The Relationships of Sleep Quality Length and Napping To Physic

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 207

Louisiana Tech University

Louisiana Tech Digital Commons


Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School

Spring 2013

The relationships of sleep quality, length, and


napping to physical performance
Rebecca M. Hoffmann

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.latech.edu/dissertations


Part of the Clinical Psychology Commons
THE RELATIONSHIPS OF SLEEP QUALITY, LENGTH,

AND NAPPING TO PHYSICAL

PERFORMANCE

By

Rebecca M. Hoffmann, B.S., M.A., M. S.

A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment


of the Requirements for the Degree
Doctor of Philosophy

COLLEGE OF EDUCATION
LOUISIANA TECH UNIVERSITY

May 2013
UMI Number: 3573575

All rights reserved

INFORMATION TO ALL USERS


The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed,
a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
Dissertation PiiblishMiQ

UMI 3573575
Published by ProQuest LLC 2013. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author.
Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC.
All rights reserved. This work is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest LLC
789 East Eisenhower Parkway
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346
LOUISIANA TECH UNIVERSITY

THE GRADUATE SCHOOL

August 23, 2012


Date

We hereby recommend that the dissertation prepared under our supervision

Rebecca M. Hoffmann

entitled__________________________________________________________________________

The Relationships of Sleep Quality/Length and Napping to Physical

Performance

be accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

i Supervisor b f Dissgrta
ertation Research

Head o f Department
Dept.of Psychology & Behavioral Sciences
Department

Recommendation concurred in:

0 a~uiM“
ft- Arfc D a ^J

Advisory Committee

Approved: Approved:

Director o f Graduate Studies Dean o f the Graduate Scnoo

Dean o f the College


GS Form 13a
(6/07)
ABSTRACT

College students suffer from more sleep disturbances than the general population.

Sleep difficulties can lead to lower levels of performance, memory, and cognitive ability.

Sleep quality is known to impact individuals’ physical and psychological health. The

relationship between sleep variables (i.e., sleep quality, sleep length, sleepiness, and a

nap/relaxation) and physical performance (i.e., flexibility, grip strength, and peak

performance) has not been fully examined. The purpose of this study was to explore the

relationship between sleep quality, sleep length, and sleepiness and physical performance

(e.g., flexibility, grip strength, and peak power), as well as to determine if a short nap

diminishes the effect of poor sleep on the same physical performance measures.

Participants of this study were students at a mid-sized southern United States

university who were recruited from classes in the College of Education. The relationship

between sleep quality, length, and sleepiness and flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump

height, and peak power was assessed using the Sleep Quality Index, the Adult Sleep-

Wake Scale, the Sleep Habits Questionnaire, the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, the

Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, hand dynamometer, vertical jump test, and sit-and-reach

box. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine the effect of

a nap/relaxation on flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak power, with

gender as an independent variable. Correlations were also conducted to determine the

relationship between sleep quality, sleep duration, and sleepiness and flexibility, grip

strength, and peak power.


APPROVAL FOR SCHOLARLY DISSEMINATION

The author grants to the Prescott Memorial Library of Louisiana Tech University

the right to reproduce, by appropriate methods, upon request, any or all portions of this

Dissertation. It was understood that “proper request” consists of the agreement, on the

part of the requesting party, that said reproduction was for his personal use and that

subsequent reproduction will not occur without written approval of the author of this

Dissertation. Further, any portions of the Dissertation used in books, papers, and other

works must be appropriately referenced to this Dissertation.

Finally, the author of this Dissertation reserves the right to publish freely, in the

literature, at any time, any or all portions of this Dissertation.

GS Form 14
(5/03)
TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT............................................................................................................................. iii

LIST OF TABLES..................................................................................................................xii

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION..............................................................................................1

Statement of the Problem....................................................................................................5

Justification..........................................................................................................................7

Literature Review ............................................................................................................... 7

Stages of Sleep.............................................................................................................. 7

N aps.......................................................................................................................10

Physiological Processes of Sleep-Wake Cycle.........................................................11

Autonomic Nervous System................................................................................ 11

Homeostatic Drive................................................................................................12

Circadian Rhythm.................................................................................................14

The Theories o f Sleep Function.................................................................................18

The Restoration M odel........................................................................................ 18

The Neurotransmitter Replenishment Theory................................................... 19

The Evolutionary/Circadian Sleep Models.........................................................19

The Developmental M odel.................................................................................. 19

The Learning M odel............................................................................................ 20

Sleep Difficulties of College Students...................................................................... 22

Factors that Affect Sleep in College Students......................................................... 26

v
vi

Effects of Inadequate Sleep Length.......................................................................... 31

Sleep and Health.........................................................................................................32

Sleep and Obesity/Health Status......................................................................... 40

Sleep and Learning/Academic Performance............................................................ 45

Sleep Quality and Physical Performance..................................................................51

Napping and Physical Performance......................................................................... 52

Relaxation................................................................................................................... 54

Relaxation Techniques......................................................................................... 54

Guided Imagery................................................................................................... 56

Hypotheses........................................................................................................................ 57

Hypothesis 1A.............................................................................................................57

Hypothesis IB .............................................................................................................57

Hypothesis 1C.............................................................................................................57

Hypothesis ID .............................................................................................................57

Justification of Hypotheses 1A, IB, 1C, and ID ...............................................58

Hypothesis 2A.............................................................................................................58

Hypothesis 2B.............................................................................................................58

Hypothesis 2C.............................................................................................................59

Hypothesis 2D.............................................................................................................59

Justification of Hypotheses 2A, 2B, 2C, and 2 D ...............................................59

Hypothesis 3A.............................................................................................................59

Hypothesis 3B.............................................................................................................60

Hypothesis 3C.............................................................................................................60
vii

Hypothesis 3D............................................................................................................. 60

Justification of Hypotheses 3A, 3B, 3C, and 3D ............................................... 60

Hypothesis 4A............................................................................................................. 60

Hypothesis 4B ............................................................................................................. 61

Hypothesis 4C ............................................................................................................. 61

Hypothesis 4D............................................................................................................. 61

Justification of Hypotheses 4A, 4B, 4C, and 4 D ............................................... 61

CHAPTER 2 METHOD......................................................................................................... 62

Participants......................................................................................................................... 62

Measures............................................................................................................................ 62

Demographics F orm ...................................................................................................62

Sleep Quality Index....................................................................................................63

Adult Sleep-Wake Scale............................................................................................ 64

Sleep Habits Questionnaire........................................................................................ 65

Epworth Sleepiness Scale.......................................................................................... 66

Karolinska Sleepiness Scale...................................................................................... 67

Physical Performance Measures...................................................................................... 67

Hand Grip Dynamometer........................................................................................... 67

Sit-and-Reach Test...................................................................................................... 68

Vertec Vertical Jump Device..................................................................................... 69

Estimated Peak Pow er................................................................................................69

Procedure........................................................................................................................... 69

Recruitment of Participants....................................................................................... 70
Control Group............................................................................................................. 71

Experimental Group....................................................................................................71

Hand Dynamometer Test Procedure......................................................................... 72

Vertical Jump Test Procedure....................................................................................72

Sit-and-Reach Test Procedure...................................................................................73

Risks and Benefits......................................................................................................73

CHAPTER 3 RESULTS.........................................................................................................74

Participants........................................................................................................................ 74

Overall Sam ple...........................................................................................................74

Control Group.............................................................................................................75

Experimental Group................................................................................................... 75

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Overall Sample............................. 76

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Overall Sample by Gender............77

Examination of Gender Differences for Overall Sample...............................................79

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Control Group............................... 79

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Control Group byGender............. 80

Examination of Gender Differences for Control Group................................................ 82

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Experimental Group.................... 82

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Experimental Group by Gender...83

Examination of Gender Differences for Experimental Group...................................... 85

Homogeneity o f Variance................................................................................................ 85

Correlations between Variables.......................................................................................86

Correlations between Variables with M ales................................................................... 88


Correlations between Variables with Females................................................................90

Hypothesis 1A................................................................................................................... 92

Hypothesis IB ................................................................................................................... 92

Hypothesis 1C................................................................................................................... 94

Hypothesis ID ................................................................................................................... 94

Hypothesis 2A................................................................................................................... 95

Hypothesis 2B ................................................................................................................... 96

Hypothesis 2C................................................................................................................... 99

Hypothesis 2D..................................................................................................................101

Hypothesis 3A ..................................................................................................................102

Hypothesis 3B..................................................................................................................103

Hypothesis 3C..................................................................................................................104

Hypothesis 3D..................................................................................................................105

Hypothesis 4A ..................................................................................................................106

Hypothesis 4B ..................................................................................................................107

Hypothesis 4C ..................................................................................................................110

Hypothesis 4D..................................................................................................................112

CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION.................................................................................................114

General Overview of Results......................................................................................... 114

Hypothesis One................................................................................................................117

Hypothesis 1A........................................................................................................... 117

Hypothesis IB ........................................................................................................... 118

Hypothesis 1C........................................................................................................... 119


X

Hypothesis ID ............................................................................................................119

Hypothesis Tw o...............................................................................................................120

Hypothesis 2A ............................................................................................................120

Hypothesis 2B ............................................................................................................121

Hypothesis 2C ............................................................................................................122

Hypothesis 2D............................................................................................................123

Hypothesis Three.............................................................................................................123

Hypothesis 3A ............................................................................................................123

Hypothesis 3B............................................................................................................124

Hypothesis 3C............................................................................................................124

Hypothesis 3D............................................................................................................125

Hypothesis Four...............................................................................................................125

Hypothesis 4A............................................................................................................125

Hypothesis 4B ............................................................................................................126

Hypothesis 4C ............................................................................................................127

Hypothesis 4D............................................................................................................127

Implications..................................................................................................................... 127

Limitations....................................................................................................................... 130

Suggestions for Future Research................................................................................... 134

APPENDIX A HUMAN USE COMMITTEE APPROVAL FORM................................137

APPENDIX B INFORMED CONSENT FORM ............................................................... 139

APPENDIX C DEMOGRAPHICS FORM .........................................................................141

APPENDIX D SLEEP QUALITY INDEX.........................................................................143


xi

APPENDIX E ADULT SLEEP-WAKE SCALE............................................................... 145

APPENDIX F SLEEP HABITS QUESTIONNAIRE........................................................149

APPENDIX G EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE...........................................................151

APPENDIX H KAROLINSKA SLEEPINESS SCALE................................................... 153

APPENDIX I SCRIPT FOR RECRUITING PARTICIPANTS........................................155

APPENDIX J ACTIVE DYNAMIC WARM-UP PROCEDURE....................................157

REFERENCES......................................................................................................................159
LIST OF TABLES

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests fo r


Overall Sam ple.............................................................................................. 76

Table 2 Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for


Overall Sample by Gender............................................................................ 78

Table 3 Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests fo r


Control Group................................................................................................ 80

Table 4 Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for


Control Group by Gender............................................................................. 81

Table 5 Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for


Experimental G roup......................................................................................83

Table 6 Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for


Experimental Group by Gender.................................................................... 84

Table 7 Summary o f Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations


fo r Scores on All Measures fo r Overall Sample..........................................87

Table 8 Summary o f Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations


fo r Scores on All Measures fo r Overall Sample with Males Only............. 89

Table 9 Summary o f Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations


fo r Scores on All Measures fo r Overall Sample with Females Only.........91

xii
CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Sleep is a natural reversible state of unconsciousness in which individuals rest

their minds and bodies. Sleep is also an active process during which complex

physiological, neurological, and hormonal changes transpire. Sleep has been found to be

imperative to learning and cognitive functioning, as well as physical and emotional

health. Difficulty sleeping is one of the most common and consistently increasing health

complaints among young adults (Hicks, Mistry, Lucero, Lee, & Pelligrini, 1989). Many

college students do not get enough sleep and may not realize the impact that lack of sleep

has on their functioning.

College years are a time of transition that entails multifaceted challenges in

academic, social, psychological, and developmental adjustment. In response to newfound

personal stressors and environmental variables, college students may compensate with

modification of sleep behaviors and patterns. Brown, Soper, and Buboltz (2001) found

that one-third of college students report regular, severe sleep difficulties. Studies have

found that college students have more sleep problems than the general population. The

National Sleep Foundation recommends that adults 18-54 years of age sleep between

seven and 8.5 hours per night. Coren (1994) indicated that over 30% o f college students

reported having difficulty sleeping. Buboltz, Brown, and Soper (2001) noted that over

70% of college students have regular sleep difficulties. In comparison to the general

1
2

population, college students tend to have more difficulty sleeping (Buboltz et al., 2001;

Coren, 1994; Lack 1986).

Sleep is an important and essential function of humans, comprising nearly one-

third of our lives. Though sleep is understood to be necessary, the specific physiologic

function of sleep remains unknown (Sejnowski & Destexhe, 2000). Disruption of sleep

has been demonstrated to have adverse neurocognitive effects. For example, meta­

analyses have shown reductions in performance on cognitive and motor tasks after sleep

deprivation (Philibert, 2005; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996). A lack of sleep alters

physiological states that affect cognitive functioning, metabolism, inflammation, and

neurobehavioral functioning (Dimitrov, Lange, Tieken, Fehm, & Bom, 2004).

Research has shown that impaired sleep has adverse effects on cognitive

performance, mood, and other physiological and psychological aspects of human

functioning (Alapin et al., 2000; Forbes et al., 2008; Jean-Louis, von Gizycki, Zizi, &

Nunes, 1998; Kahn-Greene, Killgore, Kamimori, Balkin, & Killgore, 2007; Pilcher,

Ginter, & Sadowsky, 1997; Pilcher & Ott, 1998; Tomasi et al., 2009; Van Dongen,

Maislin, Mullington, & Dinges, 2003; Yegneswaran & Shapiro, 2007). A sleep-deprived

person is likely to demonstrate impaired judgment as well as declines in executive

function, working memory and higher-level cognitive functions (Akerstedt, Kecklund,

Alfredsson, & Selen, 2007; Durmer & Dinges, 2005). Engle-Friedman et al. (2003)

reported that sleep loss in college students resulted in choice of behaviors that take a

lesser amount of effort and that those who are sleep deprived do not perceive a reduction

in their effort. These effects in a college setting may lead students to attribute poor
3

academic performance to their own abilities when poor sleep may be the source of

suboptimal functioning.

Relatively mild sleep deprivation, restriction to six hours of sleep per night,

resulted in decrements in neurocognitive task performance during an experimental sleep

deprivation protocol (Van Dongen et al., 2003). Further, insufficient sleep has been

implicated as a contributing factor in motor vehicle accidents and a number of industrial

accidents, including the Chernobyl nuclear disaster and Challenger shuttle explosion

(Lyznicki, Doege, Davis, & Williams, 1998; Mitler et al., 1988; Young, Blustein, Finn, &

Palta, 1997). More recent literature suggests that short sleep duration is also a risk factor

for cardiovascular outcomes, glucose dysregulation, and even death (Ayas et al., 2003;

Gottlieb et al., 2005; Patel et al., 2004).

Though too little sleep has a number of health consequences, many people do not

get enough sleep each night. As more and more is packed into the days, the time allotted

for sleep is curtailed. The 2008 National Sleep Poll, an annual survey of sleep habits of

Americans, found that 44% of Americans average less than seven hours of sleep a night

on work days. Only recently has there been more of a focus placed on the college-aged

population, 44% of whom reported enough severe daytime sleepiness that it interferes

with normal daily functioning (USDHHS, 2003).

Individuals commonly complain about sleep quality; 15-35% of the adult

population complains of frequent disturbances in their sleep quality, such as difficulty

falling asleep and remaining asleep (Bixler, Kales, Soldatos, Kales, & Healey, 1979;

Karacan et al., 1976; Karacan, Thomby, & Williams, 1983; Lugaresi et al., 1983;

Mellinger, Balter, & Uhlenhuth, 1985; Welstein, Dement, Redington, & Guilleminault,
4

1983). Sleep quality includes several aspects of sleep, such as sleep length, sleep latency,

number of awakenings, as well as depth of restfulness of sleep (Buysse, Reynolds, Monk,

Berman, & Kupfer, 1989).

Studies have reported a high prevalence of sleep problems and sleep-related

disturbances among young adults (Ohayon, Roberts, Zulley, Smime, & Priest, 2000;

Racinais, Hue, Blonc, & Le Gallais, 2004; Yang, Wu, Hsieh, Liu, & Lu, 2003). Poor

sleep quality can contribute to problems in many different aspects of a young adult’s life.

College students often report poor sleep quality, irregular sleep habits, and daytime

sleepiness (Alapin et al. 2000; Buboltz et al., 2001; Campos-Morales, Valencia-Flores,

Castano-Meneses, Castaneda-Figueiras, & Martinez-Guerrero, 2005; Carney, Edinger,

Meyer, Lindman, & Istre, 2006; Pilcher, Schoeling, & Prosansky, 2000; Yang et al.,

2003).

Global trends of working more and the increase in domestic and social demands

have resulted in sleep reduction and daytime sleepiness, which has stimulated interest in

conducting research on the benefits of brief naps. Naps as brief as 19.8 (Gillberg,

Kecklund, Axelsson, & Akerstedt, 1996), 10.8 (Home & Ryner, 1996), 10.2 (Takahashi

& Arito, 2000), 10 (Tietzel & Lack, 2001; Tietzel & Lack, 2002), and 9.1 (Hayashi,

Motoyoshi, & Hori, 2005) minutes have been found to improve alertness and

performance in individuals whose sleep was restricted. The improvement in alertness and

performance due to taking a brief nap could also benefit physical performance. In

addition, brief naps in the workplace have been found to be beneficial during a nightshift

(Purnell, Feyer, & Herbison, 2002; Signal & Gander, 2002).


5

Research on meditation has found that the conscious mind may play a role in

enhancing psychological and physical health (Chiesa & Serretti, 2010; Goldin, Ramel, &

Gross, 2009; Perlman, Salomons, Davidson, & Lutz, 2010; Zeidan, Gordan, Merchant, &

Goolkasian, 2010). Autogenic training (Schultz & Luthe, 1959), which produces

sensations of warmth and heaviness in the athlete's body, has been widely used by

European athletes to aid in the control of psycho-physiological activation levels (Cox,

2012).

Statement of the Problem

There are many studies that have examined the effects of physical activity on

sleep. However, few studies have examined the effects of sleep on physical performance.

In addition, many o f those studies had very few participants. Of the few studies

conducted, Racinais and colleagues (2004) found a relationship between sleep

deprivation and poor shuttle run performance in 22 adult athletes. Reilly and Piercy

(1994) determined that there was a significant effect of sleep loss on bench press, leg

press, and dead lift performance with eight male participants. In addition, sprint times

were found to be improved by a post-lunch nap following partial sleep loss with 20

young males (Waterhouse, Atkinson, Edwards, & Reilly, 2007).

The relationship between sleep and physical performance has not been fully

examined. Sleep quality has been found to be more salient to college students’ health, life

satisfaction, levels of tension and depression, and psychological well-being (Pilcher et al.,

1997), as well as general cognitive performance (Pilcher & Walters, 1997). Sleep quality

is known to impact individuals’ physical and psychological health (Pilcher et al., 1997).

A study with female college students in Taiwan found that individuals with poor sleep
6

quality had poorer flexibility, poorer muscular endurance, and lower cardiovascular

fitness compared to individuals with good sleep quality (Lee & Lin, 2007). However,

males were not included in this study, nor were sleep duration and sleepiness examined.

Daytime sleepiness in older adults was associated with physical functional impairments

and decreased exercise frequency (Chasens, Sereika, Weaver, & Umlauf, 2007).

Researchers have examined college students’ sleep habits, sleep patterns, and

sleep difficulties (Buboltz et al., 2001; Caldwell, 2002; Coren, 1994; Lack, 1986;

Trockel, Barnes, & Egget, 2000). Studies have indicated that college students have more

sleep difficulties than the general population (Buboltz et al., 2001; Coren, 1994; Lack,

1986). Sleep habits were the most predictive of college students’ academic performance

out of all health related behaviors (Trockel et al., 2000). In addition, fatigue may be

caused by a slight reduction in sleep duration, especially when paired with an erratic

sleep schedule. Caldwell (2002) found that 75% of college students experiencing fatigue

even when they get seven hours per night of sleep, combined with having a varying

schedule.

Many studies have examined the effects of sleep on academic performance, but

have neglected studying the effects of sleep on physical performance (Curcio, Ferrara, &

De Gennaro, 2005; Gomes, Tavares, & de Azevedo, 2011; Lowry, Dean, & Manders,

2010; Peters, Joireman, & Ridgeway, 2005; Singleton & Wolfson, 2009). However,

many of the same impaired functions disrupted by poor sleep are the same in physical

performance as academic performance. For example, lack of sleep alters physiological

states that affect cognitive functioning, metabolism, inflammation, and neurobehavioral

functioning (Dimitrov et al., 2004).


7

The primary purpose of this study is to determine if a short nap diminishes the

effect of poor sleep on physical performance, as well as to explore the relationship

between sleep quality and physical performance (e.g., flexibility, grip strength, and peak

power) and sleep length and physical performance. More research is needed to clearly

delineate the effects of these sleep variables on the physical performance variables. This

study will add to the existing sleep literature and initiate new knowledge on the

relationships between sleep and physical performance.

Justification

In regard to physical performance, there is little information about the effects of

limited sleep loss (Legar, Metlaine, & Choudat, 2005) and of any effects that naps may

have. Such information would be valuable when there is a requirement for physical

activity in individuals who are suffering from some degree of sleep loss. Additionally, it

is also relevant to athletes, because even small differences in physical performance may

mean the difference in winning or losing. Sleep may have a greater impact on the athlete

when they are in unfamiliar surroundings, feeling apprehensive the night before a

competition or game, or not being accustomed to noise from sports fans. Sleep can also

be affected when there is a time zone change or they have to awaken earlier than usual.

These things can all influence the athlete’s physical performance.

Literature Review

Stages of Sleep

According to the 2007 American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) standards,

individuals cycle through three stages of sleep, known as non-rapid eye movement
8

(NREM) sleep, and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. The NREM stages are referred to

as: N l, N2, and N3. Stage 3 of NREM sleep (N3) is also called delta sleep or slow-wave

sleep (SWS) (Sibler et al., 2007). NREM sleep comprises 75% o f the night, whereas

REM sleep accounts for 25% o f the night. The order that an individual cycles through

stages of sleep is N l, N2, N3, back to N2, and then REM.

Individuals cycle through these stages of sleep on average approximately every 90

to 110 minutes (Caldwell, 2003; Carlson, 1999). Each cycle of REM sleep lasts

approximately 20-30 minutes; therefore, individuals typically experience four or five

cycles of REM sleep per night. Individuals experience a greater amount of deep sleep

(stage N3) earlier in the sleep cycle, but have longer periods of REM sleep later in the

sleep cycle and prior to natural awakening. We enter NREM sleep when we start to fall

asleep.

Upon falling asleep, individuals enter Stage N l sleep as their muscles begin to

relax and there is a loss o f alertness. There is a transition from alpha waves having a

frequency of eight to 13 Hz, which is common in the state of wakefulness, to theta waves

having a frequency of 4-7 Hz, on an EEG (Carlson, 1999). A common sign of disturbed

sleep is a greater percentage of Stage N l sleep (Soldatos & Paparrigopoulos, 2005).

Research indicates that individuals with insomnia often spend more time in stage N l

sleep than those without insomnia (Carskadon & Dement, 2000). Therefore, they usually

obtain more sleep than they realize, but it may not be good quality sleep.

Stage N2 typically lasts for 10 to 25 minutes in young adults (Carlson, 1999).

Stage N2 o f sleep is characterized by sleep spindles and K complexes. Sleep spindles are

short bursts of EEG waves that occur between two and five times per minute during the
9

stages o f non-REM sleep, but predominantly in stage N2. The K complexes are abrupt,

sharp waves. These two types of wave bursts are associated with keeping an individual in

a sleeping state (Bowersox, Kaitlin, & Dement, 1985; Steriade & McCarley, 1990). This

stage accounts for 45-55% of total sleep in adults.

Stage N3 is also known as slow wave sleep and is the deepest level of sleep. In

this stage of sleep, delta waves are present, which are slow, high amplitude waves (less

than 3.5 Hz) that constitute more than 50% of brain waves during this stage (Carlson,

1999). After approximately 45 minutes in stage N3, an individual cycles back to stage N2

(Carlson, 1999). During the second time in stage N2 of sleep, the K complexes and

spindles give way to the rapid eye movements of REM sleep.

REM is the stage of sleep in which people dream and it accounts for 20-25% of

total sleep time in most adults (Carlson, 1999). REM sleep is known as paradoxical sleep

because beta waves are present but the brain paralyzes muscular activity to protect

individuals from injury through acting out dreams. The rapid eye movements will return

to K complexes and spindles return and the individual enters stage two of sleep once

again and repeats the cycle. When this cycle is disrupted individuals may experience

numerous sleep disturbances.

Following total sleep deprivation, REM sleep does not return to normal until the

second night thereafter (Carlson, 1999). This demonstrates how the body places

precedence on the physical health systems which are restored during the first stages of

sleep. Therefore, a person with chronic poor sleep habits may remain physically healthy

while the consequences of REM sleep deprivation (e.g., concentration, learning, and

mood difficulties) may not be readily attributed to inadequate sleep.


10

The utility o f REM sleep is substantiated by studies of selective sleep deprivation.

Researchers have reported that REM sleep is important for consolidating newly learned

information (De Koninck, Lorrain, Christ, Proulx, & Coulombe, 1989; Kami, Tanne,

Rubenstien, Askenasy, & Sagi, 1994; Nesca & Koulack, 1994). Studies of students have

also shown that those deprived of REM sleep perform worse on academic tests than those

who were not deprived (Kami et al., 1994; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996; Pilcher & Ott, 1998;

Pilcher & Walters, 1997; Smith & Lapp, 1991).

Naps. Short daytime naps of less than 30 minutes, which rarely contain Slow-

Wave Sleep (SWS), have been widely reported to have positive effects on daytime

alertness (Takahashi, 2003). The recuperative effects of daytime naps of less than 30

minutes have been confirmed by research. These shorts naps are mainly composed of

stage 1 and stage 2 sleep. Tietzel and Lack (2002) examined whether 30 seconds or 90

seconds of stage 1 sleep had recuperative effects and found that these ultra-brief naps had

no recuperative power. They also found that a 10-minute brief nap had a recuperative

effect, suggesting that the recuperative power of a short nap depends on stage 2 sleep, not

stage 1. However, they did not show the sleep variables of the 10-minute nap. A study by

Hayashi and colleagues (2005) examined whether the recuperative effects of short naps

depend upon which o f the sleep stages are present in the nap (i.e., stage 1 or 2 sleep).

Participants in this study were 10 university students. The results suggest that stage 2

sleep has recuperative power, while these recuperative effects are limited in stage 1 sleep.

It has been reported that the recuperative effects of sleep depend on SWS and that this is

also the case for daytime naps (Mednick, Nakayama, & Stickgold, 2003; Tilley,

Donohoe, & Hensby, 1987).


Short naps of approximately 10 minutes do not contain SWS, suggesting that

stage 2 sleep during a daytime nap, independent of SWS, has recuperative effects.

However, delta- and theta-band EEG activities during the waking-sleeping transition

period intensify rapidly during stage 2 sleep (Morikawa, Hayashi, & Hori, 1997). It could

be argued that background EEG delta activity during stage 2 sleep might contribute to the

recuperative effects of the daytime short nap. Spectral analysis of EEG during the nap

might be required. The study by Hayashi and colleagues (2005) confirmed that stage 2

sleep plays an important role in the restorative function of sleep and that a minimum of

three minutes of stage 2 sleep had recuperative effects of daytime alertness and

performance after restricted nocturnal sleep, while these effects were limited in stage 1

sleep.

Physiological Processes of the Sleep-Wake Cycle

There are three main physiological processes that directly affect the sleep-wake

cycle (National Institute of Health, 1998; Refinetti, 2000). They are the autonomic

nervous system, homeostatic drive for sleep, and circadian rhythm (National Institute of

Health, 1998; Refinetti, 2000). These are especially important, because sleep difficulties

typically result from a disruption in any of these processes.

Autonomic Nervous System. The autonomic nervous system is a subcomponent

o f the peripheral nervous system that controls individual’s level of arousal in reaction to

both environmental and internal stimuli. The autonomic system is further broken down

into the sympathetic system, which activates vital systems of the human body in response

to a stressor or anticipated danger and the parasympathetic system, which relaxes the

body. Sleep is associated with increased parasympathetic and decreased sympathetic


12

functioning (Parmeggiani, 1994). Electrical stimulation of parasympathetic activating

systems in the brain results in behavioral signs of sleeps in mammals. Specifically,

stimulation of the anterior hypothalamic and preoptic areas of the brain produces

parasympathetic signs such as a slower heart rate, lower blood pressure, and constriction

of the pupils (Parmeggiani, 1994).

On the other hand, many aspects of the sympathetic nervous system’s response to

stressors can be extremely disruptive to sleep. This response can occur just as readily to

imagined stressors as real environmental stressors. In fact, even the frustrations

associated with transient difficulties falling asleep or worries that develop as people are

trying to fall asleep can significantly impair the ability to fall asleep or stay asleep

(Parmeggiani, 1994). While most people occasionally have difficulty sleeping, it is when

such difficulties become behaviorally associated with the bedroom that chronic sleep

problems may develop. As a result, transient sleep difficulties can turn into chronic

insomnia (Hauri, 1991).

In summary, the autonomic nervous system plays a strong role in the ability to fall

and stay asleep. This system is organized to react immediately and can rapidly impair

sleep (Parmeggiani, 1994). However, autonomic relaxation is not the only factor that can

influence sleep readiness. This is important since people do not want to fall asleep any

time they begin to feel relaxed. On the other hand, people who have not slept for several

days may fall asleep relatively quickly even if under stress. The homeostatic drive thus

plays a key role in sleep propensity and can help regulate the sleep cycle.

Homeostatic Drive. The longer people remain awake the sleepier they become

(Carskadon & Dement, 1981). Periods of sleep deprivation result in shorter sleep onset
13

and even more rapid entry into deep sleep (Roehrs, Carskadon, Dement, & Roth, 2000).

In people without sleep disorders the need for sleep can often be inferred by the length of

time it takes to fall asleep or sleep latency. In addition to experimental evidence, this

makes intuitive sense and is most likely what contributed to Aristotle’s hypnotoxin theory

of sleep (Thorpy, 1991). While the term toxic may be a little strong, it does appear that

certain substances accumulate in the body while awake and are reduced during sleep.

Such substances promote sleepiness and are partially responsible for the decreased sleep

latency after sleep deprivation. When the body and brain are in the awakened stage,

ongoing metabolic activity breaks down adenosine monophosphate (AMP) into

adenosine. Adenosine is accumulated as the day progresses. This buildup of adenosine

leads to inhibition of the basal forebrain, part of the brain responsible for alertness. As

adenosine levels increase, the propensity to maintain alertness decreases, resulting in

increased drowsiness and sleep (Porkka-Heiskanen, 1999). While asleep, the metabolic

activity responsible for the buildup of adenosine decreases, thereby, allowing the body to

return to lower adenosine levels and increase alertness.

Additionally, type d2 prostaglandins, are endogenous hormones that promote

sleep. Prostaglandins (d2) stimulate neurons that inhibit the arousal response of certain

hypothalamic cells (Scamel et al., 1998). These chemicals are present throughout most of

the body. Prostaglandins are a natural by-product of immune system activity. Normally,

healthy people produce prostaglandins, but they are produced at a faster rate when the

immune system is fighting infection. This is why people often feel sleepier when ill. The

increased levels of prostaglandins that are produce while an individual is fighting an

infection tends to override the normal sleep-wake cycle and may lead to sleep at
14

inappropriate times. Additionally, prostaglandins (d2) may also play a key role in

promoting sleep in a person who has been awake for an extended period of time (Ram et

al., 1997), as their levels of prostaglandins are relatively high since they have not slept

and given the body time to reduce the level of prostaglandins.

While the autonomic nervous system can influence people’s ability to fall asleep

by affecting levels of arousal (Parmeggiani, 1994), the homeostatic processes plays a

strong role in the tendency to fall asleep. However, entering a state of relaxation after a

night of sleep deprivation still does not ensure eight hours of restful sleep outside of the

normal sleep-wake schedule (Parmeggiani, 1994). Timing that is consistent with the

circadian rhythm or sleep-wake cycle, is the third factor required for good sleep (Cohen

& Albers, 1991; Gillette & McArthur, 1996).

Circadian Rhythm. The natural environment provides living organisms with a

variety of external stimuli, many of which follow a rhythmic pattern (e.g., the lunar-tidal,

solar-daily, and seasonal-yearly patterns of light). Such patterns are quite predictable, and

animals have adapted their physiology to contend with these environmental changes

(Strubbe & Woods, 2004). Humans too have adapted to the natural rhythms in the

environment. Similar to most other animals, human beings have a number of biological

rhythms that serve as internal clocks. These rhythms vary in length, including annual

rhythms (e.g., the secretion of testosterone in males) and monthly rhythms (e.g., the

menstrual cycle in women). However, many biological rhythms are circadian rhythms

that follow approximately 24-hour cycles. Sleep follows a circadian rhythm, but one to

which human beings continue to adapt. The natural sleep-wake cycle follows a rhythm

that is closer to a 25-hour cycle, and thus must be reset on a daily basis. The normal
15

period of inactivity starts several hours after dark and persists for some time into the

daylight portion of the day (Boivin, Duffy, Kronauer, & Czeisler, 1994). Our internal

clock is re-synchronized to a 24-hour cycle by external cues such as light. These external

cues are commonly referred to as zeitgebers (German for “time givers”) (Aschoff, 1979).

It has been shown with several species of animals (including humans) that a brief period

of bright light will reset circadian rhythms after a maintained period of constant darkness

(Aschoff, 1979). With the help of sunlight as a natural zeitgeber, human beings have

adapted to a 24-hour sleep-wake cycle.

The portion of the brain that appears primarily to be responsible for regulation of

circadian rhythms is the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SN), which is located in the

hypothalamus (Refinetti & Menaker, 1992). The SN controls patterns of sleeping and

waking primarily by regulation of the secretion of the hormone melatonin from the pineal

gland. The SN becomes less active a few hours before a person’s regular bedtime, while

the pineal gland begins to release melatonin into the blood stream, causing the person to

become sleepy within about an hour (Cajochen, Kraeuchi, & Wirz-Justice, 1997). Near a

person’s normal waking time, production of melatonin decreases and the person becomes

more alert. The SN follows a seasonal rhythm as well, secreting larger amounts of

melatonin during the winter to account for the longer periods of darkness (Ralph &

Lehman, 1991). If the SN is damaged, the flow of melatonin goes largely unregulated.

Subsequently, people with such damage have difficulties maintaining alertness during the

day (Cohen & Albers, 1991).

