Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Anim. Behav.

, 1985, 33, 985-992

Was 'The Expression of the Emotions' a misleading phrase?


R O B E R T A. H I N D E
Medical Research Council Unit on the Development and Integration of Behaviour, Madingley, Cambridge,
CB3 8AA, U.K.

Abstract. The title of Darwin's book implies that emotional behaviour is a direct expression of a relatively
simple internal state. However data from both animals and humans indicate that neither emotional 'states'
nor emotional behaviour can be considered independently of the factors that give rise to them, and may
involve complex affective-cognitive interactions. Furthermore such 'expression of emotion' often involves
processes of negotiation between two or more individuals. It is suggested that an attempt to grasp the
nettle of the difficulty of defining 'emotion' may aid understanding of the processes involved, and facilitate
liaison between studies of signal movements in animals and human emotions.

Darwin's (1872) The Expression of the Emotions in siders first the ways in which the concept of
Man and Animals, though long neglected, is now emotion has been used in research and then their
properly regarded as one of the starting points for shortcomings. Following Darwin's lead, it is
modern behaviour studies. His use of a compara- assumed that human emotional behaviour and
tive approach to throw light on the ontogenetic and animal signal movements have something in com-
phylogenetic origins of expressive movements fore- mon, and evidence concerning both will be consi-
shadowed much of the later work of Tinbergen dered.
(e.g. 1952) and Lorenz (e.g. 1981). However, whilst
Darwin discussed data from both animals and
humans, ethologists have avoided the term 'emo- C O N C E P T S OF E M O T I O N
tion' because of its subjective overtones. Although
perhaps necessary over a certain period in the The phenomena of emotion are often categorized
development of ethology, this has had the unfor- into its motor aspects (emotional expression), its
tunate effect of reducing conceptual interchanges state aspects (usually expressed with ultimate phy-
between students of animal and human behaviour. siological referents) and its subjective aspects. This,
Recent work on both sides has raised new possibili- however, omits the important issue of emotional
ties for a rapprochement, but at the same time input. Partly for that reason it can be more useful to
suggests that the phrase 'the expression of the categorize the uses of the term emotion and its
emotions' has often pointed research in a wrong derivatives according to whether they are used as
direction. The phrase carries the implication that input (independent), intervening or output (depen-
the behaviour Darwin studied involved simply the dent) variables (Hinde 1972a). In this section
expression of an internal state, and distracts atten- distinctions resulting from such an approach are
tion from the crucial question of the nature of considered briefly: later it is argued that the causal
'emotion'. sequence implied by such a trichotomy is over-sim-
General agreement on a definition of emotion ple.
has indeed been hard to find, but it is argued here
that this difficulty of definition is intrinsic to the
nature of the problem. On the one hand it is useful, Emotion as Input
and perhaps essential, to distinguish between the Terms such as 'emotional stimuli' or emotional
different ways in which the term emotion and its deprivation' normally refer to a category of input.
derivatives are used in research. On the other, such It is of course important to be clear whether the
distinctions may obscure essential aspects of what Category is defined in terms of the nature of the
we are attempting to study. But understandingjust input, or in terms of the resulting state of the
why definition is difficult may help us to come to respondent. Thus Radke-Yarrow & Zahn-Waxler
terms with the problems that emotion poses for (personal communication) make a clear distinction
students of behaviour. This paper, therefore, con- between the emotional stimuli children receive

985
986 Animal Behaviour, 33, 3

from depressed versus normal mothers, and the types of subjective experience. If it is, it may be
consequences of experiencing such stimuli for the limited to intense experiences (e.g. Moruzzi 1969)
children's development and regulation of their own or to those inherently connected with a form of
relationships. In either case, of course, there is a action, as opposed to more general feelings of
real difficulty in distinguishingbetween what is and sadness, pleasure or distress (Bowlby 1969). Furth-
what is not emotional. ermore, the subjective feelings may be regarded as
mere epiphenomena, or as influencing or in-
fluenced by behaviour: here innumerable logical
Emotion as State traps await the unwary (e.g. Ryle 1949; Schwartz
Emotion is perhaps most usually seen as a state 1982).
