Subjective Responses To Alcohol in The Development and Maintenance of Alcohol Use Disorder

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

ARTICLES

Subjective Responses to Alcohol in the Development


and Maintenance of Alcohol Use Disorder
Andrea King, Ph.D., Ashley Vena, Ph.D., Deborah S. Hasin, Ph.D., Harriet deWit, Ph.D., Sean J. O’Connor, M.D.,
Dingcai Cao, Ph.D.

Objective: Alcohol use disorder (AUD) remains an urgent Results: At the end of the decade, 21% of participants met
public health problem. Longitudinal data are needed to criteria for past-year AUD. Individuals who reported the
clarify the role of acute subjective responses to alcohol in greatest alcohol stimulation, liking, and wanting at the initial
the development and maintenance of excessive drinking alcohol challenge were most likely to have developed AUD
and AUD. The authors report on 10 years of repeated 10 years later. Further, alcohol-induced stimulation and
examination of acute alcohol responses in the Chicago wanting increased in reexamination testing among those
Social Drinking Project. with the highest AUD symptoms as the decade progressed.

Methods: Young adult drinkers (N5190) participated in Conclusions: Initial stimulant and rewarding effects of al-
an initial alcohol challenge (0.8 g/kg of alcohol com- cohol predicted heavy alcohol use, and the magnitude of
pared with placebo) that was repeated 5 and 10 years these positive subjective effects increased over a 10-year
later. They were also assessed on drinking behavior and period in those who developed AUD compared with those
AUD symptoms at numerous intervals across the decade. who did not develop the disorder. The findings demon-
Retention was high, as 184 of the 185 (99%) nonde- strate systematic changes in subjective responses to alco-
ceased active participants completed the 10-year follow- hol over time, providing an empirical basis for prevention,
up, and 91% (163 of 179) of those eligible for alcohol early intervention, and treatment strategies.
consumption engaged in repeated laboratory testing
during this interval. Am J Psychiatry June 2021; 178:560–571;doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20030247

Heavy drinking is increasing among U.S. adults (1–3) and sensitization, or an increased drug effect with repeated expo-
remains a major preventable contributor to disability and sure, has also been linked to addiction (13, 14). Although both
mortality worldwide (4). It is also a strong predictor of sub- tolerance and sensitization are integral components of neuro-
sequent alcohol use disorder (AUD) (5, 6), which carries biological theories of the development and progression of
additional serious consequences for health and functioning addiction (14–16), most of the empirical evidence for these
(7). Given the global burden of alcohol misuse, identifying theories has been based on studies using animal models.
factors that increase the susceptibility to the development Examination of adaptive responses to alcohol in humans
and maintenance of AUD is a critical public health need (8). requires rigorous longitudinal investigations in persons who
One way to examine vulnerability to AUD is to character- develop AUD or progress in severity of the disorder. Such
ize acute subjective responses to alcohol at different stages of prospective, repeated-measurement studies of acute alcohol
development of the disorder (9). Two large studies have responses are challenging, as they are time and labor inten-
found that greater initial stimulation and reward responses sive and require high retention rates. Yet the knowledge from
to alcohol (10, 11), as well as lesser intoxicating and sedating such investigations is necessary to adequately test the transla-
responses (12), predict future drinking problems through tional significance of animal models of addiction to humans
young adulthood. These acute responses may change with (17) and thus to inform empirically based strategies for pre-
chronic heavy drinking as a result of neuroadaptations that vention, early intervention, and treatment. Whether the excit-
produce either tolerance or sensitization. Tolerance, or a atory, euphoric, or sedating effects of alcohol increase,
diminished response to the same dose of a drug after decrease, or remain constant over time in problem drinkers
repeated use, is a diagnostic criterion for AUD. Conversely, represents an unresolved issue in clinical psychiatry.

See related features: Editorial by Dr. Ray and colleagues (p. 483), CME course (online and p. 571) and Video by Dr. Pine (online).

560 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021


SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL IN ALCOHOL USE DISORDER

In the Chicago Social Drinking Project, we undertook such inclusion criteria were age between 21 and 35 years, weight
an analysis in our first cohort and documented that, compared between 110 and 210 pounds, good general health, not preg-
with lighter drinkers, young adult heavy drinkers were more nant or lactating, no current or past major medical or psy-
sensitive to the stimulating, motivating (“wanting”), and chiatric disorders, including DSM-IV alcohol and substance
rewarding (“liking”) effects of alcohol and were less sensitive dependence (other than nicotine), and no current use of any
to its sedative effects (10). Although stimulation and sedation psychotropic medications. Participants were included if they
were inversely correlated (18), we found that higher alcohol met criteria for being either a high- or low-risk drinker. This
stimulation, wanting, and liking, and not lower sedation, pre- distinction was defined by a predominant adult pattern of
dicted progression of AUD symptoms 5–6 years after the ini- drinking (and minimally for the past 2 years). High-risk,
tial alcohol challenge in heavy drinkers (11). Further, when heavy drinkers reported weekly binge drinking of $5 drinks
participants were retested with alcohol 5–6 years later, heavy for men, $4 drinks for women, and regular consumption of
drinkers who developed more AUD symptoms reported per- 10–40 drinks per week. Low-risk, light drinkers reported
sistently greater stimulation, wanting, and liking after con- rare or no binges and regular consumption of 1–5 drinks per
suming alcohol (at both the initial test and 5 years later), week. These criteria were based on established high- and
whereas light drinkers did not report experiencing these low-risk drinking guidelines (20, 21) and were consistent
euphoric and motivating responses (19). However, the testing with previous studies (22–25). Candidates underwent medi-
was limited to a 5-year reexamination period when partici- cal and psychiatric screening to determine eligibility for an
pants were just entering their fourth decade of life, which alcohol challenge (for details, see the supplemental methods
may not have been sufficient to fully examine AUD develop- section of the online supplement).
ment. In addition, the previous study focused mainly on
heavy drinkers and did not allow for an integrated examina- Laboratory Procedures
tion of the trajectory of both light and heavy drinkers. At each of the three testing phases, participants attended two
We now report on a more comprehensive and extended individual 4- to 5-hour laboratory sessions, one with alcohol,
10-year follow-up and reexamination of acute responses to
and the other with placebo, in randomized order and under
alcohol in the Chicago Social Drinking Project. We shift the
double-blind conditions. Sessions were conducted in a com-
analytical focus to examine differences in the acute responses
fortable living room–like environment, separated by at least
to alcohol, based on who, among the entire sample, did or
24 hours. The sessions were identical except for the alcohol
did not manifest AUD after a decade of natural drinking. The
content of the beverage, 0.8 g/kg of alcohol (adjusted for sex),
approach comprises a three-phase assessment: initial, 5-year,
or placebo with a 1% alcohol taste mask. Upon arrival, objec-
and 10-year examinations of alcohol responses from young
tive breath tests confirmed recent alcohol abstinence, and a
adulthood through middle age. We evaluated the existence of
urine sample was collected for women to verify nonpreg-
adaptive changes in subjective responses to alcohol consump-
nancy. Subjective, objective, and alcohol breath tests were
tion in the development and progression of addiction. Our
obtained throughout the session (for more details, see the
goal was to determine whether AUD is marked by increases,
supplemental methods section in the online supplement).
decreases, or no change in sensitivity to alcohol’s stimulating,
Starting at experimental time 0, the participant received
motivating, rewarding, or sedating effects.
the beverages in lidded, clear plastic cups in two equal por-
tions and, in the presence of the research assistant, con-
METHODS sumed each portion over two 5-minute periods, separated
The Chicago Social Drinking Project is a multicohort, by a 5-minute rest (22, 23, 26). The alternative substance
double-blind study with repeated, within-subject laboratory paradigm (27) was employed to reduce expectancy effects
assessments of acute responses to alcohol compared with by informing each participant that the beverage may contain
placebo, combined with long-term follow-up of drinking a stimulant, sedative, alcohol, placebo, or a combination of
behaviors and AUD symptoms. The study was approved by these substances. At the end of each approximate 4-hour
the University of Chicago institutional review board, and session, when breath alcohol concentration was ,0.04 g/dL
participants provided written informed consent. Testing ses- (28), the participant was transported home by a car service.
sions were conducted at the Clinical Addictions Research Identical procedures and instructions were followed for
Laboratory at the University of Chicago. Participants the 5- and 10-year laboratory testing reexamination phases.
attended initial laboratory sessions from March 2004 to July For ethical reasons, participants who had major medical or
2006. They underwent regular follow-up interviews and psychiatric contraindications (N53), who were pregnant or
were invited to participate in identical double-blind alcohol nursing (N53), or who were abstinent from alcohol (N53)
and placebo reexaminations 5 and 10 years later (see the were not eligible for the final 10-year reexamination sessions.
CONSORT diagram in Figure S1 in the online supplement). As nearly half (49%) of the sample no longer resided in the
area, transportation to Chicago, lodging accommodations, and
Initial Screening and Enrollment other per diem expenses were provided as warranted. Reex-
Recruitment was conducted through local media and Inter- amination sessions were scheduled for the same month as the
net advertisements and word-of-mouth referrals. Initial initial testing; the mean interval from participants’ initial

Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 561


KING ET AL.

session was 61 months (SD53.0) to the year-5 session and 122 because DSM-5 was not yet developed when the follow-up
months (SD53.7) to the year-10 session. assessments were initiated in 2005. This trajectory analysis
(34) included a zero-inflated Poisson mixture model with a
Outcome and Measures cubic trajectory for each subgroup. The number of trajectory
In the testing sessions, measures were obtained before groups was determined by the Bayesian information crite-
beverage consumption and repeated 30, 60, 120, and rion using 2ln(B10) $ 10 as strong evidence for rejecting the
180 minutes after consumption. To maintain blinding, null model. This analysis showed that a three-trajectory
the alcohol breath tests (Alco-Sensor IV, Intoximeter; group model best fit the data: a low AUD symptom sub-
St. Louis) displayed 0.000 g/dL during real-time assess- group (N5103), an intermediate AUD symptom subgroup
ments, and the actual values were downloaded later. (N562), and a high AUD symptom group (N519). The AUD
The primary dependent measures were scores on the symptoms across these subgroups corresponded to the fre-
stimulation and sedation subscale of the 14-item quency of heavy drinking across follow-up (Figure 1B). Of
Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (29) and responses to note, the classifications of individuals in the AUD symptom
two items from the Drug Effects Questionnaire (100- trajectory subgroup were fairly stable, relative to the classifi-
mm visual analogue scale) for hedonic reward (“do you cations of initial heavy drinkers in the 5-year analysis (11).
LIKE the effect you are feeling now?”) and motivational Eighty-one percent of the participants remained in their
salience (“would you like MORE of what you con- respective low, intermediate, and high AUD symptom sub-
sumed, right now?”) (30, 31). The surveys instructed group, while 15% moved by one category to the next higher
participants to focus on their current mood state and symptom group and 4% changed to the next lower group.
did not reveal the beverage content (32). Stimulation, Demographic and drinking characteristics for the AUD
sedation, liking, and wanting were examined across the symptom subgroups are shown in Table 2.
breath alcohol concentration curve by subtracting the
placebo from the alcohol response at each time point. Statistical Analysis
Secondary measures were general drug (the “feel-drug” Demographic and drinking characteristics were compared
item of the Drug Effects Questionnaire) and physiologi- between the AUD1 and AUD2 groups by t tests and chi-
cal responses, including heart rate and salivary cortisol square tests, as appropriate. Acute alcohol responses were
measures. analyzed by generalized estimating equation models (35)
that included group, time, and phase, with the latter two
Follow-Up Assessments variables treated as continuous variables. To assess potential
After each participant’s initial testing, follow-up assessments nonlinearity over time, models for each alcohol response
were conducted at 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 8, and 10 years. No subject included linear, square, and cubic terms of time. Results
was lost to follow-up (i.e., unreachable throughout this revealed that stimulation only (and not sedation, liking, or
decade of participation), reflecting our protocol to attain wanting), as measured by the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale,
strong retention (33). Continuous participation was strong required the second- and third-order polynomial terms. This
(185 of 189 living participants), with four study dropouts. is consistent with previous work showing rapid increases in
The follow-up assessments included Internet or mailed sur- stimulation during the ascending breath alcohol concentra-
veys and a telephone or in-person administration of the tion limb, followed by an inflection point and sharp declines
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV to determine during the descending breath alcohol concentration limb
AUD symptoms. The main outcome from these assessments (10). Generalized estimating equation models were also used
was the presence (AUD1) or absence (AUD2) of the disor- for secondary measures, including general subjective
der in the last year of follow-up. Twenty-one percent of par- response (the “feel-drug” item of the Drug Effects Question-
ticipants (N539) met DSM-5 criteria for AUD at year 10, naire) and physiological effects (heart rate and cortisol
forming the AUD1 group. Of these, 20 met criteria for mild secretion), which are sensitive to alcohol (36–38). The AUD
AUD, 10 for moderate AUD, and nine for severe AUD. The symptom subgroups were assessed on alcohol responses in
remaining 79% did not meet AUD criteria and formed the generalized estimating equation models similar to those con-
AUD2 group. Of these, 124 reported no AUD symptoms, ducted in the main analysis to examine subgroup, time, and
and 21 reported one symptom. A review of the preceding 10 phase effects and their interactions. All generalized estimat-
years revealed that the AUD1 group showed more AUD ing equation models adjusted for age, race, education, family
symptoms than the AUD2 group and reported heavier history of alcoholism (39), breath alcohol concentration (as
drinking (Figure 1A and Table 1). a time-varying covariate), and the number of baseline AUD
In addition to the AUD group classifications based on the symptoms.
10-year follow-up, we employed a complementary approach
to determine trajectory subgroups based on the growth and
RESULTS
progression (or regression) in AUD symptom severity
throughout all follow-up time points (10, 11, 19). The sub- Participant retention in the Chicago Social Drinking Project
group analysis was based on DSM-IV symptom counts was high, with 99% (184 of 185) of the nondeceased active