The SN receives messages from a bundle of nerves called the retinohypothalamic

tract, which extends from the non-visual photoreceptors in the retina of the eye. Refinetti
16

and Menaker (1992) that when this bundle of nerves is damaged, circadian rhythms

become free running, and resistant to zeitgebers. However, the retinohypothalamic track

appears to be able to receive input from non-visual photoreceptors in the skin as well as

the retina. Campbell and Murphy (1998) found that when a bright light was focused

behind the knees (without contact with the eyes) of human subjects that were kept in dim

light for several days, circadian rhythms of the participants were reset. Circadian rhythms

did not reset in the control group, which did not receive the light treatment.

Another circadian rhythm in humans that has been closely linked with sleep is

body temperature rhythm. It is easiest to fall asleep when body temperature is near its low

point (Campbell & Zulley, 1989). During sleep, muscular activity is almost non-existent.

Thus, a major source o f body heat is basically turned off, and body heat production is

reduced. While heat production is down, cooling production continues. The shivering

response to cooler temperatures is not active during sleep, yet the sweating response

remains, and can promote cooling. These processes help maintain the general decline in

body temperature during sleep (Hobson, 1995). During REM sleep, central temperature

control is essentially lost and reliant upon the environment or arousal to maintain stable

body temperature. It is believed neurons that control heat in the hypothalamus are rested

during REM sleep to enable them to control temperature more effectively during

wakefulness (Parmeggiani, 1977). This theory is further supported by sleep deprivation

studies that have shown that prolonged sleep deprivation generally results in a significant

drop in body temperature (Home, 1988). A study of extreme sleep deprivation in rats

indicates that if an animal is deprived of sleep for long enough, the animal apparently will

die from complications resulting from the loss o f ability to conserve body heat
17

(Rechtschaffen, Gilliand, Bergmann, & Winter, 1983). The precise relationship between

thermoregulation and sleep remains unclear, but it is evident that body temperature

relates to the sleep-wake cycle, and that sleep may be crucial for thermo regular

maintenance.

Circadian rhythms are of varying importance for human being and animal

functioning. However, circadian rhythms can become desynchronized with those in the

external environment when people change their daily routines, which can result in sleep

disturbances (Carlson, 1999). Modem society often has demands that do not match

natural circadian rhythms. This is evident in people who perform shift work and must

change from working during the day to night. Night shift workers often drive home

during the morning daylight. Since daylight is a natural zeitgeber, this may cause sleep

difficulties. Additionally, night workers often attempt to shift back into a daytime

schedule during days off, causing further circadian rhythm disruptions, and making it

utterly impossible to adjust fully to one schedule of sleep and wakefulness (Akerstedt,

1988). Poor daytime sleep and circadian rhythm disruption have been linked to poor job

performance, higher rates of traffic accidents, and health complications (Monk, Folkard,

& Wedderbum, 1996). Studies have also shown a significant relationship between shift

work and health complications, including headaches, gastric problems, and chronic

cardiovascular difficulties (Parkes, 1999; Smith, Folkard, & Fuller, 2003). Additionally,

Mitler and colleagues (1988) have found that engineering and industrial disasters happen

most frequently between the hours of midnight and 6:00 a.m.


18

The Theories of Sleep Function

Approximately one-third of our lives are spent sleep; therefore, it must serve an

important purpose. However, we do not know all of the benefits of sleep. There are

different theories o f sleep function, including the restoration model, the neurotransmitter

replenishment theory, the evolutionary/circadian sleep models, the developmental model,

and the learning model.

The Restoration Model. The restoration model suggests that we need to sleep in

order to recover from daily physical activity (Hess, 1965; Walker, 2008). If this theory is

correct, it would mean that the activity that wears down the body should increase sleep.

Research conducted on sleep-deprivation supports this view, indicating that if we want to

function at our best, we need sleep. A study of ultra-marathon runners, ages 18-26 years,

found that they slept much longer and spend a greater percentage of time in slow-wave

sleep on the two nights following their 57-mile run (Shapiro, Bortz, Mitchell, Bartel, &

Jooste, 1981). A meta-analysis of 38 studies found that on the days that individuals

exercised, they tend to sleep approximately 10 minutes longer (Youngstedt, O’Conner, &

Dishman, 1997).

It is not precisely known what gets restored during sleep, but researchers report

that a cellular waste product called adenosine plays a role (Alam et al., 2009). During

sleep, it is thought that neurotoxins are neutralized and tissue restitution occurs (Adam &

Oswald, 1977; Hartmann, 1973). Another possibility is that sleep evolved as a means for

the body to conserve energy during the night to aid in thermoregulation during the coldest

part of the day (Hobson, 1995).


19

The Neurotransmitter Replenishment Theory. The neurotransmitter

replenishment theory suggests that aminergic neurons stop firing during sleep to allow

their neurotransmitter supply to regenerate (Hobson, 1995). Neurotransmitters may be

depleted if there is a limited supply, because neurons continuously fire during

wakefulness. Most neurons in the body show decreases in activity during sleep.

Aminergic neurons that release norepinephrine and serotonin stop firing altogether which

provides a resting period so that these neurotransmitters can regenerate. It is hypothesized

that aminergic neurons continue to generate norepinephrine and serotonin while resting.

This allows these neurotransmitters, believed to be involved in learning and memory

(Siegel & Rogawski, 1988), to create a reserve necessary to assist in cognitive activities

the next day.

The Evolutionary/Circadian Sleep Models. The evolutionary/circadian sleep

models emphasize that the main purpose of sleep is to increase an individual’s chance of

survival in relation to environmental demands (Webb, 1974). Prehistorically, individuals

were not active at night, because it would have been more dangerous to be hunting, food

gathering, and traveling at night. Over the course of evolution, each species developed a

circadian sleep-wake pattern that was adaptive in terms of its status as predator or prey,

its food requirements, and its methods of defense from attack. Sleep may also have

evolved as a mechanism to conserve energy. The body’s metabolic rate is approximately

9-25% slower during sleep than during wakefulness (Reich, Geyer, & Kamovsky, 1972;

Wouters-Adriaens & Westerterp, 2006).

The Developmental Model. The developmental model suggests that sleep has a

vital role in brain development (Carlson, 1999; Hobson, 1995). Current theories focus on
20

REM sleep because of its prominence in utero and infancy (Hobson, 1995). The fetus

spends up to 80% of its time in REM sleep (Inoue et al., 1986; Petre-Quadens & DeLee,

1974; Roffwarg, Muzio, & Dement, 1966). This falls to 70% during early infancy and

further declines to 30% at six months of age. This corresponds to the most active period

of brain development. It is hypothesized that the activation during REM sleep provides an

opportunity for the brain to practice future behaviors (Roffwarg et al., 1966). When fetal

lambs were observed through Plexiglas windows implanted in the uterine wall, they were

seen making chest breathing movements during REM sleep during brain development;

they fail to explain the reason REM sleep is present in adults.

The Learning Model. The learning model suggests that the main function of

sleep is to facilitate memory consolidation and integration (Carlson, 1999; Greenberg &

Pearlman, 1974). Animal studies have suggested that REM sleep aids learning (Carlson,

1999). Animals deprived of REM sleep during training were found to learn tasks more

slowly (Smith, 1996). Daytime learning in humans also appears to be hindered by lack of

sleep (Hobson, 1995). Steriade and McCarley (1990) found that as neural pathways are

strengthened, a new protein structure is created, which in turn allows new information to

be stored in the brain. Therefore, a new memory is formed. Due to the instability of the

new protein structure, it must be refreshed. The new neural pathways formed by learning

information appear to be activated during REM sleep. This could be a reason REM sleep

is crucial to learning and memory. It has been suggested that during REM sleep, the brain

is aroused and begins to process information stored in memory which may reinforce both

new and old memories (Antrobus, 1986). Students who demonstrated a significant

increase in REM sleep followed a period of intense learning were found to perform
21

significantly better on an examination than students who did not show an increase in

REM sleep (De Koninck et al., 1989).

The relationship between REM sleep and the integration of new information into

long-term memory is not limited to the period immediately following learning. Research

suggests this relationship extends well beyond the night the information was originally

integrated (Buboltz et al., 2006). Smith and Lapp (1991) found that after students studied

for final exams, during the five-day period following exams, REM sleep actually

increased. As REM sleep increased, there was no concomitant increase in amount of

sleep, nor was there an increase in number of REM sleep periods. These findings suggest

that information was being integrated into long-term memory well after the exam.

Because the increases in REM density occurred later in the evening, those individuals

getting a full night of sleep profited from the increase in memory consolidation occurring

after the initial learning period (Buboltz et al., 2006; Smith & Lapp, 1991). Therefore,

students who cram for exams (i.e. study for long periods immediately prior to an exam)

may miss out on the potential benefits of several nights of increased REM sleep.

In addition to the potential benefits of several nights of increased REM sleep,

students who cram for exams by staying up all night also suffer a complete lack of REM

sleep and the consequence this has on memory integration. Research suggests that lack of

REM sleep coupled with sleep loss and/or poor sleep quality, are associated with

significant impairments in cognitive functioning (Buboltz et al., 2006). Logical reasoning

and decision-making in adults were demonstrated to be impacted be a single night of

sleep loss (Blagrove & Akehurst, 2001; Harrison & Home, 1999). Sleep deprivation also

appears to have a negative effect on working memory (short-term memory used for
22

thinking and problem-solving). Sleep loss due to poor sleep quality is also linked to poor

cognitive functioning, false recall of recently learned words, and decreased visuomotor

skill (Roediger & McDermott, 1995).

Sleep Difficulties of College Students

Most studies exploring the sleep habits and difficulties of college students focus

on describing general sleep difficulties rather than supplying information about the

prevalence of diagnosis. Considering that these studies report results recorded primarily

through self-report, describing rather than diagnosing is more appropriate.

From 1969 through 1989, there was a notable decline in the average length of

time students’ sleep. Hicks and Pellegrini (1991) point out that the median hours of

students sleep was 7.75 hours in 1969, 7.13 hours in 1979 and 6.75 hours in 1989. In

1969, 9% o f students reported their average sleep duration as less than 6.5 hours of sleep.

However, in 1979,26.9% of students reported receiving less than 6.5 hours sleep. The

amount of sleep continued to decline through 1989, during which 41.5% of students

reported consistently receiving less than 6.5 hours of sleep. This is supported by re­

analyzing the data found in the Hicks and Pellegrini’s (1991) study which demonstrates

that the mean hours of sleep was fairly close to the median hours reported. In 1969 mean

hours of sleep was 7.50 (SD = .92), in 1979 the mean was 7.18 (SD = 1.67) and in 1989

the mean was 6.8 (SD = 1.01). While these findings do not include sleep quality indices,

they certainly present an alarming trend in college students sleep habits. Further, this

trend discounts the idea that poor sleep is simply a part of students’ lives - it was not for

the majority of students in 1969.


23

Hicks and Pellegrini’s (1991) study is not the first to demonstrate that sleep

difficulties are becoming an increasing problem for college students. Over thirty years

ago, Domino and Fogl (1980) noted their surprise at the lack of normative sleep studies

for college students and thus proceeded to explore sleep habits and patterns of college

students. They developed and administered a sleep scale they called the SQ 51, which

was administered to random sample of undergraduate students that consisted of 60 males

and 71 females. The results indicated that 7% used medication for sleep difficulties, 13%

reported their sleep was usually restless, and 39% reported that they often had highly

irregular sleep schedules (Domino & Fogl, 1980).

Several years later, in a sample of Australian college students, Lack (1986) found

that the most commonly reported symptoms were difficulty falling asleep (18%), early

morning awakening (13.2%), general sleep difficulties (12.8%), and difficulty staying

asleep (9%). Only 8% reported never having had sleep difficulties. Notably, when sleep

complaints were examined for each student, 17% of the students had symptoms

consistent with Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome (DSPS). This is more than twice the

estimated prevalence for the general population, which is about 6-7% (American

Psychiatric Association, 1994, Lack, 1986). Further, it is noteworthy that students who

met the criteria for Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome had lower grades, greater feelings of

drowsiness, and irritability when compared to the rest of the sample. In addition to the

general findings, older subjects reported less sleep during the week, reduced perceived

need of sleep, and greater drowsiness during the day. Younger subjects reported more

variability in their sleep habits, longer sleep onset and greater difficulty falling asleep.

Females reported a greater perceived need for sleep, but reported less total sleep time in
24

general and received less adequate sleep on weekends. Females also indicated more

frequent naps, greater irritability, and were more likely to describe themselves as

insomniacs than males. There were no significant gender differences for sleep onset time,

difficulty falling asleep, difficulty staying asleep, or sleep depth (Lack, 1986).

Brown and colleagues (2001) found that a sample of US college students also

reported a higher prevalence of Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome than the general

population. These results showed a lower prevalence rate of DSPS than the Australian

sample but higher rates o f some specific sleep complaints. Sleep difficulties most

commonly reported “frequently” or “almost always” were early morning awakenings

(25.5%), general sleep difficulties (21.9%) difficulty falling asleep (19.5%), daytime

napping (15.1%), and difficulty staying asleep (10.9%). Generally, however, the findings

support a consistent pattern of poor sleep habits in college students in these two

geographically diverse samples. Students in the US sample went to bed later on the

weekend than the week and received less sleep during the week. Additionally, females

reported more frequent difficulties falling asleep and staying asleep than males.

Buboltz and colleagues (2001) found that only 11% of college students reported

symptoms consistent with good sleep quality, 73% of the sample reported moderate sleep

problems, and 15% of the sample had symptoms consistent with poor sleep quality,

according to a standardized sleep quality measure. Compared to the general adult

population, these students reported 60% greater incidence of poor sleep quality, than the

original standardization sample of working adults.

Another study (Hawkins & Shaw, 1992) examined sleep logs at three different,

week-long periods during the academic semester. The sleep logs were comprised of a
25

seven-item questionnaire that included items regarding the amount o f time in bed, level

o f satisfaction with sleep and questions related to sleep quality and quantity. The amount

o f time in bed declined as the semester progressed and the demands placed on

respondents increased. However, with the decreased time in bed, also came a decrease in

the amount of nocturnal awakenings. At the same time, students rating of sleep quality

did not change throughout the semester. It should be noted that the authors’ definition of

sleep quality was based on one item that asked, basically how good their sleep was. Thus,

these sleep quality findings are difficult to compare with other studies since researchers

usually include the length of time to fall asleep, number of nocturnal awakenings,

morning status, and general sleep satisfaction in their definitions of sleep quality. Despite

some of the limitations with certain aspects of their study, it is clear that there was a

noteworthy decrease in sleep length as the semester progressed.

Brown, Buboltz, and Soper (2002) explored the combined relationship of sleep

quality, quantity, and sleepiness on students’ academic performance. They found that

students’ ratings of sleepiness became higher as the academic quarter progressed. There

was also a reduction in the length of sleep from the beginning (M = 8.26 hours) to the end

(M - 6.81 hours) of the quarter. Using the Sleep Quality Index (Urponen, Partinen,

Vuori, & Hasan, 1991) 16.7% of the students were classified as poor sleepers, while

about 12% had symptoms consistent with Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome. Only 8.9% of

those surveyed reported that their sleep was free from difficulties. The prevalence of

symptoms was similar in this study to previous prevalence studies in college students.

After reviewing the prevalence of sleep difficulties in students, especially their

inconsistent sleep wake schedules, it seems likely that discrepancies between students’
26

social and academic schedules may negatively impact sleep quality. There is, in fact,

empirical support for this contention. Students who were able to sleep late in the morning

due to classes scheduled later in the day had more consistent sleep-wake schedules than

those required to awaken for early morning classes. Further, students who had morning

responsibilities showed evidence of partial sleep deprivation during the week. These

results indicate that students may have healthier sleep habits if they are able to take

classes and work later in the day (Machado, Varella, & Andrade, 1998).

That a high percentage of college students have poor sleep quality and habits is

probably not a surprise for most students and faculty. However, it is surprising that

despite both the empirically demonstrated high prevalence of sleep difficulties and

intuitive sense that students do not sleep well, there do not seem to be any studies that

focus on developing interventions to reduce student sleep difficulties. Nor does there

appear to be many studies suggesting that colleges and universities are concerned about

students sleep habits.

Factors that Affect Sleep in College Students

Over the past 25 years, there have been numerous studies exploring the sleep

habits, patterns, and difficulties of college students. Studies on sleep deprivation,

variation, and reduction have illuminated the numerous aversive effects that poor sleep

quality and habits can have on students (Dinges, Whitehouse, Ome, & Orne, 1988; Kami

et al., 1994; Kelly, Kelly, & Clanton, 2001; Lack, 1986; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996;

Pilcher & Walters, 1997). Sleep difficulties take a variety of forms. There is a plethora of

potential contributing factors that are salient in a college student population which may
27

impact their sleep habits and quality and in turn interfere with their functioning in a

number of areas.

For many late adolescents, beginning college is a major life transition. Often this

involves living away from family and friends for the first time. Depending upon the

intensity, level of emotional preparation, and the relationship between the student and

parents, separation from home can be a particularly difficult transition for many students

(Hoffman & Weiss, 1987; Lopez, Campbell, & Watkins, 1988; Rice, Cole, & Lapsley,

1990). A host of other factors have been identified that can affect social life and

adjustment to college, including level of self-esteem (Bettencourt, Charlton, Eubanks,

Kemahan, & Fuller, 1999), perfectionistic tendencies (Chang & Rand, 2000), and

masculinity and femininity traits (Sharpe & Heppner, 1991). Although there are many

individual factors that can affect adjustment, the research ultimately suggests that for

many the transition to college life can be a very difficult period.

College is also a time o f other transitions. Students are given responsibility for

various aspects of their lives, including sleep habits and patterns. Although many young

adults relish their autonomy and independence, they nevertheless are subjected to

substantial social and academic demands (Kleeman & Richardson, 1985; Russell &

Petire, 1992). Many college freshmen live away from their families for the first time and

may be involved with a barrage of social activities, such as fraternities and sororities, and

academic and athletic clubs. This is often in the midst of adjusting to new surroundings,

making new acquaintances, choosing a class schedule, and meeting the challenge of

college academic requirements. Given the varying schedules to which college students
28

are often subjected, it is not surprising that this population encounters a great deal of

sleep difficulties.

The demands of college adjustment bring potential for high levels of stress. Stress

and worry have been linked to poor sleep quality (McCann & Stewin, 1987). Sleep and

stress among college students appears to bring potential for a spiraling effect as changes

in sleep patterns, in and of themselves, are often identified as stressful. A study by Ross,

Neibling, and Hechert (1999) investigated 40 potential stressful situations that are

commonly faced by college students, including interpersonal, intrapersonal, academic,

and environmental sources o f stress. Of the 40 potential stressors, change in sleeping

habits was identified as one of the top five sources of stress for college students.

While many students may stay up late on weekends to engage in social events,

they also frequently engage in late night study sessions to meet academic responsibilities,

with some students reporting depriving themselves of sleep for 24-48 hours around

examination periods (Hawkins & Shaw, 1992). Since many students may be able to

identify changes in sleep habits as a source of stress, they may seek ways to combat this

potential stressor. A common method used to compensate for lost sleep during the week

is to sleep later on the weekends in an attempt to “catch up on sleep” (Machado et al.,

1998; Pilcher & Walters, 1997). Unfortunately, this type of variable sleep pattern is

conducive to a condition known as Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome (DSPS), (Brown et

al., 2001; Lack, 1986). DSPS is a circadian rhythm disorder resulting in excessive

morning drowsiness and difficulty falling asleep on weeknights.

Another factor related to sleep difficulties among college students is the use of

alcohol and sleep medication. Research indicates that the overall prevalence of binge
29

drinking (five or more drinks in a row) among college students is approximately 40-45%

(O’Malley & Johnston, 2002; Wechsler, Davenport, Dowdall, Moeykens, & Castillo,

1994; Wechsler et al., 2002; Wechsler & Nelson, 2008). Alcohol can expedite sleep onset

and, depending on the quantity, can increase the amount of slow wave sleep a person

obtains. However, the effects are paradoxical in that they actually decrease the amount of

REM sleep, and can be associated with insomnia (Roehrs & Roth, 1997). While students

who binge drink may believe they are benefiting from alcohol induced sleep because they

fall asleep faster and may sleep longer, alcohol can inhibit the most important stage of

sleep. Furthermore, students who reported drinking more alcoholic beverages fell asleep

in class more often than those who drank less (Jean-Louis et al., 1998). Sleep medications

also can have paradoxical effects. Students who choose to use sleep medications for sleep

difficulties are in danger of exacerbating the problem. While sleep medications often

leave individuals feeling less than alert due to the residual effects of the drug, most over

the counter sleep medications can also inhibit REM sleep. Even short-term use of sleep

medications can result in rebound insomnia (insomnia symptoms that are more severe

than the originals) upon discontinuation of the medication (Roehrs, Vogel, & Roth,

1990). Pharmacological treatments have demonstrated little, if any, effectiveness in

treating long-term sleep problems (Morin & Wooten, 1996).

While some college students use medication to induce sleep at night, others use

stimulants to stay awake during the day. The most common stimulant among college

students is undoubtedly caffeine (Gilbert, 1986). One study found that 42% of college

students drank coffee and 29% drank tea on a regular basis (Mathieson, Faris, Stam, &

Egger, 1992). Even in fairly mild doses (100-150 mg., about one cup of brewed coffee)
30

caffeine has been shown to cause sleep disturbances, including delayed sleep onset,

reduced sleep time, increased number of spontaneous awakenings, and increased amounts

of light sleep (Caldwell, 2003; Pressman & Orr, 1997). The duration of caffeine activity

in adults is 3-5 hours, but people can experience effects for up to 10 hours. Thus, even

one cup of coffee in the afternoon can disrupt sleep, but this may depend on the

individual’s sensitivity to caffeine (Nehlig, Daval, & Debry, 1992). People who use

caffeine on a regular basis may develop a tolerance to the drug, and be less likely to have

sleep difficulties than people that use caffeine occasionally. However, frequent caffeine

users tend to have more sleep disruptions than non-users (Roehrs & Roth, 1997).

The use of illicit stimulants is also common among college students. Low and

Gendaszek (2002) found that 35% of students from a small liberal arts college reported

using prescription amphetamines at least once in the past year without a prescription.

Similarly, 34% of the sample reported using cocaine or methylenedioxymethampheta­

mine (MDMA) in the past year, and 8% reported using at least once a week. While the

full effects of illicit stimulants are unknown, oftentimes these substances have much

longer lasting effects than milder stimulants such as caffeine, and thus have potential to

cause even greater sleep difficulties.

Cigarette smoking may be related to sleep difficulties. A total of 28% of college

smokers report they began smoking regularly after reaching college age (Wechsler,

Rigotti, Glendhill-Hoyt, & Lee, 1998). Considering the massive amounts of negative

exposure in the media and warning labels directly on packages, it is probably safe to

assume that college students are aware of the potential detrimental health effects of

smoking (i.e., cardiovascular disease, emphysema, lung cancer, etc.). However, students
31

may not be cognizant of the fact that nicotine is a stimulant. While, ironically, heavy

smoking (more than 15 cigarettes a day) does not appear to affect sleep, light smoking

has recently been linked with insomnia (Riedel, Durrence, Lichstein, Taylor, & Bush,

2004). Perhaps light smokers have not built up a tolerance to the drug, resulting in a

stronger reaction to the stimulatory effect. Thus, students who have recently begun

smoking and students who only smoke occasionally may be suffering from symptoms of

insomnia and be completely unaware of this potential source of the problem.

Effects of Inadequate Sleep Length

In a sample o f healthy adults (ages 21-38 years), restriction to four or six hours of

sleep duration per night over two weeks resulted in significant and cumulative deficits in

performance on cognitive tasks. These results of chronic sleep deprivation were

comparable to those found with total sleep deprivation for one to two days (Van Dongen

et al., 2003). Individuals who are sleep deprived demonstrate impaired judgments and are

more likely to make mistakes. Research has shown that an individual who stays awake

for 24 hours has a reduction in hand-eye coordination similar to an individual who had a

blood alcohol content o f 0.08% (Maruff, Falleti, Collie, Darby, & McStephen, 2005) and

cognitive impairment similar to an individual with blood alcohol content o f 0.05%

(Falleti, Maruff, Collie, Darby, & McStephen, 2003).

Students who sleep for less than six hours per night are prone to miss one or more

o f their REM sleep periods, because REM sleep occurs in the latter part of an eight hour

sleep period at this developmental stage in young adulthood (Kami et al., 1994; Smith &

Lapp, 1991). Supporting that REM sleep is vital to memory consolidation of newly
32

learned material, students who receive more REM sleep demonstrate better learning

efficiency (De Koninck et al., 1989).

Students who do not get a full night of sleep may be unknowingly weakening

their ability to learn new information. Also, students who do not sleep as well the night

before an exam will more likely demonstrate a decrease in test performance (De Koninck

et al., 1989; Pilcher & Ott, 1998). Additionally, college students who sleep less than six

hours per night or more than nine hours per night are more likely to have mild depressive

symptoms compared to students sleeping between six and nine hours per night (Buela-

Casal, Miro, Ianez, & Catena, 2007).

Sleep and Health

Research suggests that there is a relationship between sleep and quality/length and

physical health. One study liked chronic insomnia to coronary artery disease (Bonnet &

Arand, 1998). In a sample of over 5000 individuals over the age of 65 a link was found

between poor sleep quality and decreased physical healthy including cardiovascular

disease (Asplund, 2000; Newman, Enright, Manolio, & Haponik, 1997). Sleep difficulties

were also linked to cardiovascular disease in a sample of adults (Appels & Mulder,

1984).

One sleep disorder in particular that can lead to health difficulties is sleep

disordered breathing (e.g. sleep apnea). Mild sleep disordered breathing has been shown

to be associated with an increase in complaints of bodily pain in children as young as five

years o f age (Rosen, Palermo, Larkin, & Redline, 2002). Mild sleep disordered breathing

has also been associated with lower scores on a health-related quality o f life measure in a

sample of five to 17 year olds as well as a sample of 30 to 60 year olds (Finn, Young,
33

Palta, & Fryback, 1998). Mild sleep disordered breathing is even associated with other

chronic conditions such as arthritis, angina, hypertension, diabetes, and back problems.

In a sample of 5,090 Japanese white collar workers poor sleepers were more

likely to take sick leave, suffer physically and psychologically poor health and have

problems in work related activities and personal relationships (Doi, Minowa, & Tango,

2003). The specific factor that was related to poor sleep quality was hypertension.

Edell-Gustafsson (2002) also examined the relationship between sleep and health

in a sample of 40 men ages 45-70 who were about to undergo a coronary artery bypass

surgery in Sweden. He found that pooper overall physical health was significantly related

to longer sleep latency. These individuals displayed a decreased percentage of phases

three and four sleep.

Individuals who reported poor sleep quality also endorsed more physical illness

(Lund, Reider, Whiting, & Prichard, 2010). In this study, 12% of poor sleepers reported

missing class three times or more in the last month due to illness compared to only 4% of

the rest of the sample. In a sample of junior high students, a relationship was found

between sleep quality and physical health with participants who reported poor sleep

quality also reported a higher number of illnesses and lower scores on measures of

general health (Tanaka et al, 2003). In a community sample, any sleep problems,

inadequate sleep, difficulty falling asleep, and persistent nightmares were all associated

with higher rates of migraines (Vgontzas, Cui, & Merikangas, 2008).

There has been growing interest in sleep quality and quantity as they relate to

obesity. One longitudinal study found that in young adults (under age 40 years) there was

a negative correlation between sleep length and BMI (Hasler et al., 2004). This finding
34

has very important implications' for college students who have been shown to have a

higher degree of sleep difficulties than the general public. If this contributes to a higher

rate of obesity, this could mean that college students are at a higher risk for numerous

health complications that result from obesity.

Many early studies examined the relationship between sleep and health from a

biomedical model (Parmeggiani, 1977; Roffwarg et al., 1966; Sassin et al., 1969; Webb

& Agnew, 1974). This model purports that health and illness are a function of biology, or

that poor health is always the result of pathogens (Engel, 1977). Additionally, the

biomedical model biological processes are essentially independent from psychological

and social processes. However, recent research has shown that physiological health can

be greatly impacted by psychological factors (e.g., placebo effects), and that

psychological health can influence physiological health (e.g., biological illness resulting

in depression). Thus, psychologists (and many physicians alike) have begun to approach

the topic of health from a much broader biopsychosocial model. This model suggests that

health can best be explained by examining biological processes, psychological processes,

and social influences (Peterson, 1997). In this section, the relationship between sleep and

health will be examined from each of the components of the biopsychosocial model.

Among the most common sleep disorders is insomnia, with prevalence estimates

that range between 15% to 20% for chronic insomnia, and 30% to 40% for transient or

occasional insomnia (Mellinger et al., 1985). Chronic insomnia has been linked with the

coronary artery disease (Bonnet & Arand, 1998). In addition to adversely affecting

physiological health, insomnia can also affect mood, relationships, and overall

psychological well-being (Lacks, 1987; Sloan & Shapiro, 1993).


35

Sleep difficulties have been linked to a variety of physiological ailments in

various populations. A condition that often causes sleep difficulty is sleep-disordered-

breathing. Although sleep disordered breathing can be caused by respiratory disease, it is

generally associated with sleep apnea; a condition in which an individual’s airway is

obstructed during sleep, and the individual is awakened several times a night due to lack

o f oxygen (Thorpy & Yager, 2001). One study found that children as young as five years

old with mild-sleep-disordered-breathing (MSDB) reported significantly more bodily

pain complaints (Rosen et al., 2002). MSDB was also related to lower scores on a

measure of health-related quality of life for children ages 5-17, and increased effects were

apparent with more severe sleep disordered breathing. A study involving adults 30-60

years of age reported similar results, with even mild cases of sleep disordered breathing

resulting in decrements on a general health survey (Finn et al., 1998). The severity of

health problems related to poor sleep resulting from sleep disordered breathing is noted in

the fact that the magnitude of decrements on the health survey were comparable to other

chronic conditions, such as: arthritis, angina, hypertension, diabetes, and back problems.

Sleep difficulties appear to have an impact on physiological health in people of all

ages. In a study involving junior high school students, a link was found between general

quality of sleep and physiological health, with adolescents who reported poor sleep also

reporting a higher number of illnesses and lower scores on a measure of general health

(Tanaka, et al, 2003). Poor sleep has been linked with poor health in the geriatric

population, as well. Two studies, each consisting of over 5,000 participants over the age

of 65, linked poor sleep to reduced physical health, including limitations in activities of

daily living, and increased reports of cardiovascular disease (Asplund, 2000; Newman et
36

al., 1997). Research by Appels and Mulder (1984) found that sleep difficulties in adults

were related to cardiovascular disease, while Elashoff et al. (1983) found an association

between sleep and gastrointestinal disorders.

Another interesting area that is gaining a great deal of attention is the possible link

between sleep quality and length and obesity. Research is beginning to emerge indicating

that individuals who sleep less on a regular basis tend to increase their body mass index

(BMI). Hasler et al. (2004) in a longitudinal study found that short sleep duration was

associated with an increase in BMI for younger adults, but that the association

disappeared for individuals over the age of 40. This finding was consistent with previous

research that had examined the relationship between sleep length and obesity. The

finding that sleep is related to BMI or obesity in younger adults points to age-specific

pathophysiologic mechanisms of obesity (Comings, Gade, MacMurray, Muhleman, &

Peters, 1996). Taken together these results may indicate that the amount o f sleep that a

younger individual receives may be related to obesity. This may be especially important

in the college population as results have consistently shown that students tend to sleep

less than the general population. The finding that sleep duration is related to obesity may

have numerous implications for health as obesity has been shown to be a major

contributor to a variety o f health difficulties.

Another common source of sleep difficulties is the lack of a consistent sleep

schedule that disrupts the body’s natural sleep circadian rhythm. Having wake times that

vary even just a couple of hours can greatly affect sleep quality and cause sleep

disturbances (Lack, 1986). Ultimately, inconsistent sleep schedules can result in

decreased physiological, psychological, and social health. When it comes to college


37

students, the research clearly indicates that poor sleep habits and patterns are associated

with a variety of difficulties. Among these are increased tension, depression, decreased

psychological well-being, and generally lower life satisfaction (Pilcher et al., 1997). In a

study by Taub and Hawkins (1979), young adults who reported irregular sleeping

schedules had lesser tendencies toward achievement potential, intellectual efficiency,

self-control, and sociability. One of the more serious disruptions to a regular sleep

schedule is shift work. Studies have shown a significant relationship between shift work

and health complications, including headaches, gastric problems, depressed cellular

immune function, respiratory tract infections, and chronic cardiovascular difficulties

(Irwin etal., 1994; Parkes, 1999).

In terms of psychological health, a clear relationship has been established between

poor sleep quality and negative mood states (Bonnet, 1985; Gau, 2000; Gray & Watson,

2002; Lacks & Morin, 1992; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996). The most common negative

mood state associated with poor sleep is depression. Unfortunately, the direction of the

sleep/depression relationship (whether depression causes sleep difficulties, sleep

difficulties cause negative mood, or they interact) remains unclear. However, since sleep

difficulties are one of the hallmark symptoms of depression (APA, 1994), it is certainly

possible that the relationship is cyclical, in that depressed mood can lead to poor sleep

quality, and in return poor sleep quality can exacerbate negative mood states.

Albeit somewhat counterintuitive, there also appears to be a link between sleep

deprivation and decreased symptoms of depression. Studies have shown that short-term

sleep deprivation is correlated with positive changes in both thought content, as well as

improvement on clinical assessments of depression (Kraft, 1984; Vein & Airapetov,


38

1984). Although it is not clear why symptoms of depression often decrease following

sleep deprivation, researchers suggest that neurophysical and biochemical processes that

occur during sleep, are responsible for one’s emotional state (Vein & Airapetov, 1984).

Not surprisingly, research on REM sleep deprivation (rather than total sleep) appears to

have an effect on depressive symptoms. The negative emotion connected with

emotionally important memories appears to decrease following sleep deprivation

(Greenberg, Pearlman, Schwartz, & Grossman, 1983). However, positive changes in

mood due to short-term sleep deprivation appear to be ephemeral, as individuals in these

studies tend to return to baseline levels of depression after a regular night of sleep (Kraft,

1984; Vein & Airapetov, 1984).

There is also a body of research linking sleep quality with clinical (abnormal)

aspects of psychological health. Poor sleepers not only tend to have higher scores on

scales of depression than good sleepers, they also have been shown to score higher on

scales that measure symptoms of anxiety, and social introversion (Aikens & Mendelson,

1999; Monroe, 1967). Similarly, poor sleep appears to be related to Type A personality.

Type A behavior involves a chronic, incessant, struggle to achieve more and more in less

time, which can result in health difficulties (Friedman & Rossman, 1974; Krantz, Arbrier,

Davia, & Parker, 1988).

In addition to the manifestations of health difficulties that appear to be related to

poor sleep, sleep loss may have adverse effects on normal growth and development, as

well as maintaining a healthy immune system. Both protein synthesis and growth

hormones play a substantial role in physical growth, and are at their highest levels during

REM sleep (Parker et al., 1980; Sassin et al, 1969). After only a few nights of poor sleep,
39

metabolic and hormonal function can be adversely affected (Murphy, 2000).

