How Mindfulness Shapes The Situational Use of Emotion Regulation

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Cognition and Emotion

ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20

How mindfulness shapes the situational use of


emotion regulation strategies in daily life

Mario Wenzel, Zarah Rowland & Thomas Kubiak

To cite this article: Mario Wenzel, Zarah Rowland & Thomas Kubiak (2020): How mindfulness
shapes the situational use of emotion regulation strategies in daily life, Cognition and Emotion, DOI:
10.1080/02699931.2020.1758632

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1758632

Published online: 06 May 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 110

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcem20
COGNITION AND EMOTION
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1758632

How mindfulness shapes the situational use of emotion regulation


strategies in daily life
Mario Wenzel, Zarah Rowland and Thomas Kubiak
Institute of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Mainz, Germany

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Mindfulness is associated with a wide range of beneficial outcomes such as well- Received 21 June 2019
being. However, less is known about the mechanisms underlying these benefits. Revised 9 April 2020
Some researchers suggest that the benefits could be driven by emotion Accepted 13 April 2020
regulation, either by improving the effectiveness of emotion regulation or by
KEYWORDS
lessening the need for effortful emotion regulation. By using two longitudinal Mindfulness; emotion
Ambulatory Assessment data sets (NStudy1 = 125, NStudy2 = 175), based on a six-week regulation; emotion
randomised controlled trial and a one-week study, we tested these competing regulation strategies;
notions in daily life and found support for the latter: Mindfulness, and especially reappraisal; ambulatory
its non-judgmental acceptance facet, was significantly associated with less use of assessment
emotion regulation strategies. However, mindfulness was not significantly
associated with more effective emotion regulation strategy implementation.
Moreover, the mindfulness training focusing on present-moment attention and
awareness did not significantly influence emotion regulation. These findings
demonstrate the importance of the non-judgmental acceptance component for
emotion regulation and affective well-being.

The last two decades have seen a surge of interest in Two main theories have been put forward to
mindfulness, the open and receptive awareness of explain how mindfulness contributes to improved
the present moment (Brown & Ryan, 2003), and its well-being. According to the Mindfulness-to-Meaning
beneficial psychological effects (Keng et al., 2011). Theory (Garland et al., 2015), practicing mindfulness
Mindfulness is often described by two key com- increases the repertoire and the effectiveness of
ponents: (a) the regulation of attention to and aware- emotion regulation strategies, especially reappraisal:
ness of the present moment and (b) the non- Being aware of and accepting one’s sensations helps
judgmental acceptance of these present-moment individuals to stop their habitual and automatic reac-
experiences (Bishop et al., 2004). Meta-analytical evi- tions towards present-moment experiences by noti-
dence suggests that mindfulness trainings are cing the many facets of a momentary situation
effective in improving a large number of psychologi- which can entail both positive and negative aspects.
cal (Lomas et al., 2017) and clinical outcomes (Khoury The broadened attention can, then, help individuals
et al., 2015). For example, state mindfulness was to regulate their emotions more effectively, i.e. by
associated with less negative affect (Brown & Ryan, reappraising a given situation with a focus on its posi-
2003) and trait mindfulness, especially its acceptance tive aspects. By repeating this process across many
facet, with a better regulation of negative affect situations in daily life, individuals learn to generalize
(Coffey et al., 2010). However, research so far to find meaning in specific situations, which improves
focused on the effects of mindfulness trainings or their well-being in the long run (Garland et al., 2015).
associations with trait mindfulness, but less is In contrast to the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory,
known about the mechanisms that lead to, e.g. the Monitor-and-Acceptance Theory (Lindsay & Cres-
better well-being. well, 2017) does not explain the link between

CONTACT Mario Wenzel wenzelma@uni-mainz.de


© 2020 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 M. WENZEL ET AL.

mindfulness and well-being by improved emotion regu- However, evidence for the two theoretical accounts
lation but by a lower necessity to regulate emotions due of the association between mindfulness and emotion
to acceptance of negative and positive emotions. By regulation comes from laboratory and neuropsycholo-
practicing mindfulness, individuals learn to regulate gical research and, thus, evidence in daily life is
and focus their attention on specific stimuli such as missing. In this line of research, the link between mind-
one’s own breath in order to become aware of their fulness and affect has been characterised as a bottom-
momentary sensations instead of thinking about past up process (i.e. decreased reactivity towards emotion-
or future events (monitoring process). However, moni- eliciting stimuli in accordance to the Monitor-and-
toring is a necessary but not a sufficient condition for Acceptance Theory), or as a top-down process (i.e.
improved well-being, given that focusing without increased effectivity of regulatory efforts in accordance
accepting one’s own feelings can actually increase to the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory), in that it may
negative experiences (Barnes & Lynn, 2010; Pearson be an important precursor to reappraise emotions in
et al., 2015). Thus, individuals also need to apply an the first place (Chiesa et al., 2010; Creswell et al.,
accepting and nonjudgmental stance towards momen- 2019). The terms bottom-up and top-down refer to
tary and upcoming feelings in order to reduce affective the notion of activity in brain regions associated with
reactivity and the need to regulate their emotions effortful control over emotions such as the prefrontal
(acceptance process). This highlights the importance cortex (top-down) or in brain regions associated with
of acceptance, which can be defined as the willingness emotion experiences (bottom-up). Thus, top-down pro-
to remain present with experiences without trying to cesses are processes that influence emotions through
change them or evaluate them as good or bad regulatory efforts, whereas bottom-up processes are
(Lindsay & Creswell, 2017). Thus, both theories can be processes that more directly reduce reactivity towards
situated at different stages in the process model of emotion-eliciting stimuli without effortful control and,
emotion regulation (Gross, 2015). The Mindfulness-to- thus, influence the generation of emotions.
Meaning Theory acts during the cognitive change Support for the bottom-up perspective comes from
stage, such that acceptance helps individuals to reap- studies showing reduced activation in brain regions
praise difficult situations and, thus, increases the effec- associated with emotional processes (amygdala and hip-
tiveness of effortful regulatory efforts. The Monitor- pocampus) in experienced mindfulness meditators when
and-Acceptance Theory acts earlier in the emotion regu- they experienced pain in comparison to non-meditators
lation process by decreasing emotional reactivity to (e.g. Grant et al., 2011). This suggests a functional decou-
stressful situations, such that being in a mindful state pling of the cognitive-evaluative and sensory-discrimina-
enables an individual to monitor their arising affective tive dimensions of bodily sensations and emotions in
experiences and let them pass without the need to mindful individuals, which then signals less need for
actively regulate them. Consequently, this highlights effortful emotion regulation. Furthermore, participants
that the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory and the showed reduced amygdala activity and increased activity
Monitor-and-Acceptance Theory are not mutually exclu- in brain regions associated in attentional deployment
sive, but that practicing mindfulness can improve both only in a breath-focused attention task but not in distrac-
acceptance and effortful emotion regulation. tion-focused attention task (Goldin & Gross, 2010). Thus,
Support for the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory accepting the fluctuating nature of emotions shuns the
comes from a randomised controlled trial that found need to reappraise situations (Chambers et al., 2009)
that broadened awareness was associated with and the need to react to or avoid them (Gratz & Tull, 2010).
increased use of reappraisal in a 9-month follow-up In turn, evidence for the top-down perspective is
after a mindfulness-based stress reduction pro- provided by studies that show that state mindfulness
gramme and that it led to significantly higher levels is not only linked with activity in brain regions associ-
of awareness than cognitive–behavioural therapy ated with emotional arousal but also with activity in
(Garland et al., 2017). Regarding the Monitor-and- regions associated with cognitive control (Lutz et al.,
Acceptance Theory, there is evidence that practicing 2008). Compared to the bottom-up perspective, practi-
both monitoring and acceptance reduces feelings of cing mindfulness does not lessen the need to regulate
loneliness (Lindsay et al., 2019) and mind-wandering one’s emotions but instead leads to more effective
(Rahl et al., 2017) as well as increases positive feelings emotion regulation due to a higher recruitment of
(Lindsay et al., 2018) compared to practicing monitor- executive control processes, as reflected in increased
ing only or to an active control group. activity regulatory prefrontal cortex brain regions.
COGNITION AND EMOTION 3

