Attitudes Behind Consumers' Willingness To Use Functional Foods

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803

www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual

Attitudes behind consumers’ willingness to use functional foods


Nina Urala *, Liisa L€
ahteenm€
aki
VTT Biotechnology, P.O. Box 1500, FIN-02044 VTT, Finland
Received 17 September 2003; received in revised form 2 February 2004; accepted 23 February 2004
Available online 24 March 2004

Abstract
So-called functional foods are a new category of products that promise consumers improvements in targeted physiological
functions. The objective of this study was to quantify the attitudes behind consumers’ (n ¼ 1158) willingness to use these products.
Functional food-related statements formed seven factors describing consumers’ attitudes towards functional foods. These factors
were as follows: perceived reward from using functional foods, confidence in functional foods, necessity for functional foods,
functional foods as medicines, absence of nutritional risks in functional foods, functional foods as part of a healthy diet and the
health effects of functional foods vs. their taste. These attitude subscales differentiated between consumers (n ¼ 1158) in their re-
ported willingness to use functional foods. The best predictor for willingness to use functional foods was the perceived reward.
Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Functional foods; Consumer; Attitudes; Willingness to use; Perceived reward

1. Introduction manufacturers develop specific, functional products.


This is one reason why the development and marketing
Functional products are a new variety of foods that of functional foods is expensive and exceptionally risky.
promise targeted improvement in physiological func- Secondly, functionality creates a novelty aspect on the
tions in the body (Diplock et al., 1999). The market of food without necessarily changing the sensory quality of
functional food products is growing constantly and the the product. Consumers have to trust the information
future of functional foods looks quite optimistic (Gray, concerning the functional effect as the functional and
Armstrong, & Farley, 2003). The development and conventional product can appear to be identical when
marketing the functional foods can be, however, very used. The base product in which the health effect is added
challenging compared to the foods that conventionally can affect the credibility, too (Poulsen, 1999). Manufac-
have a high health image. turer has to offer the right information in a credible way
Functional foods differ from conventional foods in to the right consumers. On the other hand, the infor-
several ways. Firstly, conventional ‘healthy’ foods are mation concerning health effects is regulated by the
typically presented as types of foods contributing to a authorities and these regulations vary between EU-
healthy diet, e.g. low-fat products, high-fibre products, countries (Diplock et al., 1999). The role of the infor-
or vegetables, without emphasising the role of any single mation is crucial because consumers cannot perceive the
product. In functional foods, particular components are benefit directly from the product, unlike for instance
directly connected with well-defined physiological effects taste and other sensory characteristics.
and the health benefit is linked to a single product Thirdly, the manufacture of functional foods often
(L€ahteenm€aki, 2003). Usually scientifically proved sub- requires modern food technology since a constituent
stantiation about the health effect is required when needs to be added, removed or modified. This means
that there is a risk that functional products are perceived
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +358-9-456-5216; fax: +358-9-455-
as being less natural than conventional products and are
2103. thus avoided by those who value naturalness in food
E-mail address: nina.urala@vtt.fi (N. Urala). choices (Frewer, Scholderer, & Lambert, 2003).
0950-3293/$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.02.008
794 N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803

1.1. Previous research (NPI) which should have a negative correlation with
functional food attitudes if functional foods are re-
In general, Finnish consumers tend to be rather posi- garded as less natural than conventional products. The
tive towards functional foods. In one study, participants general attitude towards novelty in food was measured
(n ¼ 958) rated all health-related claims as advantages by the food neophobia scale (FNS, Pliner & Hobden,
when attached to a product used daily (Urala, Arvola, & 1992) which measures willingness to taste novel foods
L€ahteenm€aki, 2003). Respondents were very confident and has successfully predicted the choices of novel eth-
with the health-related information coming from the nic foods. However, as functional foods do not differ
authorities and quite confident with the information from conventional products in their sensory character-
coming from newspapers, retailers, work cafeterias and istics this scale may not be correlated with attitudes
even food manufacturers. Attitudes towards functional towards functional foods.
foods were more positive in Finnish consumers compared
to consumers in Denmark or the United States (Bech- 1.3. Aim of the study
Larsen & Grunert, 2003). Although the overall attitude in
Finland is positive, buyers of functional foods generate Thus, consumers’ functional food choices may be
impressions of being more innovative but not as nice as difficult to predict using existing attitude scales due to
buyers of conventionally healthy foods (Saher, Arvola, the limitations described above. To better understand
Lindeman, & L€ ahteenm€aki, 2004). what drives consumers to choose or not to choose
functional foods, their attitudes behind choices need to
1.2. Underlying attitudes be explored. Revealing and quantifying the underlying
attitudinal dimensions gives us a chance to identify what
Functional foods differ from conventional foods but are the factors that promote or obstruct willingness to
they are also viewed as being members of the particular use functional foods. Furthermore, it provides a tool to
food category to which they belong, rather than being identify those who are the most, and the least, potential
considered as a specific, homogenous group of products. users of functional foods. This enables more targeted
When consumers made choices between conventional marketing and distribution of product information.
and functional food products, their reasons behind The aim of this study was to quantify consumers’
functional food choices were different within the differ- attitudes towards so-called functional foods and to find
ent food categories (Urala & L€ ahteenm€aki, 2003). This the underlying dimensions that can predict respondents’
is supported by Poulsen (1999), who discovered that willingness to use functional food products.
both the enrichment substance and the type of product
enriched strongly affect consumers’ attitudes to func-
tional products among Danish consumers. Therefore, 2. Materials and methods
functional foods should be studied not as one homoge-
nous group but as separate products within the various In this study, the term ‘‘functional food’’ is used in a
food categories. broad sense and includes products that are marketed
Factors affecting food choice can be divided into three with health-related arguments, except nutritionally
central elements: the food, the consumers, and the envi- modified products (e.g. low-fat and low-salt products)
ronmental and economic issues. According to Shepherd that are regarded as part of a healthy diet as defined by
(1990), both an individual’s personal attitudes and envi- nutrition experts.
ronmental factors affect the food choice. Attitude can be
defined as ‘‘a psychological tendency that is expressed by 2.1. Attitude items
evaluating a particular entity with some degree of favour
or disfavour’’ (Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). Because attitudes The functional food-related statements were formu-
strongly affect food choice behaviour, they can be used to lated based on the results of a laddering study (Urala &
explain consumers’ food choices (Tuorila, 1997). L€ahteenm€aki, 2003) in which five central means-end
For predicting choices of functional foods, attitudes chains described the product attributes, consequences
related to health (Urala & L€ ahteenm€aki, 2003), natu- and values behind respondents’ food choices (healthi-
ralness (Frewer et al., 2003) and novelty are likely to be ness, taste and pleasure, security and familiarity, con-
central. Roininen, L€ ahteenm€ aki, and Tuorila (1999) venience and price). For a pilot study, 42, both
developed an attitude scale to measure general health positively and negatively worded statements, were for-
interest (GHI) in food choices. As functional foods differ mulated. These statements were tested using 33 students
from conventionally healthy foods, this scale is expected and staff members (23 males and 10 females, mean age
to correlate positively, but only moderately, with atti- 33 years) from the Technical University, Otaniemi.
tudes towards functional foods. The same authors also Based on the pre-test results, one statement was dis-
developed a scale for measuring natural product interest carded because of its high correlation with another
N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803 795

