Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Journal of Applied Psychology

© 2019 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 105, No. 6, 619 – 636
ISSN: 0021-9010 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000455

I’m Tired of Listening: The Effects of Supervisor Appraisals of Group


Voice on Supervisor Emotional Exhaustion and Performance

Hudson Sessions Jennifer D. Nahrgang


University of Oregon Arizona State University

Daniel W. Newton Melissa Chamberlin


University of Iowa Iowa State University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Employee voice, or speaking up with constructive expressions in the workplace, is beneficial to


organizations as it is often a catalyst for positive change. Despite its benefits, voice may have mixed
implications for supervisors who are frequently the targets of group members’ ideas or concerns. We
draw on the transactional theory of stress to examine the positive and negative effects of group promotive
and prohibitive voice on supervisor emotional exhaustion and performance. Specifically, we theorize and
find that supervisors appraise group promotive voice as fostering their well-being and personal growth
(i.e., challenge appraisal) and, conversely, appraise group prohibitive voice as inhibiting their well-being
and personal growth (i.e., hindrance appraisal). These appraisals, in turn, influence supervisors’ emo-
tional exhaustion and performance. Furthermore, we investigate a supervisor’s personal sense of power
as a boundary condition that influences the effects of group voice on supervisor appraisals of group voice
and subsequent emotional exhaustion and performance. We test our model using a multiwave field
sample design (Study 1) and an in-person experimental design (Study 2). Across these 2 studies, we find
negative indirect effects of group promotive voice on supervisor emotional exhaustion through challenge
appraisals of group voice and positive indirect effects of group prohibitive voice on supervisor emotional
exhaustion through hindrance appraisals of group voice as well as conditional indirect effects of
supervisors’ personal sense of power. Our model offers novel insights into supervisors’ appraisals of
group voice and the implications for their emotional exhaustion and performance.

Keywords: group voice, emotional exhaustion, well-being, challenge and hindrance appraisals

Voice—ideas or problems expressed by employees with the Harrison, & Martin, 2013; Lam & Mayer, 2014; Li, Liao, Tangi-
intention of improving the work environment (Liang, Farh, & rala, & Firth, 2017; MacKenzie, Podsakoff, & Podsakoff, 2011;
Farh, 2012; Van Dyne & LePine, 1998)— has numerous benefits Morrison & Milliken, 2000). Despite the organizational benefits of
for work groups and organizations. Speaking up facilitates infor- speaking up, research on the effects of voice to individuals has
mation sharing that improves group and organizational perfor- shown that speaking up might not always be a positive experience
mance through increased learning, high-quality decision-making, for voicing employees. Indeed, voicing employees may risk their
and valuable improvements to working conditions (Detert, Burris, reputations by challenging the status quo (Grant, 2013; Morrison
& Milliken, 2000), tire of ongoing efforts to make suggestions
(Bolino, Hsiung, Harvey, & LePine, 2015), and be viewed as
worse performers as a consequence of expressing concerns (Cham-
This article was published Online First October 14, 2019. berlin, Newton, & LePine, 2017).
X Hudson Sessions, Department of Management, Lundquist College of As suggested by this prior work, scholars generally agree on the
Business, University of Oregon; Jennifer D. Nahrgang, Department of Man-
benefits of speaking up for work groups and organizations as well
agement and Entrepreneurship, W.P. Carey School of Business, Arizona State
University; Daniel W. Newton, Department of Management and Entrepreneur- as the outcomes of speaking up for voicing employees. However,
ship, Tippie College of Business, University of Iowa; Melissa Chamberlin, the effect of voice on supervisors is less clear and worthy of
Department of Management, Ivy College of Business, Iowa State University. attention considering that supervisors are typically the targets of
We acknowledge the financial support provided by the Department of their group’s voice (Detert & Burris, 2007; Hussain, Shu, Tangi-
Management and Entrepreneurship at Arizona State University. We also rala, & Ekkirala, 2019; Liu, Tangirala, & Ramanujam, 2013).
acknowledge presentations of versions of our work at the 2017 Academy
Group voice is a group-level construct that emerges through a
of Management annual conference in Atlanta, Georgia and at the 2019
INGRoup annual conference in Lisbon, Portugal.
bottom-up process and represents the combined contributions of
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hudson group members’ input about work-related issues (Lam & Mayer,
Sessions, Department of Management, Lundquist College of Business, 2014). Even though group voice is constructive in nature and
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403. E-mail: sessions@uoregon.edu intended to benefit the work environment, supervisors may have

619
620 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

mixed reactions to group voice. On the one hand, supervisors may exhaustion and performance through the appraisal process. We
view group voice as fostering growth through improvement- find that group promotive voice enhances supervisor performance
oriented ideas, evoking positive emotions around forward-looking by reducing supervisor emotional exhaustion whereas group pro-
recommendations, and reflecting group member help and support hibitive voice diminishes supervisor performance by increasing
(e.g., Burris, 2012; Chamberlin, 2017; Lam, Rees, Levesque, & supervisor emotional exhaustion. Our efforts advance work that
Ornstein, 2018). Such positive experiences with group voice could highlights important distinctions between promotive and prohibi-
be an uplift for supervisors that reduces their emotional exhaustion tive voice for perceptions of a voicing employee’s performance
and enhances their performance. On the other hand, supervisors (e.g., Chamberlin et al., 2017; Liang et al., 2012) by addressing
may perceive group voice as introducing problems that must be how these two types of group voice alternatively reduce or amplify
addressed, evoking negative emotions related to things that have supervisors’ emotional exhaustion and performance. Second, we
gone poorly, or reflecting complaints or fault-finding (Chamberlin, advance research that has considered how voice can threaten
2017; Liang et al., 2012). Such negative experiences with group supervisors (e.g., Burris, 2012). The transactional theory of stress
voice could weigh down supervisors, thereby increasing their provides a novel and useful lens to examine the manner in which
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

emotional exhaustion and diminishing their performance. supervisors may appraise group voice as enhancing or diminishing
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The purpose of this research is to provide a more complete their personal well-being. Third, we contribute to recent efforts to
account of how group voice influences supervisor emotional ex- advance the transactional theory of stress by expressly considering
haustion and, in turn, performance. Guided by Lazarus and Folk- challenge and hindrance appraisals as distinct outcomes of work
man’s (1984) transactional theory of stress, we assert that super- conditions (LePine et al., 2016; Webster et al., 2011). We consider
visors are attuned to the personal significance of group voice in the the mediating role of challenge and hindrance appraisals between
appraisal process and experience differential outcomes depending a prevalent experience for supervisors— group voice—and impor-
on how their group speaks up. Specifically, we argue that group tant organizational outcomes. Practically speaking, our work ad-
promotive voice— group member expressions of ideas to improve dresses the benefits and burdens experienced by supervisors as
existing work practices to benefit the group or organization (Li et targets of group voice.
al., 2017; Liang et al., 2012)—will be appraised as a challenging
work condition that enhances well-being and personal growth
Theoretical Development and Hypotheses
(LePine, Zhang, Crawford, & Rich, 2016; Webster, Beehr, &
Love, 2011). In contrast, we argue that group prohibitive voice— Voice entails group members’ sharing constructive suggestions
group member expressions of problems and concerns about prac- or concerns about work-related issues that challenge the status quo
tices that may harm the group or organization (Li et al., 2017; (Liang et al., 2012; Van Dyne & LePine, 1998). Expressions of
Liang et al., 2012)—will be appraised as a hindering work condi- voice represent a relatively discretionary behavior from group
tion that inhibits well-being and personal growth (LePine et al., members and thus are voluntary and not accompanied by formal
2016; Webster et al., 2011). rewards (Van Dyne & LePine, 1998). Group voice exists when,
Supervisors’ appraisals of group voice may vary depending on within a group, two or more members voice their ideas or con-
their perceived ability to influence other organizational actors and cerns. Indeed, group voice emerges through a bottom-up process in
circumstances, a notion captured by a supervisor’s personal sense which various points of view are expressed, ideas or concerns
of power (Anderson, John, & Keltner, 2012). Supervisors’ per- build on one another, or issues pointed out by one group member
sonal sense of power is relevant to the appraisal process given that evoke additional expressions from other group members (Li et al.,
a lack of access to resources or influence over circumstances can 2017). Thus, group voice represents the combined contributions of
be appraised as stressful (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). We assert group members’ constructive suggestions or concerns about work-
that supervisors’ personal sense of power will affect the degree to related issues (Lam & Mayer, 2014). We focus on this group-level
which supervisors appraise group promotive voice as challenging construct because group voice, like other discretionary behaviors,
and group prohibitive voice as hindering. In turn, supervisors’ are most impactful at the aggregate level (Organ, 1988), and we
challenge and hindrance appraisals of group promotive and pro- examine the effects of group voice on supervisors because they are
hibitive voice (respectively) will influence the degree to which typically the targets of their group’s voice (Detert & Burris, 2007;
group voice has beneficial or detrimental effects on supervisor Hussain et al., 2019; Liu et al., 2013).
emotional exhaustion. Aligned with prior work, we further assert To examine the effects of group voice on supervisors, we draw
that emotional exhaustion will impair supervisor performance on the transactional theory of stress, which asserts that as individ-
(e.g., Bakker, Demerouti, & Verbeke, 2004). We evaluate our uals encounter various work conditions—stimuli, such as group
assertions across two studies. In Study 1, we use a multiwave field voice, that are external to oneself—they appraise the personal
sample of 150 supervisors and 493 employees to test how super- implications of these conditions (Lazarus, 1966; Lazarus & Folk-
visors differentially appraise group promotive and prohibitive man, 1984). Lazarus and Folkman (1984) theorize that the ap-
voice and the subsequent effects on their emotional exhaustion and praisal process represents the categorization of work conditions
performance. In Study 2, we conduct an in-person experiment to based on the significance for individuals’ personal well-being.
complement Study 1 by asserting causality through our manipu- However, not every work experience prompts an appraisal. In-
lations, limiting situational confounds that arise in field studies, stead, work conditions trigger appraisals when they introduce the
and accounting for individual differences through random assign- potential for either growth and gain or loss and harm (Lazarus &
ment across conditions. Folkman, 1984). Group voice is likely to trigger supervisors’
Our research makes several theoretical contributions. First, we appraisal process for two key reasons. First, group voice interrupts
demonstrate how group voice influences supervisors’ emotional the status quo of a work group with a call for supervisors to
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 621

consider new opportunities for growth and innovation or to address given that it offers solutions for the betterment of the work group
problems and concerns (Liang et al., 2012; Van Dyne & LePine, (Liang et al., 2012; Van Dyne & LePine, 1998). In support of this
1998). Thus, given that group voice expressly relates to group point, meta-analytic evidence shows that promotive voice posi-
members “rocking the boat” with input about potential gains or tively predicts supervisor ratings of the voicing employee’s job
harm to group functioning (Grant, 2013; Liang et al., 2012), performance (Chamberlin et al., 2017), suggesting that supervisors
supervisors should be attuned to the significance of group voice view solution-oriented promotive voice as a valuable contribution
and appraise group voice accordingly. to the goals and efforts of the group.
Second, group voice may be taken personally in that it reflects Based on supervisors’ positive interpretations of group promo-
on supervisors themselves. That is, supervisors are sensitive to the tive voice, we expect supervisors to appraise group promotive
embedded commentary of group voice because it implicitly pro- voice as fostering their growth and well-being. Specifically, group
vides feedback on a supervisor’s performance or even character promotive voice is likely to encourage expectations of growth as
and ability (Fast, Burris, & Bartel, 2014). Whereas supervisors supervisors receive solution-oriented input for future improve-
may interpret voice as a sign of support from group members ments. Moreover, supervisor perceptions of help and support from
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(Burris, 2012), supervisors can also feel threatened or criticized by expressions of group promotive voice should be associated with
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

