Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

+Model

NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS


Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019) xxx, xxx—xxx
1

Disponible en ligne sur

ScienceDirect
www.sciencedirect.com

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

2 Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory


3 processing strategies
4 Francisco Javier Alvarado-Rodríguez a, Rebeca Romo-Vázquez b,
5 Geisa Bearitz Gallardo-Moreno a, Hugo Vélez-Pérez b,
6 Andrés Antonio González-Garrido a,∗,1

a
7 Instituto de Neurociencias, CUCBA, Universidad de Guadalajara, 180 Francisco de Quevedo, 44130,
8 Guadalajara, Jalisco, Mexico
b
9 Departamento de Ciencias Computacionales, CUCEI, Universidad de Guadalajara, 1421 Boulevard
10 Marcelino García Barragán, 44430, Guadalajara, Jalisco, Mexico

11 Received 9 June 2019; accepted 30 September 2019

12 KEYWORDS Summary
13 Cognition; Background. — Type 1 diabetes (T1D) is a metabolic disorder characterized by recurrent hypo-
14 EEG; and hyperglycemic episodes, whose clinical development has been associated with cognitive
15 Emotion; and working memory (WM) deficits.
16 qEEG; Objective. — To contrast quantitative electroencephalography (qEEG) measures between young
17 Type 1 diabetes; patients with T1D and healthy controls while performing a visuospatial WM task with two
18 Working memory memory load levels and facial emotional stimuli.
19 Methods. — Four or five neutral or happy faces were sequentially and pseudo-randomly pre-
20 sented in different spatial locations, followed by subsequent sequences displaying the reversed
21 spatial order or any other. Participants were instructed to discriminate between these two
22 alternatives during EEG recording.
23 Results. — A significant increase in the absolute power of the delta and theta bands, distributed
24 mainly over the frontal region was found during task execution, with a slight decrease of alpha
25 band power in both groups but mainly in control individuals. However, these changes were more
26 pronounced in the T1D patients, and reached their maximum level during the WM encoding
27 phase, even on trials with the lower memory load. In contrast, changes seemed to occur more
28 gradually in controls and results differed significantly only on the trials with the higher WM load.
29 Conclusions. — These results reflect adaptive WM-processing mechanisms in which cognitive
30 strategies have evolved in T1D patients in order to meet task demands.
31 © 2019 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.
32


Corresponding author at: Instituto de Neurociencias, Universidad de Guadalajara, Francisco de Quevedo 180, Colonia Arcos Vallarta,
Guadalajara, Jalisco, 44130, Mexico.
E-mail address: gonzalezgarrido@gmail.com (A.A. González-Garrido).
1 http://www.ineuro.cucba.udg.mx/.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
0987-7053/© 2019 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 F.J. Alvarado-Rodríguez et al.

33 Introduction tional adaptive changes as means to maintain cognitive 91

performance, despite the occurrence of glycemic level 92

34 Type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1D) is a disease in which the extremes. Similar to Bolo and colleagues [9], they found no 93

35 beta cells of the pancreatic islets, which produce insulin, statistically-significant differences in the behavioral perfor- 94

36 are attacked and destroyed by the immune system [2]. mance between T1D patients and healthy matched controls 95

37 Insulin plays an important role in metabolizing glucose and, while performing a visuospatial WM task. However, their 96

38 therefore, has an essential function in regulating the energy patients showed greater fMRI activations in the inferior 97

39 supply of the brain, an organ that consumes about 25% of the frontal gyrus, basal ganglia, substantia nigra, and cere- 98

40 glucose available in the body [47]. As a result, T1D is charac- bellum. These subcortical activations, in addition to the 99

41 terized by recurrent hypo- and hyperglycemic episodes that cortical activations usually required for WM processing, 100

42 have a negative impact on the brain. These glycemic fluc- were interpreted as reflecting a compensation mechanism 101

43 tuations have also been related to cognitive deficits that that allowed those patients to maintain a behavioral perfor- 102

44 affect executive functions [23], especially working memory mance similar to that of controls. In accordance with these 103

45 [19,59]. results, and applying the same methodology, but in an effec- 104

46 Executive functions play a pivotal role in goal-directed tive connectivity study, Guàrdia-Olmos and colleagues [28] 105

47 behavior. One core executive function is working memory reported that T1D patients showed a pattern of brain con- 106

48 (WM), which operates by holding information in the mind nectivity distinct from that of healthy controls. In the T1D 107

49 and mentally working with it [4]. WM can be sub-divided group, fewer brain areas were involved in the WM task and 108

50 into several main processing phases: there was a notable lack of involvement of the frontal cor- 109

tical areas, while the red nucleus and cerebellum showed 110

strong connectivity with the core WM network. 111


51 • initial encoding of information; Embury and colleagues [18] also focused on young 112
52 • subsequent maintenance and retrieval of WM items [57]. patients without comorbid conditions and evaluated them 113

while in the normoglycemic range. They argued that this 114

53 Encoding and maintaining visuospatial information in approach would make it possible to reach stronger conclu- 115

54 WM have been associated with increased recruitment sions concerning the unique impact of diabetes on brain 116

55 of attention-associated brain areas, including parietal physiology. Interestingly, they reported no between-group 117

