Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment © 2013 American Psychological Association

2013, Vol. 4, No. 4, 332–340 1949-2715/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0033303

BRIEF REPORT

Criminal Behavior and Cognitive Processing in Male Offenders With


Antisocial Personality Disorder With and Without Comorbid Psychopathy
Rebecca E. Riser and David S. Kosson
Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science

Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) and psychopathy are 2 important syndromes with substantial
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

utility in predicting antisocial behavior. Although prior studies have identified correlations between
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

various factors and the presence of psychopathy or ASPD, most studies have focused on 1 syndrome or
the other. Consequently, it is unclear whether the 2 syndromes reflect similar pathophysiologies, whether
they are in fact 2 distinct syndromes, or whether the correlates of ASPD reflect its high comorbidity with
psychopathy. The present study addressed this issue by examining the impact of ASPD with and without
comorbid psychopathy (as assessed by the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised) on criminal offending and
cognitive processing in 674 adult male inmates at a county jail in Illinois. Participants exhibited either
ASPD and comorbid psychopathy, ASPD but not psychopathy, or neither ASPD nor psychopathy.
Participants with and without comorbid psychopathy were characterized by more criminal behavior than
controls, and inmates with ASPD and psychopathy exhibited more severe criminal behavior than those
with ASPD only. In addition, inmates with ASPD and psychopathy exhibited a different pattern of
cognitive task performance impairment than those with ASPD alone. Results replicate the findings of
Kosson, Lorenz, and Newman (2006) and provide new evidence suggesting that men with ASPD and
comorbid psychopathy are characterized by cognitive processing anomalies different from those seen in
ASPD without comorbid psychopathy.

Keywords: psychopathy, antisocial personality disorder, criminal behavior, personality disorder, cogni-
tion

Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) are two Association, Treatment Revision; DMS-IV–TR; American Psychi-
constructs that have proven useful in identifying dangerous subsets atric Association, 2000). Psychopathy is a syndrome of personality
of criminal offenders (Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1996; Serin, pathology characterized by lack of empathy, emotional shallow-
1991). Despite an abundance of research demonstrating the costs ness, superficial charm, and impulsivity (Cleckley, 1988). The
of ASPD and psychopathy, the extent of the commonalities and Psychopathy Checklist (PCL; Hare, 1980) and its revision
differences in the correlates of ASPD and psychopathy are seldom (PCL–R; Hare, 2003) are the most widely used and best validated
examined in the same study. Clarifying the relationship between instruments for the clinical assessment of psychopathy.
ASPD and psychopathy is essential to both the criminal justice and Although the DSM-IV–TR (American Psychiatric Association,
mental health systems. 2000) states that ASPD is “also known as psychopathy” (p. 645),
ASPD is characterized by pervasive disregard for the rights of ASPD diagnostic criteria focus primarily on behavioral deviance,
others, beginning in childhood and continuing into adulthood whereas psychopathy encompasses a wide range of affective,
(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric interpersonal, and behavioral disturbances. The ASPD criteria do
not include many of these interpersonal and affective features, and
the category has been criticized for overreliance on behavioral
indicators.
Rebecca E. Riser and David S. Kosson, Department of Psychology, Nevertheless, the distinction between ASPD and psychopathy is
Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science. sometimes blurred. Lee (1999) reviewed studies conducted with
We express our gratitude to Megan Mercado, Richard Riddle, and the patients who “displayed the core features of psychopathy, as
entire Lake County Jail staff for their consistent support of this research. defined in the ICD-10, DSM-IV, or in Hare’s Psychopathy Check-
Preparation of this article was supported in part by the National Institute of list” (p. 17). Lewis (1991) wrote that chronic antisocial behavior is
Mental Health, Grant MH57714: R-01, Cognitive Processing Deficits of
termed psychopathy in the psychological literature and antisocial
Criminal Psychopaths, Principal Investigator: D. S. Kosson, 1999 –2004.
personality in the psychiatric literature but goes on to refer to
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rebecca
E. Riser or David S. Kosson, Department of Psychology, Rosalind Franklin chronic antisocial behavior and psychopathy interchangeably.
University of Medicine and Science, 3333 Green Bay Road, North Kirkman (2008) and Gurley (2009) noted that even today many,
Chicago, IL 60064. E-mail: rebecca.riser@my.rfums.org, david.kosson@ including the American Psychiatric Association, view the two
rosalindfranklin.edu labels as referring to the same disorder.
332
EFFECTS OF COMORBID PSYCHOPATHY 333

The two syndromes share important correlates. Both are asso- lates. For example, both syndromes might be associated with
ciated with substance abuse (Messina, Wish, Hoffman, & Nemes, recidivism and cognitive and affective performance, but effect
2001; Smith & Newman, 1990) and predict treatment failure sizes might be larger for psychopathy. Consistent with this per-
(Bucholz, Hesselbrock, Health, Kramer, & Schuckit, 2000; Cun- spective, both syndromes do predict treatment failure and recidi-
ningham & Reidy, 1998; Hare, Clark, Grann, & Thornton, 2000; vism with psychopathy a better predictor than ASPD (Cacciola,
Rice, Harris, & Cormier, 1992) and recidivism (Harris, Rice, & Alterman, Rutherford, & Snider, 1995; Hare et al., 2000; Kranzler,
Cormier, 1991; Serin & Amos, 1995). Psychopathy and ASPD are Del Boca, & Rounsaville, 1996; Harris et al., 1991; Hemphill et
also highly comorbid within criminal populations (Coid, 2002; al., 1998).
Hare, 2003), raising the possibility that the pathophysiology of the Much more research has examined the mechanisms underlying
underlying disorders may be the same. and correlates of psychopathy than ASPD. Compared with non-
Others argue that the differences between ASPD and psychop- psychopaths, psychopaths exhibit numerous affective and cogni-
athy are important. Most psychopathic individuals meet diagnostic tive anomalies, including deficient performance in classifying and
criteria for ASPD; however most individuals with ASPD are not reduced physiological reactivity to affective stimuli (Hare, 2003;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

psychopathic (Hare, 1996). Whereas ASPD has a reported preva- Patrick, Cuthbert, & Lang, 1994; Williamson, Hare, & Wong,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