intervening between stimulus and response. This is
in keeping with Darwin's usage. A number of
further distinctions are important here. Emotion as Output
(1) Motive, state or trait? The supposed internal Emotion also often refers to a form of behaviour,
state may be thought of as closely associated with a to the output of a system. The terms 'emotional
particular type of behaviour and as lasting only so behaviour' and 'emotional expression' are of
long as that behaviour, or it may refer to a state of course always used in this way. They refer espe-
longer duration (mood) involving a proneness to cially to movements occurring in conditions of
respond in particular ways; or it may refer to more excitement, danger, joy, etc. Both the somatic and
permanent characteristics of an individual. This is a the autonomic nervous system are usually in-
facet of the state-trait continuum as discussed by volved. The movement may or may not have been
Allen & Potkay (1981). adapted for a signal function.
(2) Intervening variable or hypothetical con- Such a usage would be more valuable if there
struct? Emotion may be used as an intervening were a category of behaviour termed 'emotional'
variable, carrying no physiological implications. that could be clearly distinguished from non-emo-
Its usefulness then depends on the number of tional behaviour. Unfortunately that is not the
dependent variables associated with it, and on the case: some authors have used emotional behaviour
strength of the correlations between them (cf. as virtually co-extensive with motivated behaviour
Miller 1959). Alternatively, it may be used as a (e.g. Bindra 1959; Plutchik 1962). In addition,
hypothetical construct, with implications about experimental psychologists have used 'conditioned
physiological mechanisms, such as the brain-stem emotional response' to refer to the interruption or
reticular system (e.g. M oruzzi 1969), limbic system diminution of ongoing behaviour as the result of an
(MacLean 1958) or autonomic nervous system intruding stimulus (Estes & Skinner 1941). How-
(Mandler 1975). ever too much must not be made of this issue: at the
(3) Mode of action? Emotion may be thought of extreme, many examples of emotional behaviour
as (a) energizing behaviour directly, as when anger would be universally recognized as such, and
is spoken of as instigating aggressive responses (e.g. emotional behaviour is by no means unique
Berkowitz 1962); or (b) as a reinforcing event, as amongst biological and psychological categories in
when fear refers to an acquired drive, in the sense being clouded by borderline cases.
that an individual will learn responses that reduce
it; or (c) as a goal or positive incentive (which may
also be a reinforcer); or (d) as a modulator or General
disrupter of behaviour, especially when extreme There can surely be no dispute that such distinc-
(e.g. Leeper 1948). tions can be useful: we must be clear-headed about
(4) Specific or general? Emotion as an internal what we mean by emotion. The problems on which
state may be seen as related to many or all types of we focus will differ according to our concept of
behaviour, as in activation theories (e.g. Schlosberg emotion. Thus in the study of development, treat-
1954; Duffy 1962) or as specific to one or more ment of emotion as a dependent variable would be
limited groups of behaviour patterns. in keeping with a discussion of the development of
(5) Subjective experience? An emotional state emotional expression; emotion as an intervening
may or may not be regarded as associated with variable might concern the differentiation of emo-
subjective experience in general or with particular tions or the development of the emotional charac-
Hinde. Expression of the emotions? 987

teristics of individuals; and the study of emotion as expressions of human newborns can be embraced
an independent variable might concern the in- by the view that they reflect complex and changing
fluence of maternal depression on a child. organismic states and communicate messages to
At the same time, however, the cause-effect others (Emde et al. 1978), but with cognitive
sequence too easily implied by the independent- development the issue soon becomes greatly com-
intervening~tependentvariable trichotomy carries plicated. First, experience may affect either the
a danger of distorting nature. In the following propensity to show emotional behaviour in general
pages it is emphasized that emotion as a state is far or to show particular kinds of emotional beha-
from unitary and thus of limited value as an viour. Second, learning may affect the appraisal of