56 and frontal cortices [20,44]. However, when dealing with differences on accuracy rates on a Sternberg-type verbal WM 118

57 different WM load levels, it seems that prefrontal cor- task performed during a magnetoencephalography (MEG) 119

58 tex activity varies depending on the task phase [45]. In study. However, they did find that the T1D patients exhib- 120

59 general, there is agreement on the fact that WM is neces- ited stronger responses in the superior parietal area during 121

60 sary for language comprehension, translating instructions, encoding and diminished alpha response in the parieto- 122

61 incorporating new information into action plans, reason- occipital cortices during the maintenance phase. These 123

62 ing, creativity, and making plans and decisions [16]. Despite authors suggested that this pattern of neural dynamics in 124

63 the growing literature on WM development, its processing their young patients may reflect a compensatory strategy 125

64 mechanisms and primary neurofunctional substrates, the for decreased neural resources or efficiency by using other 126

65 precise mechanisms underlying WM impairments are still network resources, and that this compensation was similar 127

66 poorly understood. to that observed in an aging population. Another key point 128

67 One of the problems linking T1D to WM deficits is that for research in this area consists of examining the differ- 129

68 most of the studies approaching this issue have focused ent phases of WM processing (encoding vs. maintenance) 130

69 on the consequences of glycemic level extremes, often that allows more subtle between-group differences to be 131

70 inducing hypoglycemia to evaluate its effects on control identified. 132

71 participants [1,9,24,59]. As expected, both patients with While fMRI studies have provided valuable information on 133

72 T1D and control individuals are usually affected by induced specific brain pattern differences between patients with T1D 134

73 hypoglycemia in these studies, though the poorer cognitive and healthy controls during WM tasks [9,22], delineating the 135

74 performance of T1D patients is emphasized. In contrast, more active brain areas during each WM processing phase 136

75 Bolo and colleagues [9] failed to find significant differences has been more difficult due to the temporal limitations of 137

76 between patients and controls on accuracy or reaction times this technique. Electroencephalography (EEG), given its high 138

77 while performing a WM task under euglycemic or hypo- accuracy measured in milliseconds, might in contrast reflect 139

78 glycemic conditions. These authors also reported that brain differences in both phases of WM processing; that is, encod- 140

79 regions with a supplementary role in WM function, such as ing and maintenance. Although Embury et al. [18] studied 141

80 the parietal lobe and cerebellum, presented greater fMRI- this issue using MEG in T1D patients, to the best of our knowl- 142

81 BOLD activation in patients during hypoglycemia, suggesting edge, no studies have yet addressed the effects of emotional 143

82 that these areas may have been recruited to help maintain distractors on WM performance in these patients using an 144

83 cognitive performance. electrophysiological approach. 145

84 Other approaches, which did not involve inducing hypo- It is well-known that emotional events influence WM, as 146

85 glycemia, have suggested that young adults with T1D—but occurs in daily life [36,46], and there is compelling evi- 147

86 with average IQ and without comorbidities—already show dence that processes such as attention, decision-making, 148

87 neural processing that is distinct from that of their peers cognitive control, and WM all play central roles in emo- 149

88 without diabetes [18,22]. In this regard, Gallardo-Moreno tion, and that cognition and emotion dynamically influence 150

89 and collaborators [22] argued that the early impact of each other in ways that contribute to adaptive behavior 151

90 this disease during neurodevelopment could lead to func- [17,50]. Understanding the mechanisms underlying these 152

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
Type-1 diabetes affects cognitive strategies 3

