U4 - Serial vs. Parallel

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Serial vs.

Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but They
Can (And Should) be Distinguished
Author(s): James T. Townsend
Source: Psychological Science, Vol. 1, No. 1 (Jan., 1990), pp. 46-54
Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of the Association for Psychological Science
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40062391 .
Accessed: 15/06/2014 08:42

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Sage Publications, Inc. and Association for Psychological Science are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Psychological Science.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
SCIENCE
PSYCHOLOGICAL

ResearchArticle
SERIAL VS. PARALLEL
PROCESSING:
Sometimes They Look Like
Tweedledum and Tweedledee but They
Can (and Should) be Distinguished
JamesT. Townsend
Departmentof PsychologicalSciences, Purdue University

Abstract-A numberof importantmodels of informationpro- the world. It remainsa lively research topic today in diverse
cessing depend on whetherprocessing is serial or parallel. areas of pure and appliedcognitive psychology. The longevity
However,manyof the studiespurportingto settle the case use of the topic is probablydue to its fundamentalimportancein
weakexperimentalparadigmsor resultsto drawconclusions.A describinghow mentaloperationstake place.
briefhistoryof the issue is given along with examplesfrom the As intimatedabove, this topic is also closely connectedwith
literature.Thena numberof promisingmethodsare presented the issue of capacity;that is, to what extent mentalprocessing
from a varietyof sources with some discussionof theirpoten- of some type sufferswhen the numberof thingsto do mentally
tial. A briefdiscussionof the topic withregardto overallissues or the difficultyof the cognitive operationsincreases. For in-
of model testing and applicationsconcludes the paper. stance, standardserialprocessingwith each successive subtask
taking the same average durationis of limited capacity with
respect to the overall total processingtime requiredfor an in-
Somewhat informally,serial processing means strictly se- creasingnumberof subtasks.That is, the overall reactiontime
quential,withoutoverlapof the successive processingtimes on for all the subtasksincreases, the more tasks there are to do.
objectsor distinctsubsystems.In a standardtype of serialsys- However,the same serialprocessingis of unlimitedcapacityon
tem, each object takes the same average amount of time to individualitems in the sense that the average item processing
process and the next object begins processingonly when the durationper item is constantregardlessof the total numberof
previousone is completed.On the otherhand,parallelprocess- items to be done. Parallelprocessing can be either limitedor
ing signifiessimultaneousprocessingon severalobjectsor sub- unlimitedcapacityon eitherthe individualitem or on the whole
systems at the same time, althoughprocessingmay finish on set, the differencedependingon the type of parallelsystem in
differentobjects at differenttimes. In eithertype of operation, l
question(e.g., Townsend, 1974a,Townsend& Ashby, 1983).
both individualand overall processingtimes may be random. This paper will advocate the view that contemporaryre-
That is, the durationsrequiredfor processingan item or per- searchon the parallel-serialquestionoften uses methodologyor
formingan operationmay vary fromtrialto trial.This paperis logic that was shown to be faulty or at least precarioustwenty
abouttesting parallelismvs. seriality. or moreyearsago. Also, it is arguedthatfew investigationstake
The question as to whetherand when people can perform advantageof more powerfultechniques of testing the dichot-
perceptualor mental operationsin parallelbegan to receive omy that have been developed since 1968. Several pertinent
experimentaltreatmentin the late 19th century, althoughnot examples will be given from the literaturein perceptionand
underthese names. It was a naturalquestionfor the emerging cognition. In the following section, a number of promising
disciplineof psychologybecause it is inherentlyrelatedto the methodsthat can be mathematicallydemonstratedto separate
capacityof mind and how that capacityis allocatedto sundry large classes of parallelvs. serial models will be collatedfrom
cognitive and perceptualendeavors. PerhapsHamilton(1859) the literatureand explained.
was the first to attemptan empirical,if hardlyexperimental,
answerto the question. One of his techniqueswas to toss sev-
eral dice on his desk and try to assess "instantaneously"the HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE
numberof dots showing.The intentwas to determinethe num- A very brief, limitedsurveyof the recenthistoryof the issue
ber of objects that could be apprehendedsimultaneously(i.e.,
in parallel)by humanconsciousness. This interestreappeared 1. We are presentlyconcerned strictly with models that may be
in variousguises in the emergingpsychologicallaboratoriesof referredto as traditionalserialor parallelmodels. Introductionsto cer-
tain other types of models may be found in Meyer, Irwin, Osman&
Kounios(1988)and Ratcliff(1988)along with many references.More
Correspondenceand offprintrequeststo: JamesT. Townsend,De- technicalinformationon the traditionaland some of the more recent
partmentof Psychology,IndianaUniversity,Bloomington,IN 47405. modelscan be found in Luce (1986)and Townsend& Ashby (1983).