However, given that reviews showed that previous Ethical Approval


findings are based on laboratory studies with relatively
The protocol was approved by the ethics committee
small sample sizes (Chiesa et al., 2010; Creswell et al.,
of the Institute of Psychology at the Johannes Guten-
2019), we aimed at testing the two notions on the
berg University Mainz (reference codes 2015-JGU-
relationship between mindfulness and emotion regu-
psychEK-011 and 2017-JGU-psychEK-014).
lation in daily life by using two longitudinal Ambulatory
Assessment data sets, one based on a six-week ran-
domised controlled trial and the second one on a Other publications
one-week ambulatory assessment. If mindfulness is
best characterised as a top-down process that can be The dataset of Study 1 has been used for other publi-
situated during the cognitive change stage in the cations which investigated the following research
process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 2015) questions: the influence of mindfulness on affective
according to the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory, it dynamics (Rowland et al., 2018), the efficacy of an
should moderate the effectiveness of effortful strat- ultra-brief mindfulness training (Rowland et al.,
egies such as reappraisal and suppression in regulating 2019), the moderation of consecutive self-control
affect. If, however, mindfulness is better characterised (Wenzel et al., 2019), as well as the examination of
as a bottom-up process that reduces the need for the setback effect (Wenzel, Rowland, Hofmann, &
emotion regulation by accepting one’s own emotions Kubiak, in press). Study 2’s dataset will be used for
and behaviours, which can be best situated during the preregistered research questions.
the attentional deployment stages in the process
model of emotion regulation, it should be significantly
associated with less efforts to actively change these Study 1
negative emotions. This would be reflected in less Method
emotion regulation strategy use in daily life.
Participants and design
The power calculation was based on the parent study,
Disclosures which aimed at a power of 1-β = .95, an alpha level of
α = .05, and an expected total attrition rate of 10% for
Reporting
a small effect of the brief mindfulness training of
We affirm that we reported all manipulations and Cohen’s d = .33. To that end, 137 participants were
exclusions in the present study. However, we did not recruited in two study waves from October 2015
report all measures, because Study 1 was part of the until December 2016. Eleven participants dropped
SMASH study, which examined the effectiveness of a out during the study and one participant was
mindfulness intervention in influencing mindfulness, excluded due to less than 33% completed ambulatory
self-control, and affective processes (Rowland et al., assessments. The resulting 125 participants (77.6%
2016). Study 2 was part of a larger project on affect female; M = 22.9 years, SD = 5.1) took part in seven
dynamics and regulation in romantic dyads. weekly laboratory sessions with a six-week experience
sampling and six signals per day. For more information
on the inclusion criteria, design, and compensation,
Preregistration please refer to the study protocol (Rowland et al.,
Study 1 was registered with clinicalTrials.gov 2016).
(NCT02647801) and a study protocol has been pub-
lished (Rowland et al., 2016). Although Study 2 has Procedure
been preregistered (https://osf.io/957ew/) we did not In the first laboratory session, participants first signed
preregister the specific research question examined an informed consent form, provided sociodemo-
in the present research. graphic information, and completed several trait ques-
tionnaires which are detailed in the study protocol
(Rowland et al., 2016). Starting the next day and for
Data, materials, and online resources
40 subsequent days, participants received six ran-
All data, statistical analyses, and the full results can be domly distributed signals per day between 10 am
found at OSF (https://osf.io/e6ctr/). and 8 pm via the movisensXS experience sampling
4 M. WENZEL ET AL.

application (movisens GmbH, Karlsruhe, Germany), (very much), with higher values indicating higher
with the condition that two signals were always 45– levels of negative affect. To examine the reliability of
200 min apart (M = 103.4 min, SD = 34.3). Each signal the measures, we opted against Cronbach’s alpha
presented the participants with questionnaires due to the hierarchical nature of the data and report
regarding their momentary emotions, use of the between-person reliabilities (RKRN) and the
emotion regulation strategies, mindfulness as well as within-person reliabilities for changes from time
self-control related items such as desire and resist- point to time point (RCN; Shrout & Lane, 2012). The
ance. We had the participants return weekly to the lab- reliabilities and other descriptive statistics of negative
oratory in order to, on the one hand, keep adherence affect and the other measures are illustrated in
high and have the chance to personally talk about any Table 1, with both very good between-and within-
issues regarding the study and training that might person reliabilities for all measures.
have arisen during the week. On the other hand, the
weekly laboratory sessions involved the mindfulness Emotion regulation strategies. Participants in Study 1
training where one part of the participants performed rated the following emotion regulation strategies on a
a computer-based guided-breathing meditation for visual analogue scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6
five weekly sessions (mindfulness condition) whereas (almost all the time): reappraisal, suppression, distrac-
the other had the opportunity to perform these ses- tion, social sharing, rumination (Koval et al., 2015).
sions after study (control condition). With, on Each item began with “since the last beep, have
average, 75.9% completed ambulatory signals, com- you” (German: “Seit dem letzten Signal haben Sie”),
pliance to the study protocol was good. followed by “viewed the cause of your feelings from
a different perspective?” (cognitive reappraisal;
Training German: “den Grund für Ihre Gefühle aus einer
Participants in the mindfulness condition practiced anderen Perspektive betrachtet”), “talked about your
mindfulness in five weekly laboratory sessions using feelings with others” (social sharing; German: “mit
a computer-based guided breathing meditation for anderen über Ihre Gefühle gesprochen”), “suppressed
approximately 12 min each session (Levinson et al., the expression of your feelings” (expressive suppres-
2014). The participants were first introduced to the sion; German: “den Ausdruck Ihrer Gefühle unter-
meditation exercise by a research assistant that drückt”), “distracted your attention away from your
instructed them to continuously count their breath feelings” (distraction; German: “Ihre Aufmerksamkeit
from 1 to 9 on the computer by pressing a specific von Ihren Gefühlen abgelenkt”), and “brooded about
key on the keyboard for breath one to eight, and something in the past/future” (rumination; German:
another key for breath nine. In case of errors, partici- „über etwas aus der Vergangenheit/ Zukunft gegrü-
pants heard a sound to remind them to nonjudgmen- belt”). In addition to the individual emotion regulation
tally revert their attention back to their breath. strategies, we computed the mean across the strat-
Concluding the computer task, the participants egies to obtain a measure of mean strategy
could talk with the research assistant, who was endorsement.
present during the task, about their difficulties and
experiences during the meditation task. This compu- Mindfulness. Momentary mindfulness was measured
ter task was accompanied by provided breathing using a modified state version of the Mindfulness
meditation and body scan material on movisensXS, Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan,
which 45 out of 68 participants took use of (M = 4.2 2003; Study 4). To reduce participants’ burden due
times). Finally, the training significantly improved to the relatively large number of items, we chose
state and trait mindfulness as well as performance in item 8 (“I rush through activities without being really
the breath counting task (Rowland et al., 2019). attentive to them”.), 10 (“I do jobs or tasks automatically,
without being aware of what I’m doing”.), and 14 (“I find
Measures myself doing things without paying attention”.) of the
Negative affect. Based on (Kuppens et al., 2010), state version of the MAAS which showed the highest
momentary negative affect was assessed by comput- factor loadings. These three items were assessed on
ing the mean score of the four items anxious, angry, a 7-point scale Likert-type ranging from 0 (not at all),
depressed, and sad. The items were rated on a visual 3 (somewhat), to 6 (very much). Given that the items
analogue scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 100 assess lack of mindfulness, we inverted the items
COGNITION AND EMOTION 5