statement and three statements were reformulated be- Three validated attitude scales were used as reference
cause their distributions were strongly skewed. Also, 12 scales: the general health interest (GHI) scale (Roininen
new statements were reformulated. Altogether there et al., 1999), the natural product interest (NPI) scale
were 53 functional food-related statements in the final (Roininen et al., 1999) and the food neophobia scale
questionnaire. (FNS; Pliner & Hobden, 1992). The means of the GHI,
NPI and the sum of FNS were computed after reversing
2.2. Data collection and respondents the negative statements. On both general health interest
(GHI) scale and natural product interest (NPI) scale the
A Finnish marketing research company TOY-Re- respondents scored in the middle of the scale (mean 4.7,
search, Helsinki, recruited the participants in connection standard deviation 1.1 and mean 4.2, standard deviation
with two large interviewing studies. Respondents for 1.1, respectively). On the food neophobia scale (FNS),
these two studies were selected randomly from the the respondents scored also in the middle (sum 36,
phonebook and after the interview they were asked if standard deviation 10) of the scale.
they would like to answer to a questionnaire related to The respondents’ willingness to use eight functional
food. The questionnaire was sent to the voluntary foods and four reference products with positive health
respondents to be completed over a weekend. A total of images (Table 1) were determined on a 7-point scale
1158 respondents from all over Finland returned the (1 ¼ not at all willing, 7 ¼ extremely willing) and used as
completed questionnaires (response rate 60.2%). A slight the main target variables. This measurement was chosen
majority (58%) of the respondents were females and 42% because it can be related both to new and familiar foods,
of respondents were males. Respondents represented and even to product concepts. We also assumed that for
well different age groups. Mean age of respondents was consumers it would be easier to answer questions related
44 years with a range of 15–74 years. In five age groups to the consumers’ own motivations for using the prod-
(29 years or under, 30–39 years, 40–49 years, 50–59 years uct instead of evaluating the intention to buy as the
and 60 years or over) there were 21%, 19%, 23%, 18% availability of functional foods is not geographically
and 20% of the respondents, respectively. Most of the evenly distributed in Finland. The range of so-called
respondents (78%) had a minimum vocational or college- functional products is wide and in this study we used
level education and over half (62% of the respondents) generic product names complemented with one or sev-
bought at least three quarters of the food supplies used in eral brand names that belonged to the category. In this
their household. Compared with the average population way we would give similar stimuli that consumers meet
in Finland, there were more females in the sample than in when doing their shopping. As most health-related
the population in general and the respondents had a claims are not allowed, consumers have to rely on the
slightly higher level of formal education. information coming from other sources than packages.