46 Copyright© 1990AmericanPsychologicalSociety VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

James T. Townsend

may help place the situation in perspective. It seems likely that Some of these models seemed unrealistic in that each subtask
the late 20th century revival of the serial vs. parallel issues was was processed deterministically, with zero time variance (e.g.,
'
connected with the 'bottleneck" conception of human infor- Corcoran, 1971). Murdock (1971) proposed a parallel model for
mation processing (e.g., Broadbent, 1958; Treisman, 1969). search experiments. Atkinson, Holmgren and Juola (1969) of-
Theoretical emphasis on seriality of item processing in both fered a more natural nondeterministic model that mimicked se-
perception and short-term memory search increased during the rial processing and the present author showed that each type of
early 1960s with the establishment of the information process- model could mimic the set size function of the other
ing paradigm and, in particular, with the investigations of Sper- (Townsend, 1971a, 1972). Townsend (1976) and Vorberg (1977)
ling (1960, 1963), Estes and Taylor (1964, 1966), and the later developed theorems exhibiting equivalence among wide classes
treatise by Neisser (1967). of parallel and serial models.
In a crucial paper, Sternberg (1966) persuasively interpreted Following the early 1970s there was a hiatus in empirical
short-term memory search reaction time data in terms of serial tests of parallel vs. serial processing, possibly due, to some
search and disconfirmed a significant class of parallel models.2 extent, to the ability of parallel and serial models to mimic one
The crux of Sternberg's demonstration was a strikingly linear another in the common experimental paradigms.
function relating reaction time to the number of items to be Nevertheless, the issue has refused to fade away. The case
searched. His work was instrumental in firmly wedding the con- may be put forth that how subtasks are worked on is an integral
cept of increasing straight line set size functions, to seriality of description of any real-time model and that other interesting
processing in the minds of cognitive scientists. In contradistinc- questions also hinge on the temporal nature of the processing.
tion, parallel processing has, then and now, often been sup- Townsend and Ashby (1983) reviewed the parallel-serial con-
posed to imply that reaction time remains constant as the men- troversy and presented a number of fundamental ways in which
tal load is increased. serial and parallel processing differ. The phenomenon of linear
Many writers still implicitly assume that parallelism does increasing reaction time curves is no longer considered a fun-
imply constancy of both reaction times and accuracy, as more damental parallel-serial distinction because it simply indicates
contemporary references below will show. However, as recog- first and foremost, a limitation in capacity. That limitation may
nized by Egeth (1966) in an early study of serial vs. parallel be due to seriality, limited capacity parallel, or even hybrid
processing of featural dimensions, even unlimited capacity par- processing mechanisms. On the other hand, under certain con-
allel processors can yield increasing mean reaction time func- ditions flat reaction time curves and sometimes even negatively
tion in a natural way. By "unlimited capacity" here, is meant accelerated curves can be strong indicants that processing is
that the average time to process a single item does not vary with parallel (Egeth, Jonides & Wall, 1972; Townsend & Ashby,
the total number of items undergoing simultaneous (parallel) 1983, pp. 76-98). This state of affairs leads to an asymmetry in
processing. If the individual processing times vary from trial to strength of inference that has been accorded insufficient atten-
trial, as we expect with humans, such unlimited capacity par- tion in the literature.
allel models typically predict that the mean overall completion The empirical side of the parallel-serial issue never entirely
time on a set of items will increase as a function of the set size disappeared and, over the past few years, studies purporting to
(see Egeth, 1966; Townsend, 1974a, or Townsend & Ashby, bear on it have again burgeoned. Unfortunately, their theoret-
1983). ical underpinnings have often been less than desirable. There
An important aspect of such parallel processing is that all seems to have been something of a regression to the reflex
mean set size functions associated with this type of processing response, "linear increasing reaction time functions imply se-
are predicted to be negatively accelerated (Townsend & Ashby, rial processing."
1983, pp. 92-93). Sternberg (1966) took the disparity between Some of the more frequent defenses of this strategy are to
the fastest increasing parallel predictions and his observed lin- take brief note of the dilemma and then: (a) Claim parsimony or
ear functions to rule out parallel processing. However, it was plausibility as did Schneider and Shiffrin (1977), Treisman
apparently not widely appreciated at the time that this demon- (1982), and Treisman, Sykes and Gelade (1977); (b) Argue that
stration did not rule out the vast class of limited capacity par- they have acquired much data and provided a comprehensive
allel models. theory, which nullifies the hazard (e.g., Treisman & Gormican,
Soon thereafter, however, a number of authors began to 1988); or (c) Allude to the issue in a somewhat oblique fashion
notice the potential of limited capacity parallel processes to (e.g., "... a slow, possibly serial, item-by-item compari-
mimic the straight line predictions of standard serial models. son ...", Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977, p. 477). Parsimony is
unfortunately difficult to measure and while a useful concept, is
often found in the eye of the theorist and therefore should be
employed with caution. Converging evidence and an overall
2. In the interest of space and because of the popularity of the theory are to be universally applauded as an approach. How-
reaction time argument, we will concentrate on that dimension in of- ever, the
parallel-serial dilemma has, in this author's opinion
fering a historical perspective. However, much of the rationale imme- received inadequate resolution in several of the efforts at com-
diately pertains to the claim that certain types of accuracy curves in the
context of load or degraded display paradigms imply serial processing prehensive theory, particularly considering the importance the
Townsend issue bears for the data interpretations in many of the relevant
(e.g., Sagi & Julesz, 1987; Sperling, 1963, 1967). (1981)
showed how typical first order accuracy results in whole-report perfor- studies. The oblique approach, while innocuous to experts, may
mance can be predicted by either serial or parallel models. Certain seem perplexing to readers who are not already steeped in the
promising methods based on accuracy will appear later in the paper. parallel-serial debate.

VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990 47

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL
SCIENCE

Serialvs. ParallelProcessing

In followingup on the parsimonyresponse,it may be debat- that dependson the real-timefunctioningof more thana single
able as to whether the serial explanationis always the more subsystem, the question must be faced of whether the sub-
parsimoniousfor increasingreaction time functions, particu- systemsor subprocessesare carriedout in parallel,seriallyor in
larly when unaccompaniedby converging operations. Thus, some hybridfashion.
when processingbecomes closer to unlimitedcapacityparallel
processing(e.g., Schneider& Shiffrin,1977;Egeth, Jonides&
Wall, 1972)throughpractice,for instance, it seems to this au- METHODS OF TESTING SERIAL VS.
thormoreparsimoniousthatthe shiftbe froma limitedcapacity PARALLEL PROCESSING
parallelprocessor than from a serial mechanism.The former
resides within the same qualitativetype of system, unlike the First it should be acknowledgedthat as in all theory and
latter. An opposing view and detailed model are put forth by measurementin science, assumptionsmust be made; in other
Schneiderand Detweiler(1987). words, thereis no free lunch. Moreover,the degreeandtype of
Aside fromthe specific criticismsmentionedabove, what is restrictionson the class of models covered by a paradigmwill
wrongwith a "serialby convention"strategyfroma moreglob- usually differ from those of other paradigms.That is a good
al point of view? If we cannottell the differenceanyway, why reason, along with the fact that no method is perfect, to use
shouldn'twe simply call processing "serial"? One rebuttalis morethanone methodto provideconvergingevidence. Most of
that if the issue is importantenoughto reporton, or if it bears the known methods are based on reaction time but some are
criticalimplicationsfor the interpretationof one's data, then it based on accuracy. Undoubtedly,strong methods can be de-
is importantenoughto test in its own right.And the test should rived that involve reactiontime and accuracyconjointly.And,
optimallyoffer some hope of discriminatingbroadclasses of the the fact that a methodhas been most employedor studiedtheo-
opposing concepts. Linear set size functions do not perform reticallywith reactiontime does not rule out its viabilityin the
that function,but newer techniquesexist that do. contextof accuracyor vice versa. Finally,certainmethodsmay
Anotherproblemwith the "convention"approachis thatan ultimatelyprove to be more appropriatefor certain cognitive
uncontestedconventiontends to ratherquicklyevolve into an situationsor stimulusmaterialsthan others.
accepted fact. The "as if serial" becomes "the serial." This
approachalso may encourage succeeding generationsof re-
searchersto continueto beg the questionor confuse the issue Methods Based on Reaction Time
with the capacitylimits problem.
There are now, as noted above, experimental strategies The followinglist of methodsis not exhaustive,due to space
based on mathematicaldemonstrationsavailableto help deter- requirements but includes several of the most promising
mine whetherprocessingis serial or parallel.Why not employ techniques.4
them?The followingtext outlines some of the promisingtech-
niques for testing parallelismvs. seriality. They are brought /. TheMethod of Factorial Interactionswith
togetherfrom a numberof sources, some of them perhapsnot Selective Influence of CognitiveSubprocesses
readilyaccessible and some of them stated in rathertechnical This techniqueis based on a postulateof selective influence
languageandmathematics.The lattermaybe one reasonfor the by two or more experimentalfactors (see Ashby & Townsend,
apparentlag in assimilationof theoreticalresults. 1980;Steinberg, 1969).That is, it is assumedthat experimental
Obviously,the details are beyond the scope of the present factors can be found that affect separatesubprocesses(stages,
discussion.The mainobject here is to aid the readerin identi-
subsystems,etc.). The investigatormeasuresthe meanreaction
fying the tests that seem especially promisingand to provide
some intuitionabout their rationale.Certainmethodsand par-
allel-serialdistinctionsthat may lead to viable methodsin the
futurehad to be omitted.Townsendand Ashby (1983)provide anism clearly has overtones (sometimes explicit, sometimes not) of
a more complete account, but not all extant methodsare cov- parallelism(e.g., see Atkinson,Campbell,& Francis, 1976;Mandler&
ered there. Shebo, 1982;Sagi & Julesz, 1985a,b). Anotherareaof researchwhich
In closing this section, it shouldbe remarkedthat although overlapsthe parallel-serial
issue is thatof "automaticprocessing"(e.g.,
Logan, 1978, 1985;Schneider& Shiffrin,1977).Althoughthe notions
the above discussionhas been devoted primarilyto briefvisual arefarfromidentical,one way of implementingautomaticityis through
displayand short-termmemorysearch,the parallel-serialissue conversionto an unlimitedcapacity type of parallelprocessing(e.g.,
arises in manycontexts. Indeed, it is sometimespresentin dif- Schneider& Detweiler, 1987).
ferent guises.3 In constructionof almost any cognitive system 4. A nonparametricmethoddevelopedby Thomas(1969a,b) and a
test constructedby Ross andAnderson(1981),basedon a parallel-serial
distinctiondiscoveredby the author(Townsend,1976),hadto be omit-
3. One exampleis the debate, historicalin humanpsychology(e.g., ted fromthe presentreview. They may be somewhatmore difficultto
Hamilton, 1859;Hunter & Siegler, 1940;Kaufman,Lord, Reese, & implementand the backgroundrequiredfor their comprehensionis
Volkman,1949)and ongoingboth in humanand animalpsychology(in moretechnicalthanthe methodscoveredhere. Nevertheless,they may
the lattercase, see e.g., Capaldi& Miller,1988;Davis & Pdrusse,1988) prove importantin futureexperimentation.We also omit methodsof
concerningto what extent a quantityof thingscan be "subitized"vs. increasingthe load, such as the Steinbergmemoryscanningparadigm
"counted."The "counting"mechanismseems to bearimplicationsfor (Steraberg,1966)becauseit was discussedabove. Also as notedabove,
whata formalizedaccountmightcall seriality(see, e.g., Klahr& Wal- its primarystrengtharises when reaction time is flat ratherthan an
lace, 1973;Mandler& Shebo, 1982)similarly,the "subitizing"mech- increasingfunctionof load.

