Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Article

Journal of Marketing
2021, Vol. 85(5) 145–160
The Control–Effort Trade-Off in ª American Marketing Association 2021
Article reuse guidelines:

Participative Pricing: How Easing Pricing sagepub.com/journals-permissions


DOI: 10.1177/0022242921990351
journals.sagepub.com/home/jmx
Decisions Enhances Purchase Outcomes

Cindy Xin Wang, Joshua T. Beck, and Hong Yuan

Abstract
Participative pricing strategies may influence consumer purchase decisions; this research proposes specifically that firms’
delegation of pricing decisions to consumers can create a control–effort trade-off. Consumers favor greater pricing control but
are deterred by the effort involved in deciding what to pay. Strategies such as pay what you want in turn might reduce purchase
intentions due to the effort involved. In contrast, strategies that increase feelings of control but not perceived effort, such as pick
your price options that let consumers choose from a limited set of prices, could enhance pricing outcomes. A field study and four
laboratory experiments confirm these propositions. The findings demonstrate the mixed effects of participative pricing, identify
mediating mechanisms that explain these effects, and specify common moderating conditions that shape the outcomes of
participative pricing. These results have notable implications for pricing theory and practice.

Keywords
control, effort, pay what you want, pick your price, pricing
Online supplement: https://doi.org/10.1177/0022242921990351

In most purchase situations, the price is fixed, and consumers overall positive effect of a novel pick your price (PYP) strat-
choose to take it or leave it. But marketers have begun experi- egy, which allows consumers to choose a price from a set of
menting with pricing strategies that delegate some or all of the options (Ismael 2017). Both PWYW and PYP enhance feelings
price determination task to consumers. Such participative of pricing control, but PYP does not increase pricing effort,
pricing strategies, in which “consumers participate in setting because consumers find it relatively easy to make constrained
a final price for a product” (Chandran and Morwitz 2005, choices (Chernev, Böckenholt, and Goodman 2015).
p. 249), can engage consumers, boost sales, enhance brand loy- Using this control–effort framework, we investigate the
alty, and contribute to competitive positions (Chen, Koenigsberg, effects of PWYW and PYP across five studies in a variety of
and Zhang 2017; Spann and Tellis 2006). Yet many firms— purchase contexts. Study 1 establishes the strong performance
including Priceline (Bhattarai 2016), Panera Bread (Houck of PYP relative to fixed prices and PWYW in a field quasi-
2019), and the Metropolitan Museum of Art in New York experiment that involves actual purchases and outlines profit
(Welss 2018)—that have experimented with participative pricing implications. Study 2 indicates that compared with a fixed
also have abandoned it and reverted to fixed prices. We critically price, PWYW decreases purchase intentions due to the high
examine participative pricing to understand how and in which effort required, even though it confers full pricing control. In
conditions it might enhance purchase outcomes. contrast, PYP provides a sense of pricing control but does
In particular, we theorize about a tension that consumers not affect pricing effort, so it increases purchase intentions.
experience when evaluating a purchase situation that involves
participative pricing. Consumers’ participation in determining
the final price triggers a sense of greater pricing control Cindy Xin Wang is Assistant Professor of Marketing, Orfalea College of
(a positive purchase driver) but also feelings of more signifi- Business, California Polytechnic State University, USA (email: cwang96@
cant pricing effort (a negative purchase driver). Building on calpoly.edu). Joshua T. Beck is Associate Professor of Marketing, Lundquist
College of Business, University of Oregon, USA (email: jbeck2@uoregon
this proposition, we predict an overall negative effect of a pay .edu). Hong Yuan is Richard P. Booth Associate Professor of Marketing and
what you want (PWYW) pricing strategy because of the high Research Scholar, Lundquist College of Business, University of Oregon, USA
effort involved in deciding the final price. We also predict an (email: hongy@uoregon.edu).
146 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

These effects hold regardless of the price level. Study 3 demon- own price, and PWYW (for a review, see Kim, Natter, and
strates that shopping motives (to save money or time) moderate Spann [2009]). In its unique approach, PWYW involves one
the effects of effort and control on purchase intentions. buyer and one seller (vs. auctions, which involve many buyers),
Whereas PWYW is more effective than a fixed price when and the seller must take whatever price is selected (vs. negoti-
consumers are motivated to save money, PYP is more effective ations, where sellers can counter). This full delegation of price
in general. When consumers are motivated to save time, PYP is determination can be exciting for consumers and risky for
just as effective as a fixed price and more effective than firms. In a sense, PWYW is the most participative option;
PWYW. Study 4 directly manipulates pricing effort and control consumers have full say over the final price, and because they
while holding the pricing strategy (PYP) constant to validate might choose to pay $0, firms worry about the performance
the underlying process. Study 5 identifies single (vs. multiple) implications. Yet marketers grew increasingly interested in
purchase decisions as a crucial moderating condition, such that PWYW after the popular and successful 2007 launch of a
the positive effect of PYP on purchase choice attenuates over PWYW-priced album by the band Radiohead (Tyrangiel
multiple purchase decisions, which require a great deal of effort. 2007). Research since then has characterized PWYW as a novel
The current research thus makes several contributions to and attractive pricing strategy (e.g., Gneezy et al. 2010; Kim,
pricing literature. First, we explicate the mixed effectiveness Natter, and Spann 2009).
of participative pricing strategies by examining psychological
mechanisms that drive their effects. Second, we test a novel Challenges of PWYW. Despite PWYW’s initial successes, many
form of participative pricing (PYP) and show that providing firms that adopted it have reverted to fixed-price models: Price-
consumers with price options can enhance retail purchase out- line abandoned PWYW pricing for airfares in 2016 (Bhattarai
comes. Although PYP has been used in various forms in non- 2016), Panera Bread closed its last remaining PWYW restau-
profit and tipping contexts, it has not been tested in a for-profit rant in Boston (Houck 2019), and the Metropolitan Museum of
context or compared with other participative pricing strategies. Art in New York ended its PWYW ticket policy in 2018, when
In exploring PWYW and PYP, we conceptualize pricing con- the museum president and chief executive officer Daniel H.
trol and pricing effort and empirically test our control–effort Welss (2018, p. 1) described the strategy as “no longer suffi-
trade-off framework. Previous research has focused mainly on cient to meet the Museum’s daily operational demands.”
explaining why consumers would pay more than $0 and iden- Lower-than-expected payments may have caused some rever-
tified social factors, such as altruism, fairness, and price con- sions. For example, PWYW is more effective when consumers
sciousness, that determine the price paid (Kim, Natter, and are motivated by fairness (Gerpott and Schneider 2016), charity
Spann 2009; Schmidt, Spann, and Zeithammer 2015). We (Gneezy et al. 2010), or relational concerns (Stangl, Kastner,
advance this stream of literature by demonstrating a key role and Prayag 2017). In addition, offering a reference price may
of pricing effort, which can deter consumers from making cause consumers to pay only that or below that price because
PWYW purchases. Third, we show that participative pricing paying above it seems irrational. Finally, consumers simply
strategies perform differently in various contexts, such as when appear reluctant to pay for some PWYW offerings.
consumers are motivated to save time versus money and make However, PWYW is not just challenging for firms; it also
single versus multiple purchase decisions. The framework thus can be difficult for consumers. We theorize that two fundamen-
provides practical guidance for enhancing the purchase out- tal tensions exist for consumers. First, they are attracted to the
comes of participative pricing strategies in the marketplace. economically advantageous pricing terms (i.e., pay anything,
including $0), which permits them to maximize their utility.
Second, consumers must decide the final price, which takes
Conceptual Development effort; even though a $0 payment is an option, it often is not
Prices are often fixed before the point of sale, whereas partici- in the best interest of the long-term exchange (i.e., consumers
pative pricing delegates some or all of the price determination understand that sellers need revenue to maintain their ongoing
to the consumer (Kim, Natter, and Spann 2009). Forms of operations). Moreover, paying a price that appears socially
participative pricing have been in use for millennia (e.g., auc- inappropriate may have adverse reputational effects (e.g., they
tions date back to 500 BCE; Krishna 2002), and today, they may be viewed as cheap). Thus, consumers must weigh the
appear in various business models adopted by restaurants, benefits of pricing control against the costs of pricing effort.
musicians, gaming companies, travel providers, charities, and We refer to these countervailing forces as the control–effort
others (Charness and Cheung 2013; Thomaselli 2012; trade-off.
Tyrangiel 2007; Warren, Hanson, and Yuan 2021).
Pick your price (PYP) as a novel strategy. PYP, which asks con-
sumers to choose the price they prefer to pay from multiple
PWYW and PYP as Participative Pricing Strategies price options, has not been studied formally in pricing litera-
Prior marketing literature suggests that how participative pric- ture. We regard it as an evolution of PWYW, in that consumers
ing is presented to consumers matters (Krishna et al. 2002). still have the final say over the price, but they do not have to
Participative pricing also comes in many forms, including auc- generate the price options themselves. This format is increas-
tions, reverse auctions, exchanges, negotiations, name your ingly common in charity and tipping contexts. We also note a
Wang et al. 147