intervening variable (or hypothetical construct), the situation which evokes the emotion in the first
that understanding of emotion as input or emotion instance, or modulate emotional expression to fit
as output cannot be separated from emotion as the requirements of the situation or culture, or
state, and that it is more profitable to think in terms affect attempts to cope with the source of the
of causal loops than chains. It is convenient to emotion (Ekman, personal communication). This
consider the question of motivational state first. learning includes the acquisition of such elemen-
tary capacities as object permanence and anticipa-
tion, indicated for instance by the appearance of
C H A I N S OR L O O P S ? surprise (Charlesworth 1969) or laughter before the
mother's reappearance in a peek-a-boo game
What Sort of a State? (Sroufe & Waters 1976; Emde et al. 1978). It also
It is often convenient to distinguish between involves more complex capacities, such as those
emotions or affects as driving forces, and cogni- involved in the development of the self-concept and
tions as embracing the processes that modulate the capacity to empathize with others (e.g. Hoff-
their expression (cf. Piaget & Inhelder 1966, cited in man 1976; Lewis et al. 1978). As D6carie (1978) has
D6carie 1978). However it is now apparent from emphasized, the development of emotion and
studies of both child and adult human subjects that cognition are inextricably interwoven.
emotion and cognition are closely interwoven (e.g. Thus, if emotion is used to refer to an internal
Piaget & Inhelder 1958; Schachter & Singer 1962; state, that state is a far from simple one, and
Candland et al. 1977; Plutchik 1980; Ekman 1981): includes also the effects of, or expectations about,
we shall see later that a similar conclusion is the external situation. It is often not profitable to
emerging from studies of animal behaviour. It is consider emotional states independently of the
also implicit in current neuro-psychological theory: factors that gave rise to them.
for instance Bindra (1976) writes in terms of a
central motive state, generated by both physiologi-
cal and perceived environmental factors, which in How Can Emotional Input be Specified?
turn renders certain stimuli more salient by affect- In considering emotion as an independent vari-
ing their central representations (see also Arnold able, the first issue is related to the old problem of
1970). Thus we must remember both that cogni- 'what is a stimulus?' (Verplanck 1954). Is emotional
tions (in so far as they can be conceptualized input to be assessed in terms that relate ultimately
independently of emotions) affect emotions, and to physical characteristics of the input, or in terms
also that emotions (in so far as they can be of the response of the individual? Individuals
conceptualized independently of cognitions) affect respond differently to the same input, so what is an
cognitions (e.g. Zajonc 1980). If it is useful to emotional event for one may not be so for another.
distinguish between them, it is essential to think in Indeed the emotional salience of a stimulus or
terms of a continuing cognitive-emotion interac- stimulation may depend on the individual's current
tion (e.g. Candland 1977). emotional state. Thus emotional stimuli can be
This issue is crucial for the study of emotions in defined only by reference both to the external event
development, for it implies that there are multiple and to the respondent.
ways in which experience may affect the emotions A closely related issue is that the elicitation of an
or emotional development, and multiple ways in emotional state or emotional behaviour often
which the emotions may affect experience. It may depends on relations between aspects of the exter-
well be that all that matters about the emotional nal situation and aspects of the internal state. Thus
988 Animal Behaviour, 33, 3

the elicitation of much emotional behaviour is Furthermore, the consequences of a given 'emo-
often described as involving an interrupting event, tional environment' on an individual's develop-
or a discrepancy between a perceived state of ment may depend on other characteristics of that
affairs and that which was desired or expected (e.g. individual. For example the relations between the
Mandler 1975). The ethologist's interpretation of characteristics of rhesus monkey mothers and their
many signal movements as based on conflicting offspring differ according to the infant's sex (Ste-
motivations (see below) could be reinterpreted in venson-Hinde et al. 1980a, b).
