children who
there are higher proportions of males in populations with appear to “grow out of it” tend to have good comprehension
developmental disorders, the reason for the preponderance abilities, but even this group of “resolved” children can
of males with language impairments in clinical samples is experience language-related and other information process-
unknown. The picture is complicated by at least two con- ing difficulties in later childhood (Simkin and Conti-
siderations. One is the fact that in non-referred, epidemio- Ramsden 2006; Miller et al. 2008; Stothard et al. 1998). In
logical samples, the incidence of SLI among girls this sense, SLI forms one of the largest groups of young
approximates that of boys (Tomblin et al. 1997), suggesting people with special needs that professionals are likely to
that other factors (which could include comorbid disorders, encounter.
behavioural/socialization differences, greater attention paid
to boys’ problems) play a role in referral. The second is that,
on average, females seem to show an advantage in the pace The Neurobiology of Language Difficulties
of early language development (Bornstein et al. 2004; Eriks-
son et al. 2012; Lovas 2011), indicating that the calibration In the 19th century, work with adults with brain lesions
and interpretation of ‘late’ talking need to be gender- provided the first evidence for the specific location of lan-
sensitive. To date, no associations have been found between guage functions in the human brain. It was found that
language impairments and genes on the sex chromosomes. language function was strongly lateralized to the left cere-
There is strong evidence that SLI runs in families. The bral hemisphere. It was also suggested that the frontal cor-
majority of children with SLI have a family history of lan- tical area, later known as Broca’s area (Broca 1861), was
guage difficulties, with a first degree relative usually affected. responsible for spoken or “expressive language” whilst the
The contribution of genetic factors is most clearly indicated in posterior cortical area, later known as Wernicke’s area

Table 1 Definitions of SLI

International Classification of Diseases-10, ICD-10 (World Health Organization 1992) requires language difficulties greater than 2 standard
deviations (SD) below the mean, with verbal skills at least 1 SD below measures of non-verbal cognitive functioning.
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR, DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 2000) requires substantially worse
performance on measures of verbal abilities compared to non-verbal cognitive functioning. What constitutes ‘substantial’ is not defined
operationally, though functional impairment is required - i.e. SLI interferes with academic or occupational achievement, or with social interaction.
The clinical research definition of SLI (Tomblin et al. 1996) also uses threshold and discrepancy information to enable identification of SLI by
speech and language therapists. It requires a combination of language difficulties, assessed on a composite standardized language measure that
examines more than one dimension of language. The suggested threshold is that language abilities should fall 1.25 SD below the mean
(approximately, the 10th centile). Plus, adequate non-verbal cognitive functioning (i.e. a Performance IQ greater than 1 SD below the mean,
equating to a standard score of 85 or higher).
388 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

(Wernicke 1874) was responsible for comprehension or more diffused, less specialized and lateralized in young
“receptive language”. As shown in Fig. 1, these cortical children than in adults; as is the case in other cognitive
grey matter areas communicate via a major white matter domains. More recovery is observed in young children’s
pathway known as the arcuate fasciculus (Catani et al. language skills after early brain trauma than are observed
2005). in adults with similar lesions (Feldman et al. 1992: Reilly et
The above classical model has continued to provide the al. 1998; but also see Chapman et al. 2003). Neurobiological
basic scheme for understanding language organization in the maturational, developmental processes continue from child-
brain. However, more recent findings using functional hood to adulthood (Friederici et al. 2012). Available evi-
(fMRI) studies with healthy adults suggest language pro- dence suggests that left hemisphere specialization begins
duction and processing entails more widespread involve- very early in development becoming more pronounced as
ment of brain structures. It has been demonstrated that the children’s language increases in complexity (Holland et al.
temporal lobe projections of the arcuate fasciculus extend to 2007).
the left middle and inferior temporal gyrus (Powell et al. The examination of neurobiological profiles of children
2006), and that activation can extend to the fusiform and and adults with language difficulties is relatively recent and
angular gyri particularly in tasks involving receptive lan- has not yielded a neurobiological signature for language
guage functions (Binder et al. 1997). These areas are beyond impairments that can be informative in the assessment of
the classical boundaries of Wernicke’s area. There is also preschool children. Indeed, few replicable atypicalities have
evidence of activation of left (lateral and medial) prefrontal been identified (for a review see Webster and Shevell 2004).
cortical areas outside the classical Broca’s area in tasks In right handed individuals, the most consistent neuroimag-
involving language production but also in some tasks in- ing findings suggest either reduced left-sided grey and white
volving receptive language (Binder et al. 1997). Other areas matter density (Leonard et al. 2006) or absence of the
of the brain have also been shown to be activated during expected left greater than right asymmetrical pattern of
language tasks. Functional activation of the cerebellum has language cortex (Gauger et al. 1997; De Guibert et al.
been observed as well as the corpus callosum (De Smet et al. 2011). Additional, less consistent findings include unusual
2007; Binder et al. 1997). brain gyri, i.e., the presence of an extra sulcus in the inferior
More recent morphological analyses have further speci- frontal gyrus (Clark and Plante 1998; Jackson and Plante
fied the nature of language lateralization in the left hemi- 1996) and reduced volume of the caudate nucleus bilaterally
sphere. Studies have shown that in healthy adults and (Watkins et al. 2002). However, it is important to note that
children with typical language there is greater grey and the most consistent abnormalities have also been observed
white matter density in the left hemisphere structures sub- in other developmental disorders, such as autism (Stefanatos
serving language as well as left greater than right asymmetry and Baron 2011). Further research is needed to identify
in the white matter pathways connecting the major language distinctive features of brain development in individuals with
areas (Powell et al. 2006). Language neural networks are language impairments.

Assessing Language Abilities in the Preschool Child

Why Assess Language Development?

An important initial consideration has to be the purpose of


the assessment. This may well be indicated in issues raised
in the course of referral. For example, attention may be
prompted by a caregiver’s observation that there is some-
thing amiss with the child’s language in particular; however,
other prompts may be concerns about general intellectual
progress, behavioural or emotional disturbance, or the sus-
picion of ASD. Furthermore, professionals working with
children in diverse contexts may have varied goals: to assess
timeliness of language development, to provide a thorough
profile of developmental abilities, to inform a therapeutic
programme, to assess readiness for a developmental or
educational opportunity (such as pre-literacy or pre-
Fig. 1 Classical model of language organization in the brain numeracy activities), or for specific research goals. Clinical
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 389