48 VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL
SCIENCE

James T. Townsend

time under all combinations of the various factor levels and then Townsend & Ashby, 1983, see especially pp. 373-375).6
looks for interactions or the lack thereof, the latter to be clar- Schweickert (1978) showed under more general conditions that
ified further below. factors prolonging concurrent processes in stochastic PERT
A historical precursor of factorial methods was the method networks would not be superadditive. We have also further
of subtraction invented by Donders (1859). Donders assumed delineated exactly when serial models can be expected to pre-
that a mental task could be formulated to include or exclude a dict additivity (Townsend, 1984). This work has been general-
particularcognitive subprocess. By measuring the reaction time ized to non-independent parallel and serial models (Schweickert
under both conditions, an estimate could be gained of the av- & Townsend, 1989; Townsend & Schweickert, 1985). Similar
erage processing time consumed by the designated cognitive methods apply to much more complex mental architectures
process. The method of subtraction is still useful despite its (Schweickert, 1978; Schweickert & Townsend, 1989; Town-
strong assumptions (e.g., Ashby & Townsend, 1980; Gotts- send & Schweickert, 1985; Townsend & Schweickert, 1989).
danker & Schrag, 1985). Finally, it can be shown by similar techniques that if the first
In the method of factorial interactions, however, an experi- subprocess to be finished initiates the next stage, then super-
mental factor need not add or delete a subprocess, it need additivity is predicted by independent parallel processes
merely affect its processing time. A lack of interactions is re- (Townsend & Nozawa, 1988).
ferred to as additivity because the factors are affecting reaction
time in a separately additive fashion. That is, the effect of Fac- //. TheParallel-SerialTester
tor X, say, is the same whatever the level of another factor,
Snodgrass (1972) instituted a pattern matching paradigm that
Factor Y. Of course, it should be ascertained that both factors
gave promise of being able to separate certain classes of parallel
are having a definite effect before assessing the presence of and serial models. Townsend (1976) later developed a theory of
interactions. That is, both factors should lead to significant stochastic matching processes which delineated a number of
main effects. It is typical to employ analysis of variance in such fundamental distinctions between parallel and serial operations.
studies, to test for main effects and interactions. Several of these were put together in such a way as to produce
The modern method of factorial interactions may be viewed
the parallel-serial tester (PST), which can be viewed as a sim-
as a descendent of Steinberg's (1969) additive factor method,
plification of Snodgrass' original design, and which was shown
just as Steinberg's method may be interpreted as a descendent mathematically to distinguish all parallel vs. serial models based
of Donders' method of subtraction. Our method differs from on an important class of probability distributions.
Steinberg's in that we have proven that certain types of inter- Basically, the method consists of three experimental condi-
actions imply distinct classes of mental architectures.5 The orig- tions. Each condition involves the perception of two patterns
inal method postulated that the subprocesses acted in a serial
(words, pictures, categories, etc.), which we refer to as A and
fashion and concluded distinct subprocesses if the factors B. In condition CI, the subject must determine which of two
showed additive effects. If the effect was interactive, then those
positions is occupied by pattern A. Response Rl is made if it is
factors were taken as affecting the same subprocess. Thus, this in one of the positions and R2 is made if A is in the other
method could be employed only to confirm seriality together
position. There are two types of trials, AB and BA. Condition
with selective influence. Many studies found factorial additiv- CII requires four trial types, AA, AB, BA, and BB. The subject
ity, although the statistical power in some of those is open to
responds Rl only if both patterns are A and R2 otherwise. This
question (e.g., Pachella, 1974; Pieters, 1983; Theios, 1973; is a conjunction mode of processing. The final condition, CIII,
Townsend, 1984; these studies also provide general caveats also uses the four pattern types of trials as in CII, but now the
with regard to factorial methods).
response mode is disjunctive. That is, the subject responds Rl
The two major types of interactions are subadditivity and if any of the patterns is A and otherwise responds R2. Thus, in
superadditivity (e.g., Townsend, 1984). Subadditivity occurs the latter case, R2 occurs only when the stimulus BB is pre-
when the amount of prolongation caused by a given factorial sented. Note that these conditions can be blocked separately
manipulation, say of Factor X, is less when Factor Y has al- or, with appropriate cues on each trial, intermixed within
ready prolonged the reaction time. This is a negative type of blocks.
interaction. Superadditivity is just the opposite, a positive type The theory assumes that processing is self-terminating; that
of interaction. That is, the increase in reaction time caused by is the subject can cease processing when enough information
Factor X is larger under the condition where Factor Y has has been gained that a correct response can be made. It is also
already prolonged processing time. assumed that errors are few and do not covary in an important
Within our approach, it has been demonstrated that all in-
way with reaction time. It is subject to the possible criticism
dependent parallel processes predict subadditivity when pro- that subjects may not process in the same (parallel or serial)
cessing is exhaustive, that is when all subprocesses must be mode in the three conditions.
completed before a response can take place (Townsend, 1974b; PST has been generalized to be distribution free (see
Townsend & Ashby, 1983, Chapter 13 for the exact mathemat-

