Child Clin Npsychol DRug Abuse

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

24

Child Clinical Neuropsychology


of Drug Abuse
ARTHUR MACNEILL HORTON, JR.
AND ARTHUR MACNEILL HORTON, III

Introduction in the Americas. England also made chocolate


illegal in the 1700s with little success. Later,
The use of substances to modify moods, behaviors England, going the other way, in order to over-
and perceptions, drug abuse has been a problem come a balance of payments problem over tea
since the beginning of recorded history. Human purchases with China fought two wars with
beings for centuries have used alcohol and natural China in the 1700s and 1800s to be allowed to
substances such as peyote from the cactus and export opium into China and gain control of the
leaves from the opium plant for their psychoactive island of Hong Kong as a site to store imported
properties. After the industrial revolution, chemi- opium. The United States prohibited alcohol use
cal technology has produced new psychoactive in the 1920s but prohibition was a complete fail-
substances with abuse potential such as heroin, ure as a social policy and the sale of alcohol was
cocaine, LSD, PCP, designer drugs and Ecstasy. legalized by a constitutional amendment in 1933.
Indeed, the likelihood is that new psychoactive The United States has also prohibited drug use
drugs will be developed in the future and they of a number of substances but the effort has not
will present additional potential for abuse. been a complete success and significant illegal
Addiction to drugs of abuse is an important drug use has continued (SAMHSA, 2006).
issue for a number of reasons. One reason is that Survey data indicate that in 2005 (the most
prohibition of drugs of abuse has not been very recent available findings) 19.7 million Americans
successful as a social policy. Tobacco use was or 8.1% of the US population aged 12 or older
discouraged in England in the early 1600s but used illicit drugs during the month prior to the
smoking still increased and growing tobacco was survey interview (SAMHSA, 2006). Of illicit drug
a major source of income for the British colonies users, marijuana is the most commonly used illicit
drug, with 14.6 past month users or 6.0% in 2005
(SAMHSA, 2006). For other commonly used illicit
Sections of this chapter have been adapted and updated from, drugs in 2005, 2.4 million persons (1.0%) were
‘‘Horton, A. M., Jr. (1996) Neuropsychology of drug abuse. In current cocaine abusers, 1.1 million or 0.4% used
R. J. Sbordone and C. J. Long (Eds.) Ecological validity of hallucinogens (including 502,000 or 0.2% users of
neuropsychological testing (pp. 357–368) Delray Beach, Fla.: Ecstasy) (SAMHSA, 2006). For prescription psy-
GR Press/St. Lucie Press.’’ chotherapeutic drugs non-medical use there were
ARTHUR MACNEILL HORTON, JR.  Psych Associ- 6.4 million (2.6%) users aged 12 and older, of these
ates, Towson, Maryland. ARTHUR MACNEILL users 4.7 used pain relievers, 1.8 million used tran-
HORTON, III  Eastern Virginia Medical School, quilizers, 1.1 million used stimulants (including
Norfolk, VA. 512,00 using methamphetamine) and 272,000

C.R. Reynolds, E. Fletcher-Janzen (eds.), Handbook of Clinical Child Neuropsychology, 599


DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-78867-8_24, Ó Springer ScienceþBusiness Media, LLC 2009
600 CHAPTER 24

used sedatives (SAMHSA, 2006). For youths aged of marijuana for certain medical diseases are
12–17 in 2005, 9.9% were current illicit drug users allowed by state legal authorities for selected
(SAMHSA, 2006). The rate of illicit drug use individuals in the United States but on the federal
among youths aged 12–17 in 2005 was similar to level any use of marijuana is discouraged. In the
the rate of illicit drug use in 2004 (10.6%) but State of Nevada, a measure to make the use of
significantly lower than the rate of illicit drug use marijuana legal and a state monopoly was nar-
in 2002 (11.2%) (SAMHSA, 2006). rowly defeated in recent years after substantial
Alcohol use was higher. In 2005, it was federal pressure was applied.
estimated that 126 million Americans, aged 12 The above examples argue that it is impor-
or older, were recent alcohol drinkers (51.8%) or tant to realize that society’s views regarding the
more than half of the US population in that age use of addictive substances are dependent on
range (SAMHSA, 2006). Of those current alco- social factors and may change over time. The
hol users age 12 or older, 55 million (22.7%) first use of many psychoactive substances
were monthly binge drinkers and 16 million abused today, it should be noted, was for allevia-
(6.6%) were heavy drinkers (SAMHSA, 2006). tion of pain during surgery. The father of psy-
For youths aged 12–20, 10.8 million reported choanalysis, Sigmund Freud, for example,
monthly drinking (28.2%) in 2005 (SAMHSA, advocated the use of cocaine as a pain medica-
2006). Generally speaking there were few tion. Freud changed his mind regarding the use
increases in illicit drug and alcohol use from of cocaine after the addiction potential became
2004 to 2005 (SAMHSA, 2006). clear. Substances such as chocolate, tobacco and
Numbers, however, fail to capture the alcohol that were earlier prohibited by society
damage to society caused by drugs of abuse. are legal and accepted today. Alternately, many
Drug abuse has been estimated as costing the substances that are illegal now were legal some
United States more than 275 billion dollars a years ago. The problem of psychoactive drug
year (Harwood et al., 1998). Drug abuse costs abuse is of great importance and all persons
are from multiple sources. Health costs increase need to understand the multiple aspect of this
due to the pathological consequences of alcohol important problem as society must make wise
and illicit drug use. In addition, there are costs decisions as to which psychoactive substances
from the results of domestic violence, traffic should be prohibited. Perhaps the most impor-
accidents and crime (Hubbard et al., 1989). In tant scientific factor that should be considered is
addition, there are significant traumatic stress the physical effects of psychoactive substance on
effects on the spouses and children of drug the human brain. Psychoactive substances by
addicts and alcoholics that produce initial and definition are drugs that affect the human brain.
later delayed psychological damage. The possible negative effects of drugs of abuse
While there are multiple negative effects of on the human brain should be considered in
illicit drug use and excessive alcohol use, the making social decisions regarding prohibition
social question is not as simple as to suggest of psychoactive drugs. The effects on the
simple prohibition of any psychoactive substance human brain are, of course, not the only factor
as a creditable social policy. It must be noted that to consider in formulating America’s drug abuse
under some circumstances, the use of certain sub- policy but it is an area regarding which clinical
stances to modify moods and behaviors may be child neuropsychologists have unique scientific
regarded appropriate. Social norms may be seen expertise that can be used to inform national and
as strongly influencing decisions as to whether state policy makers.
the use of a psychoactive substance is pathologi- The use of psychoactive drugs that may be
cal. For example, the fact that chocolate was abused by children is an issue of great important
made illegal in England for a period of time, concern. Because children are in a developmen-
years ago, suggests that social factors rather tal period and their brains are still maturing the
than non-social issues are involved. In the United effects of drugs of abuse may be more impairing
States the government allows Native Americans than when adults whose brains are mature abuse
tribes in the southwestern United States to use similar drugs. Also, drug abuse by children of
peyote in religious rites. Also, many adults use school age may preclude education processes.
caffeine in the form of coffee, tea or cola drinks. The use of drugs of abuse by school age children
In addition, on a state-by-state basis, limited use may interfere with learning and memory
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 601