Additionally, the number of natural killer T-cells that are crucial to the body’s immune

system in fighting infection have been shown to be reduced up to 50% in individuals who

obtain less than six hours o f sleep (Irwin et al., 1996). Subsequently, loss of sleep can

exacerbate existing health difficulties, including hypertension and Type II diabetes,

resulting in more severe complications (Murphy, 2000).

The literature also suggests a relationship between sleep habits and social and

psychological health. As early as in pre-adolescence, poor sleep quality has been linked

with poor mental health (Meijer, Habekothe, & Van Den Wittenboer, (2000). One study

involving college students found that individuals considered to be psychologically

healthy reported increased somatic complaints, greater tendency toward obsessive-

compulsive activities, higher levels of interpersonal reactivity, more symptoms of

depression and anxiety, and higher social discomfort, after only a single night of poor

sleep (Zammit, 1988). Additionally, Carpenter (2001) noted that Attention Deficit

Hyperactivity Disorder is often associated with sleep difficulties. Poor sleep may

exacerbate health difficulties in individuals who are afflicted with psychological

disorders. The relationship between traumatic stress symptoms and health functioning

was fully mediated by sleep in a study comprised of urban police officers with

posttraumatic stress symptoms; only those who reported poor sleep also reported somatic

health problems (Mohr et al., 2003).

College students who are able to fall asleep faster and who have fewer sleep

disturbances also have been found to report fewer mental and social health difficulties

(Lowry, Dean, & Manders, 2010). Recent studies also have linked sleep loss to changes
40

in mood, personality, and cardiovascular disease (Blagrove & Akehurst, 2001; Boland et

al., 2002; Taylor & McFatter, 2003). Sleep loss, whether due to sleep disorders,

deprivation, or poor quality, appears to have a negative impact on the total functioning of

humans. Ultimately, poor sleep habits and patterns, as well as sleep difficulties, appear to

have significant adverse effects across measures of physical, mental and social health.

Sleep and Obesity/Health Status. Research has indicated that sleep difficulties

have made a serious impact on various aspects of human functioning (Pilcher & Walters,

1997). These studies examined the effects of sleep quality on an individual’s ability to

function. Sleep quality is generally defined by the number of nighttime awakenings

(including the amount o f time required to return to sleep), sleep latency (the amount of

time required to fall asleep, a feeling of fatigue/restfulness upon awakening in the

morning, sleep duration (the amount of time in a sleeping state) and a general satisfaction

with the sleep received (Pilcher & Walters, 1997).

Studies o f sleep quality and obesity often focused on the relationship of sleep

quality and eating behaviors. Hicks, McTighe, and Juarez (1986) found that college

students with poor sleep quality were more likely to stray from eating three meals per day

and were more likely to snack than those with good sleep quality. More recently, Tanaka

and colleagues (2002) showed that junior high school students who were poor sleepers

were less likely to eat breakfast and had poorer overall dietary habits than the good

sleepers. Specifically, this line of research suggests that sleep quality relates to eating

behavior, and thus weight gain/obesity.

A new line of obesity research has related a specific aspect of sleep quality, sleep

duration, to weight gain and obesity. Evidence from laboratory studies with animals and
41

humans indicate a link between sleep deprivation and an increased body weight

(Gangwisch, Malaspina, Boden-Albala, & Heymsfield, 2005). Further, based on results

from a longitudinal study, Kripke, Garfinkel, Wingard, Klauber, and Marler (2002) found

consistent increases in BMI among individuals who habitually slept less than seven hours

a night. Along these lines, Hasler and colleagues (2004) found results in a 13-year

longitudinal study that indicated as the amount of sleep per night decreased, BMI

increased. Finally, Taheri, Lin, Austin, Young, and Mignot (2004) found that individuals

who slept less than the recommended eight hours a night showed increased BMI, which

was proportional to the amount of decreased in sleep time.

The results of the preceding studies indicate that sleep durations of less than seven

to eight hours are associated with an increased BMI (Kripke et al., 2002; Taheri, Zeitzer,

& Mignot, 2002) and with obesity (Gangwisch et al., 2005; Hasler et al., 2004). Although

the precise mechanism relating lowered sleep duration to weight gain/obesity remains

unclear, recent research has indicated a hormonal component involving leptin and ghrelin

(Prinz, 2004).

Lower levels of leptin, a peptide hormone responsible for suppressing food intake

and increasing energy levels (Prinz, 2004), has been related to increased BMIs and sleep

duration of less than eight hours (Spiegel, Tasali, Penev, & Van Cauter, 2004; Taheri et

al., 2004). Conversely, higher levels of ghrelin, a peptide hormone responsible for

stimulating appetite, fat production, and body growth (Prinz, 2004), have been related to

increased BMIs and less than eight hours of sleep (Speigel, Tasali, Penev, & Van Cauter,

2004; Taheri et al., 2004). Spiegel and colleagues (2004) found that young men who had

their sleep curtailed had lower levels of leptin, increased ghrelin levels and increased
42

appetite and hunger ratings. It was noted that the men in the experimental condition

reported craving sweets and high starch food compared to controls. Research has

indicated that those with sleep restriction or decreased sleep show a decrease in insulin

response to glucose (Spiegel, Leproult, & Van Cauter, 1999). This typical hormone

pattern found in individuals with decreased sleep is a pattern that consistently has been

associated with decreased energy expenditure and increased appetite and obesity (Prinz,

2004).

In adults, less than seven hours of sleep per night can effect normal growth and

development as well as the maintenance of a healthy immune system. Both protein

synthesis and growth hormones play a substantial role in physical growth, and are

secreted at their highest level during REM sleep (Parker et al., 1980; Sassin et al., 1969),

which occurs at a higher frequency at the end of the sleep cycle. Further, individuals who

obtain less than six hours of sleep can have 50% reduction in natural killer T-cells that

aid the body’s immune system in fighting infections (Irwin et al., 1996). Finally, Taylor

and McFatter (2003) linked sleeplessness to cardiovascular disease.

During the sleep period following a night of sleep deprivation, SWS predominates

over the other stages and significantly less REM sleep takes place (Angus, Heslegrave, &

Myles, 1985; Bonnet, 2000). Stage four sleep increases after unusually high amounts of

exercise (Trinder, Paxton, Montgomery, & Fraser, 1985). Additionally, the greatest

amounts of growth hormone are released during SWS (Sassin et al., 1969). These

findings demonstrate the important role played by SWS in maintaining physical health.

The literature also suggests a relationship between sleep quality and psychological

health. Poor sleep quality has been linked with poor mental health as early as
43

preadolescence, with a clear relationship established between poor sleep quality and

negative mood states for adults, as well (Bonnet, 1985; Gau, 2000; Gray & Watson,

2002; Lacks & Morin, 1992; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996). Stress and worry have been

linked to poor sleep quality (McCann & Stewin, 1987), as have increased tension,

decreased psychological well-being, and general lower life satisfaction (Pilcher et al.,

1997). Similarly, poor sleep appears to be related to Type A personality, the latter

involving a chronic incessant struggle to achieve more and more in less time, which can

result in health difficulties (Krantz et al., 1988). Buboltz and colleagues (2006) found that

college students who were able to fall asleep faster with fewer sleep disturbances had

fewer mental and social health difficulties. In a study by Taub and Hawkins (1979),

adults who reported irregular sleeping schedules demonstrated fewer tendencies toward

achievement potential, intellectual efficiency, self-control, and sociability.

Research on sleep deprivation and psychological health has demonstrated that

sleep deprivation of only one night is related to increased excitability and impulsiveness

(Sicard, Jouve, & Blin, 2001; Vein, Dallakyan, Levin, & Skakun, 1983). One study

involving psychologically healthy college students found a relationship between sleep

loss and decreased psychological health, with increased somatic complaints, obsessive-

compulsive activities, higher levels of interpersonal reactivity, and higher social

discomfort after only a single night of sleep loss (Zammit, 1988). Other studies have

linked sleep loss to changes in mood (Blagrove & Akehurst, 2001).

Cognitive skills also are affects by poor sleep quality perhaps due to a relationship

of cognitive abilities and REM sleep (Pilcher & Walters, 1997). Research has

demonstrated that the last half of a full night’s sleep, specifically REM sleep, may be the
44

most important in the learning process (Smith & Lapp, 1991). When this sleep is

terminated prematurely, cognitive abilities are negatively affected. Blagrove and

Akehurst (2001) and Harrison and Home (2000) found dramatic negative effects on

cognitive functioning, impacting logical reasoning and decision making in adults

following the loss of only one night of sleep. Adults reporting excessive sleepiness also

report negative effects on working memory (short-term memory used for thinking and

problem solving), psychomotor reactivity, and increased suggestibility (Blagrove &

Akehurst, 2001). Other studies have shown that sleep difficulties can significantly impair

academic performance, learning, visuomotor skills, and general cognitive ability

(DeKonick, Lorrain, Christ, Proulx, & Coulombe, 1989; Lack, 1986; Schredl, Weber, &

Heuser, 1998). Attention is susceptible to partial sleep deprivation, poor cognitive

functioning, as well as false recall of recently learned words and decreased visuomotor

skill (Grosvenor & Lack, 1984; Kami et al., 1994; Webb & Agnew, 1974).

There also is a body of research linking sleep quality with clinical (abnormal)

aspects of psychological health. Studies indicate a relationship between higher rates of

anxiety in poor sleepers (Aikens & Mendelsen, 1999; Monroe, 1967; Zammit, 1988).

Carpenter (2001) also found that Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder is often

associated with deficits in sleep quality. Further, in a study comprised of urban police

officers with posttraumatic stress symptoms, only those who reported poor sleep quality

also reported somatic health problems (Mohr et al., 2003).

The studies of sleep disturbances and depression have yielded contradictory

results. Aikens and Mendelson (1999) found higher rates of depression were associated

with deficits in sleep quality. Zammit (1988) confirmed these results by finding increased
45

symptoms of depression after sleep loss. However, Kraft (1984) found that short-term

sleep deprivation was linked with decreased symptoms of depression. This contradiction

indicates the need for further research on the psychological effects of sleep disturbances.

Sleep and Learning/Academic Performance

The link between sleep and cognitive performance has been well documented

(Kami et al., 1994; Ohayon, Caulet, Philip, Guilleminault, & Priest, 1997; Smith & Lapp,

1991). REM sleep has been shown to be the most critical part of the sleep process, in

terms o f consolidating memory and learning experiences from the prior day into long­

term memory. REM sleep is also the most difficult to obtain in sufficient quantities, due

to the nuances of the sleep cycle. REM occurs at the end of each sleep cycle; for the first

few complete cycles, there is a quite limited amount of REM sleep occurring at the end of

each cycle. However, if a person remains asleep long enough the amount of time spent in

REM sleep becomes longer. After about 7-8 hours of sleep, the amount of REM sleep is

then sufficient to integrate the complex material acquired during the day into long-term

memory.

Animal studies have suggested that REM sleep aids learning (Carlson, 1999). In

a study on learning in rats it was found that rats learning a complex maze made the

largest gains in learning following the largest periods of REM sleep (Bloch, Hennevin, &

Leconte, 1977). Animals deprived of REM sleep on the other hand were found to learn

the task more slowly. Daytime learning in humans also appears to be hindered by lack of

sleep (Hobson, 1995). As the neural pathways are strengthened a new protein structure is

created which in turn allows the new information to be stored in the brain in the form of

memory (Steriade & McCarley, 1990). It appears that the new protein structure must be
46

refreshed to remain strong due to its instability. It is during REM sleep that new neural

pathways formed by learning information appear to be activated. This could be why REM

sleep is crucial to learning and memory. It has also been suggested that during REM sleep

the brain is aroused and begins to process information stored in memory, which may

reinforce both old and new memories as well as recently learned material (Antrobus,

1986). This may explain the large amounts of time that infants and young children spend

in REM sleep. They are consolidating new information and building new neural

pathways at prolific rates. This would give further credence to the importance of REM

sleep in the learning process of college students given that the college environment

represents a substantial increase in the cognitive demands placed on students relative to

high school.

Additionally, there appears to be greater learning efficiency among those who

obtain sufficient REM sleep. De Koninck et al, (1989) found that among students

enrolled in a 6-week French immersion course, those students who demonstrated

increased REM sleep the night following intensive learning periods were better able to

recall newly acquired information, with lower error rates as well, than were students who

did not demonstrate comparable increases in REM sleep. It should also be noted that this

study also found that increases in other stages of sleep did not result in any improvements

in memory whatsoever, further underscoring the importance of REM.

The relationship between REM sleep and the integration of information into long­

term memory is not limited to the period immediately following learning. In fact,

research suggests that this relationship extends well beyond the night that the information

was integrated. For instance, Smith and Lapp, (1991) found that when students studied
47

for final exams, that REM sleep actually increased in density in the five-day period after

their exams had taken place. Despite this increased REM sleep density, there was no

concomitant increase in the amount of sleep per se, nor was there any increase in the

number of REM sleep periods. Additionally, these periods of increased REM sleep

density were occurring in the latter part of the evening. Taken together, these findings

suggest that information was still being integrated into long-term memory well after the

exam, and since the increases in REM density were occurring later in the evening, this

means that only those individuals who were getting a full night of sleep were able to

profit from this increase in memory consolidation which was occurring after the initial

learning period. It naturally follows from these studies that students who cram, that is

study for long periods immediately prior to an exam, are missing out on the potential

benefits of several nights of increased REM sleep. These findings could also be used to

explain the superiority of distributed practice over mass practice. Individuals who spread

their learning and studying over several nights prior to an exam will get the benefits of

the increase in REM sleep density and improve the integration of new memories and

learned material (Smith & Lapp, 1991).

Considering the importance of REM sleep in improving the integration of

material into long-term memory, the question of what effects the complete lack of REM

sleep might have on memory integration naturally follows. This question would be moot

were it not for the fact that cramming for exams is virtually synonymous with the college

experience and it occurs at all levels of education and across all ability and motivation

levels. It is important to remember that this cramming can affect sleep in different ways;

for instance, some students may study for several hours into the early morning and then
48

get a few hours o f sleep, while others may simply skip sleep altogether. In either case,

students are losing out on REM sleep. In fact, research has borne out that both REM sleep

loss and/or poor sleep quality are associated with significant impairments in cognitive

functioning. A night of sleep loss has been shown to impact logical reasoning and

decision making negatively in adults (Blagrove & Akehurst, 2001; Harrison & Home,

1999). Sleep deprivation appears to affect working memory negatively (short-term

memory used for thinking and problem solving), psychomotor reactivity, and increase

suggestibility (Blagrove & Akehurst, 2000; Sagaspe, Charles, Taillard, Bioulac, &

Phillip, 2003). Sleep loss due to poor quality of sleep has also been linked to poor

cognitive functioning, false recall of recently learned words, and decreased visuomotor

skill (Roediger & McDermott, 1995).

The impact of sleep deprivation or partial sleep deprivation and poor quality of

sleep goes beyond just memory consolidation and acquisition of new knowledge.

Research has indicated that poor sleep is related to impairments in attention and

concentration ability during the day, results in less vigilance, longer reaction times and

perceptual distortions (Home, 1988; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996; Tilley & Brown, 1992).

Additionally, even a single night of restricted sleep has been shown to result in decreases

in verbal creativity, abstract thinking and concept formation (Randazzo, Muehlbach,

Schweitzer, & Walsh, 1998). These researchers also noted that sleep deprived individuals

also had difficulty verbally expressing their ideas or thoughts. This may have

implications for students in the learning environment as the impact of poor sleep quality

may interfere with their ability to ask questions and clarify important information that

may be necessary for optimal performance. As if the problem of sleep deprivation and
49

poor sleep quality impact on various aspects of performance were not enough. Students

who were sleep deprived or had poor quality of sleep rated their performance after

completion of a cognitive task as significantly better than individuals who were not sleep

deprived. They also rated their concentration and attention level as being higher than non­

sleep deprived individuals. In actuality their performance was just the opposite. It was

found that the sleep deprived individuals performed lower on the actual cognitive task

and showed signs o f concentration and attention problems while attempting the task

(Pilcher & Walters, 1997). These findings are very disturbing. If students believe that

their performance is superior when they are sleep deprived or under poor sleep quality

conditions there is little impetus for them to alter their sleep habits or seek professional

help. In conjunction with this students may make inaccurate attributions about the causes

of their actual performance. It is likely that students will attribute their poor performance

to such things as poor instruction, lack of resources, or not enough study time, but not to

poor sleep habits and quality which may be a major contributing factor to their poor

performance.

Although, the link between cognitive performance and REM sleep have been

fairly well established, it is much more difficult to establish the relationship as clearly

between sleep difficulties (e.g., decreased REM) and other more global measures of

cognitive performance such as Grade Point Average (GPA). Jenkins, Buboltz, and

Wilkinson, 2003 reported that the majority of studies that have examined the relationship

between REM sleep and cognitive performance have been conducted in laboratory

settings and have been able to manipulate the actual amount of time that individuals have

been able to spend in REM sleep. Moving outside of the laboratory environment brings
50

numerous issues that may create difficulties. The conclusions drawn may not be as

specific due to lack of experimental control, but similar patterns have emerged in the

research on the relationship between sleep habits and quality and academic performance.

Overall, the results have been fairly consistent with findings indicating that students who

report poor sleep quality and habits have lower overall GPA’s (Jenkins, Buboltz, &

Wilkinson, 2003).

In examination of the few studies that have explored this relationship a variety of

findings related to various sleep habits and quality have emerged. It is clear that no one

particular form of sleep difficulty is responsible for lower GPA’s. Lack (1986) found that

students who met the criteria for Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome (DSPS) had

significantly lower grades, greater feelings of drowsiness, and irritability when compared

to the rest of the sample. However, he did not find any relationship between grades and

overall sleep quality.

In a study o f health related variables students who went to sleep later on

weekdays and also who awoke later on weekdays showed a significant negative

correlation with GPA (Trockel et al., 2000). These findings indicate that the sleep-wake

schedule of students may have a negative impact on academic performance. Lowry and

colleagues (2010) examined the relationship between overall sleep quality and GPA.

They found that overall sleep quality was significantly negatively related to GPA. Their

study examined all aspects of sleep quality and would go counter to some of the findings

of Lack (1986) which did not find a relationship between sleep quality and grades.

Finally, Gray and Watson (2002) examined the relationship between sleep quality,

sleep length, sleep schedule and GPA. Their findings were somewhat mixed compared to
51

the previous findings. They did not find any significant relationship between overall sleep

quality or sleep length and GPA. However, they did find a significant negative

correlation between sleep schedule and GPA. It should however, be noted that the

correlations between sleep quality and length and GPA were in the negative direction, but

did not reach significance. Taken together it appears clear that sleep difficulties and

quality have an impact on student GPA. The exact nature of this relationship is difficult

to determine at this time due to the limited number of studies and the fact that the studies

have all been correlational in nature.

The evidence is clear: sleep is essential to high-caliber academic performance,

because sleep in general and REM sleep in particular are essential to the acquisition of

long-term memory and integration of new material. Even when examined outside of the

laboratory findings are fairly consistent that sleep difficulties in various forms impact the

GPA’s of students.

Sleep Quality and Physical Performance

Sleep quality and physical fitness are important for young adults’ growth and

health. However, little research has examined the relationship between sleep quality and

physical fitness in young adults. Lee and Lin (2007) conducted a study with 291 female

college students in Taiwan, which investigated the association between subjective sleep

quality and physical fitness. The Chinese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index

(PSQI) was used to measure sleep quality. The results indicate that participants with poor

sleep quality had a significant short distance for the sit-and-reach (flexibility), fewer

repetitions for the curl up test (muscular strength and endurance), and a longer time to
52

finished the 800-m run/walk test (cardiovascular fitness) than participants with good

sleep quality.

Poor sleep has been associated with reduced physical function in older adults

(Foley, Ancoli-Isreal, Britz, & Walsh, 2004; Foley, Monjan, & Brown, 1995; Kutner,

Ory, Baker, & FICSIT Group, 1994; Whitney et al., 1998), although all of these studies

used subjective measures of sleep. Abnormal sleep patterns were reported by over 15% of

older adults who also indicated ambulatory limitations (Rantanen et al., 2003). Studies

using objective measures of sleep and physical functioning are needed.

Goldman et al. (2007) examined the association between disturbed sleep and

daytime physical functioning in 2,889 older women. The study used actigraphs to assess

sleep variables. Gait speed, chair stands, and grip strength were the neuromuscular

performance measures. The results indicate that participants with poor sleep walked

slower, took longer to complete chair stands, and had lower grip strength. The

participants with a total sleep time between 6.0 and 7.5 hours tended to perform better on

neuromuscular performance. In addition, participants with more fragmented sleep (as

measured by minutes of wake after sleep onset) had worse neuromuscular performance

and were more likely to have functional impairments. Finally, women who took a nap

greater than or equal to one-hour had a higher chance of having a functional limitation

than those who took less than a 30-minute nap.

Napping and Physical Performance

A short nap is effective for recovery of attention, concentration, and brain

function (Shirakawa, Takase, Tanaka, & Yamamoto, 1999), all of which are used in
53

physical performance. Tietzel and Lack (2001) have reported that sleep inertia occurs

after a 30-minute nap, while that is not the case for a 10-minute nap.

Sleep inertia is a state of lowered arousal that an individual experiences

immediately upon awakening (Tassi & Muzet, 2000). The longer the nap, the greater the

chance there is of experiencing more effects of sleep inertia. Even naps of 30 minutes in

length show negative effects of sleep inertia (Tietzel & Lack, 2001). Brooks and Lack

(2006) found that out of 5-, 10-, 20-, and 30-minute naps, the 10-minute nap produced

immediate improvements in sleep latency, subjective sleepiness, fatigue, vigor, and

cognitive performance, and it was the most effective nap duration. There was evidence of

sleep inertia effects following the 30-minute nap; however, there was no evidence of

sleep inertia effects after the other naps.

The aim of the study by Waterhouse and colleagues (2007) was to examine the

effects of a post-lunch nap on alertness and performance after partial sleep loss.

Participants in this study were 20 males in Libya. The physical performance measures

used were handgrip strength and sprint running times over two and 20 m. Results

indicated that naps produced an improvement in sprint performance after sleep loss

during the previous night. In contrast, a nap did not improve grip strength; there was

actually a decline in grip strength. Because the researchers examined each score on the

hand dynamometer instead of taking the best of the three scores, this decline could be

explained by muscle fatigue. Additionally, naps improved alertness and decreased

sleepiness.
54

Relaxation

Relaxation techniques encompasses several types of relaxation designed to

decrease physiological arousal, which can assist in decreasing the time it takes to fall

asleep (Morin & Wooten, 1996). Research has demonstrated a relationship between

physiological arousal and sleep quality (Bonnet & Arand, 1995; Monroe 1967).

Relaxation techniques decrease sleep latency (Bootzin & Perlis, 1992; Friedman, Bliwise,

Yesavage, & Salom, 1991; Hryshko-Mullen, Broeckl, Haddock, & Peterson, 2000;

Morin, Culbert, & Schwartz, 1994; Morin & Woten, 1996). Relaxation is a popular

clinical and educational tool that benefits health, well-being, and performance (Khasky &

Smith, 1999).

Relaxation Techniques. Relaxation techniques focus on decreasing arousal level

to induce sleep onset. Different relaxation techniques include progressive muscle

relaxation, deep breathing, and mental imagery. Progressive muscle relaxation (PMR)

teaches individuals to recognize and manage physical muscle tension by systematically

tensing and relaxing the major muscle groups (Stepanski, 2000). Deep breathing is

another relaxation technique that is often combined with PMR. Mental imagery is a

technique that decreases cognitive arousal, such as racing thoughts or worrying, by

visualizing oneself in a specific scene that is associated with a calm relaxed state. Mental

imagery may entail imagining oneself on a beach or other pleasant place like lying in the

sun, if it is a positive situation for the individual. Imagining feeling o f warmth also help.

Behavioral approaches tend to work well with simple physical restlessness, while

imagery is effective for people who have both mental and physical restlessness, such as

ruminating about the day's events (Morin & Wooten, 1996).


55

For all relaxation approaches it is beneficial for the therapist or counselor to first

instruct the individual how to relax using the chosen technique in session. Homework

geared toward between-session practice with a review at the next session for fine tuning

and feedback can facilitate effectiveness. In each instance close monitoring on the part of

the therapist tends to increase compliance and effectiveness of relaxation techniques.

Counselors are encouraged to help individuals create their own relaxation tapes.

Making a tape encourages individuals to import their interests and ideas into their

treatment. For deep muscle relaxation audio-taping the individual session exercise and

giving the tape to the individual for home practice can be beneficial. Listening to a tape

when attempting to fall asleep takes less cognitive effort than trying to remember the

steps to relaxation taught by a therapist (Buboltz et al., 2002). Further, expecting those

with sleep difficulties to spend hours in therapy learning relaxation techniques may

simply create a situation for non-compliance, whereas creating a relaxation tape enables

them to continue treatment with less professional monitoring. No matter what approach

to relaxation that is used by the therapist and individual, it is important that the individual

consistently apply the approach on a regular basis. Without consistent application,

effectiveness of relaxation techniques to reduce sleep difficulties diminishes.

Efficacy studies generally support the use of relaxation therapies to decrease sleep

latency (Bootzin & Perlis, 1992; Morin & Wooten, 1996). However, not all individuals

with sleep difficulties are physiologically hyper-aroused. Some studies demonstrate

relationships between poor sleep quality and physiological arousal (Bonnet & Arand,

1995; Monroe, 1967). Relaxation therapies are likely to work for physiologically aroused

individuals that are experiencing sleep difficulties. On the other hand, some people are
56

not physiologically aroused, but are cognitively aroused. For such people, cognitive

therapeutic interventions may be more appropriate.

Guided Imagery. Guided imagery encompasses versatile mental techniques of

visualization and imagining and has become a powerful and very effective therapeutic

tool that is often applied in health and sport psychology. Guided imagery is a mental

technique that allows athletes to focus on and imagine a particular physical behavior,

action or task, integrating mind-body sensory and neuro-physiological functions

(Richardson & Latuda, 1995). The purpose of guided imagery is to evoke and imagine

physical features of a desired technical skill or sport related technical execution by

utilizing physiological arousal in imaginative and confirmative visualization that

increases technical ability, self-confidence and motivational factors (Law, Driediger,

Hall, & Forwell, 2006; Mamassis & Doganis, 2004).

Guided imagery is also an active mental process that is often used in relaxation

procedures with athletes in order to regulate their arousal before competition, lessen

maladaptive behaviors, balance negative thoughts as well as to enhance concentration and

focus (Peluso, Ross, Gfeller, & La Voie, 2005; Vempati & Telles, 2002; Watanabe et al.,

2006). Thus, guided imagery synthesizes multipurpose modalities which can be applied

for relaxation purposes in order to reduces stress and balance psycho-physiological

arousal before competitions as well as improve exercise-motor behaviors, rehearse

specific technical skills, strengthen psycho-neuromuscular functions, and reinforce

cognitive, concentrative, motivational and learning abilities of athletes (Giacobbi, Foore,

& Weinberg, 2004; Martin, Moritz, & Hall, 1999; Utay & Miller, 2006).
57

Hypotheses

The literature suggests that there is a relationship between sleep and physical

performance. However, an exhaustive review of the literature was unable to produce a

single study that examined the effects of sleep quality, sleep length, sleepiness, and a

nap/relaxation on flexibility, grip strength, and peak performance. Because previous

research suggests certain relationships between components of this study, specific

hypotheses can be posited.

Hypothesis 1A

A nap/relaxation will be related to flexibility in the lower back and hamstrings.

More specifically, individuals in the experimental group who take a 10-minute

nap/relaxation will have significantly higher scores on the sit-and-reach test.

Hypothesis IB

A nap/relaxation will be related to grip strength in the hand and forearm. More

specifically, individuals in the experimental group who take a 10-minute nap/relaxation

will have significantly higher scores on the hand dynamometer test.

Hypothesis 1C

A nap/relaxation will be related to vertical jump height. More specifically,

individuals in the experimental group who take a 10-minute nap/relaxation will have

significantly higher scores on the vertical jump test.

Hypothesis ID

A nap/relaxation will be related to peak power. More specifically, individuals in

the experimental group who take a 10-minute nap/relaxation will have significantly

higher scores on estimated peak power.


58

Justification of Hypotheses 1A, IB, 1C, and ID. Naps as brief as 19.8 (Gillberg

et al., 1996), 10.8 (Home & Ryner, 1996), 10.2 (Takahashi & Arito, 2000), 10 (Tietzel &

Lack, 2001; Tietzel & Lack, 2002), and 9.1 (Hayashi, Motoyoshi, & Hori, 2005) minutes

have been found to improve alertness and performance in individuals whose sleep was

restricted. The improvement in alertness and performance due to taking a brief nap could

also benefit physical performance. Waterhouse and colleagues (2007) found that naps

produced an improvement in sprint performance after sleep loss during the previous

night.

Relaxation is a popular clinical and educational tool that benefits health, well­

being, and performance (Khasky & Smith, 1999) and similar to a short nap in its effects.

Guided imagery is also an active mental process that is often used in relaxation

procedures with athletes in order to regulate their arousal before competition, lessen

maladaptive behaviors, balance negative thoughts, and enhance concentration and focus

(Peluso et al., 2005; Vempati & Telles, 2002; Watanabe et al., 2006).

Hypothesis 2A

Poor sleep quality will be significantly related to flexibility in the lower back and

hamstrings. More specifically, an individual who is a poor sleeper will have significantly

lower scores on the sit-and-reach test.

Hypothesis 2B

Poor sleep quality will be significantly related to grip strength in the hand and

forearm. More specifically, an individual who is a poor sleeper will have significantly

lower scores on the hand dynamometer test.


59

Hypothesis 2C

Poor sleep quality will be significantly related to vertical jump height. More

specifically, an individual who is a poor sleeper will have significantly lower scores on

the vertical jump test.

Hypothesis 2D

Poor sleep quality will be significantly related to peak power. More specifically,

an individual who is a poor sleeper will have significantly lower scores on estimated peak

power.

Justification for Hypotheses 2A, 2B, 2C, and 2D. Brown and colleagues (2002)

found that when compared to the general adult population, college students have a

decreased level of sleep quality. Poor sleep quality adversely affects visual-motor

reaction time, critical thinking abilities, vigilance, attention to task, and reaction time

(Taub, 1980). All of these are important in physical performance. Lee and Lin (2007)

found that female college students in Taiwan with poor sleep quality had a significantly

shorter distance for the sit-and-reach (flexibility), fewer repetitions for the curl up test

(muscular strength and endurance), and a longer time to finished the 800-m run/walk test

(cardiovascular fitness) than participants with good sleep quality.

Hypothesis 3A

Sleep duration will be related to flexibility in the lower back and hamstrings.

More specifically, individuals with shorter sleep duration will have significantly lower

scores on the sit-and-reach test.


60

Hypothesis 3B

Sleep duration will be related to grip strength in the hand and forearm. More

specifically, individuals with shorter sleep duration will have significantly lower scores

on the hand dynamometer test.

Hypothesis 3C

Sleep duration will be related to vertical jump height. More specifically,

individuals with shorter sleep duration will have significantly lower scores on the vertical

jump test.

Hypothesis 3D

Sleep duration will be related to peak power. More specifically, individuals with

shorter sleep duration will have significantly lower scores on estimated peak power.

Justification for Hypotheses 3A, 3B, 3C, and 3D. A study reported that the

duration of average night's sleep for college students in 1989 was 6.5 hours, representing

a decrease of about one hour per night over the previous 20 year period (Hicks &

Pellegrini, 1991). This drop is particularly notable since individuals who report sleeping

six hours or less have a greater incidence of cognitive and emotional difficulties (Hicks &

Gilliland, 1981; Taub & Berger, 1973). This increase in cognitive difficulties can impact

physical performance.

Hypothesis 4A

Sleepiness will be significantly related to flexibility in the lower back and

hamstrings. More specifically, individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have

significantly lower scores on the sit-and-reach test.


61

Hypothesis 4B

Sleepiness will be significantly related to grip strength in the hand and forearm.

More specifically, an individual with higher levels of sleepiness will have significantly

lower scores on the hand dynamometer test.

Hypothesis 4C

Sleepiness will be significantly related to vertical jump height. More specifically,

individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have significantly lower scores on the

vertical jump test.

Hypothesis 4D

Sleepiness will be significantly related to peak power. More specifically,

individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have significantly lower scores on

estimated peak power.

Justification for Hypotheses 4A, 4B, 4C, and 4D. Insufficient sleep and daytime

sleepiness rank among the most frequent o f sleep problems reported by college students

(Buboltz et al., 2001; Lack, 1986). Particularly relevant to college students, cognitive

performance effects of sleep deprivation reveal a "fatigue effect," which become worse as

time on the task increases. Furthermore, tasks may be undertaken in an effective manner,

but performance declines as the task duration extends (Durmer & Dinges, 2005). This

decline in performance can include physical performance.


CH APTER 2

M ETHOD

This section describes the methods that were used to collect and analyze the data.

The section includes participants, measures, and the procedure.

Participants

Participants were 98 students at a mid-sized southern university in the United

States who were recruited from classes in the College of Education. Participants were

informed that their participation was voluntary. The study, measures, and materials were

approved by the university’s institutional review board (Appendix A). Prior to

participating in the study, participants were asked to read and sign the consent form

(Appendix B) and were notified of their right to refuse participation. In addition, the

informed consent documents and data were stored separately to ensure confidentiality.

Measures

Demographics Form

The researcher constructed a demographics form (Appendix C), which is

comprised of eight items. These items inquire about gender, age, race/ethnic origin,

school classification, height, weight, NCAA athlete, and sports.

62
63

Sleep Quality Index

The Sleep Quality Index (SQI; Urponen et al., 1991) was used to assess sleep

quality and general sleep difficulties (Appendix D). The SQI consists of eight self-report

items with three response categories, which are weighted as 0,1, or 2 (with a response of

two indicating the most common or severe symptom). On item 1, participants choose

from three responses: “< 10 minutes,” “ 11-30 minutes,” and “ >30 minutes.” The

response categories for items 2-5 and item 7 are: “no,” “< three days/week,” and “3-7

days/week.” For item 6, the response choices are: “very or mostly alert,” “don’t know,”

and “very or mostly tired.” The response categories for item eight are: “no,”

“occasionally,” and “at least once per week.” Sample items include: “Time to fall

asleep,” “Difficulties falling asleep during the past three months,” and “Wake up too

early in the morning during the past three months.”

The eight items are added together to provide a total sleep quality score. Sleep

quality is categorized based on the following scores: 0-1 indicates good sleep quality, 2-8

indicates occasional sleep difficulties, and 9-16 indicates poor sleep quality. The SQI was

divided into good sleep quality and poor sleep quality for analysis. Participants were

categorized as having good sleep quality if they had a SQI score of 0-8, and they were

labeled as having poor sleep quality if they had a SQI score of 9-16. The means for good

sleep quality (M= 3.67, SD = 2.07) and poor sleep quality (M= 11.15, SD = 1.68) do not

overlap. The SQI has a Cronbach's alpha of .73 for men and .75 for women, indicating an

acceptable internal consistency. Validity is supported by the significant positive

relationship between sleep quality score and subjective health (Urponen et al., 1991). The
64

test-retest reliability coefficients range from .74 to .96 with undergraduate college

students, and have a mean of .84 (Jenkins, 2005).