5. Taylor (1976) promotesthe use of linear interactionsto draw


conclusionsabout the spatiotemporalnatureof processing.It can be 6. Alternativelysubadditivitycould be associated with a Wheat-
shown that ordinaryparallelprocesses do not obey the precepts re- stone Bridge (Schweickert & Townsend, 1989). Although theoreti-
quiredfor temporaloverlapin his scheme, but it may prove useful in cally possible, this seems less probablethan parallelismin the present
studyingso-called"contingentserialprocesses" (cf., Miller, 1988). circumstances.

VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990 49

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL
SCIENCE

Serialvs. ParallelProcessing

ical specification).It has receivedsome experimentalprobingin Methods Based On Accuracy


its moregeneral(Snodgrass& Townsend,1980)and in its more
specific form (Townsend& Snodgrass,1974).Withinthis lim- IV. Tests by Time Delimitation
ited experimentalarena, the results appearto depend on the Suppose a subject has n items, say 5, to be processed in
complexityof the matchingrequiredof the subject.Withmore some varietyof cognitivetask. Considerthe situationwherein
complexpatternsandprocessingrequirements,subjectsappear one condition, a durationT, say 200 msec is allotted to the
to be forced to resort to serial processing (e.g., Snodgrass, subjectfor processingall 5 items. Here, if processingis serial,
1972;Snodgrass& Townsend, 1980), whereas in the simpler only approximately40 msec can be consumedon each item. If
versionsof the paradigmand with elementarypatterns,thereis the mechanismis parallel,all items receive 200 msec of pro-
a suggestionthat subjectscan operatein parallel(Townsend& cessing. In the other condition, there are n successive time
Snodgrass,1974).PST also seems to be a promisingcandidate intervalsof lengthT, each intervalwith exactly one of the items
for extension to accuracybased experiments. madeavailablefor processing.Thus, therewill be a total of 5 x
200 = 1000msec of exposurein the secondcondition,each item
receiving200 msec of presentation.If processingis serial,each
///. The Method of Redundant Targets item gets 200 msec of work on it ratherthanthe 40 msec of the
In this visual display search method, one type of trial con- first condition.If parallelismholds, each item continuesto re-
tains no targets amongthe n items, and demandsone kind of ceive 200 msec just as in the first condition.If accuracyis just
response,for instancea "no" response.The othertype of trial as good in the first conditionas the second, then parallelpro-
presents one or more targets and requiresthe other type of cessing is supportedbecause serial processing should rightly
response,for examplea "yes" response(Egeth,Folk & Mullin, show a decrementdue to a severe reductionin availablepro-
1988; Wolford, Wessel & Estes, 1968). It is postulated that cessing time in the first condition. Conversely, if accuracy
processingis self-terminating,that is, processingcan be termi- shows a large decrement, then seriality is (somewhat more
natedas soon as the first targetis located. But the natureof the
weakly)supported.Eriksenand Spencer(1969)and Shiffrinand
designpermitsassessmentof this postulate.Thereare two ma- Gardner(1972)employedthis techniqueand acquiredsupport
jor classes of this paradigm.The firstformkeeps the perceptual for parallelprocessing.
set size constant and varies the numberof targets among the The authorand his colleagues (Townsend, 1981;Townsend
distractors(e.g., Wolford, Wessel & Estes, 1968).This form & Ashby, 1983,Chapter11;Townsend& Fial, 1968)developed
does not generallydiscriminateparallelfrom serial processing a naturalcounterpartto that paradigm,in which the first con-
but can be used to test self-terminationversus exhaustivepro- dition was identicalto the above, with T time units duration
cessing and certainother issues. The second form or class of availableon all the items simultaneously.In the second condi-
this paradigmincludesa mixtureof all-targetand all-nontarget
tion, n successive time intervals,each of durationTinare per-
displayswith the numberof items in the displayvaried.Thus, mittedfor processingeach item, and each item is allottedto a
the numberof targetsvariesperfectlywith displaysize here. As
single interval.Adaptingthe above example, each item would
the number of targets increases, all unlimitedcapacity and be exposed for 40 msec in the second condition. If the serial
manylimitedcapacityparallelmodelspredictthatreactiontime modelis the correctexplanation,there wouldbe 40 msec avail-
will decrease(e.g., Snodgrass& Townsend,1980,pp. 335-337). able under either condition so performanceshould be about
Contrarily,serialmodelspredictthat reactiontime will be con- equalin the two cases. However, if processingis parallel,each
stantacross the numberof targetspresent,becausethe average item acquiresmore time underthe first condition,200 msec as
time to process a single targetitem (rememberthat only one,
opposedto 40 msec. Therefore,acuracyis expected to decline
namelythe very first one on such trials has to be completed) fromthe firstto the second conditionunderparallelprocessing.
should not change with the total number of targets (e.g., The experimentalresultswere also in favor of parallelprocess-
Townsend & Ashby, 1983, pp. 80-92). A study by van der
ing in the applicationsof this technique(e.g., Townsend,1981).
Heijden(1975)contains both forms of the paradigmand con- An implicitassumptionin both strategiesis that serialprocess-
cludes that the combineddata supporta limitedcapacity, self-
ing is not disturbedby the sequentialpresentations.Although
terminating,parallelmodel. Caveats about applicationof the these two methodsappearto be quite powerful,a few models
methodareofferedby SnodgrassandTownsend(1980),van der
may be indiscriminabledue to parallel-serialmimicking.
Heijden,La Heij and Boer (1983)and Egeth, Folk, and Mullin
(1988). V. The Second Response Paradigm
There have been a numberof applicationsof this strategy.
In some respects, serial models are more generalthan par-
Overall, it seems fair to say that parallelprocessing is most
allel, especially within the province of reactiontime modeling
supported(see, e.g., the study and discussionby Egeth, Folk
and Mullin, 1988).If the redundancyleads to very substantial (e.g., Townsend, 1976;Townsend& Ashby, 1983).This means
that it is impossible in some cases to gain definitive support
gains in speed, then even ordinaryparallelmodels may not be both for the parallelmodel and againstthe serial, because the
able to handlethe data (e.g., Miller, 1982).Miller(1982, 1986)
and Colonius(1986)discuss some probabilistictechniquesfor
dealingwith redundancygains.7 Johnson, 1977, 1986;Krueger& Shapiro, 1980a,b). Johnson& Blum
(1988)have madeprogresstowardsettlingsome issues in this domain.
However, it still appearsfar from settledjust how the latterrelatesto
7. Thereis a more or less separateliteratureof experimentswhere the type of study discussed here, where strongredundancygains are
redundancyactuallyprolongsor has no effect on responsetimes (e.g., typicallyfound.