processes that underlie their mental abilities to are state dependent rather than enduring after
profit from educational instruction. The aim of withdrawal from alcohol and in some cases other
this chapter is to review what is currently known abused substance or substances. The residual
about psychoactive drugs of abuse, the negative neuropsychological effects of psychoactive
neuropsychological effects of exposure to psy- drugs to be examined in this chapter would be
choactive drugs and approaches to the neurop- expected to be enduring organic syndromes of
sychological assessment of children and youth greater time duration.
that have been exposed to drugs of abuse, with Regarding the other substance abuse syn-
special attention to the role of child clinical dromes, other than alcohol, the major sub-
neuropsychologists. stance abuse syndromes associated with
organic deficits recognized by (APA, 2000)
include post-hallucinogen perception disorder,
Overview PCP organic mental disorder not otherwise spe-
cified and amnesic disorder caused by sedative
Research has documented that there are hypnotic drugs.
neuropsychological effects from psychoactive
drugs of abuse (Horton, 1996). Because of the
extent of drug abuse, as earlier reported, there Brain Structures and Processes Underlying
are important implications for the assessment
and treatment of drug abusers (Spencer & Addictive Behaviors
Boren, 1990). Tarter and Edwards (1987) were
among the first researchers to address the issue Because the brain is the organ which sub-
of impaired functional adaptive capacity of indi- sumes human behavior, a basis understanding of
viduals with substance abuse problems. Tarter the brain structures and processes underlying
and Edwards (1987) averred that the use of neu- addictive behavior is essential to understand
ropsychological assessment to measure an indi- substance use and abuse. The notion is that
vidual’s organic integrity would provide a basis understanding brain–behavior relationships
for estimating an individual’s potential for will aid in gaining insight into behavior that
recovery and future adaptive ability. For chil- often appears painful and chaotic. Addiction
dren who are of school age, the degree and pat- processes based in the brain result from changes
tern of neuropsychological impairment can to the reward pathway upon exposure to psy-
provide important information to guide educa- choactive drugs such as cocaine, heroin, and
tional instructional decisions. marijuana, among others (Figure 1).
Studies to assess the neuropsychological Reward pathways promote positive condi-
treatment implications in populations of drug tioning whereby behaviors that precipitate the
abusers are few because agreement that there experience of pleasure are increased. The brain
are residual neuropsychological effects of associates the behavior to the feeling of plea-
abused drugs in adults and children is a recent sure, and the behavior increases in frequency in
advance (Spencer & Boren, 1990). It should be expectation of eliciting the same pleasurable
noted that a distinction should be made between experience again. An exposition of neurophy-
residual and transient neuropsychological defi- siology, admittedly very brief and over simpli-
cits caused by psychoactive drugs of abuse fied, is presented and is followed by examples of
(Spencer & Boren, 1990). As is well known, drug– brain interaction to provide for a simplis-
selected drug-related states such as intoxication tic understanding of the brain processes under-
and withdrawal and delirium are considered to lying abuse of psychoactive substances
be relatively transient. For example a person (Figure 2).
could drink too much alcohol and for a time be The brain is composed of 100 billion cells,
incapable of operating a motor vehicle for a known as neurons, and functions intracellular
period of time but after some hours would regain communication. Messages among and between
the ability to drive safely. While the effects on cells are transmitted through chemical and elec-
learning and memory and executive functioning trical means. The parts of a given individual
from intoxication from alcohol are serious, it is neuron allow classification relevant to the trans-
expected that with most individuals these effects mission of information (Figure 3).
602 CHAPTER 24

FIGURE 1. The reward pathway consisting of three brain structures the VTA, nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex,
and the connections between the parts (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching3.html).

FIGURE 2. Two neurons able to communicate. The arrows designate the route of transferred information
(http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching2.html).

Information from other neurons comes maintain an electrical potential. The electrical
through the dendrites and soma. Initiation of potential is the difference of electrical charge
chemical processes in the neuron are elicited in between the inside of the neuron and the outside.
response to inter-neuron transmissions. Each The difference results from concentrations of
neuron’s membrane controls the internal chemi- positively and negatively charged ions on each
cal environment of the cell. Cell membranes side of the cell membrane. Such differences seek
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 603

FIGURE 3. An overview of a neuron with its major parts labeled (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching2.html).

equilibrium (balance) where the concentrations with the postsynaptic membrane of another neu-
of ions and thus electrical charge do not differ. ron forms the synapse. The area in between the
Membranes preclude this tendency impeding the two neurons that the neurotransmitters travel is
flow of ions between the two areas. Ions are known as the synaptic cleft. When the synaptic
allowed to pass through the membrane through cleft is crossed, the neurotransmitters attach to
channels in the membrane specific for that ion receptors on the postsynaptic membrane. The
and/or charge. These channels may be activated response of the neuron to the attachment of a
through a number of mechanisms. receptor will vary with the type of neurotrans-
At baseline a neuron is said to be at resting mitter and the type of receptor. Ion channels will
potential prior to receiving signals from other neu- allow for the entering or exit of ions. Neuro-
rons. The dendrites and soma of cells contain transmitters leave the receptor after attaching
receptors which bind selectively with the chemical and then are taken from the synaptic cleft by
signals sent between neurons (neurotransmitters). uptake pumps on the terminal of the presynaptic
Certain neurotransmitters cause bindings in the membrane. The process of inter-neuron commu-
electrical potential through the opening and clos- nication is the method by which information
ing of ion channels nearby. When bombarded with moves through the brain. Reward pathways
signals from surrounding neurons that excite, an related to the development of addiction have
action potential is produced. With an action been associated with the neurotransmitter dopa-
potential, an electrical current travels from the mine, among other neurotransmitters, because
dendrites and/or soma to the axon of the neuron of its prevalence in the reward pathway. Also,
as ion channels sensitive to electrical charge are neuromodulators (endorphins) bind to both pre-
opened. The cascading effect of ions flowing across synaptic and postsynaptic receptors to modify
the plasma membrane pushes the local electrical neuronal responses synaptic activity (Figure 4).
potential past its resting equilibrium. The current The human brain has multiple structural
generated moves to the end of the axon at the subdivisions. The different brain structures sub-
terminal. Packages of stored neurotransmitters sume specific neuropsychological functions and
cause the release of the contained neurotransmit- interact with one another to yield more complex
ters into the external cellular. and abstract behaviors. Examples of brain struc-
The membrane at the terminal is known as tures related to function are the hippocampus and
the presynaptic membrane which in combination memory, parietal lobes and tactual perception,
604 CHAPTER 24