Adult Sleep-Wake Scale

The Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS; Fortunato, LeBourgeois, & Harsh, 2008)

is a 25-item self-report questionnaire developed to measure sleep quality in adults during

the past week (Appendix E). The ADSWS consists of five behavioral dimensions: going

to bed, falling asleep, maintaining sleep, reinitiating sleep, and returning to wakefulness.

The Going to Bed dimension includes the transitions from wakefulness to sleep and the

transition from sleep to wakefulness is the Returning to Wakefulness dimension. The

sleep initiation at the beginning of the sleep period is the Falling Asleep dimension and

the Maintaining Sleep dimension is the maintenance of sleep. The Reinitiating Sleep

dimension is when an individual returns to sleep after an awakening during the sleep

period.

The ADSWS has participants rate how frequently certain sleep-related behaviors

occurred on a 6-point Likert scale, with the following response options: Never (has not

happened), Once in a while (happened 20% o f the time), Sometimes (happened 40% o f

the time), Quite Often (happened 60% o f the time), Frequently, if not always (happened

80% o f the time), and Always (happened 100% o f the time). On the going to bed scale, a

sample item includes: “When it is time to go to bed, I want to stay up and do other

things.” A sample item on the falling asleep scale is: “When I’m in bed and it is time to

fall asleep, I am not sleepy.” The following is a sample item on the maintaining sleep

scale: “After I fall asleep, during the night I toss and turn in bed.” A sample item on the

reinitiating sleep scale is: “After waking up during the night, I have a hard time going
65

back to sleep.” On the returning to wakefulness scale, a sample item includes: “In the

morning, I wake up and feel ready to get up for the day.” A total score was calculated for

the ADSWS by summing the scores of the subscales and conducting a median split. The

individuals above the median were put in the good sleep quality group and individuals

below the median were put in the poor sleep quality group. The ADSWS was divided into

good sleep quality and poor sleep quality for analysis. Participants were categorized as

having good sleep quality if they had an ADSWS total score of 105 and above, and they

were labeled as having poor sleep quality if they had an ADSWS score of 104 and below.

The means for good sleep quality (M = 119.92, SD = 10.28) and poor sleep quality (M =

88.40, SD = 11.87) do not overlap.

The ADSWS has a high level of internal consistency with an alpha o f 0.83 to 0.90

(Fortunato et al., 2008). Test-retest reliability coefficients range from .67 to .82. The

coefficient alpha estimates of reliability for each of the five behavioral dimensions are as

follows: going to bed = .83; falling asleep = .84; maintaining sleep = .83; reinitiating

sleep = .89; and returning to wakefulness = .90. The ADSWS was established to have

valid indicators of sleep wake patterns (Fortunato et al., 2008).

Sleep Habits Questionnaire

The Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ; Buboltz et al., 2001) is a 9-item

questionnaire used to measure sleep habits and sleep length (Appendix F). The SHQ is

based on the instrument designed by Lack (1986). The items are open-ended for

indicating wake-up times, bedtimes, usual amount o f sleep, and other sleep-wake habits

for the week and weekend (Buboltz et al., 2001). Sample items include “On the average,

what time during the week do you go to bed,” “On the average, what time during the
66

weekend do you wake up,” and “On the average, how many hours of sleep do you get

during the week.” Individuals who slept less than six hours per night were put in the short

sleep group, individuals who slept 6-9 hours per night were put in the average sleep

group, and individuals who slept more than nine hours per night were placed in the long

sleep group. The means for the short sleep group (M= 4.93, SD = 1.03), average sleep

group (M = 7.72, SD = 1.12), and long sleep group (M = 9.79, SD = 0.63) do not overlap.

Epworth Sleepiness Scale

The Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS; Johns, 1991) is an 8-item self-report

questionnaire used to assess daytime sleepiness in adults (Appendix G). Participants

indicate how likely they are to doze off or fall asleep in different situations using a 4-

point Likert scale with ratings of 0 (would never doze) to three (high chance of dozing).

A score o f 0 = no chance o f dozing, 1 = slight chance of dozing, 2 = moderate chance of

dozing, and 3 = high chance of dozing. A total score of 0-10 means the person is within

the normal range for sleepiness, 10-12 is in the borderline range, and 12-24 is abnormal.

The average is a score o f seven or 8. The ESS takes approximately three to five minutes

to complete. The ESS was divided into low, medium, and high levels of sleepiness for

analysis. Participants were categorized as having low levels of sleepiness if they had an

ESS score of 0-10, they were labeled as having medium levels of sleepiness if they had

an ESS score of 10-12, and an ESS score of 12-24 indicated high levels of sleepiness.

The ESS has a high level of internal consistency as measured by Cronbach's alpha

of 0.88 (Johns, 1992). Correlations between the ESS total scores and sleep latency were

measured by polysomnography [r(138)= -0.379, p<0.001], as well as Multiple Sleep


67

Latency Test scores [r(27)= -0.514, p<0.01), and demonstrate convergent construct

validity.

Karolinska Sleepiness Scale

The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS; Akerstedt & Gillberg, 1990) is a one item

scale that measures sleepiness and alertness (Appendix H). The scale is structured in a

Likert format with the following response choices: 1 = extremely alert, 3 = alert, 5 =

neither alert nor sleepy, 7 = sleepy, but no difficulty remaining awake, and 9 = extremely

sleepy, fighting sleep. Even number responses are options as well; however, in contrast to

the odd numbered responses, they do not have labels attached. Kaida et al. (2006)

conducted a study where they found that the KSS was closely related to EEG and

behavioral variables, indicating a high validity in measuring sleepiness. Individuals who

had a score of 1-3 were placed in the low sleepiness group, scores of 4-6 represented the

medium sleepiness group, and scores 7-9 indicated the high sleepiness group.

Physical Performance Measures

Hand Grip Dynamometer

A hand grip dynamometer was used to assess grip strength, which measures hand

and forearm muscular strength in kilograms (kg). Hand-grip dynamometry is a useful

assessment tool for multiple purposes in clinical practice, including the assessment of

upper limb impairment; in evaluating work capacity for those with hand injuries and

other work-related injuries; the evaluation of people with other impairments and

disabilities such as rheumatoid arthritis, chronic fatigue syndrome, developmental

disabilities, muscular dystrophy, and stroke; determining the efficacy of different

treatments for people with a range of disabilities; part of an overall fitness assessment;
68

and in determining the level of effort exerted (Innes, 1999). The intra-class correlation

coefficients (ICC) with the dominant hand were 0.92 during the pre-fatigue maximal

contraction and 0.92 during the post-fatigue maximal contraction, indicating high

reliability (Reuter, Massy-Westropp, & Evans, 2011). The muscle fatigue protocol

consisted of ten 30-second sub-maximal isometric contractions. The intra-class

correlation coefficients (ICC) with the non-dominant hand were 0.97 during the pre­

fatigue maximal contraction and 0.95 during the post-fatigue maximal contraction,

indicating high reliability (Reuter et al., 2011). Pre- and post-fatigue maximal contraction

measurements using linear regression demonstrated excellent reliability (Reuter et al.,

2011 ).

Sit-and-Reach Test

To measure flexibility, specifically in the lower back and hamstring muscles, a

sit-and-reach box was utilized and measured in centimeters (cm). The sit-and-reach box

is a solid box approximately 30 cm tall. It has measurement markers on the top and

extends 26 cm over the front edge of the box toward the individual. The individuals

removed their shoes and sat on the floor with their legs stretched out in front of them with

knees straight and feet flat against the front end of the box. They lean forward at the hips

in a slow steady movement and slide their hands up the measurement markers as far as

they can go.

According to the American College of Sports Medicine’s (ACSM; 2006)

Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription, the sit-and-reach test scores for adult

males are: above 34 cm = excellent flexibility, 28 to 34 cm = above average, 23 to 27 cm

= average, 16 to 22 cm = below average, and below 16 cm = poor flexibility. The adult


69

female sit-and-reach test scores are: above 37 cm = excellent flexibility, 33 to 36 cm =

above average, 29 to 32 cm = average, 23 to 28 cm = below average, and below 23 cm =

poor flexibility.

Previous studies indicate consistently high reliability estimates (.96 < R < .99) for

the sit-and-reach test (Bravo et al., 1994; Jackson & Baker, 1986; Jackson & Langford,

1989; Shaulis, Golding, & Tandy, 1994). Studies have found moderate validity as a

measure of hamstring flexibility and relativity low validity of as a measure of lower back

flexibility (Jackson & Baker, 1986; Jackson & Langford, 1989).

Vertec Vertical Jump Device

The Vertec vertical jump measures vertical jump height and was used to estimate

peak power. The Vertec vertical jump measures vertical jump height in inches (in). The

Vertec is an adjustable metal pole standing upright with several horizontal vanes at the

top. The vanes are spaced a half inch apart and rotate when touched to indicate the height

reached. The correlation coefficient has been reported as 0.91, indicating good reliability

(Leard et al., 2007).

Estimated Peak Power

Peak power was estimated using the following formula, which was based on the

Vertical Jump Test mentioned above: 65.1 x (jump height in cm) + 25.8 x (body mass in

kg) - 1413.1 = Peak Power (Canavan & Vescovi, 2004). The estimated peak power was

measured using watts (W).

Procedure

The researcher obtained approval from the university’s Institutional Review

Board (IRB) prior to the start of data collection.


70

Recruitment of Participants

After consulting with instructors of college of education classes, announcements

were made in classes regarding participation in this research. A script was read to recruit

participants (Appendix I). Potential volunteers were informed of the purpose of the study

and were provided with information about the location and times when they may

participate. They were instructed to wear comfortable clothing that they can stretch in

during the study. Participants were informed that they would receive extra credit for their

participation in this project with approval from the instructor. The amount o f extra credit

awarded to the students was predetermined by each instructor. An alternative extra credit

activity was offered by their instructor for those who chose not to participate in this

study. They were reminded that participation in this project is voluntary and that no

identifying information was collected. They were also informed that they would not be

penalized for not volunteering and that they may stop at any time without penalty. When

each participant signed up for a date and time to participate in the research study, they

also provided their name, email address, and telephone number. The participants’ email

addresses and phone numbers were requested for contact by the researcher the night prior

to the test date to remind them about the study and to wear comfortable clothing.

Students arrived at a designated classroom in the gymnasium at the appointed

time and date. Once the students were seated, a script was read to the participants

regarding their participation. Participants were then asked to read the consent form. After

reading the consent form, the researcher asked what questions they had, and then they

were asked to sign and date the consent form. Written informed consent was then

obtained from each participant, and they received a copy of the consent form. The data
71

and the participants’ identification numbers were kept confidential. Participants were

randomly assigned to either the experimental or control group.

Control Group

Once the participants signed an Informed Consent Form, they were taken from the

classroom to the gymnasium where they were led through an active dynamic warm-up.

Next, they completed the hand dynamometer test, the sit-and-reach test, and the vertical

jump test. These measures were conducted to address practice effects, and were not used

as pre-test scores. Finally, they returned to the classroom, and were instructed to read a

newspaper for 10 minutes, which was provided to them. They were not allowed to sleep.

Next, they were asked to complete the questionnaires about sleepiness. They were then

taken from the classroom to the gymnasium where they were led through an active

dynamic warm-up. Next, they completed the hand dynamometer test, the sit-and-reach

test, and the vertical jump test. The scores from these measures were analyzed in the

results. Finally, the participants went back to the classroom to complete the remaining

questionnaires. The procedure for the active dynamic warm-up can be found in Appendix

J.

Experimental Group

Once the participants signed an Informed Consent Form, they were taken from the

classroom to the gymnasium where they were led through an active dynamic warm-up.

Next, they completed the hand dynamometer test, the sit-and-reach test, and the vertical

jump test. These measures were conducted to address practice effects, and were not used

as pre-test scores. Finally, they returned to the classroom, and were instructed to get as

comfortable as possible in their chairs and close their eyes while they listened to a guided
72

relaxation or took a nap for 10 minutes. The lights in the classroom were turned off

during the guided relaxation. Next, they were asked to complete the questionnaires about

sleepiness. They were then taken from the classroom to the gymnasium where they were

led through an active dynamic warm-up. Next, they completed the hand dynamometer

test, the sit-and-reach test, and the vertical jump test. The scores from these measures

were analyzed in the results. Finally, they went back to the classroom to complete the

remaining questionnaires.

Hand Dynamometer Test Procedure

The procedure to measure grip strength was using the Jamar hand dynamometer.

The participants were seated without an armrest with their dominant arm at a 90 degree

angle and used their dominant hand to apply as much grip pressure as possible on the

dynamometer. The researcher used the words “Harder, harder, relax” as a standard verbal

command for every participant. The researcher recorded the maximum reading (kg). The

participant repeated the test three times after 20 seconds of rest between each trial. The

researcher used the highest recorded value to assess the participant’s performance.

Vertical Jump Test Procedure

The participants did three maximum jumps using a Vertec vertical jump device.

The participants began in a standing position and performed a crouching action (counter

movement) before jumping for maximal height using a Vertec vertical jump device. They

were given three attempts at the maximal jump with 15 seconds of rest time between

jumps.
73

Sit-and-Reach Test Procedure

According to the American College of Sports Medicine, the sit-and-reach test

procedure involves having the participant sit on the floor without shoes on, with their legs

stretched out straight ahead. The soles of the feet are placed flat against the sit-and-reach

box at the 26-cm mark. Both knees should be locked and pressed flat to the floor. Next,

the participant slowly reaches forward along the measuring line with both hands (palms

facing downwards) as far as possible. The participant keeps their hands parallel and not

leading with one hand. The fingertips can be overlapped. The participant exhale and drop

their head between their arms when reaching. After two practice reaches, the participant’s

third reach, held for 1-2 seconds, was recorded as the participant’s score.

Once they finished completing the physical activities, the researcher had

participant sign their name on the instructors’ extra credit form. The researcher returned

extra credit forms to the corresponding instructor.

Risks and Benefits

Potential risks include emotional distress or discomfort associated with

completing questionnaires and performing fitness activities. In attempts to minimize

potential risks, participants were reassured that these questions are common in this area

of research and that feelings of discomfort are not unusual. Participants were provided

with a list of local mental health services, in which the participants may contact if

needed. Participants were informed that any professional services sought will be at their

own expense. Benefits of participation include earning extra credit and contributing to the

field of psychology.
CHAPTER 3

RESULTS

Participants

Overall Sample

Participants were 98 undergraduate and graduate students at a mid-sized southern

university in the United States. There were 50 female (50.5%) and 48 male (48.5%)

participants. The average age of the participants was 22.5 years (SD = 5.01, range: 17 to

47). In terms of ethnicity, 65 were Caucasian (65.7%), 26 were African American

(26.3%), 2 were Asian/Pacific Islander (2.0%), two were Hispanic (2.0%), and two

indicated Other (2.0%). The sample was comprised of individuals from all academic

classifications, 24.2% freshman (n = 24), 7.1% sophomores (n =7), 18.2% juniors (n -

18), 28.3% seniors (n = 28), and 20.2% graduate students (n = 20).

The average weight of the participants was 165 pounds (SD = 44.9, range: 91 to

320). The average Body Mass Index (BMI) of the participants was 25.8 (SD - 5.9, range:

17.7 to 49.6). The average BMI for the male participants was 26.8 (SD = 5.9, range: 18.7

to 47.2) For female participants, the average BMI was 24.7 (SD = 6.1, range: 17.7 to

49.6) The average height of the participants was 169 cm or 5-feet - seven inches (SD =

9.87, range: 141 cm or 4’8” to 190.5 or 6’3”).

74
75

Control Group

The control group consisted of 19 (42.2%) females and 26 (57.8%) males with an

average age of 21.4 (SD = 4.0; range: 17 to 37). The control group was 75.6% Caucasian,

20% African American, 2.2% Hispanic, and 2.2% Other. The control group was

comprised of 37.8% freshman, 6.7% sophomores, 17.8% juniors, 24.4% seniors, and

13.3% graduate students

In the control group, the average weight of the participants was 166.8 pounds (SD

= 36.9, range: 106 to 261). The average Body Mass Index (BMI) of the participants was

25.5 (SD = 4.5, range: 18.8 to 35.9). The average height of the participants was 171 cm or

5-feet 7-inches (SD = 9.59, range: 155 or 5 T ’ to 190.5 or 6’3”).

Experimental Group

The experimental group consisted of 31 female (58.5%) and 22 male (41.5%)

participants. The average age of the participants was 23.5 years (SD = 5.59, range: 18 to

47). The majority of the participants in the experimental group were Caucasian (58.5%),

with African American (32.1%), Asian/Pacific Islander (3.8%), Hispanic (1.9%), and

Other (1.9%). The experimental group was comprised of individuals from all academic

classifications, 13.2% were freshman, 7.5% were sophomores, 18.9% were juniors,

32.1% were seniors, and 26.4% were graduate students.

The average weight of the participants in the experimental group was 163 pounds

(SD = 51.05, range: 91 to 319.5). The average BMI of the participants in the experimental

group was 25.9 (SD = 6.90. range: 17.7 to 49.6). The average height o f the participants in

the experimental group was 167.7 cm or 5-feet 6-inches (SD = 9.89, range: 141 cm or

4’8” to 190.5 cm or 3’3”).


76

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Overall Sample

Table 1 presents the overall sample’s reliability coefficients, means, standard

deviations, and ranges for the Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS), the Sleep Quality

Index (SQI), the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ), the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS),

and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). The current sample was compared to

normative samples using one-sample /-tests with adult populations.

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests fo r Overall Sample

Measures M SD Range a /
SQI Total 4.69 3.27 0-14 .75 -1.21
ADSWS Going to bed 19.64 5.54 7-30 .87 3.47**
ADSWS Falling asleep 22.00 4.97 10-30 80 2.59**
ADSWS Maintaining sleep 21.92 5.29 6-30 .84 3.59**
ADSWS Reinitiating sleep 23.77 5.35 12-30 .90 4.01**
ADSWS Returning to awake 16.51 6.43 5-30 .91 0.79
SHQ Weekday 7.13 1.76 2-14 -1.37
SHQ Weekend 8.39 1.91 2-15 1.01
ESS Total 9.38 3.30 3-20 .58 10.44**
KSS 5.22 1.83 1-9 -2.60*
Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale; SHQ = Sleep Habits
Questionnaire; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; KSS = Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. * p < .05
two-tailed, ** p < .01 two-tailed.

With respect to the ADSWS, a one sample /-test revealed the current sample

means on the Going to Bed subscale, Falling Asleep subscale, Maintaining Sleep

subscale, and Reinitiating Sleep subscale were significantly higher than the

standardization sample o f undergraduate college students (Fortunato et al., 2008) (See

Table 1). The ESS mean o f the current sample was 9.38 (SD = 3.30), which was

significantly higher than the standardization sample of adults (M = 5.9; Johns,


77

1991), /(l, 97) = 10.44,/? < .01. The KSS mean was 5.22 (SD = 1.83), which was

significantly higher than the standardization sample of adults (M = 5.7; Kaida et al.,

2006), /( 1,96) = -2.60,p < . 05.

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Overall Sample by Gender

Table 2 presents the overall sample’s reliability coefficients, means, standard

deviations, and ranges by gender for the variables in this study. With respect to the

ADSWS, the means for males on the Going to Bed subscale, Falling Asleep subscale,

Maintaining Sleep subscale, Reinitiating Sleep subscale, and Returning to Wakefulness

subscale were significantly higher than the standardization sample of undergraduate

college students (Fortunato et al., 2008) (See Table 2). The means for females on the

Going to Bed subscale and Maintaining Sleep subscale were significantly higher than the

standardization sample of undergraduate college students (Fortunato et al., 2008). The

mean for males’ and females’ ESS was significantly higher than the standardization

sample o f adults (Johns, 1991) (See Table 2). The KSS mean for males was significantly

lower than the standardization sample of adults (Kaida et al., 2006).

Vertical Jump Test means for males and females were significantly lower than

another sample, which included young adult medical students and their spouses

(Patterson & Peterson, 2004) (See Table 2). For estimated Peak Power, the means for

males and females were significantly lower than the standardization sample, which

included young adult medical students and their spouses (Patterson & Peterson, 2004).
Table 2

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for Overall Sample by Gender

M SD Range a /
Measures Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female
SQI Total 4.21 5.19 3.10 3.40 0-14 0-14 .72 .76 -1.99 0.18
ADSWS Going to bed 19.90 19.40 6.05 5.06 7-30 9-29 .90 .82 2.52* 2.37*
ADSWS Falling asleep 22.67 21.36 4.34 5.47 10-30 10-30 .71 .84 3.14** 0.85
ADSWS Maintaining sleep 21.94 21.90 5.05 5.55 6-30 12-30 .82 .8 6 2 .6 6 * 2.42*
ADSWS Reinitiating sleep 24.96 22.62 5.00 5.48 12-30 12-30 .89 .90 4.66** 1.32
ADSWS Returning to awake 17.96 15.12 6.32 6.28 5-30 5-27 .87 .93 2.15* -0.99
SHQ Weekday 7.13 7.12 1.85 1.69 2 -1 1 4-14 -0.90 -1.03
SHQ Weekend 8 .1 0 8 .6 6 1.97 1.83 2 -1 2 4.5-15 -0.30 1.80
ESS Total 8.63 1 0 .1 0 3.09 3.36 3-15 4-20 .51 60 6 . 1 1 ** 8.84**
KSS 4.75 5.67 1.80 1.76 1 -8 3-9 -3.65** -0 . 1 1
Grip Strength Right Hand 59.21 33.88 10.51 6.77 27-76 21-46 .97 .97 -40.29 -37.40
Grip Strength Left Hand 54.23 31.33 9.24 5.94 24-68 21-44 .93 .97 -37.29 -34.58
Sit and Reach 29.44 35.56 8 .1 1 8.30 11.5-47 15.5-50 .99 .99 -1.34 1.33
Vertical Jump 16.49 12.79 4.26 4.32 7-23.5 4-22.5 -9.29** -2.14*
Peak Power 3493 2387 731 907 1482-4972 395-4487 -12.29** -4.38**
Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale; SHQ = Sleep Habits Questionnaire; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; KSS
= Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. * p < .05 two-tailed, ** p < .01 two-tailed.

-j
00
79

The means for males’ and females’ grip strength on both the right and left hands were

significantly lower than the standardization sample of adults (Mathiowetz, Kashman,

Volland, Weber, Dowe, & Rogers, 1985) (See Table 2).

Examination of Gender Differences for Overall Sample

Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics for the control group. An analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine which variables in the overall sample

were significantly different according to gender. The following variables significantly

differed based on gender: KSS Score F (l, 95) = 6.51,/? < .05, ESS Total F( 1, 96) = 5.11,

p < .05, ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep Subscale F (l, 96) = 4.86, p < .05, ADSWS Returning

to Wakefulness Subscale F (l, 96) = 4.96,p < .05, Sit-and-Reach Test F( 1, 96) = 13.63,/?

< .01, Vertical Jump Test F (l, 96) = 18.21,/? < .01, estimated Peak Power F (l, 96) =

43.96,/? < .01, Grip Strength on the Right Hand F (l, 93) = 195.92,/? < .01, and Grip

Strength on the Left Hand F (l, 93) = 207.23,/? < .01.

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Control Group

Table 3 presents the control group’s reliability coefficients, means, standard

deviations, and ranges for the Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS), the Sleep Quality

Index (SQI), the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ), the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS),

and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). The control group was compared to

normative samples using one-sample /-tests.

With respect to the ADSWS, a one sample /-test revealed the control group means

on the Going to Bed subscale, Maintaining Sleep subscale, and Reinitiating Sleep

subscales were significantly higher than the standardization sample (Fortunato et al.,

2008) (See Table 3). The ESS mean was significantly higher than the standardization
80

sample (Johns, 1991). The mean of the KSS was significantly higher than the

standardization sample (Kaida et al., 2006).

Table 3

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for Control Group

Measures M SD Range a t
SQI Total 4.64 3.33 0-14 .78 -0.92
ADSWS Going to bed 21.29 5.69 7-30 .89 4.23’**
ADSWS Falling asleep 2 2 .0 2 5.29 10-30 .87 1 .6 8
ADSWS Maintaining sleep 21.82 5.10 11-30 .78 2.40*
ADSWS Reinitiating sleep 23.36 5.47 12-30 .90 2.15*
ADSWS Returning to awake 17.73 6.93 5-30 .89 1 .6 8

SHQ Weekday 7.15 1.58 2 - 1 1 .2 -0.95


SHQ Weekend 8.16 1.84 2 -1 2 -0 . 1 1
ESS Total 9.11 3.16 3-15 .57 6.81**
KSS 4.77 1.85 1-9 -3.32**
Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-•Wake Scale; SHQ = Sleep Habits
Questionnaire; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; KSS = Karolinska Sleepiness Scale.

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for Control Group by Gender

Table 4 presents the overall sample’s reliability coefficients, means, standard

deviations, and ranges by gender for the Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS), the Sleep

Quality Index (SQI), the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ), the Epworth Sleepiness

Scale (ESS), the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS), the Sit-and-Reach Test, the Vertical

Jump Test, estimated Peak Power, the Grip Strength with the Right Hand, and the Grip

Strength with the Left Hand.

With respect to the ADSWS, the means for males on the Going to Bed subscale,

Falling Asleep subscale, Maintaining Sleep subscale, Reinitiating Sleep subscale, and

Returning to Wakefulness subscale were significantly higher than the standardization

sample (Fortunato et al., 2008) (See Table 2). The means for females on the Going to Bed

subscale and Maintaining Sleep subscale were significantly higher than the
Table 4

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests fo r Control Group by Gender

M SD Range a t
Measures Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female
SQI Total 4.19 5.26 3.58 2.94 0-14 0 -1 1 .80 .71 -1.29 0.24
ADSWS Going to bed 21.65 6.47 4.54 11-27 .95 3.12** 2 9 7 **
20.79 7-30 .73
ADSWS Falling asleep 22.65 21.16 5.25 5.37 10-30 13-30 .8 6 .87 1.90 0.37
ADSWS Maintaining sleep 21.58 22.16 4.73 5.70 11-30 14-30 .75 .83 1.70 1.65
ADSWS Reinitiating sleep 24.81 21.37 5.23 5.29 12-30 13-30 .87 .90 3.13** -0.19
ADSWS Returning to awake 19.92 14.74 6.65 6.29 6-30 5-26 .8 6 .90 3.01** -0 . 8 8
SHQ Weekday 6.99 7.36 1.62 1.55 2 -1 1 5-11 - 1 .2 0 -0 . 0 2
SHQ Weekend 8 .0 0 8.38 2.05 1.53 2 -1 2 5-10 -0.47 0.55
ESS Total 8 .8 8 9.42 3.31 3.01 3-15 4-15 .58 .57 4.59** 5.11**
KSS 4.42 5.28 1.81 1.84 1 -8 3-9 -3.59** -0.97
Grip Strength Right Hand 60.15 37.53 11.50 5.93 27-76 27-46 .97 .97 -26.99** -24.17**
Grip Strength Left Hand 55.08 33.84 10.61 5.27 24-68 27-44 .96 .97 -23.75** -22.45**
Sit and Reach 28.94 35.55 7.76 7.74 11.5-46 15.5-47 .99 .99 -1.35 .8 8
Vertical Jump 16.65 1 2 .2 1 4.42 4.27 7-23.5 4-19.5 -9.29** -1.93*
Peak Power 3510 2260 750 753 1880-4729 863-3313 -8.70** -3.98**
Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale; SHQ = Sleep Habits Questionnaire; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; KSS
= Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. * p < .05 two-tailed, ** p < .01 two-tailed.
82

standardization sample (Fortunato et al., 2008). The mean for males’ and females’ ESS

was significantly higher than the standardization sample (Johns, 1991) (See Table 4). The

KSS mean for males was significantly lower than the standardization sample (Kaida et

al., 2006).

Vertical Jump Test mean for males was significantly lower than another sample

(Patterson & Peterson, 2004) (See Table 4). For males and females, the estimated Peak

Power means were significantly lower than the standardization sample (Patterson &

Peterson, 2004). The males and females, the mean Grip Strength on the right and left

hands were significantly lower than the standardization sample (Mathiowetz et al., 1985).

Examination of Gender Differences for Control Group

Table 4 presents the descriptive statistics for the control group. An analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine which variables in the control group

were significantly different according to gender. The following variables significantly

differed based on gender: ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep Subscale F ( l , 43) = 4.70, p < .05,

ADSWS Returning to Wakefulness Subscale F (l, 43) = 6.99, p < .01, Sit-and-Reach

Test F( 1,43) = 1.99,p < .01, Vertical Jump Test F (l, 43) = 11.39,/? < .01, estimated

Peak Power F (l, 43) = 30.41,/? < .01, Grip Strength on the Right Hand F (l, 43) = 61.40,

p < .01, and Grip Strength on the Left Hand F (l, 43) = 64.18,/? < .01.

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Experimental Group

Table 5 presents the experimental group’s reliability coefficients, means, standard

deviations, and ranges for the Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS), the Sleep Quality

Index (SQI), the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ), the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS),
83

and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). The experimental group was compared to

normative samples using one-sample /-tests.

Table 5

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests for Experimental Group

Measures M SD Range a /
SQI Total 4.74 3.26 0-14 .73 -0.78
ADSWS Going to bed 18.25 5.06 9-29 .82 0.79
ADSWS Falling asleep 21.98 4.73 10-30 .73 1.97
ADSWS Maintaining sleep 2 2 .0 0 5.48 6-30 .8 8 2 .6 6 *
ADSWS Reinitiating sleep 24.11 5.27 12-30 .90 3.47**
ADSWS Returning to awake 15.47 5.84 5-27 .92 -0 . 6 6
SHQ Weekday 7.11 1.92 3-14 -0.99
SHQ Weekend 8.58 1.96 4.5-15 1.43
ESS Total 9.60 3.42 4-20 .60 7.88**
KSS 5.58 1.75 3-9 -0.48
Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-■Wake Scale; SHQ = Sleep Habits
Questionnaire; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; KSS = Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. * p < .05
two-tailed, **p < . 0 1 two-tailed.

With respect to the ADSWS, a one sample /-test revealed the experimental group

means on the Maintaining Sleep subscale and Reinitiating Sleep subscale were

significantly higher than the standardization sample (Fortunato et al., 2008) (See Table

5). The ESS mean was significantly higher than the standardization sample (Johns, 1991).

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and /-tests for Experimental Group by Gender

Table 6 presents the experimental group’s reliability coefficients, means, standard

deviations, and ranges by gender for the Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS), the Sleep

Quality Index (SQI), the Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ), the Epworth Sleepiness

Scale (ESS), the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS), the Sit-and-Reach Test, the Vertical

Jump Test, estimated Peak Power, the Grip Strength with the Right Hand, and the Grip

Strength with the Left Hand.


Table 6

Descriptive Statistics, Reliabilities, and t-tests fo r Experimental Group by Gender

M SD Range a t
Measures Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female Male Female
SQI Total 4.23 5.14 2.51 3.74 1-11 0-14 .56 .79 -1.63 0.06
ADSWS Going to bed 17.82 18.55 4.87 5.25 10-26 9-29 .77 .85 0.11 0.90
ADSWS Falling asleep 22.68 21.48 3.08 5.61 17-28 10-30 .22 .83 3.02** 0.78
ADSWS Maintaining sleep 22.36 21.74 5.49 5.55 6-30 12-30 .88 .88 2.02 1.75
ADSWS Reinitiating sleep 25.14 23.39 4.82 5.23 15-30 12-30 .91 .90 3.44** 1.80
ADSWS Returning to awake 15.64 15.35 5.14 6.37 5-22 5-27 .86 .95 -0.33 -0.56
SHQ Weekday 7.30 6.97 2.11 1.78 3-11 4-14 -0.16 -1.24
SHQ Weekend 8.23 8.82 1.91 1.99 5-12 4.5-15 0.09 1.77
ESS Total 8.32 10.52 2.85 3.54 4-14 4-20 .42 .62 3.98** 7.26**
KSS 5.14 5.90 1.75 1.70 3-8 3-9 -1.51 0.67
Grip Strength Right Hand 58.05 31.48 9.30 6.27 38-74 21-46 .96 .96 -31.01** -33.45**
Grip Strength Left Hand 53.19 26.69 7.32 5.86 38-66 21-44 .87 .96 -32.13** -28.76**
Sit and Reach 30.02 35.56 8.65 8.75 13.5-47 18-50 .99 .99 -0.53 1.00
Vertical Jump 16.30 13.15 4.14 4.38 7-22 4-22.5 -6.69** -1.21
Peak Power 3472 2465 726 993 1482-4972 395-4487 -8.51** -2.71*
Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale; SHQ = Sleep Habits Questionnaire; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; KSS

= Karolinska Sleepiness Scale. * p < .05 two-tailed, **p < .01 two-tailed.

00
4^
85

With respect to the ADSWS, the means for males on the Falling Asleep subscale

and Reinitiating Sleep subscale were significantly higher than the standardization sample

(Fortunato et al., 2008) (See Table 6). The mean for males’ and females’ ESS were

significantly higher than the standardization sample (Johns, 1991)

Vertical Jump Test mean for males was significantly lower than another sample

(Patterson & Peterson, 2004) (See Table 6). For estimated Peak Power, the mean for

males and females were significantly lower than the standardization sample (Patterson &

Peterson, 2004). For males and females, the mean Grip Strength on both the Right and

Left Hands were significantly lower than the standardization sample (Mathiowetz et al.,

1985).

Examination of Gender Differences for Experimental Group

Table 6 presents the descriptive statistics for the experimental group. An analysis

of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine which variables in the experimental

group were significantly different by gender. The following variables significantly

differed based on gender: ESS Total F( 1, 51) = 5.80, p < .05, Sit-and-Reach Test .F(l, 51)

= 5.21 ,/? < .05, Vertical Jump Test F (l, 51) = 6.96, p < .05, estimated Peak Power F(\,

51) = 16.39,/? < .01, Grip Strength on the Right Hand F( 1, 48) = 145.78, p < .01 and Grip

Strength on the Left Hand F(\, 48) = 158.79, p < .01 (See Table 6).

Homogeneity of Variance

A Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance was conducted to examine if the

groups differed significantly on any of the variables at time one. The test revealed that the

group averages were not significantly different on any of the variables at time one: SQI

Total, F( 1,93) = .10,/? >.05; ADSWS Going to Bed, F(1, 96) = .20,/? >.05; ADSWS
86

Falling Asleep, F (l, 96) = .39,/? >.05; ADSWS Maintaining Sleep, F( 1, 96) = .09,

p >.05; ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep, F (l, 96) = .00, p >.05; ADSWS Returning to

Wakefulness, F (l, 96) - 2.06, p >.05; SHQ Weekdays, F( 1, 95) = .13, p >.05; SHQ

Weekend, F( 1,96) = .42,/? >.05; KSS Score, F( 1, 95) = .03,p >.05; ESS Total F (l, 96) =

.06, p >.05; Sit-and-Reach, F (l, 96) = .15, p >.05; Grip Strength Right, F (l, 93) = .50,

p >.05; Grip Strength Left, F (l, 93) = .08,/? >.05; Vertical Jump, F (l, 96) = .05,/? >.05;

Peak Power F (l, 96) = .37,/? >.05.