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, range, and reliabilities of the variables in study 1.
Multilevel
Actual range reliability Variance Stationarity
Dependent variables M SD Min Max RKRN RCN Within-person Between-person punitroot
Awareness (MAAS) 4.78 1.31 0 6 .98 .86 55.7% 44.3% < .001
Reappraisal 0.91 1.34 0 6 – – 63.7% 36.3% < .001
Suppression 1.20 1.59 0 6 – – 63.5% 36.5% < .001
Distraction 1.30 1.64 0 6 – – 61.4% 38.6% < .001
Social Sharing 1.01 1.50 0 6 – – 73.1% 26.9% < .001
Rumination 1.77 1.84 0 6 – – 55.7% 44.3% < .001
NA 15.83 17.80 0 100 .91 .64 56.4% 47.4% < .001
Notes: MAAS = Mindfulness Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; NA = negative affect; punitroot = the p-value of the inverse normal Z-statistic of the
augmented Dickey-Fuller tests to examine the existence of a unit root. A non-significant test statistic indicates that the data contain a random
walk with a trend and are, thus, not stationary. N = 125.

and then computed the mean score, such that higher examined the mean of the emotion regulation strat-
scores indicated higher levels of present-moment egies and all five emotion regulation strategies separ-
awareness since the last assessment. Given that we ately. To control the familywise error rate, we applied
used a multifaceted instrument to assess mindfulness Šidák-correction for the five comparisons regarding
in Study 2, which differentiates between present the single strategy. In the following analyses, momen-
moment attention, acting with awareness and non- tary refers to the assessment of a state at a given
judgmental acceptance, we will refer to the unidimen- moment t, prior to the laggedt−1 momentary variable
sional MAAS as acting with awareness (awareness). (value of a momentary variable during the previous
Acting with awareness is also a facet of the most ambulatory assessment t−1), and recent to the interval
popular trait mindfulness questionnaire Five Facet between the current assessment t and the prior
Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2008) assessment t−1.
and is highly correlated with the MAAS (r = .89; Baer
et al., 2008). This not surprising given that the MAAS Decreased emotion regulation efforts (Monitor-and-
items we selected have been used in modified form Acceptance Theory). To examine how awareness was
to assess awareness in the FFMQ and in the Multidi- associated with the use of emotion regulation strat-
mensional State Mindfulness Questionnaire (Blanke egies, we computed mixed regressions predicting
& Brose, 2017) used in Study 2. the five within-person standardised strategies. In the
unadjusted analyses, we predicted the intensity of
Analytic approach. To capture the hierarchical nature the implementation of a strategy (Y) by entering the
of our data, where observations (level 1) are nested laggedt−1 within-person standardised awareness
within individuals (level 2), we computed mixed (PriorAwa) in the fixed part of the mixed regression.
regressions in Stata 15 (Stata Corporation, College In the random part, we included the random partici-
Station, TX) using the mixed command with the pants intercepts (P) and a random slope for prior
restricted maximum likelihood option. To examine awareness which allows prior awareness to vary
within-person associations, continuous Level 1 vari- between participants. The within-person model is rep-
ables were within-person standardised, such that resented by Equation 1, the between-person model by
each score of an observation of a given variable was Equation 2. Please note that we did not include the
subtracted by the person mean of that variable and autocorrelation of the strategy given that this regulat-
then divided by the person standard deviation of ory effort could had been linked to another event and,
that variable. We chose within-person standardisation thus, change in the outcome would had been more
over within-person centreing (Enders & Tofighi, 2007) difficult to interpret.
since it allows for easier interpretation of the effect
sizes and is associated with less bias when between- Yij = b00 + mP0j + (b10 + mPxPriorAwa
1j )PriorAwaij
person variance exists (Wang et al., 2019). To + 1ij (1)
examine between-person associations, we aggregated
the data on the individual and used z-standardisation
to obtain standardised effect sizes. In all analyses, we Yi = b0 + b1 Awa + b2 Groupi + 1i (2)
6 M. WENZEL ET AL.

Additionally, we investigated the impact of the (Equation 6).


brief mindfulness training on emotion regulation strat-
Yijk =b000 + mP0k + mT0jk + b100 Group1k + b200 Time2jk
egy implementation by regressing each z-standar-
dised strategy on the two-way interaction between + b300 Group × Time3jk + (b400 + mPxPERS
1k )ERSijk
group (0 = control condition, 1 = mindfulness training) + b500 Group × ERSijk + b600 Time × ERSijk
and time (0 = first week, 1 = last week) in a three-level
+ b700 Group × Time × ERSijk + 1ijk
model where observations were nested in time (T) and
time nested in participants (P; Equation 3). (6)
Alternative explanation for decreased emotion regu-
Yijk = b000 + mP0k + mT0jk + b100 Group1k lation efforts. One might argue that mindfulness is
possibly negatively associated with emotion regu-
+ b200 Time2jk + b300 Groupx Time3jk + 1ijk (3)
lation strategy use due to the, to some extent, exclu-
sive employment of emotion regulation strategies, in
Increased emotion regulation effectiveness (Mindful- that individuals only use one strategy at a time. The
ness-to-Meaning Theory). To test the assumption idea here is that mindfulness could be seen as
derived from the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory, another emotion regulation strategy that participants
we investigated whether awareness could moderate employ instead of the other strategies, which would
the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies in explain a possible negative relation between mindful-
down-regulating negative affect. For the within- ness and emotion regulation strategy use. To test this
person level, we modelled momentary negative alternative explanation, we computed mixed
affect (Y) by computing mixed regressions with regressions with random participants intercepts and
random participants intercepts (P) and entered the entered the fixed effects and random slopes of the
two-way interaction between within-person standar- strategies to predict recent emotion regulation strat-
dised prior awareness (PriorAwa) and recent emotion egy use (Y). For example, Equation 7 represents the
regulation strategy use (ERS) along with the main model for reappraisal use.
effects and random slopes, controlling for prior nega-
tive affect (PriorNA; Equation 4). Yij =b00 + mP0j