commercial application by Everlane, an online clothing retai- I have over prices?), whereas effort involves the use of cogni-
ler. A well-received “choose what you pay” promotion allowed tive resources (how effortful is it to think about price?).
shoppers to pick from among three prices when buying a prod- Because PYP involves selecting from among several
uct (Ismael 2017). We contend that PYP offers similar benefits options, it should not demand as much pricing effort. Studies
as PWYW (high pricing control) without the accompanying of choice difficulty provide a useful analog: consumers claim
disadvantages (high pricing effort). If this prediction holds, they want many choice options but also experience “choice
PYP should outperform PWYW in many pricing situations and overload” when trying to select from among a vast assortment
offer a novel, potentially powerful pricing strategy. (Chernev, Böckenholt, and Goodman 2015; Schwartz 2004). In
a participative pricing context, with maximal pricing control,
determining the price is akin to making a selection from a large
Key Mediating Roles of Pricing Control and Effort set of options (e.g., should a cookie cost $.50, $.75, $1.00,
Consumers want pricing control. Consumers have a strong $1.25, $1.50, $1.75 . . . ?). Rather than leaving the price com-
economic interest in determining a price. We define pricing pletely open-ended, a firm could reduce the required pricing
control as the perceived level of personal influence over the effort by asking consumers to pick from a limited number of
process of determining the final price paid for a good or ser- pricing options. The effort required to make such a decision
vice. From a neoclassical economic perspective, consumers may be the same or slightly more than the evaluation of a fixed
should want the highest pricing control possible to pay the price. In summary, we predict that PWYW generally underper-
lowest price possible (i.e., $0) (Allen 1938; Strotz 1955). forms fixed pricing and PYP due to the greater pricing effort it
Increased feelings of control also have an array of affective demands, which overwhelms the benefits of the greater pricing
and cognitive psychological benefits (Beck, Rahinel, and Ble- control it offers; in contrast, PYP should generally outperform
ier 2020; Koriat, Lichtenstein, and Fischhoff 1980; Langer and fixed pricing and PWYW because it primarily increases pricing
Rodin 1976). Consumers arguably hold favorable attitudes control. Formally:
toward brands that grant them a sense of pricing control
H1: Relative to fixed price, (a) PWYW generally reduces
(Henderson, Beck, and Palmatier 2011; Kim, Kaufmann, and
and (b) PYP generally increases purchase intentions.
Stegemann 2014). Pricing control also is linked to economic
rewards. Both PWYW and PYP allow consumers to pay cus- H2: Relative to a fixed price, (a) PWYW and (b) PYP
tom prices and offer higher levels of pricing control than fixed increase consumers’ feelings of pricing control, and
pricing; we posit that pricing control as a positive purchase (c) PWYW increases consumers’ feelings of pricing
driver explains positive outcomes of PWYW and PYP. effort.
H3: (a) The negative effect of PWYW (vs. fixed price) on
Consumers avoid pricing effort. Participative pricing strategies
purchase intentions is mediated by pricing effort, whereas
also trigger pricing effort, or psychological feelings of mental
(b) the positive effect of PYP (vs. fixed price) on purchase
exertion associated with the price determination. As we have
intentions is mediated by pricing control.
discussed, if consumers consider only economic utility, they
always pay the lowest price possible, which is an easy decision.
How Shopping Motives Shape the Control–Effort
Yet the average prices paid by consumers in PWYW settings
are significantly greater than zero (Kim, Natter, and Spann
Trade-Off
2009), suggesting that determining a price is a complex process Building on this framework, we consider several moderating
shaped by social and psychological factors (Gerpott and factors that may shape the (dis)advantages of PWYW and PYP.
Schneider 2016; Gneezy et al. 2012), including avoiding neg- Consumers often approach purchase decisions with different
ative feelings of guilt or shame for overpaying (wasting money) motives, such as convenience, greater selection, lower prices,
or underpaying (appearing cheap) (Kunter 2015). It can be unique offerings, or support for local merchants (Darden and
difficult to arrive at an “appropriate” PWYW price, even with Reynolds 1971; Jasper and Lan 1992; Jin, He, and Zhang
a reference price, because it is unclear whether a typical pay- 2014). In this process, saving time and saving money are com-
ment is correct (Gerpott and Schneider 2016; Kim, Kaufmann, mon decision motives (Duxbury et al. 2005).
and Stegemann 2014). Because PWYW involves limitless pay- When consumers are motivated to save money, they focus
ment options, it also might prompt busy consumers with lim- their attention on their level of control over the price, because
ited mental resources to opt out of the decision and avoid higher pricing control allows consumers to save more. The
purchasing (Iyengar and Lepper 2000). Such consumers may perceptions of pricing effort are minimized, as consumers are
be more likely to accept a fixed price from a competing seller already willing to expend effort to save money (Xia,
rather than paying the same or even a lower price under Kukar-Kinney, and Monroe 2010). Money-saving promotions,
PWYW (Gneezy et al. 2012). We present pricing effort as a such as coupons, require consumer effort (Schneider and Cur-
negative driver that explains the negative impact of PWYW on rim 1991), yet consumers are enticed by the incentives
purchase intentions. To compare PWYW with PYP, we thus (Mulhern and Padgett 1995). Coupon promotions even increase
distinguish conceptually between control and effort: control the number of shopping trips to stores (Walters and Rinne
involves a situational evaluation (how much control do 1986). Thus, consumers motivated to save money likely focus
148 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

more on pricing control and less on pricing effort, such that the H6: The number of purchase decisions moderates the
negative effect of PWYW on consumer purchases should be effect of PYP versus fixed price on purchase intentions,
attenuated. Then, both PWYW and PYP should increase pur- such that the positive effect of PYP on purchase inten-
chase intentions because they elicit high pricing control. tions is attenuated after multiple purchase decisions.
Formally: To test these hypotheses, we conduct five studies (one field
quasi-experiment and four laboratory experiments) that collec-
H4: The motivation to save money moderates the effect of
tively confirm our theoretical framework (see Figure 1) and
PWYW and PYP on purchase intentions, such that when
demonstrate how PWYW and PYP distinctly shape consumer
consumers are motivated to save money, both (a) PYP
purchases.
and (b) PWYW have positive effects on purchase inten-
tions, compared with a fixed price.
If, instead, consumers are motivated to save time, their focus Study 1
may shift away from pricing control and toward pricing effort. We examine the effects of PWYW and PYP in a field experi-
The process of making a purchase decision, especially if it ment, with an emphasis on how PYP may enhance purchases
entails extra effort, can be time consuming and exhausting; it and revenue. Consistent with H1, we expect PYP to outperform
even can reduce consumers’ brand loyalty and fairness percep- both PWYW and fixed pricing. In a retail setting, we manipu-
tions (Mehta, Rajiv, and Srinivasan 2003; Xia, Kukar-Kinney, lated the pricing strategy and measured the number of units
and Monroe 2010). Consumers prefer quick, convenient, and sold and revenues. Specifically, we partnered with a bakery
less effortful purchase decisions, particularly when they are chain and varied the pricing strategy for one of its core prod-
more motivated to save time (Childers et al. 2011; Johnson and ucts: freshly baked cookies.
Goldstein 2003). Expending effort instead demands time and
resources. Therefore, PWYW should be less effective than PYP
or fixed pricing when consumers are motivated to save time, as
Methods
the motive to save time minimizes consumers’ focus on pricing Quasi-experimental design. The study relied on random varia-
control and may enhance their focus on pricing effort, which tions of three pricing strategies (PYP, PWYW, and fixed price)
could nullify some benefits of PYP. We propose the following: at 50-minute intervals for 15 business hours (5 total hours per
strategy) throughout three afternoons. The interval was deter-
H5: The motivation to save time moderates the effect of mined by the store hours, trained staff members’ working sche-
PWYW and PYP on purchase intentions, such that when dules, and the goal of ensuring even time allocations of all
consumers are motivated to save time, (a) PYP and fixed pricing conditions. Consumers self-selected into the study by
price have similar effects but (b) PWYW has a negative entering the store.
effect on purchase intentions compared with a fixed price.
Procedure and measures. The popular bakery offers traditional
How Shopping Context Shapes the Control–Effort baked goods (e.g., pastries, muffins, cakes), including freshly
baked cookies priced at $.60 each. For this study, the bakery
Trade-Off agreed to vary its cookie pricing strategy to test the effect on
Beyond shopping motives, it is important to note that the shop- sales. No other promotions occurred during this time. The study
ping context also influences the effect of PYP on purchase ran during regular business hours, from 1:00 P.M. to 6:00 P.M.,
outcomes. A shopping trip might involve a single or multiple for three days (Thursday, Friday, and Saturday; 15 hours total).
purchase decisions, depending on consumers’ shopping goals Employees were trained as confederates but were blind to the
and purposes (Arentze, Oppewal, and Timmermans 2005; hypotheses. The pricing strategy was manipulated by signage.
Harlam and Lodish 1995). Multiple purchases in a shopping In the PWYW condition, consumers were instructed to “pay
trip incur more effort because they require multiple decisions, what you want” (no reference price). In the fixed-price condi-
involving a large number of choices. Evaluating perceived risk tion, cookies were priced at the usual $.60 each. In the PYP
and uncertainty across multiple items also increases the level of condition, consumers were asked to “pick your price” and pro-
effort required (Bettman, Luce, and Payne, 1998; Schwartz vided four prices from which to choose ($.45, $.55, $.65, $.75),
2004). Although the pricing effort of PYP remains low for an which averaged $.60. See study materials and measures for this
individual product, the evaluation of multiple price options for and following studies in the Web Appendix.
multiple products can become more effortful in aggregate. In each condition, the signage was displayed for an equal
When consumers are involved in multiple purchases, they amount of time. To approximate a random assignment, the staff
likely try to preserve mental resources to make all the needed changed the pricing strategy in randomly determined
purchase decisions, instead of expending unnecessary effort on 50-minute intervals. The staff were careful to ensure that con-
each item, even if that effort might lead to more favorable sumers were unaware of the change. The retailer recorded the
prices and more satisfying experiences. Therefore, we expect unit sales and cookie revenue generated in each condition,
that when multiple purchases are involved, the effect of PYP is which served as our primary dependent variable, and kept a
attenuated. Formally: log of questions and comments as a source of qualitative data.
Wang et al. 149

Shopping Context

Multiple
Purchases
Participative Pricing
Pricing Strategy Mechanism

+ −
Pay What You Want + Pricing Effort
(PWYW)

Purchase
Outcomes
+
Pick Your Price + Pricing Control
(PYP)
+
+ + + +

Motivation to Motivation to
Save Time Save Money

Shopping Motives

Figure 1. Conceptual model.