FIGURE 4. A synapse with the presynaptic neuron on the top releases a neurotransmitter and on the bottom the postsynaptic
neuron contains two types of receptors (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching4.html).

temporal lobes and auditory perception, visual administered to other parts of the brain even if in
cortex and sight and hypothalamus and homeos- close proximity to parts of the brain reward
tasis. Routes of communication between struc- pathway.
tures rely on intra-neuronal interaction, and the Psychoactive substance can establish rela-
interconnections send and integrate information tionships between feelings of pleasure and drug-
between and among brain regions. A particular taking behavior by activating the brain reward
example is the brain reward pathway which is pathway. This activation of the brain reward path-
associated with the development of addictions. way causes drug-taking behavior to be selected
The brain reward pathway progresses from the over behaviors such as eating or hygiene or par-
ventral tegmental area (VTA) to the nucleus enting. Changes in brain function that are good
accumbens to the prefrontal cortex. The brain examples of drug–brain interaction are cocaine,
reward pathway is activated when positive rein- heroin and marijuana. Other psychoactive drugs
forcement (a reward) occurs paired in time with a affect the brain in similar ways to these three
certain behavior (www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/Teach examples. For example, the frequently abused
ing/). The behavior in close temporal sequence drug cocaine when smoked, snorted or injected
becomes associated to the reward. Living organ yields high concentrations in the VTA and nucleus
isms perform the behavior so long as a positive accumbens where dopamine is heavily used in
reinforcer (reward) accompanies the behavior. transmissions between neurons. The high concen-
Positive reinforcers may vary in reinforcement tration of the abused drug cocaine in these areas
potency depending on temporal factors. For exam binds to dopamine’s uptake pump precluding it
ple, if a person has not eaten for several hours then from removing dopamine from the synapse.
a preferred food may be a positive reinforcer but Thereby increasing brain reward pathway stimu-
after a heavy meal more food may not be seen as a lation and causing feelings of pleasure (Figure 5).
positive reinforcer (reward), but after several Synaptic dopamine levels and dopamine
hours things would change. It is well established receptors activity both increase. Dopamine
from experimental results that stimulation of the receptors bind to a G-protein. The G-protein
nucleus accumbens or VTA with cocaine activates with the inclusion of an enzyme forms a dopamine
the brain reward pathway. Activation of the brain receptor–G-protein/adenylate cyclase complex.
reward pathway did not occur with cocaine The enzyme is turned on and produces cAMP
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 605

FIGURE 5. The normal effects of the neurotransmitter, dopamine bound to a dopamine receptor. Here, an enzyme
is activated to produce a certain molecular compound (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching2.html).

FIGURE 6. Cocaine in the synapse blocks the uptake pump increasing dopamine activity (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/
teaching/Teaching4.html).

(cyclic adenosine monophosphate) molecules increased activation of the brain reward path-
which control the neurons ability to generate way (Figure 7). Because of the temporal associa-
action potentials (Figure 6). tion of cocaine use with increased activation of
Cocaine increases the amount of cAMP in the brain reward system, use of cocaine is asso-
the postsynaptic neuron with the net effects of ciated with feelings of pleasure. The activation of
606 CHAPTER 24

FIGURE 7. The effects of increased dopamine in the presence of cocaine. Contrast to Figure 5 to see difference in activity
and production of cAMP (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching4.html).

the brain reward pathway is responsible for the Marijuana, in terms of drug–reward path-
addictiveness of cocaine. At the same time, how- way interaction, requires additional explanation
ever, cocaine invades other brain areas. For relative to the well-explained cocaine and opiates
example, use of cocaine impairs the brain’s abil- reward pathway systems. Marijuana is repre-
ity to utilize glucose (energy fuel of the brain) for sented by its active ingredient cannabinoids or
metabolic activity. Ineffective use of glucose can THC (delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol). It is estab-
cause impairment of brain functions not related lished that regions of the reward pathway such as
to the brain reward system. the VTA and nucleus accumbens contain high
Opiates and other psychoactive drugs can concentrations of THC receptors. It is postulated
activate cells in the VTA and nucleus accumbens that THC activation of the reward system follows
of the reward pathway and increase dopamine a process similar to that of the opiate with a
release. The cellular mechanisms by which opiates neighboring neuron yielding more production of
activate the brain reward system, however, differ cAMP in the postsynaptic neuron (Figure 10).
from those of cocaine. Opiates bind to opiate The human reward pathway is activated by
receptors other than the presynaptic neuron or impulses sent from the nucleus accumbens to the
the postsynaptic neuron. Opiates bind rather to prefrontal cortex. While the short term or tran-
an additional neighboring neuron. Opiate bind- sitory effects of THC are well studied, the resi-
ing to the neuron influences the neuron to send dual effect of marijuana use on the reward
signals to the dopamine terminals to increase the pathway will require additional study. As with
amount of dopamine released. Higher levels of other psychoactive drugs, THC can be found in
synaptic dopamine released produces greater pro- brain areas other than the brain reward system
duction of cAMP in the postsynaptic neuron and pathway. For example, it has been established
the brain reward pathway is activated. Again, the that presence of THC in the hippocampus
association of the use of opiates and other psy- reduces memory function, and in the cerebellum
choactive drugs with feelings of pleasure produces the presence of THC can cause transient lack of
addiction to the drug (Figures 8 and 9). coordination and loss of balance.
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 607

FIGURE 8. A side view of the brain showing where opiates are found and active in the brain (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/
teaching/Teaching4.html).

FIGURE 9. Areas where THC is found and active in the brain (http://www.nida.nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching5.html).