Correlations between Variables

Table 7 presents the correlations between all of the study variables. The SQI total

score was significantly negatively correlated with the ADSWS Going to Bed, Falling

Asleep, Maintaining Sleep, Reinitiating Sleep, and Returning to Wakefulness Subscales,

as well as with the SHQ during the week and on weekends.

The SHQ sleep length during the week was positively correlated with the

ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep Subscale, as well as with the SHQ during the weekends. The

SHQ sleep length during the weekends was positively correlated with the ADSWS

Maintaining Sleep Subscale and was negatively correlated with the ADSWS Returning to

Wakefulness Subscale. As would be expected, the ADSWS subscales all correlated at

minimal to moderate levels.

The KSS Scale and the ESS Scale were positively correlated. The KSS Scale was

negatively correlated with both the ADSWS Going to Bed and Returning to Wakefulness

Subscales, as well as with both the Grip Strength Right Hand and Left. The ESS Scale

was negatively correlated with the ADSWS Returning to Wakefulness Subscale, as well

as with the Grip Strength Right Hand and Left Hand.


Table 7

Summary o f Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations fo r Scores on All Measures fo r Overall Sample

M easures 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
1. SQI —

2. SH Q W eek -.30** —

3. SH Q W eekend -.26* .46** --

4. K SS .20 -.07 .05 _


5. ESS -.06 -.09 .13 .28** —

6. A D SW S B ed -.25** .10 .07 -.22* -.07 —

7. A D SW S Fall -.59** .03 .05 -.19 -.14 .34** —

8. A D SW S M ain -.63** .13 .20* .03 .17 .43** .55** —

9. A D SW S Rein -.50** .21* .17 -.10 -.04 .35** .62** .61** —

10. A D SW S R et -.31** -.04 -.27** -.28** -.32** .28** .27** .12 .20* _

11. Sit and Reach -.12 -.05 .09 .02 .12 .03 .10 .14 .09 -.12 —

12. V ertical Jum p -.20 .21* .09 -.07 -.09 .13 .14 16 .35** .22* -.06 —

13. G rip Strength RH -.10 -.04 -.06 -.28** -.22* -.03 .11 .07 .10 .12 -.25* .36** —

14. G rip Strength LH -.06 -.01 -.08 -.26* -.20* -.01 .09 -.08 .13 .14 -.24* .40** .96** —

15. P eak Pow er -.08 .13 .02 -.07 -.14 .10 .12 .07 .21* .06 -.23* .90** .59** .60** —

M 4.69 7.13 8.39 5.22 9.38 19.64 22.00 21.92 23.77 16.51 32.56 14.60 46.41 4 2 .6 6 2929
SD 3.27 1.76 1.91 1.83 3.30 5.54 4.97 5.29 5.35 6.43 8.72 4 .6 6 15.47 13.85 992

N o te: SQI = Sleep Quality Index; KSS = Karolinska Sleepiness Scale; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale
(ADSWS Bed = Going to Bed Subscale; ADSWS Fall. = Falling Asleep Subscale; ADSWS Main. = Maintaining Sleep Subscale; ADSWS Rein. =
Reinitiating Sleep Subscale; ADSWS Ret. = Returning to Wakefulness Subscale); Grip Strength RH = Right Hand; Grip Strength LH = Left Hand;
Sleep Length W = Week; Sleep Length WE = Weekends
* p < .05 two-tailed, ** p < .01 two-tailed.
88

The Sit-and-Reach Score was negatively correlated with Grip Strength Right

Hand, Grip Strength Left Hand, and Peak Power. The Vertical Jump Score was positively

correlated with the Grip Strength Right Hand and Grip Strength Left Hand, as well as the

ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep and Returning to Wakefulness Subscales. The estimated Peak

Power Score was positively correlated with the ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep Subscale,

Vertical Jump score, Grip Strength Right Hand, and Grip Strength Left Hand.

Correlations between Variables with Males

Table 8 presents the correlations between all of the study variables with males.

The males’ SQI total score was significantly negatively correlated with the SHQ

(Weekdays), SHQ (Weekends), Sit-and-Reach Test, as well as the ADSWS Falling

Asleep, Maintaining Sleep, and Reinitiating Sleep Subscales. The males’ SHQ

(Weekdays) was significantly positively correlated with SHQ (Weekends) and the

ADSWS Reinitiating Sleep Subscale. The males’ SHQ (Weekends) was significantly

negatively correlated with the ADSWS Returning to Wakefulness Subscale. As would be

expected, the ADSWS subscales all correlated at minimal to moderate levels.

The males’ KSS Scale and the ESS Scale were significantly positively correlated.

The males’ KSS Scale was significantly negatively correlated with both the ADSWS

Falling Asleep and Returning to Wakefulness Subscales. The males’ ESS Scale was

significantly negatively correlated to the ADSWS Falling Asleep Subscale.

Males’ Grip Strength with the Right Hand was significantly positively correlated

with Grip Strength with the Left Hand. The males’ Vertical Jump Score was significantly

positively correlated with estimated Peak Power.


Table 8

Summary o f Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations fo r Scores on All Measures fo r Overall Sample with Males Only

M easures 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
1. SQI —
2. SH Q W eek -.29* —

3. SH Q W eekend -.35* .55** _


4. K SS .20 .06 -.07 —

5. E SS -.09 -.07 .02 .30* —

6. A D SW S B ed -.10 .02 -.08 -.19 -.07 —

7. A D SW S Fall -.49** .09 .04 -.29* -.32* .33* —

8. A D SW S M ain -.54** .15 .03 .00 .13 .43** .51** —

9. A D SW S R ein -.32* .34* .11 -.04 -.17 .29* .56** .50** —

10. A D SW S R et -.17 -.20 -.31* -.42** -.20 .29* .29* .02 .03 —

11. Sit and R each -.35* -.06 .08 -.03 .05 .08 .09 .20 .15 -.07 —

12. G rip S trength R H -.08 -.13 .23 -.10 -.07 .05 .10 -.08 -.01 -.04 .07 —

13. G rip S trength LH .07 -.08 .21 -.08 .03 .12 .04 -.10 .06 .00 .09 .84** -
14. V ertical Jum p -.04 .13 -.03 -.01 -.10 .10 .12 .03 .28 .17 .12 -.02 .04 —

15. P eak P ow er .12 .02 -.05 -.07 -.09 .04 .03 -.07 .15 .16 -.10 .20 .20 .80** -
M 4.21 7.13 8.10 4.75 8.63 19.90 22.67 21.94 2 4.96 17.96 29.44 59.21 54.23 15.49 3493
SD 3.10 1.85 1.97 1.80 3.09 6.05 4.34 5.05 5.00 6.32 8.11 10.51 9.24 4 .2 6 731

N ote. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; SHQ = Sleep Habits Questionnaire; KSS = Karolinska Sleepiness Scale; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale;
ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS Bed = Going to Bed Subscale; ADSWS Fall. = Falling Asleep Subscale; ADSWS Main. =
Maintaining Sleep Subscale; ADSWS Rein. = Reinitiating Sleep Subscale; ADSWS Ret. = Returning to Wakefulness Subscale); Grip Strength RH
= Right Hand; Grip Strength LH = Left Hand. * p < .05 two-tailed, ** p < .01 two-tailed

00
vO
90

Correlations between Variables with Females

Table 9 presents the correlations between all of the study variables with females.

The females’ SQI total score was significantly negatively correlated with SHQ during the

week, as well as with the ADSWS Going to Bed, Falling Asleep, Maintaining Sleep,

Reinitiating Sleep, and Returning to Wakefulness Subscales. SHQ during the week was

significantly positively correlated with SHQ during the weekends. The SHQ during the

weekends was significantly positively correlated with the ADSWS Going to Bed,

Maintaining Sleep, and Reinitiating Sleep Subscales. As would be expected, the ADSWS

subscales all correlated at minimal to moderate levels. The ESS Scale was significantly

negatively correlated to the ADSWS Returning to Wakefulness Subscale.

The Grip Strength with the Right Hand was significantly negatively correlated to

both the ADSWS Going to Bed and Reinitiating Sleep Subscales. As would be expected,

the Grip Strength with the Right Hand was significantly positively correlated to the Grip

Strength with the Left Hand. The Grip Strength with the Left Hand was significantly

negatively correlated to both the ADSWS Going to Bed Subscale and ADSWS

Reinitiating Sleep Subscale.

In addition, the Grip Strength with the Left Hand and the estimated Peak Power

were significantly positively correlated. The Vertical Jump score was positively

correlated with both the ADSWS Maintaining Sleep and Reinitiating Sleep Subscales, as

well as estimated Peak Power.


Table 9

Summary o f Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations fo r Scores on All Measures fo r Overall Sample with Females Only

M easures 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
1. SQI 1
2. SH Q W eekdays -.31* 1
3. SH Q W eekend -.22 .35* 1
4. K SS .13 -.20 .09 1
5. ESS -.10 -.10 .18 .17 1
6. A D SW S B ed -.42** .21 .29* -.23 -.06 1
7. A D SW S Fall -.66** -.03 .10 -.06 .04 .36* 1
8. A D SW S M ain -.73** .11 .38** .05 .22 .44** 59** 1
9. A D SW S Rein -.62** .09 .31* -.06 .16 .42** .64** .72** 1
10. A D SW S Ret -.39** .12 -.18 -.06 -.36** .27 .22 .21 .29* 1
11. Sit and Reach -.04 -.05 .01 -.12 .05 .02 .21 .10 .21 -.02 1
12. G rip Strength R H .26 .01 -.08 -.11 -.08 -.36* -.20 -.24 -.43** -.23 .08 1
13. G rip Strength LH .26 .06 -.10 -.07 -.13 -.37* -.21 -.24 -.41** -.22 .10 .92** 1
14. V ertical Jum p -.25 .10 .27 .09 .08 .14 .09 .31* .31* .12 .07 -.03 .07 1
15. P eak Pow er .01 .05 .17 .21 .06 -.04 -.07 .04 .02 -.05 -.03 .24 .33* .851** 1
M 5.19 7.12 8.66 5.67 10.10 19.40 21.36 21.90 22.62 15.12 35.56 33.88 31.33 12.790 2387
SD 3.40 1.69 1.83 1.76 3.36 5.06 5.47 5.55 5.48 6.28 8.30 6.77 5.94 4 .3 2 0 907

Note. SQI = Sleep Quality Index; SHQ = Sleep Habits Questionnaire; KSS = Karolinska Sleepiness Scale; ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale;
ADSWS = Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ADSWS Bed = Going to Bed Subscale; ADSWS Fall. = Falling Asleep Subscale; ADSWS Main. =
Maintaining Sleep Subscale; ADSWS Rein. = Reinitiating Sleep Subscale; ADSWS Ret. = Returning to Wakefulness Subscale); Grip Strength RH
= Right Hand; Grip Strength LH = Left Hand. * p < .05 two-tailed, ** p < .0 1 two-tailed.
92

Hypothesis 1A

Hypothesis 1A stated that participants in the experimental group who took a 10-

minute nap/relaxation period would have significantly higher scores on the sit-and-reach

test compared to the control group. To test this hypothesis, a two-way ANOVA was

performed with gender and participant group (control or experimental) as the independent

variables, and the Sit-and-Reach Test as the dependent variable. This hypothesis was not

supported. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F( 1,

94) = 12.75,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for the participant group on

the Sit-and-Reach Test, F (l, 94) = 0.10,/? >.05. There was also a non-significant

interaction effect between the participant group and gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test,

F (l, 94) = 0.10,/? >.05. Results show that males in the experimental group (n = 22; M =

30.02) did not have significantly higher sit-and-reach scores compared to males in the

control group (n = 26; M - 28.94). In addition, females in the experimental group (n =

31; M = 35.56) did not have significantly higher sit-and-reach scores than females in the

control group (n = 19; M = 35.55).

Hypothesis IB

Hypothesis IB stated that participants in the experimental group who took a 10-

minute nap/relaxation period would have significantly higher scores on the hand

dynamometer test on both right and left hands. Two separate two-way ANOVAs were

performed. One two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and participant group

(control or experimental) as the independent variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test

on the right hand as the dependent variable. Another two-way ANOVA was performed
93

with gender and participant group (control or experimental) as the independent variables,

and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand as the dependent variable.

This hypothesis was supported (with the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right

hand). For the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, there was a significant main

effect for gender, F (l, 91) = 187.91,/? < .01. In addition, there was a significant main

effect for the participant group on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F( 1,

91) = 5.16,/? < .05. There was a non-significant interaction effect between the participant

group and gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F (l, 91) = 1.20,

p >.05. Results show that hand dynamometer scores on the right hand are significantly

higher with males in the experimental group (n = 21; M = 58.05) compared to males in

the control group (n = 26; M = 60.15). Females in the experimental group (n = 29; M —

31.48) also have significantly higher hand dynamometer scores on the right hand than

females in the control group (n = 19; M = 37.53).

This hypothesis was not supported (with the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left

hand). There was a significant main effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on

the left hand, F (l, 91) = 196.55,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for the

participant group on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F (l, 91) = 3.58,

p >.05. In addition, there was a non-significant interaction effect between the participant

group and gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F(\, 91) = 0.50,

p >.05. Results show that males in the experimental group (n = 21; M = 53.19) did not

have significantly higher hand dynamometer scores on the left hand compared to males in

the control group (n = 26; M = 55.08). In addition, females in the experimental group
94

(n = 29; M = 29.67) did not have significantly higher hand dynamometer scores on the

left hand than females in the control group (n = 19; M = 33.84).

Hypothesis 1C

Hypothesis 1C stated participants in the experimental group who took a 10-

minute nap/relaxation period would have significantly higher scores on the Vertical Jump

Test compared to the control group. To test this hypothesis, a two-way ANOVA was

performed with gender and participant group (control or experimental) as the independent

variables, and the Vertical Jump Test as the dependent variable. This hypothesis was not

supported. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F (l,

94) = 18.30,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for the participant group on

the Vertical Jump Test, F (l, 94) = 0.11,/? >.05. In addition, there was a non-significant

interaction effect between the participant group and gender on the Vertical Jump Test,

F( 1,94) = 0.53,/? >.05. Results show that males in the experimental group (n = 22;

M = 16.30) did not have significantly higher vertical jump scores compared to males in

the control group (n = 26; M = 16.65). Additionally, females in the experimental group

(n = 31; M = 13.15) did not have significantly higher vertical jump scores than females

in the control group (n = 19; M ~ 12.21).

Hypothesis ID

Hypothesis ID stated participants in the experimental group who took a 10-

minute nap/relaxation period would have significantly higher scores on Estimated Peak

Power. A two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and participant group (control or

experimental) as the independent variables, and Estimated Peak Power as the dependent

variable. This hypothesis was not supported. There was a significant main effect for
95

gender on Estimated Peak Power, F (l, 94) = 43.73,/? < .01. There was a non-significant

main effect for the participant group on Estimated Peak Power, F (l, 94) = 0.24,/? >.05.

Additionally, there was a non-significant interaction effect between the participant group

and gender on Estimated Peak Power, F( 1, 94) = 0.50,/? >.05. Results show that males in

the experimental group (n = 22; M = 3472) did not have significantly higher estimated

peak power scores compared to males in the control group (n = 26; M = 3510). Females

in the experimental group (n = 31; M = 2465) did not have significantly higher estimated

peak power scores than females in the control group (n = 19; M = 2260).

Hypothesis 2A

Hypothesis 2A stated that individuals with poor sleep quality will have

significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach Test compared to individuals with good

sleep quality (as measured by the SQI and the ADSWS). To test this hypothesis, two

separate two-way ANOVAs were performed. One two-way ANOVA was performed with

gender and sleep quality (SQI scores were divided into good and poor sleep quality

categories) as the independent variables, and the Sit-and-Reach Test as the dependent

variable. Another two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep quality

(ADSWS scores were divided into good and poor sleep quality categories) as the

independent variables, and the Sit-and-Reach Test as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

SQI. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F (l, 91) =

8.39,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as measured by

the SQI) on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F (l, 91) = 0.81,/? >.05. There was also a non­

significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) and gender
96

on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F(\, 91) = 0.08,/? >.05. Male poor sleepers (n = 5; M = 26.80)

(as measured by the SQI) did not have significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach

Test compared to male good sleepers (n = 43; M = 29.74). Female poor sleepers (n = 8;

M = 34.75) (as measured by the SQI) did not have significantly lower scores on the Sit-

and-Reach Test than female good sleepers (n = 39\ M - 36.32).

Additionally, the results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis

using the ADSWS. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Sit-and-Reach

Test, F (l, 94) = 13.98,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality

(as measured by the ADSWS) on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F( 1, 94) = 0.73, p >.05. In

addition, there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as

measured by the ADSWS) and gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F (l, 94) = 0.04,/? >.05.

Male poor sleepers (n = 22; M = 28.84) (as measured by the ADSWS) did not have

significantly lower scores on the sit-and-reach test compared to male good sleepers

(n = 26; M = 29.94). Female poor sleepers (n = 28; M = 34.79) (as measured by the

ADSWS) did not have significantly lower scores on the sit-and-reach test than female

good sleepers (n = 22; M = 36.55).

Hypothesis 2B

Hypothesis 2B stated that the individuals with poor sleep quality will have

significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test (right and left hand). Four

separate two-way ANOVAs were performed. The first ANOVA was performed with

gender and sleep quality (SQI scores were divided into good and poor sleep quality

categories) as the independent variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right

hand as the dependent variable. The second ANOVA was performed with gender and
97

sleep quality (SQI scores were divided into good and poor sleep quality categories) as the

independent variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand as the dependent

variable. The third ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep quality (ADSWS

scores were divided into good and poor sleep quality categories) as the independent

variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand as the dependent variable.

The fourth ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep quality (ADSWS scores were

divided into good and poor sleep quality categories) as the independent variables, and the

Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the SQI

and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand. There was a significant main effect

for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, ^(1, 88) = 74.58, p < .01.

There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) on the

Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F( 1, 88) = 0.07, p >.05. In addition, there was

a non-significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) and

gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F{ 1, 88) = 0.96, p >.05. Males

in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 5; M = 56.20) (as measured by the SQI) did not have

significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand compared to

males in the Good Sleep Group (n = 42; M = 59.57). Females in the Poor Sleep Group (n

= 8; M = 35.25) (as measured by the SQI) did not have significantly lower scores on the

Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand than females in the Good Sleep Group

(n = 37; M = 33.27).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the SQI

and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand. There was a significant main effect for
98

gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F(l, 88) = 97.94, p < .01. There

was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) on the Hand

Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F( 1, 88) = 0.79, p >.05. In addition, there was a non­

significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) and gender

on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F (l, 88) = 0.05,p >.05. Males in the

Poor Sleep Group (n = 5; M = 56.60) (as measured by the SQI) did not have significantly

lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand compared to males in the

Good Sleep Group (n = 42; M = 53.95). Females in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 8; M =

32.38) (as measured by the SQI) did not have significantly lower scores on the hand

dynamometer on the left hand in comparison to females in the Good Sleep Group (n = 37;

M = 30.76).

Additionally, the results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis

using the ADSWS and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand. There was a

significant main effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F( 1,

91) = 200.64, p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as

measured by the ADSWS) on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(\, 91) =

0.86, p >.05. In addition, there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleep

quality (as measured by the ADSWS) and gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the

right hand, F (l, 91) = 2.48,/? >.05. Males in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 22; M = 58.59)

(as measured by the ADSWS) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand

Dynamometer Test on the right hand compared to males in the Good Sleep Group

(n = 25; M = 59.76). In addition, females in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 27; M = 35.85)

(as measured by the ADSWS) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand
99

Dynamometer Test on the right hand than females in the Good Sleep Group (n = 21;

M = 31.33).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

ADSWS and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand. There was a significant main

effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F (l, 91) = 212.86,

p <.01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as measured by the

ADSWS) on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F (l, 91) = 0.28,/? >.05. In

addition, there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as

measured by the ADSWS) and gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand,

F (l, 91) = 3.76,/? >.05. Males in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 2 2; M= 53.05) (as measured

by the ADSWS) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test

on the left hand compared to males in the Good Sleep Group (n = 25; M - 55.28).

Additionally, females in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 27; M= 33.04) (as measured by the

ADSWS) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the

left hand in comparison to females in the Good Sleep Group (n = 21; M = 29.14).

Hypothesis 2C

Hypothesis 2C stated that individuals with poor sleep quality will have

significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test compared to individuals with good

sleep quality. Two separate two-way ANOVAs were performed. One two-way ANOVA

was performed with gender and sleep quality (SQI scores were divided into good and

poor sleep quality categories) as the independent variables, and the Vertical Jump Test as

the dependent variable. Another two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep
100

quality (ADSWS scores were divided into good and poor sleep quality categories) as the

independent variables, and the Vertical Jump Test as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

SQI. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F (l, 91) =

14.21,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as measured by

the SQI) on the Vertical Jump Test, F(l, 91) = 1.77,/? >.05. There was also a non­

significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) and gender

on the Vertical Jump Test, F (l, 91) = 1.80,/? >.05. The male poor sleepers (n = 5;

M = 16.50) from the SQI did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump

Test compared to male good sleepers (n = 43; M = 16.49). Female poor sleepers (n = 8;

M = 9.81) from the SQI did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test

than female good sleepers (n = 39; M = 13.31).

Additionally, the results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis

using the ADSWS. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Vertical Jump

Test, F (l, 94) = 16.80,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality

(as measured by the ADSWS) on the Vertical Jump Test, F (l, 94) = 2.00,/? >.05. In

addition, there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as

measured by the ADSWS) and gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F(l, 94) = 0.03,/? >.05.

Male poor sleepers (n = 22; M = 15.91) from the ADSWS did not have significantly

lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test in comparison to male good sleepers (n = 26; M =

16.98). Female poor sleepers (n = 28; M = 12.18) from the ADSWS did not have

significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test compared to female good sleepers

(n =22; M= 13.57).
101

Hypothesis 2D

Hypothesis 2D stated that the individuals with poor sleep quality will have

significantly lower scores on estimated Peak Power compared to individuals with good

sleep quality. To test this hypothesis, two separate two-way ANOVAs were performed.

One two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep quality (SQI scores were

divided into good and poor sleep quality categories) as the independent variables, and

estimated Peak Power as the dependent variable. Another two-way ANOVA was

performed with gender and sleep quality (ADSWS scores were divided into good and

poor sleep quality categories) as the independent variables, and estimated Peak Power as

the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

SQI. There was a significant main effect for gender on estimated Peak Power, F ( l , 91) =

26.26, p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep quality (as measured by

the SQI) on estimated Peak Power, F (l, 91) = 0.00,/? >.05. In addition, there was a non­

significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as measured by the SQI) and gender

on estimated Peak Power, ,F(1,91) = 0.31, p >.05. Males in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 5;

M = 3604) from the SQI did not have significantly lower scores on Peak Power compared

to males in the Good Sleep Group (n = 43; M - 3480). Females in the Poor Sleep Group

(n = 8; M = 2197) from the SQI did not have significantly lower scores on Peak Power

compared to females in the Good Sleep Group (n = 39; M = 2350).

Additionally, the results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis

using the ADSWS. There was a significant main effect for gender on estimated Peak

Power, F (l, 94) = 43.03,/? < .01. There was also a non-significant main effect for sleep
102

quality (as measured by the ADSWS) on estimated Peak Power, F( 1, 94) = 0.05, p >.05.

There was also a non-significant interaction effect between sleep quality (as measured by

the ADSWS) and gender on estimated Peak Power, F(1, 94) = 0 A \ , p > .05. Males in the

Poor Sleep Group (n = 2 2 ; M = 3483) from the ADSWS did not have significantly lower

scores on Peak Power compared to males in the Good Sleep Group (n = 26; M = 3501).

Females in the Poor Sleep Group (n = 28; M = 2428) from the ADSWS did not have

significantly lower scores on Peak Power than females in the Good Sleep Group (n = 22;

M = 2335).

Hypothesis 3A

Hypothesis 3A stated that individuals with shorter sleep duration will have

significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach Test compared to individuals with longer

sleep duration. To test this hypothesis, a two-way ANOVA was performed with gender

and sleep duration (SHQ scores were divided into short, medium, and long sleep duration

categories) as the independent variables, and the Sit-and-Reach Test as the dependent

variable. The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. There was

a significant main effect for gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F( 1,91) = 6.14, p < .05.

There was a non-significant main effect for the participant group on the Sit-and-Reach

Test, F(2, 91) = 0.47, p >.05. There was also a non-significant interaction effect between

the participant group and gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F(2, 91) = 0.66, p >.05. Male

short sleepers (n = 12; M = 29.71) did not have significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-

Reach Test in comparison to male long sleepers (n = 7 ; M = 29.29) or male medium

sleepers (n = 29; M = 29.36). Female short sleepers (n = 14; M = 32.64) did not have
103

significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach Test compared to female long sleepers

(n = 3; M = 36.17) or female medium sleepers (n = 32; M = 36.75).

Hypothesis 3B

Hypothesis 3B stated that individuals with shorter sleep duration will have

significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test (right and left hand) compared

to individuals with longer sleep duration. Two separate two-way ANOVAs were

performed. One two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep duration (SHQ

scores were divided into short, medium, and long sleep duration categories) as the

independent variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand as the

dependent variable. Another two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep

duration (SHQ scores were divided into short, medium, and long sleep duration

categories) as the independent variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left

hand as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did support this hypothesis on the right hand.

There was a significant main effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the

right hand, F (l, 88) = 73.69, p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep

duration on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(2, 88) = 0.01,/? >.05. In

addition, there was a significant interaction effect between sleep duration and gender on

the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(2, 88) = 4.22,/? < .05. Male short

sleepers (n = 11; M = 58.27) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand

Dynamometer Test with the right hand compared to male long sleepers (n = 7; M =

52.71) or male medium sleepers (n = 29; M = 61.14). Female short sleepers (n = 13;

M - 35.62) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test with
104

the right hand compared to female long sleepers (n = 3; M = 41.67) or female medium

sleepers (n = 31; M = 32.61).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis on the left

hand. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on

the left hand, F (l, 88) = 82.52, p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleep

duration on the Hand Dynamometer Test the left hand, F(2, 88) = 0.27, p >.05. In

addition, there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleep duration and gender

on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F(2, 88) = 2.11,p >.05. Male short

sleepers (n = 11; M - 54.00) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand

Dynamometer Test with the left hand compared to male long sleepers (n = 7; 51.29)

or male medium sleepers (n = 29; M = 55.03). Additionally, female short sleepers (n =

13; M = 32.77) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test

with the left hand compared to female long sleepers (n = 3; M = 38.00) or female medium

sleepers (n = 31; M = 30.32).

Hypothesis 3C

Hypothesis 3C stated that individuals with shorter sleep duration will have

significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test compared to individuals with longer

sleep duration. To test this hypothesis, a two-way ANOVA was performed with gender

and sleep duration (SHQ scores were divided into short, medium, and long sleep duration

categories) as the independent variables, and the Vertical Jump Test as the dependent

variable. The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. There was

a significant main effect for gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F (l, 91) = 8.46,/? < .01.

There was a non-significant main effect for sleep duration on the Vertical Jump Test, F(2,
105

91) = 1.61,/? >.05. Additionally, there was a non-significant interaction effect between

sleep duration and gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F(2, 91) = 0.02,/? >.05. Males in the

Short Sleep Group (n = 12; M = 16.13) did not have significantly lower scores on the

Vertical Jump Test compared to males in the Long Sleep Group (n = 7; M = 18.64) or

males in the Medium Sleep Group (n = 29; M = 16.12). In addition, females in the Short

Sleep Group (n = 14; M ~ 12.57) did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical

Jump Test in comparison to females in the Long Sleep Group (n = 3; M = 15.5) or

females in the Medium Sleep Group (n = 32; M = 12.44).

Hypothesis 3D

Hypothesis 3D stated that individuals with shorter sleep duration will have

significantly lower scores on estimated Peak Power compared to individuals with longer

sleep duration. A two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleep duration (SHQ

scores were divided into short, medium, and long sleep duration categories) as the

independent variables, and estimated Peak Power as the dependent variable. The results

of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. There was a significant main

effect for gender on estimated Peak Power, F (l, 91) = 13.85,/? < .01. There was a non­

significant main effect for sleep duration on estimated Peak Power, F(2, 91) = 2.14,

p >.05. There was also a non-significant interaction effect between sleep duration and

gender on estimated Peak Power, F(2, 91) = 1.36,/? >.05. Males in the Short Sleep Group

(n = 12; M = 3498) did not have significantly lower scores on estimated Peak Power

compared to males in the Long Sleep Group (n = 7; M = 3594) or males in the Medium

Sleep Group (n = 29;M = 3466). In addition, females in the Short Sleep Group (n = 14;

M = 2596) did not have significantly lower scores on estimated Peak Power in
106

comparison to females in the Long Sleep Group (n = 3; M = 3225) or females in the

Medium Sleep Group (n = 32; M = 2196).

Hypothesis 4A

Hypothesis 4A stated that individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have

significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach Test compared to individuals with lower

levels of sleepiness. To test this hypothesis, two separate two-way ANOVAs were

performed. One two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (KSS

scores were divided into high, medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent

variables, and the Sit-and-Reach Test as the dependent variable. Another two-way

ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (ESS scores were divided into high,

medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent variables, and the Sit-and-

Reach Test as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

KSS. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F (l, 91)

= 10.97, p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by

the KSS) on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F{2, 91) = 0.10,/? >.05. There was also a non­

significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the KSS) and gender on

the Sit-and-Reach Test, F(2, 91) = 0.28,/? >.05. Males in the KSS High Sleepiness Group

(n = 12; M = 30.58) did not have significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach Test

compared to the males in the Low Sleepiness Group (n = 16; M = 29.53) or males in the

Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 20;M = 28.68). In addition, females in the KSS High

Sleepiness Group (n = 17; M = 34.68) did not have significantly lower scores on the Sit-
107

and-Reach Test in comparison to females in the Low Sleepiness Group (n = 8;

M - 36.56) or females in the Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 24; M = 35.50).

Additionally, the results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis

using the ESS. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test,

F (l, 92) = 9.26, p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as

measured by the ESS) on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F (2,92) = 0.56, p >.05. In addition,

there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the

ESS) and gender on the Sit-and-Reach Test, F{2, 92) = 0.09,/? >.05. The ESS male High

Sleepiness Group (n = 7; M = 29.07) did not have significantly lower scores on the Sit-

and-Reach Test in comparison to the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 35; M = 29.29) or

male Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 6; M - 30.75). The ESS female High Sleepiness

Group (n = 11; M = 35.05) did not show significantly lower scores on the Sit-and-Reach

Test compared to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 28; M = 34.73) or the female

Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 11; M = 38.18).

Hypothesis 4B

Hypothesis 4B stated that individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have

significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test (right and left hand) compared

to individuals with lower levels of sleepiness. Four separate two-way ANOVAs were

performed. The first two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (KSS

scores were divided into high, medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent

variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand as the dependent variable.

The second two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (KSS scores

were divided into high, medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent
108

variables, and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand as the dependent variable.

The third two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (ESS scores were

divided into high, medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent variables,

and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand as the dependent variable. The fourth

two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (ESS scores were divided

into high, medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent variables, and the

Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

KSS and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand. There was a significant main

effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F (l, 88) = 167.20,

p <.01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the KSS)

on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(2, 88) = 1.78,p >.05. Additionally,

there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the

KSS) and gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(2, 88) = 1.79,

p >.05. The KSS male High Sleepiness Group (n = 11; Af = 55.45) did not have

significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand compared to

the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 16; M = 57.44) or male Medium Sleepiness Group

(n = 20; M = 62.70). In addition, the KSS female High Sleepiness Group (n = 17;

M = 32.71) did not have significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on

the right hand compared to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 8; M = 35.75) or

female Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 22; M = 33.55).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

KSS and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand. There was a significant main
109

effect for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F( 1, 88) = 173.34,

p < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the KSS)

on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F{2, 88) = 0.61,/? >.05. Additionally,

there was a non-significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the

KSS) and gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on left hand, F(2, 88) = 0.34, p >.05.

The KSS male High Sleepiness Group (n = 11; M = 52.45) have significantly lower

scores on the left hand in comparison to the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 16; M =

53.50) or male Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 20; M - 55.80). In addition, the KSS

female High Sleepiness Group (n = 17; M = 30.41) have significantly lower scores on the

left hand in comparison to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 8; M = 32.13) or

female Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 22; M = 31.36).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the ESS

and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand. There was a significant main effect

for gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F (l, 89) = 115.02,/? < .01.

There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the ESS) on the

Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(2, 89) = 0.86, p >.05. In addition, there was

a non-significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the ESS) and

gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand, F(2, 89) = 0.01, p >.05. The

ESS male High Sleepiness Group (n = 7\ M = 56.29) did not have significantly lower

scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand compared to the male Low

Sleepiness Group (n = 35; M = 59.86) or male Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 5;

M = 58.80). In addition, the ESS female High Sleepiness Group (n = 10; M = 31.70) did

not have significantly lower scores on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the right hand
110

compared to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 28; M = 34.61) or female Medium

Sleepiness Group (n = 10; M = 34.00).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the ESS

and the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand. There was a significant main effect for

gender on the Hand Dynamometer Test on the left hand, F( 1, 89) = 125.27,/? < .01.

There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the ESS) on the

left hand, F(2, 89) = 0.42,/? >.05. In addition, there was a non-significant interaction

effect between sleepiness (as measured by the ESS) and gender on the Hand

Dynamometer Test on left hand, F(2, 89) = 0.18,/? >.05. The ESS male High Sleepiness

Group (n = 7;M = 53.57) have significantly lower scores on the left hand in comparison

to the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 35; M = 54.49) or male Medium Sleepiness

Group (n = 5; M = 53.40). In addition, the ESS female High Sleepiness Group (n = 10;

M = 28.90) have significantly lower scores on the left hand in comparison to the female

Low Sleepiness Group (n = 28; M = 31.96) or female Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 10;

M = 32.00).

Hypothesis 4C

Hypothesis 4C stated that individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have

significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test compared to individuals with lower

levels of sleepiness. To test this hypothesis, two separate two-way ANOVAs were

performed. One two-way ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (KSS

scores were divided into high, medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent

variables, and the Vertical Jump Test as the dependent variable. Another two-way

ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (ESS scores were divided into high,
Ill

medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent variables, and the Vertical

Jump Test as the dependent variable.