+ (b10 + mPxPrior
1j
Suppression
)Prior Suppressionij
Yij = b00 + mP0j + (b10 + mPxPrior
1j
Awa
)Prior Awaij
+ (b20 + mPxPrior
2j
Distraction
)Prior Distractionij
+ (b20 + mPxERS
2j )ERSij
+ (b30 + mPxSocial Sharing
)Prior Social Sharingij
+ (b30 + mPxPrior
3j
Awax ERS
)Prior Awax ERSij 3j

+ (b40 )Prior NAij + 1ij (4) + (b40 + mPxPrior


4j
Rumination
)Prior Ruminationij

+ (b50 + mPxPrior
5j
Awa
)Prior Awaij + +1ij
For the between-person, we again aggregated the (7)
data on the individual level and regressed z-standar-
dised negative affect on the two-way interaction
between z-standardised awareness and emotion regu-
lation strategy use, controlling for the mindfulness
Results
training (Group; Equation 5).
Within- and between-person variance in emotion
Yi = b0 + b1 Awai + b2 ERSi + b3 Awax ERSi regulation strategies
We first investigated how much within-person var-
+ b4 Groupi + 1i (5) iance was in the use of the five emotion regulation
strategies. To that end, we computed a random-inter-
For the effect of the mindfulness training on strategy cept mixed regression on the unstandardised strat-
effectiveness, we regressed within-person negative egies which estimated the person mean (fixed
affect on the three-way interaction between mindful- intercept) of a strategy, the between-person variance
ness training, time and recent emotion regulation describing the variation of the intercept between par-
strategy use, controlling for prior negative affect ticipants (random intercept), and the within-person
COGNITION AND EMOTION 7

variance not explained by the between-person differ- more aware of their thoughts and behaviour than
ences (residual error). other individuals.
The results, as indicated in Table 1, show substan- Additionally, we investigated how the mindfulness
tial within-person variance. This indicates, on the one training impacted emotion regulation strategy use
hand, the presence of relatively large individual over the course of Study 1 (Table S2). To that end,
differences when using emotion regulation strategies, we regressed the z-standardised strategy, aggregated
with between-person variances around 40%. On the on the time level (that is aggregated for the first and
other hand, the large within-person variance around last week of the assessment), on the two-way inter-
60% hints at possible situational moderators, such action between mindfulness training and time (base-
as awareness, that could explain the differences in line vs. end of training) to see whether participants
emotion regulation strategy implementation in the mindfulness training reported to use emotion
intensity. regulation strategies less intensely over the course of
the study compared to participants in the control con-
dition. As illustrated in Figure 1(a), these models
Decreased emotion regulation efforts (Monitor- revealed that participants in the mindfulness training
and-Acceptance Theory) condition did not significantly use emotion regulation
As shown in Figure 1(a) (and Table S1 in the online strategies less intensely over the course of Study 1.
supplement, https://osf.io/e6ctr/), prior awareness
was significantly associated with less employment of
all five emotion regulation strategies subsequently, Increased emotion regulation effectiveness
both on the within and between person. The negative (Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory)
effect sizes were largest for suppression and distrac- With regard to whether mindfulness affects the effec-
tion as well as for the mean of all five strategies. tiveness of emotion regulation strategies, as derived
Thus, individuals reported to implement emotion from the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory for reapprai-
regulation strategies less intensely when they were sal and from biological research focusing on a top-
more aware of their thoughts and behaviour before- down regulatory perspective, the results illustrated
hand than usual and when individuals were generally in Figure 1(b) (and Tables S3 and S4 in the online

Figure 1. (a) The coefficient plot shows the standardised regression coefficients (the dots) and their 95% CIs (the whiskers) in Study 1 for the
association between emotion regulation strategy intensity and either prior levels of the mindfulness facet acting with awareness (within-person
level), the person mean of acting with awareness (between-person level), or the interaction between the mindfulness training and time (first vs.
last week). (b) The coefficient plot shows the standardised regression coefficients and their 95% CIs for the association between negative affect
and the two-way interaction between the mindfulness facet acting with awareness and emotion regulation strategy intensity (within- and
between-person level) or between negative affect and the three-way interaction between the mindfulness training, time (first vs. last week),
and emotion regulation strategy intensity.
Note: Mean = mean intensity of all emotion regulation strategies; REA = reappraisal; SUP = suppression; DIS = distraction; SOC = social sharing; RUM = rumination.
8 M. WENZEL ET AL.

Table 2. Fixed-effects estimates of recent emotion regulation strategies as a function of prior emotion regulation strategies.
Outcome (right)/Predictor (below) βreapraissal (SE) βsuppression (SE) βdistraction (SE) βsocial_sharing (SE) βrumination (SE)
Study 1
Prior reappraisal – .02* (.01) .05*** (.01) .05*** (.01) .04** (.01)
Prior suppression .04*** (.01) – .18*** (.02) .01 (.01) .04*** (.01)
Prior distraction .03** (.01) .18*** (.02) – .02 (.01) .06*** (.01)
Prior social sharing .07*** (.01) .01 (.01) .02* (.01) – .06*** (.01)
Prior rumination .03* (.01) .05*** (.01) .06*** (.01) .06*** (.01) –
Study 2
Prior reappraisal – .02 (.02) .07*** (.02) .08*** (.02) .02 (.02)
Prior suppression <.01 (.02) – .09*** (.02) <.01 (.01) .06*** (.02)
Prior distraction .06*** (.02) .05** (.02) – .04* (.02) .06** (.02)
Prior social sharing .08*** (.02) .02 (.01) .02 (.02) – <.01 (.01)
Prior rumination .03* (.02) .09*** (.02) .05** (.02) .03 (.02) –
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

supplement, https://osf.io/e6ctr/) demonstrated that Mediation analysis: can emotion regulation