153 reciprocal interactions is critical for comprehending the fun-


Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of study
154 damentals of healthy functioning. For instance, the stress
participants.
155 caused by glycemic dysregulation, daily insulin injections,
156 a special diet and regular medical checkups in T1D patients Controls T1D patients
157 could interact with cognitive deficits reported in this popula-
158 tion, particularly those affecting executive functions, which n 18 18
159 potentially play a key role in self-management and treat- Age (years) 21.61 (3.94) 22.22 (4.08)
160 ment adherence [11]. Sex (men/women) 11/7 11/7
161 Recently, our group explored WM processing in T1D Education (years) 12.17 (2.43) 11.53 (2.47)
162 patients while manipulating emotional stimuli through fMRI IQ 111.80 (7.41) 106.06 (10.04)
163 methods, using a 2-back task detecting facial identity in HbA1c (%) — 8.64 (1.72)
164 neutral, happy or fearful faces [25]. Significant differences Last fasting plasma — 132.72(48.42)
165 were found between the neurofunctional activation pat- glucose
166 terns of T1D patients and healthy controls, particularly Plasma glucose 87.83 (9.31) 237.11 (106.45)
167 characterized by greater prefrontal activation in patients (before EEG
168 when processing emotional faces, while controls exhibited recording)
169 more activation in parietal regions. The observed group Data are means (standard deviations). n: number of cases;
170 differences were interpreted as the result of a specific inter- HbA1c : glycated hemoglobin.
171 ference effect of the emotional content of the stimuli on
172 WM’s cognitive control in the patient group. Such inter- intention of maximizing their attractiveness, in the context 215
173 ference could elicit attention allocation difficulties and of two different WM load levels. Based on previous studies, 216
174 decrease the operational efficiency of WM. In the light of we hypothesize that the encoding stage of WM processing 217
175 these findings, two additional issues should be further clari- in T1D patients will be more susceptible to the impact of 218
176 fied in this clinical population: the effect of varying levels of highly attention-appealing stimuli, particularly when WM 219
177 working memory load, and the sensitivity of distinct stages load increases. 220
178 of WM processing in the time domain. With the above mentioned purpose, we set out to 221
179 Jin and colleagues [36] found significant effects of evaluate the effect of WM load and two highly attentional- 222
180 emotional content on WM task performance and ERP distracting stimuli on qEEG measures while T1D patients 223
181 correlates with encoding, maintenance and retrieval pro- and controls performed visuospatial WM tasks (distinguishing 224
182 cesses. Specifically, they reported that positive emotions encoding and maintenance/retrieval phases), and compare 225
183 improved WM performance by facilitating maintenance and those results to a task-preceding resting period. 226
184 retrieval processing, while negative content exhibited an
185 enhancement effect only during encoding and maintenance.
186 Guimond and colleagues [29], meanwhile, examined how Methods 227

187 happy and fearful emotional distractors impact performance


188 on WM tasks by varying the cognitive load using the fMRI Participants 228

189 technique. They found that patients with schizophrenia had


190 greater difficulty in performing a WM task than healthy Individuals diagnosed with T1D were recruited from two 229

191 controls when regulating emotions, and suggested that the associations that specialize in treating diabetes mellitus: 230

192 increase in WM load may have placed a high demand on sub- the Endocrinology Services of the Centro Médico Nacional 231

193 jects’ executive networks and interfered with their ability de Occidente and the Fray Antonio Alcalde Hospital. The 232

194 to regulate emotional distractors. These findings led us to sample consisted of 18 T1D patients with at least 8 years 233

195 hypothesize that this could also occur in patients with T1D. of disease evolution who reported adequate adherence 234

196 We agree with Embury and colleagues’ hypothesis [18] that to treatment and optimal daily glucose control, and 18 235

197 the compensatory strategies observed in aging are likely to healthy control individuals matched according to gender, 236

198 also be found in T1D patients. Moreover, Hou and colleagues age and educational level. All participants were right- 237

199 [32] recorded EEGs from healthy adults (young and seniors) handed. Their age range was 14—26 years, and they had 238

200 under three different conditions: resting state, 0-back task, completed at least 9 years of schooling. The T1D patients 239

201 and 2-back task. They found that age-related disturbances (11 males) had a mean age of 21.61 (± 3.94) years (mean 240

202 were more prominent during the high-load task, especially schooling: 11.53 ± 2.47 years), while the matched controls 241

203 in the theta band, and suggested that this band plays an had a mean age of 22.22 (± 4.08) years (mean schooling: 242

204 integrative role on WM tasks that could be attributed to com- 12.17 ± 2.43 years). Table 1 shows the demographic charac- 243

205 pensatory activation in aging. Therefore, it seems that the teristics of the study participants. 244

206 study of different memory loads could reflect subtle differ- All participants had an average IQ (90—110; Wechsler 245

207 ences in brain dynamics between T1D patients and healthy Adult Intelligence Scale: WAIS-IV), and had no antecedents 246

208 participants. In our view, the approach used by Guimond of neurodevelopmental, neurological or psychiatric dis- 247

209 and colleagues [29] and Hou and colleagues [32] to explore orders. According to medical records, the T1D patients 248

210 WM disturbances in this population merits further evalua- had no hospital admissions due to diabetes in the 2 years 249

211 tion. Accordingly, the main aim of the present study was prior to study participation, nor any diabetic complications 250

212 to comparatively explore quantitative EEG measures during (retinopathy, nephropathy or neuropathy). Informed written 251

213 performance of a visuospatial WM task in T1D patients and consent was obtained from participants or their parents, in 252

214 controls, using neutral and happy faces as stimuli, with the the case of minors. The experimental protocol was approved 253

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 F.J. Alvarado-Rodríguez et al.

Figure 1 Example of a sequence with a correct reversed order in the spatial location of the stimuli presentation. Each stimulus
lasted 1000 ms on the screen with variable inter-stimulus intervals and a response time of 1500 ms.