similar terms. Thus the impact of a potentially Finally, the manner in which individuals inter-
emotional stimulus may depend on the nature pret emotional behaviour of others depends in part
and/or previous experience of the individual con- on their own natures. Erode (1980) found that some
cerned, or on the simultaneous elicitation of incom- human mothers interpret a cry that is intense and
patible courses of action. prolonged in the newborn period as indicative of
In the same vein, the 'chronic emotional condi- anger, whilst other mothers believe their babies do
tions', to use Radke-Yarrow & Zahn-Waxler's not express anger until they show temper tantrums
term (personal communication), to which a child is at a year old.
exposed, may depend on the characteristics of the
child and/or the child's relationship with the
caregiver and others about him. For instance, in a Does Emotional Behaviour Express an Emotional
study of pre-school children and their mothers State?
Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde (in press) found that We have seen that emotional states can often not
mothers who scored highly on a self-report ques- be considered independently of the external factors
tionnaire assessing various mood items, tended in that gave rise to them, and that external emotional
an independent set of observations to show more stimuli cannot be considered independently of the
'physical friendliness' to daughters than did responding individual. What about emotion as the
mothers with low scores, but this was not the case output: emotional behaviour?
for sons. The correlations between physical frien- The implication of the title of Darwin's book is
dliness and the maternal mood items depression, that emotional behaviour is the outward ex-
anxiety and outward irritability were 0.53 pression of an emotional state, and that there is a
(P<0.05), 0.72 (P<0.01) and 0'51 (P<0-05) for one-to-one correspondence between the two. Such
daughters, but -0.26, - 0 . 0 6 and 0.06 for sons. a correspondence is assumed by much current work
Apparently expression of the mother's emotional on emotional behaviour (e.g. Hopf, Herzog &
state differs between mother-daughter and moth- Ploog, personal communication). But if the inter-
er-son relationships. nal state is complex one must ask what it is that the
In humans the chronic emotional conditions to emotional behaviour expresses? Cross-cultural evi-
which a child is exposed may also depend in part on dence indicates that emotional expression is inhi-
the expectations of the family members concerned. bited and/or exaggerated to different extents in
Thus the home relationships of 50-month-old different cultures (e.g. Ekman & Friesen 1969; Eibl
children assessed as shy, differed according to their Eibesfeldt 1975), and child developmentalists
sex: whereas shy girls tended to have easy mother- attempting to measure emotional states have been
child relationships, shy boys tended to have rather impressed by the same problem (Lewis & Michal-
tense ones (Simpson & Stevenson-Hinde 1985). By son 1982). Several lines of comparative data also
contrast, girls high on the characteristics active, suggest a certain lability between aspects of the
irregular, attention span, unmalleable, intense, internal state and behaviour (Hinde, in press).
moody and assertive showed a greater tendency to Firstly, characteristics of an expressive movement
have tense mother-child relationships than did may remain constant even though independent
boys high on those characteristics (Hinde & Steven- evidence indicates considerable variation in inter-
son-Hinde, in press). Apparently mothers respond nal state. Thus the repetition rate of the bow-coos
to particular temperament characteristics in girls of doves is remarkably constant in each species,
differently from the way they would respond in and independent of the length of the bow-cooing
boys. Shy girls are acceptable, shy boys are not, and bout, the latter presumably indicating motivation
the characteristics active, intense, moody, etc. are (Morris 1975).
seen as even less appropriate in girls than in boys. Secondly, the sequelae of the threat postures of
Hinde: Expression of the emotions? 989

tits (Parus spp.) change with time of year (Stokes rarely be deterred, and the signaller will usually
1962). In so far as those sequelae are indicative of flee. Recently empirical support for the view that
the state accompanying the expressive movement, the subsequent behaviour of the signaller depends
the correspondence between state and movement in part on that of the recipient of the signal has
would seem to be imprecise. become available (Bossema & Burgler 1980).