prior awareness did not significantly moderate the efforts explain the relationship between
effectiveness of any of the five strategies in reducing awareness and NA?
negative affect, with effect sizes close to zero both The goal of both the Monitor-and-Acceptance Theory
on the within- and between-person level. Thus, it and the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory is to explain
can be assumed that awareness is not associated how mindfulness is related to well-being and health.
with the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies So far, we found evidence for the link between aware-
in daily life. ness and less emotion regulation use. In the next step,
Regarding the mindfulness training, the three-way we wanted to examine whether less emotion regu-
interaction between the training, time, and each strat- lation use can explain the association between aware-
egy did not reveal any significant differences in nega- ness and negative affect by computing mediation
tive affect. Thus, participants in the mindfulness models. We first established whether prior levels of
condition did not report more effective emotion regu- awareness and subsequent levels of negative affect
lations efforts over the course of the training com- were significantly associated in our data, which was
pared to the control condition. the case for both the within-person, β = −.13, SE
= .01, p < .001, 95% CI [−.16, −.10], and between-
person level, β = −.58, SE = .07, p < .001, 95% CI
[−.73, −.44].
Alternative explanation for decreased emotion Given the established link between mindfulness
regulation efforts and well-being in our data, we computed the
As illustrated in Table 2, all laggedt−1 strategy mediation model, which is illustrated in Figure 2. We
measures were positively and mostly significantly bootstrapped the indirect effect via mean strategy
associated with subsequent ERS use. Thus, only prior use with n = 1000 repetitions, which indicated that
levels of awareness but not of other strategies were 25% / 32% (within-/between-person) of the total
negatively associated with subsequent strategy use. effect of β = −.13/−.58 was mediated by mean

Figure 2. Standardised regression coefficients on the within- / between-person level in Study 1 for the relationship between the mindfulness
facet acting with awareness (X) and negative affect (Y) as mediated by the averaged intensity of implemented emotion regulation strategies (M).
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
COGNITION AND EMOTION 9

strategy use, β = −.03, SE = .003, 95% CIbca [−.04, −.02] assess several trait questionnaires. After completing
and β = −.18, SE = .09, 95% CIbca [−.39, −.05]. The the questionnaires, participants scheduled the first lab-
direct effects were still significant on both levels, oratory session via an online calendar. After signing an
hinting at other possible mediators. informed consent form in the first lab session, partici-
pants were explained how to use the movisensXS
experience sampling application (movisens GmbH,
Discussion Karlsruhe, Germany). Starting the next day and for 7
To summarise, Study 1 provides evidence for the subsequent days, participants received signal-triggered
hypothesis derived from the Monitor-and-Acceptance prompts to complete self-reports on their momentary
Theory, such that awareness was associated with less emotions, emotion regulation strategies, and mindful-
emotion regulation efforts both within and between ness. Twelve signals were distributed within 12 h with
individuals. Moreover, emotion regulation efforts and two consecutive signals required to be at least 30 min
stress exposure could explain considerably (around apart (M = 58.3 min, SD = 20.6). The couples could
40% for both levels) the negative link between aware- choose the start time within a day, either 9, 10, or 11
ness and negative affect. In turn, the hypothesis am, which set the end time to 9, 10, or 11 pm, respect-
derived from the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory ively. For each day and for each start condition, the
that awareness improves the effectiveness of times were randomly generated and, then, presented
emotion regulation strategies, especially of reapprai- at fixed times to ensure that couples received the
sal, could not be confirmed in Study 1. To replicate signals at the same time (https://osf.io/957ew/). Compli-
these results and better guard it against the possibility ance to the study protocol was acceptable with 67.2%
of reporting a false positive, we re-analyzed another completed ambulatory signals.
ambulatory assessment dataset in Study 2. This
dataset not only included the awareness facet of
mindfulness but also present-moment attention and, Measures
importantly, the non-judgmental acceptance facet, Affect. Momentary negative affect was assessed as in
which enables us to more directly examine the impor- Study 1 but on a scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to
tance of acceptance in the interplay between mindful- 6 (very much).
ness, emotion regulation, and affect.
Emotion regulation strategies. The emotion regu-
lation strategies in Study 2 were the same as in
Study 2 Study 1 but were assessed with different items
Method based on Pavani et al. (2017). Each item, ranging
from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much), began with
Participants and design “since the last signal or since awakening” (German:
The parent study preregistered to stop collecting data “Seit dem letzten Signal/ seit dem Aufwachen”), fol-
after 90 romantic dyads were enrolled, which was lowed by “I have seen what positive things a negative
slightly exceeded by 2 dyads due to scheduling event can bring me” (positive reappraisal; German:
issues. Participating couples, of which at least one par- “habe ich gesehen, welche positiven Dinge ein nega-
ticipant within a couple was an undergraduate tives Ereignis mit sich bringen kann”), “I have talked
student, could choose between receiving five hours about my feelings with others” (social sharing;
of course credit or 50 € (app. US$ 57). One couple German: “habe ich mit anderen über meine Gefühle
dropped out after the first day and seven participants gesprochen”), “I have tried not to show or express
were excluded due to responding to less than 33% of my emotions” (expressive suppression; German:
the signals. Thus, the final sample comprised 175 par- “habe ich versucht, meine Emotionen nicht zu
ticipants (52.0% female; M = 25.0 years, SD = 5.4). zeigen oder auszudrücken”.), “I have diverted my
Please note that participants of Study 1 were not eli- attention away from my problems” (distraction;
gible for Study 2. German: “habe ich meine Aufmerksamkeit von
meinen Problemen weggeleitet”), and “I have wasted
Procedure time thinking of a past event again and again” (rumi-
If interested, individuals received an e-mail, which con- nation; German: „habe ich meine Zeit damit vergeu-
tained information about the study and a weblink to det, immer wieder über ein Ereignis zu grübeln”).
10 M. WENZEL ET AL.

Table 3. Means, standard deviations, range, and reliabilities of the variables in study 2.
Multilevel
Actual range reliability Variance Stationarity
Dependent variables M SD Min Max RKRN RCN Within-person Between-person Between-dyad punitroot
Awareness (MSMQ) 4.06 1.15 0.89 5.98 .96 .91 35.7% 51.7% 12.6% < .001
Attention (MSMQ) 3.75 0.85 1.45 5.87 .95 .84 54.3% 42.7% 3.0% < .001
Acceptance (MSMQ) 4.96 0.94 1.49 6 .96 .78 25.5% 53.3% 21.1% < .001
Reappraisal 0.88 0.89 0 3.39 – – 51.0% 37.0% 12.0% < .001
Suppression 0.95 0.89 0 3.75 – – 47.2% 39.1% 13.7% < .001
Distraction 1.29 1.10 0 3.93 – – 43.2% 44.6% 12.2% < .001
Social Sharing 1.50 1.11 0 5.30 – – 54.3% 29.2% 16.5% < .001
Rumination 0.95 0.99 0 3.92 – – 44.1% 44.9% 11.0% < .001
NA 0.60 0.64 0 2.85 .91 .50 30.3% 46.5% 23.2% < .001
Notes: MSMQ = Multidimensional State Mindfulness Questionnaire; NA = negative affect; punitroot = the p-value of the inverse normal Z-statistic of
the augmented Dickey-Fuller tests to examine the existence of a unit root. A non-significant test statistic indicates that the data contain a
random walk with a trend and are, thus, not stationary. N = 175.