254 by the Ethics Committee of the Neuroscience Institute • resting EEG, which corresponded to a resting period pre- 295

255 (ET112016-226; Universidad de Guadalajara). ceding the task during which subjects simply watched a 296

fixation point displayed in the center of a black back- 297

ground for three minutes; 298

256 Experimental task • visuospatial WM encoding, that is, EEG epochs beginning 299

at the instant when the final stimulus of the first series 300

257 Participants performed a visuospatial working memory task was presented (fourth in the shorter series, fifth in the 301

258 with simultaneous EEG recording. Stimulus delivery and longer ones); 302

259 response collection were controlled by E-prime Studio 2.0 • visuospatial WM maintenance, that is, EEG epochs starting 303

260 software. During experimental sessions, which were always at the instant when the final stimulus of the second series 304

261 scheduled in the morning after having a regular breakfast, was presented (fourth in the shorter series, fifth in the 305

262 participants were seated comfortably in a quiet, dimly-lit longer ones). 306

263 room, 60 cm away from a LCD monitor. They were instructed


264 to focus their attention on a fixation point located in the For practical purposes, from now on we will refer to these 307
265 center of the screen in order to minimize head movements. phases as ‘‘visuospatial encoding’’ and ‘‘visuospatial main- 308
266 The experimental task used neutral (N) and happy (H) faces tenance’’. All EEG epochs selected during these two phases 309
267 as stimuli, which were presented sequentially and pseudo- corresponded only to trials with correct responses and the 310
268 randomly at eight different screen locations. After a short baseline was corrected using each subject’s resting state 311
269 delay of 1000 ms, a second sequence was presented, and average. 312
270 subjects were instructed to press the left button of the
271 mouse if the latter sequence corresponded to the correct
272 inversed spatial order of the first one, but the right but- Artifact rejection 313
273 ton if it did not. Fig. 1 shows the experimental flow chart.
274 The experiment consisted of a total of 120 trials divided into
In order to achieve the maximum number of artifact-free 314
275 three blocks of 40 sequences each, including 60 trials with
EEG epochs per condition, the blind source separation (BSS) 315
276 four stimuli and the other 60 with five, both counterbal-
technique was employed to remove artifacts, which were 316
277 anced across the blocks. Each stimulus lasted 1000 ms with
mainly ocular, according to the methodology presented in 317
278 an inter-stimuli interval of 1000 ms. The presentation order
Romo-Vazquez and colleagues [56]. 318
279 of the blocks was pseudo-randomized.
First, any trial with EOG voltage deflection greater than 319
280 Recognition of the emotional content of the faces used
50 ␮V (relative to baseline) was discarded. Then, an expert 320
281 in this task was previously validated in a group of university
reviewed each EEG recording rejecting the epochs with 321
282 students with the same age range as the participants.
artifacts by visual inspection. Subsequently, EEG segments 322

(20 seconds in length) containing the recordings of interest 323

at the center of the epoch were used for further artifact 324
283 Electrophysiological recordings rejection. Only the source corresponding to the ocular arti- 325

fact was eliminated. Once it was removed, the entire record 326
284 EEGs were recorded from 21 scalp electrodes placed in was rebuilt. Only 11.75% of the data had to be transformed 327
285 accordance with the International 10—20 system (Fp1, Fp2, using the BSS-SOBIRO algorithm [5] due to the presence 328
286 F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, T3, C3, Cz, C4, T4, T5, P3, Pz, P4, T6, of ocular artifacts. In addition, 2% of the total EEG chan- 329
287 O1, O2) and two periocular electrodes (EOG), all referenced nels were lost, so they were removed from the analysis 330
288 to linked earlobes. A 32-channel electroencephalograph and replaced by the average information for the group 331
289 (MEDICID-04 system) with low and high cut filters set at 0.3 involved. Although similar studies have replaced the infor- 332
290 and 30 Hz, respectively, was used. Interelectrode impedance mation from lost channels with their contralateral measures 333
291 was kept below 5 k . EEG data were digitized at a sampling from the same patient [10,34][e.g. 10,34], we did not 334
292 rate of 200 Hz. employ this method because our aim was to analyze EEG 335
293 Artifact-free, 1000-ms EEG epochs were selected by changes associated with cognitive performance, so replac- 336
294 visual inspection in three different main conditions: ing the information on one side with the data recorded in 337

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
Type-1 diabetes affects cognitive strategies 5

Table 2 Behavioral results while performing the experimental task.

Lower WM load Higher WM load


Happy Neutral Happy Neutral

Correct responses (%) Patients 80.74 (13.9) 81.85 (14.9) 80.37 (13.4) 76.30 (15.0)
Controls 89.26 (10.6) 91.67 (7.2) 87.96 (8.8) 82.41 (12.4)
Response times (ms) Patients 541.62 (76.7) 535.52 (86.8) 556.53 (80.9) 550.05 (114.2)
Controls 466.63 (131.3) 458.36 (130.1) 489.25 (137.0) 470.02 (124.2)
Data are means (standard deviations).