group members who challenge the status quo given supervisors’ feeling valued and respected, which has been shown to increase
general “need to avoid embarrassment, threat, and feelings of perceived well-being (e.g., Anderson, Kraus, Galinsky, & Keltner,
vulnerability or incompetence” (Fast et al., 2014; Morrison & 2012; Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Singh-Manoux, Adler, & Mar-
Milliken, 2000, p. 708). According to Lazarus and Folkman mot, 2003). Thus, as supervisors experience group promotive
(1984), such personalization of positive or negative commentary voice, we argue they will see the potential for growth and im-
reflects vulnerability (i.e., consideration of personal adequacy in proved well-being and appraise group voice as challenging. At the
important matters to the individual) and would prompt an appraisal same time, the sense of a potential opportunity for gain embedded
of the work condition. in group promotive voice is likely to reduce supervisors’ evalua-
Whether supervisors positively or negatively appraise group tion of group voice as causing potential harm or loss (i.e., hin-
voice may depend on the manner in which group members speak drance appraisals of group voice).
up. In other words, how group members express themselves to a
supervisor may influence whether a supervisor considers group Hypothesis 1: Group promotive voice will be (a) positively
voice to promote his or her growth and well-being (i.e., challenge related to supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice and
appraisal) or inhibit his or her growth and well-being (i.e., hin- (b) negatively related to supervisor hindrance appraisals of
drance appraisal; Cavanaugh, Boswell, Roehling, & Boudreau, group voice.
2000; LePine et al., 2016). As we describe in the section that
follows, groups may speak up with promotive voice and/or pro- Supervisor Appraisals of Group Prohibitive Voice
hibitive voice. Both types of voice are forms of constructive input
wherein group members intend to improve work functioning In addition to promotive voice, group members may also engage
(Maynes & Podsakoff, 2014), but they also differ in several critical in prohibitive voice as they speak up with concerns about work
ways (Liang et al., 2012). We assert that these differences influ- practices and procedures that are harmful to the group or organi-
ence supervisors’ appraisals of group voice as challenging or zation. In contrast to the future-orientation of group promotive
hindering. That is, the extent to which a supervisor experiences voice, group prohibitive voice typically focuses on past problems
group promotive voice and group prohibitive voice influences a that should be corrected or halted (Chamberlin et al., 2017).
supervisor’s challenge appraisal of group voice—an assessment of However, similar to group promotive voice, group prohibitive
potential gain or enhanced well-being from group voice—and voice is still constructive in nature because it is intended by group
hindrance appraisal of group voice—an evaluation of prospective members to call attention to problems that could subsequently be
threat or harm to well-being from group voice.1 These appraisals, resolved for the benefit of the work group (Liang et al., 2012).
in turn, affect supervisors’ well-being (e.g., emotional exhaustion Although group members may intend to resolve problems vis-à-vis
as a negative indicator of well-being; Sonnentag, 2015) and per- prohibitive voice, Liang, Farh, and Farh (2012) suggest that su-
formance (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; LePine et al., 2016). pervisors may interpret prohibitive voice as motivated by negative
intentions. One reason for this negative interpretation is that group

Supervisor Appraisals of Group Promotive Voice


1
We note that challenge and hindrance appraisals represent two distinct
Group promotive voice refers to ideas and suggestions ex- constructs rather than two ends of a continuum (Lazarus & Folkman,
pressed by group members about potential opportunities to im- 1984). Our model allows for the notion that supervisors can simultaneously
prove work practices and procedures (Li et al., 2017; Liang et al., experience both challenge and hindrance appraisals of group voice, while
2012). Speaking up with promotive voice involves sharing ideas proposing that group promotive voice will be positively (negatively) asso-
ciated with challenge (hindrance) appraisals of group voice and group
focused on a future ideal state (Liang et al., 2012). Such forward- prohibitive voice will be positively (negatively) associated with hindrance
thinking expressions signal group members’ investments in long- (challenge) appraisals of group voice. This approach aligns with prior
term improvements and innovation (Qin, DiRenzo, Xu, & Duan, investigations, which have demonstrated that challenge and hindrance
2014). Moreover, the forward-looking ideas and suggestions con- appraisals tend to be negatively correlated (e.g., LePine et al., 2016;
Webster et al., 2011). Thus, although it is possible that group promotive
sist of solutions for how the work group can perform more effec- voice or group prohibitive voice might positively predict both challenge
tively (Chamberlin et al., 2017). Consequently, supervisors may be and hindrance appraisals of group voice, prior empirical research suggests
apt to interpret group promotive voice as helpful and supportive that this outcome is unlikely.
622 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

prohibitive voice is tainted by the underlying problems that are opportunities and address problems expressed by their work
expressed, and supervisors may have a strong aversion to seeing groups. This perceived efficacy leads to increased positive feelings
their own inadequacies reflected in such negative feedback (Ar- and receptivity toward group member voice (Fast et al., 2014).
gyris, 1990; Kluger & DeNisi, 1996). Relatedly, receiving group Further, a high personal sense of power increases supervisors’
members’ expressions of concerns can be accompanied by nega- attention on the potential rewards introduced by group voice,
tive emotions and provoke defensiveness in supervisors regarding enhancing supervisors’ sensitivity to opportunities embedded in
past shortcomings or failures (Detert & Edmondson, 2011; Mor- group promotive voice and diminishing their sensitivity to threats
rison & Milliken, 2000). Further, given that prohibitive voice may couched in group prohibitive voice (e.g., Lin & Johnson, 2015).
not be seen as solution oriented, supervisors are unlikely to per- Given the effect of perceiving sufficient personal resources to
ceive the same degree of help and support from group prohibitive address group voice and the associated increase of attention on
voice as they do from group promotive voice. Instead, supervisors
potential rewards, we expect supervisors with a high personal
are more likely to perceive group prohibitive voice as bordering on
sense of power to more favorably appraise group promotive and
complaining or fault-finding (Liang et al., 2012). In support of the
prohibitive voice (i.e., stronger challenge appraisals of group pro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

notion that supervisors react negatively to group prohibitive voice,


motive voice and weaker hindrance appraisals of group prohibitive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

meta-analysis has shown that supervisors rate employees who


speak up with prohibitive voice as lower performers (Chamberlin voice).
et al., 2017), suggesting supervisors view prohibitive voice as Conversely, a low personal sense of power is associated with
detrimental to the goals and efforts of the work group. perceptions of limited personal resources to influence people and
Given supervisors’ negative associations with group prohibitive circumstances at work (Anderson et al., 2012) and greater sensi-
voice, we expect that they will appraise group prohibitive voice as tivity to potential threats (Keltner et al., 2003). Supervisors with a
discouraging growth and diminishing their well-being. Specifi- low personal sense of power may perceive that they are incapable
cally, group prohibitive voice should be associated with harm and of responding to opportunities and resolving concerns expressed
setbacks as the group’s prohibitive input will be tinged by the by group members. This perception of inefficacy leads to ego
expressed problems or concerns. Additionally, group prohibitive defensiveness in response to group voice (Fast et al., 2014), which
voice may stifle perceived well-being as supervisors feel defensive is likely associated with negative interpretations of group promo-
about the negative personal commentary reflected in group pro- tive and prohibitive voice. Moreover, supervisors will likely focus
hibitive voice. This evaluation of setbacks and perceived threat to on the risky elements of voice, compared with the potential ben-
their well-being aligns with hindrance appraisals of group voice. efits, when they experience the heightened attention to threats
Conversely, this sense of incurred harm to group functioning (e.g., associated with a low personal sense of power (Anderson &
Chamberlin et al., 2017) should reduce an evaluation of group Berdahl, 2002; Anderson & Galinsky, 2006; Morrison, 2014).
voice as fostering potential gains or enhancing well-being (i.e., Thus, we expect supervisors with a low personal sense of power to
challenge appraisals of group voice). have less favorable appraisals of group promotive and prohibitive
voice (i.e., weaker challenge appraisals of group promotive voice
Hypothesis 2: Group prohibitive voice will be (a) positively
and stronger hindrance appraisals of group prohibitive voice).
related to supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice and
(b) negatively related to supervisor challenge appraisals of
Hypothesis 3: A supervisor’s personal sense of power will
group voice.
moderate the relationship between group promotive voice and
supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice such that the
Supervisor Personal Sense of Power relationship is stronger (weaker) with a high (low) personal
sense of power.
The transactional theory of stress suggests that stress results
from work conditions that exceed an individual’s perception of
Hypothesis 4: A supervisor’s personal sense of power will
available personal resources (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). In line
moderate the relationship between group prohibitive voice and
with this emphasis on personal resources, we consider how super-
supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice such that the
visors’ appraisals of group voice are affected by their personal
relationship is weaker (stronger) with a high (low) personal
sense of power, a social-relational construct that refers to the
extent to which supervisors perceive influence over circumstances sense of power.
at work, including the attitudes, behaviors, and outcomes of others
(Anderson et al., 2012; Morrison, 2014; Morrison, See, & Pan, 2
A supervisor’s personal sense of power shares conceptual space with
2015). Applied to group voice, the extent to which supervisors other constructs from the stress literature, such as control or autonomy.
believe they have personal power, or can access personal resources However, as Morrison et al. (2015, p. 551) note, “unlike control at work or
to address group voice, is likely to influence how they appraise job autonomy, which reflects an employee’s perceived ability to influence
his or her own work behaviors and outcomes . . . sense of power is
group voice as challenging or hindering (Morrison & Rothman, inherently a social-relational concept: It reflects influence over other indi-
2009; Morrison et al., 2015; Tangirala & Ramanujam, 2008).2 viduals’ attitudes, behaviors, and outcomes.” We consider a supervisor’s
A high personal sense of power suggests a self-evaluation of personal sense of power because it aligns with the transactional theory of
sufficient personal resources to influence matters at work (Ander- stress, reflects the social nature of group voice, and provides a strong
theoretical framework for addressing supervisors’ orientation toward re-
son et al., 2012) and increased attention on potential rewards wards and threats (Anderson et al., 2012; Keltner et al., 2003). Thus, a
(Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). Supervisors with a high supervisor’s personal sense of power is better suited to our examination of
personal sense of power should perceive that they can manage supervisor reactions to group voice.
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 623