Table 1
Descriptions of the product categories, respondents’ evaluations of willingness to use them, their evaluated healthiness and share (%) of respondents
who recognized ( ¼ at least 2 on the five-point scalea ), and used functional and reference products occasionally or frequently ( ¼ at least 4 on the five-
point scalea )
Category or product description Willingness to useb Evaluated healthinessc At least Users
recognized
Mean St. dev. Mean St. dev. % of respon- % of respon-
dents dents
FF Probiotic/gut-friendly juice 3.9 1.9 4.8 1.4 88 33
FF Juice with added calcium 4.0 1.9 4.8 1.4 85 27
FF Milk with added calcium 4.1 2.1 5.2 1.5 81 28
FF Cholesterol-lowering spread 3.9 2.0 5.3 1.5 98 21
FF Blood pressure lowering milk drink 3.3 2.0 4.6 1.6 65 7
FF Meat products with added fibre 2.9 1.7 4.1 1.5 35 4
FF Sweets and chewing gums with xylitol 5.5 1.9 5.4 1.6 98 80
FF Energy drinks 2.6 1.8 2.4 1.5 91 19
PH Low-salted food products 4.8 1.9 5.8 1.3 91 53
PH Low-fat cheese 5.5 1.8 5.8 1.3 99 78
PH Organic bread 5.0 1.9 5.6 1.5 95 50
PH Rye bread 6.6 1.1 6.6 0.8 100 97
FF ¼ functional food, PH ¼ product with positive health image ( ¼ reference product).
a
1 ¼ I do not recognize this product (or this kind of product), 2 ¼ I recognize this product, but I have not tasted it, 3 ¼ I have tasted this product,
but I do not use it, 4 ¼ I use this product occasionally and 5 ¼ I use this product frequently.
b
Evaluated on the 7-point scale (1 ¼ not at all willing, 7 ¼ extremely willing).
c
Evaluated on the 7-point scale (1 ¼ not at all healthy, 7 ¼ extremely healthy).
796
Table 2
Description of the attitude scales describing consumers’ willingness to use functional foods (FF)
Scale abbreviation, name and items in English Mean St. dev. Factor Original Original Alpha Variance
loading factor loading explained %
1. FF REW
Reward from using FF (scale mean ¼ 3.4, scale sd ¼ 1.2) 0.89 10.4
I get pleasure from eating functional foods 3.4 1.5 0.81 1
The idea that I can take care of my health by eating functional foods gives me pleasure 3.7 1.6 0.73 1
Functional foods make me feel more energetic 3.3 1.4 0.68 1
Functional foods help to improve my mood 3.4 1.6 0.68 1

N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803


My performance improves when I eat functional foods 3.4 1.5 0.60 1
I actively seek out information about functional foods 3.7 1.7 0.38 7 (0.41)
I willingly try even unfamiliar products if they are functional 2.9 1.7 0.38 1
2. FF CON
Confidence in FF (scale mean ¼ 4.0, scale sd ¼ 1.0) 0.87 9.7
The safety of functional foods has been very thoroughly studied 4.2 1.5 0.74 2
One can get reliable information about the health effects of functional foods 4.2 1.5 0.72 2
I trust the information given about health effects 4.3 1.5 0.64 2
Using functional foods is completely safe 4.5 1.5 0.57 2
Functional foods are science-based top products 3.7 1.5 0.54 2
R I do not believe that the stated health effects are based on thorough study 4.0 1.5 0.54 2
I believe that functional foods fulfil their promises 3.9 1.4 0.43 2
R In my opinion, nutrition experts do not know the health effects of functional foods 4.1 1.3 0.41 2
R Exaggerated information is given about health effects 3.4 1.4 0.36 2
3. FF NEC
Necessity for FF (scale mean ¼ 4.5, scale sd ¼ 1.2) 0.86 7.4
I can promote my health by consuming functional foods 4.4 1.5 0.59 3
It is great that modern technology allows the development of functional foods 4.9 1.4 0.58 3
R Functional foods are completely unnecessary 5.0 1.4 0.57 3
R Functional foods are a total sham 4.8 1.6 0.53 3
Functional foods promote my well-being 4.2 1.6 0.44 1 (0.47)
R For me functional foods are useless 4.2 1.6 0.43 3
Functional foods make it easier to follow a healthy lifestyle 4.8 1.6 0.42 3
R For a healthy person it is worthless to use functional foods 3.7 1.7 0.36 4 (0.36)
4. FF MED
FF as medicine (scale mean ¼ 3.6, scale sd ¼ 1.1) 0.68 6.2
R I only want to eat foods that do not have any medicine-like effects 3.5 1.8 0.49 4
R Medicines and foods should be kept strictly separate 3.0 1.6 0.43 4
R Functional foods are needed by people who have specific health problems 3.5 1.8 0.38 4
R The growing number of functional foods on the market is a bad trend for the future 4.5 1.6 0.37 3 (0.47)
R Health effects are not appropriate in delicacies 4.2 1.7 0.36 4
R It is pointless to add health effects to otherwise unhealthy foods 3.0 1.8 0.36 4
N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803 797

The exception was cholesterol lowering margarine and

R ¼ negative statement. These statements were recoded with reversed values before final data analysis. The loading on the factor is the loading on that scale where the item is now. Within parentheses
blood pressure lowering milk drink as they were mar-
keted with these statements in their packages.
3.9

4.1

2.8
Perceived healthiness and familiarity of the products
were also assessed. Healthiness was evaluated on a 7-
point scale (1 ¼ not at all healthy, 7 ¼ extremely heal-
thy). Familiarity with the products was measured on a
0.75

0.73

0.58
5-point scale in which 1 ¼ I do not recognize this prod-
uct (or this kind of product), 2 ¼ I recognize this prod-
uct, but I have not tasted it, 3 ¼ I have tasted this
product, but I do not use it, 4 ¼ I use this product
(0.43)

(0.42)

(0.36)
occasionally, and 5 ¼ I use this product frequently. The
scale was planned in such a way that unfamiliar and
familiar products, or products with different use fre-
quencies can be rated using the same scale. For instance,
spread is usually used several times per day but meat
5
5
5
1
5

6
6
4
6

7
7
4

products might only be used a few times per month.