338 its contralateral homologous side could have affected the absolute energy of the frequency bands—␦ (1—3 Hz), ␪ 378

339 interpretation of results. (4—7 Hz), ␣ (8—13 Hz) and ␤ (14—30 Hz)—was estimated at 379

each one of the 19 electrode sites. To reduce individual 380

data variability, we analyzed the median of the energy for 381


340 Data analysis
each frequency band by condition (i.e. resting, visuospa- 382

tial encoding, visuospatial maintenance, and two WM load 383


341 All analyses were primarily designed to comparatively eval-
levels), in each subject. 384
342 uate two key effects in T1D patients and healthy controls,
For topographic purposes, and due to the important 385
343 as follows:
role attributed to the frontal (particularly prefrontal) brain 386

region when processing information in WM, EEG power spec- 387


344 • the impact of task performance on quantitative EEG meas- tral measures were evaluated in two main extensive regions: 388
345 ures (absolute power in different frequency bands: delta, frontal (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, F8 and Fz), and posterior 389
346 theta, alpha and beta); (O1, O2, P3, P4, Pz, T5 and T6). First, resting qEEG meas- 390
347 • the impact of memory load while using complex emotional ures were analyzed using a Repeated Measure ANOVA with 391
348 facial stimuli [shorter versus longer sequences; i.e. lower Frequency Band (4 levels: delta, theta, alpha, beta) and 392
349 (LL) versus higher (HL) memory load trials]. Group (2 levels: T1D, Controls) as factors. Subsequently, RM- 393

ANOVAs were executed to analyze qEEG while performing 394


350 The analysis of the particular effects of the emotional the task using three within-subject factors: Frequency Band 395
351 content of the stimuli lies beyond the scope of the present (delta, theta, alpha, beta), Task Phase (Encoding, Mainte- 396
352 study; nonetheless their potential impact on behavioral per- nance), and WM load (lower: LL, higher: HL), along with 397
353 formance was properly tested. Group (T1D, Controls) as the between-subject factor. All 398
354 Due to the comprehensive differences in the blood glu- calculations were performed considering an alpha value of 399
355 cose levels between T1D patients and controls prior to the 0.05. Greenhouse-Geisser corrections to the df were applied 400
356 EEG recording, Pearson correlation analyses were performed as needed, with the corrected probabilities reported. Addi- 401
357 with the purpose of exploring this effect on qEEG measures tionally, post hoc tests were calculated to explore any trends 402
358 in both groups. in the observed changes using Bonferroni adjustments for 403

multiple comparisons. 404

359 Behavioral analysis


Results 405
360 Behavioral responses were computed for both groups and
361 analyzed using SPSS (IBM Corp., 2016 release). Analysis of
362 variance (ANOVA) was performed using Group (2: patients, Behavioral results 406

363 controls) as the between-group factor, with WM load (2: LL:


364 shorter sequences; HL: longer sequences) and Emotion [2: The T1D patients achieved a significantly lower number 407

365 (H: trials with happy faces), (N: trials with neutral faces)] of correct responses than controls (F(1,34) = 4.67; P = 0.038) 408

366 as the within-subject factors, along with the percentage of with prolonged reaction times in all conditions (F(1,34) = 4.47; 409

367 correct responses and response times as dependent varia- P = 0.042). The complete behavioral results are summarized 410

368 bles. in Table 2. In addition, WM load influenced the number of 411

correct responses, as participants had a significantly lower 412

number of correct responses during the trials with the higher 413
369 Quantitative EEGs WM load (F(1,34) = 13.52; P = 0.001). In fact, higher WM load 414

level did not affect the number of correct responses when 415
370 Thirty EEG artifact-free epochs corresponding to correct trials with happy facial expressions were evaluated. The 416
371 responses were selected from each subject and condition, statistics on the behavioral results are shown in Table 3. 417
372 while balancing their distribution into three identical blocks
373 of the experimental stream of stimuli (40 sequences each)
374 to avoid confounding effects like initial behavioral adjust- Electrophysiological results 418

375 ments, the settlement of cognitive strategies, fatigue, etc.


376 For each EEG epoch selected, the power spectrum was Quantitative resting phase EEG measures for anterior and 419

377 calculated using Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT). The posterior brain regions failed to show significant differ- 420

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 F.J. Alvarado-Rodríguez et al.

Table 3 Summary results of the ANOVA for behavioral results.

F df P 2 1-ˇ

Correct responses Load 13.520 1,34 0.001 0.285 0.946


Emotion 3.420 1,34 0.073 0.091 0.435
Group 4.667 1,34 0.038 0.121 0.555
Load*Group 1.066 1,34 0.309 0.030 0.171
Emotion*Group 0.003 1,34 0.956 0.000 0.050
Load*Emotion 8.673 1,34 0.006 0.203 0.816
Load*Emotion*Group 0.387 1,34 0.538 0.011 0.093
Response Times Load 3.816 1,34 0.059 0.101 0.475
Emotion 1.839 1,34 0.184 0.051 0.261
Group 4.466 1,34 0.042 0.116 0.537
Load*Group 0.022 1,34 0.883 0.001 0.052
Emotion*Group 0.255 1,34 0.617 0.007 0.078
Load*Emotion 0.250 1,34 0.621 0.007 0.077
Load*Emotion*Group 0.218 1,34 0.643 0.006 0.074
F: Snedecor’s F statistic; P: statistical significance; df: degrees of freedom; 2 : effect size; 1-ˇ: statistical power.