562 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021


SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL IN ALCOHOL USE DISORDER

FIGURE 1. Mean alcohol use disorder (AUD) symptom count and heavy drinking occasions per month across 10 years of follow-up
in a study of the subjective responses to alcohol in the development and maintenance of AUDa

A
6 14

Heavy Drinking Occasions per Month


12
5

10
AUD Symptom Count

8
3
6

2
4

1 2

0 0
Initial 1 2 4 5 6 8 10 Initial 1 2 4 5 6 8 10
AUD+ at year 10
Follow-Up Year Follow-Up Year
AUD– at year 10
B
6 14

12
Heavy Drinking Occasions per Month
5

10
AUD Symptom Count

8
3
6

2
4

1
2

0 0
Initial 1 2 4 5 6 8 10 Initial 1 2 4 5 6 8 10
Follow-Up Year High AUD symptom count Follow-Up Year
Intermediate AUD symptom count
Low AUD symptom count

a
Panel A depicts results for the presence of AUD (AUD1 group) or the absence of AUD (AUD2 group) as determined at year 10, and panel B
depicts results for AUD symptom subgroups. Data shown are mean values, and error bars indicate standard error. The left panel depicts AUD
symptoms at each follow-up interval for each group, and the right panel depicts the frequency of binge drinking during follow-up. As shown
in panel A, the AUD1 and AUD2 groups were identified based on AUD symptom counts per DSM-5 at year 10 of follow-up. As shown in
panel B, three trajectory subgroups were identified based on symptom counts from DSM-IV criteria for alcohol abuse and alcohol dependence
(0–11 symptoms possible) over 10 years of follow-up, and symptom counts were ascertained during initial testing and at years 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 8,
and 10 (dotted lines represent model fits). Because the study began before DSM-5 was released, symptom counts for years 1–5 were based
on the 11 criteria for DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence (as reflected in the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV). For unity, these
DSM-IV criteria also were used for the follow-up in years 6–10 for trajectory analysis. Notably, only one symptom is different between the cri-
teria for DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence compared with those for DSM-5 AUD.

participants completing follow-up at 10 years (i.e., 184 out of 91% of the 178 and 179 participants eligible for reexamina-
190 participants from the original sample, or 97%; see Figure tion at each phase, respectively.
S1 in the online supplement). The 5-year and 10-year labora- The AUD1 and AUD2 groups at 10 years did not differ on
tory reexamination sessions were conducted in 156 and 163 most demographic characteristics; however, as expected, the
participants, respectively. These rates represent 88% and AUD1 group reported heavier alcohol consumption and

Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 563


KING ET AL.

TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of groups with alcohol use disorder (AUD) (AUD1) and without AUD (AUD2) at
year 10 at initial and year 10 testing in a study of the subjective responses to alcohol in the development and maintenance of AUD
AUD1 at Year 10 (N539) AUD– at Year 10 (N5145)
Significance
Characteristic Initial Testing Year 10 Testing Initial Testing Year 10 Testing Testing
N % N % N % N %
Male 25 64 – – 75 52 – – ns
Whitea 32 82 – – 108 74 – – ns
Family history of AUDb 7 18 – – 35 24 – – ns
Married or living with partner 3 8 17 44 20 14 98 68 ns
Nicotine dependencec 6 15 10 26 3 2 6 4 Group, p,0.01
Substance dependencec 3 8 1 3 1 1 0 0 ns
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Age (years) 24.6 3.0 34.6 3.0 25.9 3.2 35.9 3.2 ns
Education (years) 15.5 1.7 16.4 2.1 16.2 1.8 17.7 3.0 ns
Beck Depression Inventory score 4.1 3.3 6.5 5.7 2.2 2.6 3.2 3.8 ns
Spielberger State Anxiety t score 49.2 8.9 51.8 7.8 43.6 6.8 46.6 5.9 ns
AST (U/L) 21.7 6.7 21.0 5.3 22.6 9.5 20.5 10.4 ns
ALT (U/L) 20.1 10.6 22.1 11.3 22.1 17.2 21.5 16.8 ns
Drinking days in past monthd 14.3 6.9 20.6 6.3 9.7 5.1 15.0 6.9 Group, p,0.05
Heavy drinking days in past 7.5 4.5 13.7 6.6 3.5 4.3 4.9 4.9 Group-by-phase
monthd interaction, p,0.01
Alcohol problemse 11.3 5.0 18.7 6.2 6.8 4.6 8.6 5.4 Group-by-phase
interaction, p,0.01
AUD symptoms during follow-upe – – 4.5 1.8 – – 1.2 1.5 Group, p,0.001
a
Race was provided by participants from a list of options consistent with National Institutes of Health classifications.
b
Family history of AUD was defined as having one biological primary or two or more biological secondary relatives with AUD.
c
Nicotine dependence and substance dependence were determined using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID), indicating the number of
participants who met past-year criteria at year 1 and during any follow-up time point across 10 years.
d
From the timeline follow-back calendar for the month preceding initial testing and maxima across follow-up time points for days that any alcohol was
consumed and days with heavy drinking, defined as $5 drinks per occasion for men and $4 drinks for women, based on the standard definition of one
drink (i.e., 12 oz. of beer, 5 oz. of wine, or 1.5 oz. of liquor).
e
From the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test and the SCID, indicating the past year at initial testing and mean scores or the number of 11 DSM-IV
symptoms met across follow-up time points for 10 years.