Results tend to pay?” Second, employees anecdotally reported that


consumers spent a longer time thinking about the transaction,
The retailer sold 526 units and generated $260 in revenue from
whereas they responded more quickly and decisively in the
cookie sales (Figure 2). To understand the role of pricing strat-
PYP condition.
egy, we first examined whether the pricing condition affected
units sold by calculating the proportion in each pricing condi-
tion as a percent of the total units sold across conditions. Comparison with a Traditional Sales Promotion
A chi-square test of proportions revealed a significant differ- Because the average price in the PYP condition was $.50 per
ence in proportion by pricing condition (fixed price ¼ 30.80%, unit (vs. the usual price of $.60 per unit), the PYP strategy
PWYW ¼ 22.43%, PYP ¼ 46.77%; w2 ¼ 49.72, p < .001). The arguably may have generated more unit sales merely due to
store sold more units in the PYP condition than the fixed-price lower prices. We instead propose that the effects are driven by
condition (w2 ¼ 17.67, p < .01) or the PWYW condition greater pricing control, paired with an appropriate amount of
(w2 ¼ 46.43, p < .001). In addition, PWYW resulted in fewer required effort (choosing rather than forming the price). To
units sold than did fixed pricing (w2 ¼ 7.25, p < .01). Units explore these two possibilities, we collected additional data
purchased in the PYP condition generated revenues of $122.10, in a discount condition in the same setting and priced the
compared with $40.70 in the PWYW and $97.20 in the cookies at $.50 highlighted by a sign that also indicated the
fixed-price conditions. The business did not share cost informa- regular price of $.60. The PYP condition in the main study
tion, but with a conservative assumption of 33% variable costs still outperforms the fixed discount price in this study (with
($.20 per cookie), the profits in the PYP condition are higher the caveat that these data were collected a week apart): A $.50
($73.36) than in the fixed price ($64.80) and PWYW ($16.90) discounted price generated 203 unit sales (cf. 246 sold with
conditions. In this calculation, PYP increased profits by 13% PYP; w2 ¼ 4.30, p < .05). We also recorded the units sold the
relative to fixed prices and by 334% relative to PWYW. next day, during the same experimental window (1:00–6:00
The average price paid by participants in the PYP condition P.M.) but without a prominent pricing sign, as an additional
was $.50 per unit (vs. $.34 in PWYW and $.60 in fixed price). control condition. The bakery generated 161 unit sales, nearly
That is, participants in the PWYW condition purchased fewer identical to the amount sold in the fixed-price condition in the
units and also paid lower prices (F(2, 523) ¼ 521.10, p < .001). main study (162 units, w2 ¼ .003, p ¼ .96).
See price paid details in the Web Appendix. The qualitative
data also provide some insights into consumers’ reactions to the
pricing strategies. First, the PWYW strategy results in greater Discussion
pricing effort, such that consumers voiced questions such as, Study 1 provides initial support for our proposed framework.
“What price should I pay?” and “What price do other people Consistent with H1, we find that, relative to a conventional
150 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

Revenue Generated by Pricing Strategy

$140.00
Pay What You Want
16% Fixed Price $120.00
22%
$100.00

$73.80
$80.00
$61.20
$64.80
$60.00
Pick Your Price
34% Discount
Price $40.00
28% $16.52
$49.20
$20.00 $40.80
$32.40
$23.60
Proportion of Units Sold by
$-
Pricing Strategy Fixed Price Discount Price PYP PWYW

Cost Profit

Figure 2. Study 1: effect of pricing strategies on sales performance.

fixed price, PWYW has a negative effect on purchase inten- mediating roles of pricing effort and pricing control on pur-
tions, whereas PYP has a positive effect. Sales records show chase intentions (H3) in Study 2. Fourth, we rule out perceived
that PYP outperforms all other pricing strategies, with 108% fairness as an alternative mediator. Bidding is generally per-
more units sold than PWYW, 52% more units than the fixed ceived as fairer than retailer-set prices (Haws and O’Bearden
price (as typically used by the store), and 21% more units than a 2006), and consumers might view PYP or PWYW as similar
discount strategy. Although consumers paid slightly less on to bidding; we ultimately do not find evidence for this role,
average ($.50 vs. $.60), the significant sales increase in the though.
PYP, relative to the discount, condition demonstrates the strong
potential of PYP pricing strategies. Notably, the negative
effects of PWYW emerged despite a lack of reference price, Methods
suggesting that consumers were not simply paying at or below
a reference price. Participants and design. This study used a 3 (pricing strategy:
Although we could not measure consumer control and effort PWYW, PYP, fixed price)  2 (price level: high, low)
directly in Study 1, the anecdotal evidence suggests that between-subjects design. We recruited 382 U.S. adults
consumers viewed PWYW as more effortful and asked more (Mage ¼ 35.62 years; 41.4% female) from Amazo Mechanical
questions about it. The purchase differences between PWYW Turk (MTurk) for a nominal payment. They read about a ficti-
and PYP also indicate that PYP increases purchases and profits tious genetic testing service (Zyntech) and rated their purchase
by enhancing pricing control (relative to fixed price) but alle- intentions. Participants then completed postsurvey measures
viates pricing effort (relative to PWYW). and were thanked.

Procedure and measures. The pricing strategy and price-level


manipulations were embedded in the service description.
Study 2 Half of the participants were assigned to the high-price condi-
Study 2 has several goals. First, with a lab experiment, we tion. Among this group, those assigned to the fixed-price con-
further validate our core predictions that, relative to a dition read that the price was $199; those in the PWYW
fixed-price strategy, consumers are more likely to purchase condition read that most consumers pay $199, but they could
when they can use PYP and less likely to do so using PWYW. pay any amount they felt was appropriate; and those in the PYP
Second, we test the generalizability of these effects across high condition read that they could choose a price to pay from a list
and low price levels. Third, to gain insights into the underlying ($189, $199, or $209). For the other half, assigned to the
process, we include measures of pricing control and pricing low-price condition, participants in the fixed-price condition
effort. We predict that PWYW enhances perceived pricing con- read that the price was $49; those in the PWYW condition read
trol (H2a) and effort (H2b), whereas PYP enhances pricing con- that most consumers pay $49, but they could pay any price they
trol (H2c) but has no discernible impact on pricing effort. We felt was appropriate; and those in the PYP condition read that
further predict that pricing effort relates negatively, and pricing they could choose a price to pay from a list ($39, $49, and $59).
control relates positively, to purchase intentions. We test these In the PYP conditions, the average price thus was equivalent to
Wang et al. 151

the reference price in the PWYW conditions and the retail price Table 1. Study 2: Means and Standard Deviations of Purchase
in the fixed-price conditions. Intentions, Pricing Control, and Pricing Effort.
Participants rated their purchase intentions on a single Purchase Intentions Pricing Control Pricing Effort
seven-point scale item (“How likely are you to purchase
genetic testing from Zyntech?”; 1 ¼ “extremely unlikely,” and Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
7 ¼ “extremely likely”). They also reported their pricing effort
Fixed price 4.07 1.95 3.48 1.64 3.36 1.44
using three items (“The pricing strategy made me feel like the PWYW 3.44 1.98 5.26 1.15 4.35 1.62
decision to purchase this genetic test was an effortful one,” PYP 4.83 1.76 4.78 1.28 3.64 1.24
“The pricing strategy demanded effort for me to reach a pur-
chase decision,” and “Compared to other types of pricing, the
pricing strategy for this product made it easy for me to reach a high/low price level conditions reveal similar statistical
purchase decision” [reverse coded]; 1 ¼ “strongly disagree,” patterns (i.e., price level does not moderate the effects).
and 7 ¼ “strongly agree”; a ¼ .76). They rated perceived
pricing control using five items (“The way this genetic testing Pricing effort. We submitted pricing effort to a 3 (pricing strat-
is priced made me feel like I was in control,” “When deciding egy)  2 (price level) ANOVA. The results reveal a significant
whether or not to get a genetic testing from Zyntech, I felt I was main effect of pricing strategy (F(2, 376) ¼ 16.09, p < .001,
in control of the price,” “The price of this genetic testing ser- Z2 ¼ .08), a nonsignificant main effect of price level
vice was controlled by me,” “The pricing strategy would (F(1, 376) ¼ 1.45, p ¼ .23, Z2 ¼ .004), and a nonsignificant
demonstrate how much control I have,” and “The pricing strat- two-way interaction (F < 1, n.s.). Contrasts reveal that, relative
egy made me feel like I am in control of the price”; to the PYP condition (M ¼ 3.64, SD ¼ 1.24), participants
1 ¼ “strongly disagree,” and 7 ¼ “strongly agree”; a ¼ .95). reported similar levels of effort in the fixed-price condition
For perceived fairness, they rated two items (“The pricing (M ¼ 3.36, SD ¼ 1.44; t(256) ¼ 1.67, p ¼ .12, d ¼ .21) but
strategy was fair” and “The price was fair”; 1 ¼ “strongly higher pricing effort in the PWYW condition (M ¼ 4.35,
disagree,” and 7 ¼ “strongly agree”; r ¼ .72) adapted from SD ¼ 1.62; t(253) ¼ 3.94, p < .001, d ¼ .50). Pricing effort
Darke and Dahl (2003). Finally, participants provided their is also greater in the PWYW condition than in the fixed-price
demographic information. condition (t(249) ¼ 5.12, p < .001, d ¼ .65).

Pricing control. We submitted pricing control to a 3 (pricing


Results strategy)  2 (price level) ANOVA. The results reveal signif-
Manipulation checks. Participants completed a pricing strategy icant main effects of pricing strategy (F(2, 376) ¼ 56.98,
attention check (1 ¼ “one price was offered,” 2 ¼ “pick-your- p < .001, Z2 ¼ .23) and price level (F(1, 376) ¼ 10.04,
price with a list of options,” 3 ¼ “pay whatever you want,” and p < .01, Z2 ¼ .03) and a nonsignificant two-way interaction
4 ¼ “not sure”), which 348 (91.1%) of 382 participants (F < 1, n.s.). Across pricing strategies, pricing control in the
answered correctly, as well as a price-level attention check PYP condition (M ¼ 4.78, SD ¼ 1.28) is higher than in the
(1 ¼ “more than $100,” 2 ¼ “less than $100,” and 3 ¼ “not fixed-price condition (M ¼ 3.48, SD ¼ 1.64; t(256) ¼ 7.11,
sure”), which 353 (92.4%) participants answered correctly. p < .001, d ¼ .89) but lower than in the PWYW condition
Excluding participants who failed the attention checks did not (M ¼ 5.26, SD ¼ 1.15; t(253) ¼ 3.14, p < .01, d ¼ .39).
materially alter the significance of the results, so we report the Pricing control also is greater in the PWYW than in the
results with all participants. fixed-price condition (t(249) ¼ 9.93, p < .001, d ¼ 1.26). Dif-
ferences across pricing strategies for the high- and low-price
Purchase intentions. We submitted purchase intentions to a 3 conditions follow similar statistical patterns.
(pricing strategy)  2 (price level) analysis of variance We empirically confirm the discriminant validity of pricing
(ANOVA). The results reveal significant main effects of pric- effort and control (r ¼ .177) by calculating their average var-
ing strategy (F(2, 376) ¼ 17.82, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .09) and price iances extracted (AVE). The square of the parameter estimate
level (F(1, 376) ¼ 7.68, p < .01, Z2 ¼ .02) and a nonsignificant for this pair of constructs (phi ¼ .03) is less than the mean of its
two-way interaction (F < 1, n.s.). Participants indicate weaker AVE estimates (.75), so the two measures are distinct (Fornell
purchase intentions when the service demands a high price and Larcker 1981). We also report the results of an alternate
(M ¼ 3.87, SD ¼ 2.04) rather than a low one (M ¼ 4.39, regression analysis for each focal outcome for this and remain-
SD ¼ 1.88). Across pricing strategies, we observe that purchase ing studies in the Web Appendix.
intentions in the PYP condition (M ¼ 4.83, SD ¼ 1.76) are
significantly higher than in the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.07, Mediation by pricing effort and control. We conducted mediation
SD ¼ 1.95; t(256) ¼ 3.29, p < .01, d ¼ .41) or PWYW analyses with Hayes’s (2017) PROCESS macro (model 4;
(M ¼ 3.44, SD ¼ 1.98; t(253) ¼ 5.93, p < .001, d ¼ .75) 10,000 bootstrapped samples), to test the effect of pricing strat-
conditions. They are lower in the PWYW than in the egy on purchase intentions, using pricing effort and control as
fixed-price condition (t(249) ¼ 2.54, p < .01, d ¼ .32) mediators. We coded pricing strategy such that the effects of
(Table 1). The differences across pricing strategies for the PWYW and PYP are relative to those of a fixed-price
152 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