421 ences between the groups in both the overall power and was significantly lower in controls. Figs. 2 and 3 show 438

422 in the individual frequency bands. Even though we did the spatial representation of the absolute power for 439

423 not observe significant behavioral results for the emotional delta and theta frequency bands, respectively. Besides, 440

424 facial stimuli, we decided to analyze its possible influence the significant interaction between Frequency Band and 441

425 on qEEG. However, no significant effects were found. We Task Phase (F(3,102) = 4.07, P = 0.009, 2 = 0.107, 1-ˇ = 0.832) 442

426 did observe that the spectral power measurements tended showed a relevant decrease of beta power from encoding 443

427 to be lower in T1D than controls, but this tendency reversed to maintenance in WM. An additional relevant interac- 444

428 during task performing periods, thus probably contributing tion between Frequency Band and WM load (F(3,102) = 29.06, 445

429 to the final outcomes. P < 0.001, 2 = 0.461, 1-ˇ = 1.00) revealed that delta, theta, 446

430 While performing the task, the results for the anterior and beta powers significantly increased during higher 447

431 brain region revealed significant effects for the factors Fre- WM load trials, with delta showing the highest incre- 448

432 quency Band (F(3,102) = 24.56, P < 0.001, 2 = 0.419, 1-ˇ = 1.00) ment (P < 0.001). The significant interaction between Task 449

433 and WM load (F(1,34) = 15.69, P < 0.001, 2 = 0.316, 1-ˇ = Phase and WM load (F(1,34) = 17.48, p < 0.001, 2 = 0.340, 1- 450

434 0.97). There was a significant interaction between Fre- ˇ = 0.982) denoted that main changes between Task Phases 451

435 quency Band and Group (F(3,102) = 4.39, P = 0.006, 2 = 0.114, occurred during LL (P < 0.001), even though there were also 452

436 1-ˇ = 0.861), indicating that delta and theta powers were significant changes during HL (P = 0.04). Finally, a signifi- 453

437 significantly higher in T1D patients, while alpha power cant interaction was found between Frequency Band, Task 454

Figure 2 Spatial representation of the absolute power of the delta band—normalized with respect to the energy measured during
resting in the same group—in the two main experimental visuospatial WM phases and WM load levels, for both T1D patients and
controls.

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
Type-1 diabetes affects cognitive strategies 7

Figure 3 Spatial representation of the absolute power of the theta band—normalized with respect to the energy measured during
resting in the same group—in the two main experimental visuospatial WM phases and WM load levels, for both T1D patients and
controls.

455 Phase and WM load (F(3,102) = 14.81, P < 0.001, 2 = 0.303, 1- Task Phase (F(3,102) = 47.71, P < 0.001, 2 = 0.584, 1-ˇ = 1.00), 466

456 ˇ = 1.00), reinforcing the notion that delta and theta powers exposing the significant increase of delta power during the 467

457 significantly varied during LL in both Task Phases (P < 0.001, visuospatial WM maintenance (see Fig. 1). Another signif- 468

458 and P = 0.04, respectively), while significant variations on icant interaction between Frequency Band and WM load 469

459 alpha and beta powers were restricted to HL (P = 0.011, and (F(3,102) = 16.54, P < 0.001, 2 = 0.327, 1-ˇ = 1.00) described 470

460 P = 0.003, respectively). how HL condition determined a relevant increment for 471

461 On the other hand, statistical analysis of the posterior delta power and a slight decrement in alpha power. Fig. 4 472

462 region showed significant effects for Frequency Band depicts the spatial distribution of the absolute power of the 473

463 (F(3,102) = 34.31, P < 0.001, 2 = 0.502, 1-ˇ = 1.00) and Task alpha frequency band. Ultimately, the interaction between 474

464 Phase (F(1,34) = 25.43, p < 0.001, 2 = 0.428, 1-ˇ = 0.998), Task Phase and WM load (F(1,34) = 4.21, P = 0.04, 2 = 0.110, 475

465 with significant interactions between Frequency Band and 1-ˇ = 0.513) showed that greater changes between the 476

Figure 4 Spatial representation of the absolute power of the alpha band—normalized with respect to the energy measured during
resting in the same group—in the two main experimental visuospatial WM phases and WM load levels, for both T1D patients and
controls.

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
8 F.J. Alvarado-Rodríguez et al.