Mindfulness. Mindfulness was captured via the Multidi- Decreased emotion regulation efforts (Monitor-
mensional State Mindfulness Questionnaire (Blanke & and-Acceptance Theory)
Brose, 2017), which consists of three subscales with Figure 3(a) (and Table S5 and S6 in the online sup-
three items each: present-moment attention (atten- plement, https://osf.io/e6ctr/) shows the results for the
tion), acting with awareness (awareness), and non- bottom-up approach: In the unadjusted analyses,
judgmental acceptance (acceptance). Participants awareness was significantly associated with lesser inten-
were asked to report how they have felt, behaved or sity of strategy implementation both within-person and
what they have thought since the last prompt/ since between-person, replicating the results reported in
waking up by answering all items on a scale from 0 Study 1. However, the significant associations greatly
(does not apply at all) to 6 (applies strongly). reduced in size and were no longer significant in the
adjusted analyses when the mindfulness facets were
Analytic approach. The analytical approach was entered simultaneously. Instead, only acceptance was
largely the same as in Study 1. To respect the dyadic significantly associated with strategy use, both on the
structure of the data, where observations (level 1) within- and between-person level. Thus, individuals
were nested within individuals (level 2), and addition- who accept their thoughts and feelings do not only
ally, nested in dyads (level 3), we computed three-level reported less intense regulation of their emotions
mixed models for the within-person processes using than other individuals but also in the very next
the within-person standardised variables. For the moment. These results highlight the importance of
between-person processes, we computed two-level the acceptance facet of mindfulness for emotion regu-
models (participants and dyads) using the z-standar- lation and provides further evidence for the multi-fac-
dised aggregated variables. To improve readability of torial structure of mindfulness that should be reflected
the result section, we only present the results for by using mindfulness questionnaires that respect this
mean strategy use. The results for each emotion regu- structure.
lation strategy as well as the full results, the analysis
plan, and the data can be found in the online sup- Increased emotion regulation effectiveness
plement at (https://osf.io/dp5gv/). (Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory)
The results for the top-down approach mirrored the
results in Study 1, with none of the mindfulness
Results
facets significantly moderating the mean emotion
Within- and between-person variance in emotion regulation effectiveness (Figure 3(b); Table S7 and S8).
regulation strategies The only exception was attention in the between-
Again, there was substantial within- and between- person analyses (Table S8 in the online supplement,
person variance in the relevant variables, as shown in https://osf.io/e6ctr/) which showed that mean
Table 3. Thus, the data is well suited to examine the emotion regulation endorsement was only significantly
relationships on both the within- and between-person associated with negative affect in participants who
level. reported less present-moment attention, β = .40, SE
COGNITION AND EMOTION 11

Figure 3. (a) The coefficient plot shows the standardised regression coefficients (the dots) and their 95% CIs (the whiskers) in Study 2 for the
association between mean intensity of all emotion regulation strategies (Mean ERS) and the three mindfulness facets acting with awareness,
present-moment attention, and non-judgmental acceptance, both on the within-person (wp) and between-person level (bp). (b) The coefficient
plot shows the standardised regression coefficients and their 95% CIs for the association between negative affect and the two-way interaction
between the three mindfulness facets and mean intensity of all emotion regulation strategies. The mindfulness facets were entered separately
(unadjusted) and simultaneously (adjusted).

= .10, p < .001, 95% CI [.20, .61], in comparison to those emotions, and behaviours is associated with a reduction
who reported more attention on the present moment, of subsequent emotion regulation efforts which cannot
β = .03, SE = .11, p = .751, 95% CI [−.18, .25]. All other be interpreted as the result of exclusive emotion regu-
emotion regulation strategies were not significant. lation strategy employment.

Alternative explanation for decreased emotion Mediation analysis: can emotion regulation
regulation efforts efforts explain the relationship between
As in Study 1, strategy use at the previous observation acceptance and NA?
did not show negative association with subsequent As in Study 1, awareness was significantly associated
strategy use as did mindfulness. Instead the associations with negative affect, β = −.06, SE = .02, p < .001, 95%
were positive, indicating that prior effortful emotion CI [−.09, −.03] within-person and β = −.31, SE = .07,
regulation endorsement was significantly associated p < .001, 95% CI [−.44, −.18] between-person.
with increased regulation efforts subsequently. Consid- However, these associations became non-significant
ering all results, Study 2 provides further evidence that in the adjusted analyses when all three mindfulness
only mindful acceptance of one’s own thoughts, facets were included, β = −.01, SE = .01, p = .332, 95%

Figure 4. Standardised regression coefficients on the within- / between-person level in Study 2 for the relationship between the mindfulness
facet non-judgmental acceptance (X) and negative affect (Y) as mediated by the averaged intensity of implemented emotion regulation strat-
egies (M).
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
12 M. WENZEL ET AL.

CI [−.04, .01] within-person and β = .11, SE = .07, turn, frees the individual to do affectively more ben-
p = .113, 95% CI [−.03, .25] between-person. Instead, eficial things than endorsing effortful emotion
acceptance was the strongest predictor of negative regulation.
affect, both within individuals, β = −.12, SE = .02, p Based on these results, we confirm the assumption
< .001, 95% CI [−.15, −.09], and between individuals, that mindfulness is associated with bottom–up reac-
β = −.68, SE = .06, p < .001, 95% CI [−.81, −.56]. We, tivity rather than top-down regulatory processes in
thus, focused on the acceptance facet of mindfulness relatively meditation-naïve individuals (Chiesa et al.,
in the following mediation analyses. 2010) and provide direct support for the Monitor-
To test whether participants’ regulatory efforts and-Acceptance Theory (Creswell et al., 2019) but
would mediate the relation between acceptance and not for the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory (Garland
negative affect, we again conducted mediation ana- et al., 2015). The only exception was social sharing,
lyses with bootstrapped indirect effects (n = 1,000 which did not show significant within-person associ-
resamples). As Figure 4 illustrates, mean emotion regu- ations with prior mindfulness and was not significantly
lation was associated with both acceptance and nega- affected by the mindfulness training. One possibility is
tive affect, resulting in an indirect effect of β = −.025, that mindfulness may have improved the perceived
SE = .003, 95% CIbca [−.03, −.02] that mediated 22.0% quality of interpersonal relationships and communi-
of the total within-person association and in an indirect cation (e.g. Barnes et al., 2007; Pratscher et al., 2018),
effect of β = −.18, SE = .09, 95% CIbca [−.38, −.05] that which may have cancelled out the reduction of
mediated 25.6% of the total between-person associ- social sharing, in that individuals still like to share
ation between acceptance and negative affect. thoughts and emotions with their friends.