condition. The positive effect of PYP on purchase intentions is paid them a nominal fee. Participants were assigned to one of
mediated by pricing control (indirect effect b ¼ .46, SE ¼ .12; nine conditions.
95% confidence interval [CI] ¼ [.24, .71]) but not by pricing
effort (indirect effect b ¼ .09, SE ¼ .06; 95% CI ¼ [.21, Procedure and measures. Participants first completed a priming
.02]). Alternatively, the effect of PWYW on purchase inten- task that manipulated their shopping motives (Cacioppo and
tions was mediated by both pricing control (indirect effect Petty 1981; Gino and Mogilner 2014). Participants in the
b ¼ .63, SE ¼ .15; 95% CI ¼ [.35, .93]) and pricing effort save-time (money) condition had to list four to five reasons
(indirect effect b ¼ .33, SE ¼ .09; 95% CI ¼ [.52, .16]). that saving time (money) is important when making a purchase
We further discuss these mediation patterns in the Web decision. Participants in the baseline condition did not com-
Appendix. plete any writing task. All participants read a description of
renters’ insurance available from American Family Insurance
Company. The pricing information appeared on the following
Discussion page. In the fixed-price condition, participants read that the
Study 2 provides additional support for our proposed frame- price was $180/year. In the PYP condition, they could choose
work. Consistent with H1, we find that, relative to a fixed price, one of three prices to pay: $140/year, $180/year, or $220/year.
PWYW undermines, whereas PYP increases, purchase inten- In the PWYW condition, consumers were told they could pay
tions, regardless of price level. We also find empirical support any price they considered appropriate. Participants then rated
for H2, such that both PWYW and PYP increase consumers’ pricing effort (a ¼ .86), pricing control (a ¼ .95), perceived
perceived pricing control (cf. fixed price), and PWYW fairness (r ¼ .79), and purchase intentions. Finally, they
increases consumers’ perceived pricing effort. In other words, provided demographic information.
PYP elicits greater pricing control but does not affect pricing
effort, relative to a fixed price. The results confirm the mediat-
ing roles of pricing control and pricing effort too (H3). Thus, Results
PYP enhances purchase intentions through greater pricing con- Manipulation checks. Participants completed the pricing atten-
trol. For PWYW, we observe mediation by both perceived tion check from Study 2, and 608 (90.1%) of 675 participants
control and perceived effort, in countervailing pathways. In passed it. They also completed a motive attention check
addition, we rule out the perceived fairness (see the Web (1 ¼ “save time,” 2 ¼ “save money,” and 3 ¼ “no writing
Appendix) of the pricing strategy as an alternative explanation task”), which 588 (87.1%) of them passed. The effects did not
for the outcomes in this and remaining studies. change when participants who failed the attention checks were
excluded, so we report the results obtained from all
participants.
Study 3 Purchase intentions. We submitted purchase intentions to a 3
In Study 3, we build on Study 2 findings by examining shop- (pricing strategy)  3 (motive) ANOVA. The results revealed
ping motives as moderators of the pricing control and effort significant main effects of pricing strategy (F(2, 666) ¼ 9.38,
pathways. It may be possible to shift consumers’ perception of p < .001, Z2 ¼ .03) and motives (F(2, 666) ¼ 15.11, p < .001,
pricing control or effort by altering their shopping motives. Z2 ¼ .04). Participants in the PYP condition (M ¼ 4.91,
When consumers are motivated to save money, they are less SD ¼ 1.77) indicate stronger purchase intentions than those in
focused on the expenditure of effort (Duxbury et al. 2005; the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.30, SD ¼ 1.96; t(449) ¼ 3.47, p < .001,
Kivetz 2003), and control over the price paid helps them d ¼ .33) or PWYW (M ¼ 4.23, SD ¼ 1.97; t(446) ¼ 3.84,
achieve their goal (Charness and Cheung 2013; Kim, Natter, p < .001, d ¼ .36) conditions. In the fixed-price and PWYW
and Spann 2009). When consumers are motivated to save time, conditions, we find similar purchase intentions (t < 1, p ¼ .70,
ease and efficiency are critical (Leclerc, Schmitt, and Dubé d ¼ .04). When motivated to save time (M ¼ 3.94, SD ¼ 2.03),
1995; Thaler 1985). Thus, we predict that PWYW and PYP participants indicate weaker purchase intentions than those
(vs. fixed price) both increase purchase intentions among motivated to save money (M ¼ 4.83, SD ¼ 1.69;
consumers motivated to save money, but PWYW’s effect on t(447) ¼ 5.04, p < .001, d ¼ .48) or those in the baseline
purchase intentions will remain negative among consumers condition (M ¼ 4.66, SD ¼ 1.92; t(449) ¼ 3.87, p < .001,
motivated to save time (H4, H5). To test these effects, we use d ¼ .37). People motivated to save money and those in the base-
a different service category (i.e., insurance). line condition indicate similar purchase intentions (t < 1,
p ¼ .32, d ¼ .09).
We also find a significant two-way (pricing strategy 
Methods motive) interaction (F(4, 666) ¼ 9.44, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .05)
Participants and design. This study used a 3 (pricing strategy: (Figure 3). In the baseline condition, participants in the PWYW
PWYW, PYP, fixed)  3 (motive: save time, save money, condition (M ¼ 3.95, SD ¼ 2.04) reveal weaker purchase
baseline) between-subjects design. We recruited 675 U.S. intentions than those in the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.59,
adults (Mage ¼ 38.7 years; 41.2% female) from MTurk and SD ¼ 1.91; t(148) ¼ 1.98, p < .05, d ¼ .33) and PYP
Wang et al. 153

In the baseline condition, participants in the PWYW condi-


6.5
5.39 tion (M ¼ 4.99, SD ¼ 1.35) reported greater pricing effort than
6.0 5.42 those in the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.22, SD ¼ 1.52; t(148) ¼ 3.28,
5.5 5.03 p < .01, d ¼ .54) and PYP (M ¼ 4.25, SD ¼ 1.54;
t(148) ¼ 3.13, p < .01, d ¼ .51) conditions. In the PYP and
Purchase Intentions