Supervisor Emotional Exhaustion overextended and helpless. Thus, supervisors will be unlikely to
achieve high levels of energetic activation and confidence (Quinn
A key outcome of Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) theorizing on et al., 2012). Therefore, we expect that supervisor hindrance ap-
stress and appraisals is perceived enhancements or threats to praisals of group voice will increase supervisors’ emotional ex-
well-being. Specifically, challenge and hindrance appraisals have haustion. In sum, group prohibitive voice will positively relate to
opposing effects on the extent to which individuals feel emotion- hindrance appraisals of group voice, which, in turn, increases
ally overwhelmed by their work (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). supervisors’ emotional exhaustion. Thus, we expect that group
Guided by this theorizing and our focus on the potential embedded prohibitive voice will have a positive indirect relationship with
costs of group voice for supervisors, we examine supervisor well- supervisor emotional exhaustion through hindrance appraisals of
being in terms of emotional exhaustion. Emotional exhaustion is group voice and that this positive indirect effect will be moderated
an impairment of psychological well-being that entails “feelings of by a supervisor’s personal sense of power.
being overextended and depleted of one’s emotional and physical
resources” (Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter, 2001, p. 399; Sonnentag, Hypothesis 5: Group promotive voice will (a) have a negative
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kuttler, & Fritz, 2010). We focus on emotional exhaustion (rather indirect effect on supervisor emotional exhaustion through
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

than the depersonalization or personal accomplishment facets of supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice, and (b) this
burnout) because emotional exhaustion is the facet that tends to indirect effect will be moderated in the first stage by a super-
have the strongest relationship with outcome variables and cap- visor’s personal sense of power such that the indirect effect
tures the core meaning of burnout (Cropanzano, Rupp, & Byrne, will be stronger (weaker) when their personal sense of power
2003; Lee & Ashforth, 1993, 1996; Wright & Bonett, 1997). is high (low).
As a subjective interpretation that work conditions will produce
personal gain, growth, development, and well-being (LePine et al., Hypothesis 6: Group prohibitive voice will (a) have a positive
2016), challenge appraisals of group voice are likely to reduce indirect effect on supervisor emotional exhaustion through
supervisors’ emotional exhaustion. This reasoning is grounded in supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice, and (b) this
theorizing from Lazarus and Folkman (1984, p. 34), who summa- indirect effect will be moderated in the first stage by a super-
rize the well-being outcomes of challenge appraisals compared to visor’s personal sense of power such that the indirect effect
hindrance appraisals as follows: will be weaker (stronger) when their personal sense of power
is high (low).
Challenged persons are likely to have better morale, because to be
challenged means feeling positive about demanding encounters, as
reflected in the pleasurable emotions accompanying challenge. The Supervisor Performance
quality of functioning is apt to be better in challenge because the
person feels more confident, less emotionally overwhelmed, and more Thus far, we have argued that the type of group voice and a
capable of drawing on available resources than the person who is supervisor’s personal sense of power will shape supervisors’ chal-
inhibited or blocked. lenge and hindrance appraisals of group voice and that these
appraisals will have differential effects on supervisor emotional
Consistent with their theorizing, challenge appraisals of work exhaustion. To extend our theorizing, we build on prior work that
conditions, such as group voice, are likely to result in enhanced has considered behavioral outcomes of the appraisal process (Bo-
work morale and confidence, positive emotions, and a resistance to swell, Olson-Buchanan, & LePine, 2004; LePine et al., 2016; Ohly
being emotionally overwhelmed. These outcomes further suggest & Fritz, 2010; Webster et al., 2011) to consider how supervisor
that challenge appraisals of group voice have an activating and emotional exhaustion relates to supervisor performance. Supervi-
morale-boosting effect that is antithetical to emotional exhaustion, sor performance is a particularly relevant outcome to organiza-
given that emotional exhaustion includes a perceived inability of tions, as it reflects supervisors’ contributions to day-to-day orga-
individuals to achieve high levels of energetic activation and a nizational operations, requires significant energy (Lanaj, Foulk, &
sense of helplessness (Quinn, Spreitzer, & Lam, 2012). Therefore, Erez, 2019), and has a cascading effect that influences employee
we expect that challenge appraisals of group voice will be nega- performance (Barling & Frone, 2017; Ten Brummelhuis, Haar, &
tively related to emotional exhaustion. To summarize, group pro- Roche, 2014).
motive voice should positively relate to challenge appraisals of We expect emotional exhaustion to be negatively related to
group voice, which, in turn, reduces supervisor emotional exhaus- supervisor performance given that emotionally exhausted individ-
tion. Thus, we expect that group promotive voice will have a uals have depleted energy and reduced wherewithal to address
negative indirect relationship with supervisor emotional exhaus- work responsibilities (Bakker et al., 2004; Sonnentag, 2015). Ad-
tion through challenge appraisals of group voice and that this ditionally, emotionally exhausted individuals have diminished
negative indirect effect will be moderated by a supervisor’s per- self-efficacy in solving work-related problems, which can create a
sonal sense of power. particularly deleterious effect on the performance of supervisors,
In contrast, hindrance appraisals capture individuals’ subjective who are charged with addressing problems within their work
interpretation that work conditions may result in personal loss, groups (Bakker, Demerouti, Taris, Schaufeli, & Schreurs, 2003).
constraints, or harm (LePine et al., 2016); diminished morale and Thus, emotionally exhausted supervisors should perform less ef-
confidence; negative emotions; and a tendency to be emotionally fectively due to depleted personal resources and reduced self-
overwhelmed by work conditions (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). The efficacy, and we extend our hypotheses to address the indirect
resulting negative perceptions of feeling blocked or inhibited by effects of group promotive and prohibitive voice on supervisor
group voice should increase the extent to which supervisors feel performance.
624 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

Hypothesis 7: Group promotive voice will (a) have a positive measure of supervisor performance (75% response rate). Using
indirect effect on supervisor performance through supervisor full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) path analysis in
challenge appraisals of group voice and supervisor emotional Mplus 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2015), we arrived at a final sample
exhaustion, and (b) this indirect effect will be moderated in the of 150 supervisors and 493 employees. One-way ANOVAs dem-
first stage by a supervisor’s personal sense of power such that onstrated that there were no significant differences in terms of age,
the indirect effect will be stronger (weaker) when their per- gender, organizational tenure, dyadic tenure, or work experience
sonal sense of power is high (low). for the sample of employees at Time 1 compared with Time 3 or
for the sample of supervisors at Time 2 compared with Time 3.
Hypothesis 8: Group prohibitive voice will (a) have a negative Measures. All measures were rated on a 5-point Likert scale
indirect effect on supervisor performance through supervisor from 1 ⫽ strongly disagree to 5 ⫽ strongly agree unless otherwise
hindrance appraisals of group voice and supervisor emotional
indicated.
exhaustion, and (b) this indirect effect will be moderated in the
Group promotive and prohibitive voice. Employees rated pro-
first stage by a supervisor’s personal sense of power such that
motive and prohibitive voice using Liang et al.’s (2012) 10-item
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the indirect effect will be weaker (stronger) when their per-


measure and were given the following lead-in instructions: “Items
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sonal sense of power is high (low).


on this page refer to your experience speaking up to your super-
visor about changes at work.” Example items of promotive voice
Study 1 are “I proactively voice out constructive suggestions that would
help my work group reach its goal” and “I make constructive
Method suggestions to improve my group’s operations” (␣ ⫽ .94). Exam-
ple items of prohibitive voice include: “I point out problems when
Sample and procedure. We tested our hypotheses in two they appear in the unit” and “I speak up honestly with problems
studies.3 We first examined our hypotheses in a sample of 150 that might cause serious loss to the work unit” (␣ ⫽ .89).
supervisors and 493 employees at a large university in the United For both group promotive and prohibitive voice, we used an
States. Our employee sample was 60% female and participants, on additive model derived by averaging lower-level scores (Chan,
average, were 39-years-old (SD ⫽ 12.42) and had been with the 1998). In our case, group-level voice is the average of employee
university for 5 years (SD ⫽ 6.72). Our supervisor sample was ratings of voice working under a given supervisor, which is con-
54% female and participants, on average, were 44-years-old (SD ⫽ sistent with prior research on group voice (Lam & Mayer, 2014).
9.33) and had been with the university for 11 years (SD ⫽ 8.39). Our ICCs and mean rwg values were acceptable; promotive voice:
The average supervisor– employee relationship tenure was 2.39
ICC(1) ⫽ .06, p ⫽ .05, ICC(2) ⫽ .22, mean rwg ⫽ .84 (SD ⫽ .24);
years (SD ⫽ 3.29), and the average work group size was 3.29
prohibitive voice: ICC(1) ⫽ .07, p ⫽ .04, ICC(2) ⫽ .23, mean
employees (SD ⫽ 1.70).
rwg ⫽ .82 (SD ⫽ .22; James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984; LeBreton &
Our sample drew from administrative and support staff from
Senter, 2008). These results suggest that the aggregation of group
two colleges and two supporting business units within the univer-
member voice to the group level is appropriate. Given research that
sity. The supervisors in our sample had a large degree of diversity
demonstrates that centralization of voice in teams has important
in their roles in that they were comprised of supervisors from
implications for team performance (e.g., Sherf, Sinha, Tangirala,
enrollment management (N ⫽ 50), the university technology office
& Awasty, 2018), we conducted additional analyses to examine
(N ⫽ 46), the business school (N ⫽ 43), and the honors college
whether the amount of variance in group voice affects supervisor
(N ⫽ 11). In our initial conversations with the leadership of these
appraisals of group voice. High variance in group voice suggests a
separate offices, it was clear that supervisors received both ideas
“squeaky wheel” who speaks up more than others, which could
and suggestions (promotive voice) as well as expressions of con-
negatively affect a supervisor’s appraisals of group voice. We
cern and problems (prohibitive voice), though the specific subject
examined whether rwg and within-group standard deviation of
matter of the voice messages varied by department. The diversity
in the roles of supervisors in our sample increases the generaliz- voice moderated the relationships between group voice and ap-
ability of our findings, and we account for this variation across praisals of group voice but found no moderating effects or differ-
units methodologically through multilevel modeling (Kerlinger & ences in our pattern of results when modeled as controls. These
Lee, 2000). checks support our emphasis on mean levels of group voice rather
To reduce common method bias, we separated our variables by than within-group variation.
time and source by collecting data from employees and supervisors Challenge and hindrance appraisals of group voice.
in three waves that were each approximately 1 month apart (Doty Supervisors rated challenge appraisals of group voice and hin-
& Glick, 1998; Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). drance appraisals of group voice using the three-item measures
We surveyed employees at Time 1, supervisors at Time 2, and both developed by LePine et al. (2016). The items were adapted to
employees and supervisors at Time 3. We provided incentives to capture supervisors’ appraisals of constructive voice from their
participants through drawings for gift cards prizes, ranging from work groups. The following survey lead-in instructions were pro-
$10 to $50. At Time 1, 616 employees completed measures of vided: “The items on this page refer to your experience when your
promotive and prohibitive voice. At Time 2, 193 supervisors group of employees speak up with ideas and problems to change
completed measures of challenge appraisals and hindrance ap-
praisals of group voice, and supervisors’ personal sense of power. 3
We received IRB approval from Arizona State University for our two
At Time 3, 170 supervisors completed a measure of emotional studies (IRB #00004919, titled “Voice Fatigue”; IRB #00007942, titled
exhaustion (88% response rate), and 464 employees completed a “Understanding Supervisor Reactions to Employee Voice”).
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 625