lence rate of 50 –75% among prison inmates, only 20 –30% of 1987); deficient passive avoidance learning (Lykken, 1957; New-
inmates meet PCL–R diagnostic criteria for psychopathy (Hare, man & Kosson, 1986); and poorer performance than nonpsycho-
2003; Hare, Hart, & Harpur, 1991). Consequently, Stevens (1993) pathic offenders on neuropsychological tests of orbitofrontal func-
argued that psychopathy is important for identifying a more ho- tion (Lapierre, Braun, & Hodgins, 1995; Mitchell, Colledge,
mogenous group of offenders.1 Leonard, & Blair, 2002). No body of systematic laboratory re-
Psychopathy also predicts antisocial behavior (including recid-
search addresses these processes in ASPD individuals.
ivism) over and above ASPD, including success versus failure
Studies of cognitive function have also yielded substantial evi-
during conditional releases from prisons (Hart, Kropp, & Hare,
dence that individuals with psychopathy display intact cognitive
1988) and postdiction of substance use disorder symptoms after
function under baseline conditions but display relatively poorer
controlling for ASPD symptoms (Walsh, Allen, & Kosson, 2007).
performance on a variety of cognitive tasks under conditions placing
Psychopathy and ASPD also differ in regards to the impact of
substantial demands on left hemisphere (LH)-specific attention, mo-
environmental factors. Whereas some studies suggest that quality
tor, language, and perceptual systems, as predicted by the LH activa-
of family environment is unrelated to age of onset or severity of
tion (LHA) hypothesis (Kosson, 1998; Llanes & Kosson, 2006; Suchy
antisocial behavior in individuals with psychopathic traits, family
environment is a crucial factor for onset of antisocial behavior in & Kosson, 2006). Given that this has not been examined in ASPD, it
general (Devita, Forth, & Hare, 1990; Wootton, Frick, Shelton, & remains possible that ASPD individuals also exhibit LHA deficits.
Silverthorn, 1997; but see Edens, Skopp, & Cahill, 2008). Simi- Because approximately 90% of those inmates classified as psy-
larly, the environment to which ASPD offenders return after prison chopathic have comorbid ASPD, it is not feasible to examine
is strongly correlated with their reoffending; psychopathic offend- behavior, cognition, and affect in individuals with only ASPD,
ers have a high risk of reoffending regardless of the environment only psychopathy, and both psychopathy and ASPD. A more
to which they return (Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998).2 practical alternative is to study groups of individuals characterized
Limited evidence also suggests differences in cognitive and by ASPD with and without comorbid psychopathy.
affective correlates. Psychopathy is associated with reduced affec- To examine whether ASPD with comorbid psychopathy was
tive facilitation of lexical decision, but ASPD is not (Lorenz & distinct from ASPD without psychopathy, using the PCL–R to
Newman, 2002a, 2002b; Williamson, Harpur, & Hare, 1991). assess psychopathy and DSM-IV criteria for the assessment of
Deficits in startle potentiation for aversive stimuli have also been ASPD, Kosson et al. (2006) divided individuals into those who
linked to psychopathy and not to ASPD (Vaidyanathan, Hall, met criteria for ASPD but not psychopathy (ASPD without psy-
Patrick, & Bernat, 2011). Conversely, general verbal deficits have chopathy), those who met criteria for both ASPD and psychopathy
been linked to ASPD (Malloy, Noel, Rogers, Longabaugh, & (ASPD with comorbid psychopathy), and those who did not meet
Beattie, 1989), whereas language anomalies and left hemisphere diagnostic criteria for either ASPD or psychopathy (controls).
(LH) impairments in psychopathy appear more specific (Hare, Although both ASPD groups were characterized by more criminal
Williamson, & Harpur, 1988; Suchy & Kosson, 2006). Sargeant,
Daughters, Curtin, Schuster, and Lejuez (2011) reported that 1
Furthermore, previous studies illustrating the predictive utility of
ASPD and psychopathy differ with respect to their association ASPD in regards to substance abuse and treatment outcomes (Bucholz,
with persistence in goal-directed behavior during an aversive sit- Hesselbrock, Health, Kramer, & Schuckit, 2000; Messina, Wish, Hoffman,
uation: The ASPD diagnosis was associated with lower distress & Nemes, 2001) have more commonly drawn from psychiatric than from
tolerance, but psychopathic traits were associated with higher forensic samples (where the prevalence rate of ASPD is much lower and
distress tolerance. Taken together, such findings suggest the pos- ASPD may have more discriminating power). Despite evidence signaling
the need for caution regarding the predictive utility of ASPD in prison
sibility that distinct emotional and cognitive mechanisms may settings, the predictive utility of psychopathy has been demonstrated re-
underlie these disorders. peatedly in offender samples.
2
Alternatively, the disorders could reflect the same underlying Several recent studies also suggest apparent associations between
mechanisms with psychopathy representing a more severe form of environmental factors and psychopathic traits (Kimonis, Frick, Munoz, &
Aucoin, 2008; Krischer & Sevecke, 2008; Schraft, Kosson, & McBride, in
ASPD (Reid, 2001). From this perspective, the two disorders press). However, most of these studies do not address the possibility that
might differ in the degree of their association with external criteria conduct disorder or ASPD symptoms may account for the apparent asso-
with no difference in the pattern of cognitive or affective corre- ciation.
334 RISER AND KOSSON

behavior than controls, those with ASPD and psychopathy exhib- forming assessment strategies and may ultimately prove impor-
ited greater criminal activity and weaker emotion facilitation than tant in developing interventions to target the psychopathology
those with ASPD only. Furthermore, the degree of affective facil- underlying these syndromes.
itation correlated inversely with criminal activity in participants
with ASPD plus psychopathy, but correlated nonsignificantly with Method
criminal activity in ASPD-only participants. These findings pro-
vide preliminary evidence for a qualitative distinction between
ASPD with versus without comorbid psychopathic features, and
Participants
suggest the possibility that different psychological mechanisms The sample was comprised of 674 males incarcerated at a
may characterize these two subgroups of ASPD offenders. How- county jail in Illinois between the ages of 18 and 45 (M ⫽ 26.10,
ever, until these findings are replicated in an independent sample, SD ⫽ 6.69) who met all inclusion criteria (see below). The sample
the generality of these distinctions cannot be evaluated. Further- was 45% Caucasian, 49.3% African American, and 4.7% Latino,
more, emotional processing was the only mechanism examined in with 1% reporting other ethnic backgrounds. Exclusion criteria
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