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

KSS. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F{ 1,91) =

14.58,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the

KSS) on the Vertical Jump Test, F(2, 91) = 0.17,/? >.05. Additionally, there was a non­

significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the KSS) and gender on

the Vertical Jump Test, F(2,91) = 1.24,/? >.05. The KSS male High Sleepiness Group (n

= 12; M = 15.46) did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test in

comparison to the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 16; M = 16.22) or male Medium

Sleepiness Group (n = 20; M - 17.33). In addition, the KSS female High Sleepiness

Group (n = 17; M= 13.76) did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump

Test in comparison to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 8; M - 12.13) or female

Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 24; M= 12.35).

The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

ESS. There was a significant main effect for gender on the Vertical Jump Test, F (l, 92) =

9.39,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the

ESS) on the Vertical Jump Test, F(2, 92) = 0.84,/? >.05. There was also a non-significant

interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the ESS) and gender on the

Vertical Jump Test, F(2, 92) = 1.02,/? >.05. The ESS male High Sleepiness Group (n = 7;

M = 16.64) did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test compared to

the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 35; M = 16.49) or the male Medium Sleepiness

Group (n = 6; M = 16.33). In addition, the ESS female High Sleepiness Group (n = 11;
112

M = 12.55) did not have significantly lower scores on the Vertical Jump Test in

comparison to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 28; M = 11.93) or the female

Medium Sleepiness Group (n = 11; M = 15.23).

Hypothesis 4D

Hypothesis 4D stated that individuals with higher levels of sleepiness will have

significantly lower scores on estimated Peak Power compared to individuals with lower

levels o f sleepiness. Two separate two-way ANOVAs were performed. One two-way

ANOVA was performed with gender and sleepiness (KSS scores were divided into high,

medium, and low sleepiness categories) as the independent variables, and estimated Peak

Power as the dependent variable. Another two-way ANOVA was performed with gender

and sleepiness (ESS scores were divided into high, medium, and low sleepiness

categories) as the independent variables, and estimated Peak Power as the dependent

variable. The results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis using the

KSS. There was a significant main effect for gender on estimated Peak Power, F (l, 91) =

38.38,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as measured by the

KSS) on estimated Peak Power, F(2, 91) = 0.74,/? >.05. In addition, there was a non­

significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the KSS) and gender on

estimated Peak Power, F(2,91) = 3.02,/? >.05. The KSS male High Sleepiness Group (n

= 12; M = 3188) did not have significantly lower estimated Peak Power scores compared

to the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 16; M = 3399) or the male Medium Sleepiness

Group (n = 20; M = 3751). In addition, the KSS female High Sleepiness Group (n = 17;

M = 2669) did not have significantly lower estimated Peak Power scores in comparison
113

to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 8; M = 2107) or the female Medium Sleepiness

Group (n = 24; M = 2281).

Additionally, the results of the two-way ANOVA did not support this hypothesis

using the ESS. There was a significant main effect for gender on estimated Peak Power,

F (l, 92) = 26.04,/? < .01. There was a non-significant main effect for sleepiness (as

measured by the ESS) on estimated Peak Power, F(2, 92) = 1.57,/? >.05. There was also

a non-significant interaction effect between sleepiness (as measured by the ESS) and

gender on estimated Peak Power, F(2, 92) = 1.01,/? >.05. The ESS male High Sleepiness

Group (n = 7; M = 3419) did not have significantly lower estimated Peak Power scores in

comparison to the male Low Sleepiness Group (n = 35; M = 3497) or the male Medium

Sleepiness Group (n = 6; M = 3553). In addition, the ESS female High Sleepiness Group

(n = 11; M = 2180) did not have significantly lower estimated Peak Power scores

compared to the female Low Sleepiness Group (n = 28; M - 2249) or the female Medium

Sleepiness Group (n = 11; M = 2946).


CHAPTER 4

DISCUSSION

This last chapter discusses the general overview o f the results, followed by a

discussion of each hypothesis. The general implications for the study are also discussed,

followed by the limitations of the study and future directions.

General Overview of Results

The results o f this study revealed that the majority (68%) of participants

experienced occasional sleep difficulties, as measured by the SQI. Only 15% of the

sample reported good sleep quality and over 13% indicated that their sleep quality was

poor. This means that nearly nine out of 10 participants regularly experienced moderate

to poor sleep quality. An examination of item responses on the SQI revealed that over

25% of the participants experienced insomnia and 13% take sleep medication.

Additionally, over half of the participants indicated that they felt tired in the morning.

Almost 40% of the participants reported difficulty falling asleep and over 40% indicated

disturbed sleep. In addition, 33% of participants wake up during the night and over 40%

awaken too early in the morning. This may indicate that even though they are not having

major sleep problems, they are still experiencing sufficient sleep problems to feel tired in

the morning due to lack of sleep.

114
115

The participants slept an average of seven hours 13 minutes a day on week nights

and eight hours 39 minutes on weekend nights. O f the participants, 26% slept six hours

30 minutes or less during the week, while 10% slept nine hours or more. According to the

National Sleep Foundation, an adult needs from seven to nine hours of sleep per night.

This indicates that 26% o f the participants in this study slept less than the recommended

amount. Over 35% reported experiencing sleepiness outside the normal range.

Additionally, the current sample had a higher average on the ESS and KSS compared to

other studies, which indicates these participants had a higher level of sleepiness. This

provides further evidence suggesting that participants are experiencing sleep difficulties.

The results also revealed that the current sample had significantly lower vertical

jump height, peak power, and grip strength compared to other studies. This difference in

grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak power in this sample, compared to other

studies, may be in part due to the sleep difficulties participants are experiencing. The

results were compared to other studies with similarly aged college students and were not

athletes or active Kinesiology students.

The relationship between sleep quality/sleepiness and physical performance

varied. Lower grip strength was associated with increased sleepiness, as measured by

both the ESS and KSS. According to the evolutionary/circadian rhythm models of sleep,

sleep may have evolved to conserve energy. According to Berger and Phillips (1995),

phylogenetic and ontogenetic associations between sleep and endothermy are consistent

with the hypothesis that sleep evolved in conjunction with endothermy to offset the high

energetic cost of endothermy. The electrophysiological and thermoregulatory continuum

of slow wave sleep, circadian torpor and hibernation substantiates a primordial link
116

between sleep and energy conservation. Sleep constitutes a circadian and circannual

rhythm o f hypometabolic adaptation to biospheric energy cycles that is usually entrained

through light-mediated suppression of melatonin secretion. When energy stores decline,

energy is conserved by lowering body temperature (Tb) proportionally during sleep or by

increasing the daily duration of sleep. Those who are not sleepy tend to have better grip

strength because they conserved enough energy while they slept; therefore they were alert

and had energy to perform well on the grip strength test.

Vertical jump height was positively related to returning to sleep after an

awakening during the sleep period on the ADS WS. Vertical jump height was also

positively related to the transition from sleep to wakefulness on the ADSWS. It may be

that those who have reduced ability to reinitiate sleep following an awakening or to wake

up in the morning have difficulty generating the power for vertical jump height. In

addition, there was a significant positive relationship between peak power and returning

to sleep after awakening. This could mean that those who have difficulty reinitiating

sleep after an awakening have not slept through the proper sleep cycles, which in turn

may affect peak power.

There were no significant relationships between sleep duration (as measured on

the SHQ) and anaerobic performance, indicating that the length of time one sleeps may

not impact immediate, short-term physical activities. It could be that the time of day the

study was conducted affected physical performance. The data was collected from 8:00-

10:00am and 4:00-6:00pm. Most of the data was collected in the later afternoon/early

evening. Due to the time frame that most of the data was collected, the results could be

impacted by the “post-lunch dip,” which would reflect 12-hour biologic cycles of
117

sleepiness (Hayashi, Morikawa, & Hori, 2002). It could also be that because the physical

performance measures were short in duration, the participants were able to perform even

though they were tired. Individuals may have been able to function well enough on the

physical performance measures because they were immediate, short-term physical

activities, despite their levels of sleepiness. However, it is possible that if the physical

performance measures were longer in duration, individuals may not have been able to

overcome the effects of sleepiness like they could in brief periods of time.

Hypothesis One

Hypotheses 1A through ID posited that individuals who took a 10-minute

nap/relaxation would have significantly better flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump

height, and peak power than individuals in the control group. Analysis indicated that the

10-minute nap/relaxation group did significantly improve grip strength (Hypothesis IB).

However, the 10-minute nap/relaxation group did not significantly impact flexibility

(Hypothesis 1A), vertical jump height (Hypothesis 1C), or peak power (Hypothesis ID).

Hypothesis 1A. Hypothesis 1A stated that a 10-minute nap/relaxation would

significantly improve flexibility. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. There are numerous sports in which flexibility plays an important role. Even

though a 10-minute nap/relaxation period did not significant improve flexibility, it still

could benefit athletes in sports where even a small amount of increase in flexibility could

assist them in performing better and perhaps even lead to winning.

The ineffectiveness o f this intervention for flexibility may be due to several

reasons. One reason is that there may be no effect of a nap/relaxation on flexibility.

Another possible reason is the 10-minute time frame of the nap/relaxation that was used.
118

It is possible that a 10-minute nap/relaxation did not allow participants adequate time to

sleep; therefore, not improving alertness and flexibility. The nap/relaxation may have

been more effective if conducted over a longer period of time, allowing participants to

have the necessary sleep/relaxation time to increase their flexibility. In addition, the

sleep/relaxation may need to be monitored with an Actigraph to determine if individuals

are actually sleeping for 10 minutes. It is possible that by monitoring participants with an

Actigraph, it would be assured that they slept/relaxed for 10 minutes, which may have led

to them experiencing changes in their flexibility. Relaxation does not mean that muscles

are physically lengthening, which would result in improvements. Napping has been found

to increase alertness, memory, motor skills, decision-making, and mood, which are not

components in performing well on a sit-and-reach test.

Hypothesis IB. Hypothesis IB stated that a 10-minute nap/relaxation would

significantly improve grip strength. The results of the ANOVA did support this

hypothesis on the right hand, but not the left hand. This means that a 10-minute

nap/relaxation period may improve grip strength. The 10-minute nap/relaxation period

may have resulted in reduced muscle tension or increased focus, which enabled

participants to feel relaxed and improve their grip strength. Short daytime naps of less

than 30 minutes, which rarely contain SWS, have been shown to have positive effects on

daytime alertness (Takahashi, 2003). This has been experimentally confirmed after a

normal night of sleep in young adults and elderly individuals, after a restricted night of

sleep, and during prolonged sustained performance (Naitoh, Kelly, & Babkoff, 1992).

Tietzel and Lack (2002) found that a 10-minute brief nap had a recuperative effect,

suggesting that the recuperative power of a short nap depends on stage 2 sleep, not stage
119

1. According to Hayashi et al. (2005), a daytime nap containing three minutes of stage 2

sleep has recuperative effects, whereas these effects are limited following only stage 1

sleep. However, these results are in contrast to Waterhouse, Atkinson, Edwards, and

Reilly (2007) who found that a post-lunch nap did not improve grip strength. In the study

by Waterhouse et al. (2007), there was actually a decline in grip strength. Because the

researchers examined the grip strength scores individually instead of taking the best of

the three scores, this decline could be explained by muscle fatigue (Waterhouse et al.,

2007).

Hypothesis 1C. Hypothesis 1C stated that a 10-minute nap/relaxation would

significantly improve vertical jump height. The results of the ANOVA did not support

this hypothesis. The ineffectiveness of this intervention for vertical jump height may be

due to several reasons. One reason is that there may be no effect of a nap/relaxation on

vertical jump height. It is also important to remember that the current sample had

significantly higher levels o f sleepiness than the standardization groups for the ESS and

KSS. This suggests that the current sample may not have been able to benefit from the

10-minute nap/relaxation, because they needed a longer nap/relaxation period to improve

their vertical jump height.

Hypothesis ID. Hypothesis ID stated that a 10-minute nap/relaxation would

significantly improve peak power. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. The ineffectiveness of this intervention for peak power may be due to several

reasons. One reason could be that there may be no effect of a nap/relaxation on peak

power. It may also be possible that the 10-minute nap/relaxation may have been

beneficial in different formats (e.g., Progressive Muscle Relaxation, Visual Imagery),


120

instead of a guided breathing relaxation. In addition, it could be possible that the 10-

minute nap/relaxation would be more effective if participants would have been able to lie

down. Participants may have been able to fall asleep or feel more relaxed if they felt

comfortable by being able to lie down. By lying down, they may have achieved the

relaxation needed for improvement in peak power. Individuals are more likely to fall

asleep while lying down than sitting in a chair, because people regularly sleep lying down

and their bodies are cued to become sleepy when they lay down.

Hypothesis Two

Hypotheses 2A through 2D posited that individuals with good sleep quality would

have significantly better flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak power

than individuals with poor sleep quality. Results revealed that sleep quality did not

significantly impact flexibility (Hypothesis 2A), grip strength (Hypothesis 2B), vertical

jump height (Hypothesis 2C), or peak power (Hypothesis 2D).

Hypothesis 2A. Hypothesis 2A stated that poor sleep quality will reduce

flexibility. The results of the ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. The hypothesis not

being significantly supported for flexibility may be due to the possibility that flexibility is

not affected by sleep quality. Sleep quality was assessed using the SQI and ADSWS, but

no significant results were found using the ADSWS and flexibility. However, the

participants with good sleep quality based on the SQI did have somewhat better

flexibility than participants with poor sleep quality, even though it was not statistically

significant. This could be an advantage to athletes in several sports in which flexibility is

important. Even though sleep quality did not significantly improve flexibility, getting a
121

good night’s sleep could still be an advantage to athletes in sports where any increase in

flexibility could influence performance.

These results are in contrast to a study conducted by Lee and Lin (2007) who

found that that female Taiwan college students with poor sleep quality had a significantly

shorter distance for the sit-and-reach (flexibility) than females with good sleep quality.

The study by Lee and Lin (2007) used the Chinese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep

Quality Index (PSQI) to measure sleep quality and the current study used the SQL It is

possible that the different results are due to measurement issues as different scales were

used to assess sleep quality.

Hypothesis 2B. Hypothesis 2B stated that poor sleep quality will reduce grip

strength. The results of the ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. The hypothesis not

being significantly supported for grip strength may be due to the possibility that grip

strength is not affected by sleep quality. Sleep quality was assessed using the SQI and

ADSWS, but no significant results were found using the ADSWS and grip strength.

However, even though the results were not statistically significant, the participants with

good sleep quality did have a five point improvement in grip strength compared to

participants with poor sleep quality. This could mean that getting a good night’s sleep

may benefit athletes in sports in which an improvement in grip strength could impact

performance outcome. Dam, Ewing, Ancoli-Israel, Ensrud, Redline, and Stone (2008)

found that less REM sleep appeared to be associated with weaker grip strength in older

men. Therefore, another possible reason is that individuals with good sleep quality may

have more REM sleep, which could have improved grip strength. In addition, studies of

students have also shown that those deprived of REM sleep perform worse on academic
122

tests than those who were not deprived (Kami et al., 1994; Pilcher & Huffcutt, 1996;

Pilcher & Ott, 1998; Pilcher & Walters, 1997; Smith & Lapp, 1991). Students who

demonstrated a significant increase in REM sleep followed a period of intense learning

were found to perform significantly better on an examination than students who did not

show an increase in REM sleep (De Koninck et al., 1989). Therefore, it is a possibility

that an increase in REM sleep could improve physical performance.

These results are also inconsistent with a study conducted by Goldman et al.

(2007) who found that older women with poor sleep quality (>1.6 hours of wake after

sleep onset) had 4% weaker grip strength than participants with good sleep quality. It is

possible that because physiological measures to assess sleep quality were not used, sleep

quality was not accurately measured. It is also possible that individuals could have

overcome the effects of poor sleep for a brief period of time and performed well enough

on the hand dynamometer. In addition, older women could be more easily affected by

poor sleep quality than college students, because college students may be more resilient

to the effects of sleep.

Hypothesis 2C. Hypothesis 2C stated that individuals with poor sleep quality will

have reduced vertical jump height. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. Sleep quality was assessed using the SQI and ADSWS, but no significant

results were found with vertical jump height. The hypothesis not being supported for

vertical jump height may be due to several reasons. There may be no effect of sleep

quality on vertical jump height. Another possible reason that this hypothesis was not

supported is that participants could have been tired from the dynamic warm-up,
123

especially if they were not athletes. The warm-up may have tired people physically,

which could have hindered their vertical jump height.

Hypothesis 2D. Hypothesis 2D stated that poor sleep quality will reduce peak

power. The results of the ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. Sleep quality was

assessed using the SQI and ADSWS, but no significant results were found with peak

power. The hypothesis not being supported for peak power may be because there may be

no effect of sleep quality on peak power. The time of day could impact peak power. For

instance peak power may have been better in the morning than the afternoon, because

individuals may have been active all day and were tired by the time they participated in

the study. In addition, because peak power was based on a brief physical performance

measure and that people can overcome sleep issues for brief periods, sleep quality may

not have had a significant impact on peak power.

Hypothesis Three

Hypotheses 3A through 3D posited that individuals with short sleep duration

would have significantly reduced flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak

power compared to individuals with long sleep duration. Results revealed that sleep

duration did not significantly impact flexibility (Hypothesis 3A), grip strength

(Hypothesis 3B), vertical jump height (Hypothesis 3C), or peak power (Hypothesis 3D).

Hypothesis 3A. Hypothesis 3A stated that long or average sleep durations would

significantly improve flexibility. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. The hypothesis not being supported for flexibility may be because flexibility

may not be affected by sleep duration. Another possible reason that this hypothesis was
124

not supported is that individuals may not require the recommended number of hours of

sleep per night to let their muscles rest so that they are flexible.

Hypothesis 3B. Hypothesis 3B stated that longer sleep duration will significantly

improve grip strength. The results of the ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. The

hypothesis not being supported for grip strength may be because grip strength may not be

affected by sleep duration. Another possible reason that this hypothesis was not

supported is that the time o f day could impact grip strength. Goh, Tong, Lim, Low, and

Lee (2001) found that grip strength increased progressively during the day, but decreased

during the night. Cappaert (1999) found similar results, indicating that grip strength

peaked in the afternoon. Therefore, the time of day for testing could have impacted grip

strength performance. The post-nap dip could account for an increase in sleepiness,

which could have negatively impacted grip strength performance (Hayashi et al., 2002).

Additionally, this hypothesis may not have been supported possibly due to the increased

use of technology, such as typing on keyboards, which could contribute to the overuse of

individuals’ muscles and therefore decrease grip strength.

Hypothesis 3C. Hypothesis 3C stated that longer sleep duration will significantly

improve vertical jump height. The results of the ANOVA did not support this hypothesis.

The hypothesis not being supported for vertical jump height may be because sleep

duration does not have an effect on vertical jump height. These results are inconsistent

with a study conducted by Takeuchi, Davis, Plyley, Goode, and Shephard (1985) who

found that that sleep deprived males had significantly decreased vertical jump height. The

study by Takeuchi and colleagues (1985) had participants undergo 64 hours of sleep

deprivation. It is possible that short sleep duration (< six hours of sleep per night) does
125

not have the same effect as prolonged sleep deprivation. In addition, individuals may

have reported what time they lay down in bed instead of when they actually fall asleep.

This could mean that these individuals are not actually long sleepers and the results may

not be significant.

Hypothesis 3D. Hypothesis 3D stated that longer sleep duration will significantly

improve peak power. The results of the ANOVA did not support this hypothesis. The

hypothesis not being supported for peak power may because sleep duration does not

affect peak power. Another possibility is that the long sleepers may be getting more REM

sleep, which could contribute to the increase in peak power. In addition, people may only

need a minimum amount of sleep for their muscles to recuperate, which could mean that

individuals with short sleep duration would not have a reduction in peak power.

Furthermore, peak power was estimated based on vertical jump height, and they are

positively correlated with each other. Is it not surprising that peak power did not

significantly improve when there was not a significant improvement in vertical jump

height.

Hypothesis Four

Hypotheses 4A through 4D posited that compared to individuals with higher

levels of sleepiness, individuals with lower levels of sleepiness will have significantly

better flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak power. Results revealed

that sleepiness did not significantly impact flexibility (Hypothesis 4A), grip strength

(Hypothesis 4B), vertical jump height (Hypothesis 4C), or peak power (Hypothesis 4D).

Hypothesis 4A. Hypothesis 4A stated that higher levels of sleepiness will

significantly reduce flexibility. The results of the ANOVA did not support this
126

hypothesis. The hypothesis not being supported for flexibility may be due to several

reasons. Flexibility may not be affected by sleepiness. Another possible reason that this

hypothesis was not supported is that the Sit-and-Reach Test was not long in duration.

Particularly relevant to college students, Durmer and Dinges (2005) found that cognitive

performance effects of sleep deprivation revealed a "fatigue effect," which became worse

as time on the task increased. Furthermore, performance declined as the task duration

extended. This decline in performance can include physical performance. Possibly

assessing measures o f flexibility that are longer in duration than a sit-and reach test

would result in a significant impact of sleepiness on flexibility. Therefore, it is possible

that higher levels of sleepiness may adversely affect flexibility if assessed in an extended

measure.

Hypothesis 4B. Hypothesis 4B stated that higher levels of sleepiness will

significantly impair grip strength. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. The hypothesis not being supported for grip strength may be because grip

strength is not affected by sleepiness. It is also possible that due to the short time frame to

complete the grip strength task, people were able to overcome sleepiness. Participants

may not have been able to overcome sleepiness during a longer task, such as long­

distance running. Individuals participated in the dynamic warm-up twice within a two-

hour period, which may have countered the effects of sleepiness, as the warm-up

increased heart rate and physiological arousal. Research has shown that the active,

dynamic warm-up has demonstrated positive effects on performance. However,

participants in this study were not athletes; therefore, many of them are not likely to be

physically active and were not use to engaging in that type of physical activity. In
127

addition, many of the male participants were competitive with one another during both of

the active, dynamic warm-ups. This could have led to an increase in heart rate and

physiological arousal, which may have temporarily diminished the effects of sleepiness.

Hypothesis 4C. Hypothesis 4C stated that higher levels of sleepiness will

significantly reduce vertical jump height. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. The hypothesis not being supported for vertical jump height may be due to

several reasons. There may be no effect of sleepiness on vertical jump height. Another

possible reason that this hypothesis was not supported is that the Vertical Jump Test is

too short and simplistic of a task to be impacted by sleepiness. In addition, sleepy

individuals may have been able to turn on the motivation in order to perform for a short

period o f time.

Hypothesis 4D. Hypothesis 4D stated that higher levels of sleepiness will

significantly reduce peak power. The results of the ANOVA did not support this

hypothesis. The hypothesis may not be supported for peak power because there may not

be a significant effect of sleepiness on peak power. It is also possible that sleepiness

affects tasks which are more cognitive in nature. Because there is not much of a cognitive

aspect to peak power, sleepiness may not have an impact on peak power.

Implications

This study has important implications for athletes. The fact that there was a

significant improvement in grip strength due to a 10-minute nap/relaxation highlights the

importance of sleep. These results could have an impact on sports such as baseball,

basketball, football, tennis, and wrestling, in which grip strength plays an important role.
128

This suggests that athletes in these sports could potentially benefit from a 10-minute

nap/relaxation prior to competing.

Even though a 10-minute nap/relaxation period did not significantly improve

flexibility, it could still benefit athletes in several sports in which flexibility is important.

This could be an advantage to athletes in sports at the professional or collegiate levels

where even a small amount of difference could assist them in winning. At the

professional level, winning could affect millions of dollars. College scholarships may be

affected by whether or not the individual wins.

Sleep quality did improve flexibility and grip strength, even though it was not

significant. Long sleepers also had an improvement in grip strength, vertical jump height,

and peak power. In addition, average sleepers had an increase in flexibility and

individuals who were alert had better peak power. The implication of these results is that

getting a good night’s sleep could be an advantage to athletes in sports where any

increase in flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, or peak power could influence

performance outcome. This also could have implications in occupational tasks which

require grip strength, as well as activities of daily living, which may also have an impact

on possible work outcomes.

The current study had a higher average on the ESS and KSS compared to other

studies, indicating that this sample had a higher level of sleepiness than other samples.

Additionally, over 35% of this sample reported experiencing sleepiness outside the

normal range. Reduced sleep time has explained excessive sleepiness of several patient

and nonpatient groups. For example, a sample of sleep clinic patients was identified as

having excessive daytime sleepiness, which can be attributed to chronic insufficient sleep
129

(Roehrs, Zorick, Sicklesteel, Wittig, & Roth, 1983). These patients had objectively

documented excessive sleepiness, “normal” nocturnal sleep with unusually high sleep

efficiency (time asleep-time in bed), and report about two hours more sleep on each

weekend day than each weekday. Regularizing bedtime and increasing time in bed

produces a resolution of these symptoms and normalizes Multiple Sleep Latency Test

(MSLT) results (Manber, Bootzin, Acebo, & Carskadon, 1996). Therefore, the increased

sleepiness of healthy young adults can be attributed to insufficient nocturnal sleep. When

the sleepiest 25% of a sample of young adults was given extended time in bed (10 hours)

for as long as five to 14 consecutive nights, their sleepiness was reduced to a level

resembling the general population mean (Roehrs, Shore, Papineau, Rosenthal, & Roth

1996). Therefore, reducing sleepiness can be done by regulating bedtime and extending

time in bed. Lower grip strength was associated with increased sleepiness in the current

study. It is possible that by reducing sleepiness, athletes may experience an increase in

their grip strength.

In the current study, vertical jump height was positively related to the transition

from sleep to wakefulness. It is possible that those who have reduced ability to wake up

in the morning are sleepy and therefore have difficulty generating the power for vertical

jump height. This could mean that by reducing sleepiness, athletes may be able to see an

increase in their vertical jump height. Therefore, it may be important for athletes to

regulate their bedtime and increase their time in bed in order to reduce sleepiness.

Bonnet and Arand (2005) found that a 5-minute walk immediately preceding

Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) evaluations masked the impact of a 50% reduction

o f nocturnal sleep and continued for at least 90 minutes after the task. This indicated that
130

exercise before performing tasks provided a temporary reversal of some psychomotor

decline resulting from sleep loss. In the current study, individuals engaged in two

dynamic warm-ups (lasting 10-15 minutes each) in a two-hour time period prior to

participating in the physical performance tasks. Therefore, individuals’ performance may

not have shown a decrease because the psychomotor deficits were masked by

participating in the dynamic warm-ups. However, athletes who are competing in an event

that lasts longer than 90 minutes may not be able to mask the effects of sleep loss by

exercising beforehand. This could mean that if baseball players exercised before a game,

after 90 minutes, they may experience a decrease in grip strength, which could affect

their swing. The implication for baseball players is to get a good night’s sleep, so they do

not experience a reduction in grip strength. It is also possible that the results of the

current study could have been significant if individuals did not participate in the dynamic

warm-ups. It may also be that if the study tested participants several hours after the

dynamic warm-ups, then they may have had a decrease in grip strength because the

effects of sleep loss would not be masked by the warm-up.

Limitations

There are several limitations to the current study. One limitation is in regards to

the generalizability of the study. The current sample consisted of 98 undergraduate and

graduate students who ranged in age from 17 to 47. The sample is not representative of

the general population, due to the fact that the participants are all college students.

Therefore, it is not clear if the results would generalize to individuals with different levels

of education. In addition, it is unclear if the results would generalize to non-current

students. The participants attend a public university in the Southern United States.
131

Therefore, the generalizability of the results to students attending private colleges and

universities, as well as universities and colleges outside the Southern United States, is

questionable.

Furthermore, other limitations of the study include the small sample with unequal

sample size and no pre-test was conducted to determine if groups differed beforehand,

even though random assignment of participants was implemented. Regarding the unequal

sample size, SPSS autocorrects the formula to calculate the sum of squares in unequal

sample sizes in two-way ANOVA analyses, which was used for this study. The study was

originally designed to be a within and between subjects design; however, due to the lack

of participants, it resulted in a post-test only study. Pre-test measures would have been

ideal to determine if the groups were different at the onset of the study. However, due to

the difficulty in recruiting participants, the post-tests were only conducted. Therefore, it

is difficult to determine what, if any, effects of the nap/relaxation condition had an effect

on physical performance.

Another limitation of the study is that, on the SHQ, only 26.3% of the participants

slept <6 hours per night, which is the number of hours used to classify an individual as

having a short sleep duration. On the SHQ, only 10.1% of the participants slept less than

nine hours per night, which indicates an individual as having a long sleep duration. Most

o f the participants (61.6%) had average sleep duration (greater than six and less than nine

hours of sleep per night). The current study may not have included enough individuals in

each group to determine if there was a significant change in physical performance, which

could mean that there was not enough power to indicate a significant effect in the results.
132

An additional limitation of the current study is that all of the sleep-related data

was self-reported. Therefore, the results must be interpreted under the assumption that the

participants responded truthfully. Participants were told that their responses would be

kept confidential. Participants were instructed to detach their consent forms from the

questionnaire packet before turning it in, to ensure the confidentiality of their responses.

In addition, the questionnaires with the participant’s identification number did not include

any personal identifying information, which ensured that participant data was

anonymous. Despite these efforts, participants may not have responded truthfully.

Participants may have over-reported or under-reported their responses, because they

could have been inclined to respond in a socially desirable manner. Social desirability

was not controlled for in this study.

Another limitation is that participants may not have been motivated to take the

appropriate time to complete the questionnaires on sleep quality, sleep duration, and

sleepiness accurately. The participants may not have cared how they physically

performed on the Vertical Jump test, given that the current sample did not consist of

athletes and had significantly lower scores on physical performance measures than other

samples. Individuals’ level of motivation for participating in the study may have affected

the results as well. Due to participants being given extra credit for their participation;

their motive may have been to improve their grade instead of trying their best with the

physical performance measures, despite being encouraged to try their best on all

measures. Participants may not have tried hard enough to see an improvement in their

physical performance without the motivation to do their best.


133

An additional limitation is in regards to the time frame that the questionnaires

inquired about for this study. All of the sleep-related data was self-reported and the

questionnaires requested information over a specific period of time. Therefore, memory

bias could have impacted the results. Participants in this study may not have been

accurate or consistent in recalling or reporting their sleep quality, sleep length, or level of

sleepiness. The SQI asked respondents to assess their sleep quality over the past three

month and the ADSWS asked participants to assess their sleep quality over the past

week. It could be quite difficult for an individual to remember their sleep patterns over an

extended period of time.

Another limitation is that the current study was unable to use a force plate to

measure peak power. Therefore, peak power was estimated using a formula based on the

Vertical Jump Test. Future research should use an actual measure of peak power instead

of estimating peak power. However, the equations for estimated peak power do have a

high reliability.

An additional limitation is that the physical performance measures used in this

study do not require psychomotor reactivity and therefore may have not been impacted

by sleepiness. Blagrove and Akehurst (2001) found that adults reporting excessive

sleepiness also report negative effects on psychomotor reactivity. However, flexibility,

grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak power are not dependent on psychomotor

abilities. A tennis return, sprint start, and karate block are dependent on psychomotor

abilities and if those measures were used in this study, then they may have been

significantly affected by sleepiness.


134

Another limitation is that the physical performance measures used in this study do

not require visual-motor reaction time, critical thinking abilities, vigilance, attention to

task, or reaction time; therefore, they may have not been impacted by sleep quality. Taub

(1980) found that poor sleep quality adversely affects visual-motor reaction time, critical

thinking abilities, vigilance, attention to task, and reaction time, which are all important

in physical performance. However, flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, and

peak power are not dependent on psychomotor abilities. A tennis return, sprint start, and

karate block do require visual-motor reaction time, critical thinking abilities, vigilance,

attention to task, or reaction time and if they were the measures used in this study, then

they may have been significantly affected by sleep quality.

An additional limitation is that the physical performance measures used in this

study do not require much cognitive skill and therefore may have not been impacted by

sleep duration. Hicks and Gilliland (1981), as well as Taub and Berger (1973), found that

individuals who report sleeping six hours or less have a greater incidence o f cognitive

difficulties. This increase in cognitive difficulties may impact physical performance.

However, flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, and peak power are not

dependent on cognitive skill. Hitting a baseball or throwing a football are more dependent

on cognitive skill, and they may have been significantly affected by sleep duration if they

were used in this study. It is important to note that given the limitations, the results of

this study do make an important contribution to the literature.

Suggestions for Future Research

Future research should be expanded to include a sample more representative of

the general population, including individuals of different educational level,


135

socioeconomic status, geographic region, and age. The study should also be replicated

with a larger sample size and pre-test measures to be able to determine if the sample sizes

are different prior to the experimental condition. In addition, future research should be

expanded to include individuals who are not currently students. These suggestions may

assist in improving the generalizability of study. It is also important that the effectiveness

of the 10-minute nap/relaxation be examined with a sample of more individuals

experiencing sleep difficulties. This will help ensure that the intervention has a chance to

be effective because the sample would represent equal sample sizes among the groups.

It is highly recommended that future research measure sleep quality and sleep

length using objective measures, such as an Actigraph. By using physiological measures

of sleep quality, it would greatly reduce human error, as well as the chance of memory

bias impacting the results. Physiological measures may also significantly reduce

individuals inaccurately assessing their sleep quality over time. Another way to assess

sleep length more accurately is through the use o f a sleep diary. Future research should

also examine other forms o f physical performance measures, such as additional anaerobic

or aerobic activities. Waterhouse and colleagues (2007) found that naps produced an

improvement in sprint performance after sleep loss during the previous night. However,

the current study did not measure sprinting, which is another anaerobic activity. Future

research could examine aerobic activity, such as long-distance running.

Future directions also could include comparing individuals who experience

anxiety to those who do not experience anxiety. Individuals who experience anxiety may

experience arousal, maladaptive behaviors, negative thoughts, and difficulty

concentrating, which could impede their physical performance. Guided imagery is often
136

used in relaxation procedures with athletes in order to regulate their arousal before

competition, lessen maladaptive behaviors, and balance negative thoughts as well as to

enhance concentration and focus (Peluso et al., 2005; Vempati & Telles, 2002; Watanabe

et al., 2006). Therefore, individuals who experience anxiety may benefit from guided

imagery. Finally, it is unclear if a nap would have a beneficial effect on participants who

have been partially deprived of sleep compared to this without partial sleep deprivation.

Future research could examine physical performance between individuals who are

partially sleep deprived verses those who do not suffer from partial sleep deprivation.

With additional understanding o f the relationship between sleep and physical

performance, there are practical implications for athletes. Athletes in sports in which grip

strength plays an important role, such as baseball, basketball, football, tennis, and

wrestling, could potentially benefit from a 10-minute nap/relaxation prior to competing.