Psychoactive Substance Abuse Research drug using behaviors would be necessary. Fol-
Issues lowing sections will present cursory reviews of
the neuropsychological test research data cur-
In order to understand the residual effects rently available regarding the residual effects of
of different psychoactive substances of abuse various psychoactive abused substances. There
some discussion of possible residual effects of are, however, many very serious methodological
608 CHAPTER 24

FIGURE 10. High level of dopamine resulting from THC binding to its receptor on a neighboring neuron (http://www.nida.
nih.gov/pubs/teaching/Teaching5.html).

difficulties involved with measuring residual substances is primarily a product of drug avail-
drug abuse effects in human beings. Prior ability. In short, the psychoactive substance
reviews by Reed and Grant (1990) and Horton available is abused. In estimating residual neu-
(1996) addressed many of these issues. As is well ropsychological effects of a particular drug of
known, some neuropsychological tests are cor- abuse such as cocaine, heroin or marijuana it is
related with demographic variables such as age, very difficult to disengage the effects of a single
gender, ethnicity and education. The issue of drug if the research subjects have used all three
correcting for age, gender and education metho- drugs in combination with alcohol.
dological confounds is very complex. While age, Similarly determining the amount of drugs
gender, ethnicity and education norms for a taken by research subjects is very problematic.
number of neuropsychological tests have been Most frequently research studies ask patients
developed to address this problem area (Heaton, to estimate post hoc the amount or amounts of
Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004), there are multi- a drug or drugs they had previously abused.
ple methodological limitations to the new demo- The research subjects’ self-report is solicited
graphic norms and the problems of correcting days, weeks, months and years after the epi-
demographic variables that are too numerous to sode or episodes of substance abuse. Substance
be detailed (Fastenau & Adams, 1996; Fastenau, abusing patients may be expected in some cases
1998; Horton, 1999, Reitan, 1967; Reitan & to have impaired memory abilities. Also recall
Wolfson, 1995, 2005). of what drugs were abused can be confounded
Also there is the vexing issue of use of multi- with the type and pattern of learning and mem-
ple substances of abuse. What substances drug ory deficits.
abusers have abused is difficult to control in The issue of assessing residual drugs effects
research investigations. Most drug abusers are based on the mode of consumption is also pro-
known as ‘‘garbage can’’ abusers. In brief, they blematic. Psychoactive drugs such as cocaine or
have used a wide variety of psychoactive sub- heroin may be ingested through a number of
stances with the most critical variable being modalities. For example, drugs can be taken
availability. Research studies characterize drug through needle injection, orally or through the
abusers as having a preference for one substance, nose. Each method of consumption can produce
but in reality, their daily consumption of addictive different effects with respect to the action of the
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 609

drug and possibly any residual neuropsycholo- possible to use true random research designs.
gical impairment. Rather, only quasi-experimental research designs
In addition, pre-existing and co-existing using non-equivalent control groups can be uti-
medical risk factors can also cause residual neu- lized. Therefore, all drug abuse research with
ropsychological deficits that could be mistaken human beings has limited control of potentially
for residual neuropsychological deficits due to confounding variables. The point for concluding
exposure to drugs of abuse. Tarter and Edwards that a specific drug of abuse may cause residual
(1987) were among the first researchers to iden- neuropsychological deficits is that low neuropsy-
tify many of these factors. They noted that pre- chological and intelligence test scores may be due
morbid and co-existing risk factors could also to another factor other than exposure to a specific
influence a person’s reactions to various drugs. drug. In neuropsychological testing, it is well
For example, pre-existing factors such as learn- established that there are four methodological
ing disabilities and attention-deficit hyperactiv- methods of inference (Reitan & Wolfson, 1993),
ity disorder could both present as residual such as level of performance, patterns of impair-
neuropsychological deficits and possibly interact ment, specific deficits and left–right comparisons.
with similar conditions to produce greater levels The usual criterion used to conclude there are
of neuropsychological impairment. With respect residual neuropsychological deficits following
to co-morbid conditions or factors, a variety of exposure to drugs of abuse is level of performance
serious psychiatric conditions could co-occur or, in other words, low scores on neuropsycholo-
with substance abuse and these mental disorders gical and intelligence tests. A better model would
are very difficult conditions to diagnose in an require that the other methodologies of inference
addict population. Also, it has been averred of neuropsychological testing (Horton & Wedding,
that poor nutrition (Tarter & Edwards, 1987) 1984; Parson & Farr, 1981; Reitan & Wolfson,
can impair neurocognitive functioning. In addi- 1993) such as patterns of impairment, specific
tion, drugs of abuse can produce medical pro- deficits and left–right comparisons be satisfied
blems in various organ systems other than the before it can be concluded that a specific drug of
human brain and thus have secondary effects on abuse would cause residual neuropsychological
a person’s mental ability. Multiple psychologi- deficits.
cal, psychiatric, medical and nutritional factors
can produce residual neuropsychological effects
that would need to be distinguished from resi- Marijuana/Cannabis
dual neuropsychological deficits of drugs of
abuse in human beings Benedict and Horton Residual neuropsychological deficits in
(1992). marijuana abusers are subtle as initial research
An additional research issue is that of the studies (Mendelson & Mayer, 1972; Grant,
criteria to accept that a neurotoxic substance Rockford, Flemming, & Stunkard, 1973; Carlin
may cause residual neuropsychological deficits and Trupin, 1977) failed to find significant neu-
in human beings. In many cases, researchers are ropsychological deficits in marijuana users.
willing to conclude that a drug may cause residual The initial studies contained few measures of
neuropsychological deficits simply based on a short-term memory functioning and that could
level of performance model. That is to say that be the reason for the negative results. Later stu-
for many researchers the sole criterion, for con- dies which included better measures of short-
cluding that a drug of abuse causes residual neu- term memory, however, suggested that memory
ropsychological deficits, is low scores on various functions may be impaired after consistent
neuropsychological and intelligence tests marijuana use (Page, Fletcher, & True, 1988;
(Parson & Far, 1981). The problem with such a Schwartz, Gruenewald, Klitzner, & Fedio,
conceptualization is that multiple factors may 1989). Additional research (Pope & Yurgelun-
cause low scores on neuropsychological and intel- Todd, 1996) suggested there were residual
ligence tests. When multiple factors are involved neuropsychological effects on memory and
selecting a single factor as the only cause is very executive functioning at least for a period of
poor logic. The particular problem with drug 6 weeks. Bolla, Eldreth, Matochik and Cadet
abuse research is that because of the health pro- (2005) noted short-term (25 days abstinence)
blems associated with drugs of abuse it is not decision-making deficits and decreased
610 CHAPTER 24