5.0 4.59
4.27 3.95 fixed-price conditions, there was a similar level of pricing
4.5 4.20
4.09 effort (t < 1, p ¼ .94, d ¼ .001), replicating the pattern of
3.36
4.0 effects from Study 2 for the baseline condition. Yet in the
3.5
save-time condition, PWYW (M ¼ 5.00, SD ¼ 1.74) increased
pricing effort relative to the baseline condition (M ¼ 3.64,
3.0
SD ¼ 1.62; t(148) ¼ 5.38, p < .001, d ¼ .88) but not the PYP
2.5 condition (M ¼ 4.74, SD ¼ 1.35; t(148) ¼ 1.12, p ¼ .26,
2.0 d ¼ .18), while participants in the PYP reported higher pricing
Save Time Save Money Baseline effort than those exposed to the fixed price (t(148) ¼ 4.52,
Shopping Motivation p < .001, d ¼ .74). Finally, in the save-money condition, pric-
Fixed Price PWYW PYP ing effort across all three pricing strategies is similar
(MPWYW ¼ 4.66, SD ¼ 1.43; Mfixed price ¼ 4.63, SD ¼ 1.31;
Figure 3. Study 3: moderating effects of shopping motives on MPYP ¼ 4.65, SD ¼ 1.23; ts < 1, n.s.).
purchase intentions.
Notes: Error bars ¼ +1 SEs. Pricing control. We submitted pricing control to a 3 (pricing
strategy)  3 (motive) ANOVA, which shows a significant
(M ¼ 5.42, SD ¼ 1.49; t(148) ¼ 5.05, p < .001, d ¼ .83) main effect of pricing strategy (F(2, 666) ¼ 66.37, p < .001,
conditions. In the PYP condition, we find stronger purchase Z2 ¼ .17), a nonsignificant main effect of motive (F(2, 666) ¼
intentions than in the fixed-price condition (t(150) ¼ 2.99, 1.38, p ¼ .25, Z2 ¼ .004), and a significant two-way interaction
p < .01, d ¼ .49), replicating the pattern of effects from Study (F(4, 666) ¼ 7.07, p < .01, Z2 ¼ .04). Pricing control in the
2 for the baseline condition. Yet in the save-time condition, PWYW condition (M ¼ 5.48, SD ¼ 1.28) is higher than in the
PWYW (M ¼ 3.36, SD ¼ 2.02) results in weaker purchase fixed-price condition (M ¼ 4.06, SD ¼ 1.58; t(449) ¼ 10.48,
intentions than in the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.20, SD ¼ 1.99; p < .001, d ¼ .99) and the PYP condition (M ¼ 5.06,
t(148) ¼ 2.57, p < .05, d ¼ .42) or PYP (M ¼ 4.27, SD ¼ 1.19; t(446) ¼ 3.60, p < .001, d ¼ .34). Pricing control
SD ¼ 1.98; t(148) ¼ 2.79, p < .01, d ¼ .46) conditions. also is higher in the PYP condition than the fixed-price condi-
Participants in the PYP and fixed-price conditions indicate tion (t(449) ¼ 7.58, p < .001, d ¼ .72).
similar purchase intentions (t < 1, p ¼ .83, d ¼ .03). Finally, In the baseline condition, PWYW (M ¼ 5.56, SD ¼ 1.05)
in the save-money condition, PWYW participants (M ¼ 5.39, increased pricing control relative to the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.06,
SD ¼ 1.15) offer stronger purchase intentions than those in the SD ¼ 1.65; t(148) ¼ 6.62, p < .001, d ¼ 1.09) but not PYP
fixed-price condition (M ¼ 4.09, SD ¼ 1.96; t(149) ¼ 4.96, (M ¼ 5.33, SD ¼ .99; t(148) ¼ 1.38, p ¼ .17, d ¼ .23) condi-
p < .001, d ¼ .81), but they are similar to the PYP condition tions. PYP significantly increased pricing control relative to the
(M ¼ 5.03, SD ¼ 1.62; t(146) ¼ 1.56, p ¼ .12, d ¼ .26). The fixed-price condition (t(150) ¼ 5.75, p < .001, d ¼ .94), repli-
PYP condition also leads to stronger purchase intentions than cating the pattern of effects from Study 2. The pattern was
the fixed-price condition (t(147) ¼ 3.18, p < .01, d ¼ .52). slightly different in the save-time condition: PWYW
(M ¼ 5.11, SD ¼ 1.58) increased control relative to
Pricing effort. As we detailed in the “Conceptual Development” fixed-price (M ¼ 4.51, SD ¼ 1.34; t(148) ¼ 2.51, p < .05,
section, we predict that consumers’ motivation to save time or d ¼ .41) but not PYP (M ¼ 4.87, SD ¼ 1.26; t(148) ¼ 1.02,
money will alter perceptions of pricing effort and pricing con- p ¼ .31, d ¼ .17) conditions. Participants in the PYP condition
trol. We submitted pricing effort to a 3 (pricing strategy)  3 reported marginally greater pricing control than those exposed
(motive) ANOVA. The results reveal a significant main effect to a fixed price (t(148) ¼ 1.70, p ¼ .09, d ¼ .28). Finally,
of pricing strategy (F(2, 666) ¼ 13.52, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .04), a participants revealed the largest difference in pricing condi-
nonsignificant main effect of motive (F(2, 666) ¼ 1.05, tions in the save-money condition: PWYW (M ¼ 5.76,
p ¼ .35, Z2 ¼ .003), and a significant two-way interaction SD ¼ 1.07) increased pricing control relative to fixed price
(F(4, 666) ¼ 5.69, p < .01, Z2 ¼ .03). Across pricing strategies, (M ¼ 3.62, SD ¼ 1.61; t(149) ¼ 9.61, p < .001, d ¼ 1.58) and
participants exert greater effort in PWYW (M ¼ 4.88, PYP (M ¼ 4.96, SD ¼ 1.29; t(146) ¼ 4.11, p < .001, d ¼ .68).
SD ¼ 1.52) than in the fixed-price condition (M ¼ 4.17, PYP also increased control relative to the fixed-price condition
SD ¼ 1.54; t(449) ¼ 4.93, p < .001, d ¼ .47) and the PYP (t(147) ¼ 5.59, p < .01, d ¼ .92).
condition (M ¼ 4.54, SD ¼ 1.39; t(446) ¼ 2.47, p < .05, We again find support for discriminant validity (r ¼ .12); the
d ¼ .23). Pricing effort also is greater in the PYP than square of the parameter estimate between the effort and control
fixed-price condition (t(449) ¼ 2.67, p < .05, d ¼ .25). Next, constructs (phi ¼ .01) is less than the mean of the pair’s AVE
we examine the interaction. (AVE ¼ .80; Fornell and Larcker 1981). In addition, we report
154 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

the detailed significant moderated mediation results in the Web reviewed a list of ten price options, ranging from $29.99 to
Appendix. 47.99 in $2 increments. In the low-control PYP condition, they
saw the price options from the PYP condition but also read,
Discussion “You can pick your own price from the list below, but the
product may not be available at the price you pick.” Partici-
The results from Study 3 provide evidence in support of H4 and pants indicated their purchase likelihood and the maximum
H5. In the baseline condition, participants indicated higher pur- amount they would be willing to pay for the tickets. Finally,
chase intentions in the PYP (vs. fixed-price) condition and participants provided demographic information.
lower purchase intentions in the PWYW (vs. fixed-price)
condition. Perceived effort and control mediated these relation-
ships, as in Study 2. Alternatively, when the motive to save Results
time was activated, PWYW resulted in lower purchase inten-
Manipulation checks. We used the three-item pricing effort
tions than in the PYP and fixed-price conditions, and purchase
(a ¼ .89) and five-item pricing control (a ¼ .95) measures
intentions did not differ between PYP and fixed price. In short,
from Studies 2 and 3. In addition, of the 260 participants,
the mediation evidence indicates that the motive to save time
234 (90.0%) passed the pricing attention check (1 ¼ “One price
magnified perceptions of effort and reduced the benefits of
was offered,” 2 ¼ “Pick-your-price with a short list of options
PYP, whereas the motive to save money minimized effort and
[3 options],” 3 ¼ “Pick-your-price with a long list of options
magnified perceptions of control in a way that bolstered
[10 options],” and 4 ¼ “Pick-your-price with a short list of
PWYW and PYP. The results across Studies 1–3 also indicate
options [3 options] but product may not be available”). The
that the effects on purchase intentions are robust, regardless of
effects did not change when we excluded participants who
the presence or absence of a reference price, in the PWYW
failed the attention checks, so we report the results with all
condition.
participants.
Verifying the effort and control manipulations, a one-way
Study 4 ANOVA of pricing effort revealed a significant effect of pricing
Thus far, we have provided process evidence through measured strategy on perceived effort (F(3, 256) ¼ 4.83, p < .01,
mediation (pricing effort and control) and situational modera- Z2 ¼ .05). The high-effort PYP condition (M ¼ 4.80,
tion (motives to save time vs. money), which confirms that the SD ¼ 1.69) significantly increased perceived pricing effort,
poor performance of PWYW is due to greater pricing effort, relative to the fixed-price (M ¼ 3.94, SD ¼ 1.62;
whereas the superior performance of PYP results from t(129) ¼ 2.97, p < .01, d ¼ .52), PYP (M ¼ 3.80, SD ¼ 1.61;
increased pricing control without a corresponding increase in t(129) ¼ 3.46, p < .01, d ¼ .61), and low-control PYP (M ¼ 4.08,
effort. In our studies, the PYP conditions include a limited SD ¼ 1.65; t(130) ¼ 2.45, p < .05, d ¼ .43) conditions. The
number of price options (three to four), which minimizes reported levels of pricing effort were similar across the latter
perceived pricing effort, and an implicit promise to sell the three conditions (ts < 1, n.s.). A one-way ANOVA of pricing
offering at whatever price the participant selects, which control also showed a significant effect (F(3, 256) ¼ 11.96,
encourages perceived pricing control. In Study 4, to test the p < .001, Z2 ¼ .12), such that those in the low-control PYP
control–effort trade-off further, we modify the features of the condition (M ¼ 3.93, SD ¼ 1.73) reported significantly lower
PYP condition to increase the sense of effort and reduce pricing control than those in the PYP (M ¼ 4.78, SD ¼ 1.54;
the sense of control. We thus can examine how variations in t(127) ¼ 2.97, p < .01, d ¼ .53) and high-effort PYP
PYP designs affect its advantages. Finally, we examine movie (M ¼ 5.17, SD ¼ 1.51; t(130) ¼ 4.40, p < .001, d ¼ .77)
ticket sales as the purchase context. conditions but similar levels of control to those in the
fixed-price condition (M ¼ 3.69, SD ¼ 1.74; t < 1, n.s.). In
the PYP condition, we find higher pricing control than in the
Methods fixed-price condition (t(126) ¼ 3.76, p < .001, d ¼ .67) but
Participants and design. We recruited 260 U.S. adults directionally lower control than in the high-effort PYP condition
(Mage ¼ 35.45 years; 43.5% female) from MTurk for a nominal (t(129) ¼ 1.45, p ¼ .15, d ¼ .26).
payment. Participants were assigned to one of four pricing
conditions (fixed price, PYP, high-effort PYP, low-control Purchase intentions. We submitted purchase intentions to a
PYP) in a between-subjects design. one-way ANOVA. The results reveal a significant main effect
of pricing strategy (F(3, 256) ¼ 4.59, p < .01, Z2 ¼ .05; see the
Procedure and measures. Participants first read a description Web Appendix). That is, participants express higher purchase
about a Regal Premiere Movie eTicket 4-Packs, then saw the intentions in the PYP condition (M ¼ 5.25, SD ¼ 1.46) than in
pricing information on a subsequent page. In the fixed-price the fixed-price (M ¼ 4.39, SD ¼ 2.02; t(126) ¼ 2.76, p < .01,
condition, the pack is listed for $37.99 plus tax. In the PYP d ¼ .49), high-effort PYP (M ¼ 4.10, SD ¼ 2.01; t(129) ¼ 3.70,
condition, participants read that consumers can choose a price p < .001, d ¼ .65), and low-control PYP (M ¼ 4.34, SD ¼ 1.96;
they want to pay from a list that included $29.99, $37.99, and t(127) ¼ 2.99, p < .01, d ¼ .53) conditions. No significant
$45.99 plus tax. In the high-effort PYP condition, participants difference arises among the fixed-price, high-effort PYP, and
Wang et al. 155