General discussion Theoretical implications


This study is the first to compare two notions of the Our results demonstrate that non-judgmental accep-
relationship between mindfulness and emotion regu- tance may be a key mechanism that broadens one’s
lation in daily life derived from theoretical and neuro- awareness of affective cues so that positive or nega-
biological accounts of mindfulness. We did not find tive stimuli may be monitored without applying
evidence for a top-down approach compatible with effortful control in the form of emotion regulation,
the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory where mindful- as only the non-judgmental facet of the MSMQ was
ness reliably moderates the effectiveness of emotion significantly associated with less emotion regulation
regulation strategies, evidenced by very small and use but not the attention or the awareness facet
mostly non-significant effect sizes for the interactions when all three mindfulness facets were entered simul-
between the mindfulness facets and emotion regu- taneously. These results are in line with recent
lation strategies on negative and positive affect as research demonstrating that removing the practice
well as very small within-person variations in these of acceptance skills from mindfulness trainings signifi-
interactions. Instead, mindfulness was significantly cantly reduced the beneficial effects on positive affect
associated with less use of emotion regulation, provid- (Lindsay et al., 2018) and significantly attenuated
ing evidence for a bottom-up process compatible with stress-reduction benefits (Chin et al., 2019). With
the Monitor-and-Acceptance Theory: Participants who regard to the Monitor-and-Acceptance Theory, the
reported higher levels of mindfulness, especially of results of Study 2 demonstrate that only nonjudgmen-
non-judgmental acceptance, than usual or than tal acceptance was significantly associated with
others also reported to subsequently use less affective benefits, which represents the acceptance
emotion regulation strategies than usual or than factor of the Monitor-and-Acceptance Theory,
others. In addition, we found that emotion regulation whereas attention and awareness, which both reflect
use significantly mediated the negative relation the monitoring factor, were not significantly associ-
between mindfulness, especially acceptance, and ated. Thus, our results highlight the importance of
negative affect, indicating that mindful individuals the acceptance factor of the Monitor-and-Acceptance
feel better due to better acceptance of their own, Theory, demonstrating that mindfulness trainings
sometimes undesirable, thoughts and behaviours. should not only focus on formal practices of focusing
Thus, accepting one’s own thoughts and emotions on the present moment but should also incorporate
reduces the need to actively change them which, in components on facilitating one’s own perceptions of
COGNITION AND EMOTION 13

present-moment experiences with an open and that the result are robust regarding different operatio-
accepting stance (e.g. Lindsay et al., 2018). nalizations of the emotion regulation strategies and
We also tested the alternative explanation that not due to specific wordings of the items.
mindfulness is another emotion regulation strategy Given the investigation period of six weeks in
and that the negative associations are driven by the, Study 1 and the frequent ambulatory assessment
to some extent, exclusive employment of emotion with 12 signals per day in Study 2, each emotion regu-
regulation strategies. First, empirically, this was not lation strategy was assessed with only a single item in
the case, as prior levels of strategy use were positively order to decrease the burden for the participants. The
instead of negatively associated with subsequent downside is that this limits the reliability of the
levels of strategy use, unlike mindfulness. Second, emotion regulation strategy assessment. However,
we think that mindfulness differs in regard to given that emotion regulation strategies are compara-
common emotion regulation strategies, in that the tively narrow concepts, using single items provides a
five strategies investigated in the present research good trade-off between reliability and space con-
are conceptualised to be used situationally, e.g. straints, two hardly compatible issues ambulatory
when confronted with negative affect in a situation assessment studies often have to deal with (Brans
without much control, individuals might employ sup- et al., 2013; Koval et al., 2015).
pression to temporarily deal with the unpleasant Finally, we need to highlight our concerns about
experience. Given the accepting and non-judgmental generalizability of results with regard to the general
focus on the present moment (Bishop et al., 2004), population given that we mostly recruited female
mindfulness is better characterised as a general state undergraduate students with 18 and 30 years. The
and embodied ethical stance (Grossman, 2015) that sample of Study 2 included a higher share of the
reduces affective reactivity rather than an emotion male and academic population, but still could not be
regulation strategy that is only employed on demand. considered as representative of the general
Study 1 also included a brief mindfulness training population.
over the course of the six weeks of assessment.
However, we did not find evidence that practicing
mindfulness led to less emotion regulation, which Conclusion
might be due to the relative briefness and the focus
To sum up, our findings support the view that mindful-
of the mindfulness training on the awareness facet
ness, especially its non-judgmental acceptance facet,
of mindfulness. Thus, future research might use a
is a bottom-up process that is negatively associated
more elaborated training that focuses on acceptance
with the use of several emotion regulation strategies,
to increase the likelihood of detecting meaningful
suggesting that acceptance is associated with less
effect sizes with regard to strategy use.
need or motivation to regulate emotions. These
results highlight the value of ambulatory assessment
Limitations as a complementary tool to laboratory research to
study the relation between mindfulness and
This study has several limitations to discuss. Firstly, we
emotion regulation.
used different operationalizations and different word-
ings in both data sets to capture the emotion regu-
lation strategies. Whereas Study 1 assessed cognitive Acknowledgements
reappraisal, focusing on viewing the cause of one’s
feelings from a different perspective in order to We would like to thank Kimberly Holtz, Tobias Kammann, Luisa
Sofie Ludwig, Markus Müssig, Maike Vogel, and Julius Welzel
change it, Study 1 assessed positive reappraisal, for the great help in collecting the data. The research presented
which focused on a positive perspective without in this article was supported by a scholarship of the Stipendien-
necessarily changing the cause of one’s feelings. As stiftung Rheinland-Pfalz awarded to Zarah Rowland, and by an
for rumination, the operationalisation in Study 2 only intramural grant of the Johannes Gutenberg University
referred to brooding about events in the past and awarded to Mario Wenzel.
not also in the future as in Study 1. On the one
hand, future research should avoid these issues by
using consistent operationalizations of emotion regu- Disclosure statement
lation strategies. On the other hand, our results show No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).
14 M. WENZEL ET AL.