Although the respondents were quite familiar with
functional food examples, the example products were
not used very frequently. Table 1 shows the mean scores
0.58
0.49
0.42
0.40
0.35

0.66
0.65
0.40
0.33

0.54
0.40
0.25

of respondents’ willingness to use, the perceived


healthiness ratings and the familiarity for both func-
tional and reference food products.
1.7
1.8
1.6
1.6
1.6

1.6
1.5
1.3
4.4

1.6
1.6
1.7

2.3. Data analysis

The rating scales of the negatively worded items were


3.3
3.5
3.7
3.9
2.8

3.6
3.9
3.6
4.5

3.4
3.1
3.6

reversed and the items with clearly skewed distributions


were rejected. After this procedure, 47 statements were
entered into final data analysis. Factor analysis (maxi-
I am prepared to compromise on the taste of a food if the product is functional
I do not believe that functional foods will ensure a long and healthy life for me

mum likelihood, varimax rotation) was used for


Functional foods are consumed mostly by people who have no need for them

For me, good taste is more important than health effects in a food product

grouping the statements into independent subsets


(Beardsworth, Haslam, Keil, Goode, & Sherrat, 1999).
Functional foods can repair the damage caused by an unhealthy diet

Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin and Bartlett’s tests were used for


are the loadings of the original factor that they have a highest loading on.
In some cases, functional foods may be harmful for healthy people
Absence of nutritional risk in FF (scale mean ¼ 3.9, scale sd ¼ 0.8)

testing the suitability of the correlation matrix for factor


A functional food can counteract an otherwise unhealthy diet

The new properties of functional foods carry unforeseen risks

analysis. According to the factor analysis (maximum


People would be healthier if they used more functional foods
FF as part of a healthy diet (scale mean ¼ 3.4, scale sd ¼ 1.2)

I do not buy functional foods unless I know how they taste


If used in excess, functional foods can be harmful to health

likelihood, varimax rotation), eight dimensions (eigen-


I can prevent disease by eating functional foods regularly

value greater than 1.0) were found. One of these


dimensions was discarded because there were no state-
ments strongly loaded on it. After this, the data were
Taste of FF (scale mean ¼ 3.3, scale sd ¼ 1.2)

analysed again and the number of factors was forced


into seven. These seven dimensions explained 44% of the
total variance (Table 2).
Factor scores were saved and used in repeated-mea-
sures ANOVA as between-subject covariates. In addi-
tion to the factor scores of FF-statements, three
reference scales were also used as between-subjects co-
variates. Within-subjects factor was formed from the
respondents’ scores of willingness to use 8 functional
and 4 conventionally healthy food products. The pur-
5. FF PART

7. FF TAST
6. FF RISK

pose of this analysis was to test how the factor scores


and reference scales (e.g. respondents’ attitudes) affect
the respondents’ willingness to use functional foods (8
example products) and conventional products (4 refer-
R

R
R
R
R

ence products).
798 N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803

2.4. Scale construction using functional foods (FF REW). It is noteworthy that
there were no taste-related statements on this factor.
In principle, attitude scales were formulated accord- Consumers who rate high on this scale perceive the use
ing to the factor loadings that were extracted from the of functional foods to be more rewarding than the
factor analysis. However, if an item did not fit into the consumers’ who rate lower.
main content of the factor, it was either discarded or The second factor was termed confidence in functional
shifted to a more suitable scale in which the item had a foods (FF CON) and it contains items that describe
moderate, although not the highest, loading. If an item consumers’ attitude towards the claims and information
was moderately loaded on several dimensions, the con- about functional foods or their health effects. In other
tent of the statement and the scale were compared and words, this factor describes how individuals trust the
the item was included in the topically most appropriate information and how strongly they believe in the sci-
scale or discarded. When the subscales were formulated, entific basis of promised health effects. Also, some
they were analysed one by one with factor analysis to see statements referred to consumers’ concerns of possible
if the scales separately load to one dimension. Cron- overdose of functional components. Statements were on
bach’s alpha was used in testing the reliability of the a more general level than the statements on the first
scales. factor and did not relate to one’s own behaviour as such.
After the scales were constructed, one scale (perceived Consumers who rate high on this scale trust the infor-
reward form using FF) was used as an example to show mation about functional food compared to the con-
the practical implication of the FF-scales. The respon- sumers who rate lower.
dents were divided into three groups according to their The third factor was termed necessity for functional
scale mean in this scale (lowest 25%, middle 50% and foods (FF NEC) and it describes how essential con-
upper 25%). The respondents in the lowest quartile sumers think that functional foods are for oneself or
experienced less reward from using functional foods people in general. Respondents who score high on this
compared to those respondents in the middle and upper scale think that by using functional foods they can
groups, i.e. the remaining 75% of the respondents. On promote a healthy lifestyle. This dimension describes
the other hand, respondents in the upper quartile per- only the general necessity of functional products and
ceived the greatest reward from using functional food does not refer to any disease.
compared to the remaining 75% of the respondents. The fourth factor was termed functional foods as
These three attitude groups were used in predicting medicines (FF MED) and consisted of items that de-
consumers’ willingness to use functional foods and thus scribe consumers’ attitude towards medicine-like effects
showing the effect of this attitude in a practical point of in functional foods. All the statements loaded on this
view. factor were negatively worded. Respondents who rated
The effects of gender, age and education on func- high on this scale have a more positive attitude towards
tional food attitudes were studied by 3-way ANOVA, in functional food products that could be used for the same
which the respondents’ scale values were explained by purposes as medicines, for instance for lowering cho-
gender (men or women), age group (29 years or under, lesterol.
30–39 years, 40–49 years, 50–59 years or 60 years or The fifth factor was termed functional foods as part of
over) and educational group (basic nine years, voca- a healthy diet (FF PART) and it describes the use of
tional school or college and university). functional foods as part of a normal, healthy diet.
Consumers who rate high on this scale perceive that the
use of functional food may counteract otherwise un-
3. Results healthy food choices.
The sixth factor describes the absence of nutritional
3.1. Attitudes towards functional foods risks of functional foods (FF RISK). This dimension
describes respondents’ suspiciousness towards possible
On the first dimension there were seven, only posi- harmful effects of functional foods. All the items on this
tively worded statements and it explained 10% of the factor were negatively worded stating a possible threat
variance after varimax rotation (Table 2). This scale or side-effects of functional foods.
comprised items that describe the rewarding aspect The seventh factor contains items concerning health
coming from the use of functional foods. It included effects vs. taste of functional foods (FF TAST). This taste
statements that reflected especially the feelings derived factor describes the possible conflict between a pleasant
from the individual’s own, personal consumption of taste and the health benefit when consumers’ choose
functional foods. The main focus of this dimension is functional foods. Consumers who score high on this
that using functional foods improves one’s health and scale are more ready to compromise on pleasant taste if
performance and thus gives a tool to take care of one- the product is functional compared to the consumers
self. This dimension was termed perceived reward from who score lower.
N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803 799