this study. Given the evidence for performance differences con- included current use of medication with neurocognitive side ef-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sistent with theories positing specific cognitive deficits in psycho- fects, presence of psychotic symptoms, inability to read English,
pathic offenders, it is important to examine whether individuals with left-handedness (for analyses of cognitive processing only), esti-
ASPD alone exhibit performance deficits on neurocognitive tasks mated IQ below 70 (using the Shipley Institute of Living Scale—
similar to those seen in individuals with ASPD and psychopathy. Revised), and lack of information about prior criminal charges.
Inmates were classified into one of three groups: ASPD plus
The Current Study psychopathy (ASPD ⫹ PSY; n ⫽ 145), ASPD without psychopa-
thy (ASPD-only; n ⫽ 201), and neither psychopathy nor ASPD
The primary purpose of the current study was to investigate the (controls; n ⫽ 328). The ASPD ⫹ PSY group consisted of inmates
generalizability of prior findings that offenders with ASPD with- who met diagnostic criteria for ASPD and also met PCL–R criteria
out psychopathy differ in important ways from those with ASPD for psychopathy (PCL–R total scores of 30 or above). The ASPD-
comorbid with psychopathy. We examined whether the differences only group consisted of inmates who met diagnostic criteria for
in criminal behavior reported in Kosson et al. (2006) would rep- ASPD but had PCL–R total scores below 30. Controls consisted of
licate in an independent sample and whether the reported differ- inmates with PCL–R scores below 30 who did not meet diagnostic
ences in emotional processing would generalize to cognitive pro- criteria for ASPD. Of the 674 inmates, 156 men comprised the
cessing mechanisms associated with psychopathy. By using the sample used in analyses for cognitive processing; they met all of
global-local processing task, it was possible to test whether the the above inclusion criteria and had completed the global-local
LHA deficits associated with psychopathy are specific to psycho- task. The overall sample appeared representative of offender sam-
pathic offenders with ASPD or generalize to offenders with ASPD ples in prior psychopathy studies in the United States. (M age ⫽
but not psychopathy. Prior studies have shown that LH perceptual 27.54, SD ⫽ 6.57; M years of education ⫽ 11.52, SD ⫽ 2.03; M
systems are more active in processing local visual stimuli, and estimated WAIS–R full scale ⫽ 93.10, SD ⫽ 12.59). The mean
right hemisphere (RH) perceptual systems are more active in PCL–R score of 23.8 (SD ⫽ 6.9) was similar to that previously
processing global visual stimuli. Because the LHA hypothesis reported for inmate samples (Hare, 2003).
posits that psychopaths display general cognitive inefficiency
when LH processing systems are substantially and differentially
Classification Measures
activated, it predicts that psychopaths will respond more slowly
than nonpsychopaths to both global and local targets in a local bias PCL–R. The PCL–R is a 20-item expert-rater measure of the
condition that presents most targets at the local level and activates interpersonal, affective, and behavioral features of psychopathy
primarily LH perceptual processing systems. The LHA hypothesis that is typically completed following an in-depth semistructured
predicts no performance differences in a global bias condition that interview and a review of available file and/or other collateral
activates RH systems differentially. information. The PCL–R items are rated on a 3-point ordinal scale
We evaluated three perspectives: that ASPD with and without from 0 (definitely does not apply) to 2 (definitely applies) based on
psychopathy reflect the same underlying disorder, that the two the extent to which information about an individual’s behavior in
reflect distinct underlying mechanisms, and that the two are multiple domains and from multiple sources (self-reports, behav-
distinct but only in severity. If the two syndromes reflect the ioral observation, collateral sources) matches specific item de-
same underlying pathophysiology, then both groups should scriptions. Total scores range from 0 to 40, and adults scoring 30
manifest similar tendencies on measures of both criminal be- and above are traditionally classified as psychopaths (Hare, 2003).
havior and cognitive processing. If ASPD with and without As in Kosson et al. (2006) and several other studies (e.g., see
psychopathy reflect distinct mechanisms, analyses should re- Brinkley, Schmitt, Smith, & Newman, 2001; Kosson, Smith, &
veal different patterns of performance in cognitive processing Newman, 1990), PCL–R scores were modified to reduce overlap
and differences in criminal behavior. If psychopathy is only a between psychopathy scores and indices of antisocial behavior.
more severe version of ASPD we should see similar associa- Scores on two items directly related to criminal conduct (Juvenile
tions with criminal behavior and cognitive task performance Delinquency and Criminal Versatility) were deleted, and scores on
impairments that differ only in degree. Clarifying the nature of remaining items were prorated. Good interrater agreement for
the behaviors and mechanisms associated with ASPD with PCL–R total scores has been reported across settings. Internal
versus without psychopathy has important implications for in- consistency is high as indicated by alpha coefficients of .84 for
EFFECTS OF COMORBID PSYCHOPATHY 335

total scores across samples and item-total correlations of .30 or condition using the right hand. Median response latencies and
higher for most individual items across samples. The PCL–R is accuracy were calculated.
considered a homogenous scale with mean interitem correlations of Target frequency was varied to manipulate LH versus RH
.22 (Hare, 2003). Interrater agreement for PCL–R total scores in the processing. In the neutral condition (N), targets appeared at the
current sample was also good as indicated by an average intraclass local and global level equally often. In the global condition (G),
correlation between interviewer and observer ratings of .87. targets appeared at the global level 80% of the time and at the local
Assessment of ASPD. The diagnosis of ASPD was deter- level 20% of the time. Conversely, in the local condition (L),
mined based on interview questions addressing DSM-IV criteria targets appeared at the local level 80% of the time and at the global
and a review of available file material. In most cases, ASPD level 20% of the time. The presentation of one kind of target (e.g.,
information was explicitly recorded at the time of the interview. local targets) four times as often as the other kind (e.g., global targets)
For 83 cases in which ASPD information was assessed but not was designed to differentially activate the relevant hemisphere-
coded at the time of the interview, existing interviews were re- specific perceptual system (Fink et al., 1997; Van Kleeck, 1989).
viewed to code ASPD symptoms for this study. Interrater agree- Participants received the conditions either in the order global bias-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ment on assessments of ASPD from prior interviews based on neutral-local bias (GNL), or in the order local bias-neutral-global bias
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