Thirdly, the occurrence of certain expressive Such considerations suggest the view that emo-
movements (e.g. threat displays in birds) may be tional behaviour may lie along a continuum from
suppressed or accentuated by behavioural tenden- behaviour that is more or less purely expressive to
cies normally irrelevant to them, e.g. a tendency to behaviour concerned primarily with a process of
behave sexually (Hinde 1953; Kruijt 1964; Baer- negotiation between individuals (Hinde, in press).
ends 1975). In animals, bird song lies nearer the expressive end,
Fourthly, in animals, as in man, deception threat postures nearer the negotiation end. In man,
(Menzel 1971; Ekman 1981) and 'manipulation' spontaneous and solitary laughter are primarily
(Dawkins & Krebs 1978) may occur. expressive, the ingratiating smile primarily nego-
Thus the relation between emotional state and tiating. However most emotional expressions in-
emotional behaviour is by no means simple. volve both. This has been demonstrated by Zivin
Further evidence along the same lines is presented (in press) from data obtained in a situation devised
in the next section. by Camras (1977). Two children were placed in a
situation in which they had to compete for a
desirable object. If they could see each others'
Emotional Expression and Interaction faces, the plus face, a facial expression often used in
Some of Darwin's discussion of emotional beha- agonistic contexts, often appeared. If an opaque
viour was concerned with threat postures. partition was placed between them, the incidence of
Although complex, these have been profitably this expression dropped markedly. The decrease
interpreted as depending on mutually incompatible can presumably be ascribed to the fact that negotia-
internal tendencies, usually to attack the rival, to tion was no longer possible: that it continued at all
flee and to stay put (e.g. Tinbergen 1952; Baerends is evidence for an expressive element.
1975). Ethologists have assumed that the internal As Berscheid's (in press) detailed analysis shows,
state expressed by a given posture can be assessed the behaviour of the partner in an ongoing relation-
from the behaviour that accompanies or follows it, ship often confirms or disrupts expectancies, inter-
for instance from the probability of a bird attac- rupts ongoing sequences, or arouses conflicting
king or fleeing after posturing (e.g. Moynihan motivations. Interactions within ongoing relation-
1955). Now from the point of view of natural ships thus provide a likely forum for emotional
selection, it would not be adaptive for an individual expression. Use of the term 'negotiation' is in
to communicate to a rival the likelihood that he harmony with this view, emphasizing in addition
would attack or flee. Such signals make sense only that much interaction involves attempts to control
if the threatening individual either is attempting to the partner or to adapt one's own behaviour to that
bluff, deceive or manipulate the rival (Dawkins & of the partner.
Krebs 1978), or else is uncertain what to do next, The view that examples of emotional behaviour
because what he should do depends in part on the lie along a continuum from expression to negotia-
behaviour of the other (Hinde 1972b, 1981). The tion is of course closely linked to the concept of
latter hypothesis integrates a number of facts about continuing emotion-cognition interaction. The ex-
animal signals. For instance, some avian threat pressive movement depends on the current internal
postures are frequently followed by fleeing by the state but is coupled with assessments of the other
threatening bird, but there seem to be few threat individual's internal state, which in turn affects the
postures that are regularly followed by attack. This actor's internal state. Indeed it implies that emo-
makes sense if what is being signalled is of the type tional behaviour may be interpretable not in terms
'I want to stay put, but if you approach I am likely of a basic unitary emotional state, and not even in
to attack' or 'likely to flee'. In the former case, the terms of an affective--cognitive interaction within
recipient of the signal is unlikely to approach, and an individual, but often only in terms of interac-
so attack by the signaller will follow relatively tions between affective-cognitive states in two
infrequently. In the latter case, the recipient will individuals (see Berscheid, in press). Our neat
990 Animal Behaviour, 33, 3

distinction of emotion as either an independent, an in the ways in which it is used. One route seemed to
intervening or a dependent variable is clearly in be to distinguish clearly between emotion as an
disarray: all three must be studied simultaneously. independent, as an intervening and as a dependent
There is of course also a developmental problem variable. Even this, however, is fraught with diffi-
here. The cry of the newborn can presumably be culties which are inherent in the behavioural
regarded as purely expressive. But reciprocal inter- phenomena themselves. Attempts to relate emo-
change plays a crucial role in the communicative tion to one or other part of an emotional stimulus-
exchange even of young infants: for instance, they emotional state-emotional behaviour sequence
become distressed if their normally friendly encounter the difficulties that the stimuli cannot be
mothers remain impassive in a face-to-face interac- defined independently of the state, the state is
tion (Tronick et al. 1978). The full development of complex and involves appraisal of the stimulus,
communicationinvolvingnegotiation requires ulti- and the emotional behaviour is affected by the state
mately sophisticated capacities implying some and by the nature of the recipient, and may itself
appreciation of intentionality and perspective-tak- affect the state. This is not a council of despair. We
ing. In so far as the 2-month-old's smile conveys all know that nature is complicated: we must
'keep it up, I like it', it can be regarded as a step sharpen our conceptual tools as best we can, and
along that road. It is relevant here that the have faith that in using them to untangle the
emotions best understood by young children are complexity we shall see how to fashion better ones.