activation in the left medial orbital frontal cortex deficits in a United States sample of children of
and increased activation in the cerebellum. mothers who used cannabis.
Grant, Gonzalez, Carey, Natarajan and
Wolfson (2003) conducted an excellent quantita-
tive synthesis of empirical research (meta-analysis) Hallucinogens/LSD
pertaining to the residual neuropsychological
effects of cannabis in adult human subjects. Research studies of hallucinogens/LSD
After screening 1,014 studies for methodological have not identified marked residual neuropsy-
flaws, they found 15 studies that were methodo- chological deficits. Early studies by McGlothlin,
logically adequate and which provided data on Arnold and Freeman (1969) and Acord and
704 cannabis users and 484 non-users. Grant et Baker (1973) presented initial research findings
al. (2003) calculated effects sizes for eight neu- that suggested possible visual abstraction and
ropsychological ability domains (motor ability, concept formation deficits. These neuropsycho-
executive functioning, learning and memory, logical deficits were extremely mild and may
etc.). Essentially an effects size is a statistical have been due to the pre-existing or co-morbid
method for comparing disparate research stu- risk factors identified by Tarter and Edwards
dies. In the simplest case the means of control (1987). In the period of time since the early stu-
and experimental groups are compared for dif- dies no further replications had been conducted.
ferences using either the standard deviation of Additional well-controlled research that con-
the control group or a pooled standard deviation firmed earlier findings would be necessary before
composed of the standard deviations of both the reaching a conclusion regarding residual neu-
experimental and control groups. In brief, if the ropsychological deficits.
experimental and control groups differ less than
a quarter standard deviation the effect is small,
Ecstasy
half a standard deviation a moderate effect and a
strong effect for a full standard deviation differ-
The drug ‘‘Ecstasy’’ (3, 4-methylenedioxy-
ence. The Grant et al., (2003) research group
methamphetamine or ‘‘MDMA’’) has been con-
found small but significant effects sizes only for
sidered a stimulant but it also has hallucinogenic
learning and forgetting domains. Grant et al. properties which have been more recently appre-
(2003) postulated that where cannabis com- ciated. Ecstasy is different from other hallucino-
pounds are found to have therapeutic value, the gens and research studies have demonstrated that
use of marijuana for therapeutic purposes might Ecstasy can impair memory in abstinent users
have an acceptable margin of safety in medical (Bolla, McCann, & Ricaurte, 1998; Zakzanis &
settings as the effects size of residual neuropsy- Young, 2001). More recent research has demon-
chological deficits was small and limited to a strated learning and memory deficits can persist
single neuropsychological domain. up to a year post-abstain (Parrott, 2001; Rene-
The quantitative meta-analysis described man, Booj, Majoie, van den Brink, & den Hee-
above was limited to adult human subjects ten, 2001). In addition, use of Ecstasy was
only. There is no comparable meta-analysis for associated with task-switching and the degree
children unfortunately. It has been argued that of neuropsychological deficit was related to the
children and adolescents who are in the process extent of drug usage (Dafters, 2006).
of development and undergoing neuropsycholo-
gical developmental changes, however, may be
more vulnerable than adults to the residual neu- Opiates
ropsychological deficits effects of cannabis. For
example, Fried and Smith (2001) have published Research studies on opiates and residual
research that concluded that executive dysfunc- neuropsychological deficits are contradictory.
tion was found in children of mothers who An early study with heroin users in a Veterans
abused small amounts of cannabis in a Canadian Administration Medical Center by Fields and
sample. Similarly, Goldschmidt, Day and Fullerton (1975) found no evidence for residual
Richardson (2000) published research that con- neuropsychological deficits. Rounsaville,
cluded there were residual neuropsychological Novelly, Kleber, and Jones (1982) at Yale
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 611

University published research that suggested and the study used a small sample size and has
heroin addicts who also were polydrug users not been replicated. Indeed, a PCP organic men-
had neuropsychological deficits and addicts tal disorder category that existed in DSM–III-R
with the most impairment had childhood his- was dropped from (APA, 2000). Few conclu-
tories of hyperactivity and a poorer academic sions can be confidently drawn regarding the
record. The next year, however, the same potential effects of PCP use on neuropsycholo-
research group (Rounsaville, Jones, Novelly, gical functioning.
and Kleber, 1982) who earlier found neuropsy-
chological impairment conducted a follow-up
study (using the same heroin addict subjects as Cocaine
in 1981 study) also found their heroin addict
users performed better than demographically In an early study, O’Malley and Gawin
similar controls on neuropsychological tests. (1990) found a mild degree of neuropsychologi-
One possibility is the first study failed to ade- cal impairment in chronic cocaine users but the
quately control for demographic and premorbid level of deficits found was mild. More recent
factors. Another possibility could be that heroin studies (Bolla, Funderberk, & Cadet, 2000;
addicts, like professional boxers, are a physiolo- Simon et al., 2002; Strickland et al., 1993; Van
gically superior group and even with a degree of Gorp et al., 1999) clearly document residual
neuropsychological impairment they are more neuropsychological deficits with respect to
able than normal individuals. There has been cocaine abusers. Jovanovski, Erb, and Zakzanis
little subsequent research on residual neuropsy- (2005) note that neuropsychological abilities
chological impairment in heroin addicts. with respect to attention, executive functioning,
working memory and declarative memory were
the most frequently observed pattern of residual
Sedatives neuropsychological deficits.
There have also been neuroimaging find-
In an early study, Judd and Grant (1975) ings that support the notion of residual neuro-
reported residual neuropsychological deficits psychological deficits. For example, Strickland
among sedative abusers but their patients had et al. (1993) correlated the results of neuropsy-
also been abusers of stimulants, alcohol and opi- chological with single photon emission com-
ates. Bergman, Borg, and Holm (1980) found puterized tomography (SPECT) findings. In
residual neuropsychological deficits in subjects addition, Volkow et al. (1992) found decreased
that were treated only for illicit sedative abuse blood flow in the frontal lobes of cocaine users.
and better controlled for polydrug abuse. Allen Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users were
and Landis (1998) note the fact that elderly indi- suggested to be due to strokes and seizures
viduals are at greater risk for adverse reactions (Volkow, Mullani, Gould, Adler, & Krajewski,
such as confusion and delirium and neuropsy- 1988).
chological impairment. The evidence for organic
deficits is so well accepted that (APA, 2000)
contains a category for sedative–hypnotic, Stimulants
amnestic impairment.
Animal research has suggested the possibi-
lity of residual neuropsychological deficits in
Phencyclidine (PCP) humans due to stimulants but prior studies failed
to demonstrate deficits (Reed & Grant, 1990).
PCP or ‘‘angel dust’’ has been reported to Recent human studies (Kalechstein, Newton, &
provoke violent uncontrollable psychotic out- Green, 2003; Nordahl, Salo, & Leamon, 2003)
bursts in the absence of an external threat. have indicated neuropsychological deficits in
Research studies have not found strong evidence attention executive functions and memory are
for residual neuropsychological deficits in PCP related to methamphetamine use. Verbal mem-
users. In one of the few studies on PCP, Carlin, ory impairment in methamphetamine addicts
Grant, Adams, and Reed (1979) found very has been noted to be due to poor verbal encoding
subtle evidence for neuropsychological deficits and retrieval strategies (Woods et al. (2005).
612 CHAPTER 24