purposes may mandate a very detailed and wide ranging How to Approach Language Assessment in Preschool
language assessment; for some research purposes, a coarser Children
but still informative guide (such as Mean Length of Utter-
ance, MLU, Miller 1981) may be sufficient or all that is Language assessment aims to provide a profile of a child’s
practicable. In the present review, we concentrate on the language difficulties. In order to achieve this aim, practi-
detailed assessment of preschoolers for clinical purposes. tioners need to establish the nature and extent of a child’s
language abilities in relation to the child’s age and develop-
Identification mental expectations. In the case of the preschool child,
expertise in obtaining representative behaviours from young
There are a number of instruments designed to screen for children and in interpreting different sources of information
language problems. However, a comprehensive review of is of particular relevance.
screening tests for early language delay carried out by Law There are both theoretical and practical issues to bear in
and colleagues (Law et al. 1998) demonstrates that screen- mind here. Theoretical assumptions about the nature of the
ing instruments are not sensitive enough nor specific enough relationship between language and other cognitive capaci-
to identify language difficulties. They produce too many ties (e.g., modular versus domain general) may influence
false positives and false negatives, i.e., they do not distin- approaches to assessment (see Karmiloff-Smith 1998; Rice
guish sufficiently accurately those young children who do and Wexler 2001; Thomas and Karmiloff-Smith 2002). Fur-
have problems from those who do not. This is not surpris- thermore, it is an oversimplification (albeit sometimes a
ing, given that there is great variability in the timing of early necessary oversimplification) to assume that language is a
language acquisition in the typical population (Bates et al. discrete and readily measured property that the child pos-
1995), that there are few, if any, robust early predictors of sesses, akin to his or her current height or weight. Language
language difficulties (Reilly et al. 2009, 2010), and that use, for the most part and certainly in the preschool years,
there are no strong, identifiable genetic or neurobiological involves interacting with others: hence, it is a joint product,
markers of language impairments (Bishop 2006; Webster not a solely individual one (Durkin 1987; Tomasello 2003;
and Shevell 2004). Veneziano and Parisse 2010). The quantity and quality of a
Currently, there is no policy for universal screening of child’s language performances will vary according to inter-
young children for language difficulties, for example in the actional contexts. Developmentalists are very familiar with
UK, USA or Australia. Consequently, practitioners are likely the notion of a zone of proximal development (the distance
to encounter young children for assessment when someone between what a child can do alone and what he or she can do
who knows them has already expressed concern about their in interactions with a more experienced partner (Vygotsky
language or other aspects of their development (Glascoe 1987). Even a basic measure such as MLU can differ, in the
1997). In the preschool child, this is usually parents, family same child, depending on with whom he or she is interacting
members or nursery/preschool staff. and with what goals. For example, Lovas (2011) found that
Early identification, in addition to providing information the gender of the child and the gender of the parent inter-
and support to adults who are concerned about a preschool acted to affect MLUs. With 2-year-olds, the longest MLUs
child’s language development, affords the opportunity for were reported in father-daughter conversations and the
early intervention. Language intervention with preschool shortest in father-son dyads.
children has the potential to change the developmental A closely related practical consideration is that preschool
course of their language difficulties and improve long-term children are known to behave differently in different con-
outcomes. Evidence suggests that there is more fluidity in texts and with different interactive partners, depending on
language growth in the preschool and early school years: level of familiarity, mood and the environment. Skilled and
some young children with language impairment experience experienced clinicians can do a lot to ensure that the child is
accelerated growth during this early period of development at ease but the testing context may still be substantially
(Bishop and Edmundson 1987). However, the available different from the child’s everyday contexts of language
literature also suggests that in older children and young use. This may be particularly pertinent when assessing chil-
people with SLI the rate of growth in more global language dren from socially disadvantaged backgrounds or shy chil-
abilities in the expressive/receptive modalities is likely to be dren, whose language performance may be especially
stable (Conti-Ramsden et al. 2012). The above considera- inhibited in unfamiliar settings.
tions, coupled with evidence of the efficacy of speech and Another important consideration facing assessors in
language therapy treatment, particularly for interventions of many communities is that the child (and/or his or her lin-
longer duration (see Law et al. 2003; 2010 for meta-analysis guistic environment) may not be monolingual (Crutchley et
and systematic review), make a strong argument for early al. 1997). As well as the direct complication of the lan-
detection of language impairments. guages used, there may well be associated cultural
390 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

variations in respect of how children interpret and respond (how many words the child has) and expressive language
to interrogation by a strange adult. For an informative dis- development (is the child putting words together?). There
cussion of issues and strategies involved in the assessment are fewer markers of early receptive language delay which
of bilingual children see De Lamo White and Jin (2011). can lead to under-detection of language difficulties involv-
Comprehensive assessment and contextualized interpreta- ing understanding (Reilly et al. 2009, 2010). Second, the
tion is needed; a model that takes a sociocultural perspec- assessment of receptive language abilities provides valuable
tive. This approach provides a more sensitive framework for information as to the nature of the child’s problems. Diffi-
taking into consideration cultural and linguistic diversity by culties with language comprehension have prognostic value
encompassing not only norm-referenced and criterion- and are indicators of potential persistent difficulties (Bishop
reference measures but also dynamic assessment. The RIOT and Edmundson 1987; Conti-Ramsden and Durkin 2011).
procedure that advocates review, interview, observe and test Children with expressive difficulties only and good compre-
(Cheng 1990; 1997) illustrates how to put this framework hension are more likely to progress well (Bishop and Adams
into practice. 1990; Paul 1996). Third, children use cues from a variety of
Thus, an informative assessment usually requires not sources to understand language. They have strategies at their
only a multi-method approach, but an approach involving disposal that enable them to respond to language that they
multiple informants. The use of formal tools such as stand- only partially understand (Chapman 1978; Shatz 1978). As
ardised tests complemented with more informal assessment mentioned previously, in the assessment of the preschool
strategies, such as naturalistic observations, is likely to child a multi-method, multi-informant approach is the gold
provide a more meaningful evaluation of the child’s lan- standard. In the case of language comprehension this is
guage skills. In addition, complementing practitioner assess- particularly true. It is desirable to carry out more formal
ments with information from other relevant informants, such assessments of language comprehension in the preschool
as caregivers, is likely to provide a more accurate assess- child as naturalistic observations do not always provide
ment of the language abilities of the preschool child. Most enough constraints in the child’s environment to be able to
preschool children requiring language assessment have a ascertain if it is the language per se that the child has
relevant adult, usually a parent or guardian, who is understood.
concerned about their development and will be accompa-
nied by them to the assessment sessions. Investigating the Assessing Different Dimensions of the Language System
concerns of the accompanying adult, i.e., what has led to the
referral, gathering their observations via interview techni- Language is a complex system comprised of a number of
ques and questionnaires can provide valuable information dimensions or components that enable an individual to com-
that adds to the data obtained from direct observation and municate effectively. These include phonology (the sound
assessment of the child. system), the lexicon (vocabulary), semantics (meaning),
grammar (structure), pragmatics (communicative functions
Assessing Both Expressive and Receptive Language Skills and conventions for language use), and discourse (the inte-
gration of utterances into longer stretches of conversation or
Learning language involves being able to produce as well as narrative). Language is also a dynamic system whereby dif-
understand language appropriately. As previously discussed, ferent components work together and change throughout de-
neurobiological evidence suggests that expressive and re- velopment. There is evidence, for example, that early lexical
ceptive language abilities are likely to involve activation of development and grammatical development are highly corre-
different cortical areas. Difficulties in one modality of lan- lated in typically developing children (Bassano et al. 2004;
guage do not necessarily predict difficulties in the other, Bates and Goodman 1997; Dionne et al. 2003; Szagun et al.
suggesting potential different neurobiological profiles for 2006). There are also findings from children with language
different patterns of language impairments. It is known, for problems suggesting that difficulties with a dimension of
example, that children can have expressive language diffi- language can have cascading effects on other components of
culties within the context of adequate receptive language language. For example, it is known that delays in lexical
(Leonard 1998). It is therefore crucial to examine both acquisition can affect the development of grammar in late
language understanding as well as language production in talkers and children with SLI (Conti-Ramsden and Jones
preschool children if we are to achieve valid profiles of 1997; Moyle et al. 2007).
children’s language abilities and/or difficulties. Thus, the assessment of preschool children’s language
There are three further considerations that underline the skills requires that the various dimensions of the language
need to include both language modalities in assessment. system are considered in constructing a profile of the child’s
First, many early signs of language difficulties rely on the abilities. Measures of a single language dimension are inad-
child’s productive abilities such as expressive vocabulary equate in ascertaining the nature of a child’s difficulties and
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 391