30.0% of the sample was good, k ⫽ .87. (LNG). As in Kosson et al. (2007), principal analyses of the local bias
and global bias conditions included only men administered that con-
Criminal History Measures dition first to ensure that the impact of each hemispheric activation
condition was not diluted by other conditions (see Kosson et al., 2007,
Three indices of criminal activity were utilized to compare the for additional details). Neutral condition performance comparisons
ASPD-only, ASPD ⫹ PSY and control groups on participation in collapsed across the two orders.
violent crime, nonviolent crime, and criminal versatility.
In accordance with prior studies (Hare & McPherson, 1984;
Kosson et al., 1990; Kosson et al., 2006), violent offenses con- Results
sisted of robberies, murders, assaults, sexual assaults, kidnappings,
and weapons offenses. Nonviolent offenses consisted of burglaries Overview and Preliminary Analyses
and thefts, frauds and forgeries, drug offenses, negligence and
major driving offenses, escapes, arsons, obstructions of justice, Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and follow-up t tests were
and miscellaneous minor crimes. Criminal versatility was opera- used to compare groups on indices of criminal behavior and
tionalized as the number of different categories in which inmates cognitive processing. In cases of heterogeneity of variance, the
were charged with or reported committing offenses. Welch F’ and t’ tests were used in comparing group means. There
were no group differences in estimated IQ or age, F(2, 672) ⫽
1.03, p ⫽ .36, F(2, 672) ⫽ 2.42, p ⫽ .09, respectively, and no
Laboratory Measure of Cognitive Processing
relationship between ethnicity and group membership, ␹2(6) ⫽
Data on the global-local task were collected as part of a prior 6.20, p ⫽ .40 (see Table 1). The groups differed in education, F(2,
study (Kosson, Miller, Kosson, Byrnes & Leveroni, 2007). Partic- 672) ⫽ 9.87, p ⬍ .001, with the control group averaging more
ipants sat in front of a 14-inch Dell computer screen and viewed education (M ⫽ 11.72, SD ⫽ 1.66) than the ASPD-only group
centrally located stimuli. During each trial, participants were pre- (M ⫽ 11.07, SD ⫽ 1.64) but not the ASPD ⫹ PSY group (M ⫽
sented with one hierarchical stimulus consisting of the target letter 11.43, SD ⫽ 1.76). These demographic variables were considered
H or S at either the local or global level and the distractor letter A as possible covariates; however, none of these demographic vari-
or E at the opposite level for a duration of 150 ms. Participants ables were associated with indices of criminal behavior or cogni-
were asked to respond by pressing a key on the keyboard to tive performance in any condition.
indicate the target letter presented (H or S). Large global letters Of the 674 inmates in the criminal activity analyses, outliers
measured 30 mm ⫻ 45 mm (approximately 2.86° ⫻ 4.30°), and were identified for 12 control, 9 ASPD-only, and 4 ASPD ⫹ PSY
the 12–14 smaller individual letters that comprised each global inmates. To reduce their influence, extreme values were trans-
stimulus were local letters measuring 6 mm ⫻ 8.5 mm (.57° ⫻ formed to one unit greater than the most extreme value within 3
.81°). Participants completed eight practice trials and 64 trials per SDs of the group mean (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Of the 156

Table 1
Demographic Characteristics for Inmate Groups

Education IQ Age Ethnicity


Inmate group Sample size M SD M SD M SD AA–C–L–O
a
Control 328 11.80 1.63 92.76 11.06 26.91 7.07 152–163–10–3
ASPD only 201 11.07b 1.71 92.13 10.16 25.62 6.48 87–103–8–3
ASPD ⫹ PSY 145 11.21b 1.97 92.15 11.27 27.22 6.79 81–57–6–1
Note. Education is years completed. IQ is estimated WAIS-R from the Shipley Institute of Living Scale-Revised. Age is in years. Means with different
subscripts differ significantly, p ⬍ .05. Control ⫽ neither antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) nor psychopathy; ASPD -only ⫽ ASPD met but not
psychopathy; ASPD ⫹ PSY ⫽ ASPD met in addition to psychopathy; AA ⫽ African American; C ⫽ Caucasian; L ⫽ Latino; O ⫽ Other.
336 RISER AND KOSSON

inmates completing the global-local cognitive task, there were 4 psychopathic offenders versus controls, the ASPD ⫹ PSY group
outliers identified. Because one inmate obtained an extreme score responded more slowly than the control group, t(48) ⫽ 12.28, p ⫽
for only one of six indices, that value was modified as noted above. .03, d ⫽ 3.54. The ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were also slower than
The remaining three inmates had outlying values on at least three ASPD-only inmates, t’(36) ⫽ 2.36, p ⫽ .02, d ⫽ .79. However, the
of six indices and were excluded from analyses, yielding a final ASPD-only group did not differ from controls, t(50) ⫽ .16, p ⫽
sample of 153 inmates for cognitive analyses: 72 control inmates, .87, d ⫽ .04 (see Table 3).
41 ASPD-only inmates, and 40 ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates. The groups also differed in response latencies for global targets
in the local bias condition, F(2, 67) ⫽ 3.75, p ⫽ .03. As for local
targets, ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were slower than ASPD-only
Primary Analyses
inmates in responding to global targets, t(36) ⫽ 2.65, p ⫽ .01, d ⫽
Analyses of criminal behavior. The groups differed in crim- .88 (see Table 3). Controls were also slower than ASPD-only
inal versatility, F(2, 671) ⫽ 53.41, p ⬍ .001. Both ASPD-only inmates, a difference that approached significance, t(50) ⫽ 1.82,
inmates and ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were charged with more types p ⫽ .07, d ⫽ .51. However, ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were not slower
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of offenses than controls: t(527) ⫽ ⫺4.83, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .42, for than controls, t(48) ⫽ ⫺1.33, p ⫽ .19, d ⫽ .38.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ASPD-only inmates versus controls, and t’(242) ⫽ ⫺9.60, p ⬍ Global bias condition trials. There were no significant group
.001, d ⫽ 1.23, for controls versus ASPD ⫹ PSY. Moreover, differences in response latencies in the global condition at either
ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were charged with more types of crimes the local target level, F(2, 78) ⫽ .31, p ⫽ .73, or at the global
than ASPD-only inmates, t(344) ⫽ ⫺5.35, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .58 (see target level, F(2, 78) ⫽ .20, p ⫽ .82 (see Table 3).
Table 2). Neutral block trials. There were no significant group differ-
The groups differed in charges for violent offenses, ences in response latencies in the neutral block to either local targets,
F’(2, 305) ⫽ 28.64, p ⬍ .001. As shown in Table 2, ASPD-only F(2, 153) ⫽ .05, p ⫽ .952 or to global targets, F(2, 153) ⫽ 2.10, p ⫽
inmates and ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were charged with more .13 (see Table 3).
violent offenses than controls: t’(357) ⫽ ⫺2.97, p ⫽ .003, d ⫽ .31,
t’(192) ⫽ ⫺7.42, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ 1.07, respectively. In addition,
Discussion
ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were charged with more violent crimes than
ASPD-only inmates, t’(253) ⫽ ⫺4.89, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .61. Although substantial research has examined psychopathy and
The groups differed in charges for nonviolent offenses, F’(2, ASPD, the relationships between the two are seldom examined in
295) ⫽ 24.23, p ⬍ .001. Again, ASPD-only inmates and ASPD ⫹ the same study. Replicating Kosson et al. (2006), the current study
PSY inmates were charged with more nonviolent offenses than demonstrated that ASPD in the absence of psychopathy is an
controls: t’(317) ⫽ ⫺4.65, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .52, t’(189) ⫽ ⫺5.91, important clinical construct but that psychopathy makes a unique
p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .86, respectively. Finally, ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates contribution to the prediction of criminal behavior and cognitive
were charged with more nonviolent crimes than ASPD-only in- functioning. Inmates with only ASPD were charged with more
mates, t’(279) ⫽ ⫺2.05, p ⫽ .04, d ⫽ .25 (see Table 2). violent and nonviolent charges and characterized by greater crim-
Additional analyses of relationships between group classifi- inal versatility than control inmates. These findings provide evi-
cation and criminal behavior. Because it could be argued the dence that the correlates of ASPD do not solely reflect its comor-
greater antisociality of the ASPD ⫹ PSY group resulted from bidity with psychopathy. Removing the subset of individuals with
using a more strict selection criteria for psychopathy than ASPD many psychopathic features from a large sample of ASPD inmates
(PCL–R score of 30/40 ⫽ 75% of maximum criteria vs. 3/7 adult did not eliminate relationships between ASPD and criminality.4
ASPD criteria ⫽ 42.85% of maximum criteria), as in Kosson et al. Moreover, even after deleting scores on the two PCL–R items
(2006), we repeated the above analyses using an ASPD cutting most directly associated with antisociality (Juvenile Delinquency
score more comparable to that used to classify psychopathy. These and Criminal Versatility), the ASPD ⫹ PSY group displayed more
analyses compared a severe ASPD-only group (meeting at least criminal behavior than controls and, despite the substantial crim-
five of seven ASPD symptoms, or at least 71.4% of criteria) to the inality exhibited by ASPD-only inmates, APSD ⫹ PSY inmates
other groups. averaged more violent and nonviolent charges and a greater variety
Results were similar to those reported above. There were group of charges compared with ASPD-only inmates. The difference in
effects for criminal versatility, F(2, 531) ⫽ 54.68, p ⬍ .001,
violent changes, F’(2, 135) ⫽ 30.14, p ⬍ .001, and nonviolent 3
The severe ASPD-only group was charged with more different types of
charges, F’(2, 132) ⫽ 25.43, p ⬍ .001. Independent t tests repli- crimes than controls, t(387) ⫽ ⫺4.87, p ⬍ .001, d ⫽ .495, and fewer
cated the pattern of differences reported above for criminal versa- different types of crimes than the ASPD⫹PSY group, t(204) ⫽ ⫺2.19, p ⫽
tility and violent charges.3 The only exception was that the differ- .029, d ⫽ .306. For violent charges, the severe ASPD-only group was
ence between ASPD-only and the ASPD ⫹ PSY group for charged with more violent crimes than controls, t’(71) ⫽ ⫺3.14, p ⫽ .002,
nonviolent offenses dropped to a statistical trend, t(205) ⫽ .440, d ⫽ .745, and fewer violent charges than the ASPD⫹PSY group, t(205) ⫽
⫺2.09, p ⫽ .038, d ⫽ .29. The severe ASPD-only inmates were also
p ⫽ .06, d ⫽ .661, suggesting that the difference in severity of charged with more nonviolent offenses than controls, t’(69) ⫽ ⫺4.54, p ⬍
diagnostic criteria contributed to the group difference for nonvio- .001, d ⫽ 1.09.
4
lent charges. Because the psychopathy construct appears to be dimensional, the
Analyses of cognitive processing. exclusion of individuals with PCL-R scores of 30 or higher from the
ASPD-only group does not entirely remove all variance due to psycho-
Local bias condition trials. The groups differed in response pathic traits. It remains possible that moderate levels of psychopathic traits
latencies for local targets in the local bias condition, F(2, 67) ⫽ account for some of the variance in criminal activity that appears to
3.69, p ⫽ .03. Replicating results of Kosson et al. (2007) for characterize individuals with ASPD only.
EFFECTS OF COMORBID PSYCHOPATHY 337