more drinking problems than the AUD2 group at the 10-year concentration curve, and stimulation level increased in inten-
reexamination and at the initial testing phase (Table 1). Across sity through the reexamination phases (group-by-time-by-
the test phases, the groups did not differ significantly on aver- phase interaction, p50.016; Table 3). Alcohol also initially
age breath alcohol concentration (see Figure S2A in the online increased ratings of motivational salience (wanting) in the
supplement), with both showing a rapid rising breath alcohol AUD1 group relative to the AUD2 group across the entire
concentration limb that reached peak level 60 minutes after breath alcohol concentration curve, and wanting also
the initiation of the beverage consumption, followed by a slow increased in intensity through the reexamination phases
breath alcohol concentration declining limb (group-by-time- (group-by-phase interaction, p50.001). Initial hedonic reward
by-phase interaction: x255.78, df58, p50.672). Thus, while (liking) from alcohol was also higher in the AUD1 group
reflecting individual variability in alcohol pharmacokinetics as (p50.017), and this effect increased through reexamination,
they relate to oral administration (40), the mean values of but the escalation was not statistically significant (group-by-
breath alcohol concentration of the AUD groups did not sub- phase interaction, p50.104). Finally, both groups showed
stantially differ across the testing phases. increases in alcohol-induced sedation, but this effect did not
In terms of subjective response, alcohol produced mark- differ by group or test phase (group-by-phase interaction,
edly different effects in the 10-year AUD1 and AUD2 groups. p.0.385). Reanalyzing the data based on DSM-IV criteria for
These differences were evident starting at the initial testing alcohol dependence yielded similar results.
phase and increased further over the course of the reexamina- As with the AUD groups, for the AUD symptom groups,
tion phases (Figure 2). Overall, the AUD1 group exhibited breath alcohol concentration curves did not differ across
increasing sensitivity for pleasurable alcohol responses during subgroups (see Figure S2B in the online supplement). For
the decade of participation, while the AUD2 group showed alcohol responses, a similar pattern of results emerged for
low initial levels on these measures with little to no change the AUD symptom subgroups as for the AUD1 and AUD2
through the testing phases. Specifically, alcohol produced ini- groups (Figure 3). Relative to the low AUD symptom sub-
tially higher stimulation in the AUD1 group relative to the group, the intermediate and high AUD symptom subgroups
AUD2 group during the early portion of the breath alcohol exhibited initially heightened alcohol stimulation, wanting,

564 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021


SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL IN ALCOHOL USE DISORDER

TABLE 2. Characteristics of alcohol use disorder (AUD) symptom trajectory subgroups at initial testing and year 10 testing in a
study of the subjective responses to alcohol in the development and maintenance of AUD
Low AUD Symptom Intermediate AUD High AUD Symptom
Group (N5103) Symptom Group (N562) Group (N519)
Initial Year 10 Initial Year 10 Initial Year 10 Significance
Characteristic Testing Testing Testing Testing Testing Testing Testing
N % N % N % N % N % N %
Male 48 47 – – 40 65 – – 12 63 – – ns
Whitea 75 73 – – 50 81 – – 15 79 – – ns
Family history of 28 27 – – 10 16 – – 4 21 – – ns
AUDb
Married or living 15 15 71 69 6 10 37 60 2 10 7 37 ns
with partner
Nicotine 2 2 3 3 4 6 6 10 3 16 7 37 Group, p,0.05
dependencec
Substance 0 0 0 0 1 2 0 0 3 16 1 5 ns
dependencec
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Age (years) 26.2 3.3 36.2 3.3 24.8 3.0 34.7 3.0 25.3 2.9 35.3 2.9 ns
Education (years) 16.4 1.8 18.0 3.2 15.9 1.5 17.0 1.8 15.1 2.2 15.6 2.2 Group-by-phase
interaction,
p,0.05
Beck Depression 1.8 2.3 2.9 3.2 3.2 3.3 4.0 3.6 5.1 3.8 9.1 6.2 Group-by-phase
Inventory score interaction,
p,0.001
Spielberger State 50.1 12.7 46.5 4.6 49.6 10.1 48.2 6.9 49.2 11.4 53.4 8.2 Group-by-phase
Anxiety t score interaction,
p,0.001
AST (U/L) 22.2 4.8 19.3 7.4 22.5 6.8 22.8 12.9 23.0 7.7 20.8 6.5 ns
ALT (U/L) 21.9 11.9 19.6 14.6 21.2 9.5 24.6 17.8 22.1 14.0 22.9 14.6 ns
Drinking days in past 8.0 4.3 13.3 6.8 13.6 5.5 18.8 5.7 15.4 7.1 22.8 5.5 Group-by-phase
monthd interaction,
p,0.01
Heavy drinking days 2.0 3.4 3.0 3.8 6.9 4.0 10.0 4.5 9.0 4.6 16.7 6.9 Group-by-phase
in past monthd interaction,
p,0.01
Alcohol problemse 5.0 3.3 6.4 3.8 11.0 4.3 14.4 4.8 12.4 5.4 21.9 6.2 Group-by-phase
interaction,
p,0.01
AUD symptoms – – 0.5 0.7 – – 2.9 1.4 – – 6.1 1.3 Group, p,0.001
during follow-upe
a
Race was provided by participants from a list of options consistent with National Institutes of Health classifications.
b
Family history of AUD was defined as having one biological primary or two or more biological secondary relatives with AUD.
c
Nicotine dependence and substance dependence were determined using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID), indicating the number of
participants who met past-year criteria at year 1 and during any follow-up time point across 10 years.
d
From the timeline follow-back calendar for the month preceding initial testing and maxima across follow-up time points for days that any alcohol was
consumed and days with heavy drinking, defined as $5 drinks per occasion for men and $4 drinks for women, based on the standard definition of one
drink (i.e., 12 oz. of beer, 5 oz. of wine, or 1.5 oz. of liquor).
e
From the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test and the SCID, indicating the past year at initial testing and mean scores or the number of 11 DSM-IV
symptoms met across follow-up time points for 10 years.

and liking. For alcohol stimulation and wanting, these On secondary measures, alcohol increased feel-drug rat-
responses were potentiated through the reexamination ings during the rising breath alcohol concentration limb
phases (i.e., augmented across the decade of participation), (time, p50.026; see Table S1 in the online supplement); this
with liking remaining elevated but not increasing further as response became more intense through the reexamination
the decade progressed (Figure 3). Further, initial subjective phase for the AUD1 group (group-by-phase interaction,
responses to alcohol significantly predicted the frequency of p50.004). Alcohol-induced heart rate and cortisol increases
heavy drinking through follow-up; this behavioral outcome did not differ by group or across test phases (see Table S1
was used in our previous work (10), and associations were and Figure S3 in the online supplement). The same results
evident over the decade for liking and wanting (all r values, were observed when based on an analysis of AUD symptom
0.19; all p values, ,0.01), stimulation (r50.14, p50.06), and subgroups (see Table S1 and Figure S4 in the online supple-
sedation (r520.17, p,0.05). ment). For all analyses, there were no sex differences.

Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 565


KING ET AL.