50 VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

James T. Townsend

parallelmodelsarecontainedwithinthe serialclass. It is critical There are several ways of exploitingthe foregoingpredic-


to understandthatthis does not meanthata parallelmachineof tion. One is to removea singleconfusionfromeach trialreport
this type, made out of wires, gears or neurons, works in real and then use an appropriatemodel to estimate similarityand
time like a serialmachine- only thatthe mathematicaldescrip- bias parameters(e.g., the similaritychoice model, see Luce,
tion of the parallelclass of machinesis containedwithin the 1963;Shepard,1958;Townsend, 1971b;Townsend& Landon,
mathematicaldescriptionof the serial class for a particular 1982). If the similarityestimates are substantiallylarger for
paradigm. pairsof items that are "obviously" more similar,or deemedto
Interestingly,in movingto the domainof accuracyexperi- be so in regularrecognitionexperimentswith similarstimuli,
ments, the tables are turnedin that the parallelclass of models then supportis garneredfor parallelprocessing.
is often more generalthan the serial. This is because the pro- As in paradigm(MethodV) just above, this methodcannot
cessing state space (more technically, the probabilitysample discriminateparallel processes from hybrid processes which
space) for informationaccrualover time is more complex for permitpartialprocessingon the separateitems at any point in
the parallelmodels in general. In essence, parallelmodels as time. Randomtime sharing models are of this variety (e.g.,
well as certainhybridtime sharingmodels (see Townsend & Townsend& Ashby, 1983,pp. 61-65)as are certainquasi-serial
Ashby, 1983,pp. 61-65and470-471)can predictthatif process- modelswhich posit a sequentialsweep across the items, where
ing is stoppedat an arbitrarypointin time, any numberof items only partialinformationmay be acquiredfrom each item in the
may be in a state of partialprocessing. For instance, if the sweep (e.g., Eriksen& Murphy, 1987;Schulman,Remington,
cognitive system is processingfeatures in parallelon several & McLean, 1979;Yantis, 1988).
items, then cessation of processingcan leave each item with This techniqueis being preparedfor use along with the sec-
some featurescompleted.In contrast,it is a hallmarkof serial ond responseMethodIII (Van Zandt& Townsend,in prepara-
processingas the conventionhas been maintainedover the past tion). The only data of which we are aware that immediately
twenty years or so, that one item is completedat a time, with relateto this strategycome from a whole-reportstudy by Wol-
the succeedingitem not being starteduntil the last is finished ford and Hollingsworth(1974).Althoughtheir interestwas not
(e.g., Townsend,1974a).Therefore,if processingis sharplyter- in the parallel-serialissue per se, their confusionanalyses may
minated,at most one item should be in a state of partialpro- be supportiveof parallelprocessingsince they discoveredsub-
cessing.Thiswill not ordinarilyshow up in the overallaccuracy stantialevidence for visual confusions. However, they did not
resultsof a typical experiment.(But see the next section for a correctfor the possibilitythat one confusioncould occur from
relatedtechniquewhere it can.) serialprocessingon each trial.
In order to exploit this distinction, Townsend and Evans
(1983)developed a techniquebased on a second response on
each itemto be processed.It was demonstratedthatthe pattern DISCUSSION
of accuracyon the second responsesdifferedfor serialvs. par-
allel models. Null hypothesesfor serial processingwithinsev- It seems appropriateto give an example of how an investi-
eral levels of constraintson respondingin the serial models gator might go about applying the above strategies. Certain
were introducedand the results appliedto a pilot experiment. strategiesare more naturalin some contexts than others. What
Withinthe study, the data passed the tests for the most lenient about the popularsituationswhere the stimuliare made up of
serialhypothesisbut ran into troublewith the more restrictive severalitems andon targettrialsone of the items is a targetand
criteria.Currently,we are developingalternativeparallelmod- on the remainderof the trials, no item is a target (Atkinson,
els for testing against the serial class and for examinationof Holmgren& Juola, 1969;Steinberg, 1966;Townsend& Roos,
statisticalpowerof the serialnullhypothesistests (VanZandt& 1973; Treisman & Gormican, 1988)? The subject responds
44
Townsend, in preparation).A potential vulnerabilityof this yes" in the formercase and "no" in the latter. Accuracy is
strategyis thatin some applications,the secondresponsemight typically high and the major dependent variable is reaction
be based more on the first response than on the cognitive or time. The majorindependentvariableis set size, thatis, number
perceptualprocessingassociated with the first response. For of items in the stimulusdisplay. As noted earlier, if reaction
this reason,it is helpfulto pairthis methodwith others such as time increasesvery much (this itself is usually a subjectiveas-
the one following. pect of the experiments)then processing is said to be serial,
especially if the reaction times appear more or less linearly
VI. A Similarityand ConfusionTechnique relatedto set size (again,tests of linearityarerarelyperformed).
A naturalstrategy within the context of accuracy experi- If reactiontime curves are more or less flat, then processingis
ments is to examine the pattern of confusions across items. said to be parallel.
Because parallelprocesses typically leave items in a partially How can we be sure that processing is not simply limited
processedstate when processingdurationis terminatedby end capacityparallelin the formercase? As Treismanand Gormi-
of exposure,a responsesignalor the like, as discussedabove in can (1988)observe, convergingevidence is requiredto support
Method V, it is expected that the frequency of confusions that claim. One immediate and natural supplementarytech-
amongsimilaritems shouldbe greaterthanin the case of serial niquemightbe the redundanttargetsparadigm(ReactionTime
processing.That is, when processingis serial, there shouldbe MethodIII), as employedby Egeth and his colleaguesand oth-
at most one item thatcan be confusedwith a similaralternative ers (see, e.g., Egeth, Folk & Mullin, 1988). If reaction time
on each trial. Thus, there shouldnever be more than one item decreases when the number of items to be processed in-
confusedwith those most similarto itself except by chance. creases- in the target trial case, the numberof targets- then

VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990 51

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL
SCIENCE

Serial vs. Parallel Processing

parallelprocessingis supported.The most parsimoniouscon- noted earlier,the theoreticalfindingsimply that sums of aver-
clusionis that in the regularsingle-targetparadigm,processing age reactiontimes fromcertainconditionsmustbe equalif pro-
is still parallelbut in thatcase, limitedcapacity.Withextremely cessing is serial, but not if processingis parallel.Further,they
simple stimulusitems, for example, as employedin certainof guaranteethat perfect or near perfect fits to data could not be
the conditionsof Treismanand Gormican(1988),the other re- simultaneouslyattainedby both types of models.
action time methods seem most apt to provideadditionalevi- On more complex items, those made up of several dimen-
dence. Both the confusionparadigm(MethodVI) and the sec- sions or features, the accuracy or reactiontime methodspre-
ond responseparadigm(MethodV) postulatethat the items to sented above may be equally attractive. Of course in most
be processedare composedof constituentinformationthatmay cases, which is most appropriatewill be dictatedby the topic
be partiallyprocessed (e.g., features, dimensions),which may under study. Nevertheless, it is an interestingquestion as to
not be the case for search for a long line amonga numberof whetherthe outcomes of accuracyand reactiontime methods,
other short lines (as in Treisman& Gormican,1988).The fac- even withinthe same type of stimuliand responseinstructions,
torialstrategymay provideone mode of attack. Most theoret- will yield the same conclusions. Sometimes a general theory
ical results have been derived for forced exhaustiveserial or can predictwhat shouldbe the case, and then appropriatespe-
parallelprocessing,that is, wherethe subjectsmustprocess all cific tests can be applied.
items in order to make a correct response (Sternberg,1969;
Townsend & Ashby, 1983;Townsend & Schweickert, 1985).
However, as noted earlierTownsend and Nozawa (1988) re- Solving the Parallel vs. Serial Dilemma
cently developed comparabletheoremsfor minimumprocess-
ingtimes. Littlehas been accomplishedfor self-terminating pro- One often hearsthe questionasked as to whetherthe paral-
cessing with single targets,but this shouldnot prove difficult. lel-serialquestion has been, or can be, "solved." Clearlythe
If one requiresevidenceaboutthe serialvs. parallelquestion issue cannot be resolved by any mathematicalwork alone.
when the set size is large, then a reasonableapplicationof the However, mathematicswith the properempiricalinterpretation
factorialmethodwould be to arbitrarilyor randomlydividethe has the potentialto at least demonstratewhat cannotwork, and
items into two groups.These two groupswouldreceive manip- with a little luck and perseverance,to offer experimentalde-
ulationof some factors, such as brightnessand spatialsepara- signs that may be able to answerthe theoreticalquestionbeing
tion, that affect processingspeed. This parallel-serialtest can posed.
be applied for any given set size, thus avoiding the artifact Occasionally,psychologicalintuitionalone can lead to a par-
associatedwith set size. adigmthattests most modelsof two opposingprinciplesagainst
A variationof this paradigmthatis of some interestwouldbe one another. Even in such cases, it is reassuringto see the
to place the two groupsof items in separatespatialor temporal reasoningbackedup by mathematicaldemonstration.This can
locations. In principle,all items could be factoriallymanipu- show: (a) Thatthe experimentreallycan test the two principles,
lated, but that would only be feasible with a relatively small ratherthan being confoundedwith anotherthirdissue, such as
numberof stimulusitems. Subadditivitywith regardto the fac- the capacityquestionin the parallel-serialcontroversy,and (b)
torial manipulationon "no" trials would support parallelity That large classes of models based on the two principlesare
(e.g., Townsend, 1984;Townsend& Ashby, 1983,pp. 373-375; tested as opposed to ratherspecial cases. For instance, it was
Townsend& Schweickert,1985)in this context. Superadditiv- comfortingto findthatPST (MethodII) could settle the parallel-
ity would supportan architecturethat is neitherserialnor par- serialquestionin a way that did not dependon particularprob-
allel (Schweickert& Townsend, 1989;Townsend& Schweick- abilityformulationsof the models (Townsend& Ashby, 1983,
ert, 1985). Additivitywould support seriality.8As far as the Chapter13). A rigorousmathematicalformulationmay also aid
authoris aware,factorialmethodshave not so far been applied in statisticaltesting and other facets of the overall procedure.
in the ways discussed in this and the previousparagraph. Of course, whether in a verbally based or a mathematically
Another appropriatestrategy might be PST, the parallel- derivedmethod,the paradigmmust be implementedin the cru-
serial tester (Method II). In an applicationto the multi-item cible of experimentation.9
searchexperiments,therewouldagainbe a divisionof the stim- Even if a paradigmis capableof providingan answerto the
ulus set into two groupsof items. For instance,the investigator serial-parallelquestion, and tests are carriedout with due at-
mightdividea visualarrayinto a left vs. a rightsegment.In PST tentionto initialand boundaryconditions, statisticalrules and
either the rightor the left side or both could contain a target. so on, we still cannotguaranteethe answerto be anythingbut
Three differentconditions impose differentresponse require- local. In psychologyit is rarethat changingthe circumstances
ments on the subject, but with the same type of stimuli. As just a little bit does not alter the results, and sometimes the

8. (a) Strictly speaking,with a sizeable class of processes called 9. Unfortunately,there seems to exist a wall between the more
EmbellishedWheatstoneBridges,the two processesunderstudymust qualitativeand the more quantitativetheoriststhat has not been com-
at least lie in two separatesubgraphsof processeswhicharethemselves pletelypermeable.The morequalitativetheoristsoften state (privately)
connectedby a single"path."A specialcase of this is serialprocessing that they attemptto read the pertinentmathematicalapproachesbut
per se wherethe two processeslie in a singleserialchainof processes, findmuchof it too abstruse.Similarly,quantitativetheoristsfrequently
(b) Differentialparallelvs. serialpredictionscan also be madefor target claimto attemptto writein such a way as to communicateto the other
present trials even if processing is self-terminating(Townsend & group.It appearsthateven moreeffortshouldbe madeon both sides to
Nozawa, 1988). penetratethis barrier.