low-control PYP conditions (ts < 1, n.s.). Although we (Mage ¼ 36.09 years; 34% female) from MTurk and paid them
manipulated pricing effort and pricing control, we tested a nominal fee.
whether the control and effort manipulation checks served as
mediators of the effects of pricing conditions on purchase Procedure and measures. In the single-product-evaluation con-
intentions. The results indicated that there was a significant dition, participants were presented with an electric toothbrush
mediation that was consistent with our framework (see the Web (Quip) and given a brief product description and price. In the
Appendix). fixed-price condition, participants read that the electric tooth-
brush sells for $30 and were asked to indicate their purchase
choice (“Please indicate whether you would purchase the Quip
Discussion Electric Toothbrush”; 0 ¼ “I would not purchase this product,”
This study validates our theoretical model and confirms the and 1 ¼ “I would purchase this product for $30”). In the PYP
role of pricing effort and pricing control in determining the condition, participants were instructed that they could choose
effectiveness of participative pricing strategies, using direct the price to pay for the electric toothbrush from a list of $25,
manipulations. When pricing effort increases, consumers are $30, and $35. Participants indicated their purchase choice
less likely to purchase, even if the pricing strategy offers more (“Please indicate whether you would purchase the Quip Elec-
pricing control. When pricing control decreases, consumers tric Toothbrush”; 0 ¼ “I would not purchase this product,”
also show weaker purchase intentions, even when the pricing 1 ¼ “I would purchase this product for $25,” 2 ¼ “I would
strategy evokes the same level of pricing effort. Our studies purchase this product for $30,” and 3 ¼ “I would purchase this
consistently show that PYP is an effective participative pricing product for $35”).
form that induces higher purchase intentions in various product In the multiple-product-evaluation condition, participants
categories, both online and offline. But is it effective in all were presented an electric toothbrush (Quip), along with three
shopping contexts? other products: dish soap (Palmolive), laundry detergent
(Tide), and a box of storage quart bags (Ziploc), with a brief
description and price (presentation order was counterbalanced).
In the fixed-price condition, participants read that the electric
Study 5 toothbrush sells for $30, the dish soap sells for $5, the laundry
The successful manipulation of pricing effort associated with detergent for $11.97, and the storage bags for $7.48. In the PYP
PYP in Study 4 revealed an attenuating effect on purchase condition, participants were told that they may pick their
intentions, but we do not observe a full reversal (i.e., negative purchase price (toothbrush: $25, $30, $35; dish soap: $4, $5,
effect). Perhaps the high-effort threshold required to reduce $6; detergent: $ 9.97, $11.97, $13.97; and bags: $5.98, $7.48,
purchase intentions was not reached by ten price choice $8.98). Participants indicated their purchase choice for each
options. In Study 5, we examine the role of effort differently product (similar to the single evaluation protocol), rated pricing
by manipulating the number of PYP decisions made across effort (a ¼ .88), pricing control (a ¼ .92), and perceived
products. We predict that having to choose prices for multiple fairness (r ¼ .81) and provided demographic information.
products will increase pricing effort sufficiently to reduce
participants’ purchase choices, relative to fixed prices (H6).
In addition, we sought to examine the revenue implications Results
of PYP more directly. In Study 1, participants paid a specific Manipulation checks. Of the 420 participants, 412 (98%) passed
price, yet we were unable to observe nonpurchases. In Studies the attention check. The results are not different when those
2–4, participants indicated their purchase intentions and max- who failed the attention check are excluded from the sample, so
imum willingness to pay, but this permits only an approxima- we report the results with all participants.
tion of revenue. Study 5 addresses these limitations by
recording purchase choice (yes/no) and the corresponding Purchase choice. We conducted a logistic regression to assess the
payment amount in an experiment where participants make effect of pricing strategy (0 ¼ fixed, 1 ¼ PYP), product evalua-
selections in a shopping simulation. From this, we are able to tion (0 ¼ single, 1 ¼ multiple), and their interaction (pricing
assess the revenue implications for a single product paired with strategy  evaluation) on purchase choice for the electric
PYP (vs. fixed price) and for a shopping basket of products toothbrush (0 ¼ no purchase, 1 ¼ purchase) as the focal depen-
paired with PYP (vs. fixed price). We anticipate a positive dent variable that was included in all four conditions. We
effect of PYP for a single product and a negative effect for a observed a significant pricing strategy  evaluation interaction
basket of products. (B ¼ 2.86, SE ¼ .45, Z ¼ 6.37, Wald ¼ 40.52, p < .001)
(Figure 4). As we predicted, in the single-product-evaluation
condition, PYP increased purchase choice (fixed price:
Methods 54.29%, PYP: 85.71%; B ¼ 1.62, SE ¼ .34, Z ¼ 4.75,
Participants and design. This study adopted a 2 (pricing strategy: p < .001). In the multiple-product-evaluation condition, PYP
PYP vs. fixed)  2 (product evaluation: single vs. multiple) reduced purchase choice (fixed price: 70.48%, PYP: 40.95%;
between-subjects design. We recruited 420 U.S. adults B ¼ 1.24, SE ¼ .29, Z ¼ 4.23, p < .001).
156 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

Mfixed ¼ 4.24, SD ¼ 1.66; F(1, 416) ¼ 2.06, p ¼ .15, Z2 ¼ .005),


Single-PYP a significant main effect of evaluation (Mmultiple ¼ 4.53,
SD ¼ 1.49; Msingle ¼ 4.17, SD ¼ 1.68; F(1, 416) ¼ 5.68,
p < .05, Z2 ¼ .01), and a significant two-way interaction (F(1,
Condition

Single-Fixed
416) ¼ 3.72, p ¼ .05, Z2 ¼ .009). In the single-evaluation con-
Multiple-PYP dition, participants indicated similar level of pricing effort for
PYP and fixed price (MPYP ¼ 4.13, SD ¼ 1.63; Mfixed ¼ 4.20,
Multiple-Fixed SD ¼ 1.73; t(208) ¼ .30, p ¼ .76, d ¼ .04). In the
multiple-evaluation condition, PYP (M ¼ 4.79, SD ¼ 1.32)
0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% increased pricing effort relative to the fixed price (M ¼ 4.27,
Purchase Choice SD ¼ 1.60; t(208) ¼ 2.57, p ¼ .01, d ¼ .36).
Purchase No Purchase
Pricing control. We submitted pricing control to a 2 (pricing strat-
egy)  2 (product evaluation) ANOVA. The results reveal a
Figure 4. Study 5: moderating effect of shopping context on purchase significant main effect of pricing strategy (MPYP ¼ 5.15,
choice. SD ¼ 1.22; Mfixed ¼ 4.14, SD ¼ 1.59; F(1, 416) ¼ 53.16,
Notes: Data represent the proportion of respondents indicating that they
would purchase (yes vs. no) a toothbrush paired with a PYP (vs. fixed price) in a
p < .001, Z2 ¼ .11), a nonsignificant main effect of product
single-evaluation (toothbrush only) or multiple-evaluation (toothbrush and evaluation (M multiple ¼ 4.73, SD ¼ 1.37; M single ¼ 4.55,
other PYP products) context. SD ¼ 1.62; F(1, 416) ¼ 1.71, p ¼ .19, Z2 ¼ .004), and a non-
significant two-way interaction (F(1, 416) ¼ 1.19, p ¼ .28,
Next, we examined the effects of PYP on purchase choice Z2 ¼ .003).
for the alternate products included only in the multiple- Notably, pricing control and effort exhibited adequate
product-evaluation condition. We generally observed a nega- discriminant validity (r ¼ .185); the square of their parameter
tive (or directionally negative) effect of PYP on purchase estimate (phi ¼ .03) is less than the mean of their AVE esti-
choice: dish soap (PYP ¼ 73.33%, fixed price ¼ 89.52%, mates (AVE ¼ .78; Fornell and Larcker 1981). Consistent with
w2 ¼ 1.69, p ¼ .19), detergent (PYP ¼ 61.90%, fixed previous studies, pricing control and effort significantly
price ¼ 90.48%, w2 ¼ 5.63, p < .05), and storage bags mediated the effect of pricing strategies on purchase choices
(PYP ¼ 60.95%, fixed price ¼ 88.57%, w2 ¼ 5.36, p < .05). (see the Web Appendix).

Revenue estimation. We used the purchase choice and price


given/selected1 to estimate the revenue generated in each con- Discussion
dition. We first examined only the toothbrush revenue, as it is The results from Study 5 confirm our framework in several key
the only product that was displayed in all four conditions. In the ways. First, we conceptually replicate mediation findings from
single-evaluation condition, we found that PYP generated a previous studies in the single-product condition where PYP
total of $2,595 in toothbrush revenue (vs. $1,710 in the increased purchase choice, and this effect was mediated by
fixed-price condition), an average of $24.71 per participant increases in pricing control but not effort. Alternatively, in the
(vs. $16.29 in the fixed-price condition; t(187.692) ¼ 8.43, multiple-product condition, PYP reduced purchase choice for
p < .001). In the multiple evaluation condition, we found that the focal product, and the negative effect of PYP was mediated
PYP generated a total of $1,215 in toothbrush revenue (vs. by both pricing control and effort, which is comparable to the
$2,220 in the fixed-price condition), an average of $11.57 per pattern of effects observed for PWYW in Study 2. Furthermore,
participant (vs. $21.14 in the fixed-price condition; we found compelling evidence of PYP as a critical strategy for
t(208) ¼ 4.97, p < .001). Next, we examined only the single products that can backfire for multiple products. PYP
multiple-evaluation condition to assess the shopping basket increased total toothbrush revenue by 52% in the
revenue generated across all products. We found that the PYP single-product condition, whereas it reduced revenue by 45%
condition generated $2,705 (vs. $4,523 in the fixed-price con- in the multiple-product conditions. Notably, these revenue dif-
dition), for an average of $25.75 per participant (vs. $43.07 in ferences account for purchase choice (yes/no) and, conditional
the fixed-price condition; t(208) ¼ 7.43, p < .001). on a purchase, the price selected. The choice data suggest that
the increase in revenue in the single-product condition is
Pricing effort. We submitted pricing effort to a 2 (pricing strat- largely driven by an increase in purchases, whereas the reduc-
egy)  2 (evaluation) ANOVA. The results reveal a nonsigni- tion in revenue in the multiple-product condition is driven by a
ficant main effect of pricing strategy (MPYP ¼ 4.46, SD ¼ 1.52; decrease in purchases. Interestingly, there was an increase in
toothbrush purchases in the multiple- (vs. single-) product
1 condition. We speculate that the increase was caused by the
Participants who opted not to purchase a given product were assigned a price
of $0. consideration set (Bettman, Luce, and Payne 1998; Kahneman
2
We report adjusted degrees of freedom when Levene’s test indicated unequal and Miller, 1986; Trijp, Hoyer, and Inman 1996). The tooth-
variance between groups. brush may have seemed more attractive (e.g., technologically
Wang et al. 157