Table 2
Group Means and Standard Deviations for Criminal Conduct

Inmate group
Control (n ⫽ 328) ASPD only (n ⫽ 201) ASPD ⫹ PSY (n ⫽ 145)
Criminal variable M SD M SD M SD
a b c
Number of charges for violent offenses 2.10 3.03 3.03 3.75 5.47 5.10
Number of charges for nonviolent offenses 9.28a 9.03 14.18b 13.19 17.43c 15.50
Number of different types of charges 3.61a 1.69 4.35b 1.73 5.41c 1.96
Note. Means with different subscripts differ significantly, p ⬍ .05. Control ⫽ neither antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) nor psychopathy; ASPD
only ⫽ ASPD met but not psychopathy; ASPD ⫹ PSY ⫽ ASPD met in addition to psychopathy.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the number of nonviolent charges reflected a relatively small effect the local level was expected to substantially and differentially
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

size (d ⫽ .25). However, the differences between ASPD-only activate LH perceptual and motor systems. However, the ASPD-
versus ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates in number of violent charges (d ⫽ only group did not exhibit a similar pattern of cognitive processing
.61) and criminal versatility (d ⫽ .58) represented medium effect impairments. Not only did ASPD-only inmates fail to differ from
sizes, suggesting that ASPD plus comorbid psychopathy has controls in response latencies to local bias condition targets, but
unique predictive utility in understanding criminal behavior. they were also faster than inmates with ASPD plus comorbid
Furthermore, most of these relationships replicated when reex- psychopathy in responding to both global and local targets in this
amined using stringent criteria to form a group of offenders with condition, and the effect sizes for these differences were quite
severe ASPD. Analyses yielded identical findings for versatility large (d ⫽ .79 for local targets; d ⫽ .88 for global targets). In
and violent charges and remained marginally significant for non- contrast, no differences were found among the three groups for
violent criminal activity. Accordingly, although the severity of either target type in the global bias condition. This pattern is
diagnostic criteria for psychopathy may have contributed to the
consistent with the LHA hypothesis because the global bias con-
group difference for nonviolent charges, it did not account for
dition does not differentially activate LH processing systems.
group differences in criminal versatility and violent charges. In
Because this study was the first to examine the LHA hypothesis
short, among inmates with ASPD, the contribution of comorbid
using ASPD-only and APSD plus psychopathy groups, results not
psychopathy to understanding real world criminal behavior is not
only provide evidence of a distinction between ASPD-only and
attributable to the severity of criteria used to diagnose psychopa-
thy. This pattern of findings is not consistent with the view that ASPD ⫹ PSY groups, but also offer the first evidence that LHA
psychopathy is simply a more severe form of ASPD. performance impairments are specific to inmates with psycho-
The group differences in cognitive processing are also incon- pathic traits; there was no evidence for poor performance in LHA
sistent with the view that the two syndromes reflect different levels conditions among inmates with ASPD only. Of course, it remains
of the same underlying construct. There were significant group important to examine the specificity of impairments predicted by
effects for responses to both the global and local targets in the local the LHA hypothesis and other cognitive perspectives (e.g., New-
bias condition. The ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates were much slower than man, Schmitt, & Voss, 1997) using other paradigms. However, in
controls in responding to local bias and local targets (d ⫽ 3.54) and general terms, current findings are in accord with those of Kosson
were slightly but nonsignificantly slower than controls for local et al. (2007) in suggesting that mechanisms underlying psychop-
bias and global targets (d ⫽ .38). This pattern of results is con- athy appear to be specific rather than general. This study was also
sistent with the LHA hypothesis as responding to most targets at the first to demonstrate cognitive processing differences between