FIGURE 2. Subjective alcohol responses during initial testing and at 5-year and 10-year reexaminations in alcohol use disorder
(AUD) groups as determined at year 10 in a study of the development and maintenance of AUDa

Initial Testing Year 5 Year 10


25 25 25
AUD+ at year 10
20 AUD– at year 10 20 20

15 15 15
Stimulation

10 10 10

5 5 5

0 0 0

–5 –5 –5
–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180
25 25 25

20 20 20

15 15 15
Sedation
Change Between Alcohol and Placebo Session Scores

10 10 10

5 5 5

0 0 0

–5 –5 –5
–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180
50 50 50

40 40 40

30 30 30

20 20 20
Liking

10 10 10

0 0 0

–10 –10 –10

–20 –20 –20


–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180
50 50 50

40 40 40

30 30 30
Wanting

20 20 20

10 10 10

0 0 0

–10 –10 –10

–20 –20 –20


–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180
Time (minutes) Time (minutes) Time (minutes)

a
Data shown are mean values, with error bars indicating standard error, of the change in scores (alcohol session scores minus placebo session
scores) for the four primary subjective response measures at each time point and at each testing phase for the presence of AUD (AUD1
group) at year 10 (N539) or the absence of AUD (AUD2 group) at year 10 (N5145). The AUD1 and AUD2 groups were identified based on
AUD symptom counts per DSM-5 at year 10 of follow-up. The y-axis ranges for liking and wanting differ slightly given the range of values and
because liking is based on a scale with the midpoint as neutral. Details on the statistical testing are provided in Table 3.

566 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021


SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL IN ALCOHOL USE DISORDER

TABLE 3. Summary of a generalized estimating equation analysis of primary alcohol response outcomes in groups with alcohol use
disorder (AUD) (AUD1) and without AUD (AUD2) at year 10 and in AUD symptom trajectory subgroupsa
Primary
Outcome b SE p b SE p b SE p b SE p b SE p b SE p
AUD group-by-phase AUD group-by-time AUD group-by-time-
AUD group Time Phase interaction interaction by-phase interaction
Stimulation 1.126 2.192 0.607 –4.089 4.383 0.351 –0.037 0.137 0.788 –0.305 0.289 0.292 6.027 6.300 0.339 2.541 0.987 0.010
Sedation –1.980 1.958 0.312 1.885 0.373 <0.001 –0.030 0.099 0.760 –0.182 0.210 0.385 –1.250 0.789 0.113 0.159 0.124 0.198
Wanting 5.534 6.093 0.364 –2.819 1.172 0.016 –0.830 0.332 0.012 2.203 0.702 0.002 –1.367 2.368 0.564 0.268 0.371 0.470
Liking 11.454 4.691 0.015 –8.699 0.996 <0.001 –0.574 0.282 0.042 1.091 0.598 0.068 –1.463 2.016 0.468 –0.148 0.316 0.639
AUD symptom group-
AUD symptom AUD symptom group- AUD symptom group- by-time-by-phase
trajectory subgroups Time Phase by-phase interaction by-time interaction interaction
Stimulation 0.759 1.380 0.582 –9.354 7.061 0.185 0.326 0.297 0.273 –0.281 0.176 0.111 5.104 3.820 0.181 1.818 0.600 0.002
Sedation –0.927 1.270 0.465 1.822 0.812 0.025 0.123 0.216 0.569 –0.125 0.128 0.328 –0.133 0.479 0.781 –0.001 0.075 0.991
Wanting 10.741 3.792 0.005 –4.727 2.475 0.056 –1.959 0.733 0.008 1.054 0.433 0.015 1.033 1.456 0.478 –0.090 0.229 0.693
Liking 8.712 2.972 0.003 –8.215 2.079 <0.001 –1.194 0.616 0.052 0.567 0.364 0.119 –0.553 1.223 0.651 –0.075 0.192 0.696
a
Data are results from a generalized estimating equation analysis and include the cubic term for stimulation and linear terms for sedation, wanting, and
liking as the best fit for these four variables. For stimulation, the three-way interaction of AUD group by time by phase and of AUD group by time-
squared by phase were also significant. All generalized estimating equations analyses controlled for baseline AUD count, breath alcohol concentration,
sex, race, and family history of alcoholism. Boldface indicates statistical significance.

DISCUSSION neuroadaptations in the circuits mediating motivational


salience, rendering them hypersensitive to the drug and its
AUD is phenotypically complex (41), and a better under- associated stimuli (14, 44). Until now, however, this idea
standing of factors that increase vulnerability to sustained had not been examined in a longitudinal manner in
excessive drinking is crucial for effective prevention, early humans. Although cross-sectional studies of the continuum
intervention, and treatment development (42). The present of alcohol use severity support this idea (45–49), to our
study provides the first repeated assessment of acute alcohol knowledge, our study, which includes repeated alcohol
responses in the same people across a substantial 10-year challenge testing in the same participants over an extensive
period of adulthood. The results comprise two main find- period, provides the first longitudinal support for the cen-
ings: first, heightened sensitivity to the pleasurable effects of tral tenet of this theory.
alcohol (stimulation, liking, and wanting) in young adult- There has been substantial disagreement among clini-
hood preceded the development of AUD through middle cians and researchers as to whether AUD is progressive,
age; and second, sensitivity to alcohol stimulation and want- what behavioral or subjective changes occur in response to
ing increased across repeated testing over a decade in per- alcohol over extended periods of use, and what processes
sons who developed AUD. Trajectory analyses of AUD underlie neuroadaptive changes that result from chronic
symptom progression over follow-up confirmed these find- drinking. It is known that chronic exposure to alcohol leads
ings by demonstrating amplification of alcohol stimulation to pharmacodynamic tolerance such that users can with-
and wanting as a function of AUD severity across a decade. stand a higher breath alcohol concentration than inexperi-
Notably, physiological responses such as heart rate and cor- enced drinkers before experiencing stupor, coma, and
tisol did not predict AUD development. Taken together, eventual death (50). Although tolerance to the subjective
these findings support the idea that the positive stimulating effects of alcohol is a hallmark symptom of AUD, it has been
and motivating effects of alcohol increase during the devel- challenging to track the development of such tolerance in
opment and maintenance of AUD. The findings do not sup- humans. In a similar vein, it has been difficult to document
port the idea that low sensitivity to alcohol increases the the progressive stages of allostasis, another construct
risk for AUD (12) or that a reward deficit stage emerges in believed to contribute to the disorder.
the progression to addiction (43). Several issues are relevant when reconciling the results
The study results are consistent with the central tenet of the present study with existing theory. First, retrospective
of the incentive-sensitization theory, namely, that motiva- patient reports suggesting the need for more alcohol to get
tional (“wanting”) but not hedonic (“liking”) processes the same effects as compared with when one first started
become sensitized in the progression to addiction (14). In drinking regularly are not sufficient support for chronic tol-
our study, the individuals who developed AUD over the erance. The well-controlled findings from the present study
10-year period reported increases in wanting alcohol over are not consistent with the notion of robust chronic toler-
time, while liking of alcohol remained high and stable over ance of alcohol’s subjective effects, so anecdotal patient
time. The incentive-sensitization theory is based on behav- reports of lessened effects of alcohol over time may be influ-
ioral and pharmacological studies in rodents demonstrating enced by factors other than acute alcohol response. Second,
wanting and liking as distinct components of reward and studies of chronic exposure to alcohol derived from animal
mediated by separate neural systems. In the animal studies, models of addiction, while providing critical information
repeated exposure to the drug produced incremental about the behavioral and physiological correlates of

Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 567


KING ET AL.