things at work.” Sample items of challenge appraisals of group Supervisor performance. Supervisor performance was mea-
voice are: “Working to fulfill the demands of listening to employee sured with a four-item measure developed by Liden, Wayne, and
ideas improves my personal growth and well-being” and “I feel Stilwell (1993) and subsequently adapted by Nahrgang, Morgeson,
that listening to employee ideas promotes my ability to work and Ilies (2009). Employees rated their supervisor’s performance
toward my personal accomplishment” (␣ ⫽ .79). Sample items of with items such as: “This team leader performs very well” and
hindrance appraisals of group voice are: “Working to fulfill the “This team leader is very effective” (␣ ⫽ .97). The ICCs were
demands of listening to employee ideas gets in the way of my acceptable (ICC(1) ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .001, ICC(2) ⫽ .51), and the mean
personal growth and well-being” and “I feel that listening to rwg value was .85 (SD ⫽ .26), which indicates a high level of
employee ideas hinders my ability to work toward my personal uniformity (James et al., 1984; LeBreton & Senter, 2008).
accomplishment” (␣ ⫽ .91). Control variables. We controlled for group size given that an
Supervisor personal sense of power. Supervisors responded increased number of employees engaging in voice likely influ-
to the 10-item measure of a personal sense of power from Morri- ences supervisors’ challenge and hindrance appraisals of voice.
son, See, and Pan (2015) on a 5-point scale (1 ⫽ not at all to 5 ⫽ Given that supervisors may experience other work conditions
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a great deal). The lead-in to the items was: “At work, I am . . .” besides voice, we also controlled for general job demands using
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The items are “powerful,” “influential,” “in-control,” “dominant,” four items from Karasek’s (1979) measure (␣ ⫽ .77). We exam-
“in-charge,” “independent,” “action-oriented,” “weak,” “depen- ined the relevance of several other control variables—supervisor
dent,” and “powerless” (the last three items were reverse coded; age, gender, race, and tenure with the organization—as research
␣ ⫽ .80). has shown that demographics may influence stress, emotional
Supervisor emotional exhaustion. Supervisors rated their emo- exhaustion, and performance (Bonanno, Galea, Bucciarelli, & Vla-
tional exhaustion using the nine-item measure from Maslach and hov, 2007; Wright & Bonett, 1997). Analyses revealed that none of
Jackson (1993; ␣ ⫽ .92). We used the emotional exhaustion facet these demographic control variables significantly altered our re-
from the burnout scale, rather than overall burnout, depersonaliza- sults. Overall, inclusion of these demographic controls strength-
tion, personal accomplishment or cognitive depletion, because
ened our effects, but our pattern of results was unchanged if these
emotional exhaustion captures the essence of burnout and, as such,
variables were not modeled. Based on recommendations to ex-
tends to have the strongest relationship with outcome variables
clude controls when they do not change the results of the analysis
(Cropanzano et al., 2003; Lee & Ashforth, 1993, 1996; Wright &
(Becker, 2005; Carlson & Wu, 2012), we did not retain any
Bonett, 1997).
demographic controls in our final model in order to maximize the
A helpful reviewer raised the point that our measures of chal-
ratio of study participants to parameters (Cohen, 1992).
lenge and hindrance appraisals of group voice use the word “well-
being,” which could be confounded with our measure of emotional
exhaustion. We note that the measures we used for challenge and Analyses and Results
hindrance appraisals (LePine et al., 2016) and emotional exhaus-
tion (Maslach & Jackson, 1993) have been previously validated Descriptive statistics, correlations, and reliabilities are shown in
and published. We conducted a principal components factor anal- Table 1. We verified the factor structure of our model in a
ysis with varimax rotation of the three challenge appraisals of multilevel confirmatory factor analysis using Mplus 7.4 (Muthén
group voice items, the three hindrance appraisals of group voice & Muthén, 2015). The nine-factor measurement model consisted
items, and the nine emotional exhaustion items. The factor analysis of group promotive voice, group prohibitive voice, supervisor
revealed three factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 that ex- personal sense of power, supervisor challenge appraisals of group
plained 70.6% of the variance. The largest cross-loading of the voice, supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice, supervisor
challenge appraisals of group voice and hindrance appraisals group emotional exhaustion, supervisor performance, supervisor job de-
voice items on the emotional exhaustion factor was .14. These mands, and group size. The model exhibited acceptable fit,
findings, when combined with validation efforts conducted by ␹2(470) ⫽ 1019.42, p ⬍ .001; CFI ⫽ .93; RMSEA ⫽ .05; SRMR
previous research, give us confidence the measures are not con- between ⫽ .11; SRMR within ⫽ .03. All factor loadings were
founded. significant.

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics, Correlations, and Reliabilities (Study 1)

Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Group promotive voice 3.53 .59 (.94)


2. Group prohibitive voice 3.08 .53 .55ⴱⴱ (.89)
3. Challenge appraisals of group voice 4.00 .53 .23ⴱⴱ ⫺.01 (.79)
4. Hindrance appraisals of group voice 1.70 .56 ⫺.17ⴱ .08 ⫺.46ⴱⴱ (.91)
5. Supervisor personal sense of power 3.66 .54 .12ⴱ .02 .15 ⫺.21ⴱⴱ (.80)
6. Supervisor emotional exhaustion 2.36 .82 ⫺.03 .02 ⫺.17ⴱⴱ .14 ⫺.36ⴱⴱ (.92)
7. Supervisor performance 3.91 1.25 .10 .01 ⫺.06 ⫺.04 ⫺.07 ⫺.11ⴱⴱ (.97)
8. Group size 3.29 1.70 .12ⴱ .04 ⫺.03 .00 .01 ⫺.13ⴱⴱ .10ⴱ —
9. Job demands 4.07 .62 ⫺.02 ⫺.08 .16 ⫺.21 ⫺.06 .38ⴱⴱ .00 ⫺.05 (.77)
Note. N ⫽ 150.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
626 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

To consider the interactions in our model, we first mean cen- p ⬍ .001) and negatively related to hindrance appraisals of group
tered our predictors to remove nonessential multicollinearity be- voice (b ⫽ ⫺.25, SE ⫽ .12, p ⫽ .03). Hypothesis 2a predicted that
tween the variables and their product terms (Cohen, Cohen, West, group prohibitive voice would be positively related to supervisor
& Aiken, 2003; Cortina, Chen, & Dunlap, 2001). To test the hindrance appraisals of group voice, and Hypothesis 2b predicted
hypothesized indirect effects, we also modeled the direct effects of that group prohibitive voice would be negatively related to super-
group promotive and prohibitive voice on supervisor emotional visor challenge appraisals of group voice. As shown in Figure 1,
exhaustion and supervisor performance (e.g., MacKinnon, Lock- group prohibitive voice was positively related to supervisor hin-
wood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002). Researchers advocate drance appraisals of group voice (b ⫽ .22, SE ⫽ .09, p ⫽ .02) and
testing indirect effects using resampling methods because the negatively related to supervisor challenge appraisals of group
products of path coefficients are not normally distributed (Mac- voice (b ⫽ ⫺.16, SE ⫽ .04, p ⬍ .001). Thus, Hypotheses 2a and
Kinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, 2004). Accordingly, we tested 2b were also supported.
our indirect effects with the Monte Carlo method using the RMe- Hypothesis 3 predicted that a supervisor’s personal sense of
diation software package (Tofighi & MacKinnon, 2011). More- power would moderate the relationship between group promotive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

over, we examined conditional indirect effects by testing for a voice and challenge appraisals of group voice. We did not find
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

significant difference between the high and low conditions of a support for this hypothesis as the interaction term was not signif-
supervisor’s personal sense of power for group promotive and icant (b ⫽ ⫺.07, SE ⫽ .06, p ⫽ .27). Hypothesis 4 predicted that
prohibitive voice to supervisor emotional exhaustion and perfor- a supervisor’s personal sense of power would moderate the posi-
mance through supervisor challenge and hindrance appraisals of tive relationship between group prohibitive voice and hindrance
group voice. Because supervisors in our sample belong to different appraisals of group voice. As shown in Figure 2, the interaction
colleges and business units within the university, there is the between group prohibitive voice and a supervisor’s personal sense
potential for nonindependence of observations due to systematic of power on hindrance appraisals of voice was significant
differences (Bliese, 2000). To account for these differences, we ran (b ⫽ ⫺.21, SE ⫽ .08, p ⫽ .01). We plotted and tested simple
a two-level model in which we clustered on supervisors’ organi- slopes for a high and low group (i.e., plus or minus one standard
zational unit. This approach derives clustered standard errors, deviation) to examine the conditional effects of group prohibitive
which provides a more accurate and conservative test of predic- voice on hindrance appraisals of group voice (Cohen et al., 2003).
tions. All other variables were modeled at the within-level. Our The positive relationship between group prohibitive voice and
final model presented acceptable fit to the data: ␹2(19) ⫽ 39.13, supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice was weaker and not
p ⫽ .004; CFI ⫽ .90; RMSEA ⫽ .08; SRMR ⫽ .06. significant when personal sense of power was high (b ⫽ .11, SE ⫽
Hypothesis 1a predicted that group promotive voice would be .10, p ⫽ .31), but stronger and significant when personal sense of
positively related to supervisor challenge appraisals of group power was low (b ⫽ .33, SE ⫽ .10, p ⫽ .001), supporting
voice, and Hypothesis 1b predicted that group promotive voice Hypothesis 4.
would be negatively related to supervisor hindrance appraisals of Hypothesis 5a predicted that group promotive voice would have
group voice. As shown in Figure 1, both Hypotheses 1a and 1b a negative indirect effect on supervisor emotional exhaustion
were supported, as group promotive voice was positively related to through supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice, and Hy-
supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice (b ⫽ .28, SE ⫽ .06, pothesis 5b predicted that this relationship would be moderated by

Time 2
(Supervisor Reported)

Supervisor
Personal Sense of
Power
Time 1
(Employee Reported) -.07 -.21*
Supervisor
Group .28** Challenge
Promotive Voice Appraisals of Time 3
Time 3
Group Voice -.28**
** (Supervisor Reported) (Employee Reported)
-.16
Supervisor -.26** Supervisor
**
.17 -.10** Emotional
Performance
Exhaustion
-.25*
.24**
Supervisor
Group Hindrance
Prohibitive Voice .22* Appraisals of
Group Voice

Figure 1. Path model of the differential effects of group promotive and prohibitive voice on supervisor
appraisals of group voice, supervisor emotional exhaustion, and supervisor performance (Study 1). Paths from
group promotive voice and group prohibitive voice to supervisor emotional exhaustion and supervisor perfor-
mance were modeled because they are necessary for testing our indirect and conditional indirect effects. Only
hypothesized relationships are depicted. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01, two-tailed.
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 627

Hypothesis 7a predicted that group promotive voice would have


a positive serial indirect effect on supervisor performance through
supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice and emotional
exhaustion, and Hypothesis 7b predicted that supervisors’ personal
sense of power would moderate this serial indirect effect. We note
that the model explained 6.6% of variance in supervisor perfor-
mance. Supervisor emotional exhaustion was negatively related to
supervisor performance (b ⫽ ⫺.26, SE ⫽ .07, p ⬍ .001), and the
serial indirect effect from Hypothesis 7a was supported (indirect
effect ⫽ .02; 95% CI [.005, .038]). Given that a supervisor’s
personal sense of power did not moderate the relationship between
group promotive voice and supervisor challenge appraisals of
group voice, we did not test Hypothesis 7b.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hypothesis 8a predicted that group prohibitive voice would