3.2. Consumers’ willingness to use functional foods neophobia scale (FNS) affected slightly negatively the
willingness to use all functional food examples, low-
Factor scores of FF-dimensions, e.g. consumers’ salted food products and organic bread. FNS was not
attitudes explained their willingness to use functional the strongest determinant in predicting the willingness to
and conventionally healthy foods differently (repeated use these products, but had consistently negative impact
measures ANOVA) (Table 3). on the willingness to use all product examples regardless
The clearest effects on the willingness to use func- of their nature (Table 3).
tional foods were found between respondents who had
different attitude towards the perceived reward from 3.3. Building the scales
using functional foods (FF REW). Respondents who
obtained reward from using functional foods were those Seven subscales were formed according to the highest
most willing to use all the functional food examples loadings on the seven factors described above. With the
except sweets with xylitol sweetened sweets. Among the exception of the health vs. taste factor (FF TAST), all
reference products FF REW affected positively the the subscales had good, or at least moderate, reliabilities
willingness to use low-fat cheese and organic bread, but when measured with the Cronbach alpha (Table 2).
the effect was lower than in functional foods. High, statistically significant intercorrelations between
In addition to the perceived reward, the necessity for FF-scales were found. The strongest factor, perceived
functional foods (FF NEC) affected positively the will- reward, correlated highly with taste (FF TAST), neces-
ingness to use functional food products. Particularly sity (FF NEC) and compensating dietary (FF PART)
strongly it predicted the willingness to use functional factors and moderately with trust on information about
products that have an impact of risk factors of cardio- functional foods (FF CON) (Table 4). Attitudes towards
vascular diseases, merely cholesterol lowering spread functional foods having medicinal effects (FF MED)
and blood pressure lowering milk drink, and gut friendly and lack of perceived risks (FF RISK) seemed to be
yoghurt. FF NEC affected positively also the willingness more independent subscales.
to use reference products. General health interest (GHI) correlated positively
Also the taste of functional foods (FF TAST) pre- but mildly with attitudes towards functional foods
dicted strongly the willingness to use products with (Table 4). The strongest correlation was between GHI
cardiovascular disease related health effects. The reli- and placing health effects before taste, whereas GHI
ability of is scale was, however, weak and we do not showed very weak correlation with medicine-like effects
discuss these results further. or risk. Natural product interest (NPI) had very low
Confidence in information about functional foods correlations with functional food attitudes and they
(FF CON) increased willingness to use probiotic juice were both positive and negative (Table 4). The food
and products with added fibre and calcium, but not neophobia scale (FNS) had low, both positive and
cholesterol-lowering spread or blood pressure lowering negative, correlations with functional food scales, hence
milk drink. negative attitudes towards functional foods were not
Functional foods as part of a healthy diet (FF PART) strongly related to food neophobia (Table 4).
and the absence of nutritional risks (FF RISK) seem to Perceived reward was used to show the implications
be less important when respondents rated their willing- of FF scales (Fig. 1) on willingness ratings. In particular,
ness to use functional foods. Although risk may be those with the most positive attitudes towards reward
perceived, they did not influence willingness to use from using functional foods rated their willingness
functional foods. Respondents’ attitude towards func- higher that the middle or low attitude groups (Fig. 1).
tional products as part of a healthy diet and perceived
risks affected only the willingness to use xylitol sweet- 3.4. Effects of gender, age and education on respondents’
ened sweets. People who saw functional foods as part of functional food attitudes
a healthy diet and did not perceive risks connected to
functional foods were less willing to use xylitol sweet- There were two subscales in which females and males
ened sweets. scored differently. Females scored slightly lower on both
General health interest (GHI) did not predict the the FF PART [F ð1; 1092Þ ¼ 6:0, p < 0:05] and FF
willingness to use any of the functional food products, TAST [F ð1; 1092Þ ¼ 4:4, p < 0:05] scales. Females ten-
but predicted strongly the willingness to use low-salted ded to think that they cannot counteract an otherwise
food products and low-fat cheese (conventionally heal- unhealthy diet by using functional foods and they did
thy products). Among the functional food products NPI not perceive functional foods to be as much a part of a
affected slightly negatively only the willingness to use healthy diet as did males. Females were also not as ready
energy drinks, but among conventionally healthy prod- as males to compromise on taste for a health benefit
ucts natural product interest (NPI) affected strongly from functional foods. Age affected the attitude scores
positively the willingness to use organic bread. Food on the four subscales. Younger respondents scored
800
N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803
Table 3
The effect (standardised parameter estimate: Beta) of the FF- and reference scales on the willingness to use functional and reference (conventionally healthy) products
Factor FF-scale Functional food examples Reference product examples
name Probi- Juice Milk with Cholesterol Blood pres- Meat prod- Energy Sweets and Low-salted Low-fat Organic Rye bread
otic/gut with added lowering sure lower- ucts with drinks chewing food prod- cheese bread
friendly added calcium spread ing milk added fibre gums with ucts
juice calcium drink xylitol
Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta Beta
Factor 1 scores FF REW 0.47 0.39 0.38 0.47 0.55 0.45 0.31 0.01 0.11 0.23 0.22 )0.07
Factor 2 scores FF CON 0.25 0.27 0.22 0.16 0.13 0.16 0.13 0.13 0.17 0.12 0.18 0.03
Factor 3 scores FF NEC 0.38 0.28 0.31 0.40 0.40 0.23 )0.03 0.23 0.28 0.20 0.24 0.20
Factor 4 scores FF MED 0.23 0.17 0.00 0.13 )0.04 0.16 0.10 0.20 0.06 0.08 0.09 )0.07
Factor 5 scores FF PART )0.09 )0.10 0.10 0.15 0.17 0.10 0.04 )0.26 )0.10 )0.11 )0.16 )0.06
Factor 6 scores FF RISK )0.04 )0.01 0.06 0.10 0.06 )0.03 0.04 )0.21 )0.16 0.01 )0.04 0.01
Factor 7 scores FF TAST 0.21 0.15 0.20 0.42 0.43 0.22 )0.02 )0.15 0.27 0.10 0.09 )0.05
GHI 0.13 0.06 0.05 0.10 0.11 )0.02 )0.10 0.02 0.54 0.55 )0.12 0.05
NPI 0.08 0.04 )0.01 )0.09 )0.14 )0.06 )0.17 0.04 )0.05 )0.13 0.69 0.07
FNS )0.14 )0.23 )0.16 )0.16 )0.18 )0.20 )0.19 )0.16 )0.20 )0.12 )0.36 )0.06
FF REW ¼ reward from using FF, FF CON ¼ confidence in FF, FF NEC ¼ necessity for FF, FF MED ¼ FF as medicine, FF PART ¼ FF as part of a healthy diet, FF RISK ¼ absence of
nutritional risk in FF, FF TAST ¼ taste of FF, GHI ¼ general health interest, NPI ¼ natural product interest, FNS ¼ food neophobia scale; statistically significant (p < 0:01) betas are written in bold.
N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803 801