Table 3
Group Means and Standard Deviations for Cognitive Processing

Inmate group
Control ASPD only ASPD ⫹ PSY
Response latency variable M SD M SD M SD
b c
Local bias, global trials 812.31 232.03 700.52 184.02 914.44 304.62
Local bias, local trials 579.48a 158.76 572.75a 120.59 694.81c 193.66
Global bias, global trials 592.65 147.45 591.50 109.02 613.02 113.79
Global bias, local trials 732.87 244.64 706.73 207.04 763.38 222.32
Neutral block, global trials 650.73 160.52 637.00 124.39 703.24 184.32
Neutral block, local trials 643.84 190.12 643.95 154.65 654.28 195.65
Note. Sample sizes were 32, 40, and 73 for control groups in the local bias condition, global bias condition, and neutral blocks respectively; 20, 20, and
41 for the ASPD-only groups in the local bias, global bias, and neutral blocks respectively; 18, 21, and 42 for the ASPD ⫹ PSY groups in the local bias
condition, global bias condition, and neutral block respectively. Means with different subscripts differ significantly, p ⬍ .05. Control ⫽ neither antisocial
personality disorder (ASPD) nor psychopathy; ASPD-only ⫽ ASPD met but not psychopathy; ASPD ⫹ PSY ⫽ ASPD met in addition to psychopathy.
338 RISER AND KOSSON

the two ASPD groups and extends the findings of Kosson et al. terized by distinct underlying neural, cognitive, and emotional
(2006) beyond the domain of emotional processing. mechanisms (see also Sargeant et al., 2011).
Current results differed slightly from those of Kosson et al. Several limitations of this study are noteworthy. First, the sam-
(2007). Whereas that study had suggested that psychopathy was ple was restricted to county jail inmates. However, because crimes
associated with relatively general performance impairments under resulting in a sentence to a state prison require a felony conviction
local bias conditions, the deficits observed in this study in offend- and because prisons only include inmates already convicted of
ers with ASPD ⫹ PSY appear somewhat more specific. The offenses, it is sometimes argued that prison samples are less
ASPD ⫹ PSY inmates performed differently than the ASPD-only representative of offenders than samples that include many arrest-
inmates for both local and global targets under local bias condi- ees (Blumstein, Cohen, Roth, & Visher, 1986). The replication of
tions, however they differed significantly from non-ASPD/non- prison sample findings with a county jail sample suggests that the
psychopathic offenders only with respect to local targets. Although criminal behavior findings are relatively robust. Nonetheless, fu-
the effect size for the difference in response latencies to global ture studies should attempt to replicate the cognitive findings
targets was not trivial (d ⫽ .38), it was much smaller than the reported here in other kinds of correctional settings and outside of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

effect size for the difference in response latencies to local targets correctional settings. In addition, it is unknown whether the cog-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(d ⫽ 3.54) and suggests the possibility that only the performance nitive differences reported here will generalize to other cognitive
difference for local targets may reflect a true neurocognitive def- tasks. Moreover, because some relationships between psychopathy
icit. However, no conclusions about whether relative performance and emotional processing impairment do not replicate across dif-
impairments reflect true neuropsychological deficits can be drawn ferent emotional processing paradigms (Brook, Brieman, & Kos-
until the performance of psychopathic individuals can be com- son, 2013), future studies should examine ASPD-only individuals
pared directly to performance norms for nonclinical participants. and ASPD ⫹ PSY individuals on a variety of cognitive and
Consequently, although the group difference for global targets emotional processing tasks. Although we corrected PCL–R scores
might also prove reliable given a larger sample, it appears that for two items to reduce the impact of criminal behavior on psy-
requiring the presence of both ASPD and psychopathic traits chopathy ratings, it remains possible that criminal conduct could
results in a more homogeneous group of offenders with more have contributed to PCL–R scores on several other items. Though
specific performance impairment under LHA conditions than re- many studies of psychopathy-crime relationships do not correct for
quiring only psychopathic traits. Further studies examining the scores on any PCL items (e.g., Hemphill et al., 1998; Porter, Birt,
cognitive performance of offenders with ASPD plus psychopathy & Boer, 2001), correcting for scores on several additional items
may be important not only for resolving the relationship between could further protect against predictor-criterion confounds but
ASPD and psychopathy but for understanding mechanisms under- risks eliminating substantive variance in the assessment of psy-
lying offenders with psychopathic personality. chopathic traits.
In summary, current findings add to earlier evidence for impor- Alternative approaches to conducting the analyses are worth
tant distinctions between ASPD versus ASPD plus psychopathy consideration. Given the evidence that psychopathy appears to be
based on antisocial behavior and emotional processing. Future continuous, examining these relationships using a dimensional
studies should address the replicability of the group differences approach might also prove informative.5 In addition, because we
now reported in both cognitive and emotional processing as well as subdivided offenders with ASPD into groups based on whether
the possibility of additional differences between these ASPD PCL–R scores were at or above versus below 30, our ASPD ⫹
groups in physiological, emotional, and cognitive processing. In PSY and ASPD-only groups overlap somewhat with the older
fact, a number of other recent studies suggest additional important distinction between psychopathic, middle, and nonpsychopathic
distinctions between ASPD with versus without psychopathy. In groups. More concretely, most of our ASPD-only participants
particular, Rogers and Rogstad (2010) reported that offenders with (83%) had PCL–R scores between 20 and 30, but only about half
ASPD plus psychopathy were characterized by more severe alex- (49%) of our control group participants had PCL–R scores below
ithymia than offenders with ASPD alone. In addition, Gregory et 20. In short, although there is some overlap between the ASPD-
al. (2012) recently argued that there are structural differences only group and the group sometimes referred to as the middle
between the brains of violent offenders with ASPD-only versus group in older studies, it is not clear whether an analysis based
ASPD plus psychopathy. solely on traditional PCL–R cutoffs would yield results similar to
Taken together such findings do not appear consistent with or different from the results of the current study. Finally, given the
proposed personality disorder revisions for the upcoming DSM-V. substantial and asymmetric overlap between ASPD and psychop-
Although designed to capture the psychopathy syndrome more athy, ASPD ⫹ PSY, ASPD-only and psychopathy-only groups
effectively than DSM–IV, current proposals continue to treat could not be directly compared on external criteria. Although the
ASPD as a single diagnostic category (http://www.dsm5.org/ direct comparison of these groups would require very large sam-
proposedrevision/Pages/PersonalityDisorders.aspd). Although the ples, such comparisons would be particularly informative.
aim of including psychopathy in the DSM-V is laudable, merging Despite limitations, findings corroborate the hypothesis that
the two constructs into one syndrome would result in a consider- ASPD with and without comorbid psychopathic features are syn-
able reduction in diagnostic specificity. If one goal of the DSM-V
revision is to increase the validity and utility of diagnoses, current 5
However, the substantial correlation between psychopathy scores and
and other recent findings suggest that allowing for classification of dimensional measures of ASPD would result in a loss of substantial
individuals with ASPD both with and without comorbid psychop- variance in the measure of each construct and render interpretation of
athy would help to identify specific groups of individuals charac- unique associations difficult (Lynam, Hoyle, & Newman, 2006).
EFFECTS OF COMORBID PSYCHOPATHY 339