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

have a negative serial indirect effect on supervisor performance


Figure 2. Interaction of supervisor personal sense of power and group through supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice and
prohibitive voice on supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice emotional exhaustion, and Hypothesis 8b predicted that super-
(Study 1). visors’ personal sense of power would moderate this serial
indirect effect. The serial indirect effect put forward by Hy-
pothesis 8a was supported (indirect effect ⫽ ⫺.01; 95% CI
[⫺.019, ⫺.002]). Regarding Hypothesis 8b, we found that the
a supervisor’s personal sense of power. Although the specific link serial indirect effect between group prohibitive voice and su-
was not formally hypothesized, we found that challenge appraisals pervisor performance voice was weaker and not significant
of group voice had a negative relationship with emotional exhaus- when personal sense of power was high (indirect effect ⫽ ⫺.01;
tion (b ⫽ ⫺.28, SE ⫽ .10, p ⫽ .01). We formally tested our 95% CI [⫺.015, .005]) and stronger and significant when
hypothesized indirect effects with the product of coefficients ap- personal sense of power was low (indirect effect ⫽ ⫺.02; 95%
proach (MacKinnon et al., 2002) and found support for the nega- CI [⫺.028, ⫺.006]). Importantly, the difference in the indirect
tive indirect effect between group promotive voice and supervisor effects at high and low levels of personal sense of power was
emotional exhaustion through supervisor challenge appraisals of significant (⌬ indirect effect ⫽ .01; 95% CI [.007, .025]). Thus,
group voice (indirect effect ⫽ ⫺.08; 95% CI [⫺.100, ⫺.032]). Hypotheses 8a and 8b were both supported.
Given that a supervisor’s personal sense of power did not moderate We also conducted a supplemental analysis. In addition to
the relationship between group promotive voice and supervisor controlling for the effects of group size in our primary analysis, we
challenge appraisals of group voice, we did not test conditional examined whether group size influenced the direction or strength
indirect effects for this pathway. Thus, Hypothesis 5a was sup- of the relationship between group promotive and prohibitive voice
ported and Hypothesis 5b was not. and supervisor appraisals of group voice. The interaction of group
Hypothesis 6a predicted that group prohibitive voice would promotive voice and group size on challenge appraisals of group
have a positive indirect effect on supervisor emotional exhaus- voice was significant (b ⫽ ⫺.04, SE ⫽ .01, p ⬍ .001). The plot of
tion through supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice, this interaction indicates that group promotive voice had a positive
and Hypothesis 6b predicted that this relationship would be relationship with challenge appraisals of group voice at high levels
moderated by a supervisor’s personal sense of power. Although of group size and an even stronger positive relationship at low
not formally hypothesized, we found that supervisor hindrance levels of group size. The interaction of group prohibitive voice and
appraisals of group voice had a positive relationship with su- group size on hindrance appraisals of group voice was also sig-
pervisor emotional exhaustion (b ⫽ .24, SE ⫽ .07, p ⬍ .001). nificant (b ⫽ .05, SE ⫽ .02, p ⫽ .001). The plot of this interaction
To test the indirect effects, we again used the product of shows that group prohibitive voice had a positive relationship with
coefficients approach and found support for the positive indi- hindrance appraisals of group voice at high levels of group size
rect effect between group prohibitive voice and supervisor and a weaker, but still positive, relationship at low levels of group
emotional exhaustion through supervisor hindrance appraisals size. Although group size moderates these two relationships, we
of group voice (indirect effect ⫽ .05; 95% CI [.013, .059]). still found a positive relationship between group promotive voice
Thus, Hypothesis 6a was supported. We also found that the and challenge appraisals of group voice in both high and low group
indirect effect between group prohibitive voice and supervisor size, which is in line with our theorizing. Further, in accordance
emotional exhaustion voice was weaker and nonsignificant with our theorizing, group prohibitive voice also exhibited a pos-
when personal sense of power was high (indirect effect ⫽ .03; itive relationship with hindrance appraisals of group voice in
95% CI [⫺.034, .050]) and stronger and significant when conditions of high and low group size (although the simple slope
personal sense of power was low (indirect effect ⫽ .08; 95% CI for the relationship between group prohibitive voice and hindrance
[.043, .089]). As an indication of moderated mediation, the appraisals of group voice at low group size was positive but only
difference in the indirect effects at high and low levels of significant for a one-tailed test). Importantly, with the inclusion of
personal sense of power was significant (⌬ indirect ef- group size and the associated interaction terms, we found that our
fect ⫽ ⫺.05; 95% CI [⫺.101, ⫺.007]). Thus, Hypothesis 6b main effects outlined in Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 2a, and 2b all remained
was also supported. significant.
628 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

Discussion Forty-nine percent of the sample was female. On average, partic-


ipants spent 34 min (SD ⫽ 8.80) on the exercise.
In Study 1, we tested our model across a sample of supervisors After welcoming participants, the experimenter informed par-
with diverse work roles. We found that group promotive voice had
ticipants that they would complete a scenario that evaluates how
a negative indirect effect on supervisor emotional exhaustion
they manage workflow. Participants were randomly assigned to
through challenge appraisals of group voice and had a positive
either a high or low power condition. Personal power was manip-
indirect effect on supervisor performance through challenge ap-
ulated using the exercise developed by Galinsky, Gruenfeld, and
praisals of group voice and emotional exhaustion. We did not,
Magee (2003) wherein participants spent at least 2 min describing
however, find support for supervisors’ personal sense of power
a situation in which they experienced high or low power (e.g.,
moderating the relationship between group promotive voice and
Morrison et al., 2015).4 We then presented a scenario to partici-
supervisor challenge appraisals of group voice. Thus, supervisors
pants that asked them to assume the role of Jamie Peters, the
may positively view the experience of receiving group promotive
supervisor of a sales team of six employees responsible for selling
voice irrespective of their sense of power. Further, we found that
printers and office supplies. Participants were provided with an
group prohibitive voice had a positive indirect effect on supervisor
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

emotional exhaustion through hindrance appraisals of group voice organizational chart that displayed a sales director overseeing their
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and a negative indirect effect on supervisor performance through role and the names of each of their employees. We informed
hindrance appraisals of group voice and emotional exhaustion. We participants that it was Monday morning, they had just arrived at
also found support for the moderating role of a supervisor’s work after the weekend, and they were about to check their e-mail.
personal sense of power on the relationship between group pro- We provided these details to establish background information
hibitive voice and hindrance appraisals of group voice as well as about the scenario in preparation for our voice manipulations.
the related conditional indirect effects. Thus, supervisors appear to Following the power manipulation and summary of the scenario,
be more positively appraise problems and concerns in cases of participants were presented with a series of e-mails from their
high power but appraise group prohibitive voice as more hindering inbox and asked to review each one. Participants reviewed 10
in nature when they feel a diminished sense of power. messages, beginning with a review of Jamie’s most recent e-mail
Although Study 1 has numerous strengths, certain limitations to the team. In this e-mail, Jamie sent an update to the team that
exist. For example, we can make limited inferences about causal- their “performance was about the same as the first quarter of 2018”
ity, although our integration of existing theory and time separation and “of the five sales teams at the company, [they were] right in
of our variables provide some support for causality. Our findings the middle of the pack (third of five).” Participants reviewed this
may also be confounded by situational factors and individual message first to provide context for the remaining nine e-mails.
differences. Given these limitations, we examined the core rela- This message did not invite group member responses in order to
tionships in our model in a controlled environment (Study 2). reflect the discretionary nature of voice (Van Dyne & LePine,
Using an experimental study design, we manipulated group pro- 1998).
motive and prohibitive voice as well as participants’ personal We randomly assigned participants to one of two group voice
sense of power in order to provide further support to our Study 1 conditions: group promotive voice or group prohibitive voice. In
findings by making a stronger argument for causality, mitigating the group promotive voice condition, participants received five
situational confounds, and addressing individual differences through e-mail messages containing promotive voice, and in the group
random assignment across conditions. prohibitive voice condition, participants received five e-mail mes-
sages containing prohibitive voice (see Table 2). By holding the
Study 2 message content constant across the two voice conditions, we
sought to focus our manipulation explicitly on the type of voice
(e.g., McClean, Martin, Emich, & Woodruff, 2018). The first
Method message for both group voice conditions was adapted from Mc-
Sample and procedure. Our Study 2 sample was comprised Clean, Martin, Emich, and Woodruff (2018). The remaining four
of 135 undergraduate students, who were offered course credit to messages for the group voice conditions were developed for this
participate in an in-person lab study. Scholars have reasoned that study and interspersed among four additional messages. The four
lab studies “involving undergraduate participants that occur in an additional messages were of similar length to the group voice
environment that was created for research purposes” can advance messages and closely paralleled the structure of the other mes-
theory by effectively ruling out alternative explanations and em- sages. Topics of these messages included an invitation to help with
ploying random assignment and effective manipulations (Colquitt, a new-employee orientation, a notice about paving the parking lot,
2008, p. 616). In our study, we randomly assigned participants to an announcement about a security training, and a reminder that
one of four conditions: 2 Group Voice Conditions (Group Promo-
tive or Group Prohibitive Voice) ⫻ 2 Power Conditions (High or 4
The instructions for the power manipulation were as follows (Galinsky,
Low Power). Participants in our study completed an adapted Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003): “Before you read the scenario, please recall a
“in-basket exercise” (e.g., Desai & Kouchaki, 2017; Gibson, 1999) particular instance in which [you had power over another individual or
in which participants act as a supervisor processing different items individuals/someone else had power over you]. By power, we mean a
that have accumulated in his or her “in-basket”—in this study, 10 situation in which [you controlled the ability of another person or persons/
someone had control over your ability] to get something [they/you] wanted,
e-mails that had come into the supervisor’s inbox over the week- or [were in a position to evaluate those individuals/was in a position to
end. Participants were an average age of 22-years-old (SD ⫽ 2.63) evaluate you]. Please describe this situation in which you [had power/did
and had an average of 1.52 years of work experience (SD ⫽ 2.63). not have power]—what happened, how you felt, etc.”
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 629

Table 2
Adapted In-Basket Exercise: Group Voice Conditions (Study 2)

Issue Condition Message

Sales team script Group promotive I think that we should come up with a new and improved script in order to give us more flexibility in
voice meeting customer needs going forward. My idea is for a new script that could include much more
leeway around add-on products for our customers so that we can better meet their needs in the
future. I think a new script will help all of us do better (McClean et al., 2018).
Group prohibitive I think that we should get rid of our ineffective script because it has restricted our flexibility in
voice meeting customer needs in the past. My concern is that this script makes it much harder to add
products on for our customers and is harming our ability to meet their needs right now. I think
getting rid of the script will fix the harm done (McClean et al., 2018).
Bonus system Group promotive We should develop a new bonus system to better encourage our team. A new bonus system would
voice better align with our current customers and levels of sales. We would have more incentive to try
for our sales targets because we would be confident we can reach them. As a result, our team
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

would feel better about our work. Looking forward, I think it would help our morale.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Group prohibitive We should get rid of our outdated bonus structure because it’s discouraging our team. Our current
voice bonus system is out of touch with our current customer and levels of sales. We have had no
incentive to keep trying for higher sales targets because we doubt we can reach them. As a result,
our team has felt bad about our work. Looking back, I worry it has hurt our morale.
Customers Group promotive What if we began pursuing sales to bigger companies? Bigger companies tend to spend more on our
voice products than smaller companies. I think selling to bigger companies is an opportunity because it
would mean less work for more benefit to our sales in upcoming quarters. My idea is to pursue
sales to bigger companies to improve our performance.
Group prohibitive What if we stop pursuing sales to smaller companies? Smaller companies tend to spend less on our
voice products than bigger companies. I think selling to smaller companies is a problem because it has
meant more work for little benefit to our sales in prior quarters. My concern is that our pursuit of
sales to smaller companies has hurt our performance.
Suppliers Group promotive An opportunity I see relates to our suppliers. We could shift business to our best suppliers because
voice they do a reliable job of getting products to our customers. Working more with these good
suppliers could make us look good to our customers because they deliver on time. This would
improve the image of our company.
Group prohibitive A problem I see relates to our suppliers. We have relied on some poor suppliers that have done an
voice unreliable job of getting products to our customers. Working with these poor suppliers has made us
look bad to our customers because they deliver late. This has harmed the image of our company.
Work environment Group promotive We could make the shared space where our desks are located quieter, which would help our
voice concentration. We could set aside an office for people to make and take phone calls. This would
also improve privacy in the group, which would also help us to get along better. Working in a
quieter area would help morale and productivity in the group.
Group prohibitive The shared space where our desks are located is too noisy, which has been distracting. People make
voice and take calls in that space, and we all have to listen to them. This has also created privacy
problems, which have led to interpersonal disputes. Having to work in such a noisy area has hurt
morale and productivity in the group.