Table 4
Correlation coefficients (Pearson) between FF and reference scales
FF REW FF CON FF NEC FF MED FF PART FF RISK FF TAST GHI NPI FNS
FF REW 1.00 0.34 0.19 0.11
FF CON 0.56 1.00 0.23 )0.01 0.00
FF NEC 0.68 0.72 1.00 0.28 0.09 0.01
FF MED 0.33 0.42 0.58 1.00 0.04 )0.05 )0.16
FF PART 0.68 0.56 0.60 0.29 1.00 0.22 0.10 0.16
FF RISK 0.27 0.45 0.38 0.31 0.21 1.00 0.08 )0.11 )0.04
FF TAST 0.75 0.43 0.55 0.28 0.55 0.19 1.00 0.46 0.31 0.11
FF REW ¼ reward from using FF, FF CON ¼ confidence in FF, FF NEC ¼ necessity for FF, FF MED ¼ FF as medicine, FF PART ¼ FF as part of
a healthy diet, FF RISK ¼ absence of nutritional risk in FF, FF TAST ¼ taste of FF, GHI ¼ general health interest, NPI ¼ natural product interest,
FNS ¼ food neophobia scale; statistically significant (p < 0:01) correlation coefficients are written in bold.

lowest 25% middle group upper 25%

extremely willing 7

2
Functional food examples Reference product examples
not at all willing
1
m

d
e

re

ks

ol
ad

ts

e
m

d
ic

es
in

ea

ea
iu

lit

uc
iu

fib

in
re
ju

dr

he

br
lc

br
xy
lc

dr

od
sp

d
ly

ca

ca

ilk

tc

ic

ye
de

ith
gy

pr
nd

an
m
d

-fa
d

R
in

ad

w
er

od
de
ie

de

rg
g
er

w
En
fr

s
in

ith
ad

fo
ad

O
m
w

Lo
ut

er
lo

gu

d
ith

ith
/g

te
s
ol

lo
tic

g
w

ct

al
er

in

-s
du
re
io

ilk

st

ew
ic

w
ob

su

ro
M

le
Ju

Lo
ch
ho

es
Pr

tp
pr

d
C

ea

an
od

ts
lo

ee
B

Sw

Fig. 1. Effect of consumers’ perceived reward from using FF (FF REW) on the willingness to use functional foods.