In addition, Thompson et al. (2004) found struc- impairment in 50% of a sample of polydrug
tural abnormalities in the brains of human sub- users. In addition, Judd and Grant (1975) pro-
jects who have used methamphetamine. vided evidence for impairment of neuropsycho-
logical functioning. There is concern, however,
that polydrug users who demonstrate neuropsy-
Inhalants/Solvents chological deficits suffer from possible con-
founds with respect to risk factors that are
There is strong evidence for residual neu- medical and psychiatric in nature. Some of the
ropsychological deficits after exposure to inha- poly drug users studied also abused alcohol and
lants/solvents. One of the first studies was a case the effects seen are more due to alcohol con-
report by Bigler (1979) which found a general- sumption than specific drug effects but others
ized pattern of neuropsychological deficits. have suggested greater impairment when cocaine
A subsequent study by Korman, Matthews and and alcohol are combined (Bolla et al., 2000).
Lovett (1981) found clear neuropsychological Additional research is needed to address this
impairment by inhalant abusers. Korman et al. issue.
(1981) found that inhalant abusers were signifi-
cantly more impaired than other drug abusers
with clear neuropsychological effects found on Summary
both global measures (such as intelligence and
achievement measures) and more specific tests As noted by Spencer and Boren (1990) there
(such as perception of speech sound, sensory are residual neuropsychological deficits from
perception and set shifting ability). The nature repeated drug abuse. Nonetheless, the magni-
of the neuropsychological deficits found by tude of effects is subtle. Future research should
Korman et al. (1981) suggested that inhalant investigate subcortical effects of the action of
abuse produces neuropsychological deficits that drug abuse on the brain rather than use instru-
are severe and widespread. Similarly, Tsushina ments that were developed to focus on cortical
and Towne (1977) found glue sniffers were neu- functioning. As the most common pattern of
ropsychologically impaired. Moreover, Berry, deficits found are on measures of attention,
Heaton, and Kirley (1977) found a group of memory and executive functioning, very subtle
chronic inhalant abusers impaired on assessments of memory, attention and executive
neuropsychological tests. Exposure to organic sol- functioning are needed.
vents has also been shown to cause long-term In terms of general conclusions regarding
neuropsychological impairment on measures of residual neuropsychological deficits, it is clear
speed of processing, memory and verbal fluency that
(Wood and Liossi (2005). Another study (Rosen-
1) These neuropsychological deficits are
berg, Grigsby, Dreisbach, Busenbark, & Grigsby,
very subtle.
2002) found that solvent abusers performed worse
2) They do not impair gross language,
than other drug abusers on neuropsychological
motor functioning or sensory– perceptual
tests of working memory and executive cognitive
functioning.
functions. Interestingly, Rosenberg et al., 2002)
3) They involve higher levels neurocognitive
also found a dose– response relationship between
abilities such as attention, short-term
solvent abuse and MRI findings. Consistent and
memory and executive functions such as
strong research findings make it possible to con-
visual abstract problem solving and com-
clude that inhalants/solvents cause residual neu-
plex concept formation.
ropsychological impairment.
It may very well be that some drug abusers
can perform relatively well at low level positions
Polydrug Abuse or relatively non-demanding social situations,
yet they will show pronounced problems with
Polydrug abusers are more the norm than mentally demanding employment tasks or in
an exception in drug abuse research. An early complex social situations.
national study by Grant, Mohns, Miller, and This chapter has briefly discussed brain
Reitan (1976) demonstrated neuropsychological structures and processes underlying addictive
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 613