advanced) when compared with dish soap, detergent, and bags. but PWYW is more effective in donation (vs. nondonation)
And perhaps this contrast explains the purchase increase, which contexts, presumably because consumers are willing to expend
further manifest the complexity when consumers evaluate effort if their purchase benefits others.
products and make purchase decisions. In summary, we
demonstrate the robust evidence in favor of the control–effort
trade-off for participative pricing, which can be influenced
by the pricing strategy (PYP vs. PWYW) or the purchase con-
Contributions to Pricing Theory
text where single versus multiple products are paired with a By adopting a consumer perspective on participative pricing,
participative pricing strategy. this study advances pricing theory in several ways. First, we
uncover two key psychological mechanisms that underpin par-
ticipative pricing: control and effort. Their trade-off helps
General Discussion explicate some mixed findings about the performance of parti-
With this research, we propose a control–effort trade-off frame- cipative pricing (Chen, Koenigsberg, and Zhang 2017; Gerpott
work to understand how participative pricing shapes consu- and Schneider 2016). It represents a shift in thinking, relative to
mers’ purchase decisions. We examine participative pricing most pricing literature, which takes a price-paid perspective
by focusing on two forms (PWYW and PYP) that delegate and prioritizes maximizing profit. Although such research can
price determinations to consumers. With PWYW, the process provide guidance for matching offerings, price points, and con-
of price delegation increases feelings of pricing control and sumer heterogeneity to improve pricing efficiency (Christopher
effort, and pricing effort explains its general negative effect and Machado 2019; Dhar and Hoch 1996), it cannot specify or
on purchase intentions in routine purchase contexts (retail food explain consumers’ psychological responses. We fill a research
in Study 1, health services in Study 2, financial services in gap related to consumers’ decision processes in participative
Study 3, entertainment in Study 4, and household products in pricing settings, thereby explaining why PWYW underper-
Study 5). Because PYP increases only feelings of pricing con- forms in routine purchase contexts, while also specifying some
trol, not pricing effort, it generally outperforms PWYW (and factors (purchase motives, donation context) that can enhance
fixed prices) across purchase domains. We theorize and present its performance. The negative effects of PWYW on purchase
evidence that these effects occur because consumers favor pric- intentions hold regardless of the presence of reference prices;
ing control but also want to avoid the costs associated with the positive effects of PYP thus do not appear to be due to
engaging in pricing effort. merely a reference price effect.
Several factors shape this control–effort trade-off for parti- Second, by comparing different forms of participative pric-
cipative pricing. In Study 3, when consumers are motivated to ing according to our control–effort trade-off framework, we
save time, PWYW underperforms PYP and fixed price, and show that PYP, a relatively newer and untested pricing strat-
PYP exhibits performance similar to that of fixed prices. This egy, can enhance purchase outcomes. By offering price
outcome occurs because the focus on pricing control options, PYP balances the trade-off: it provides a high sense
diminishes; having control over the final price does not facil- of control but does not demand more effort, so it offers an
itate time savings. It also stems from the magnification of improved form of price participation. In Study 1, the PYP
pricing effort (PWYW) because effort undermines time- strategy significantly outperforms all other pricing strategies,
saving attempts. Alternatively, when consumers are motivated including a discount with the same economic value, so its
to save money, PWYW and PYP both outperform fixed prices. performance benefit cannot be explained by mere economics.
The money-saving motive enhances the focus on pricing con- Rather, these results emphasize the unique impacts of the con-
trol (PWYW, PYP), such that consumers feel as if they can trol–effort trade-off. As an added benefit, PYP is unlikely to
achieve a satisfactorily low price, and it reduces feelings of erode product or brand quality perceptions, in contrast with
effort. In Study 5, multiple PYP product decisions increase frequent discounts, which can have long-term negative conse-
pricing effort, so PYP is not as effective as a fixed price. Thus, quences for sellers (Anderson and Simester 2001; Mela, Gupta,
shopping motives (saving time or money) and shopping con- and Lehmann 1997).
texts serve as key moderators, with practical insights. Third, we contribute generally to research on consumer
In a parallel investigation in donation contexts (see the Web decision making by identifying contexts in which consumers’
Appendix), both PWYW and PYP perform significantly better purchase decisions differ, even with the same pricing strategy.
than a fixed-price strategy. We thus complement research that Boundary conditions enhance and attenuate the negative
demonstrates the efficacy of PWYW in donation contexts by effects of pricing effort. For example, fixed prices can be more
not only conceptually replicating previously findings (Gneezy effective than PYP for multiple product decisions; PWYW can
et al. 2010) but also providing evidence that PYP offers an be as effective as PYP if consumers are motivated to save
appealing and novel pricing strategy. Choosing how much to money or their purchase involves donations to charity. How-
donate from a list of options ($1, $3, or $5) is a popular soli- ever, if consumers are motivated to save time, the negative
citation method, but PYP strategies for products or services effect of PWYW on purchase intentions is amplified. In gen-
have not been examined empirically. We find that PYP eral, PYP is more effective when used for a single product (vs.
increases purchase intentions regardless of donation context, multiple products simultaneously).
158 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

Contributions to Pricing Practice Various factors influence the outcomes of participative pric-
ing. While this research focuses on pricing effort and its direct
In addition to explaining why PWYW might decrease pur-
effects, future research could explore other relevant factors to
chases, we specify when PWYW will be more or less effective.
expand our knowledge on participative pricing and deepen our
Managers can selectively implement PWYW to enhance their
understanding of consumers’ purchase decisions. Generalizing
pricing performance. Our finding that PWYW decreases pur-
our control–effort trade-off framework, one might observe sim-
chase intentions in specific domains also is consistent with the
ilar effects for other marketing practices that demand consumer
choices of various companies to abandon PWYW (Houck participation, such as sharing pricing information online or
2019; Welss 2018). Adopting PWYW strategies requires providing feedback in reviews. Limiting effort expenditures
careful consideration, and even if marketing research were to in such exchange-extending activities arguably could improve
indicate that consumers favor higher pricing control (Study 2), the participation outcomes. Other factors affect consumers’
the execution might fail, due to consumers’ reluctance to willingness to expend effort, such as when they consider
expend effort to determine their own price (Studies 1–3). Alter- expensive products (e.g., automobiles). When the value, price,
natively, firms might benefit more generally from adopting or risks of purchase are higher, buyers devote more effort to the
PYP strategies. purchase decision (List and Lucking-Reiley 2000).
Consumers’ willingness to exert pricing effort also varies Our control–effort trade-off framework also might clarify
across shopping contexts. For example, our research suggests the implications of other pricing strategies. For example,
PWYW is more effective when consumers are motivated to name-your-own-price options might trigger a sense of high
save money, such as during the holiday season (Shively effort (because consumers try to predict which low price will
2018). Yet PWYW likely is less effective for busy consumers be acceptable to the seller) and also strong money-saving
just seeking to get their holiday shopping done quickly. For motives that offset it. In addition, our research may inspire
online stores that emphasize monetary savings, both PYP and exploration into other pricing strategies from a control–effort
PWYW could be more effective than conventional fixed prices. trade-off perspective. In terms of pricing strategies, PYP and
However, firms should be cautious with the use of PYP in PWYW are analogous to a first-price auction. In pricing prac-
shopping trips that involve multiple product purchases, as we tice, the minimum starting bid and a maximum “buy it now”
find that the advantage of PYP is impaired when consumers pricing options for products listed on eBay offer a structure for
select prices for multiple products in a single shopping trip, the final price to be determined by potential buyers. These
which increases effort. In fact, fixed price would be more structured options are similar to PYP, which strikes a compro-
effective than PYP in multiple-product decisions; PYP is more mise or balance in the control–effort trade-off. Further inves-
effective when paired with a single product. Contexts that tigation in other pricing areas such as those will enrich our
involve donations are another scenario for which participative control–effort trade-off framework.
pricing, especially PWYW, promises benefits; consumers
already expect to expend effort for the benefit of others. Con-
sistent with research demonstrating PWYW’s benefits for Associate Editor
donations (Gneezy et al. 2010), we find that both PYP and Praveen Kopalle
PWYW outperform fixed prices in charitable contexts.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
Limitations and Avenues for Further Research the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Building on previous research (e.g., Gneezy et al. 2012), we
uncover critical roles of pricing control and effort in determin- Funding
ing the performance of participative pricing and identify mod- The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
erating roles of shopping motives and shopping contexts. ship, and/or publication of this article.
Continued research could aim to elucidate consumer character-
istics and product attributes that also explain the performance
advantages and disadvantages of different forms of participa- References
tive pricing. It also might address the varying dynamics and Allen, Roy G. (1938), Mathematical Analysis for Economists. New
trade-offs associated with brand attitudes and profitability. York: MacMillan.
Although we observe positive effects on revenue and profits Anderson, Eric T. and Duncan I. Simester (2001), “Price Discrimina-
in Study 1 and revenue estimates from Study 5, the tion as an Adverse Signal: Why an Offer to Spread Payments May
willingness-to-pay results in Studies 2–4 indicate inconsistent Hurt Demand,” Marketing Science, 20 (3), 315–27.
patterns (see the Web Appendix). Perhaps pricing strategies Arentze, Theo A., Harmen Oppewal, and Harry J. Timmermans
that offer high pricing control are more effective for building (2005), “A Multipurpose Shopping Trip Model to Assess Retail
favorable brand attitudes, which could be useful for new firms Agglomeration Effects,” Journal of Marketing Research, 42(1),
seeking to acquire consumers. 109–15.
Wang et al. 159