midyear evaluations were coming up. These messages represent the exercise was intended to encourage participants to process the
common issues that a supervisor may face as general challenge and e-mails from their group rather than as a performance measure,
hindrance demands. At the end of the adapted in-basket exercise, given that there is not one “best” way to respond.
participants were asked to write a response to their group about
their input.
Analyses and Results
Measures. After completing the adapted in-basket exercise,
participants responded to several outcome measures. We used the We coded the group voice conditions (1 ⫽ group promotive
same measures of challenge appraisals of group voice (␣ ⫽ .79) voice, 0 ⫽ group prohibitive voice) and personal sense of power
and hindrance appraisals of group voice (␣ ⫽ .90) as Study 1 conditions (1 ⫽ high, 0 ⫽ low) for our analyses. We used analysis
(LePine et al., 2016). These items referred directly to the group’s of variance (ANOVA) to examine our manipulation checks and the
comments in the adapted in-basket exercise (e.g., “working to differences across the manipulated conditions in our experimental
fulfill the demands of managing employee comments [improves/ study. We examined our indirect effects and conditional indirect
gets in the way] of my personal growth and well-being”). We also effects using Hayes’s (2012) PROCESS macro (v2.16.3) in SPSS
used the same nine-item measure of emotional exhaustion as Study (Version 24). This approach utilizes bias-corrected bootstrapped
1 (Maslach & Jackson, 1993). However, we asked participants confidence intervals (5,000 bootstrap samples) to estimate indirect
how they feel “right now” as a lead-in to this measure and removed effects as well as conditional indirect effects, while controlling for
wording that referred to the participant’s job to make the items the direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent
applicable to the participant’s emotional exhaustion after complet- variable.
ing the adapted in-basket exercise (␣ ⫽ .96). Although participants Manipulation checks. We measured the extent to which par-
composed an e-mail in response to their group’s voice, this part of ticipants, acting in the role of supervisors, experienced a personal
630 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

sense of power using the same 10-item measure from Morrison et supported. Hypothesis 4 predicted that a personal sense of power
al. (2015) used in Study 1. Participants responded to the prompt, would moderate the relationship between group prohibitive voice
“Right now, I feel . . .” followed by the measure (1 ⫽ not at all to and hindrance appraisals of group voice. As shown in Figure 3c,
5 ⫽ a great deal; ␣ ⫽ .89). An ANOVA supported a strong participants in the group prohibitive voice condition rated hin-
positive main effect for the power manipulation (F ⫽ 33.86, p ⬍ drance appraisals in the low power condition significantly higher
.001, Mean ⫽ 3.34 vs. 4.06). We also confirmed that the manip- than they did in the high power condition (Mean ⫽ 2.74 vs. 2.26;
ulations of the group voice conditions were effective using the F ⫽ 3.97, p ⫽ .048). Thus, Hypothesis 4 was supported.
lead-in “Think about your team’s comments to you. My team . . .” As predicted by Hypothesis 5a, we found a negative indirect
followed by two options: “Expressed opportunities and ideas to effect between the group promotive voice condition and emotional
me” and “Expressed problems and concerns to me.” For our group exhaustion through challenge appraisals of group voice compared
voice conditions, 126 out of 135 participants correctly responded to the group prohibitive voice condition (indirect effect ⫽ ⫺.25;
to our manipulation check (93.3%), and our results remained the 95% CI [⫺.458, ⫺.078]). Hypothesis 5b predicted that this indi-
same whether or not we included 126 or 135 participants. Given rect effect would be moderated by personal sense of power. We did
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the consistency of these results, we retained all 135 participants in not test this conditional indirect effect given the nonsignificant
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

our final sample for the sake of maximizing the ratio of study interaction between the group promotive voice condition and per-
participants to parameters (Cohen, 1992). sonal sense of power. Thus, Hypothesis 5a was supported, and
Hypotheses testing. Descriptive statistics and correlations for Hypothesis 5b was not supported.
our variables are shown in Table 3. Hypothesis 1a predicted that In support of Hypothesis 6a, we found a significant positive
group promotive voice would be positively related to challenge indirect effect between the group prohibitive voice condition and
appraisals of group voice, and Hypothesis 1b predicted that group emotional exhaustion through hindrance appraisals of group voice
promotive voice would be negatively related to hindrance apprais- compared with the group promotive voice condition (indirect
als of group voice. Hypothesis 2a predicted that group prohibitive effect ⫽ .50; 95% CI [.312, .744]). In accordance with Hypothesis
voice would be positively related to hindrance appraisals of group 6b, we then tested the indirect effect between the group prohibitive
voice, and Hypothesis 2b predicted that group prohibitive voice voice condition and emotional exhaustion through hindrance ap-
would be negatively related to challenge appraisals of group voice. praisals of group voice at high and low levels of personal sense of
Figures 3a and 3b show the means for challenge appraisals of power. We found the indirect effect to be weaker in the high
group voice and hindrance appraisals of group voice across the personal sense of power condition (indirect effect ⫽ .33; 95% CI
group voice conditions. Challenge appraisals of group voice were [.111, .571]) and stronger in the low personal sense of power
rated higher in the group promotive voice condition compared with condition (indirect effect ⫽ .67; 95% CI [.371, 1.035]) compared
the group prohibitive voice condition (Mean ⫽ 4.35 vs. 3.70; F ⫽ with the group promotive voice condition. We found that the
29.77, p ⬍ .001). Conversely, hindrance appraisals of group voice difference between these conditional indirect effects in the high
were rated lower in the group promotive voice condition compared and low conditions of personal sense of power was also significant
to the group prohibitive voice condition (Mean ⫽ 1.68 vs. 2.50; (⌬ indirect effect ⫽ .34; 95% CI [.021, .769]). Thus, Hypotheses
F ⫽ 33.54, p ⬍ .001). These findings provide support for Hypoth- 6a and 6b were supported. We note that the model explained
eses 1a, 1b, 2a, and 2b. 37.8% of variance in emotional exhaustion.
In regards to supervisors’ personal sense of power, we did not
find a significant main effect for personal sense of power on either
Discussion
challenge appraisals of group voice, F ⫽ 1.06, p ⫽ .31 or hin-
drance appraisals of group voice, F ⫽ 1.99, p ⫽ .16. Hypothesis In our experimental study, we manipulated group promotive and
3 predicted that a personal sense of power would moderate the prohibitive voice as well as one’s personal sense of power. Con-
relationship between group promotive voice and challenge ap- sistent with Study 1, we found further support for the positive
praisals of group voice. Participants in the group promotive voice effects of group promotive voice on challenge appraisals of group
condition did not rate challenge appraisals differently in the low voice and the positive effects of group prohibitive voice on hin-
power condition compared to the high power condition (Mean ⫽ drance appraisals of group voice. In addition, we found that a
4.40 vs. 4.31; F ⫽ 3.29, p ⫽ .07). Thus, Hypothesis 3 was not personal sense of power moderated the relationship between group

Table 3
Descriptive Statistics, Correlations, and Reliabilities (Study 2)

Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5

1. Group voice condition — — —


2. Personal sense of power condition — — .02 —
3. Challenge appraisals of group voice 4.05 .77 .43ⴱⴱ .08 (.79)
4. Hindrance appraisals of group voice 2.06 .92 ⫺.45ⴱⴱ ⫺.11 ⫺.48ⴱⴱ (.90)
5. Emotional exhaustion 2.08 .98 ⫺.34ⴱⴱ ⫺.11 ⫺.39ⴱⴱ .61ⴱⴱ (.96)
Note. N ⫽ 135 (group promotive/high power ⫽ 37; group promotive/low power ⫽ 35; group prohibitive/high power ⫽ 31; group prohibitive/low power ⫽
32). Group voice conditions coded 1 ⫽ group promotive voice, 0 ⫽ group prohibitive voice; personal sense of power condition coded 1 ⫽ high power,
0 ⫽ low power.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01.
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 631

voice could be appraised as both a challenge and a hindrance.


Although theory suggests this is unlikely given the generally
negative relationship between challenge and hindrance appraisals
(see Footnote 1), we see value in separately exploring the effects
of group promotive and prohibitive voice on supervisor appraisals
of group voice. Overall, Study 2 provides further support for our
theorizing and complements the external validity of our field study
(Study 1) by offering high internal validity.

General Discussion

Theoretical Implications
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Our work makes several theoretical contributions. First, we


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

offer more focused consideration of the role of the supervisor in


the group voice process. In this regard, our model represents
“learning more by crossing levels” (Hackman, 2003, p. 905) as we
consider how voice, generally extolled as beneficial for employ-
ees, work groups, and organizations (e.g., Li et al., 2017; Mac-
Kenzie et al., 2011; Morrison, 2014), has mixed effects at the
supervisor level. Specifically, we demonstrate that supervisors are
personally affected by group voice as they appraise it as a work
condition that can potentially promote or hinder their growth and
well-being. This finding is a key point for the voice literature as
supervisors are most often the targets of voice and serve as key
facilitators for the implementation of subordinate voice (Burris,
Detert, & Chiaburu, 2008; Detert & Burris, 2007; Liu et al., 2013;
Tangirala & Ramanujam, 2012). Thus, although blanket recom-
mendations that group members freely speak up to their managers
with suggestions and concerns have value because voice, in the
aggregate, helps make the work group and organization more
effective, such recommendations are also laden with previously
unarticulated costs for supervisors who can become emotionally
exhausted by group voice.
Relatedly, although promotive and prohibitive voice are both
considered constructive forms of voice that are beneficial to the
work group or organization (Liang et al., 2012), our findings
uphold the importance of treating promotive and prohibitive voice
as separate constructs with distinguishable effects. Consistent with
meta-analytic evidence that shows employees receive lower rat-
ings of performance with prohibitive voice compared with promo-
tive voice (Chamberlin et al., 2017), we theorize and find that
supervisors appraise and experience group promotive voice differ-
Figure 3. (a) The differential effects of group voice on supervisor chal- ently than prohibitive voice. Specifically, just as supervisors can
lenge appraisals of group voice (Study 2). (b) The differential effects of
be energized or exhausted by various responsibilities (Courtright,
group voice on supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice (Study 2).
Colbert, & Choi, 2014; Sherf, Tangirala, & Venkataramani, 2019),
(c) Interaction of group prohibitive voice and supervisor personal sense of
power on supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice (Study 2). they can similarly experience an uplift from group promotive voice
or be worn down by group prohibitive voice. These findings have
broader implications for how supervisors receive input. For exam-
prohibitive voice and hindrance appraisals of group voice. Fur- ple, we find that supervisors internalize group promotive voice in
thermore, and again in line with Study 1, we found support for the a positive manner, which aligns with research on the self-
negative effects of group promotive voice on emotional exhaustion enhancing effects of positive performance feedback (Jordan &
and the positive effects of group prohibitive voice on emotional Audia, 2012). Thus, promotive voice not only boosts work group
exhaustion through hindrance appraisals of group voice. Impor- and organizational outcomes (e.g., Li et al., 2017; MacKenzie et
tantly, the design of Study 2 enabled us to separately examine the al., 2011) but also the voice targets’ well-being and performance.
effects of group promotive voice and group prohibitive voice on In contrast, receiving negative feedback about the functioning of
appraisals of group voice, given that participants were exposed the supervisor’s work group via group prohibitive voice prompts a
solely to one voice type or the other. This approach allows us to hindrance appraisal from supervisors. This parallels findings from
more directly assess whether either group promotive or prohibitive Kluger and DeNisi (1996) about individuals’ having an adverse
632 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

reaction to negative performance feedback (cf. Balven, 2018; Folkman, 1984; Sonnentag & Frese, 2003), a challenge appraisal
Bond & Anderson, 1987; Fisher, 1979) while suggesting negative by itself (i.e., the subjective interpretation of a work condition)
feedback need not be expressly directed at the target’s own per- does not require any coping efforts that would trigger strain.
formance to provoke an unfavorable reaction. In sum, our work Although more research is needed on how appraisals, demands,
points to the importance of understanding how supervisors inter- and coping fit together, our work suggests that examining apprais-
nalize input, such as group promotive and prohibitive voice, to als is a worthwhile initiative.
their benefit or detriment.
In addition to considering how group voice may beneficially or
Limitations and Future Research
adversely affect supervisors, we also extend research on how voice
can be viewed as threatening to supervisors (e.g., Burris, 2012). Despite the strengths of our studies, several limitations exist. As
Specifically, we find that a supervisor’s personal sense of power is with all field research, we can make limited inferences about
an important boundary condition for appraisals of group prohibi- causality in Study 1. To address this concern, we ground our
tive voice but less so for appraisals of group promotive voice. Our predictions in the transactional theory of stress (Lazarus & Folk-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

finding that problems expressed to supervisors have a particularly man, 1984), and our findings generally support the theorized
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