lower on the FF REW [F ð1; 1089Þ ¼ 10:4, p < 0:001], FF TAST between female and male were only 0.2 and
FF PART [F ð1; 1089Þ ¼ 10:2, p < 0:001] and FF TAST 0.3 point, respectively, on the 7-point scale and the scale
[F ð1; 1089Þ ¼ 20:8, p < 0:001] scales but slightly higher means between five age and four educational groups
on the FF MED [F ð1; 1089Þ ¼ 3:7, p < 0:01] scale than were within one point.
the older respondents. Thus, younger consumers seemed
to get less reward from using functional foods, they did
not think that they could repair an unhealthy diet by 4. Discussion
using functional foods and they were not as ready to
compromise on the taste of functional foods for the We found seven dimensions that describe consumers’
ensuing health effects. Younger respondents are also attitudes towards functional foods. These attitudes be-
more positive towards adding medicine-like health ef- hind functional food related dimensions reflect the as-
fects in food products. On two subscales, there were pects found in previous functional food studies:
differences between educational groups. Respondents healthiness, taste, pleasure, security and familiarity
who had university degree education rated higher on the (Poulsen, 1999; Urala & L€ahteenm€aki, 2003). Func-
FF MED [F ð1; 1090Þ ¼ 6:2, p < 0:001] and lower on the tional food related attitudes affected the willingness to
FF PART [F ð1; 1090Þ ¼ 5:2, p < 0:01] than the less- use functional food products in different ways depending
educated respondents. on the type of functional product. This result confirms
It is noteworthy that even there were statistically our earlier findings that consumers do not perceive
significant differences between females and males, age functional foods as one homogenous group.
and educational groups, the differences in the scale Among the functional foods there are, however,
means of these groups were quite small. For instance the product based subgroups in which consumers’ reported
differences between the scale means of FF PART and willingness to use seem to be affected by same attitudes.
802 N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803

Reported willingness to use all functional foods could be In some earlier functional food studies, a question of
predicted by rewarding feeling and necessity for func- the medicine-like effects of functional foods has been
tional foods. Besides these two attitudes, among the raised (Bech-Larsen & Grunert, 2003; Frewer et al.,
products that promise clear lowering of well-established 2003; Poulsen, 1999; Urala & L€ahteenm€aki, 2003).
and commonly recognized disease related risks (prod- Consumers may connect medicine-like effects with
ucts with cardiovascular disease related claim) taste can functional foods in general, but in our study consumers’
be compromised and willingness to use products that attitude towards medicine-like effect did not affect will-
promote general well-being (probiotic/gut friendly juice, ingness to use functional foods when the willingness to
drinks with added calcium and meat products with use existing product samples was evaluated.
added fibre) need confidence in functional foods. Risk issues concerning functional foods are consid-
Consumers’ willingness to use functional foods was ered, but according to our results they are not seen as
best predicted by their attitude that describes their per- highly relevant when evaluating product examples. Fre-
ceived reward from using functional foods. This feeling wer et al. (2003) present that from the consumers’ point
of reward from consuming functional products may re- of view the perceived risks of functional foods would be
flect the generally accepted disposition in western coun- one barrier not to consume functional products. In our
tries in which individuals are increasingly considered as study the perceived nutritional risks did not, however,
being responsible for their own health and well-being. In affect the reported willingness to use functional foods.
particular, the roles of diet and eating behaviour have Taste is one of the most important choice factors
been raised as one of the most important factors affecting when choosing functional foods (Urala & L€ahteenm€ aki,
an individual’s well-being and state of health. Rozin 2003). In a study of Tuorila and Cardello (2002), bitter
(1997) argued strongly that eating is a moral issue and salty off-flavours decreased the liking and likelihood
nowadays. Our earlier laddering interviews also showed of consumption of juice samples with associated health
that consumers connect functional foods with control claims. In the present study, the dimension describing
over life and health, being a better person and feelings consumers’ attitudes to the taste was strongly dependent
of well-being (Urala & L€ ahteenm€ aki, 2003). The use of on the dimension describing the reward from using
functional foods may offer a new, less-demanding way of functional products (FF REW). Interestingly the atti-
gaining an ethical reward through food choices: con- tude towards taste affected strongly the willingness to
sumers feel that they take care of themselves and make use both cholesterol lowering spread and blood pressure
the ‘right’ choices that are socially acceptable. milk drink. These products may have such a strong
This rewarding feeling may be connected not only to health related message that those who are motivated in
control over one’s own health but also to the positive using these products are even ready to compromise the
impressions that an individual perhaps wants to evoke taste. The taste dimension has, however, quite weak
among other people. Saher et al. (2004) found that reliability and few items. Due these limitations the re-
subjects who had functional foods on their shopping list sults should be interpreted with caution.
were regarded as more innovative but less friendly Some of the dimensions were formed from positively
compared to consumers whose shopping list contained worded items and other from solely negatively formu-
conventional products. By choosing and using func- lated statements. This finding may suggest that respon-
tional foods consumers may achieve a modern and dents had not formed definite pro or con opinions about
positive impression of themselves. Functional foods the items, but responded according to the verbal for-
provide consumers a modern way to follow a healthy mulation of the item.
lifestyle, which differs from the conventionally healthy We found, that developed functional food attitude
diet defined by nutrition experts. One way of making scales are distinct from general health interest and pro-
functional foods more attractive to the sceptical con- vide a new tool for measuring consumer attitudes to-
sumers is to emphasise the good feeling that is derived wards health-based reasons in food choices. Because
from using functional foods. food neophobia had slightly negative effect on the will-
Necessity for functional foods was not specific for ingness to use functional products the functional foods
functional foods. This attitude affected also the will- may be, to some extent, perceived as new products;
ingness to use reference products to some extent. Espe- however the effect was not specific to the functional
cially the necessity for functional foods predicted foods and was similar to other modified products such
significantly the respondents’ reported willingness to use as low salted food products and organic bread. Func-
all, both functional and reference products, except en- tional foods were not perceived unnatural: natural
ergy drinks. Necessity did not, however, correlate with product interest did not affect respondents’ willingness
the general health interest. Perceived necessity may re- to use functional foods. Our study does not support the
flect some other aspect of more general health interest earlier assumptions (Frewer et al., 2003) that the per-
than the general health interest scale measuring nutri- ceived unnaturalness would reduce consumers’ willing-
tionally related healthiness. ness to use functional foods.
N. Urala, L. L€ahteenm€aki / Food Quality and Preference 15 (2004) 793–803 803