Beck, Joshua T., Ryan Rahinel, and Alexander Bleier (2020), Pay-What-You-Want Pricing and Charitable Giving,” Science,
“Company Worth Keeping: Personal Control and Preferences for 329 (5989), 325–27.
Brand Leaders,” Journal of Consumer Research, 46 (5), 871–86. Gneezy, Ayelet, Uri Gneezy, Gerhard Riener, and Leif D. Nelson
Bettman, James R., Mary Frances Luce, and John W. Payne (1998), (2012), “Pay-What-You-Want, Identity, and Self-Signaling in
“Constructive Consumer Choice Processes,” Journal of Consumer Markets,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109
Research, 25 (3), 187–217. (19), 7236–40.
Bhattarai, Abha (2016), “Priceline Just Dumped the Features That Harlam, Bari A. and Leonard M. Lodish (1995), “Modeling Consu-
Made It Famous,” The Washington Post (September 8), https:// mers’ Choices of Multiple Items,” Journal of Marketing Research,
www.washingtonpost.com/news/the-switch/wp/2016/09/08/price 32 (4), 404–18.
line-just-dumped-the-feature-that-made-it-famous-bidding-on-air Haws, Kelly L. and William O. Bearden (2006), “Dynamic Pricing
line-tickets/. and Consumer Fairness Perceptions,” Journal of Consumer
Cacioppo, John T. and Richard E. Petty (1981), “Social Psychological Research, 33 (3), 304–11.
Procedures for Cognitive Response Assessment: The Thought- Hayes, Andrew F. (2017), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and
Listing Technique,” Cognitive Assessment (11), 309–342. Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach.
Chandran, Sucharita and Vicki G. Morwitz (2005), “Effects of Parti- New York: The Guilford Press.
cipative Pricing on Consumers’ Cognitions and Actions: A Goal Henderson, Conor M., Joshua T. Beck, and Robert W. Palmatier
Theoretic Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (2), (2011), “Review of the Theoretical Underpinnings of Loyalty
249–59. Programs,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21 (3), 256–76.
Charness, Gary and Tsz Cheung (2013), “A Restaurant Field Houck, Brenna (2019), “Panera’s Utopic Pay-What-You-Want
Experiment in Charitable Contributions,” Economics Letters, 119 Restaurant Dream Is Dead,” Eater (Feburary 5), https://www.
(1), 48–9. eater.com/2019/2/5/18212499/panera-cares-closing-pay-what-
Chen, Yuxin, Oded Z. Koenigsberg, and John Zhang (2017), “Pay-as-
you-can-restaurant.
You-Wish Pricing,” Marketing Science, 36 (5), 780–91.
Ismael, Amir (2017), “Everlane Has Brought Back Its ‘Choose What
Chernev, Alexander, Ulf Böckenholt, and Joseph Goodman (2015),
You Pay’ Sale,” Business Insider (December 27), https://www.
“Choice Overload: A Conceptual Review and Meta-Analysis,”
businessinsider.com/everlane-choose-what-you-pay-sale-2017-12.
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25 (2), 333–58.
Iyengar, Sheena and Mark Lepper (2000), “When Choice Is Demoti-
Childers, Terry, Christopher Carr, Joann Peck, and Stephen Carson
vating: Can One Desire Too Much of a Good Thing?” Journal of
(2011), “Hedonic and Utilitarian Motivations for Online Retail
Personality and Social Psychology, 79 (6), 995–1006.
Shopping Behavior,” Journal of Retailing, 77 (4), 511–35.
Jasper, Cynthia R. and Pi-Nan Rosa Lan (1992), “Apparel Catalog
Christopher, Ranjit M. and Fernando S. Machado (2019), “Consumer
Patronage: Demographic, Lifestyle and Motivational Factors,”
Response to Design Variations in Pay-What-You-Want Pricing,”
Psychology & Marketing, 9 (4), 275–96.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 47 (5), 879–98.
Jin, Liyin, Yanqun He and Ying Zhang (2014), “How Power States
Darden, William R. and Fred D. Reynolds (1971), “Shopping Orienta-
Influence Consumers’ Perceptions of Price Unfairness,” Journal of
tions and Product Usage Rates,” Journal of Marketing Research, 8
Consumer Research, 40 (5), 818–33.
(4), 505–08.
Johnson, Eric J. and Daniel Goldstein (2003), “Do Defaults Save
Darke, Peter R. and Darren W. Dahl (2003), “Fairness and Discounts:
The Subjective Value of a Bargain,” Journal of Consumer Lives?” Science, 302 (5649), 1338–39.
Psychology, 13 (3), 328–38. Kahneman, Daniel and Dale T. Miller (1986), “Norm Theory: Com-
Dhar, Sanjay K. and Stephen J. Hoch (1996), “Price Discrimination paring Reality to Its Alternatives,” Psychological Review, 93 (2),
Using In-Store Merchandising,” Journal of Marketing, 60 (1), 136–53.
17–30. Kim, Ju-Young, Katharina Kaufmann, and Manuel Stegemann (2014),
Duxbury, Darren, Kevin Keasey, Hao Zhang, and Shue Loong Chow “The Impact of Buyer–Seller Relationships and Reference Prices
(2005), “Mental Accounting and Decision Making: Evidence on the Effectiveness of the Pay What You Want Pricing Mechan-
under Reverse Conditions Where Money Is Spent for Time Saved,” ism,” Marketing Letters, 25 (4), 409–23.
Journal of Economic Psychology, 26 (4), 567–80. Kim, Ju-Young, Martin Natter, and Martin Spann (2009), “Pay What
Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker (1981), “Evaluating Structural You Want: A New Participative Pricing Mechanism,” Journal of
Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Marketing, 73 (1), 44–58.
Error,” Journal of Marketing Research, 18 (1), 39–50. Kivetz, Ran (2003), “The Effects of Effort and Intrinsic Motivation on
Gerpott, Torsten J. and Christina Schneider (2016), “Buying Beha- Risky Choice,” Marketing Science, 22 (4), 477–502.
viors When Similar Products Are Available Under Pay-What-You- Koriat, Asher, Sarah Lichtenstein, and Baruch Fischhoff (1980),
Want and Posted Price Conditions: Field-Experimental Evidence,” “Reasons for Confidence,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 65, 135–45. Human Learning and Memory, 6 (2), 107–18.
Gino, Francesca and Cassie Mogilner (2014), “Time, Money, and Krishna, Aradhna, Richard Briesch, Donald R. Lehmann, and Hong
Morality,” Psychological Science, 25 (2), 414–21. Yuan (2002), “A Meta-Analysis of the Impact of Price Presentation
Gneezy, Ayelet, Uri Gneezy, Leif D. Nelson, and Amber Brown on Perceived Savings,” Journal of Retailing, 78 (2), 101–18.
(2010), “Shared Social Responsibility: A Field Experiment in Krishna, Vijay (2002), Auction Theory. San Diego: Academic Press.
160 Journal of Marketing 85(5)

Kunter, Marcus (2015), “Exploring the Pay-What-You-Want Payment Spann, Martin and Gerard J. Tellis (2006), “Does the Internet Promote
Motivation,” Journal of Business Research, 68 (11), 2347–57. Better Consumer Decisions? The Case of Name-Your-Own-Price
Langer, Ellen J. and Judith Rodin (1976), “The Effects of Choice and Auctions,” Journal of Marketing, 70 (1), 65–78.
Enhanced Personal Responsibility for the Aged: A Field Experi- Stangl, Brigitte, Margit Kastner, and Girish Prayag (2017), “Pay-
ment in an Institutional Setting,” Journal of Personality and Social What-You-Want for High-Value Priced Services: Differences
Psychology, 34 (2), 191–98. Between Potential, New, and Repeat Customers,” Journal of Busi-
Leclerc, France, Bernd H. Schmitt, and Laurette Dubé (1995), ness Research, 74 (May), 168–74.
“Waiting Time and Decision Making: Is Time Like Money?” Strotz, Robert H. (1955), “Myopia and Inconsistency in Dynamic
Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (1), 110–19. Utility Maximization,” Review of Economic Studies, 23 (3),
List, John A. and David Lucking-Reiley (2000), “Demand Reduction 165–80.
in Multiunit Auctions: Evidence from a Sportscard Field Experi- Thaler, Richard (1985), “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,”
ment,” American Economic Review, 90 (4), 961–72. Marketing Science, 4 (3), 199–214.
Mehta, Nitin, Surendra Rajiv, and Kannan Srinivasan (2003), “Price Thomaselli, Rich (2012), “Priceline Kills the Messenger Because Ads
Uncertainty and Consumer Search: A Structural Model of Consid- Worked Too Well,” Advertising Age (January 30), http://adage.
eration Set Formation,” Marketing Science, 22 (1), 58–84. com/article/news/priceline-kills-messenger-ads-worked/232409/.
Mela, Carl F., Sunil Gupta, and Donald R. Lehmann (1997), “The Trijp, Hans, Wayne Hoyer, and J. Jeffrey Inman (1996), “Why
Long-Term Impact of Promotion and Advertising on Consumer Switch? Product Category–Level Explanations for True Variety-
Brand Choice,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34 (2), 248–61. Seeking Behavior,” Journal of Marketing Research, 33 (3),
Mulhern, Francis J. and Daniel T. Padgett (1995), “The Relationship 281–92.
Between Retail Price Promotions and Regular Price Purchases,” Tyrangiel, Josh (2007), “Radiohead Says: Pay What You Want,” Time
Journal of Marketing, 59 (4), 83–90. (October 1), http://content.time.com/time/arts/article/
Schmidt, Klaus M., Martin Spann, and Robert Zeithammer (2015), 0,8599,1666973,00.html.
“Pay What You Want as a Marketing Strategy in Monopolistic and Walters, Rockney G. and Heikki J. Rinne (1986), “An Empirical-
Competitive Markets,” Management Science, 61 (6), 1217–36. Investigation into the Impact of Price Promotions on Retail Store
Schneider, Linda G. and Imran S. Currim (1991), “Consumer Pur- Performance,” Journal of Retailing, 62 (3), 237–66.
chase Behaviors Associated with Active and Passive Deal- Warren, Nathan, Sara Hanson, and Hong Yuan (2021), “Feeling
Proneness,” International Journal of Research in Marketing, 8 Manipulated: How Tip Request Sequence Impacts Customers and
(3), 205–22. Service Providers” Journal of Service Research, 24 (1), 66–83.
Schwartz, Barry (2004), The Paradox of Choice: Why More Is Less. Welss, Daniel H. (2018), “The Met’s Updated Admissions Policy,”
New York: Ecco. The Met (January 4), https://www.metmuseum.org/blogs/now-at-
Shively, Holly (2018), “Shoppers Find Deals on Black Friday,” the-met/2018/updated-admissions-policy-daniel-weiss.
Dayton Daily News (accessed March 10, 2019), https://www.day Xia, Lan, Monika Kukar-Kinney, and Kent B. Monroe (2010),
tondailynews.com/news/shoppers-find-deals-black-friday-but- “Effects of Consumers’ Efforts on Price and Promotion Fairness
some-say-not-fun/eXMsY3MLD4mOlC4im7GsSO/. Perceptions,” Journal of Retailing, 86 (1), 1–10.

You might also like