FIGURE 3. Subjective alcohol responses during initial testing and at 5-year and 10-year reexaminations in alcohol use disorder
(AUD) symptom trajectory subgroups in a study of the development and maintenance of AUDa

Initial Testing Year 5 Year 10


25 25 25
Symptom count
20 High AUD 20 20
Intermediate AUD
15 Low AUD 15 15
Stimulation

10 10 10

5 5 5

0 0 0

–5 –5 –5

–10 –10 –10


–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180

25 25 25

20 20 20

15 15 15
Change Between Alcohol and Placebo Session Scores

Sedation

10 10 10

5 5 5

0 0 0

–5 –5 –5

–10 –10 –10


–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180

40 40 40

30 30 30

20 20 20

10 10 10
Liking

0 0 0

–10 –10 –10

–20 –20 –20

–30 –30 –30


–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180

60 60 60

50 50 50

40 40 40

30 30 30
Wanting

20 20 20

10 10 10

0 0 0

–10 –10 –10

–20 –20 –20


–30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180 –30 0 30 60 120 180

Time (minutes) Time (minutes) Time (minutes)

a
Data are shown as mean values, with error bars indicating standard error, for low AUD symptom count (N5103), intermediate AUD symptom
count (N562), and high AUD symptom count (N519) trajectory groups at the initial and reexamination phases. Subjective response data are
change scores (alcohol session scores minus placebo session scores) at each time point. The y-axis ranges for liking and wanting differ
slightly given the range of values and because liking is based on a scale with the midpoint as neutral. Details on the statistical testing are pro-
vided in Table 3.

568 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021


SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL IN ALCOHOL USE DISORDER

consumption (51), are not able to shed light on changes in this issue is recent collaborative work by the first author
the euphoric effects of drinking in humans and how these (A.K.) and international colleagues showing that older ado-
change over time. Third, some theories of addiction, such as lescent heavy drinkers exhibit sensitivity to alcohol stimula-
allostasis, indicate development of a deficit in reward or a tion with heightened tonic wanting (58), so prospective
change in aversive states and stress responses. Although our studies with younger participants, perhaps in locations
data do not provide support for this idea, it is possible that where adolescent drinking is permitted, would be valuable in
this putative stage requires more than a decade to develop determining the earliest precipitants of adaptive responses to
or that it exists only in excessive drinkers prone to negative alcohol. Third, although our sample developed AUD at a
affect or extremely aversive withdrawal. Alternatively, while higher rate than that of 12.7% in the general population (1),
certain tenets of the allostasis theory may relate to animal the final sample size of the AUD1 group was modest. How-
models, they may not translate to manifestations of AUD as ever, using two similar but not overlapping outcome
observed within the demographic characteristics of our approaches may ameliorate concerns about sample size.
study sample. Importantly, our within-subject, longitudinal Comparing the AUD1 and AUD2 groups at year 10 pro-
findings demonstrated that the perceived pleasurable effects vided a practical clinical endpoint used by clinicians, and
of alcohol are not diminished as AUD severity progressed notably, 64% of AUD1 subjects met DSM-IV alcohol depen-
during a 10-year period. However, cross-sectional neuroim- dence at some point through follow-up, compared with 7%
aging studies suggest that heavy drinkers relative to lighter in the AUD2 group. The AUD symptom trajectory construct
social drinkers exhibit reduced activation of the nucleus provided a data-driven approach, affording consistency with
accumbens after alcohol infusion (52–54). Although it is dif- our previous work (11, 19). All of the participants in the high
ficult to reconcile the contrasting findings, given the differ- AUD symptom subgroup met alcohol dependence through
ences in samples and methodologies, we may speculate that follow-up, compared with 26% and 0% in the intermediate
other neurobiological substrates (i.e., the basal ganglia) or and low AUD symptom groups, respectively. Although an
circuits underlie the persistent positive subjective response examination of consequences and distress related to exces-
to alcohol. Nevertheless, this possibility highlights the need sive drinking was beyond the scope of this article, persons in
for studies employing both neurobiological techniques and the AUD1 group had a higher likelihood of seeking advice
validated subjective measures. or help with drinking or of seriously considering getting help
The Chicago Social Drinking Project has several relative to the AUD2 group (49% compared with 8%).
strengths, including a prospective design with alcohol and In sum, this study is the most extensive repeated human
placebo testing at three phases, outstanding retention, and testing study of its kind, examining the adaptive processes
determination of drinking and AUD symptoms from early to underlying the development and maintenance of AUD.
middle adulthood. Most notably, we were able to attain Alcohol-induced subjective stimulation increased over time,
near-perfect retention of participants across the decade. The and euphoric processes did not wane as AUD symptoms
inclusion of dual follow-up outcomes, that is, categorical emerged. Instead, these effects either stayed the same or
AUD diagnosis and trajectory analyses of symptom growth increased, challenging the notion that conventional chronic
over time, provides a powerful picture of the development tolerance to alcohol’s subjective effects plays a key role in
of alcohol problems (55). Relatedly, as this longitudinal study escalation of excessive drinking. The findings are consistent
began in 2004, nearly a decade before the 2013 release of with the incentive-sensitization theory, as alcohol wanting
DSM-5, the trajectory analysis was based on DSM-IV symp- increased across a decade in persons developing AUD. How-
tom counts for alcohol abuse and dependence. Thus, symp- ever, the study results were not consistent with the allostasis
tom counts may have been “undercounts,” and the actual model of a progressive deficit in reward. In terms of preven-
symptom count severity may well have been higher if DSM- tion of alcohol problems, rather than a sole focus on toler-
5 were available because DSM-IV included the item for legal ance, young adults might be informed that marked
problems, which was determined to have infrequent stimulant-like, pleasurable, and appetitive effects after con-
endorsement (56), and did not include the item for craving, suming alcohol are risk factors for the development and
which is more often endorsed (57). maintenance of addiction. Finally, pharmacological and
The study also has limitations. First, the standardized behavioral interventions focused on reducing positive
body weight–adjusted and sex-adjusted fixed dose of alcohol rewarding effects and motivational salience during acute
was chosen to produce a sharp rise in breath alcohol concen- alcohol consumption may be crucial in future medication
tration to minimize variability. However, this procedure did development and treatment of AUD.
not allow for other clinically relevant aspects of drinking,
including both self-paced drinking and choice to drink in the
AUTHOR AND ARTICLE INFORMATION
presence of both alcohol and other reinforcers or consequen-
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of
ces. Second, we were not able to test participants younger
Chicago, Chicago (King, Vena, deWit); Department of Epidemiology,
than 21 years of age, and it is possible that alcohol-related Mailman School of Public Health, and Department of Psychiatry, Col-
changes had already occurred before the participants lege of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York
enrolled in the Chicago Social Drinking Project. Relevant to (Hasin); Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of

Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 569


KING ET AL.