the 'fundamental' emotions of surprise, anger,
shame, etc., whereas emotions like pride, which
presumably involve a greater cognitive element, are ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
not understood until later.
A further issue concerns the extent to which an This work was supported by the Medical Research
individual's emotional behaviour affects his own Council and the Royal Society. The paper was
emotional state. Some would argue that emotional based on a paper given at a Symposium of the
behaviour depends on the individual's appraisal of International Society for the Study of Child De-
his/her own autonomic arousal. Whilst the impor- velopment, at which Dr Marian Radke-Yarrow
tance of proprioceptive feedback or other conse- and Dr Siegfried Hopf also presented papers.
quences of emotional behaviour on the actor is
controversial, Mandler (1975) assumes that the
REFERENCES
autonomic activity and its cognitive appraisal each
affect the other during an emotional episode. Here
Allen, B. P. & Potkay, C. R. 1981. On the arbitrary
again there is a developmental problem. Does such distinction between states and traits. J. Personal. Soc.
positive feedback operate from birth? Psychol., 41, 916 928.
We have seen that emotional states are likely to Arnold, M. 1970. Perennial problems in the field of
be complex, that there may be no simple relation emotion. In: Feelings and Emotions (Ed. by M. Arnold),
New York: Academic Press.
between an emotional state and its expression, and Baerends, G. P. 1975. An evaluation of the conflict
that its expression may involve negotiation with hypothesis as an explanatory principle for the evolu-
another individual. Furthermore all these consider- tion of displays. In: Function and Evolution in Behaviour
ations apply equally to that other individual: (Ed. by G. P. Baerends, C. Beer & A. Manning), pp.
emotional behaviour in a social situation may 187-227. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Berkowitz, L. 1962. Aggression. New York: McGraw-
involve negotiation between two individuals, each Hill.
one's perceptions of the other's state and predic- Berscheid, E. In press. Emotional experience in close
tions about the other's future behaviour influenc- relationships: implications for child development. In:
ing his own state and thus his own emotional The Effects of Early Relationships Upon Children's
Soeioemotional Development (Ed. by Z. Rubin & W.
behaviour. It is hardly surprising that the study of Hartrup). New Jersey: Erlbaum.
emotions in real life situations poses problems. Bindra, D. 1959. Motivation: A Systematic Reinterpre-
tation. New York: Ronald Press.
Bindra, D. 1976. A Theory of Intelligent Behaviour. New
CONCLUSION York: Wiley.
Bossema, I. & Burgler, R. R. 1980. Communication
We started by recognizing the confusion surround- during monocular and binocular looking in European
ing the concept of emotion, and the need for clarity jays (Garrulus g. glandarius). Behaviour, 74, 274-283.