52 VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

James T. Townsend

psychological processes underlying them. Yet, there is reason lines, letters, and lexicality. In B.E. Shepp and S. Ballesteros (Eds.), Object
Perception: Structure and Process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
to believe that many local settings should provide answers to Egeth, H., Jonides, J., & Wall, S. (1972). Parallel processing of multielement
questions about processing if they are put in the right fashion. displays. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 674-698.
Of course, in certain settings the background theory predicts Eriksen, C.W., & Murphy, T.D. (1987). Movement of attentional focus across the
visual field: A critical look at the evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 43,
that two different environments, types of stimuli, or instruc- 299-305.
tions, processing will be parallel in one, serial in the other. For Eriksen, C.W., & Spencer, T. (1969). Rate of information processing in visual
perception: Some results and methodological considerations. Journal of
instance, processing complexity seems to be one determining Experimental Psychology Monographs, 79(2, Pt 2).
factor of whether operations can be parallel (and sometimes of Estes, W.K., & Taylor, H.A. (1964). A detection method and probabilistic models
unlimited capacity) rather than serial. Some of the Snodgrass for assessing information from brief visual displays. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 52, 446-454.
and Townsend (1980) results with PST, mentioned in the con- Estes, W.K., & Taylor, H.A. (1966). Visual detection in relation to display size
text of Method V, seem to be of this sort, and complexity plays and redundancy of critical elements. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 9-16.
an explicit role of this nature in the Treisman line of research Gottsdanker, R., & Shragg, G.P. (1985). Verification of Donders' Subtraction
Method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per-
(e.g., Treisman & Gormican, 1988). formance, 11, 765-776.
But what if significant generality of mechanism is, as most Hamilton, J. (1859). Lectures on metaphysics and logic (Vol. 1). Edinburgh:
Blackwood.
would hope, a real possibility in psychology? Is then the opti- Hunter, W.S., & Siegler, M. (1940). The span of visual discrimination as a func-
mal approach for making progress in psychology, the critical tion of time and intensity of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
testing of important issues like parallel vs. serial processing? ogy, 26, 160-179.
Johnson, N.F. (1977). A pattern unit model of word identification. In D. LaBerge
(See also, Massaro, 1987; Meyer, Yantis, Osman & Smith, & S.J. Samuels (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Perception and com-
1984; Miller, 1982; for other recent examples of testing opposed prehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Johnson, N.F. (1986). On the detection of letters within redundant arrays. Per-
binary concepts.) Or is it better to formulate a model that is ception & Psychophysics, 40(2), 93-100.
based on specific choices on such issues, for example parallel, Johnson, N.F., & Blum, A.J. (1988). When redundancy hurts letter detection: An
exhaustive, independent processing, but perhaps with sufficient attempt to define one condition. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 147-155.
Kaufman, E.L., Lord, M.W., Reese, T.W., & Volkman, J. (1949). The discrim-
complexity and parameters that it can be probed in a broader ination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology, 62, 498-525.
set of experiments? In the opinion of the author, these two Klahr, D., & Wallace, J.G. (1973). The role of quantification operators in the
strategies should be complementary. Indeed, in a complex development of conservation. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 301-327.
Krueger, L.E., & Shapiro, R.G. (1980a). Repeating the target neither speeds nor
young field like psychology, it would seem foolish to concen- slows its detection: Evidence for independent channels in letter processing.
trate only on one to the neglect of the other. Each strategy has Perception & Psychophysics, 28, 68-76.
Krueger, L.E., & Shapiro, R.G. (1980b). Why search for target absence is so slow
advantages in regions where the other has weaknesses. (and careful): The more targets there are, the more likely you are to miss
In any event, the availability of experimental methods that one. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor-
can test serial against parallel processing may offer some hope mance, 6, 662-685.
for resolving other tough issues as well. Logan, G.D. (1978). Attention demands of visual search. Memory & Cognition,
6(4), 446-453.
Logan, G.D. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications, and future
directions. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39(2), 367-386.
Acknowledgments- This work was supportedby NationalScience Luce, R.D. (1963). Detection and recognition. In R.D. Luce, R.R. Bush, & E.
Foundation,Memoryand CognitiveProcesses Award #8710163. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology, 103-189. New
Thanksto Kilsoon Cummingsfor her typing of this manuscript. York: Wiley.
Thanksalso to HowardEgeth, JerryBusemeyer,CharlesEriksen, Luce, R.D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental
two refereesand the Editorfor helpfulcritiquesof this paper. organization. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mandler, G., & Shebo, B.J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component
processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 1-22.
Massaro, D.W. (1987). Speech perception by ear and eye: A paradigm for psy-
chological inquiry. In B. Dodd & R. Campbell (Eds.), Hearing by Eye:
REFERENCES Experimental Studies in the Psychology of Lipreading (Chap. 1). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Meyer, D.E., Irwin, D.E., Osman, A.M., & Kounios, J. (1988). The dynamics of
Ashby, F.G., & Townsend, J.T. (1980). Decomposing the reaction time distribu- cognition and action: Mental processes inferred from speed-accuracy de-
tion: Pure insertion and selective influence revisited. Journal of Mathemat-
composition. Psychological Review, 95, 183-237.
ical Psychology, 27(2), 93-123.
Meyer, D.E., Yantis, S., Osman, A., & Smith, J.E.K. (1984). Discrete vs. con-
Atkinson, J., Campbell, F.W., & Francis, M.R. (1976). The magic number 4: A tinuous models of response preparation: A reaction time analysis. In S.
new look at visual numerosity judgments. Perception, 5, 327-334. Kornblum & J. Requin (Eds.), Preparatory States and Processes.
Atkinson, R.C., Holmgren, J.E., & Juola, J.F. (1969). Processing time as influ-
enced by the number of elements in a visual display. Perception & Psycho- Miller, J.O. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant
signals. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 247-279.
physics, 6, 321-326.
Miller, J.O. (1986). Time course of coactivation in bimodal divided attention.
Broadbent, D.E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon.
Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 331-343.
Capaldi, E.J., & Miller, D.J. (1988). Counting in rats: Its functional significance
and the independent cognitive processes that constitute it. Journal of Ex- Miller, J.O. (1988). Discrete and continuous models of information processing:
Theoretical distinctions and empirical results. Acta Psychologica, 67, 191-
perimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 3-17.
257.
Colonius, H. (1986). Measuring channel dependence in separate activation mod-
els. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 251-255. Murdock, B.B., Jr. (1971). A parallel-processing model for scanning. Perception
Corcoran, D.W.J. (1971). Pattern recognition. Middlesex, PA: Penguin. & Psychophysics, 10, 289-291.
Davis, H., & P6russe, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
issues, current evidence and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Pachella, R. (1974). The interpretation of reaction time in information processing
Sciences, 11, 561-615. research. In B.H. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: Tuto-
Donders, F.C. (1859). On the speed of mental processes. (W.G. Koster, Trans.). rials in performance and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
In W.G. Koster (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 2). Amsterdam: Pieters, J.P.M. (1983). Steinberg's additive factor method and underlying psy-
North Holland. chological processes: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Bul-
Egeth, H. (1966). Parallel versus serial processes in multidimensional stimulus letin, 93, 4U-426.
discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 245-252. Ratcliff, R. (1988). Continuous versus discrete information processing: Modeling
Egeth, H., Folk, C, & Mullin, P. (1988). Spatial parallelism in the processing of accumulation of partial information. Psychological Review, 95(2), 238-255.

VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990 53

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PSYCHOLOGICAL
SCIENCE

Serial vs. Parallel Processing

Ross, B.H., & Anderson, J.R. (1981). A test of parallel versus serial processing time distributions. Paper presented at the 7th Annual Mathematical Psy-
applied to memory retrieval. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 24(3), chology Meetings, Ann Arbor, MI.
183-223. Townsend, J.T. (1976). Serial and within-stage independent parallel model equiv-
Sagi, D., & Julesz, B. (1985a). "Where" and "what" in vision. Science, 228, alence on the minimum completion time. Journal of Mathematical Psychol-
1217-1219. ogy, 14, 219-238.
Sagi, D., & Julesz, B. (1985b). Detection vs. discrimination of visual orientation. Townsend, J.T. (1981). Some characteristics of visual whole report behavior.
Perception, 14, 619-628. Ada Psychologica, 47, 149-173.
Sagi, D., & Julesz, B. (1987). Short-range limitation on detection of feature dif- Townsend, J.T. (1984). Uncovering mental processes with factorial experiments.
ferences. Spatial Vision, 2(1), 39-49. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 28(4), 363-400.
Schneider, W., & Detweiler, M. (1987). A connectionist/control architecture for Townsend, J.T., & Ashby, F.G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psy-
working memory. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 21, 53-119. chological processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R.M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human infor- Townsend, J.T., & Evans, R. (1983). A systems approach to parallel-serial test-
mation processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Re- ability and visual feature processing. In H.G. Geissler (Ed.), Modern issues
view, 84, 1-66. in perception (pp. 166-189). Berlin: VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissen-
Schulman, G.L., Remington, R.W., & McClean, J.P. (1979). Moving attention schaften.
through visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Percep- Townsend, J.T., & Fial, R. (1968). Spatiotemporal characteristics of multisymbol
tion and Performance, 5, 522-526. perception. Paper presented at the 1st Annual Mathematical Psychology
Schweickert, R. (1978). A critical path generalization of the additive factor Meetings, Stanford, CA.
method: Analysis of a stroop task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 18, Townsend, J.T., & Landon, D.E. (1982). An experimental and theoretical inves-
105-139. tigation of the constant ratio rule and other models of visual letter recogni-
Schweickert, R., & Townsend, J.T. (1989). A trichotomy: Interactions of factors tion. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 25, 119-163.
prolonging sequential and concurrent mental processes in stochastic dis- Townsend, J.T., & Nozawa, G. (1988). Strong evidence for parallel processing
crete mental (PERT) networks. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, with simple dot stimuli. Paper presented at the 29th Meeting of the Psy-
328-347. chonomic Society, Chicago.
Shepard, R.N. (1958). Stimulus and response generalization: Deduction of the Townsend, J.T., & Roos, R.N. (1973). Search reaction time for single targets in
generalization gradient from a trace model. Psychological Review, 65, 242- multiletter stimuli with brief visual displays. Memory and Cognition, 1,
283. 319-332.
Shiffrin, R.M., & Gardner, G.T. (1972). Visual processing capacity and atten- Townsend, J.T., & Schweickert, R. (1985). Interactive effects of factors prolong-
tional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 72-82. ing processes in latent-mental networks. In G. d'Ydewalle (Ed.), Cognition,
Snodgrass, J.G. (1972). Reaction times for comparisons of successively presented information processing, and motivation (Vol. 3, pp. 255-276). 23rd Interna-
visual patterns: Evidence for serial self-terminating search. Perception & tional Congress of Psychology. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Psychophysics, 12(4). Townsend, J.T., & Schweickert, R. (1989). Toward the trichotomy method of
Snodgrass, J.G., & Townsend, J.T. (1980). Comparing parallel and serial models: reaction times: Laying the foundation of stochastic mental networks. Jour-
Theory and implementation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human nal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, 309-327.
Perception and Performance, 6, 330-354. Townsend, J.T., & Snodgrass, J.G. (1974). A serial vs. parallel testing paradigm
Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psy- when "same" and "different" comparison rates differ. Paper presented at
chological Monographs, 74(11, Whole No. 498). the Psychonomic Society, Boston, MA.
Sperling, G. (1963). A model for visual memory tasks. Human Factors, 5, 19-31. Treisman, A.M. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psycholog-
Sperling, G. (1967). Successive approximations to a model for short-term mem- ical Review, 76, 282-299.
ory. Acta Psychologica, 27, 285-292. Treisman, A. (1982). Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for fea-
Steinberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652- tures and for objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8(2), 194-214.
654. Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence
Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donder's from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95(1), 15-48.
method. In W.G. Koster (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 2). Am- Treisman, A., Sykes, M., & Gelade, G. (1977). Selective attention and stimulus
sterdam: North Holland. integration. In Stanislaw Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI.
Taylor, D.A. (1976). Stage analysis of reaction time. Psychological Bulletin, 83, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
161-191. Van der Heijden, A.H.C. (1975). Some evidence for a limited capacity parallel
Theios, J. (1973). Reaction time measurement in the study of memory processes: self-terminating process in simple visual search tasks. Acta Psychologica,
Theory and data. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and 39, 21-41.
Motivation (Vol. 7). New York: Academic Press. Van der Heijden, A.H.C, La Heij, W., & Boer, J.P.A. (1983). Parallel processing
Thomas, E.A.C. (1969a). Distribution free tests for mixed probability distribu- of redundant targets in simple visual search tasks. Psychological Research,
tions. Biometrika, 56(3), 475. 45, 235-254.
Thomas, E.A.C. (1969b). Alternative models for information processing: Con- Van Zandt, P. A., & Townsend, J.T. Testing serial and parallel processing hy-
structing non-parametric tests. British Journal of Mathematical and Statis- potheses in visual whole report experiments. Manuscript in preparation.
tical Psychology 22, 105. Based on MA thesis of Van Zandt by same title, Purdue University, 1988.
Townsend, J.T. (1971a). A note on the identifiability of parallel and serial pro- Vorberg, D. (1977). On the equivalence of parallel and serial models of informa-
cesses. Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 161-163. tion processing. Paper presented at the 10th Annual Mathematical Psychol-
Townsend, J.T. (1971b). Theoretical analysis of an alphabetic confusion matrix. ogy Meetings, Los Angeles.
Perception & Psychophysics, 9, 40-50. Wolford, G.L., & Holhngsworth, S. (1974). Evidence that short-term memory is
Townsend, J.T. (1972). Some results on the identifiability of parallel and serial not the limiting factor in the tachistoscopic full-report procedure. Memory
and Cognition, 2, 796-800.
processes. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 25,
168-199. Wolford, G.L., Wessel, D.L., & Estes, W.K. (1968). Further evidence concern-
Townsend, J.T. (1974a). Issues and models concerning the processing of a finite ing scanning and sampling assumptions of visual detection models. Percep-
number of inputs. In B.H. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: tion & Psychophysics, 3, 439-444.
Tutorials in performance and cognition, (pp. 133-168). Hillsdale, NJ: Yantis, S. (1988). On analog movements of visual attention. Perception & Psy-
Lawrence Erlbaum. chophysics, 43, 203-206.
Townsend, J.T. (1974b). Independent parallel models and analyses of reaction (Received 5/4/89;Accepted 5/16/89)

54 VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY 1990

This content downloaded from 185.44.77.34 on Sun, 15 Jun 2014 08:42:06 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like