477 WM load levels occurred during the encoding phase, while even though this emotional content is not relevant to task 534

478 greater changes between Task Phases occurred during LL completion. 535

479 trials. Second, although there is at yet no general consensus on 536

this topic, the role of delta waves during mental tasks has 537

480 Correlation analyses been associated with cortical deafferentation or the inhibi- 538

tion of the sensory afferences that interfere with internal 539

concentration [30,31]. In fact, interference control is con- 540


481 Correlation analyses between blood glucose levels and EEG
sidered an executive-attention function of the prefrontal 541
482 quantitative measures failed to show significant effects.
cortex [21] in which attention is expedited by suppressing 542
483 However, delta power at frontal regions, during both encod-
distraction; thus, the presence of EEG delta oscillations may 543
484 ing (r = 0.465, P = 0.05) and maintenance (r = 0.441, P = 0.06)
signal the inhibition of other processes that could interfere 544
485 stages, reached an achievable tendency exclusively in
with solving the mental task (see [30], for a comprehensive 545
486 healthy individuals while processing the lower level WM
review]. In the present experiment, patients showed signif- 546
487 load.
icant frontal increase of delta energy during encoding that 547

was only seen in controls on the trials with the higher WM 548
488 Discussion load. This result reinforces the notion that even lower WM 549

load levels might demand more processing resources in T1D 550

489 The main aim of the present work was to characterize elec- patients. Moreover, these additional efforts seem to per- 551

490 trophysiologically the dynamic brain activity of young people sist over frontal areas in patients during WM maintenance, 552

491 with T1D and healthy controls during WM encoding and main- also involving posterior locations, as observed in the control 553

492 tenance associated with the execution of a cognitive task, group. 554

493 while evaluating the effect of the WM load level using faces Furthermore, our T1D patients showed a substantial 555

494 as stimuli. This was done by adding a differential emotional increase of the absolute power of the theta band in frontal 556

495 content, irrelevant to task fulfilment, in order to enhance areas during the WM encoding phase, which was only seen 557

496 attentional distraction. in controls while performing the higher WM load level. 558

497 Although neuropsychological evaluation did not discern Increased frontal theta activity has been shown to corre- 559

498 signs of cognitive impairment, the behavioral results for the late positively with visual working memory capacity [38], 560

499 experimental task showed significant between-group dif- increased WM load [49], and coding of new information [41]. 561

500 ferences, as the T1D patients showed a lower number of This suggests that frontal theta activity is associated with 562

501 correct responses with prolonged reaction times. Though enhanced motivation and attention [38]. Recently, it has 563

502 not statistically significant, similar behavioural trends been proposed that during stimulus encoding, the theta fre- 564

503 have been observed in T1D patients with good glycemic quency band produces a positive subsequent memory effect 565

504 control [22,27]. and that the magnitude of frontal post-encoding-related 566

505 Despite the observed behavioral differences, an impor- theta activity correlates with the discrimination accuracy 567

506 tant and necessary distinction between the clinical of the test stimulus [55]. Moreover, recent evidence sug- 568

507 characteristics of the current group of T1D patients and gests that oscillations in frontal theta power reflect specific 569

508 those regularly reported in the literature must be made. control adjustments of behavior [14]; thus, they are consid- 570

509 This is because all our experimental subjects had regu- ered a neural marker of mid-prefrontal cortex engagement 571

510 lar medical follow-ups and no hospital admissions due to to support goal-directed control [12,13]. All these find- 572

511 diabetes in the 2 years prior to their participation in this ings suggest that the differences observed in the absolute 573

512 study. Also, they had a higher mean educational level than theta power in our groups could also reinforce the idea 574

513 that corresponding to the Mexican population as a whole, that they represent part of the additional cognitive efforts 575

514 coupled with optimal daily glucose control. In this con- required by T1D patients to fulfill task demands. In addi- 576

515 text, differences in cognitive processing between the groups tion, they support previous hypotheses assuming that T1D 577

516 may be interpreted as being caused directly by the impact patients need to deploy greater processing resources in 578

517 of the illness on central nervous system development and order to match the cognitive processing level shown by 579

518 current operation. controls [9,22]. 580

519 Behavioral responses showed two clear additional find- No significant differences were found between T1D 581

520 ings in both groups. First, an increase in WM load generated patients and controls for the absolute powers of delta and 582

521 a significant reduction in the number of correct responses, theta bands during resting. In contrast, slight decreases 583

522 though this effect was not seen when happy faces were in delta and theta activity were previously described at 584

523 used as stimuli. This finding is consistent with previ- rest for T1D patients without antecedents of early severe 585

524 ous reports which postulate that emotional information hypoglycemia [3]. Several other studies, however, have 586

525 can enhance and facilitate episodic memory consolidation reported an increase in the energy of the slower fre- 587

526 [43,48], WM capacity [51], n-back task performance [15], quency bands in patients with diabetes during rest, but 588

527 and visuospatial WM abilities [26]; thus facilitating WM this has been attributed mainly to a history of severe 589

528 processing on more difficult trials probably via greater cor- hypoglycemia [7,8,10,33] or to the hypoglycemic state 590

529 tex activation associated with what has been defined as itself [54,60,61]. 591

530 ‘‘emotional attention’’ [52,53]. Stated briefly, under cer- Another relevant result, which is worth pointing out, is 592

531 tain circumstances—such as in the present experiment—the that controls showed widespread significantly lower alpha 593

532 emotional nature of stimuli can attract additional atten- activity while performing the task with respect to T1D 594

533 tional processing resources and so facilitate task execution, patients. This difference may be related to an effect on 595

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
Type-1 diabetes affects cognitive strategies 9

596 the implementation of inhibitory processes [35,58] that has Despite limitations due primarily to the relatively small 654