dromes characterized by distinct mechanisms and challenge the Hare, R. D. (1980). A research scale for the assessment of psychopathy in
perspective that the two reflect the same underlying pathophysi- criminal populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 111–
ology. Further studies that take into account the asymmetric over- 117.
lap between psychopathy and ASPD are needed to establish the Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy and Antisocial Personality Disorder: A
generality of the distinctions reported here and in Kosson et al. Case of Diagnostic Confusion. Psychiatric Times, 13(2), 39 – 40.
(2006) and in order to examine other correlates of these two costly Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (2nd ed.).
Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.
syndromes associated with antisocial behavior.
Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., & Thornton, D. (2000). Psychopathy and
the predictive validity of the PCL-R: An international perspective.
References Behavioral Science and the Law, 18, 623– 645. doi:10.1002/1099-
0798(200010)18:5⬍623::AID-BSL409⬎3.0.CO;2-W
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the
ual of mental disorder: Text revisions (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
DSM–IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnor-
Author.
mal Psychology, 100, 391–398. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.391
Blumstein, A., Cohen, J., Roth, J., & Visher, C. A. (1986). Criminal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hare, R. D., & McPherson, L. M. (1984). Violent and aggressive behavior


careers and career criminals (vol. 1). Washington, DC: National Acad-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

emy Press. by criminal psychopaths. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry,


Brinkley, C. A., Schmitt, W. A., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (2001). 7, 35–50. doi:10.1016/0160-2527(84)90005-0
Construct validation of a self-report psychopath scale: Does Levenson’s Hare, R. D., Williamson, S. E, & Harpur, T. J. (1988). Psychopathy and
SRPS measure the same construct as Hare’s PCL-R? Personality and language. In T. E. Moffitt & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Biological contri-
Individual Differences, 31, 1021–1038. doi:10.1016/S0191- butions to crime causation (pp. 68 –92). Dordrecht, the Netherlands:
8869(00)00178-1 Martinus Nijhoif.
Brook, M., Brieman, C., & Kosson, D. S. (2013). Emotional processing in Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Cormier, C. A. (1991). Psychopathy and
Psychopathy Checklist-assessed psychopathy: A review of the literature. violent recidivism. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 625– 637. doi:
Manuscript submitted for publication. 10.1007/BF01065856
Bucholz, K. K., Hesselbrock, V. M., Health, A. C., Kramer, J. R., & Hart, S. D., Kropp, P. R., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Performance of male
Schuckit, M. A. (2000). Research report. A latent class analysis of psychopaths following conditional release from prison. Journal of Con-
antisocial personality disorder symptom data from a multi-centre family sulting and Clinical Psychology, 56. 227–232. doi:10.1037/0022-006X
study of alcoholism. Addiction, 95, 553–567. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443 .56.2.227
.2000.9545537.x Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and
Cacciola, J. S., Alterman, A. I., Rutherford, M. J., & Snider, E. C. (1995). recidivism: A review. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 139 –
Treatment response of antisocial substance abusers. Journal of Nervous 170. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8333.1998.tb00355.x
and Mental Disease, 183, 166 –171. doi:10.1097/00005053-199503000- Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Munoz, L. C., & Aucoin, K. J. (2008).
00007 Callous-unemotional traits and the emotional processing of distress cues
Cleckley, H. (1988). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby. in detained boys: Testing the moderating role of aggression, exposure to
Coid, J. (2002). Personality disorders in prisoners and their motivation for community violence, and histories of abuse. Development and Psycho-
dangerousness and disruptive behaviour. Criminal Behaviour and Men- pathology, 20, 569 –589. doi:10.1017/S095457940800028X
tal Health, 12, 209 –226. Kirkman, C. A. (2008). Psychopathy: A confusing clinical construct.
Cunningham, M. D., & Reidy, T. (1998). Antisocial personality disorder Journal of Forensic Nursing, 4, 29 –39.
and psychopathy: Diagnostic dilemmas in classifying patterns of anti- Knight, R. A., & Prentky, R. A. (1990). Classifying sexual offenders: The
social behavior in sentencing evaluations. Behavioral Sciences & the development and corroboration of taxonomic models. In W. L. Marshall,
Law, 16, 333–351. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0798(199822)16:3⬍333:: D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of sexual assault:
AID-BSL314⬎3.0.CO;2-N Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender (pp. 23–52). New York,
Devita, E., Forth, A., & Hare, R. D. (1990). Family background of male
NY: Plenum Press.
criminal psychopaths [Abstract]. Canadian Psychology, 31, 346.
Kosson, D. S. (1998). Divided visual attention in psychopathic and non-
Edens, J. F., Skopp, N. A., & Cahill, M. A. (2008). Psychopathic features
psychopathic offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 24,
moderate the relationship between harsh and inconsistent parental dis-
373–391. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00176-1
cipline and adolescent antisocial behavior. Journal of Clinical Child and
Kosson, D. S., Lorenz, A., & Newman, J. P. (2006). Effects of co-morbid
Adolescent Psychology, 37, 472– 476. doi:10.1080/15374410801955938
psychopathy on criminal offending and emotional processing male of-
Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak,
fenders with antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
R. S. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural mechanisms involved in the
processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual chology, 115, 798 – 806. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.798
stimuli. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 120, 1779 –1791. doi:10.1093/ Kosson, D. S., Miller, S. K., Byrnes, K. A., & Leveroni, C. L. (2007).
brain/120.10.1779 Testing neuropsychological hypotheses for cognitive deficits in psycho-
Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., Mednick, S. A., & Venables, P. (2007). Early pathic criminals: A study of global-local processing, Journal of the
temperamental and psychophysiological precursors of adult psycho- International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 267–276. doi:10.1017/
pathic personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 508 –518. S1355617707070294
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.508 Kosson, D. S., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Evaluating the
Gregory, S., ffytche, D., Simmons, A., Kumari, V., Howard, M., Hodgins, construct validity of psychopathy in Black and White male inmates: The
S., & Blackwood, N. (2012). The antisocial brain: Psychopathy matters: preliminary studies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 250 –259.
A structural MRI investigation of antisocial male violent offenders. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.99.3.250
Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 962–972. doi:10.1001/archgenpsy- Kranzler, H. R., Del Boca, F. K., & Rounsaville, B. J. (1996). Comorbid
chiatry.2012.222 psychiatric diagnosis predicts three-year outcomes in alcoholics: A
Gurley, J. R. (2009). A history of changes to the criminal personality in the posttreatment natural history study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 57,
DSM. History of Psychology, 12, 285–304. doi:10.1037/a0018101 619 – 626.
340 RISER AND KOSSON