they are less reliable the younger the child, which is of particular dimensions of language. Language assessments
particular concern in the preschool period (Thal and Katch that provide more general language measures usually cover
1996). We stress that assessment of a single dimension of a number of language components, most commonly vocab-
language can also be misleading. Of particular concern is ulary, semantics and grammar, and both modalities of lan-
the use of measures of vocabulary as the sole indicator of guage, that is expression and comprehension. They afford
language ability (Dockrell 2001). A 4 year old preschool practitioners a profile of the child’s difficulties. For children
child may have adequate vocabulary for his/her age but may over the age of 3 years, one of the most commonly used
still have problems with semantics and grammar (Gray et al. preschool assessments of this type is the Clinical Evaluation
1999). Longitudinal data also suggest that children can have of Language Fundamentals - Preschool2 UK (Semel et al.
adequate vocabulary at an earlier point of development but 2006). This instrument was originally designed and normed
then demonstrate language difficulties later in the preschool in the US (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals –
period (Reilly et al. 2009). Preschool, Wiig et al. 1992). Its popularity in the UK led to
its British standardisation (see Table 2). This instrument also
Language Assessment Instruments has an Australian and New Zealand version with its own
standardisation (Clinical Evaluation of Language Funda-
Language assessment involves appraising the nature and mentals - Preschool Second Edition, Australian and New
extent of a child’s difficulties. This entails establishing the Zealand Standardised Edition, Wiig et al. 2006). It is always
language skills a child has and evaluating them in relation to best practice to use local norms if at all possible and this
what is expected of typically developing children of the entails checking what is available on a regular basis as
same age. Given the variability observed in early language updated versions of commonly used instruments are contin-
development, norm-referenced assessments are of particular ually being developed.
value in the preschool years. Table 2 below provides a For children under 3 years, the Preschool Language Scale
description of commonly used, norm-referenced instruments is a popular instrument for directly assessing infants and
for assessing preschool children’s language abilities. young children’s language skills. This tool was also origi-
As can be seen from Table 2, instruments vary on the size nally designed and normed in the USA (Zimmerman et al.
and characteristics of the sample used to provide normative 1979) but also has a UK version and standardisation (see
data. The basis for the norms provided, along with the Table 2). There is a language adaptation for Australian chil-
validity and reliability of the instrument, are important con- dren but no standardisation with this population (Preschool
siderations in the interpretation of the results of any assess- Language Scale, Fourth Edition Australian Language Adap-
ment as the information is, to some extent, as good as the ted, Zimmerman et al. 2002).
instrument used (Dockrell 2001). Instruments that focus on specific dimensions of lan-
Standardised tests, based on direct child assessment, are guage can be useful when practitioners want to examine a
thought to be the most formal of assessments as they specify component of language in more depth. One case in point is
controlled conditions under which the child can demonstrate the assessment of specific aspects of morphology and gram-
his/her abilities. The rationale behind standard conditions mar which characterise children with SLI. The Rice/Wexler
for assessment is that it affords fair comparisons between the Test of Early Grammar Impairment (Rice and Wexler 2001)
performance of the child in question and his/her peers who included in Table 2 provides an in-depth analysis of prob-
also experienced the “exact same” conditions when the lematic morphemes, such as past tense “ed” in 3 to 8 year
norms were being derived. Intelligence quotient (IQ) is old children which has been shown to be heritable in chil-
perhaps the most commonly known assessment of this type. dren with SLI (Falcaro et al. 2008). There are specific tests
IQ tests for preschool children such as the Wechsler Pre- that cover virtually all dimensions of the language system in
school and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third UK Edition a particular modality (expressive versus receptive skills).
(Wechsler 2003) or the British Ability Scales Third Edition Instruments that measure receptive vocabulary such as the
(Elliot and Smith 2011) usually include subtests that mea- British Picture Vocabulary Scale Third Edition (Dunn and
sure language abilities. The Wechsler, for example, provides Dunn 2009) and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
information as to how to derive both a verbal IQ as well as a Fourth Edition (Dunn and Dunn 2007) are commonly used.
non-verbal IQ (see Table 2). Nonetheless, given the avail- As previously mentioned, these types of single measures on
ability of published standardised tests dedicated to the as- their own are inadequate for determining a full picture of the
sessment of language, these are more often the instruments language abilities of young children.
of choice for language assessment. There are also standardised instruments that use parental
Language assessment instruments range from those that report and observation to assess the language abilities of pre-
sample the breadth of children’s language skills and provide school children. There is evidence that parents can distinguish
general language measures versus those that focus on between verbal and non-verbal abilities and that they can
392 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

Table 2 Norm-referenced instruments for assessing language abilities in preschool children

Instrument and author Age range Norms Description

British Ability Scales Third 3 years to 17 years 1,480 children in Assesses cognitive ability and educational
Edition (BAS3) 11 months the UK achievement using two batteries:
Elliot, C. D. & Smith, P. The Early Years Battery assesses verbal
development, reasoning, perception and
memory, together with understanding of
basic quantitative concepts.
The School Age Battery assesses reasoning,
perception, processing speed and memory
using verbal, numerical and figural materials.
The latter include abstract shapes, pictures
and three-dimensional materials.
British Picture Vocabulary Scale 3 years to 16 years 3,278 children in Assesses receptive vocabulary in children.
Third Edition (BPVSIII) the UK For each question the examiner says a word
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. and the child responds by selecting the
picture (from four options) that best
illustrates the word’s meaning. The questions
broadly sample words that represent a range
of content areas such as actions, animals, toys
and emotions and parts of speech such as
nouns, verbs or attributes.
Clinical Evaluation of Language 3 years to 6 years 588 children in Measures a broad range of expressive and
Fundamentals - Preschool 2 11 months the UK receptive language skills in young children.
UK Edition (CELF-Preschool2 UK) It provides seven norm-referenced
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., subtests in:
& Secord, W. Sentence Structure
Word Structure
Expressive Vocabulary
Concepts and Following Directions
Recalling Sentences
Basic Concepts
Word Classes.
The following supplementary subtests can
be used to give additional information
about the child’s skills outside of the
testing situation:
Recalling Sentences in Context
Phonological Awareness
Pre-Literacy Rating Scale
Descriptive Pragmatics Profile.
Early Repetition Battery (ERB) 2 years to 5 years 418 children in Consists of two expressive tasks: the Preschool
Seeff-Gabriel, B., Chiat, S., 11 months the UK Repetition Test (PSRep), comprising real and
& Roy, P. non-words controlled for length and prosodic
structure; and the Sentence Imitation
Test (SIT) comprising sentences controlled
for length and syntactic complexity.
Expressive One-Word Picture 2 years to 80+ years 2,394 individuals Co-normed tests to make comparisons
Vocabulary Test, Fourth in the US of a child’s receptive and expressive
Edition (EOWPVT-4) and vocabulary skills.
the Receptive One-Word The EOWPVT-4 requires the child to name
Picture Vocabulary the object, action, or concept illustrated
Test, Fourth Edition on a test plate. With the ROWPVT-4, the
(ROWPVT-4) child identifies one of four illustrations
Martin, N. A. that depict the meaning
& Brownell, R. (2010a, b) of a stimulus word.
Expressive Vocabulary 2 years 6 months 3,540 individuals A test of expressive vocabulary and word
Test (EVT-2) to 90+ years in the US retrieval that requires no reading
Williams, K. T. (2007) or writing.
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 393

Table 2 (continued)

Instrument and author Age range Norms Description

Designed to coordinate with the PPVT-4.