Medicine, Indianapolis, and Department of Biomedical Engineering, 14. Robinson TE, Berridge KC: The neural basis of drug craving: an
Purdue University, West Lafayette, Ind. (O’Connor); Department of incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res
Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, University of Illinois at Chicago, Rev 1993; 18:247–291
Chicago (Cao). 15. Koob GF, Le Moal M: Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregu-
Send correspondence to Dr. King (aking@bsd.uchicago.edu). lation. Science 1997; 278:52–58
16. Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, et al: Excessive ethanol drinking
Presented in part at the 41st annual meeting of the Research Society on
following a history of dependence: animal model of allostasis.
Alcoholism, San Diego, June 16–20, 2018.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 22:581–594
Supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism 17. Spanagel R: Animal models of addiction. Dialogues Clin Neurosci
grants AA-013746 to Dr. King, AA-023839 to Dr. Cao, AA-07611 to Dr. 2017; 19:247–258
O’Connor, and AA-025309 to Dr. Hasin, and by NIDA grants 18. King AC, Cao D, deWit H, et al: The role of alcohol response
DA-043469 to Drs. Vena and deWit and DA-018652 to Dr. Hasin. phenotypes in the risk for alcohol use disorder. BJPsych Open
ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT00961792. 2019; 5:e38
The authors thank Patrick McNamara for project coordination, data 19. King AC, Hasin D, O’Connor SJ, et al: A prospective 5-year
collection, and database management and Royce Lee, M.D., and Jon re-examination of alcohol response in heavy drinkers progress-
Grant, M.D., for medical supervision and oversight. ing in alcohol use disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2016; 79:489–498
20. Dawson DA: US low-risk drinking guidelines: an examination of
Dr. King has served on an advisory board for Pfizer and as a consultant four alternatives. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2000; 24:1820–1829
to the Respiratory Health Association. The other authors report no 21. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism: Rethinking
financial relationships with commercial interests.
Drinking (https://www.rethinkingdrinking.niaaa.nih.gov)
Received March 3, 2020; revision received July 13 2020; accepted 22. King AC, Houle T, de Wit H, et al: Biphasic alcohol response dif-
August 10, 2020; published online Jan. 5, 2021. fers in heavy versus light drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2002;
26:827–835
REFERENCES 23. King AC, Epstein AM: Alcohol dose-dependent increases in
1. Grant BF, Chou SP, Saha TD, et al: Prevalence of 12-month alco- smoking urge in light smokers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2005;
hol use, high-risk drinking, and DSM-IV alcohol use disorder in 29:547–552
the United States, 2001–2002 to 2012–2013: results from the 24. McCaul ME, Wand GS, Eissenberg T, et al: Naltrexone alters
National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Condi- subjective and psychomotor responses to alcohol in heavy drink-
tions. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74:911–923 ing subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000; 22:480–492
2. McKetta S, Keyes KM: Heavy and binge alcohol drinking and 25. Wiers RW, van de Luitgaarden J, van den Wildenberg E, et al:
Challenging implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in
parenting status in the United States from 2006 to 2018: an anal-
young heavy drinkers. Addiction 2005; 100:806–819
ysis of nationally representative cross-sectional surveys. PLoS
26. Brumback T, Cao D, King A: Effects of alcohol on psychomotor
Med 2019; 16:e1002954
performance and perceived impairment in heavy binge social
3. Hasin DS, Shmulewitz D, Keyes K: Alcohol use and binge drink-
drinkers. Drug Alcohol Depend 2007; 91:10–17
ing among U.S. men, pregnant and non-pregnant women ages
27. Conrad M, McNamara P, King A: Alternative substance para-
18–44: 2002–2017. Drug Alcohol Depend 2019; 205:107590
digm: effectiveness of beverage blinding and effects on acute
4. Griswold MG, Fullman N, Hawley C, et al: Alcohol use and bur-
alcohol responses. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2012; 20:382–389
den for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: a systematic
28. National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism:
analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet
Recommended Council Guidelines on Ethyl Alcohol Administra-
2018; 392:1015–1035 tion in Human Experimentation. 2005 (https://www.niaaa.nih.
5. Chassin L, Pitts SC, Prost J: Binge drinking trajectories from
gov/Resources/ResearchResources/job22.htm#levels)
adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: pre- 29. Martin CS, Earleywine M, Musty RE, et al: Development and
dictors and substance abuse outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol validation of the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale. Alcohol Clin
2002; 70:67–78 Exp Res 1993; 17:140–146
6. Zucker RA, Wong MM, Clark DB, et al: Predicting risky drinking 30. Johanson CE, Uhlenhuth EH: Drug preference and mood in
outcomes longitudinally: what kind of advance notice can we humans: diazepam. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1980; 71:269–273
get? Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2006; 30:243–252 31. Morean ME, de Wit H, King AC, et al: The drug effects ques-
7. Parry CD, Patra J, Rehm J: Alcohol consumption and non- tionnaire: psychometric support across three drug types. Psycho-
communicable diseases: epidemiology and policy implications. pharmacology (Berl) 2013; 227:177–192
Addiction 2011; 106:1718–1724 32. Rueger SY, McNamara PJ, King AC: Expanding the utility of the
8. Rehm J: The risks associated with alcohol use and alcoholism. Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (BAES) and initial psychometric
Alcohol Res Health 2011; 34:135–143 support for the Brief-BAES (B-BAES). Alcohol Clin Exp Res
9. Ray LA, Bujarski S, Roche DJO: Subjective response to alcohol 2009; 33:916–924
as a Research Domain criterion. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2016; 33. Smith LJ, McNamara PJ, King AC: Optimizing follow-up and
40:6–17 study retention in the 21st century: advances from the front line
10. King AC, de Wit H, McNamara PJ, et al: Rewarding, stimulant, in alcohol and tobacco research. Drug Alcohol Depend 2017;
and sedative alcohol responses and relationship to future binge 175:171–178
drinking. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68:389–399 34. Jones BL, Nagin DS: A note on a Stata plugin for estimating group-
11. King AC, McNamara PJ, Hasin DS, et al: Alcohol challenge based trajectory models. Sociol Methods Res 2013; 42:608–613
responses predict future alcohol use disorder symptoms: a 35. Liang K-Y, Zeger SL: Longitudinal data analysis using general-
6-year prospective study. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75:798–806 ized linear models. Biometrika 1986; 73:13–22
12. Schuckit MA: Low level of response to alcohol as a predictor of 36. Conrod PJ, Peterson JB, Pihl RO: Reliability and validity of
future alcoholism. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:184–189 alcohol-induced heart rate increase as a measure of sensitivity to
13. Wise RA, Bozarth MA: A psychomotor stimulant theory of the stimulant properties of alcohol. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
addiction. Psychol Rev 1987; 94:469–492 2001; 157:20–30