behaviors. Difficulties involved in assessing the concluded that a specific drug of abuse would
residual neuropsychological effects of various cause residual neuropsychological deficits.
psycho–active substances have been alluded to
and a very selective review of neuropsychologi-
cal test results with drug addicts has been pre- References
sented. The current research with respect to the
neuropsychological effects of abused drugs is Acord, L. D., & Barker, D. D. (1973). Hallucinogenic drugs
composed of a small number of flawed studies and cerebral deficit. Journal of Nervous and Mental
so there is ample room for additional research. Diseases, 156, 281–283.
A crucial research issue is that of the criteria Allen, D. N., & Landis, R. K. B. (1998). Neuropsychological
to accept that exposure to a drug may cause correlates of substance abuse disorders. In P. J. Snyder &
residual neuropsychological deficits in human P. D. Nussbaum (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology: A
beings. In the past, researchers were willing to pocket handbook for assessment (pp. 591–612).Washing-
conclude that a drug may cause residual neurop- ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and sta-
sychological deficits simply based on a level of
tistical manual of mental disorders. (4th ed.) Test Revi-
performance model. Many researchers would sion. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
accept that a drug of abuse causes residual neu- Association.
ropsychological deficits if there are low scores on Benedict, R. H. B., & Horton, A. M. Jr. (1992). Neuropsy-
various neuropsychological and intelligence chology of alcohol induced brain damage: Current per-
tests. The conceptualization is flawed because spectives on diagnosis, recovery, and prevention, In D.
multiple factors may cause low scores on neuro- Templer, L. Hartlage, & W. G. Cannon (Eds.), Preven-
psychological and intelligence tests. When multi- table brain damage: Brain vulnerability and brain health
(pp. 146–160). New York: Springer
ple factors are involved selecting a single factor
Bergman, H; Borg, S., & Holm, L. (1980). Neuropsycholo-
as the only cause is very poor logic. The major gical impairments and the exclusive abuse of sedatives
problem with drug abuse research with human or hypnotics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137,
beings is that because of the health problems 215–217.
associated with drugs of abuse it is not possible Berry, G. Heaton, R. & Kirley, M. (1977) Neuropsychologi-
to use true random research designs. Rather, cal deficits of chronic inhalant abusers. In B. Rumaek &
only quasi-experimental research designs using A. Temple (Eds.), Management of the prisoned patient
non-equivalent control groups can be utilized. (pp. 59–83). Princeton: Sciences press.
Bigler, E. D. (1979) Neuropsychological evaluation of ado-
Therefore, all drug abuse research with human
lescent patients hospitalized with chronic inhalant
beings has limited control of potentially con- abuse. Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 8–12.
founding variables. The point for concluding Bolla, K. I., McCann, D. U., & Ricaurte, G. A. (1998)
that a specific drug of abuse may cause residual Memory impairment in abstinent MDMA (‘‘ecstasy’’)
neuropsychological deficits is that low neurop- users. Neurology, 51, 1532–1537.
sychological and intelligence test scores may be Bolla, K. I., Funderberk, F. R., & Cadet, J. L. (2000). Differ-
due to another factor that exposure to a specific ential effects of cocaine and cocaine alcohol on neuro-
drug. In neuropsychological testing, it is well cognitive performance. Neurology, 54, 2285–2299.
Bolla, K. I., Eldreth, D. A., Matochik, & Cadet, J. L. (2005).
established that there are four methodological
Neural substrates of faulty decision-making in absti-
methods of inference (Reitan & Wolfson, 1993), nent marijuana users. Neuroimage, 26, 480–492.
such as level of performance, patterns of impair- Carlin, A. S., & Trupin E. (1977). The effects of long-term
ment, specific deficits and left–right compari- chronic cannabis use on neuropsychological function-
sons. The usual criterion used to conclude there ing. International Journal of Addiction, 12, 617–624.
are residual neuropsychological deficits follow- Carlin, A., Grant, I, Adams, K., & Reed, R. (1979). Is
ing exposure to drugs of abuse is level of perfor- phencyclidine (PCP) abuse associated with organic
mance or, in other words, low scores on mental impairment? American Journal of Drug and
neuropsychological and intelligence tests. A bet- Alcohol Abuse, 6, 273–281.
Dafters, R. I., (2006). Chronic ecstasy (MDMA) use is asso-
ter model would require that the other meth- ciated with deficits in task-switching but not inhibition
odologies of inference of neuropsychological of memory updating executive functions. Drug and
testing (Reitan & Wolfson, 1993) such as pat- Alcohol Dependence, 83, 181–184.
terns of impairment, specific deficits and left–- Fastenau, P. S. (1998). Validity of regression-based norms:
right comparisons be satisfied before it can be an empirical test of the comprehensive Norms with
614 CHAPTER 24

Older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu- Hubbard, R. L., Marsden, M. E., Rachal, J. V., Harwood, H.
ropsychology, 20, 906–916. J., Cavanaugh, E. R., et al. (1989).Drug abuse treat-
Fastenau, P. S., & Adams, K. M. (1996). Heaton, Grant, and ment: A national study of effectiveness. Chapel Hill:
Matthews’s comprehensive norms: An overzealous University of North Carolina Press.
attempt. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- Jovanovski, D., Erb, S., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2005). Neuro-
psychology, 18, 444–448. cognitive deficits in cocaine users: a quantitative review
Fields, F. S., & Fullerton, J. (1975). Influences of heroin of the evidence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
addiction on neuropsychological functioning. Journal Neuropsychology, 27, 189–204.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 114. Judd, L. L., & Grant, I. (1975). Brain dysfunction in chronic
Fried, P. A., & Smith A. M. (2001). A literature review of the sedative users. Journal of Psychedelic Drugs, 7, 143–149.
consequences of prenatal marijuana exposure: An Kalechstein, A. D., Newton, T. F., & Green, M. (2003).
emerging these of a deficiency in executive functioning. Methamphetamine dependence is associated with neu-
Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 23, 1–11. rocognitive impairment in the initial phases of absti-
Goldschmidt, L., Day, N. L., & Richardson, G. A. (2000). nence. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
Effect of prenatal marijuana exposure on child behavior Neurosciences, 15, 215–220.
problems at age 10. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, Korman, M., Matthews, R. W., & Lovitt, R. (1981). Neu-
22, 325–336. ropsychological effects of abuse on inhalants. Percep-
Grant, I, Rochford, J, Fleming, & Stunkard, A (1973). tual & Motor Skills, 53, 547–553.
A Neuropsychological assessment of the effects of mod- McGlothlin, W. H., Arnold, D. D., & Freedman, D. X.
erate marijuana use. Journal of Nervous and Mental (1969). Organicity measures following repeated LSD
Disease, 156, 278–280. ingestion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 704–709.
Grant, I, Mohns, L., Miller, M., & Reitan, R (1976). Mendelson, J. H. & Meyer, R. E. (1972). Behavioral and
A neuropsychological study of polydrug users. Archives biological concomitants of chronic marijuana smoking
of General Psychiatry, 33, 973–978. by heavy and casual users. In Marijuana: A signal of
Grant, I, Gonzales, Carey, C., Natarajan, L., & Wolfson, T. misunderstanding (Vol. 1). Washington, DC: U.S.
(2003). Non-acute (residual) Neurocognitive effects of Government Printing Office.
cannabis use: a meta-analytic study. Journal of the Nordahl, T. E., Salo, R., & Leamon, M. (2003). Neuropsy-
International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 679–689. chological effects of chronic methamphetamine use on
Harwood, H. J., Fountain, D., Livermore, G. et al. (1998). neurotransmitters and cognition: A review. Journal of
Economic costs to society of alcohol and drug abuse and Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 15,
mental illness: 1992. Washington, DC: National Insti- 317–325.
tute on Drug Abuse and National Institute on Alcohol O’Malley, S. S., & Gawin, F. H. (1990). Abstinence sympto-
and Alcohol Abuse. matology and neuropsychological deficits in chronic
Heaton, R. K., Miller, S. W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I (2004). cocaine abusers. In J. W. Spencer & J. J. Boren (Eds.),
Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead- Residual effects of abused drugs on behavior
Reitan battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsycholo- (pp. 179–190). National Institute on Drug Abuse
gical norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Research Monograph 101, OHHS Pub. No (ADM)
Lutz, FL: Psychological assessment Resources. 90–1719. Washington, DC: Supt. of Docs., U.S. Gov’t
Horton, A. M., Jr. (1993). Future directions in the develop- Printing Office.
ment of Addiction assessment instruments. In B. J. Page, J. B., Fletcher, J., & True, W. P. (1988). Psychosocio-
Rounsaville, F. M. Tims, A. M. Horton, Jr., & B. J. cultural perspectives on chronic cannabis use: the Costa
Sowder (Eds.), Diagnostic source book on drug abuse Rican followup. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 20,
research and treatment (pp. 87–92) Rockville, Mary- 57–65.
land: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Parrott, A. C. (2001). Human psychopharmacology of
Horton, A. M., Jr. (1996). Neuropsychology of drug abuse. Ecstasy (MDMA): A review of 15 years f empirical
In R. J. Sbordone & C. J. Long (Eds.), Ecological research. Human Psychopharmacology, 16, 557–577.
validity of neuropsychological testing (pp. 357–368). Parson, O. A., & Farr, S. P. (1981). The neuropsychology of
Delray Beach, Fla.: GR Press/St. Lucie Press. alcohol and drug use. In S. Filskov & T. J. Boll (Eds.),
Horton, A. M. Jr. (1999). Prediction of brain damage sever- The handbook of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 320–365).
ity: Demographic corrections. International Journal of New York: Wiley.
Neuroscience. 97, 179–183. Pope, H. G., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (1996). The residual
Horton, A. M. Jr., & Fogelman, C. J. (1991). Clinical beha- cognitive effects of heavy marijuana use in college stu-
vioral therapy with the addicted elderly client. In P. A. dents. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Wisocki (Ed.), Handbook of clinical behavioral therapy for 275, 521–527.
the elderly client (pp. 299–315). New York: Plenum Press. Reed, R., & Grant, I. (1990). The long term neurobehavioral
Horton, A. M. Jr., & Wedding, D. (1984). Introduction to consequence of substance abuse: conceptual and meth-
clinical and behavioral neuropsychology. New York: odological challenge for future research. In J. W. Spencer &
Praeger. J. J. Boren (Eds.), Residual effects of abused drugs on
CHILD CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF DRUG ABUSE 615