Hinde: Expression o f the emotions? 991

Bowlby, J. 1969. Attachment and Loss. Vol. 1 Attachment. Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. In press. Relation-
London: Hogarth Press. ships, personality and the social situation. In: Key
Camras, L. 1977. Facial expression used by children in a Issues in Interpersonal Relationships (Ed. by R. Gil-
conflict situation. Child Devel., 48, 1431-1435. mour & S. Duck). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Candland, D. K. 1977. The persistent problems of Hoffman, M. L. 1976. Empathy, role-taking, guilt, and
emotion. In: Emotion (Ed. by D. K. Candland, J. P. development of altruistic motives. In: Moral Develop-
Fell, E. Kenn, A. I. Leshner, R. M. Tarpy, & R. ment: Current Theory and Research (E& by T. Likona),
Plutchik). Monterey, California: Brooks/Cote Publish- pp. 124~143. New York: Holt.
ing Company. Kruijt, J. P. 1964. Ontogeny of social behaviour in
Candland, D. K., Fell, J. P., Kenn, E., Leshner, A. I., Burmese red junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus Bonna-
Tarpy, R. M. & Plutchik, R. (Eds) 1977. Emotion. terre). Behav. Suppl., 12, 1-201.
Monterey, California: Brooks/Cole Publishing. Leeper, R. W. 1948. A motivational theory of emotion to
Charlesworth, W. R. 1969. The role of surprise in replace 'emotion and disorganized response'. Psychol.
cognitive development. In: Studies in Cognitive De- Rev., 55, 5~1.
velopment (Ed. by D. Elkind & J. H. Glavell). New Lewis, M., Brooks, J. & Haviland, J. 1978. Hearts and
York: Oxford University Press. faces: a study in the measmement of emotion. In: The
Darwin, C. 1872. The Expression o f the Emotions in Man Development of Affect (Ed. by M. Lewis, & L. A.
and Animals. London: John Murray. Rosenblum), pp. 77-124. New York: Plenum Press.
Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. 1978. Animal signals: Lewis, M. & Michalson, L. 1982. The measurement of
information or manipulation? In: Behavioural Ecology." emotional state. In: Measuring Emotions in Infants and
An Evolutionary Approach (Ed. by J. R. Krebs & N. B. Children (Ed. by C. E. Izard), pp. 178-207. Cambridge:
Davies), pp. 282 312. Oxford: Blackwell. Cambridge University Press.
D6carie, T. G. 1978. Affect development and cognition in Lorenz, K. Z. 1981. The Foundations of Ethology. New
a Piagetian context. In: The Development o f Affect (Ed. York: Springer-Verlag.
by M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum), pp. 183-204. New MacLean, P. D. 1958. Contrasting functions of limbic
York: Plenum Press. and neocortical systems of the brain and their relevance
Duffy, E. 1962. Activation and Behavior. New York: to psychophysiological aspects of medicine. Am. J.
Wiley. Med., 25, 611-626.
Eibl Eibesfeldt, I. 1975. Ethology. New York: Holt, Mandler, G. 1975. Mind and Emotion. New York: Wiley.
Rinehart. Menzel, E. W. 1971. Communication about the environ-
Ekman, P. 1981. Mistakes when deceiving. Ann. N. Y. ment in a group of young chimpanzees. Foliaprimatol.,
Acad. Sci., 364, 269 278. 15, 220-232.
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W, V. 1969. The repertoire of Miller, N. E. 1959. Liberalization of basic S-R concepts.
nonverbal behavior. Semiotica, 1, 49-98. In: Psychology, a Study of a Science: Study L Vol. I(Ed.
Emde, R. N. 1980. Toward a psychoanalytic theory of by S. Koch), pp. 196-292. New York: McGraw-Hill.
affect. II Emerging models of emotional development Morris, D. 1957. 'Typical intensity' and its relation to the
in infancy. In: The Course of Life: Psychoanalytic problem of ritualisation_ Behaviour, 11, 1-12.
Contributions Toward Understanding Personality De- Moruzzi, G. 1969. The emotional significance of the
velopment (Ed. by S. Greenspan & G. Pollock). Wash- ascending reticular system_ Arch. ItaL Biol., 96, 17-28.
ington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. Moynihan, M. I955. Some aspects of reproductive
Emde, R. N., Kligman, D. H., Reich, J. H. & Wade, T. D. behaviour in the black-headed gull (Larus ridibundus
1978. Emotional expression in infancy: 1. Initial studies ridibundus L.) and related species. Behav. Suppl., 4,
of social signaling and emergent model. In: The 1-201.