570 ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021


SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO ALCOHOL IN ALCOHOL USE DISORDER

37. Newlin DB, Byrne EA, Porges SW: Vagal mediation of the effect 49. Bujarski S, Jentsch JD, Roche DJO, et al: Differences in the sub-
of alcohol on heart rate. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1990; 14:421–424 jective and motivational properties of alcohol across alcohol use
38. King A, Munisamy G, de Wit H, et al: Attenuated cortisol severity: application of a novel translational human laboratory
response to alcohol in heavy social drinkers. Int J Psychophysiol paradigm. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018; 43:1891–1899
2006; 59:203–209 50. Le AD, Mayer JM: Aspects of alcohol tolerance in humans and
39. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Hermansen L, et al: Alcohol prob- experimental animals, in Pharmacological Effects of Ethanol on
lems in adoptees raised apart from alcoholic biological parents. the Nervous System. Edited by Deitrich RA, Erwin VG. Boca
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1973; 28:238–243 Raton, Fla, CRC Press, 1995, pp 251–268
40. Ramchandani VA, Plawecki M, Li TK, et al: Intravenous ethanol 51. Tabakoff B, Hoffman PL: Animal models in alcohol research.
infusions can mimic the time course of breath alcohol concentra- Alcohol Res Health 2000; 24:77–84
tions following oral alcohol administration in healthy volunteers. 52. Gilman JM, Ramchandani VA, Davis MB, et al: Why we like to
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2009; 33:938–944 drink: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the
41. Reilly MT, Noronha A, Goldman D, et al: Genetic studies of alco- rewarding and anxiolytic effects of alcohol. J Neurosci 2008;
hol dependence in the context of the addiction cycle. Neuro- 28:4583–4591
53. Gilman JM, Ramchandani VA, Crouss T, et al: Subjective and
pharmacology 2017; 122:3–21
neural responses to intravenous alcohol in young adults with
42. Kranzler HR, Soyka M: Diagnosis and pharmacotherapy of alco-
light and heavy drinking patterns. Neuropsychopharmacology
hol use disorder: a review. JAMA 2018; 320:815–824
2012; 37:467–477
43. Koob GF, Le Moal M: Addiction and the brain antireward sys-
54. Spagnolo PA, Ramchandani VA, Schwandt ML, et al: Effects of
tem. Annu Rev Psychol 2008; 59:29–53
naltrexone on neural and subjective response to alcohol in
44. Berridge KC, Robinson TE: What is the role of dopamine in
treatment-seeking alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol Clin Exp
reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?
Res 2014; 38:3024–3032
Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1998; 28:309–369 55. Schuckit MA: The answer you get depends on the question you
45. Hobbs M, Remington B, Glautier S: Dissociation of wanting and ask. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75:754–755
liking for alcohol in humans: a test of the incentive-sensitisation 56. Saha TD, Chou SP, Grant BF: Toward an alcohol use disorder
theory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005; 178:493–499 continuum using item response theory: results from the National
46. Ostafin BD, Marlatt GA, Troop-Gordon W: Testing the incentive- Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psy-
sensitization theory with at-risk drinkers: wanting, liking, and chol Med 2006; 36:931–941
alcohol consumption. Psychol Addict Behav 2010; 24:157–162 57. Murphy CM, Stojek MK, Few LR, et al: Craving as an alcohol
47. Cofresı RU, Bartholow BD, Piasecki TM: Evidence for incentive use disorder symptom in DSM-5: an empirical examination in a
salience sensitization as a pathway to alcohol use disorder. Neu- treatment-seeking sample. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2014;
rosci Biobehav Rev 2019; 107:897–926 22:43–49
48. Bujarski S, Ray LA: Subjective response to alcohol and associated 58. Chavarria J, Fridberg DJ, Obst E, et al: Subjective alcohol
craving in heavy drinkers vs. alcohol dependents: an examination responses in high- and low-risk adolescents: results from the
of Koob’s allostatic model in humans. Drug Alcohol Depend Dresden Longitudinal Study on Alcohol Use in Young Adults.
2014; 140:161–167 Addiction (in press)

Continuing Medical Education Examination Questions: King et al.


You can earn CME credits by read- 1. What theory purports that addiction involves the development of a deficit in reward
ing this article. Three articles in every or change in aversive states and stress response?
American Journal of Psychiatry issue a. Incentive-sensitization.
comprise a short course for up to 1 b. Tolerance.
AMA PRA Category 1 Credit™ each. The c. Allostasis.
course consists of reading the article d. Disease model.
and answering three multiple-choice
questions with a single correct answer. 2. Heightened sensitivity to what response domain(s) increased across a decade of
CME credit is issued only online. Read- repeated testing in persons developing alcohol use disorder?
ers who want credit must subscribe to a. Stimulation and wanting.
the AJP Continuing Medical Education b. Sedation.
Course Program (psychiatryonline. c. Cortisol and heart rate.
org/cme), select The American Jour- d. Attention bias.
nal of Psychiatry at that site, take the
course(s) of their choosing, complete 3. How might the findings inform providers treating patients concerned about
an evaluation form, and submit their exacerbations in drinking and symptoms of alcohol use disorder?
answers for CME credit. A certificate a. Inform patients that their adrenal response to alcohol is overactive.
for each course will be generated upon b. Discuss that abstinence is the only way to cure addiction.
successful completion. This activity is c. Tell patients that most people respond to alcohol the same way.
sponsored by the American Psychiatric d. Discuss that experiencing more rewarding and stimulating responses may put
Association. patients at risk for continued excessive drinking and addiction.

Am J Psychiatry 178:6, June 2021 ajp.psychiatryonline.org 571

You might also like