MacArthur-Bates Communicative infant scale 8 to 1,461 children in A standardised parent reporting system
Development Inventories (CDIs), 16 months; the US used to assess monolingual children’s
Second Edition toddler scale 16 lexical growth. The infant scale looks at
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., to 30 months comprehension, word production and
Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., aspects of symbolic and communicative
Reznick, S., & Bates, E. gesture. The toddler scale examines word
production and the early phases
of grammar.
MacArthur-Bates Communicative 30 to 37 months 356 children in The CDI III is a brief upward extension of
Development Inventories the US the CDI approach that is suitable for
Third Edition (CDI III) assessing language skills in children
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., between 30 and 37 months. The first
Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., component is a 100-item vocabulary
Reznick, S., & Bates, E. checklist. The second component
consists of 13 questions about the child’s
word combinations. The third component
consists of 12 questions, to be answered
“yes” or “no,” asking about various aspects
of comprehension, semantics and syntax.
New Reynell Developmental 2 years to 7 years 1,266 children in The Comprehension Scale explores aspects
Language Scales (NRDLS) 5 months the UK of a child’s understanding of selected
Edwards et al. (2011) vocabulary items and grammatical features.
The Production Scale examines the child’s
production of the same features of language.
Parallel sections within the scales aid
comparison of a child’s comprehension
and production skills.
Peabody Picture Vocabulary 2 years 6 months 3,540 individuals A measure of receptive vocabulary with a
Test-IV (PPVT-IV) to 90+ years in the US test that requires no reading or writing
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, D. M. Designed to coordinate with the EVT-2.
Preschool Language Assessment 3 years to 5 years 463 children in The Norm-Referenced Assessment
Instrument-Second 11 months the US comprises six subtests that provide a
Edition (PLAI-2) Discourse Ability Score:
Blank et al. (2003) Matching — linking of verbal and
perceptual information
Analysis — identification and/or combining
of perceptual components
Reordering — reduction or restructuring
salient perceptual cues
Reasoning — prediction of events and
justification of ideas
Receptive Mode — nonverbal response
Expressive Mode — verbal response.
The Non-standardized Assessment consists
of two pragmatic measures: Adequacy
of Response and Interfering Behaviors
Preschool Language Scale, birth to 6 years 800 children in Comprises two subscales: Auditory
Fourth Edition UK (PLS-4UK) 5 months the UK Comprehension and Expressive
Zimmerman et al. (2008) Communication.
The test includes tasks that assess skills in
the areas of preverbal behaviors, as well as
linguistic skills in the areas of semantics,
morphology, syntax, integrative language
skills, and preliteracy skills.
Provides Auditory Comprehension and
Expressive Communication subscale
scores and a Total Language score.
394 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

Table 2 (continued)

Instrument and author Age range Norms Description

Receptive-Expressive Emergent up to 3 years Parent reports Uses the behavioral observations of


Language Test Third Edition describing the parents or guardians to identify major
(REEL-3) behavior of language problems. It consists of two
Bzoch et al. (2003) 1,112 infants core subtests, Receptive Language and
and toddlers Expressive Language and also a
in the US supplementary subtest, the Inventory
of Vocabulary Words.
Renfrew Language Scales 3 years to 8 years 594 children in Action Picture Test assesses the age
Renfrew, C. (1998) the UK levels of the information content and
the grammatical usage from short
sentence answers to specified questions.
3 years to 8 years 573 children in Bus Story Test assesses the age level
the UK of consecutive speech used from
information content, sentence length
and grammatical usage in
retelling a story.
3 years to 9 years 741 children in Word Finding Vocabulary Test assesses
the UK the extent to which pictures of objects,
arranged in order of difficulty, can be
named correctly.
Rice/Wexler Test of Early 3 to 8 years 393 children with Identifies morphemes and syntactic
Grammar Impairment (TEGI) typical development structures that children diagnosed
Rice, M. L. & Wexler, K. and 444 with a with language disorders
diagnosed language characteristically lack.
disorder in the US
Sequenced Inventory of 4 to 48 months 252 children in the US Receptive section includes behavioral
Communication Development, items that test sound and speech
Revised (SICD-R) discrimination, awareness,
Hedrick et al. (1984) and understanding.
Expressive section includes three types
of behavior (imitating, initiating,
and responding), as well as two
distinct areas of expressive
measurement (length and grammatical
and syntactic structures of verbal
output and articulation).
Structured Photographic Expressive 3 years to 5 years 1,747children in Probes a child’s ability to generate
Language Test—Preschool 2 11 months the US early developing morphological and
(SPELT-P 2) syntactic forms.
Dawson et al. (2005) Photographs of everyday situations
and objects are paired with simple
verbal questions and statements to
elicit specific morphological and
syntactic structures.
Target structures include prepositions,
articles, plurals, possessive nouns and
pronouns, subject pronouns, auxiliary
verbs, copulas, present participles,
past tense, third person markers,
as well as negatives, conjoined
sentence, “wh” question, interrogative
reversal, infinitive phrase,
propositional complement, relative
clause, and front embedded clause.
Test of Early Language Development 2 years to 7 years 2,217 children in Yields an overall Spoken Language
Third Edition (TELD-3) 11 months the US score and also scores for Receptive
Hresko et al. (1999) Language and Expressive
Language subtests.
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 395

Table 2 (continued)

Instrument and author Age range Norms Description

Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, birth to age 90 5,013 individuals A measure of personal and social skills
Second Edition (Vineland-II) in the US needed for everyday living in the
Sparrow et al. (2005) domains of: Communication, Daily
Living Skills, Socialization, Motor
Skills and Maladaptive
Behavior (optional).
The Communication domain evaluates
the receptive, expressive, and written
communication skills of the child.
Wechsler Preschool and 2 years 6 months to 805 children in Provides subtest and composite scores
Primary Scale of 3 years 11 months; the UK that represent intellectual functioning
Intelligence – Third UK 4 years to 7 years in verbal and performance cognitive
Edition (WPPSI – III UK) 3 months domains, as well as providing a
Wechsler, D. composite score that represents a
child’s general intellectual ability.
Consist of 14 subtests: Block Design,
Information, Matrix Reasoning,
Vocabulary, Picture Concepts, Symbol
Search, Word Reasoning, Coding,
Comprehension, Picture Completion,
Similarities, Receptive Vocabulary,
Object Assembly, and Picture Naming.
Provides Verbal and Performance IQ scores as
well as a Full Scale IQ score. In addition,
the Processing Speed Quotient can be derived
for children aged 4–7 years 3 months, and a
General Language Composite can be
determined for children in both age bands.