deleterious effect on supervisor emotional exhaustion and perfor- direction of effects. Further, we bolster our findings in a field study
mance when a supervisor’s personal sense of power is low builds with an experimental study (Study 2), which enables stronger
on work that suggests that a supervisor’s perception of limited causal inference by manipulating group voice conditions. Comple-
efficacy at work can negatively affect the voice process (Fast et al., menting the external validity offered by Study 1 with the internal
2014). Indeed, our findings suggest that expressions of perceived validity provide by Study 2 gives us greater confidence in our
harm from prohibitive voice may be more salient to supervisors findings. Although our findings from Study 1 and Study 2 are
than expressions of opportunities because people are, in general, well-aligned, we did not capture the relationship between emo-
more averse to loss (e.g., Tversky & Kahneman, 1992). Thus, tional exhaustion and performance in our experimental study
supervisors may be able to accept and address the group’s prob- (Study 2). We note that the negative relationship between emo-
lems in cases of high perceived power but more likely to feel tional exhaustion and performance is well-established by other
burdened by problems expressed via group prohibitive voice when research (see Sonnentag, 2015 for a review). Thus, although we
their perceived power is lacking. did not consider the negative relationship between emotional ex-
Finally, our work advances the transactional theory of stress by haustion and performance in both studies, we view Study 2 as
addressing how and why group promotive and prohibitive voice replicating the relationships that are most novel and central to our
are appraised in distinct ways. Our delineation of different apprais- contribution, specifically the effects of group voice on supervisor
als of group promotive and prohibitive voice builds on recent work emotional exhaustion through supervisor challenge and hindrance
that has emphasized a need to distinguish between work experi- appraisals of group voice.
ences themselves and appraisals of those experiences (LePine et Second, in our efforts to manage common method bias in Study
al., 2016; Ohly & Fritz, 2010; Tuckey, Searle, Boyd, Winefield, & 1 by separating our measures across points in time, we note that
Winefield, 2015; Webster et al., 2011). More specifically, our this approach introduces limitations with missing data given par-
research points to the importance of measuring and evaluating ticipant attrition (Newman, 2014). To address this concern, we
appraisals as opposed to simply measuring demands. We suggest evaluated selective attrition by considering changes in the compo-
this effort will help reconcile theorizing from Lazarus and Folk- sition of our employee and supervisor samples across points in
man (1984) about the positive implications of challenge appraisals time, and found no significant differences in our sample across
of work conditions with the negative implications of challenge time points for a number of demographic variables. We also
demands themselves. Specifically, Lazarus and Folkman (1984) evaluated ICCs and rwg’s related to our constructs rated by group
theorize that challenge appraisals should result in high work mo- members and found significant ICCs and high rwg’s, suggesting a
rale and confidence, positive emotions, and a resistance to being high level of group member agreement and supporting aggregation
emotionally overwhelmed. As far as we know, we are the first of group promotive voice, group prohibitive voice, and supervisor
empirical study to posit a negative relationship between challenge performance (Chan, 1998). Although having more raters (i.e., no
appraisals and emotional exhaustion. The lack of previous empir- missing data) makes aggregation more reliable, our approach of
ical research on this point, and theory that supports the finding, aggregating group member ratings appears to have been appropri-
suggests that future research is needed. More specifically, given ate and reliable (Bliese, 2000).
evidence that categories of challenge demands (e.g., pressure to Additionally, survey research can be limited by challenges in
complete tasks, time urgency, workload) are positively associated construct validity, or the correspondence between the conceptual
with strain (Podsakoff, LePine, & LePine, 2007), we call on future definition of variables and their operationalization (Schwab,
research to further examine the relationship between stressors, 1980). For example, our measures of challenge and hindrance
strain, coping, and emotional exhaustion. In addition to our and appraisals could potentially be confounded with emotional exhaus-
others’ efforts (LePine et al., 2016; Webster et al., 2011) to tion in capturing perceived well-being consequences rather than
measure and evaluate appraisals of work conditions, examining the appraisal event. To ensure construct validity, the conceptual
individuals’ coping efforts may be a line of inquiry to explain the definitions of our variables drew from well-developed theory (i.e.,
disconnect between the positive implications of challenge apprais- Anderson et al., 2012; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Liang et al.,
als and the straining effects of challenge demands themselves. For 2012; Maslach & Jackson, 1993). Further, we relied on existing
example, although problem- and emotion-focused efforts exerted validated measures to align with established definitions (i.e., LeP-
to cope with challenge demands may induce strain (Lazarus & ine et al., 2016; Liang et al., 2012; Maslach & Jackson, 1993).
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 633

Within our own applications of these measures, we found rela- tional leaders should be cognizant of the tension between the benefits
tively high reliabilities, suggesting that our measures were suffi- gained by encouraging group voice and the potentially draining con-
ciently free from error (Schwab, 2005). We also found support for sequences for supervisors as the targets of group members’ ideas and
our factor structure within the results of our exploratory factor concerns.
analysis for our measures of supervisor challenge appraisals of
group voice, supervisor hindrance appraisals of group voice, and
supervisor emotional exhaustion. As a means of further supporting References
the validity of our focal constructs, the manipulations of group Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Exam-
promotive and prohibitive voice and personal sense of power in ining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Jour-
our experimental study represent an alternative operationalization nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362–1377. http://dx.doi
of our predictors and yielded consistent results with our survey .org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1362
measures of these constructs. Anderson, C., & Brion, S. (2014). Perspectives on power in organizations.
Finally, future studies that target the distribution of group voice Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behav-
ior, 1, 67–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091259
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

may be insightful (e.g., Sherf et al., 2018), especially its relationship


Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to supervisor appraisals of group voice and group outcomes. Although European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 511–536. http://dx.doi.org/
we found no significant effects for the distribution of group voice 10.1002/ejsp.324
within our study, a focused investigation may provide more nuanced Anderson, C., John, O. P., & Keltner, D. (2012). The personal sense of
findings. The dispersion of voice across work group members could power. Journal of Personality, 80, 313–344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
also be expanded to explore differences in group members’ use of .1467-6494.2011.00734.x
promotive or prohibitive voice and further examine the effects of Anderson, C., Kraus, M. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Keltner, D. (2012). The
group size. Social networks analysis or latent profile analysis may local-ladder effect: Social status and subjective well-being. Psychological
provide a clearer picture of how promotive and prohibitive voice can Science, 23, 764 –771. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611434537
be optimally distributed within groups, particularly for group perfor- Argyris, C. (1990). Overcoming organizational defenses. Boston, MA:
Allyn & Bacon.
mance and supervisors’ reactions toward group voice.
Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., Taris, T. W., Schaufeli, W. B., & Schreurs,
P. J. (2003). A multigroup analysis of the job demands-resources model
Practical Implications in four home care organizations. International Journal of Stress Man-
agement, 10, 16 –38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1072-5245.10.1.16
Our research broadly points to practical tradeoffs between ad- Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & Verbeke, W. (2004). Using the job
dressing the course corrections that come from employee voice demands-resources model to predict burnout and performance. Hu-
while mitigating prospective supervisor emotional exhaustion man Resource Management, 43, 83–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
from attending to group voice. To this end, our examination of hrm.20004
group promotive and prohibitive voice suggests that supervisors Balven, R. M. (2018). When people at work go astray, what to say and how
are likely to be less emotionally exhausted and consequently more to say it: A typology and test of the effect of moral feedback on unethical
behavior (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Arizona State University,
effective performers when they are the target of group promotive
Tempe, AZ.
voice. As such, organizations can encourage group members to Barling, J., & Frone, M. R. (2017). If only my leader would just do
speak up about issues with opportunity- and solution-oriented something! Passive leadership undermines employee well-being through
expressions. In particular, our experimental study suggests that role stressors and psychological resource depletion. Stress and Health,
employees and work groups that are able to couch their comments 33, 211–222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smi.2697
as promotive in nature may help maintain supervisor well-being Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
and performance. Speaking up in a promotive manner will not only interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psycho-
benefit supervisors by enhancing their well-being, and perfor- logical Bulletin, 117, 497–529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117
mance, but, as others have found, improve the performance eval- .3.497
uations of voicing employees (Chamberlin et al., 2017). Becker, T. E. (2005). Potential problems in the statistical control of
variables in organizational research: A qualitative analysis with recom-
Our findings also point to the importance of supervisors’ personal
mendations. Organizational Research Methods, 8, 274 –289. http://dx
sense of power as a factor that can reduce and perhaps mitigate the .doi.org/10.1177/1094428105278021
negative effects of group prohibitive voice on supervisor emotional Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and
exhaustion and performance. Thus, organizations can help supervisors reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein
better receive the constructive input of their work groups by increas- & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods
ing supervisors’ personal sense of power. This increase in power can in organizations (pp. 349 –381). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
be achieved by providing access to organizational resources, increas- Bolino, M. C., Hsiung, H. H., Harvey, J., & LePine, J. A. (2015). “Well,
ing the value of resources already possessed, and enhancing the I’m tired of tryin’!” Organizational citizenship behavior and citizenship
impression of holding valued resources in the eyes of others (Ander- fatigue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 56 –74. http://dx.doi.org/
son & Brion, 2014). In order to reduce the negative effects of group 10.1037/a0037583
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. (2007). What
prohibitive voice on supervisor emotional exhaustion and perfor-
predicts psychological resilience after disaster? The role of demograph-
mance, organizations can also foster increased personal connections ics, resources, and life stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
among multiple supervisors as helping supervisors to become better chology, 75, 671– 682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.671
connected to influential others will also improve the flow of sugges- Bond, C. F., & Anderson, E. L. (1987). The reluctance to transmit bad news:
tions and problems to others in the organization (Detert et al., 2013) Private discomfort or public display?. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
and therefore may reduce supervisors’ burdens. Overall, organiza- chology, 23, 176 –187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(87)90030-8
634 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