Getting a good night’s sleep also could be an advantage to athletes in sports where any

increase in flexibility, grip strength, vertical jump height, or peak power could influence

performance outcome.
APPENDIX A

HUMAN USE COMMITTEE

APPROVAL FORM

137
138

LOUISIANA TECH
U N I V E R S I T Y
MEMORANDUM
OFFICE OF UNIVERSITY RESEARCH

TO: Ms. Rebecca Hoffmann and Dr. Walt Buboltz

FROM: Barbara Talbot, University Research

SUBJECT: HUMAN USE COMMITTEE REVIEW

DATE: December 19,2011

In order to facilitate your project, an EXPEDITED REVIEW has been done for your proposed
study entitled:
‘T h e Relation of Sleep Length/Quality and Napping on
Physical Performance’’

HUC 888

The proposed study’s revised procedures were found to provide reasonable and adequate
safeguards against possible risks involving human subjects. The information to be collected may
be personal in nature or implication. Therefore, diligent care needs to be taken to protect die
privacy of the participants and to assure that die data are kept confidential. Informed consent is a
critical part of the research process. The subjects must be informed dial their participation it
voluntary. It is important that consent materials be presented in a language understandable to
every participant If you have participants in your study whose first language is not English, be
sure that informed consent materials are adequately explained or translated. Since your reviewed
project appears to do no damage to the participants, the Human Use Committee grants approval
of the involvement of human subjects as outlined.

Projects should be renewed annually. This approve! was finalised on December 19, 2011 and
this project will need to receive a continuation review bp the IRS if the project, including data
analysis, continues beyond December 19, 2012. Any discrepancies in procedure or changes that
have been made including approved changes should be noted in the review application. Projects
involving NIH funds require annual education training to be documented. For more information
regarding this, contact the Office of University Research.

You are requested to maintain written records of your procedures, data collected, and subjects
involved. These records will need to be available upon request during the conduct of the study
and retained by the university for three years after the conclusion of the study. If changes occur
in recruiting of subjects, informed consent process or in your research protocol, or if
unanticipated problems should arise it is the Researchers responsibility to notify the Office of
Research or IRB in writing. The project should be discontinued until modifications can be
reviewed and approved.

If you have any questions, please contact Dr. Mary Livingston at 257-4315.

A MEMBER OF THE UNIVERSITY OF LOUISIANA SYSTEM

P.O. BOX 3092 • RUSTON, LA 71272 • TELEPHONE (SIS) 157-S07S • FAX (SIS) 257-5079
A N EQ U A L O rrO R T U N m UNIVERSITY
APPENDIX B

INFORMED CONSENT FORM

139
140

HUMAN SUBJECTS CONSENT FORM


The following is a brief sum m ary of th e project in w hich you a re a sk ed to participate. P le ase read
th is inform ation before signing th e sta te m e n t below.______________________________________________

TITLE OF PROJECT: The Relation of Sleep Length/Quality and Napping on Physical Performance

PURPOSE OF STUDY/PROJECT: The purpose of this study is to examine the impact of sleep
quality/length and napping on physical performance.

PROCEDURE: Prior to participation, you must sign an informed consent. After consent forms are signed,
you will be asked to complete two questionnaires, participate in a dynamic warm-up, complete the hand grip
dynamometer test, sit-and reach test, and vertical jump and force plate test, take a nap or read a
newspaper, participate in a dynamic warm-up again, complete the hand grip dynamometer test, sit-and
reach test, and vertical jump and force plate test again, and complete a packet of surveys. You will be
making two trips from the classroom to the gymnasium, in one two-hour block of time. The maximum your
participation will take is two hours. Please answer the questions truthfully and to the best of your ability.
Surveys and informed consent forms will be collected separately. At the exit, you will be provided with
information regarding the availability of tutoring and counseling services and information regarding how to
obtain a summary of the results of the study. Data will be analyzed to determine relationships among the
variables.

INSTRUMENTS: The survey booklet includes general questions about your background and history, and
specific surveys about your sleep habits, physical activity, mood, and body image.

RISKS/ALTERNATIVE TREATMENTS: The participant u n d e rsta n d s th a t Louisiana Tech is n o t able to


offer financial c o m p en satio n n o r to a b so rb th e c o s ts of m edical treatm en t sh o u ld you be injured a s a
re su lt of participating in th is re se a rch . Participants might experience soreness, muscle/tendon strains or
soreness, and ligament sprains due to maximal jumps. In case of emergency, an individual certified in First
Aid and CPR will be on site during testing.

BENEFITS/COMPENSATION: Som e participants may be offered extra class credit for participation. If extra
credit is provided by your instructor, he/she must offer an alternative assignment for extra credit if you do not
choose to participate in this study.

I , _________________________________ , a tte s t with my sig n a tu re th a t I have read and u n d ersto o d the


following d escrip tio n o f th e study. "The Relation of Sleep Length/Quality a n d N apping on Physical
P erform ance," an d its p u rp o se s a n d m ethods. I u n d e rsta n d th a t my participation in th is re se a rc h is
strictly voluntary a n d mv participation o r refusal to participate in th is study will n o t affect mv
relationship with Louisiana Tech University. Further, I u n d e rsta n d th a t I m ay w ithdraw a t any tim e o r
refu se to a n sw e r any q u e stio n s w ithout penalty. Upon com pletion of th e study, I u n d e rsta n d th a t the
re su lts will be freely available to m e upon req uest. I u n d e rstan d th a t th e re su lts of my su rv ey will be
confidential, a c c e ssib le only to th e principal investigators, mvself. o r a legally ap p ointed
rep resen tativ e. I have n o t b een re q u ested to w aive nor d o I w aive any of my rig h ts related to
participating in th is study.

S ig n atu re of P articipant o r G uardian Date

CONTACT INFORMATION: The principal experim enters listed below may be c o n tac ted to an sw e r
q u e stio n s a b o u t th e research , s u b je c ts' rights, o r related m atters.
PROJECT DIRECTORS: R ebecca Hoffmann and Walt Buboltz, Ph.D.
EMAIL: rmr026@ latech.edu; buboltz@latech.edu
PHONE: (410) 581-3835; (318) 257-4039

M em bers of th e Hum an U se C om m ittee of Louisiana Tech University m ay a lso b e co n ta c te d if a


problem c a n n o t b e d isc u s s e d with th e experim enters:
Dr. Les Guice (318) 257-3056
Dr. Mary M. Livingston (318) 257-2292 or (318) 257-4315
APPENDIX C

DEMOGRAPHICS FORM

141
142

Participant #:
Demographics Form

Please complete the following information.

1. What is your gender? 2. What is your age?


1. Male
2. Female

3. What is your race/ethnic origin? (Circle 4. What are you classified as?
all that apply) 1. Freshman
1. African American/Black 2. Sophomore
2. Asian/Pacific Islander 3. Junior
3. Caucasian/White 4. Senior
4. Hispanic 5. Graduate student
5. Native American
6. Other, please specify:

5. What is your height (feet/inches)? 6. What is your weight (in pounds)?

7. Are you an NCAA athlete? 8. If yes, then what sport do you play?
1. Yes
2. No
APPENDIX D

SLEEP QUALITY INDEX

143
144

Sleep Quality Index (SQI)

I n s t r u c t i o n s : P le a se a n s w e r th e fo llo w in g q u e s tio n s to th e b e s t o f y o u r a b ility b y c irc lin g th e


re s p o n s e th a t b e s t fits y o u . I f u n s u re , p le a s e g iv e y o u r b e s t g u e s s ._____________________________

1. Time to fall asleep

< 10 minutes 11-30 minutes >30 minutes

2. Suffered from insomnia during the past three months

No < 3 days/week 3-7 days/week

3. Difficulties falling asleep during the past 3 months

No < 3 days/week 3-7 days/week

4. Disturbed night sleep during the past 3 months

No < 3 days/week 3-7 days/week

5. Nocturnal awakening during the past 3 months

No < 3 days/week 3-7 days/week

6. Tiredness in the morning during the past 3 months

Very or Mostly Alert Don’t Know Very or Mostly Tired

7. Wake up too early in the morning during the past 3 months

No < 3 days/week 3-7 days/week

8. Use of sleeping medication during the past 3 months

No Occasionally At least once per week


APPENDIX E

ADULT SLEEP-WAKE SCALE

145
146

Adult Sleep-Wake Scale (ASWS)

D irec tio n s: U sing the choices below , circle how often the follow ing things have happened durine the past
week.
N ev e r - has not happened

O n ce in a w h ile - happened 20% o f the tim e

S o m etim es - happened 40% o f the tim e

Q u ite O fte n - happened 60% o f the tim e

F re q u e n tly , if n o t alw ay s - happened 80% o f the tim e

A lw ay s - happened 100% o f the tim e

Q uestions 1 - 5 are only about you Going to B ed at bedtim e.

W hen it is tim e to go to b e d ...

1 .) ... I w ant to stay up and do other things (fo r exam ple: read, work, o r w atch TV ).

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f n ot alw ays A lw ays

In gen eral...

2.) ...I have to m ake m y se lf go to bed.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

3 . ) . . . It is very hard for m e to go to bed on tim e.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

4 . ) . . . I “put o f f ’ o r delay going to bed.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

5.) How long do you usually “put o f f ’ or delay going to bed?

(a) < 15 min (b) 15-30 min (c) 30-45 min (d) 45-60 m in (e) 60-90 min (f) > 90 min

Rem em ber: T hink about the past week.

Q uestions 6 - 10 are only about you Falling Asleep after “ lights out.”

W hen I’m in bed and it is tim e to fall asleep ...

6 . ) . . . I am not sleepy.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

7 . ) . .. I am unable to settle down.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays
147

In general...

8.) ...I try to m ake m y self go to sleep.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

9 . ) . .. I fall asleep quickly.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

10.) H ow long does it usually take you to fall asleep after “lights out” ?

(a) < 1 5 m in (b) 15-30 m in (c) 30-45 m in (d) 45-60 m in (e) 60-90 m in (f) >90 min

Q uestions 11 - 15 are only about how you Sleep during the night (som eone else could have told you
these things).

A fter 1 fall asleep, during the n ig h t...

1 1 .)... I toss and turn in bed.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

12.) ...1 am very restless.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

13.) ...I aw aken m ore than once.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

In gen eral...

14.) ...I sleep w ithout arousals o r aw akenings.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

15.) How often do you usually w ake up during the night?

(a) N ever (b) O nce (c) T w ice (d) 3 tim es (e) 4 tim es (f) M ore than 4 tim es

Rem em ber: T hink about the past week.

Q uestions 1 6 - 2 0 are only about you Going back to sleep after w aking up during the night.

A fter w aking up during the n ig h t...

1 6 .)... 1 have a hard tim e going back to sleep.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

17.) ... I drift o ff back to sleep

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays
148

1 8 .)... I am calm and relaxed.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

19.) ...I roll over and go right back to sleep.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

20.) H ow long does it usually take you to go back to sleep after w aking during the night?

(a) < 5 min (b) 5-10 min (c) 10-15 min (d) 15-20 m in (e) 20-30 m in (f) > 30 min

Q uestions 21-25 are only about you Waking Up in the morning.

In the m orning, I w ake u p ...

2 1 .) .. .and feel ready to get up for the day.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

22.) ...re ste d and alert.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

23.) ...a n d ju st ca n ’t get going.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, i f not alw ays A lw ays

In g en eral...

24.) ...I am slow -to-start in the morning.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays

25.) ...I find it difficult to get out o f bed in the morning.

N ever O nce in a w hile Som etim es Q uite O ften Frequently, if not alw ays A lw ays
APPENDIX F

SLEEP HABITS QUESTIONNAIRE

149
150

Sleep Habits Questionnaire (SHQ)

Instructions: Write in your answers to the following questions.

1. On the average, what time during the week do you go to bed?_________________

2. On the average, what time during the week do you wake u p ?_________________

3. On the average, what time during the weekend do you go to bed?_____________

4. On the average, what time during the weekend do you wake up?______________

5. On the average, how many hours of sleep do you get during the week?_________

6. On the average, how many hours of sleep do you get on the weekend?_________

7. Ideally I would like to g et hours of sleep during the week each night.

8. Ideally I would like to g et hours of sleep on the weekend each night.

9. About how many minutes does it take you to fall asleep after lying down in bed?
APPENDIX G

EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

151
152

Epworth Sleepiness Scale

How likely are you to doze off or fall asleep in the following situations, in contrast to
feeling just tired? This refers to your usual way of life in recent times. Even if you
haven’t done some o f these things recently try to work out how they would have affected
you.

Use the following scale to choose the most appropriate number for each situation:

0 = would never doze

1 = slight chance of dozing

2 = moderate chance of dozing

3 = high chance of dozing

It is important that you answer each question as best you can.

Situation Chance of Dozing (0-3)

Sitting and reading 0 2 3

Watching TV 0 2 3

Sitting, inactive in a public place 0 2 3


(e.g. a theatre or a meeting)

As a passenger in a car for an hour without a break

Lying down to rest in the afternoon when circumstances


Permit 0

Sitting and talking to someone 0 2 3

Sitting quietly after a lunch without alcohol 0 2 3

In a car, while stopped for a few minutes in the traffic 0 2 3

Copyright © MW Johns 1990-1997. Used under license.


APPENDIX H

KAROLINSKA SLEEPINESS SCALE

153
154

Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS)

Directions: Please rate how sleepy you currently feel on the following scale by circling

the number.

(1) Extremely alert

(2)

(3) Alert

(4)

(5) Neither alert nor sleepy

(6)

(7) Sleep - but no difficulty remaining awake

(8)

(9) Extremely sleepy - fighting sleep


APPENDIX I

SCRIPT FOR RECRUITING PARTICIPANTS

155
156

Script for R ecruiting Participants

Hi, my name is Becky Hoffmann. I am a fourth year doctoral student here at

Tech. I am currently conducting research for my dissertation. Part of that includes

inviting students in Psychology and Kinesiology courses to participate in the research and

to offer you an opportunity to earn extra credit and contribute to the field of psychology.

I will be passing around a list of dates and times that you may sign-up to

participate. On the left side, please write down your name and telephone number next to

the date and time you wish to participate. Then, tear off the information on the right side

which lists the date and time you signed up for, as well as the location you will report to.

Please wear comfortable clothing that you can stretch in. I am requesting your phone

number, because you will receive a phone call the day before reminding you about the

time you signed up for and to wear comfortable clothing. If you are unavailable during

the dates and times listed and would still like to participate in the research, you can email

me to set up an alternative time. My email address is: rmr026@latech.edu. Your

participation in this study is voluntary. All information provided in this study will remain

anonymous and confidential. The amount of extra credit you receive will be determined

by your professor. What questions do you have? Thank you very much for signing up to

participate in this study.


APPENDIX J

ACTIVE DYNAMIC WARM-UP PROCEDURE

157
158

Active Dynamic Warm-up (Pre-Practice/Game)

1. Complete Jumping Jack Series


a. Jumping Jacks 2 x 10
b. Split Jack Forward 2 x 10
c. Highland Fling 2 x 10
d. Long Striders 2 x 10
2. Neck Clock 1 x 5 each
3. Shoulder Roll forward and backward 1 x 5 each
4. Long Arm Swings 1 x 5 each
5. Arm Circle Giant forward and backwards 1 x 10 each
6. Arm Hugs 1 x 10
7. Trunk Twists 1 x 5 each
8. Prisoner Squat Arms Forward 1x5
9. Heel to Toe Raise 1 x 5 each
10. Lunge Forward 1 x 5 each
11. Lunge Backwards 1 x 5 each
12. Lateral Lunge 1 x 5 each
13. Inverted Toe Touch 1 x 10 yd
14. Straight Leg March 1 x 10 yd
15. Quadricep Stretch Walking 1 x 10 yd
16. Knee Hug Walking 1 x 10 yd
17. Walking Lunge Same Elbow Same Leg 1 x 10 yd
18. Walking Lunge with Twist 1 x 10 yd
19. Prone Scorpion 1 x 5 each
20. Acceleration A Skip 1 x 10 yd
21. Butt Kickers 1 x 10 yd
22. Slow Backpedal 1 x 20 yd
23. Fast Backpedal 1 x 20 yd
24. Carioca 2 x 20 yd
25. Power Skip 1 x 20 yd
REFERENCES

Adam, K., & Oswald, I. (1977). Sleep is for tissue restoration. Journal o f the Royal

College o f Physicians o f London, 11, 376-388.

Aikens, J. E., & Mendelson, W. B. (1999). A matched comparison of MMPI responses in

patients with primary snoring or obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 22(3), 355-359.

Akerstedt, T. (1988). Sleepiness as a consequence of shift work. Sleep, 11, 17-34.

Akerstedt, T., & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active

individual. International Journal o f Neuroscience, 52(1-2), 29-37.

Akerstedt, T., Kecklund, G., Alffedsson, L., & Selen, J. (2007). Predicting long-term

sickness absence from sleep and fatigue. Journal o f Sleep Research, 16, 341-345.

Alam, M. N., Kumar, S., Rai, S., Methippara, M., Szymusiak, R., & McGinty, D. (2009).

Role ofadenosine A (l) receptor in the perifomical-lateral hypothalamic area in

sleep-wake regulation in rats. Brain Research, 1304, 96-104.

Alapin, I., Fichten, C. S., Libman, E., Creti, L., Bailes, S., & Wright, J. (2000). How is

good and poor sleep in older adults and college students related to daytime

sleepiness, fatigue, and ability to concentrate? Journal o f Psychosomatic

Research, 49, 381-390.

American College o f Sports Medicine. (2005). ACSM's Guidelines fo r Exercise Testing

and Prescription. Lea & Febiger / Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia.

159
American College o f Sports Medicine. (2006). ACSM's Guidelines fo r Exercise Testing

and Prescription. Lea & Febiger / Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual o f mental

disorders, 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Angus, R. G., Heslegrave, R. J., & Myles, W. S. (1985). Effects of prolonged sleep

deprivation, with and without chronic physical exercise, on mood and

performance. Psychophysiology, 22(3), 276-282.

Antrobus, J. S. (1986). Dreaming: Cortical activation and perceptual thresholds. Journal

o f Mind and Behavior, 7, 193-212.

Appels, A., & Mulder, P. (1984). Imminent myocardial infarction: A psychological study.

Journal o f Human Stress, 10, 129-134.

Aschoff, J. (1979). Circadian rhythms: Influences of internal and external factors on the

period measured in constant conditions. Zeitschrift fiir Tierpsychologie, 49, 225-

249.

Asplund, R. (2000). Sleep, health, and visual impairment in the elderly. Archives of

Gerontology and Geriatrics, 30(1), 7-15.

Ayas, N. T., White, D. P., Manson, J. E., Stampfer, M. J., Speizer, F. E., Malhotra, A., &

Hu, F. B. (2003). A prospective study of sleep duration and coronary heart disease

in women. Archives o f Internal Medicine, 163(2), 205-209.

Berger, R. J., & Phillips, N. H. (1995). Energy conservation and sleep. Behavioral Brain

Research, 69, 65-73.


Bettencourt, B., Charlton, K., Eubanks, J., Kemahan, C., & Fuller, B. (1999).

Development of collective self-esteem among students: Predicting adjustment to

college. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 21(3), 213-222.

Bixler, E. O., Kales, A., Soldatos, C. R., Kales, J. D., & Healey, S. (1979). Prevalence of

sleep disorders in the Los Angeles Metropolitan area. American Journal o f

Psychiatry, 1 3 6 ,1257-1262.

Blagrove, M., & Akehurst, L. (2000). Effects of sleep loss on confidence-accuracy

relationships for reasoning and eyewitness memory. Journal o f Experimental

Psychology Applied, 6(1), 59-73.

Blagrove, M., & Akehurst, L. (2001). Personality and the modulation of effects of sleep

loss on mood and cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(5), 819-

828.

Bloch, V., Hennevin, E., & Leconte, P. (1977). Interaction between post-trial reticular

stimulation and subsequent paradoxical sleep in memory consolidation processes.

In: Neurobiology o f sleep and memory (Drucker-Colin RR, McGaugh JL, eds), pp

255-272. New York: Academic.

Boivin, D. B., Duffy, J. F., Kronauer, R. E., & Czeisler, C. A. (1994). Sensitivity of the

human circadian pacemaker to moderately bright light. Journal o f Biological

Rhythms, 9(3-4), 315-331.

Boland, L. L., Shahar, E., Iber, C., Knopman, D. S., Kuo, T. F., & Nieto, F. J. (2002).

Measures o f cognitive function in person with varying degrees of sleep-disordered

breathing: The Sleep Heart Health Study. Journal o f Sleep Research, 11, 265-272.
162

Bonnet, M. H. (1985). Effect o f sleep disruption on sleep, performance, and mood. Sleep,

8( 1), 11-19.

Bonnet, M. H. (2000). Sleep deprivation. In Principles and Practice o f Sleep Medicine

(3rd ed.) (eds M. H. Kryger, T. Roth & W. C. Dement), pp. 53-71. Philadelphia,

PA: Saunders.

Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1995). 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and

matched normal sleepers. Sleep, 18(7), 581-588.

Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (1998). Sleepiness as measured by the MSLT varies as a

function of preceding activity. Sleep, 2 1 ,477-483.

Bootzin, R. R., & Perlis, M. L. (1992). Nonpharmacologic treatments of insomnia. The

Journal o f Clinical Psychiatry, 53, 37-41.

Bowersox, S., Kaitlin, K., & Dement, W. (1985). EEG spindle activity as a function of

age: Relationship to sleep continuity. Brain Research, 334(2), 303-308.

Bravo, G., Gauthier, P., Roy, P., Tessier, D., Gaulin, P., Dubois, M., & Peloquin, L.

(1994). The functional fitness assessment battery: Reliability and validity for

elderly women. Journal o f Aging and Physical Activity, 2, 67-79.

Brooks, A., & Lack, L. (2006). A brief afternoon nap following nocturnal sleep

restriction: Which nap duration is most recuperative? Sleep, 29(6), 831-840.

Brown, F. C., Buboltz, W. C., & Soper, B. (2002). Relationship of sleep hygiene

awareness, sleep hygiene practices, and sleep quality in university students.

Behavioral Medicine, 28, 33-38.

Brown, F., Soper, B., & Buboltz, W. (2001). Prevalence of delayed sleep phase syndrome

in university students. College Student Journal, 35(3), 472-476.


163

Buboltz, W. C., Brown, F., & Soper, B. (2001). Sleep habits and patterns of college

students: A preliminary study. Journal o f American College Health, 50(3), 131-

135.

Buboltz, W. C., Jr., Loveland, J., Jenkins, S. M., Brown, F., Soper, B., & Hodges, J.

(2006). College student sleep: Relationship to health and academic performance.

In F. Columbus (Ed.), College Students: Stress, Mental Health and Coping

Strategies (pp. 1-39). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

Buboltz, W. C., Soper, B., Brown, F., & Jenkins, S. (2002). Treatment approaches for

sleep difficulties in college students. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 15(3),

229-237.

Buela-Casal, G., Miro, E., Ianez, M. A., & Catena, A. (2007). Relation between habitual

sleep duration and depressed mood state: Somatic versus cognitive symptoms.

International Journal o f Clinical and Health Psychology, 7(3), 615-631.

Buysse, D., Reynolds III, C., Monk, T., Berman, S., & Kupfer, D. (1989). The Pittsburgh

Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research.

Journal o f Psychiatric Research, 28(2), 193-213.

Cajochen, C., Kraeuchi, K., & Wirz-Justice, A., (1997). The acute soporific action of

daytime melatonin administration: Effects on the EEG during wakefulness and

subjective alertness. Journal o f Biological Rhythms, 12(6), 636-643.

Caldwell, J. A. (2002). A survey of aircrew fatigue in a sample of U.S. army aviation

personnel. Aviation, Space, & Environmental Medicine, 73,473-480.

Caldwell, J. P. (2003). Sleep: The Complete Guide to Sleep Disorders and a Better

N ight’s Sleep. Buffalo, NY: Firefly Books Ltd.


164

Campbell, S. S., & Murphy, P. J. (1998). Extraocular circadian phototransduction in

humans. Science, 279, 396 -399.

Campbell, S., & Zulley, J. (1989). Evidence for circadian influence on human slow wave

sleep during daytime sleep episodes. Psychophysiology, 26(5): 580-585.

Campos-Morales, R. M., Valencia-Flores, M., Castano-Meneses, A., Castaneda-

Figueiras,S., & Martinez-Guerrero, J. (2005). Sleepiness, performance, and mood

state in a group of Mexican undergraduate students. Biological Rhythm Research,

36, 9-13.

Canavan, P. K., & Vescovi, J. D. (2004). Evaluation of power prediction equations: Peak

vertical jumping power in women. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,

36(9), 1589-1593.

Cappaert, T. (1999). Review: Time of day effect on athletic performance: An update.

Journal o f Strength and Conditioning Research, 13(4), 412-421.

Carlson, N. R. (1999). Foundations o f Physiological Psychology (4th Ed.). Needham

Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Carney, C. E., Edinger, J. D., Meyer, B., Lindman, L., & Istre, T. (2006). Daily activities

and sleep quality in college students. Chronobiology International, 22(3), 623-

637.

Carpenter, S. (2001). Sleep deprivation may be undermining teen health. Monitor on

Psychology, 32, 9.

Carskadon, M., & Dement, W. (1981). Cumulative effects of sleep restriction on daytime

sleepiness. Psychophysiology, 18, 107-113.


165

Carskadon, M., & Dement, W. (2000). Normal human sleep. In M. Kryger, T. Roth, &

W. Dement (Eds.), Principles and practice o f sleep medicine (3rd ed., pp. 15-25).

Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.

Chang, E., & Rand, K. (2000). Perfectionism as a predictor of subsequent adjustment:

Evidence for a specific diathesis-stress mechanism among college students.

Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 47, 129-137.

Chasens, E., Sereika, S., Weaver, T., & Umlauf, M. (2007). Daytime sleepiness, exercise,

and physical function in older adults. Journal o f Sleep Research, 16( 1), 60-65.

Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical

features of mindfulness meditations. Psychological Medicine, 4 0 ,1239-1252.

Cohen, R., & Albers, H. (1991). Disruption of human circadian and cognitive regulation

following a discrete hypothalamic lesion: A case study. Neurology, 48, 1533-

1539.

Comings, D. E., Gade, R., MacMurray, J. P., Muhleman, D., & Peters, W. R. (1996).

Genetic variants of the human obesity (OB) gene: Association with body mass

index n young women, psychiatric symptoms, and interaction with the dopamine

D2 receptor (DRDz) gene. Molecular Psychiatry, 1, 325-335.

Coren, S. (1994). The prevalence of self-reported sleep disturbances in young adults.

International Journal o f Neuroscience, 79, 67-73.

Cox, R. H. (2012). Sport Psychology: Concepts and Applications. New York: McGraw-

Hill.

Curcio, G., Ferrara, M., & De Gennaro, L. (2005). Sleep loss, learning capacity and

academic performance. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10, 323-337.


166

Dam, T. T., Ewing, S., Aneoli-Israel, S., Ensrud, K., Redline, S., & Stone, K. (2008).

Association between sleep and physical function in older men: The osteoporotic

fractures in men sleep study. Journal o f the American Geriatrics Society, 59(9),

1665-1673.

De Koninck, J., Lorrain, D., Christ, G., Proulx, G., & Coulombe, D. (1989). Intensive

language learning and increases in rapid eye movement sleep: Evidence of a

performance factor. International Journal o f Psychophysiology, 5(1), 43-47.

Dimitrov, S., Lange, T., Tieken, S., Fehm, H. L., & Bom, J. (2004). Sleep associated

regulation of T helper 1/T helper 2 cytokine balance in humans. Brain, Behavior,

and Immunity, 18(4), 341-348.

Dinges, D. F., Whitehouse, W. G., Ome, E. C., & Ome, M. T. (1988). The benefits of a

nap during prolonged work and wakefulness. Work & Stress, 2, 139-153.

Doi, Y., Minowa, M., & Tango, T. (2003). Impact and correlates of poor sleep quality in

Japanese white-collar employees. Sleep, 26, 467-71.

Domino, G., & Fogl, A. (1980). Sleep patterns in college students. Psychology: A

Quarterly Journal o f Human Behavior, 17(4), 7-14.

Durmer, J., & Dinges, D. (2005). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation.

Seminars in Neurology, 25( 1), 117-129.

Edell-Gustafsson, U. M. (2002). Sleep quality and responses to insufficient sleep in

women on different work shifts. Journal o f Clinical Nursing, 11(2), 280-287.

Elashoff, J. D., Van Deventer, G., Reedy, T. J., Ippoliti, A., Samloff, I. M., Kurata, J.,

Billings, M., & Isenberg, M. (1983). Long-term follow-up of duodenal ulcer

patients. Journal o f Clinical Gastroenterology, 5(6), 509-515.


167

Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine.

Science, 196, 129-136.

Engle-Friedman, M., Riela, S., Golan, R., Ventuneac, A., Davis, C., Jefferson, A., &

Major, D. (2003). The effect o f sleep loss on next day effort. Journal o f Sleep

Research, 12, 113-124.

Falleti, M. G., Maruff, P., Collie, A., Darby, D. G., & McStephen, M. (2003). Qualitative

similarities in cognitive impairment associated with 24 h o f sustained wakefulness

and a blood alcohol concentration of 0.05%. Journal o f Sleep Research, 12(4),

265-274.

Finn, L., Young, T., Palta, M., & Fryback, D. G. (1998). Sleep-disordered breathing and

self-reported general health status in the Wisconsin Sleep Cohort study. Sleep, 21,

587-592.

Foley, D. J., Ancoli-Isreal, S., Britz, P., & Walsh, J. K. (2004). Sleep disturbances and

chronicdisease in older adults: Results of the 2003 National Sleep Foundation

Sleep in America Survey. Journal o f Psychosomatic Research, 56, 497-502.

Foley, D. J., Monjan, A. A., & Brown, S. L. (1995). Sleep complaints among elderly

persons: An epidemiological study of three communities. Sleep, 18, 425-432.

Forbes, E., Bertocci, M., Gregory, A., Ryan, N., Axelson, D., Birmaher, B., & Dahl, R.

(2008). Objective sleep in pediatric anxiety disorders and major depressive

disorder. Journal o f the American Academy o f Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

47(2), 148-155.
Fortunato, V., LeBourgeois, M. K., & Harsh, J. (2008). Development of a five

dimensional measure of sleep quality. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 65,488-514.

Friedman, L., Bliwise, D. L., Yesavage, J. A., & Salom, S. R. (1991). A preliminary

study comparing sleep restriction and relaxation treatments for insomnia in older

adults. Journal o f Gerontology, 46, 1-8.

Friedman, M., & Rosenman, R. H. (1974). Type A Behavior and Your Heart. New York:

Knopf.

Gangwisch, J. E., Malaspina, D., Boden-Albala, B., & Heymsfield, S. B. (2005).

Inadequate sleep as a risk factor for obesity: Analyses of the NHANES I. Sleep,

28, 1289-1296.

Gau, S. F. (2000). Neuroticism and sleep-related problems in adolescence. Sleep, 23,

495-502.

Giacobbi, P., Jr., Foore, B., & Weinberg, R.S. (2004). Broken clubs and expletives: The

courses of stress and coping responses of skilled and moderately skilled golfers.

Journal o f Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 166-182.

Gilbert, R.J. (1986). Caffeine: The most popular stimulant. New York: Chelsea House

Publishers.

Gillberg, M., Kecklund, G., Axelsson, J., & Akerstedt, T. (1996). The effects of a short

daytime nap after restricted night sleep. Sleep, 19, 570-575.

Gillette, M., & McArthur, A. (1996). Circadian actions of melatonin at the

suprachiasmatic nucleus. Behavioural Brain Research, 75(1-2), 135-139.


Goh, V., Tong, T., Lim, C., Low, E. & Lee, L. (2001). Effects of one night o f sleep

deprivation on hormone profiles and performance efficiency. Military Medicine,

166(5), 427-436.

Goldin, P., Ramel, W., & Gross, J., (2009). Mindfulness meditation training and self-

referential processing in social anxiety disorder: Behavioral and neural effects.

Journal o f Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16, 242-257.

Goldman, S. E., Stone, K. L., Ancoli-Isreal, S., Blackwell, T., Ewing, S. K., Boudreau,

R., Cauley, J. A., Hall, M., Matthews, K. A., & Newman, A. B. (2007). Poor sleep

is associated with poorer physical performance and greater functional limitations

in older women. Sleep, 30(10), 1317-1324.

Gomes, A. A., Tavares, J., & de Azevedo, M. H. (2011). Sleep and academic

performance in undergraduates: A multi-measure, multi-predictor approach.

Chronobiology International, 28(9), 786-801.

Gottlieb, D. J., Punjabi, N. M., Newman, A. B., Resnick, H. E., Redline, S., Baldwin, C.

M., Nieto, F. J. (2005). Association of sleep time with diabetes mellitus and

impaired glucose tolerance. Archives o f Internal Medicine, 765(8), 863-867.

Gray, E. K., & Watson, D. (2002). General and specific traits of personality and their

relation to sleep and academic performance. Journal o f Personality, 70, 177-206.

Greenberg, R., & Pearlman, C. (1974). Cutting the REM nerve: An approach to the

adaptive role of REM sleep. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 17(4), 513-

521.

Greenberg, R., Pearlman, C., Schwartz, W. R., & Grossman, H. Y. (1983). Memory,

emotion, and REM sleep. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 92(3), 378-381.


170

Grosvenor, A., & Lack, L. C. (1984). The effect of sleep before or after learning on

memory. Sleep, 7, 155-167.

Harrison, Y., & Home, J. A. (1999). One night of sleep loss impairs innovative thinking

and flexible decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 78, 128-145.

Harrison, Y., & Home, J. A. (2000). Sleep loss and temporal memory. The Quarterly

Journal o f Experimental Psychology, 55/1(1), 271-279.

Hartmann, E. L. (1973). The Functions o f Sleep. London: Yale University Press.

Hasler, G., Buysse, D. J., Klaghofer, R., Gamma, A., Ajdacic, V., Eich, D., Rossler, W.,

& Angst, J. (2004). The association between short sleep duration and obesity in

young adults: A 13-year prospective study. Sleep, 27(4), 661-666.

Hauri, P. J. (1991). Case Studies in Insomnia. New York, NY: Plenum.

Hawkins, J., & Shaw, P. (1992). Self-reported sleep quality in college students: A

repeated measures approach. Sleep, 15, 545-549.

Hayashi, M., Morikawa, T., & Hori, T. (2002). Circasemidian 12 h cycle of slow wave

sleep under constant darkness. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113(9), 1505-1516.

Hayashi, M., Motoyoshi, N., & Hori, T. (2005). Recuperative power of a short daytime

nap with or without stage 2 sleep. Sleep, 28(1), 829-836.

Hess, W. R. (1965). Sleep as a phenomenon of the integral organism. In Sleep

Mechanisms, K. Akert, C. Bally, and J. P. Schade (Eds.), Elsevier, Amsterdam,

3-8.
171

Hicks, R. A., & Gilliland, M. (1981). Habitual sleep duration and the premature decline

o f aging-sensitive abilities in young adults. Bulletin o f the Psychonomic Society,

18(6), 305-308.

Hicks, R., Mistry, R., Lucero, K., Lee, L., & Pelligrini, R. (1989). The sleep duration and

sleep satisfaction of college students: Striking changes over the last decade (1978-

1988). Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 806.

Hicks, R. A., McTighe, S., & Juarez, M. (1986). Sleep duration and eating behaviors in

college students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 25-26.