Funding Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in


cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue.
This work was supported by Stipendienstiftung Rheinland-Pfalz; Psychological Methods, 12(2), 121–138. https://doi.org/10.
Internal University Research Funding. 1037/1082-989X.12.2.121
Garland, E. L., Farb, N. A., Goldin, P., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2015).
Mindfulness broadens awareness and builds eudaimonic
meaning: A process model of mindful positive emotion regu-
References lation. Psychological Inquiry, 26(4), 293–314. https://doi.org/10.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, 1080/1047840X.2015.1064294
S., Walsh, E., Duggan, D., & Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Construct Garland, E. L., Hanley, A. W., Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2017).
validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditat- Testing the mindfulness-to-meaning theory: Evidence for
ing and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329–342. mindful positive emotion regulation from a reanalysis of
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191107313003 longitudinal data. PLOS One, 12(12), e0187727. https://doi.
Barnes, S., Brown, K. W., Krusemark, E., Campbell, W. K., & Rogge, org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187727
R. D. (2007). The role of mindfulness in romantic relationship Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based
satisfaction and responses to relationship stress. Journal of stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social
Marital and Family Therapy, 33(4), 482–500. https://doi.org/ anxiety disorder. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(1), 83–91.
10.1111/j.1752-0606.2007.00033.x https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018441
Barnes, S. M., & Lynn, S. J. (2010). Mindfulness skills and Grant, J. A., Courtemanche, J., & Rainville, P. (2011). A non-ela-
depressive symptoms: A longitudinal study. Imagination, borative mental stance and decoupling of executive and
Cognition and Personality, 30(1), 77–91. https://doi.org/10. pain-related cortices predicts low pain sensitivity in Zen med-
2190/IC.30.1.e itators. Pain, 152(1), 150–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., 2010.10.006
Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Gratz, K. L., & Tull, M. T. (2010). The relationship between
Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational emotion dysregulation and deliberate self-harm among inpa-
definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), tients with substance use disorders. Cognitive Therapy and
230–241. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bph077 Research, 34(6), 544–553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-
Blanke, E. S., & Brose, A. (2017). Mindfulness in daily life: A multi- 009-9268-4
dimensional approach. Mindfulness, 8(3), 737–750. https://doi. Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future
org/10.1007/s12671-016-0651-4 prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/
Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Grossman, P. (2015). Mindfulness: Awareness informed by an
Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 13(5), 926–939. https://doi.org/ embodied ethic. Mindfulness, 6(1), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.
10.1037/a0032400 1007/s12671-014-0372-5
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindful-
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal ness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. https:// Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1041–1056. https://doi.org/
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006
Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion Khoury, B., Sharma, M., Rush, S. E., & Fournier, C. (2015).
regulation: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals:
29(6), 560–572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005 A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 78(6),
Chiesa, A., Brambilla, P., & Serretti, A. (2010). Functional neural 519–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.03.009
correlates of mindfulness meditations in comparison with Koval, P., Brose, A., Pe, M. L., Houben, M., Erbas, Y., Champagne,
psychotherapy, pharmacotherapy and placebo effect. Is D., & Kuppens, P. (2015). Emotional inertia and external
there a link? Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 22(3), 104–117. https:// events: The roles of exposure, reactivity, and recovery.
doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-5215.2010.00460.x Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 15(5), 625–636. https://doi.org/
Chin, B., Lindsay, E. K., Greco, C. M., Brown, K. W., Smyth, J. M., 10.1037/emo0000059
Wright, A. G. C., & Creswell, J. D. (2019). Psychological mechan- Kuppens, P., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L. B. (2010). Emotional inertia and
isms driving stress resilience in mindfulness training: A ran- psychological maladjustment. Psychological Science, 21(7), 984–
domized controlled trial. Health Psychology: Official Journal 991. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610372634
of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Levinson, D. B., Stoll, E. L., Kindy, S. D., Merry, H. L., & Davidson, R.
Association, https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000763 J. (2014). A mind you can count on: Validating breath counting
Coffey, K. A., Hartman, M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). as a behavioral measure of mindfulness. Frontiers in
Deconstructing mindfulness and constructing mental health: Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01202
Understanding mindfulness and its mechanisms of action. Lindsay, E. K., Chin, B., Greco, C. M., Young, S., Brown, K. W.,
Mindfulness, 1(4), 235–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671- Wright, A. G. C., Smyth, J. M., Burkett, D., & Creswell, J. D.
010-0033-2 (2018). How mindfulness training promotes positive
Creswell, J. D., Lindsay, E. K., Villalba, D. K., & Chin, B. (2019). emotions: Dismantling acceptance skills training in two ran-
Mindfulness training and physical health: mechanisms and domized controlled trials. Journal of Personality and Social
outcomes. Psychosomatic Medicine, 81(3), 224–232. https:// Psychology, 115(6), 944–973. https://doi.org/10.1037/
doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000675 pspa0000134
COGNITION AND EMOTION 15

Lindsay, E. K., & Creswell, J. D. (2017). Mechanisms of mindfulness Mindfulness, 9(4), 1206–1215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-
training: Monitor and acceptance theory (MAT). Clinical 017-0859-y
Psychology Review, 51, 48–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr. Rahl, H. A., Lindsay, E. K., Pacilio, L. E., Brown, K. W., & Creswell, J.
2016.10.011 D. (2017). Brief mindfulness meditation training reduces
Lindsay, E. K., Young, S., Brown, K. W., Smyth, J. M., & Creswell, J. D. mind-wandering: The critical role of acceptance. Emotion
(2019). Mindfulness training reduces loneliness and increases (Washington, D.C.), 17(2), 224–230. https://doi.org/10.1037/
social contact in a randomized controlled trial. Proceedings of emo0000250
the National Academy of Sciences, 116(9), 3488–3493. https:// Rowland, Z., Wenzel, M., & Kubiak, T. (2016). The effects of com-
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1813588116 puter-based mindfulness training on self-control and
Lomas, T., Medina, J. C., Ivtzan, I., Rupprecht, S., Hart, R., & Eiroa- mindfulness within ambulatorily assessed network
Orosa, F. J. (2017). The impact of mindfulness on well-being systems across health-related domains in a healthy student
and performance in the workplace: An inclusive systematic population (SMASH): Study protocol for a randomized con-
review of the empirical literature. European Journal of Work trolled trial. Trials, 17(1), 570. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-
and Organizational Psychology, 26(4), 492–513. https://doi. 016-1707-4
org/10.1080/1359432X.2017.1308924 Rowland, Z., Wenzel, M., & Kubiak, T. (2018). A mind full of happi-
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., Davidson, R. J., & ness: How mindfulness shapes affect dynamics in daily life.
Baune, B. (2008). Regulation of the neural circuitry of Emotion (Washington, D.C.). https://doi.org/10.1037/
emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative emo0000562
expertise. PLOS One, 3(3), e1897. https://doi.org/10.1371/ Rowland, Z., Wenzel, M., & Kubiak, T. (2019). Effects of an ultra-
journal.pone.0001897 brief computer-based mindfulness training on mindfulness
Pavani, J.-B., Vigouroux, S. L., Kop, J.-L., Congard, A., Dauvier, B., & and self-control: A randomised controlled trial using a 40-
Denissen, J. (2017). A network approach to affect regulation day ecological momentary assessment. Mindfulness, 10(11),
dynamics and personality trait-induced variations: 2312–2326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01204-9
Extraversion and neuroticism moderate reciprocal influences Shrout, P., & Lane, S. P. (2012). Psychometrics. In M. Mehl, & T. S.
between affect and affect regulation strategies. European Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily
Journal of Personality, 31(4), 329–346. https://doi.org/10. life (pp. 302–320). Guilford Press.
1002/per.2109 Wang, L., Zhang, Q., Maxwell, S. E., & Bergeman, C. S. (2019). On
Pearson, M. R., Lawless, A. K., Brown, D. B., & Bravo, A. J. (2015). Standardizing within-person effects: Potential problems of
Mindfulness and emotional outcomes: Identifying subgroups global standardization. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1–
of college students using latent profile analysis. Personality 22. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2018.1532280
and Individual Differences, 76, 33–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Wenzel, M., Rowland, Z., Zahn, D., Kubiak, T., & Carlson, E. (2019).
j.paid.2014.11.009 Let there Be variance: Individual differences in consecutive
Pratscher, S. D., Rose, A. J., Markovitz, L., & Bettencourt, A. (2018). self-control in a laboratory Setting and daily life. European
Interpersonal mindfulness: Investigating mindfulness in inter- Journal of Personality, 33(4), 468–487. https://doi.org/10.
personal interactions, co-rumination, and friendship quality. 1002/per.2208

You might also like