Boswell, W. R., Olson-Buchanan, J. B., & LePine, M. A. (2004). Relations Doty, D. H., & Glick, W. H. (1998). Common methods bias: Does common
between stress and work outcomes: The role of felt challenge, job methods variance really bias results? Organizational Research Methods,
control, and psychological strain. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64, 1, 374 – 406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109442819814002
165–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00049-6 Fast, N. J., Burris, E. R., & Bartel, C. A. (2014). Managing to stay in the
Burris, E. R. (2012). The risks and rewards of speaking up: Managerial dark: Managerial self-efficacy, ego defensiveness, and the aversion to
responses to employee voice. Academy of Management Journal, 55, employee voice. Academy of Management Journal, 57, 1013–1034.
851– 875. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0562 http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2012.0393
Burris, E. R., Detert, J. R., & Chiaburu, D. S. (2008). Quitting before Fisher, C. D. (1979). Transmission of positive and negative feedback to
leaving: The mediating effects of psychological attachment and detach- subordinates: A laboratory investigation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
ment on voice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 912–922. http://dx 64, 533–540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.64.5.533
.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.4.912 Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to
Carlson, K. D., & Wu, J. (2012). The illusion of statistical control: Control action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453– 466.
variable practice in management research. Organizational Research http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.453
Methods, 15, 413– 435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428111428817 Gibson, C. B. (1999). Do they do what they believe they can? Group
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Cavanaugh, M. A., Boswell, W. R., Roehling, M. V., & Boudreau, J. W. efficacy and group effectiveness across tasks and cultures. Academy of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(2000). An empirical examination of self-reported work stress among Management Journal, 42, 138 –152.
U.S. managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 65–74. http://dx.doi Grant, A. M. (2013). Rocking the boat but keeping it steady: The role of
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.85.1.65 emotion regulation in employee voice. Academy of Management Jour-
Chamberlin, M. (2017). Exploring supervisor responses to employees nal, 56, 1703–1723. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.0035
who share bad news: Why and under what conditions are messengers Hackman, J. R. (2003). Learning more from crossing levels: Evidence from
shot? (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Arizona State University, airplanes, hospitals, and orchestras. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
Tempe, AZ. 24, 905–922. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.226
Chamberlin, M., Newton, D. W., & Lepine, J. A. (2017). A meta-analysis Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for ob-
of voice and its promotive and prohibitive forms: Identification of key served variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process model-
associations, distinctions, and future research directions. Personnel Psy- ing. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf
chology, 70, 11–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/peps.12185 Hussain, I., Shu, R., Tangirala, S., & Ekkirala, S. (2019). The voice
Chan, D. (1998). Functional relations among constructs in the same content bystander effect: How information redundancy inhibits employee voice.
domain at different levels of analysis: A typology of composition mod- Academy of Management Journal, 62, 828 – 849. http://dx.doi.org/10
els. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 234 –246. http://dx.doi.org/10 .5465/amj.2017.0245
.1037/0021-9010.83.2.234 James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. interrater reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 Psychology, 69, 85–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.69.1.85
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple Jordan, A. H., & Audia, P. G. (2012). Self-enhancement and learning from
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York, performance feedback. Academy of Management Review, 37, 211–231.
NY: Routledge. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.2010.0108
Colquitt, J. A. (2008). Publishing laboratory research in AMJ: A question Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental
of when, not if. Academy of Management Journal, 51, 616 – 620. http:// strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly,
dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMJ.2008.33664717 24, 285–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2392498
Cortina, J. M., Chen, G., & Dunlap, W. P. (2001). Testing interaction effects Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach,
in LISREL: Examination and illustration of available procedures. Organi- and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284. http://dx.doi.org/
zational Research Methods, 4, 324 –360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ 10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265
109442810144002 Kerlinger, F., & Lee, H. (2000). Foundations of behavioral research (4th
Courtright, S. H., Colbert, A. E., & Choi, D. (2014). Fired up or burned ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
out? How developmental challenge differentially impacts leader behav- Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions
ior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 681– 696. http://dx.doi.org/10 on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary
.1037/a0035790 feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 254 –284.
Cropanzano, R., Rupp, D. E., & Byrne, Z. S. (2003). The relationship of http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.254
emotional exhaustion to work attitudes, job performance, and organiza- Lam, C. F., & Mayer, D. M. (2014). When do employees speak up for their
tional citizenship behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 160 – customers? A model of voice in a customer service context. Personnel
169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.160 Psychology, 67, 637– 666.
Desai, S. D., & Kouchaki, M. (2017). Moral symbols: A necklace of garlic Lam, C. F., Rees, L., Levesque, L. L., & Ornstein, S. (2018). Shooting
against unethical requests. Academy of Management Journal, 60, 7–28. from the hip: A habit perspective of voice. Academy of Management
http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2015.0008 Review, 43, 470 – 486. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.2015.0366
Detert, J. R., & Burris, E. R. (2007). Leadership behavior and employee Lanaj, K., Foulk, T. A., & Erez, A. (2019). Energizing leaders via self-
voice: Is the door really open? Academy of Management Journal, 50, reflection: A within-person field experiment. Journal of Applied Psy-
869 – 884. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2007.26279183 chology, 104, 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000350
Detert, J. R., Burris, E. R., Harrison, D. A., & Martin, S. R. (2013). Voice Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New
flows to and around leaders: Understanding when units are helped or York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
hurt by employee voice. Administrative Science Quarterly, 58, 624 – Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New
668. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0001839213510151 York, NY: Springer.
Detert, J. R., & Edmondson, A. C. (2011). Implicit voice theories: Taken- LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2008). Answers to 20 questions about
for-granted rules of self-censorship at work. Academy of Management interrater reliability and interrater agreement. Organizational Research
Journal, 54, 461– 488. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.61967925 Methods, 11, 815– 852. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428106296642
THE EFFECTS OF GROUP VOICE ON SUPERVISORS 635

Lee, R. T., & Ashforth, B. E. (1993). A further examination of managerial and target openness. Personnel Psychology, 68, 547–580. http://dx.doi
burnout: Toward an integrated model. Journal of Organizational Behav- .org/10.1111/peps.12087
ior, 14, 3–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.4030140103 Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los
Lee, R. T., & Ashforth, B. E. (1996). A meta-analytic examination of the Angeles, CA: Author.
correlates of the three dimensions of job burnout. Journal of Applied Nahrgang, J. D., Morgeson, F. P., & Ilies, R. (2009). The development of
Psychology, 81, 123–133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.81.2.123 leader–member exchanges: Exploring how personality and performance
LePine, M. A., Zhang, Y., Crawford, E. R., & Rich, B. L. (2016). Turning influence leader and member relationships over time. Organizational
their pain to gain: Charismatic leader influence on follower stress Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108, 256 –266. http://dx.doi
appraisal and job performance. Academy of Management Journal, 59, .org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.09.002
1036 –1059. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2013.0778 Newman, D. A. (2014). Missing data: Five practical guidelines. Organi-
Li, A. N., Liao, H., Tangirala, S., & Firth, B. M. (2017). The content of the zational Research Methods, 17, 372– 411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
message matters: The differential effects of promotive and prohibitive team 1094428114548590
voice on team productivity and safety performance gains. Journal of Ap- Ohly, S., & Fritz, C. (2010). Work characteristics, challenge appraisal,
plied Psychology, 102, 1259 –1270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000215 creativity, and proactive behavior: A multi-level study. Journal of Or-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Liang, J., Farh, C. I., & Farh, J. L. (2012). Psychological antecedents of ganizational Behavior, 31, 543–565. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.633
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

promotive and prohibitive voice: A two-wave examination. Academy of Organ, D. W. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The good
Management Journal, 55, 71–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0176 soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., & Stilwell, D. (1993). A longitudinal-study on the Podsakoff, N. P., LePine, J. A., & LePine, M. A. (2007). Differential
early development of leader member exchanges. Journal of Applied Psy- challenge stressor-hindrance stressor relationships with job attitudes,
chology, 78, 662– 674. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.4.662 turnover intentions, turnover, and withdrawal behavior: A meta-analysis.
Lin, S. H. J., & Johnson, R. E. (2015). A suggestion to improve a day keeps Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 438 – 454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
your depletion away: Examining promotive and prohibitive voice behaviors 0021-9010.92.2.438
within a regulatory focus and ego depletion framework. Journal of Applied Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003).
Psychology, 100, 1381–1397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000018 Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the
Liu, W., Tangirala, S., & Ramanujam, R. (2013). The relational anteced-
literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology,
ents of voice targeted at different leaders. Journal of Applied Psychol-
88, 879 –903. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879
ogy, 98, 841– 851. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032913
Qin, X., DiRenzo, M. S., Xu, M., & Duan, Y. (2014). When do emotionally
MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2011). Challenge-
exhausted employees speak up? Exploring the potential curvilinear
oriented organizational citizenship behaviors and organizational effec-
relationship between emotional exhaustion and voice. Journal of Orga-
tiveness: Do challenge-oriented behaviors really have an impact on the
nizational Behavior, 35, 1018 –1041. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.1948
organization’s bottom line? Personnel Psychology, 64, 559 –592. http://
Quinn, R. W., Spreitzer, G. M., & Lam, C. F. (2012). Building a sustain-
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01219.x
able model of human energy in organizations: Exploring the critical role
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., &
of resources. The Academy of Management Annals, 6, 337–396. http://
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other
dx.doi.org/10.1080/19416520.2012.676762
intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. http://
Schwab, D. P. (1980). Construct validity in organizational behavior. In
dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.83
B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational
Mackinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence
behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 3– 43). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling
Schwab, D. P. (2005). Research methods for organizational studies (2nd
methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99 –128. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4 ed.). London, UK: Routledge.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1993). Maslach Burnout Inventory. Palo Sherf, E. N., Sinha, R., Tangirala, S., & Awasty, N. (2018). Centralization
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. of member voice in teams: Its effects on expertise utilization and team
Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annual performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103, 813– 827. http://dx
Review of Psychology, 52, 397– 422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev .doi.org/10.1037/apl0000305
.psych.52.1.397 Sherf, E. N., Tangirala, S., & Venkataramani, V. (2019). Why managers do
Maynes, T. D., & Podsakoff, P. M. (2014). Speaking more broadly: An not seek voice from employees: The importance of managers’ personal
examination of the nature, antecedents, and consequences of an ex- control and long-term orientation. Organization Science, 30, 447– 466.
panded set of employee voice behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2018.1273
99, 87–112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034284 Singh-Manoux, A., Adler, N. E., & Marmot, M. G. (2003). Subjective
McClean, E., Martin, S. R., Emich, K. J., & Woodruff, T. (2018). The social status: Its determinants and its association with measures of
social consequences of voice: An examination of voice type and gender ill-health in the Whitehall II study. Social Science & Medicine, 56,
on status and subsequent leader emergence. Academy of Management 1321–1333. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00131-4
Journal, 61, 1869 –1891. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amj.2016.0148 Sonnentag, S. (2015). Dynamics of well-being. Annual Review of Orga-
Morrison, E. W. (2014). Employee voice and silence. Annual Review of nizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2, 261–293. http://
Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1, 173–197. dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111347
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091328 Sonnentag, S., & Frese, M. (2003). Stress in organizations. In W. C. Borman,
Morrison, E. W., & Milliken, F. J. (2000). Organizational silence: A barrier D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of
to change and development in a pluralistic world. Academy of Manage- psychology: Industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 12, pp.
ment Review, 25, 706 –725. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.2000.3707697 453– 491). New York, NY: Wiley. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/04712
Morrison, E. W., & Rothman, N. B. (2009). Silence and the dynamics of 64385.wei1218
power. In J. Greenberg & M. Edwards (Eds.), Voice and silence in Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional
organizations (pp. 175–202). Bingley, UK: Emerald. exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits
Morrison, E. W., See, K. E., & Pan, C. (2015). An approach-inhibition of psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76, 355–
model of employee silence: The joint effects of personal sense of power 365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2009.06.005
636 SESSIONS, NAHRGANG, NEWTON, AND CHAMBERLIN

Tangirala, S., & Ramanujam, R. (2008). Exploring nonlinearity in em- Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory:
ployee voice: The effects of personal control and organizational identi- Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncer-
fication. Academy of Management Journal, 51, 1189 –1203. http://dx.doi tainty, 5, 297–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00122574
.org/10.5465/amj.2008.35732719 Van Dyne, L., & LePine, J. A. (1998). Helping and voice extra-role
Tangirala, S., & Ramanujam, R. (2012). Ask and you shall hear (but not behaviors: Evidence of construct and predictive validity. Academy of
always): Examining the relationship between manager consultation and Management Journal, 41, 108 –119.
employee voice. Personnel Psychology, 65, 251–282. http://dx.doi.org/ Webster, J. R., Beehr, T. A., & Love, K. (2011). Extending the challenge-
10.1111/j.1744-6570.2012.01248.x hindrance model of occupational stress: The role of appraisal. Journal of
Ten Brummelhuis, L. L., Haar, J. M., & Roche, M. (2014). Does family life Vocational Behavior, 79, 505–516. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2011
help to be a better leader? A closer look at crossover processes from
.02.001
leaders to followers. Personnel Psychology, 67, 917–949. http://dx.doi
Wright, T. A., & Bonett, D. G. (1997). The contribution of burnout to work
.org/10.1111/peps.12057
performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 18, 491– 499.
Tofighi, D., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2011). RMediation: An R package for
mediation analysis confidence intervals. Behavior Research Methods,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

43, 692–700. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0076-x


Tuckey, M. R., Searle, B. J., Boyd, C. M., Winefield, A. H., & Winefield,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

H. R. (2015). Hindrances are not threats: Advancing the multidimen- Received January 25, 2019
sionality of work stress. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 20, Revision received August 29, 2019
131–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038280 Accepted September 8, 2019 䡲

You might also like