provide reliable information on the language abilities of their deficits in different systems. Memory, information process-
offspring (Klee et al. 1998; Saudino et al. 1998). These types of ing, temporal auditory processing and specific linguistic
instruments are particularly useful with very young children. mechanisms involved in the representation of grammar have
One of the most widely used tools is the MacArthur-Bates all been proposed as influential in the aetiology of SLI. One
Communicative Development Inventories Second Edition, bet- prominent approach highlights memory impairments in SLI.
ter known as the CDI (Fenson et al. 2007). This instrument is In this special issue, the assessment of memory in preschool
designed to assess the language abilities of infants and toddlers children is discussed in more detail by Patricia Bauer. In this
(see Table 2). The CDI has been translated/adapted and stand- paper, we will highlight one aspect of memory that appears
ardised in a number of languages, including Danish (Wehberg to be particularly relevant to the development of language
et al. 2007), Finnish (Stolt et al. 2008), French (Kern 2007b), skills and SLI.
German (Szagun et al. 2006), Icelandic (Thordardottir and Ellis In an influential and evolving model of working memory,
Weismer 1996), Irish (O’Toole and Fletcher 2010), Italian Baddeley (1986, 2003, 2012) conceptualises phonological
(Camaioni et al. 1991), Spanish (Jackson-Maldonado et al. short-term memory (PSTM) as a domain-specific area for
1993), and Swedish (Eriksson and Berglund 1999). A number the temporary storage of verbal information. PSTM reports
of additional assessment materials have been developed, in- to a domain-general central executive that oversees and
cluding a two-page questionnaire (the CDI III) and scoring coordinates aspects of attention and memory in cognitive
software. For the latest developments on the CDI visit their tasks. Research into the memory capacity of children with
website at http://www.sci.sdsu.edu/cdi/ SLI has consistently shown deficits which appear predom-
inantly specific to the verbal domain as most children with
Phonological Short-Term Memory: Why It Should SLI perform within the normal range on visuospatial mem-
Be Included in the Language Assessment of Preschool ory tasks (Archibald and Gathercole 2006b). Gathercole and
Children Baddeley (1989, 1990) were among the first to demonstrate
that nonword repetition, a measure of PSTM, was a fairly
Why do some children have what appear to be more specific reliable marker of SLI as it discriminated between children
language difficulties? Different approaches emphasize with language impairments and either age or language
396 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

matched typically developing peers. As mentioned previ- Language is a complex system and its successful or
ously, PSTM as indexed by nonword repetition abilities has unsuccessful acquisition by children involves a number of
also been found to be heritable as evidenced by twin and internal constitutional factors as well as a number of exter-
family studies involving children with language impair- nal environmental factors interacting across development. In
ments (Bishop et al. 1996; Bishop 2006). the assessment of language abilities in children, this multi-
Measures of PSTM, particularly as indexed by nonword factorial model should always be kept in mind.
repetition abilities, have since been widely used in research
with children with SLI. However, there is no clear evidence
that its utility has been translated into clinical practice. The Concluding Remarks
majority of studies have involved school-age children
(Archibald and Gathercole 2006a; Botting and Conti- In this review we have made a number of important assump-
Ramsden 2001; Conti-Ramsden et al. 2001) and available tions that need to be made clear. We have taken it as given
tools for measuring PSTM have also focused on children that language assessment should be culturally appropriate,
over 4 years of age (Gathercole and Baddeley 1996). Re- taking into consideration linguistic, social and economic
cently, nonetheless, research attention has turned to pre- factors. The case of children learning English as a second
school children. Chiat and Roy (2007, 2008) studied language illustrates this point: some of the language diffi-
nonword and word repetition abilities in children as young culties observed in this population can be similar to that
as 2 years of age. They found that early difficulties with found in children with SLI (Paradis 2005). We have pre-
phonological processing and memory, as indexed by non- supposed that practitioners involved in the assessment of
word and word repetition at 2 to 3 years of age, were not preschool children have the relevant expertise in language
only correlated with concurrent language difficulties but development, language difficulties and language assessment
were also predictive of language problems 2 years later (at to allow them to make appropriate selections, observations,
4 to 5 years of age). These authors have now made available administrations and interpretations of information provided
an instrument, the Early Repetition Battery (Seeff-Gabriel et by the tools and methods they use (Paul 2006). Within this
al. 2008) that provides a tool for the assessment of PSTM context it follows that language assessment of preschool
abilities in children as young as 2 years of age (see Table 2). children should view the child as a whole and include a
There is now an opportunity to add a measure of PSTM to thorough consideration of other areas of functioning.
the assessment of preschool children. The inclusion of such The dynamic, complex nature of language and the variabil-
a measure can provide another source of evidence for the ity in the timing of its acquisition poses a number of challenges
presence of language difficulties in young children. for practitioners interested in the assessment of language diffi-
Furthermore, it has been argued that processing-dependent culties in preschool children. The evaluation of children’s
tasks, such as nonword repetition, have more validity across behaviours in relation to typically developing peers of the same
different languages (Campbell et al. 1997; Conti-Ramsden age forms the basis for assessment and requires careful consid-
2003). In the assessment of bilingual children and children with eration of the context, content and instrumentation. Multi-
differing backgrounds, - for example, children of migrant fam- method, multi-informant approaches are considered best prac-
ilies who do not speak, or are less proficient in, the majority tice. Assessment of language skills in preschool children should
language - the assessment of memory processes provides a involve an evaluation of both expressive and receptive skills,
promising tool for differentiating language impairments from should include an evaluation of more than one dimension of
linguistic differences attributable to experiential factors. language and if possible also include a measure of phonological
It is important to note that our recommendation is for the short-term memory abilities. There are currently no genetic or
inclusion of a measure of PSTM, in particular tasks involv- neurobiological markers for language impairment. The search
ing nonword repetition, as an indicator of language difficul- for early behavioural correlates of language impairment has
ties but not for its sole use. Although memory difficulties are proved to be remarkably difficult. Progress in the assessment of
likely to be involved in language impairments (Bishop preschool children’s language abilities requires further research
2002; Gathercole and Baddeley 1990; Lum et al. 2012), on the genetic, neurobiological and behavioural underpinnings
they are neither necessary nor sufficient for language diffi- of language development and language impairments.
culties to occur. There are children who have typical lan-
guage and poor PSTM and there are children who have
language difficulties and typical PSTM abilities (Conti- References
Ramsden et al. 2001). It is the identification of areas of
language difficulty and the presence of memory difficulties
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
together that is informative in the assessment of young manual of mental disorders (Revised 4th Edition). Washington,
children’s language skills. DC: Author.
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 397