5. Conclusions American consumers’ perception of functional foods. Appetite,


49, 9–14.
Diplock, A. T., Agget, P. J., Ashwell, M., Bornet, F., Fern, E. B., &
For both consumers and researchers, it is difficult to Roberfroid, M. B. (1999). Scientific concepts of functional foods
follow and predict the progress in the functional food in Europe: consensus document. British Journal of Nutrition, 81,
market (Frewer et al., 2003; Gray et al., 2003; Menrad, 127.
2003). Functional food scales offer a new tool for Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes.
monitoring consumers’ attitudes and predict the success Orlando: Hartcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publisher.
Frewer, L., Scholderer, J., & Lambert, N. (2003). Consumer accep-
of functional food products even on the concept level. tance of functional foods: issues for the future. British Food
The perceived reward from using functional food and Journal, 10, 714–731.
the confidence in functional foods seem to be the most Gray, J., Armstrong, G., & Farley, H. (2003). Opportunities and
crucial factors in consumers’ willingness to use func- constrains in the functional food market. Nutrition & Food Science,
tional foods. Consumers may perceive risks coming 33, 213–218.
L€ahteenm€aki, L. (2003). Consumers and functional foods. In T.
from the use of functional foods in general, but the Mattila-Sandholm & M. Saarela (Eds.), Functional dairy products.
perceived risks do not affect ratings describing con- Cambridge: Woodhead Publication Ltd.
sumers’ willingness to use functional products. The Menrad, K. (2003). Market and marketing of functional food in
functional food scales will be developed further into Europe. Journal of Food Engineering, 56, 181–188.
more precise and shorter form to provide a feasible tool Pliner, P., & Hobden, K. (1992). Development of a scale to measure
the trait of food neophobia for edible substances. Appetite, 19, 105–
to measure attitudes toward functional foods. 120.
Poulsen, J. B. (1999). Danish consumers’ attitudes towards functional
foods, Working paper no. 62, MAPP, Arhus, Denmark.
Roininen, K., L€ahteenm€aki, L., & Tuorila, H. (1999). Quantification
Acknowledgements
of consumer attitudes to health and hedonic characteristics of
foods. Appetite, 33, 7188.
The authors would like to acknowledge TEKES Rozin, P. (1997). Moralization. In A. M. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.),
(National Technology Agency of Finland), which partly Morality and health. London: Routledge.
funded this research. This study is part of a joint project: Saher, M., Arvola, A., Lindeman, M., & L€ahteenm€aki, L. (2004).
Impressions of functional food consumers. Appetite, 42, 79–89.
entitled ‘Tools for consumer-oriented product develop-
Shepherd, R. (1990). Attitudes and beliefs as determinants of food
ment’, and belongs to the Tailored Technologies for choice. In R. L. McBride & H. J. H. MacFie (Eds.), Psychological
Future Foods programme at VTT Biotechnology. basis of sensory evaluation. London: Elsevier Applied Science.
Tuorila, H. (1997). Attitudes as determinants of food consumption.
Encyclopedia of Human Biology (2nd ed.) (Vol. 1), pp. 559–606.
Tuorila, H., & Cardello, A. V. (2002). Consumer responses to an off-
References flavor in juice in the presence of specific health claims. Food Quality
and Preference, 13, 561–569.
Beardsworth, C., Haslam, C., Keil, T., Goode, J., & Sherrat, E. (1999). Urala, N., & L€ahteenm€aki, L. (2003). Reasons behind consumers’
Contemporary nutritional attitudes and practices: a factor analysis functional food choices. Nutrition & Food Science, 33, 148–158.
approach. Appetite, 32, 127–143. Urala, N., Arvola, A., & L€ahteenm€aki, L. (2003). Strength of health-
Bech-Larsen, T., & Grunert, K. G. (2003). The perceived healthiness of related claims and their perceived advantage. International Journal
functional foods: a conjoint study of Danish, Finnish and of Food Science and Technology, 38, 815–826.

You might also like