Hicks, R., & Pellegrini, R. (1991). The changing sleep habits of college students.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1106.

Hobson, J. A. (1995). Sleep. New York: Scientific American Library.

Hoffman, J. A., & Weiss, B. (1987). Family dynamics and presenting problems in college

students. Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 34, 157-163.

Horne, J.A. (1988). Why we sleep - the functions o f sleep in humans and other mammals.

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Home, J. A., & Reyner, L. A. (1996). Counteracting driver sleepiness: Effects of

napping, caffeine, and placebo. Psychophysiology, 33, 306-309.

Hryshko-Mullen A. S., Broeckl, L. S., Haddock, C. K., & Peterson, A. L. (2000).

Behavioral treatment of insomnia: The Wilford Hall insomnia program.

Military Medicine. 165, 200-207.

Innes, E. (1999). Handgrip strength testing: A review of the literature. Australian

Occupational Therapy Journal, 46(3), 120-140.


172

Inoue, M., Koyangi, T., Nakahara, H., Hara, K., Hori, E., & Nakano, H. (1986).

Functional development of human eye movement in utero assessed quantitatively

with real-time ultrasound. American Journal o f Obstetrics and Gynecology, 155,

170-174.

Irwin, M., Mascovich, A., Gillin, J. C., Willoughby, R., Pike, J., & Smith, T. L. (1994).

Partial sleep deprivation reduces natural killer cell activity in humans.

Psychosomatic Medicine, 56(6), 493-498.

Irwin, M., McClintick, J., Costlow, C., Fortner, M., White, J., & Gillin, J. C. (1996).

Partial night sleep deprivation reduces natural killer and cellular immune

responses in humans. The Journal o f the Federation o f American Societies fo r

Experimental Biology, 10, 643-653.

Jackson, A. W. & Baker, A. A. (1986). The relationship of the sit and reach test to

criterion measures of hamstring and back flexibility in young females. Research

Quarterly fo r Exercise and Sport, 57(3), 183-186.

Jackson, A., & Langford, N. J. (1989). The criterion-related validity of the sit and reach

test: Replication and extension o f previous findings. Research Quarterly for

Exercise and Sport, 60(4), 384-387.

Jean-Louis, G., von Gizycki, H., Zizi, F., & Nunez, J. (1998). Mood states and sleepiness

in college students: Influences of age, sex, habitual sleep, and substance use.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87(2), 507-512.

Jenkins, S. M. (2005). Sleep habits and patterns o f college students and their relationship

to selected personality characteristics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation:

Louisiana Tech University, Ruston, LA.


173

Johns, M. W. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth

Sleepiness Scale. Sleep, 14(6), 50-55.

Johns, M. W. (1992). Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale.

Sleep, 15, 376-381.

Kahn-Greene, E. Killgore, D., Kamimori, G., Balkin, T., & Killgore, W. (2007). The

effects of sleep deprivation on symptoms of psychopathology in healthy adults.

Sleep Medicine, 8(3), 215-221.

Kaida, K., Takahashi, M., Akerstedt, T., Nakata, A., Otsuka, Y., Haratani, T., &

Fukasawa, K. (2006). Validation of the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale against

performance and EEG variables. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117, 1574-1581.

Karacan, I., Thomby, J., Anch, M., Holzler, C., Warheit, G., Schivab, J., & Williams, R.

(1976). Prevalence o f sleep disturbances in a primarily urban Florida county.

Social Science and Medicine, 10, 239-244.

Karacan, I., Thomby, J. I., & Williams, R. L. (1983). Sleep disturbances: a community

survey. In: C. Guilleminault and E. Lugaresi, (Eds) Sleep/Wake Disorders:

Natural History, Epidemiology, and Long-Term Evolution. Raven Press, New

York: 37-60.

Kami, A., Tanne, D., Rubenstein, B. S., Askenasy, J. J. M., & Sagi, D. (1994).

Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement o f a perceptual skill.

Science, 265, 679-682.

Kelly, W. E., Kelly, K. E., & Clanton, R. C. (2001). The relationship between sleep

length and grade-point average among college students. College Student Journal,

35, 84-87.
Khasky, A. D., & Smith, J. C. (1999). Stress, relaxation states and creativity. Perceptual

and Motor Skills, 88 (2), 409-416.

Kleeman, G. L., & Richardson, R. C. (1985). Student characteristics and perceptions of

university effectiveness. Review o f Higher Education, 9(1), 5-20.

Kraft, A. M. (1984). Changes in thought content following sleep deprivation in

depression. Comprehensive Psychology, 25, 283-289.

Krantz, D. S., Arbrier, J. M., Davia, J. E., & Parker, J. S. (1988). Type A behavior and

coronary bypass surgery: Intraoperative blood pressure and perioperative

complications. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 273-254.

Kripke, D., Garfinkel, L., Wingard, D., Klauber, M., & Marler, M. (2002). Mortality

associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Archives o f General Psychiatry, 59,

131-136.

Kutner, N. G. S. K., Ory, M. G., Baker, D. I., & FICSIT Group. (1994). Older adults

perceptions of their health and functioning in relation to sleep disturbance, falling,

and urinary incontinence. Journal o f the American Geriatrics Society, 42, 757-

762.

Lack, L. C. (1986). Delayed sleep and sleep loss in university students. Journal o f the

American College Health, 35(5), 105-110.

Lacks, P. (1987). Behavioral Treatment fo r Persistent Insomnia. New York: Pergamon

Books.

Lacks, P., & Morin, C. M. (1992). Recent advances in the assessment and treatment of

insomnia. Journal o f Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 586-594.


Law, B., Driediger, M., Hall, C., & Forwell, L. (2006). Imagery use, perceived pain, limb

functioning and satisfaction in athletic injury rehabilitation. New Zealand Journal

o f Physiotherapy, 34( 1), 10-16.

Leard, J. S., Cirillo, M. A., Katsnelson, E., Kimiatek, D. A., Miller, T. W., Trebincevic,

K., & Garbalosa, J. C. (2007). Validity of two alternative systems for measuring

vertical jump height. Journal o f Strength and Conditioning Research, 2/(4),

1296-1299.

Lee, A. J. Y., & Lin, W. H. (2007). Association between sleep quality and physical

fitness in female young adults. Journal o f Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness,

47(4), 462-467.

Legar, D., Metlaine, A., & Choudat, D. (2005). Insomnia and sleep disruption: Relevance

for athletic performance. Clinics in Sports Medicine, 24, 269-285.

Lopez, F. G., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E. (1988). The relation of parental divorce

to college student development. Journal o f Divorce, 12, 83-98.

Low, K.G., & Gendaszek, A.E. (2002). Illicit use of psychostimulants among college

students: A preliminary study. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 7(3), 283-287.

Lowry, M., Dean, K., & Manders, K. (2010). The link between sleep quantity and

academic performance for the college student. Sentience, 3, 16-19.

Lugaresi, E., Cirignotta, F., Zucconi, M., Mandini, S., Lenzi, P. L., & Coccagna, G.

(1983). Good and poor sleepers: An epidemiological survey of the San Marion

population. In : Guilleminault, C., and Lugaresi, E., eds. Sleep/Wake Disorders:

Natural History, Epidemiology, and Long-Term Evolution. New York: Raven

Press.
Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and

predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal o f

Adolescent Health, 46, 124-132.

Lyznicki, J. M., Doege, T. C., Davis, R. M., & Williams, M. A. (1998). Sleepiness,

driving, and motor vehicle crashes. Council on Scientific Affairs, American

Medical Association. The Journal o f the American Medical Association, 279(23),

1908-1913.

Machado, E., Varella, V., & Andrade, M. (1998). The Influence of Study Schedules and

Work on the Sleep-wake Cycle of College Students. Biological Rhythm Research,

29(5), 578-584.

Mamassis, G., & Doganis, G. (2004). The effects of a mental training program on

juniors’ precompetitive anxiety, self-confidence and tennis performance. Journal

o f Applied Sport Psychology, 39(2), 118-137.

Manber, R., Bootzin, R. R., Acebo, C., & Carskadon, M. A. (1996). The effects of

regularized sleep-wake schedules on daytime sleepiness. Sleep, 19(5), 432-441

Martin, K. A., Moritz, S. E., & Hall, C. R. (1999). Imagery use in sport: A literature

review and applied model. The Sport Psychologist, 13(3), 245-268.

Maruff, P., Falleti, M. G., Collie, A., Darby, D., & McStephen, M. (2005). Fatigue

related impairment in the speed, accuracy and variability of psychomotor

performance: Comparison with blood alcohol levels. Journal o f Sleep Research,

14(1), 21-27.
Mathieson, C. M., Fans, P. D., Stam, H. J., & Egger, L. A. (1992). Health behaviors in a

Canadian community college sample: Prevalence of drug use and inter

relationships among behaviours. Revue Canadienne de Sante Publique, 83, 264-

267.

Mathiowetz, V., Kashman, N., Volland, G., Weber, K., Dowe, M., & Rogers, S. (1985).

Grip and pinch strength: Normative data for adults. Archives o f Physical Medicine

and Rehabilitation, 66(2), 69-74.

McCann, S. J. H., & Stewin, L. L. (1987). Worry, anxiety, and preferred length of sleep.

Journal of Genetic Psychology, 149, 413-418.

Mednick, S., Nakayama, K., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Sleep-dependent learning: A nap

is as good as night. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 697-698.

Meijer, A. M., Habekothe, H. T., & Van Den Wittenboer, G. L. (2000). Time in bed,

quality of sleep and school functioning of children. Journal o f Sleep Research,

9(2), 145-153.

Mellinger, G. D., Balter, M. B., & Uhlenhuth, E. H. (1985). Insomnia and its treatment:

Prevalence and correlates. Archives o f General Psychiatry, 4 2 ,225-232.

Mitler, M. M., Carskadon, M. A., Czeisler, C. A., Dement, W. C., Dinges, D. F., &

Graeber, R.C. (1988). Catastrophes, sleep, and public policy: Consensus report.

Sleep, 11(\), 100-9.

Mohr, D., Vedantham, K., Neylan, T., Metzler, T. J., Best, S., & Marmar, C. R. (2003).

The mediating effects of sleep in the relationship between traumatic stress and

health symptoms in urban police officers. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(3), 485-


178

Monk, T. H., Folkard, S., & Wedderbum, A. I. (1996). Maintaining safety and high

performance on shiftwork. Applied Ergonomics, 27(1), 17-23.

Monroe, L. (1967). Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor

sleepers. Journal o f Abnormal Psychology, 72, 255-264.

Morikawa, T., Hayashi, M., & Hori, T. (1997). Auto power and coherence analysis of

delta-theta band EEG during the waking-sleeping transition period.

Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 103, 633-641.

Morin, C. M., Culbert, J. P., Schwartz, S. M. (1994). Nonpharmacological interventions

for insomnia: A meta-analysis of treatment efficacy. The American journal o f

psychiatry, 757(8), 1172-1780.

Morin, C. M., & Wooten, V. (1996). Psychological and pharmacological approaches to

treating insomnia. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 521-542.

Murphy, S. L. (2000). Deaths: Final data for 1998. National Vital Statistics Report,

45(11), 1-105.

Naitoh, P., Kelly, T. L., & Babkoff, H. (1992). Napping, stimulant, and four-choice

performance. In: Broughton, R. J., Oglivie, R. D., eds. Sleep, Arousal, and

Performance. Boston: Birkhauser.

National Institute of Health (1998). Understanding Sleep (98-3440-c). Bethesda, MD:

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.

National Sleep Foundation (2005). 2005 Sleep in America poll. Washington, DC: Author.

Nehlig, A., Daval, J. L., & Debry, G. (1992). Caffeine and the central nervous system:

Mechanisms of action, biochemical, metabolic and psychostimulant effects. Brain

Research Reviews, 17(2), 139-170.


179

Nesca, M., & Koulack, D. (1994). Recognition memory, sleep and circadian rhythms.

Canadian Journal o f Experimental Psychology, 48, 359-379.

Newman, A. B., Enright, P. L., Manolio, T. A., & Haponik, E. F. (1997). Sleep

disturbance, psychosocial correlates, and cardiovascular disease in 5201 older

adults: The Cardiovascular Health Study. Journal o f the American Geriatrics

Society, 45( 1), 1-7.

Ohayon, M. M., Caulet, M., Philip, P., Guilleminault, C., & Priest, R. G. (1997). How

sleep and mental disorders are related to complaints of daytime sleepiness.

Archives o f Internal Medicine, 157(22), 2645-2652.

Ohayon, M. M., Roberts, R. E., Zulley, J., Smime, S., & Priest, R. G. (2000). Prevalence

and sleep patterns of problematic sleep among older adolescents. Journal o f the

American Academy o f Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 1549-1556.

O’Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2002). Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use

among American college students. Journal o f Studies on Alcohol, Supplement, 14,

23-39.

Parker, D. C., Rossman, L., Kripke, D. F. et al. (1980). Endocrine rhythms across sleep

wake cycles in normal young men under basal state conditions. In: Orem, J.,

Barnes, C. D. eds. Physiology in Sleep. New York: Academic Press.

Parkes, K. R. (1999). Shiftwork, job type, and the work environment as joint predictors of

health outcomes. Journal o f Occupational Health Psychology, 4(3), 256-268.

Parmeggiani, P. L. (1977). Interaction between sleep and thermoregulation. Waking

Sleep, 1, 123-132.
180

Parmeggiani, P.L. (1994). The autonomic nervous system in sleep. In M.H. Kryger T.

Roth, & W.C. Dement (Eds.), Principles and practice o f sleep medicine.

Philadelphia: Saunders.

Patel, S. R., Ayas, N. T., Malhotra, M. R., White, D. P., Shemhammer, E. S., Speizer, F.

E., Stampfer, F. E., Hu, F. B. (2004). A prospective study of sleep duration and

mortality risk in women. Sleep, 27(3), 440-4.

Patterson, D. D., & Peterson, D. H. (2004). Vertical jump and leg power norms for young

adults. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 8, 33-41.

Peluso, E. A., Ross, M. J., Gfeller, J. D., & La Voie, D. J. (2005). A comparison of

mental strategies during athletic skills performance. Journal o f Sports Science and

Medicine, 4, 543-549.

Perlman, D. M., Salomons, T. V., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2010). Differential effects

on pain intensity and unpleasantness of two meditation practices. Emotion, 10(1),

65-71.

Peters, B.R., Joireman, J., & Ridgeway, R.L. (2005). Individual differences in the

consideration of future consequences scale correlate with sleep habits, sleep

quality, and GPA in university students. Psychological Reports, 96, 817-824.

Peterson, C. (1997). Psychology: A Biopsychosocial Approach (2nd Ed.) New York:

Longman.

Petre-Quadens, I., & DeLee, C. (1974). Sleep-cycle alterations during pregnancy,

postpartum and the menstrual cycle. In: Ferin M, Halberg F, Richart RM, Van

Wiele RL, eds. Biorhythms and Human Reproduction, New York, NY: John

Wiley & Sons, 335-351.


181

Philibert, I. (2005). Sleep loss and performance in residents and nonphysicians: A

metaanalytie examination. Sleep, 28( 11), 1392-402.

Pilcher, J. J., Ginter, D. R., & Sadowsky, B. (1997). Sleep quality versus sleep quantity:

Relationships between sleep and measures of health, well-being and sleepiness in

college students. Journal o f Psychosomatic Research, 42(6), 583-596.

Pilcher, J. J., & Huffcutt, A, I. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: A

metaanalysis. Sleep, 19(4), 318-26.

Pilcher, J. J., & Ott, E. S. (1998). The relationship between sleep and measures of health

and well-being in college students: a repeated measures approach. Behavioral

Medicine, 23, 170-178.

Pilcher, J. J., Schoeling, S. E., & Prosansky, C. M. (2000). Self-report sleep habits as

predictors of subjective sleepiness. Behavioral Medicine. 25(4), 161-168.

Pilcher, J., & Walters, A. (1997). How sleep deprivation affects psychological variables

related to college students' cognitive performance. Journal o f American College

Health, 46(3), 121-126.

Porkka-Heiskanen, T. (1999). Adenosine in sleep and wakefulness. Annals o f Medicine,

31, 125-129.

Pressman, M. R., & Orr, W. C. (Eds.). (1997). Understanding sleep: The evaluation

and treatment o f sleep disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Prinz, P. (2004). Sleep, appetite, and obesity-What is the link? Public Library o f

Science and Medicine, 1, 186-188.


182

Purnell, M. T., Feyer, A.-M., & Herbison, G. P. (2002). The impact of a nap opportunity

during the night shift on the performance and alertness of 12-h shift workers.

Journal o f Sleep Research, 11, 219-227.

Racinais, S., Hue, O., Blonc, S., & Le Gallais, D. (2004). Effect of sleep deprivation on

shuttle run score in middle-aged amateur athletes: Influence of initial score.

Journal o f Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 44, 246-248.

Ralph, M. R., & Lehman, M. N. (1991). Transplantation: A new tool in the analysis of

the mammalian hypothalamic circadian pacemaker. Trends in Neurosciences, 14,

362-366.

Ram, A., Pandey, H. P., Matsumura, H., Kasahara-Orita, K., Nakajima, T., Takahata, R.,

Satoh, S., Terao, A., & Hayaishi, O. (1997). CSF levels of prostaglandins,

especially the level of prostaglandin D2 , are correlated with increasing propensity

towards sleep in rats. Brain Research, 751, 81- 89.

Randazzo, A. C., Muehlbach, M. J., Schweitzer, P. K., & Walsh, J. K. (1998). Cognitive

function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10-14. Journal o f Sleep

Research & Sleep Medicine, 21, 861-868.

Rantanen, T., Volpato, S., Ferrucci, L., Heikkinen, E., Fried, L. P., & Guralnik, J. M.

(2003). Handgrip strength and cause-specific and total mortality in older disabled

women: Exploring the mechanism. Journal o f the American Geriatrics Society,

51, 636-641.

Rechtschaffen, A., Gilliland, M. A., Bergmann, B. M., & Winter, J. B. (1983).

Physiological correlates of prolonged sleep deprivation in rats. Science, 221, 182-

184.
183

Refinetti, R. (2000). Circadian rhythm of locomotor activity in the pill bug,

Armadillidiumvulgare (Isopoda). Crustaceana, 73, 575-583.

Refinetti, R., & Menaker, M. (1992). The circadian rhythm of body temperature of

normal and tau-mutant golden hamsters. Journal o f Thermal Biology, 17, 129-

133.

Reich, P., Geyer, S. J., & Kamovsky, M. L. (1972). Metabolism of brain during sleep and

wakefulness. Journal o f neurochemistry, 19(2), 487-497.

Reilly, T., & Piercy, M. (1994). The effect o f partial sleep deprivation on weight lifting

performance. Ergonomics, 3 7 ,107-115.

Reuter, S. E., Massy-Westropp, N., & Evans, A. M. (2011). Reliability and validity of

indices of hand-grip strength and endurance. Australian Occupational Therapy

Journal, 58(2), 82-87.

Rice, K. G., Cole, D., & Lapsley, D. K. (1990). Separationindividuation, family cohesion

and adjustment to college: Measurement validation and a test of a theoretical

model. Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 37, 195-210.

Richardson, P. A., & Latuda, L. M. (1995). Therapeutic imagery and athletic injuries.

Journal o f Athletic Training, 30, 10-12.

Riedel, B. W., Durrence, H. H., Lichstein, K. L., Taylor, D. J., & Bush, A. J. (2004). The

relation between smoking and sleep: the influence of smoking level, health, and

psychological variables. Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 2(1), 63-78.

Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering

words not presented in lists. Journal o f Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803-814.


Roehrs, T., Carskadon, M., Dement, W., & Roth, T. (2000). Daytime Sleepiness and

Alertness. In: Principles and Practice o f Sleep Medicine, 3rd Edition, pp. 43-52.

Roehrs, T. & Roth, T. (1997). Hypnotics, alcohol, and caffeine: Relation to insomnia. In:

Pressman, MR.; Orr, WC., editors. Understanding sleep: The evaluation and

treatment o f sleep disorders. Vol. 339-355. Washington, DC: American

Psychological Association.

Roehrs, T., Shore, E., Papineau, K., Rosenthal, L., & Roth T. (1996). A two-week sleep

extension in sleepy normals. Sleep, 19(1), 576-582.

Roehrs, T., Vogel, G., & Roth, T. (1990). Rebound insomnia: Its determinants and

significance. American Journal o f Medicine, 88, 39S-42S.

Roehrs, T.A., Zorick, F., Sicklesteel, J., Wittig, R., & Roth, T. (1983). Age-related sleep

wake disorders at a sleep disorder center. Journal o f the American Geriatric

Society, 31, 364-370.

Roffwarg, H. P., Muzio, J. N., & Dement, W. C. (1966). Ontogenic development of the

human sleep-dream cycle. Science, 152, 604-619.

Rosen, C. L., Palermo, T. M., Larkin, E. K., & Redline, S. (2002). Health-related quality

of life and sleep-disordered breathing in children. Sleep, 25(6), 657-666.

Ross, S. E., Niebling, B. C., & Hechert T. M. (1999). Sources of stress among college

students. College Student Journal, 33, 312-318.

Russell, R. K., & Petrie, T. A. (1992). Academic adjustment of college students:

Assessment and counseling. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook o f

counseling psychology (2nd ed.) (pp. 485-511). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Sagaspe, P., Charles, A., Taillard, J., Bioulac, B., & Philip, P. (2003). Inhibition and

working memory: Effect of acute sleep deprivation on a random letter generation

task. Canadian Journal o f Experimental Psychology, 57(4), 265-273.

Sassin, J. F., Parker, D. C., Johnson, L. C., Rossman, L. G., Mace, J. W., Gotlin, R. W.

(1969). Effects of slow wave sleep deprivation on human growth hormone release

in sleep: Preliminary study. Life Science, 8(23), 1299-1307.

Scamel, T., Gerashchenko, D., Urade, Y., Onoe, H., Saper, C., & Hayaishi, O. (1998).

Activation of ventrolateral preoptic neurons by the somnogen prostaglandin D2.

Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences, 95, 7754-7759.

Schredl, M., Weber, B., & Heuser, I. (1998). REM sleep and memory. Psychologische

Beitrage, 40, 340-349.

Schultz, J. H., & Luthe, W. (1959). Autogenic training: A psychophysiologic approach

in psychotherapy. New York: Grune and Stratton.

Sejnowski, T.J., & Destexhe, A. (2000). Why do we sleep? Brain Research, 886{ 1-2),

208-223.

Shapiro, C. M., Bortz, R., Mitchell, D., Bartel, P., & Jooste, P. (1981). Slow-wave sleep:

A recovery period after exercise. Science, 214, 1253-1254.

Sharpe, M. J., & Heppner, P. P. (1991). Gender role, gender-role conflict, and

psychological well-being in men. Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 38, 323-

330.

Shaulis, D., Golding, L. A., & Tandy, R. D. (1994). Reliability of the AAHPERD

functional fitness assessment across multiple practice sessions in older men and

women. Journal o f Aging and Physical Activity, 2(3), 273-279.


Shirakawa, S., Takase, M., Tanaka, H., & Yamamoto, Y. (1999). Improvement effects of

chronobiological-scheduled nap sleep on nighttime sleep in the aged. Japanese

Journal o f Clinical Electroencephalography, 41, 708-712.

Sibler, M. H., Ancoli-Israel, S., Bonnet, M. H., Chokroverty, S., Grigg-Damberger, M.

M., Hirshkowitz, M., Kapen, S., Keenan, S. A., Kryger, M. H., Penzel T.,

Pressman, M. R., & Iber, C. (2007). The visual scoring of sleep in adults. Journal

o f Clinical Sleep Medicine, 3, 121-131.

Sicard, B., Jouve, E., & Blin, O. (2001). Risk propensity assessment in military special

operations. Military Medicine, 166, 871-874.

Siegel, J. M., & Rogawski, M. A. (1988). A function for REM sleep: Regulation of

noradrenergic receptor sensitivity. Brain Research, 472, 213-233.

Signal, L., & Gander, P. H. (2002). Psychomotor performance improvements with a

short workplace nap on the night shift: the benefits of stage 1 sleep. Sleep, 25,

A116.

Singleton, R.A., & Wolfson, A.R. (2009). Alcohol consumption, sleep, and academic

performance among college students. Journal o f Alcohol and Drugs, 70, 355-363.

Sloan, E. P., & Shapiro, C. M. (1993). Editorial. Journal o f Psychosomatic Research, 37,

1- 2 .

Smith, C. (1996). Sleep states, memory processes and synaptic plasticity. Behavioral

Brain Research, 78, 49-56.

Smith, C. S., Folkard, S., & Fuller, J. A. (2003). Shiftwork and working hours. In J. C.

Quick (Ed.), Handbook o f Occupational Health Psychology. Washington:

American Psychological Association.


187

Smith, C., & Lapp, L. (1991). Increase in number of REMs and REM density in

humans following an intensive learning period. Sleep, 14, 325-330.

Soldatos, C. R., & Paparrigopoulos, T. J. (2005). Sleep physiology and pathology:

Pertinence to psychiatry. International Review o f Psychiatry, 17(4), 213-28.

Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., & Van Cauter, E. (1999). Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and

endocrine function. Lancet, 354, 1435-1439.

Spiegel, K., Tasali, E., Penev, P., & Van Cauter, E. (2004). Brief communication: Sleep

curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels,

elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Annals o f Internal

Medicine, 141(11), 846-850.

Stepanski, E. (2000). Behavioral therapy for insomnia. In: M. Kryger, T. Roth, W.

Dement (Eds.), Principles and Practice o f Sleep Medicine, Third Edition. W. B.

Saunders Co, Philadelphia.

Steriade, M., & McCarley, R. W. (1990). Brainstem control o f wakefulness and sleep.

New York: Plenum Press.

Strubbe, J. H., & Woods, S. C. (2004). The timing of meals. Psychological Reviews, 111,

128-141.

Taheri, S., Zeitzer, J. M., & Mignot, E. (2002). The role of hypocretins (orexins) in sleep

regulation and narcolepsy. Annual Review o f Neuroscience, 25, 283-313.

Taheri, S., Lin, L., Austin, D., Young, T., & Mignot, E. (2004). Short sleep duration is

associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index.

PLoS Medicine, 1(3), 62.


188

Takahashi, M. (2003). The role of prescribed napping in sleep medicine. Sleep Medicine

Reviews, 7(3), 227-235.

Takahashi, M, & Arito, H. (2000). Maintenance of alertness and performance by a brief

nap after lunch under prior sleep deficit. Sleep, 23, 813-819.

Takeuchi, L., Davis, G. M., Plyley, M., Goode, R., & Shephard, R. J. (1985). Sleep

deprivation, chronic exercise and muscular performance. Ergonomics, 28(3), 591-

601.

Tanaka, H., Taira, K., Arakawa, M., Masuda, A., Yamamoto, Y., Komoda, et al. (2002).

An examination of sleep health, lifestyle and mental health in junior high school

students. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 56(3), 235-236.

Tanaka, H., Fujita, N., Takanashi, M., Hirabuki, N., Yoshimura, H., Abe, K., &

Nakamura, H. (2003). Effect of stage 1 sleep on auditory cortex during pure tone

stimulation: Evaluation by functional magnetic resonance imaging with

simultaneous EEG monitoring. American Journal o f Neuroradiology, 24, 1982-

1988.

Tassi, P., & Muzet, A. (2000). Sleep inertia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4, 341-353.

Taub, J. M. (1980). Effects of ad-lib extended-delayed sleep on sensorimotor

performance, memory, and sleepiness in the young adult. Physiology and

Behavior, 25, 77-87.

Taub, J., 8c Berger, R. (1973). Performance and mood following variations in the length

and timing of sleep. Psychophysiology, 10, 559-570.

Taub, J. M., & Hawkins, D. R. (1979). Aspects of personality associated with irregular

sleep habits in young adults. Journal o f Clinical Psychology, 35, 296-304.


189

Taylor, D. J., & McFatter, R. M. (2003). Cognitive performance after sleep deprivation:

Does personality make a difference? Personality and Individual Differences, 34,

1179-1193.

Thorpy, M. J. (1991). History of sleep and man. In M. J. Thorpy & J. Yager (Eds.), The

encyclopedia o f sleep and sleep disorders. New York: Oxford.

Thorpy, M. J., & Yager, J. (2001). The Encyclopaedia of Sleep and Sleep Disorders. 2nd

Edition. Facts on File Inc.: New York.

Tietzel, A. J., & Lack, L. C. (2001). The short-term benefits of brief and long naps

following nocturnal sleep restriction. Sleep, 24, 293-300.

Tietzel, A. J., & Lack, L.C. (2002). The recuperative value of brief and ultra-brief naps

on alertness and cognitive performance. Journal o f Sleep Research, 11, 213-218.

Tilley, A., & Brown, S. (1992). Sleep deprivation. In A.P. Smith & D.M. Jones (Eds.),

Handbook o f human performance. Vol. 3. State and trait (pp. 237-259). London:

Academic Press.

Tilley, A., Donohoe, F., & Hensby, S. (1987). Homeostatic changes in slow wave sleep

during recovery sleep following restricted nocturnal sleep and partial slow wave

sleep recovery during an afternoon nap. Sleep, 10, 600-605.

Tomasi, D., Wang, R. L., Telang, F., Boronikolas, V., Jayne, M. C., Wang, G. J., Fowler,

J. S., & Volkow, N.D. (2009). Impairment of attentional networks after 1 night

of sleep deprivation. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 233-240.

Trinder, J., Paxton, S. J., Montgomery, I., & Fraser, G. (1985). Endurance as opposed to

power training: Their effect on sleep. Psychophysiology, 22(6), 668-673.


190

Trockel, M. T., Barnes, M. D., & Egget, D. L. (2000). Health-related variables and

academic performance among first-year college students: Implications for sleep

and other behavior. Journal o f American College Health, 4 9 ,125-38.

United States Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health,

National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, & National Center on Sleep Disorders

Research. (2003). 2003 National sleep disorders research plan (NIH

Publication No. 03-5209).

Urponen, H., Partinen, M., Vuori, I., & Hasan, J. (1991). Sleep quality and health:

Description of the Sleep Quality Index. In J. Peter (Ed.), Sleep and Health Risk.

Berlin: Springer, 555-558.

Utay, J., & Miller, M. (2006). Guided imagery as an effective therapeutic technique: A

brief review of its history and efficacy research. Journal o f Instructional

Psychology, 33, 40-43.

Van Dongen, H. P., Maislin, G., Mullington, J. M., & Dinges, D. F. (2003). The

cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on

neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction

and total sleep deprivation. Sleep, 26(2), 117-26.

Vein, A. M., & Airapetov, R. G. (1984). Nocturnal polygraphic studies during treatment

of depressed patients with sleep deprivation. Zh Nevropatol Psykhiatr, 84, 577-

583.

Vein, A. M., Dallakyan, I. G., Levin, Y. I., & Skakun, K. E. (1983). Physiological and

psychological consequences of single sleep deprivation. Human Physiology, 8,

392-396.
191

Vempati, R. P., & Telles, S. (2002). Yoga-based guided relaxation reduces sympathetic

activity judged from baseline levels. Psychological Reports, 90, 487-494.

Vgontzas, A., Cui, L., & Merikangas, K. R. (2008), Are sleep difficulties associated with

migraine attributable to anxiety and depression? Headache, 48, 1451-1459.

Walker, M. P. (2008). Cognitive consequences o f sleep and sleep loss, Sleep Medicine,

9(1), S29-S34.

Watanabe, E., Fukuda, S. Hara, H., Maeda, Y., Ohira, H., & Shirakawa, T. (2006).

Differences in relaxation by means of guided imagery in a healthy community

sample. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 12(2), 60-66.

Waterhouse, J., Atkinson, G., Edwards, B., & Reilly, T. (2007). The role of a short post

lunch nap in improving cognitive, motor, and sprint performance in participants

with partial sleep deprivation. Journal o f Sports Sciences, 25(14), 1557-1566.

Webb, W. B. (1974). Sleep as an adaptive response. Perceptual Motor Skills, 38, 1023-

1027.

Webb, W. B., & Agnew, H. W. J., (1974). The effects of a chronic limitation of sleep

length. Psychophysiology, 11, 265-274.

Wechsler, H., Davenport, A., Dowdall, G., Moeykens, B., & Castillo, S. (1994). Health

and behavioral consequences of binge drinking in college: A national survey of

students at 140 campuses. The Journal o f the American Medical Association, 272,

1672-1677.
Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Nelson, T. F., & Lee, H. (2002). Trends

in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts: Findings

from 4Harvard School of Public Health college alcohol study surveys: 1993 2001.

Journal o f American College Health, 50, 203-217.

Wechsler, H., & Nelson, T. F. (2008). What we have learned from the Harvard School of

Public Health College Alcohol Study: Focusing attention on college student

alcohol consumption and the environmental conditions that promote it. Journal o f

Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69, 481-490.

Wechsler, H., Rigotti, N. A., Gledhill-Hoyt, J. & Lee, H. (1998). Increased levels of

cigarette use among college students: A cause for national concern. The Journal

o f the American Medical Association, 280(19), 1673-1678.

Welstein, L., Dement, W. C., Redington, D., & Guilleminault, C. (1983). Insomnia in the

San Francisco Bay area: a telephone survey. In: Guilleminault C, Lugest E (eds)

Sleep-wake disorders: Natural history, epidemiology, and long-term evolution.

Raven, New York, pp 73-86.

Whitney, C. W., Enright, P. L., Newman, A. B., Bonekat, W., Foley, D., & Quan, S. F.

(1998). Correlates o f daytime sleepiness in 4578 elderly persons: The

cardiovascular health study. Sleep, 21, 27-36.

Wouters-Adriaens, M. P., & Westerterp, K. R. (2006). Basal metabolic rate as a proxy for

overnight energy expenditure: The effect of age. The British Journal o f Nutrition,

95(6), 1166-1170.
Yang, C. M , Wu, C. H., Hsieh, M. H., Liu, M. H., & Lu, F. H. (2003). Coping with

sleep disturbances among young adults: A survey of first-year college students in

Taiwan. Behavioral Medicine, 29(3), 133-138.

Yegneswaran, B., & Shapiro, C. (2007). Do sleep deprivation and alcohol have the same

effects on psychomotor performance? Journal o f Psychosomatic Research, 63,

569-572.

Young, T., Blustein, J., Finn, L., & Palta, M. (1997). Sleep-disordered breathing and

motor vehicle accidents in a population-based sample of employed adults. Sleep,

20(8), 608-13.

Youngstedt, S. D., O'Connor, P. J., & Dishman, R. K. (1997). The effects of acute

exercise on sleep: A quantitative synthesis. Sleep, 20(3), 203-214.

Zammit, G. K. (1988). Subjective ratings of the characteristics and sequelae of good and

poor sleep in normal. Journal o f Clinical Psychology, 44, 123-130.

Zeidan, F., Gordon, N. S., Merchant, J., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). The Effects of Brief

Mindfulness Meditation Training on Experimentally Induced Pain. The Journal o f

Pain, 11(3), 199-209.

You might also like