Anisfeld, M., Rosenberg, E. S., Hoberman, M. J., & Gasparini, D. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 156–
(1998). Lexical acceleration coincides with the onset of combina- 173.
torial speech. First Language, 18, 165–184. Bishop, D. V. M., Holt, G., Line, E., McDonald, D., McDonald, S., &
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006a). Nonword repetition: Watt, H. (2012). Parental phonological memory contributes to
a comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing prediction of outcome of late talkers from 20 months to 4 years:
Research, 49, 970–983. a longitudinal study of precursors of specific language impair-
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006b). Short-term and ment. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. doi:10.1186/
working memory in specific language impairment. International 1866-1955-4-3.
Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41, 675–693. Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands
Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2005). Preschoolers’ talk about future of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: a study
situations. First Language, 25, 5–18. using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psy-
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University chology and Psychiatry, 43, 917–930.
Press. Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: an overview. as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evi-
Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208. dence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, chiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391–403.
D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a Blank, M., Rose, S. A., & Berlin, L. J. (2003). Preschool language
population cohort of children in South Thames: the special needs assessment instrument-second edition (PLAI-2). Austin: ProEd.
and autism project (SNAP). Lancet, 368, 210–215. Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). Specific and
Barre, N., Morgan, A., Doyle, L. W., & Anderson, P. J. (2011). general language performance across early childhood: stability
Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very and gender considerations. First Language, 24, 267–304.
low birth weight: a meta-analysis. The Journal of Pediatrics, 158, Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Non-word repetition and
766–774. language development in children with specific language impair-
Bassano, D., Laaha, S., Maillochon, I., & Dressler, W. U. (2004). Early ment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication
acquisition of verb grammar and lexical development: evidence Disorders, 36, 421–432.
from periphrastic constructions in French and Austrian German. Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic
First Language, 24, 33–70. difficulties and specific language impairment: can we distinguish
Bates, E., Dale, P. S., & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine
their implications for theories of language development. In P. and Child Neurology, 45, 515–524.
Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child lan- Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children’s first word combinations. Monographs
guage (pp. 96–151). Cambridge: Blackwell. of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41, 1–104.
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siege de la faculté du langage
the lexicon. Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-time articulé; suivies d’une observation d’aphamie. Bulletins de la
processing. Language & Cognitive Processes, 12, 507–584. Société Anatomique de Paris, 6, 330–357.
Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Bzoch, K. R., League, R., & Brown, V. L. (2003). Receptive-expressive
Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. emergent language test third edition (REEL-3). San Antonio:
British Journal of Developmental Psychology. doi:10.1111/ Pearson.
j.2044-835X.2011.02043.x. Camaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E., & Volterra, V. (1991). A
Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and parent report instrument for early language assessment. First
production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200. Language, 11, 345–358.
Bernhardt, B. M., Kemp, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Early word-object Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., & Janosky, J. (1997).
associations and later language development. First Language, 27, Reducing bias in language assessment: processing-dependent
315–328. measures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Cox, R. W., Rao, S. M., & 40, 519–525.
Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain language areas identified by Catani, M., Jones, D. K., & Ffytche, D. H. (2005). Perisylvian language
functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neurosci- networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57, 5–7.
ence, 17, 353–362. Chapman, R. S. (1978). Comprehension strategies in children. In J. F.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: development and Kavanaugh & W. Strange (Eds.), Speech and language in the
disorders of language comprehension in children. Hove: Psychol- laboratory, school, and clinic (pp. 308–327). Cambridge: MIT
ogy Press. Press.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s Communica- Chapman, S. B., Max, J. E., Gamino, J. F., McGlothlin, J. H., & Cliff,
tion Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects S. N. (2003). Discourse plasticity in children after stroke: age at
of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psy- injury and lesion effects. Pediatric Neurology, 29, 34–41.
chology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–893. Cheng, L. (1990). The identification of communication disorders in
Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). The role of genes in the etiology of specific Asian-Pacific students. Journal of Childhood Communication
language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorder, 35, Disorders, 13, 113–119.
311–328. Cheng, L. (1997). Diversity: challenges and implications for assessment.
Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). Developmental cognitive genetics: how Journal of Children’s Communication Development, 19, 55–62.
psychology can inform genetics and vice versa. Quarterly Journal Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: an evaluation
of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1153–1168. of performance in typically developing and clinically referred
Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
relationship between specific language impairment, phonological 50, 429–776.
disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2008). Early phonological and sociocognitive
and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050. skills as predictors of later language and social communication
Bishop, D. V. M., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4- outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 635–
year-olds: distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. 645.
398 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

Clark, M., & Plante, E. (1998). Morphology of the inferior frontal Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2009). British picture vocabulary scale:
gyrus in developmentally language-disordered adults. Brain and third edition (BPVS-III). London: GL Assessment Limited.
Language, 61, 288–303. Durkin, K. (1987). Minds and language: social cognition, social conflict
Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Processing and linguistic markers in young and the acquisition of language. Mind and Language, 2, 105–140.
children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Durkin, K. (1995). Developmental social psychology. Oxford: Black-
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1029–1037. well Publishers.
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999a). Classification of children Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2007). Language, social behavior,
with specific language impairment: longitudinal considerations. and the quality of friendships in adolescents with and without a
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1195– history of specific language impairment. Child Development, 78,
1204. 1441–1457.
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999b). Characteristics of children Edwards, S., Letts, C., & Sinka, I. (2011). New Reynell developmental
attending language units in England: a national study of 7-year- language scales. London: GL Assessment Limited.
olds. International Journal of Language & Communication Dis- Eigsti, I. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). The impact of child maltreatment on
orders, 34, 359–366. expressive syntax at 60 months. Developmental Science, 7, 88–102.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholin- Elliot, C. D., & Smith, P. (2011). British ability scales: third edition
guistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal (BAS3). London: GL Assessment Limited.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748. Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2007). Phonological short-term development inventories: words and gestures. First Language, 19,
memory, language and literacy: developmental relationships in 55–90.
early adolescence in young people with SLI. Journal of Child Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pereira, M.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 147–156. P., Wehberg, S. Marjanovič-Umek, L., et al. (2012). Differences
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2011). Specific language impair- between girls and boys in emerging language skills: evidence
ment. In D. Skuse, H. Bruce, L. Dowdney, & D. Mrazek (Eds.), from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental
Child psychology and psychiatry: frameworks for practice (pp. Psychology, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x.
180–186). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. Falcaro, M., Pickles, A., Newbury, D. F., Addis, L., Banfield, E.,
Conti-Ramsden, G., Durkin, K., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2009). Fisher, S. E., Monaco, A. P., Simkin, Z., Conti-Ramsden, G., &
Specific language impairment and school outcomes. I: identifying The SLI Consortium. (2008). Genetic and phenotypic effects of
and explaining variability at the end of compulsory education. phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, specific language impairment. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 7,
44, 15–35. 393–402.
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Jones, M. (1997). Verb use in specific language Feldman, H. M., Holland, A. L., Kemp, S. S., & Janosky, J. E. (1992).
impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Re- Language development after unilateral brain injury. Brain and
search, 40, 1298–1313. Language, 42, 89–102.
Conti-Ramsden, G., St. Clair, M., Pickles, A., & Durkin, K. (2012). Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick,
Developmental trajectories of verbal and nonverbal skills in indi- S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development.
viduals with a history of SLI: from childhood to adolescence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. doi:10.1044/ 59, 1–173.
1092-4388(2012/10-0182) Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S.,
Crutchley, A., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1997). Bilingualism & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative develop-
and specific language impairment in children attending language ment inventories (CDIs). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing
units. International Journal of Language & Communication Dis- Co., Inc.
orders, 32, 267–276. Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2011). Causes and consequences of
Culp, R. E., Watkins, R. V., Lawrence, H., Letts, D., Kelly, D. J., & variability in early language learning. In I. Arnon & E. V. Clark
Rice, M. L. (1991). Maltreated children’s language and speech (Eds.), Experience, variation, and generalization: learning a first
development: abused, neglected, and abused and neglected. First language (Trends in Language Acquisition Research, vol. 7) (pp.
Language, 11, 377–389. 179–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Dawson, J., Eyer, J. A., & Fonkalsrud, J. (2005). Structured photo- Fischel, J., Whitehurst, G., Caulfield, M., & De Baryshe, B. (1989).
graphic expressive language test—preschool (2nd ed.). DeKalb: Language growth in children with expressive language delay.
Janelle Publications. Paediatrics, 83, 218–227.
De Guibert, C., Maumet, C., Jannin, P., Ferré, J.-C., Tréguier, C., Friederici, A. D., Oberecker, R., & Brauer, J. (2012). Neurophysiolog-
Barillot, C., et al. (2011). Abnormal functional lateralization and ical preconditions of syntax acquisition. Psychological Research,
activity of language brain areas in typical specific language im- 76, 204–211.
pairment (developmental dysphasia). Brain, 134, 3044–3058. Fusaro, M., Harris, P. L., & Pan, B. A. (2012). Head nodding and head
De Lamo White, C., & Jin, L. (2011). Evaluation of speech and language shaking gestures in children’s early communication. First Lan-
assessment approaches with bilingual children. International guage. doi:10.1177/0142723711419326.
Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 46, 613–627. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of
De Smet, H. J., Baillieux, H., De Deyn, P. P., Marien, P., & Paquier, P. STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal
(2007). The cerebellum and language: the story so far. Folia study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.
Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 59, 165–170. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory
Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic deficits in language-disordered children: is there a causal connec-
evidence for directional effects of early lexical and grammatical tion? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.
development. Child Development, 74, 394–412. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The children’s test of
Dockrell, J. E. (2001). Assessing language skills in preschool children. nonword repetition. London: The Psychological Corporation.
Child Psychology & Psychiatry Review, 6, 74–85. Gauger, L. M., Lombardino, L. J., & Leonard, C. M. (1997). Brain
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test - morphology in children with specific language impairment. Jour-
IV (PPVT-IV). San Antonio: Pearson. nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1272–1284.
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 399