Krischer, M. K., & Sevecke, K. (2008). Early traumatization and psychop- Rice, M. E., Harris, G. T., & Cormier, C. A. (1992). An evaluation of a
athy in female and male juvenile offenders. International Journal of Law maximum security therapeutic community for psychopaths and other
and Psychiatry, 31, 253–262. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.008 mentally disordered offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 399 – 412.
Lapierre, D., Braun, C. M., & Hodgins, S. (1995). Ventral frontal deficits doi:10.1007/BF02352266
in psychopathy: Neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychologia, Rogers, R., & Rogstad, J. E. (2010). Psychopathy and APD in non-forensic
33, 139 –151. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(94)00110-B patients: Improved predictions or disparities in cut scores? Journal of
Lee, J. H. (1999). The treatment of psychopathic and antisocial personality Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 353–362.
disorders: A review. Clinical Decision Making Support Unit, Broadmoor Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1996). A review and
Hospital, Berkshire. meta-analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist and Psychopathy Check-
Lewis, C. E. (1991). Neurochemical mechanisms of chronic antisocial list—Revised: Predictive validity of dangerousness. Clinical Psychol-
behavior (psychopathy). A literature review. Journal of Nervous and ogy: Science and Practice, 3, 203–215. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.1996
Mental Disease, 179, 720 –727. doi:10.1097/00005053-199112000- .tb00071.x
00002 Sargeant, M. N., Daughters, S. B., Curtin, J. J., Schuster, R., & Lejuez,
Llanes, S., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Divided visual attention and left C. W. (2011). Unique roles of antisocial personality disorder and psy-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

hemisphere activation among psychopathic and nonpsychopathic of- chopathic traits in distress tolerance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
fenders. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

120, 987–992. doi:10.1037/a0024161


9 –18. doi:10.1007/s10862-006-4533-2 Schraft, C. V., Kosson, D. S., & McBride, C. K. (in press). Exposure to
Lorenz, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2002a). Utilization of emotion cues in violence within home and community environments and psychopathic
male and female offenders with antisocial personality disorder: Results tendencies in adolescents. Criminal Justice and Behavior.
from a lexical decision task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, Serin, R. C. (1991). Psychopathy and violence in criminals. Journal of
513–516. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.3.513 Interpersonal Violence, 6, 423– 431. doi:10.1177/088626091006004002
Lorenz, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2002b). Deficient response modulation Serin, R. C., & Amos, N. L. (1995). The role of psychopathy in the
and emotion processing in low-anxious Caucasian psychopathic offend- assessment of dangerousness. International Journal of Law and Psychi-
ers: Results from a lexical decision task. Emotion, 2, 91–104. doi:
atry, 18, 231–238. doi:10.1016/0160-2527(95)00008-6
10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.91
Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Alcohol and drug abuse—
Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopath personality. The
dependence disorders in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 6 –10. doi:10.1037/
offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 430 – 439. doi:10.1037/
h0047232
0021-843X.99.4.430
Lynam, D. R., Hoyle, R. H., & Newman, J. P. (2006). The perils of
Stevens, G. F. (1993). Applying the diagnosis of antisocial personality to
partialling: Examples from aggression and psychopathy. Assessment, 13,
imprisoned offenders: Looking for hay in a haystack. Journal of Of-
328 –341. doi:10.1177/1073191106290562
fender Rehabilitation, 19, 1–26. doi:10.1300/J076v19n01_01
Malloy, P., Noel, N., Rogers, S., Longabaugh, R., & Beattie, M. (1989).
Suchy, Y., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Forming, switching, and maintaining
Risk factors for neuropsychological impairment in alcoholics: Antisocial
mental sets among psychopathic offenders during verbal and nonverbal
personality, age, years of drinking, and gender. Journal of Studies on
tasks: Another look at the left-hemisphere activation hypothesis. Journal
Alcohol and Drugs, 50, 422.
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 538 –548. doi:
Messina, N., Wish, E., Hoffman, J., & Nemes, S. (2001). Diagnosing
antisocial personality disorder among substance abusers: The SCID 10.1017/S135561770606070X
versus The MCMI-II. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th
Abuse, 27, 699 –717. doi:10.1081/ADA-100107663 ed.). New York, NY: Harper Collins.
Mitchell, D. G. V., Colledge, E., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2002). Vaidyanathan, U., Hall, J. R., Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. M. (2011).
Risky decisions and response reversal: Is there evidence of orbito-frontal Clarifying the role of defensive reactivity deficits in psychopathy and
cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia, 40, antisocial personality using startle reflex methodology. Journal of Ab-
2013–2022. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00056-8 normal Psychology, 120, 253–258. doi:10.1037/a0021224
Newman, J. P., & Kosson, D. S. (1986). Passive avoidance leaning in Van Kleeck, M. H. (1989). Hemispheric differences in global versus local
psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psy- processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: New data
chology, 95, 252–256. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.95.3.252 and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1165–
Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A., & Voss, W. D. (1997). The impact of 1178. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90099-7
motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: Assessing the Walsh, Z., Allen, L. C., & Kosson, D. S. (2007). Beyond social deviance:
generality of the response modulation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Substance-specific relationships with PCL-R facets. Journal of Person-
Psychology, 106, 563–575. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.563 ality Disorders, 21, 273–288. doi:10.1521/pedi.2007.21.3.273
Patrick, C. J., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Emotion in the Williamson, S., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1987). Violence: Criminal
criminal psychopath: Fear imagery processing. Journal of Abnormal psychopaths and their victims. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science,
Psychology, 103, 523–534. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.103.3.523 19, 454 – 462. doi:10.1037/h0080003
Porter, S., Birt, A. R., & Boer, D. P. (2001). Investigation of the criminal Williamson, S., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing
and conditional relsease of Canadian federal offenders as a function of of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28, 260 –273.
psychopathy and age. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 647– 661. doi: doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb02192.x
10.1023/A:1012710424821 Wootton, J. M., Frick, P. J., Shelton, K. K., & Silverthorn, P. (1997).
Reid, W. H. (2001). Antisocial personality, psychopathy, and forensic Ineffective parenting and childhood conduct problems: The moderating
psychiatry. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 7, 55–58. doi:10.1097/ role of callous unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
00131746-200101000-00008 Psychology, 65, 301–308. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.65.2.292.b

You might also like