behavior. Research Monograph 101 (pp 10–56). Rock- Schwartz, R. H., Gruenewald, P. J., Klitzner, M., & Fedio, P.
ville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. (1989). Short–term memory impairment in cannabis–-
Reitan, R. M. (1967). Psychological changes associated with dependent adolescents. American Journal of Diseases of
aging and cerebral damage. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, Children, 143, 1214–1219.
42, 653–673. Simon, S. L., Domier, C. P., Sim, T., Richardson, K, Raw-
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead–Reitan son, R. A., & Ling, W. (2002). Cognitive performance
Neuropsychological Battery (2 nd ed.,). Tucson, AZ: of current methamphetamine and cocaine abusers.
Neuropsychology Press. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 21, 61–71.
Reitan R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995). Influence of age and Spencer, J. W., & Boren, J. J. (1990). Residual effects of
education on neuropsychological test results. The Clin- abused drugs in behavior. Research Monograph 101,
ical Neuropsychologist, 9, 151–158. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2005). The effect of age and Strickland, T. L., Mena, I., Villanueva–Meyer, J., Miller, B.,
education transformations on neuropsychological test Mehringer, C. M., Satz, P., & Myers, H. (1993) Cere-
scores of persons with diffuse or bilateral brain damage. bral perfusion and neuropsychological consequences of
Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 181–189. chronic cocaine use. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
Reneman, L., Booij, J., Majoie, C. B. L. M., van den Brink, Clinical Neurosciences, 5, 419–427.
W., & den Heeten, G. J. (2001). Investigating the poten- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administra-
tial neurotoxicity of Ecstasy (MDMD): An imaging tion (2006). Results from the 2005 National Survey on
approach. Human Psychopharmacology, 16, 579–588. Drug Use and Health (Office of Applied Studies,
Rosenberg, N. L., Grigsby, J., Dreisbach, J., Busenbark, D., & NHSDA Series H-30, DHHS Publication No. SMA
Grigdby, P. (2002). Neuropsychological impairment 06-4194). Rockville, MD.
and MRI abnormalities associated with chronic solvent Van Gorp, W. G., Wilkins, J. N., Hinkin, C. H., Moore, L. H.,
abuse. Journal of Toxicology and Clinical Toxicology, Horner, M. D., & Plotkin, D. (1999). Declarative and
40(1), 21–34. procedural memory functioning in abstinent cocaine
Rounsaville, B. J., Novelly, R. A., & Kleber, H. D. (1980). abusers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 85–89.
Neuropsychological impairments in opiate addicts: Volkow, N. D., Mullani, N., Gould, K. L., Adler, S., &
Risk factors. Annuals of the New York Academy of Krajewski, K. (1988). Cerebral blood flow in chronic
Science, 362, 79–80. cocaine users: A study with positron omission tomo-
Rounsaville, B. J., Jones, C. Novelly, R. A., & Kleber, H. D. graphy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 641–648.
(1982). Neuropsychological functioning in opiate addicts. Volkow, N. D., Hitzmann, R., Wang, G. J., Fowler, J. S.,
Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 170, 209–216. Wolf, A. P., Dewey, S. L., et al. (1992). Long-term
Tarter, R. E., & Edwards, K. L. (1987). Brief and compre- frontal metabolic changes in cocaine abusers. Synapse,
hensive neuropsychological assessment of alcohol and 11, 184–190.
substance abuse. In L. C. Hartlage, J. L. Hornsby, & Wood, R. L., & Liossi, C. (2005). Long-term neuropsycho-
M. J. Asken, (Eds.), Essentials of neuropsychological logical impact of brief exposure to organic solvents.
assessment (pp. 138–162), New York: Springer. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 20(5), 655–665.
Thompson, P. M., Hayashi, K. M., Simon, S. L., Geaga, J. A., Woods, S. P., Rippeth, J. D., Conover, E., Gonzalez, R.,
Hong, M. S., Sui, Y., et al. (2004). Structural abnormal- Cherner, M., Heaton, R. K., (2005). Deficient strategic
ities in the brains of human subjects who have used control of verbal encoding and retrieval in individuals
methamphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, with methamphetamine dependence. Neuropsychology,
6028–6036. 19, 35–43.
Tsushina, W., & Towne, W. (1977). Effects of paint sniffing Zakzanis, K. K., & Young, D. A. (2001). Memory impair-
on neuropsychological test performance. Journal of ment in abstinent MDMA (‘‘ecstasy’’) users: a longitu-
Abnormal Psychology, 86, 402–407. dinal investigation. Neurology, 56(7), 966–969.

You might also like