Glascoe, F. P. (1997). Parents’ concerns about children’s development: a meta analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
prescreening technique or screening test? Pediatrics, 99, 522– Research, 47, 924–943.
528. Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2010). Speech and language therapy
Gogate, L. K., & Hollich, G. (2010). Invariance detection within an interventions for children with primary speech and language
interactive system: a perceptual gateway to language develop- delay and disorder (Review). The Cochrane Library, issue 5, 1–
ment. Psychological Review, 117, 496–516. 77. The Cochrane Collaboration. London: Wiley.
Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment.
rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Lan- Cambridge: The MIT Press.
guage, 17, 171–184. Leonard, C. M., Eckert, M. A., & Kuldau, J. M. (2006). Exploiting
Gray, S., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1999). The diagnostic accuracy of human anatomical variability as a link between genome and
four vocabulary tests administered to preschool-age children. cognome. Genes, Brain & Behaviour, 5, 64–77.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 196– Locke, J. L. (1989). Babbling and early speech: continuity and indi-
206. vidual differences. First Language, 9, 191–205.
Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral Lovas, G. S. (2011). Gender and patterns of language development in
discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First mother-toddler and father-toddler dyads. First Language, 31, 83–
Language, 24, 123–147. 108.
Hedrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., & Tobin, A. R. (1984). Sequenced Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. T. (2012).
inventory of communication development, revised (SICD-R). Seat- Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language
tle: University of Washington Press. impairment. Cortex. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.001.
Hoff, E. (2009). Language development (4th ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth/ Majorano, M., & D’Odorico, L. (2011). The transition into ambient
Cengage Learning language: a longitudinal study of babbling and first word produc-
Hoff, E. (2010). Context effects on young children’s language use: the tion of Italian children. First Language, 31, 47–66.
influence of conversational setting and partner. First Language, Martin, N. A., & Brownell, R. (2010a). Expressive One-Word Picture
30, 461–472. Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition (EOWPVT-4). Novato: Academic
Holland, S. K., Vannest, J., Mecoli, M., Jacola, L. M., Tillema, J.-M., Therapy Publications.
Karunanayaka, P. R., et al. (2007). Functional MRI of language Martin, N. A., & Brownell, R. (2010b). Receptive One-Word Picture
lateralization during development in children. International Jour- Vocabulary Test Fourth Edition (ROWPVT-4). Novato: Academic
nal of Audiology, 46, 533–551. Therapy Publications.
Hresko, W. P., Reid, D. K., & Hammill, D. D. (1999). The test of early Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Lloyd, C. A. (2012). Infants‘ behaviors as
language development-3 (TELD-3). Austin: Pro-Ed. antecedents and consequents of mothers’ responsive and directive
Jackson, T., & Plante, E. (1996). Gyral morphology in the posterior utterances. First Language.
sylvian region in families affected by developmental language McKinley, N., & Larson, V. (1989). Students who can’t communicate:
disorder. Neuropyschology Review, 6, 81–94. speech-language service at the secondary level. Curriculum Re-
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D., Marchman, V., Bates, E., & port, 19, 1–8.
Gutierrez-Clellen, V. (1993). Early lexical development in McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Infants are sensitive to within
Spanish-speaking infants and toddlers. Journal of Child Lan- category variation in speech perception. Cognition, 95, B15–B26.
guage, 20, 523–549. Miller, J. F. (1981). Assessing language production in children. Balti-
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to under- more: University Park Press.
standing developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Miller, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Finneran, D. (2008). Grammaticality
2, 389–398. judgements in adolescents with and without language impairment.
Kern, S. (2007a). Influence of preterm birth on early lexical and International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
grammatical development. First Language, 27, 159–173. 43, 346–360.
Kern, S. (2007b). Lexicon development in French-speaking infants. Misyak, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2011). Genetic variation and
First Language, 27, 227–250. individual differences in language. In I. Arnon & E. V. Clark
Kirjavainen, M., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). I (Eds.), Experience, variation, and generalization: learning a first
want hold Postman Pat’: an investigation into the acquisition of language (Trends in Language Acquisition Research, vol. 7) (pp.
infinitival marker ‘to’. First Language, 29, 313–339. 223–238). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Klee, T., Carson, D., Gavin, W., Hall, L., Kent, A., & Rece, S. (1998). Moyle, M. J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J. L., & Lindstrom, M. J.
Concurrent and predictive validity of an early language screening (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammat-
programme. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Re- ical development in typical and late talking children. Journal of
search, 26, 33–38. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 508–528.
Krentz, U. C., & Corina, D. P. (2008). Preference for language in early Newbury, D. F., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Paracchini, S., Buckingham,
infancy: the human language bias is not speech specific. Devel- L., Clark, A., et al. (2009). CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonolog-
opmental Science, 11, 1–9. ical short-term memory in language impairment. The American
Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Evolution, nativism and learn- Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 1–9.
ing the development of language and speech. In M. Gopnik (Ed.), Nippold, M. A., & Schwarz, I. E. (2002). Do children recover from
The inheritance and innateness of grammars (pp. 7–44). New specific language impairment? Advances in Speech-Language
York: Oxford University Press. Pathology, 1, 41–49.
Laakso, M., & Soininen, M. (2010). Mother-initiated repair sequences O’Toole, C., & Fletcher, P. (2010). Validity of a parent report for Irish-
in interactions of 3-year-old children. First Language, 30, 329– speaking toddlers. First Language, 30, 199–217.
353. O’Neill, D. K., Main, R. M., & Ziemski, R. A. (2009). ‘I like Barney’:
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (1998). Screen- preschoolers’ spontaneous conversational initiations with peers.
ing for speech and language delay: a systematic review of the First Language, 29, 401–425.
literature. Health Technology Assessment, 2, 1–184. Papousek, M., & Papousek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal
Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2003). The efficacy of treatment for matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical
children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: infants. First Language, 9, 137–157.
400 Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401