Development of Affect (Ed. by M. Lewis & L. A. Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. 1958. The Growth of Logical
Rosenblum), pp. 125-148. New York: Plenum Press. Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence. New York:
Estes, W. K. & Skinner, B. F. 1941. Some quantitative Basic Books.
properties of anxiety. J. Exp. Psyehol., 29, 390-400. Plutchik, R. 1962. The Emotions." Facts, Theories and a
Hinde, R. A. 1953. The conflict between drives in the New Model. New York: Random House.
courtship and copulation of the chaffinch. Behaviour, 5, Plutchik, R. 1980. Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Syn-
1-31. thesis. New York: Harper & Row.
Hinde, R. A. 1972a. Concepts of emotion. In: Ciba Ryle, G. 1949. The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchison.
Foundation Symposium. Physiology, Emotion and Psy- Schachter, S. & Singer, J. E. 1962. Cognitive, social and
chosomatic Illness, pp. 3-13. Amsterdam: Elsevier, physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol.
Excerpta Medica, North-Holland. Rev., 69, 379-399.
Hinde, R. A. 1972b. Social Behaviour and its Development Schlosberg, H. 1954. Three dimensions of emotion.
in Subhuman Primates. Eugene, Oregon: University of Psychol. Rev., 61, 81-88.
Oregon Books. Schwartz, G. E. 1982. Psychophysiological patterning
Hinde, R. A. 1981. Animal signals: ethological and and emotion revisited: a systems perspective. In: Mea-
games-theory approaches are not incompatible. Anim. suring Emotions in Infants and Children (Ed. by C. E.
Behav., 29, 535 542. Izard), pp. 67-96. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Hinde, R. A. In press. Signals--expression and negotia- Press.
tion. In: The Development of Expressive Behaviour (Ed. Simpson, A. E. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. 1985. Tempera-
by G. Zivin). New York: Academic Press. mental characteristics of 3-4 year old boys and girls
992 Animal Behaviour, 33, 3

and child-family interactions. J. Child Psychol. Psy- B. 1978. The infant's response to entrapment between
chiat., 26, 43-54. contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. J.
Sroufe, L. A. & Waters, E. 1976. The ontogenesis of Am. Acad. Child Psyehiat., 17, 1-13.
smiling and laughter: a perspective on the organization Verplanck, W. S. 1954. Burrhus F. Skinner. In: Modern
of development in infancy. Psychol. Rev., 83, 173-189. Learning Theory (Ed. by W. K. Estes, S. Koch, K.
Stevenson-Hinde, J., Stillwell-Barnes, R. & Zunz, M. MacCorquodale, P. E. Meehl, C. G. Mueller, W. N.
1980a. Subjective assessment of rhesus monkeys over Schoenfeld & W. S. Verplanck), pp. 267-316. New
four successive years. Primates, 21, 66-82. York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Stevenson-Hinde, J., Stillwell-Barnes, R. & Zunz, M. Zajonc, R. B. 1980. Feeling and thinking: preferences
1980b. Individual differences in young rhesus monkeys: need no inferences. Am. Psychol., 35~ 151-175.
consistency and change. Primates, 21, 498 509. Zivin, G. In press. Watching the sands shift: conceptualiz-
Stokes, A. W. 1962. The comparative ethology of great, ing development of nonverbal mastery. In: The De-
blue, marsh and coal tits at a winter feeding station. velopment of Non-Verbal Communication in Children
Behaviour, 19, 208-218. (Ed. by G. Zivin). New York: Academic Press.
Tinbergen, N. 1952. Derived activities: their causation,
biological significance, origin and emancipation during
evolution. Q. Rew Biol., 17, 1-32. (Received 31 May 1984; revised 30 July 1984; MS.
Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S. & Brazelton, number: 2549)

You might also like