Paradis, J. (2005). Grammatical morphology in children learning En- Silva, P. A. (1987). Epidemiology, longitudinal course, and some
glish as a second language: implications for similarities with associated factors: An update. In W. Yule & M. Rutter (Eds.),
specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Language development and disorders (pp. 1–15). Oxford: Black-
Services in Schools, 36, 172–187. well Scientific Publications.
Paul, R. (1996). Clinical implications of the natural history of slow Simkin, Z., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Evidence of reading diffi-
expressive language development. American Journal of Speech- culties in subgroups of children with specific language impair-
Language Pathology, 5, 5–21. ment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22, 315–331.
Paul, R. (2006). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence: Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland
assessment and intervention (3rd ed.). St. Louis: Mosby-Year Book. adaptive behavior scales 2nd edition (Vineland-II). Circle Pines:
Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Conti-Ramsden, G., Falcaro, M., Simkin, Z., American Guidance Service.
Charman, T., et al. (2009). Loss of language in early development St Clair, M. C., Durkin, K., Conti-Ramsden, G., & Pickles, A. (2010).
of autism and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Growth of reading skills in children with a history of specific
Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 843–852. language impairment: the role of autistic symptomatology and
Powell, H. W., Parker, G. J., Alexander, D. C., Symms, M. R., Boulby, language-related abilities. British Journal of Developmental Psy-
P. A., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., et al. (2006). Hemispheric asym- chology, 28, 109–132.
metries in language-related pathways: a combined functional MRI St Clair, M. C., Pickles, A., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2011).
and tractography study. NeuroImage, 32, 388–399. A longitudinal study of behavioural, emotional and social diffi-
Rapin, I. (1996). Developmental language disorders: a clinical update. culties in individuals with a history of specific language impair-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 643–656. ment (SLI). Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 186–199.
Reilly, J. S., Bates, E. A., & Marchman, V. A. (1998). Narrative Stefanatos, G. A., & Baron, I. S. (2011). The ontogenesis of language
discourse in children with early focal brain injury. Brain and impairment in autism: a neuropsychological perspective. Neuro-
Language, 61, 335–375. psychological Review, 21, 252–270.
Reilly, S., Bavin, E. L., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., Eadie, P., Cini, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., & Lehtonen, L. (2008). Early
et al. (2009). The Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS): a lexical development of Finnish children – a longitudinal study.
prospective, longitudinal study of communication skills and ex- First Language, 28, 259–279.
pressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and 24 months. Inter- Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., &
national Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 344–357. Kaplan, C. (1998). Language impaired preschoolers: a follow-up
Reilly, S., Wake, M., Ukoumunne, O. C., Bavin, E., Prior, M., Cini, E., into adolescence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 41,
et al. (2010). Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: 407–418.
findings from early language Victoria study. Pediatrics, 126, Szagun, G., Steinbrink, C., Franik, M., & Stumper, B. (2006). Devel-
e1530–e1537. opment of vocabulary and grammar in young German-speaking
Renfrew, C. (1998). Renfrew language scales. Chesterfield: Winslow children assessed with a German language development invento-
Press. ry. First Language, 26, 259–280.
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (2001). Rice/Wexler test of early grammar Thal, D., & Katch, J. (1996). Predicaments in early identification of
impairment. SanAntonio: Psychological Corporation. specific language impairment: does the early bird always catch the
Robinson, R. J. (1991). Causes and associations of severe and persis- worm? In K. Cole, P. Dale, & D. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of
tent specific speech and language disorders in children. Develop- communication and language (pp. 1–28). Baltimore: Paul H.
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 943–962. Brookes.
Roulstone, S., Loader, S., Northstone, K., Beveridge, M., & the Thal, D., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in
ALSPAC team. (2002). The speech and language of children late talkers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604–
age 25 months: descriptive data from the Avon longitudinal study 612.
of parents and children. Early Childhood Development and Care, Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Are developmental
172, 259–268. disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from
Rutter, D. R., & Durkin, K. (1987). Turn-taking in mother–infant connectionist modelling. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
interaction: an examination of vocalizations and gaze. Develop- 25, 727–788.
mental Psychology, 25, 54–61. Thordardottir, E. T., & Ellis Weismer, S. (1996). Language assessment
Saaristo-Helin, K., Kunnari, S., & Savinainen-Makkonen, T. (2011). via parent report: development of a screening instrument for
Phonological development in children learning Finnish: a review. Icelandic children. First Language, 16, 265–285.
First Language, 31, 342–363. Thordardottir, E. T., Ellis Weismer, S., & Evans, J. L. (2002). Conti-
Saudino, K. J., Dale, P. S., Oliver, B., Petrill, S. A., Richardson, V., nuity in lexical and morphological development in Icelandic and
Rutter, M., et al. (1998). The validity of parent-based assessment English-speaking 2-year-olds. First Language, 22, 3–28.
of the cognitive abilities of 2-year-olds. British Journal of Devel- Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings of word
opmental Psychology, 16, 349–363. comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10,
Schipke, C. S., & Kauschke, C. (2011). Early word formation in 172–174.
German language acquisition: a study on word formation growth Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory
during the second and third years. First Language, 31, 67–82. of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Seeff-Gabriel, B., Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2008). Early repetition battery. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at
London: Pearson Assessment. infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722.
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2006). Clinical evaluation of Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., &
language fundamentals - preschool 2 UK edition (CELF-Pre- O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment
school2 UK). London: Pearson Assessment. in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hear-
Shatz, M. (1978). On the development of communicative understand- ing Research, 40, 1245–1260.
ings: an early strategy for interpreting and responding to mes- Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., & Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the
sages. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 271–301. diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten chil-
Short-Meyerson, K. J. (2010). Preschoolers’ establishment of mutual dren. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39,
knowledge during scripted interactions. First Language, 30, 219–236. 1284–1294.
Neuropsychol Rev (2012) 22:384–401 401

Tyack, D., & Ingram, D. (1977). Children’s production and compre- Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische symptomenkomplex. Breslau: M.
hension of questions. Journal of Child Language, 4, 211–224. Cohn und Weigert.
Veneziano, E., & Parisse, C. (2010). The acquisition of early verbs in Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). CELF-preschool: clinical
French: assessing the role of conversation and of child-directed evaluation of language fundamentals—preschool version. San
input. First Language, 30, 287–311. Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech (N. Minick, Trans.). New Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. A., & Semel, E. (2006). Clinical evaluation of
York: Plenum. language fundamentals-preschool: Australian standardised edi-
Wake, M., Tobin, S., Girolametto, L., Okoumunne, O. C., Gold, L., tion (2nd ed.). Sydney: Harcourt Assessment.
Levickis, P., et al. (2011). Outcomes of population based language Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test, second edition
promotion for slow to talk toddlers at ages 2 and 3 years: let’s (EVT-2). San Antonio: Pearson Education.
learn language cluster randomised control trial. British Medical World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification for
Journal, 343, d4741. mental and behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for re-
Watkins, K. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R. E., search. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Connelly, A., Friston, K. J., et al. (2002). MRI analysis of an Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1979). Preschool
inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnor- language scale. Columbus: Charles Merrill.
malities. Brain, 125, 465–478. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002). Preschool
Webster, R. I., & Shevell, M. I. (2004). Neurobiology of specific language scale, fourth edition (PLS-4): Australian language
language impairment. Journal of Child Neurology, 19, 471–481. adapted. Australia: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelli- Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2008). Preschool
gence – third edition UK (WPPSI-III UK). Oxford: The Psycho- language scales UK adaptation (PLS-4UK). London: The Psy-
logical Corporation. chological Corporation.
Wehberg, S., Vach, W., Bleses, D., Thomsen, P., Madsen, T. O., & Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Rice, M. L., & Slegers, D. W. (2007).
Basbøll, H. (2007). Danish children’s first words – analysing Late language emergence at 24 months: an epidemiological study
longitudinal data based on monthly CDI parental reports. First of prevalence, predictors, and covariates. Journal of Speech,
Language, 27, 361–383. Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1562–1592.

You might also like