597 been associated with the alpha band [6,42]. In fact, alpha sample size, this study found specific interactions between 655

598 band frequencies have been largely associated with spatial WM load and task performance in a context in which the 656

599 attention [37,40] even though their actual functional role is stimuli presented were emotional faces, and was able to 657

600 still under debate. Recently, Keitel and colleagues [39] pos- discern, electrophysiologically, between T1D patients and 658

601 tulated that the attentional modulation of dynamic visual healthy controls during the different steps of WM processing. 659

602 stimulation relies on cortical mechanisms, including retino- These findings suggest the need to continue analyzing these 660

603 topic alpha suppression. In this context, the dissimilarity effects using other types of cognitive tasks, and to evaluate 661

604 between T1D and controls on task-related alpha band activ- the potential specific contributions of other variables, such 662

605 ity might imply that T1D patients fail to dynamically control as sex, age, and years of disease evolution, among others. 663

606 their spatial attention, at least with similar efficiency to


607 controls.
608 One remarkable point was the lack of significant correla- Funding 664

609 tion between the blood glucose level at the beginning of the
610 experiment and qEEG measures. This result emphasizes the This work was supported by SEP-PRODEP NTPC Mexico, 665

611 notion that spectral parameters of the EEG are relatively project number 12967810-245213-511-6/18-9169/UDG-PTC- 666

612 stable, thus increasing its usefulness to study pathologi- 1413, 2018. 667

613 cal conditions. In this regard, the tendency observed in


614 healthy individuals to relate glycemia and delta power could
615 represent a physiological adaptive response to preserve
Disclosure of interest 668

616 homeostasis; such a mechanism would be probably affected


617 by the progress of a metabolic disease as T1D. The authors declare that they have no competing interest. 669

618 Taken together, the effects found on quantitative EEG


619 measures from the earlier steps of WM processing (encod-
References 670
620 ing), particularly those on the delta and theta frequency
621 bands, can be explained as an expression of a ‘‘switch-all-
[1] Allen KV, Pickering MJ, Zammit NN, Hartsuiker RJ, Traxler MJ, 671
622 on’’ strategy adopted by T1D patients. Several plausible
Frier BM, et al. Effects of acute hypoglycemia on working mem- 672
623 explanations for this finding could be the early recruit- ory and language processing in adults with and without Type 1 673
624 ment of an ‘‘excessive’’ amount of cognitive resources Diabetes. Diabetes Care 2015;38:1108—15. 674
625 in anticipation of subsequently higher task demands, dif- [2] American Diabetes, Association. Diagnosis and classification of 675
626 ficulties in gradually adapting the recruitment process to diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care 2014;37:81—90. 676
627 increasing task demands, difficulties with the control of [3] Åsvold BO, Sand T, Hestad KA, Bjørgaas MR. Quantitative 677

628 spatial attention or all of the above. Regardless of which EEG in type 1 diabetic adults with childhood exposure to 678

629 may be the best explanation, they all suggest an executive severe hypoglycaemia: a 16 year follow-up study. Diabetologia 679

630 failure in the ‘‘on-line’’ evaluation of cognitive demands, 2011;54:2404—8. 680

631 and/or the existence of an adaptive mechanism to fulfill [4] Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Developments in the concept of work- 681
ing memory. Neuropsychology 1994;8:485—93. 682
632 cognitive demands that mainly affects the earlier steps of
[5] Belouchrani A, Cichocki A. Robust whitening procedure in blind 683
633 WM processing. source separation context. Electronics Lett 2000;36:2050—3. 684
[6] Benedek M, Schickel RJ, Jauk E, Fink A, Neubauer AC. Alpha 685
power increases in right parietal cortex reflect focused internal 686
attention. Neuropsychologia 2014;56:393—400. 687
634 Conclusions [7] Bjørgaas M, Sand T, Gimse R. Quantitative EEG in type 688
1 diabetic children with and without episodes of severe 689

635 Our results suggest that patients with T1D exhibit a dis- hypoglycemia: a controlled, blind study. Acta Neurol Scand 690

636 tinctive behavioral and quantitative EEG pattern compared 1996;93:398—402. 691
[8] Bjørgaas M, Sand T, Vik T, Jorde R. Quantitative EEG during con- 692
637 to that observed in healthy controls during performance
trolled hypoglycaemia in diabetic and non diabetic children. 693
638 of a visuospatial WM task. These group differences are
Diabet Med 1998;15:30—7. 694
639 observable even when certain favorable patient charac- [9] Bolo NR, Musen G, Jacobson AM, Weinger K, McCartney RL, Flo- 695
640 teristics, such as proper educational level, good social res V, et al. Brain activation during working memory is altered 696
641 integration, optimal metabolic control, and good clinical in patients with type 1 diabetes during hypoglycemia. Diabetes 697
642 development without recent hypo- or hyperglycemic events, 2011;60:3256—64. 698
643 were present. [10] Brismar T, Hyllienmark L, Ekberg K, Johansson BL. Loss of tem- 699

644 Interestingly, qEEG changes reflect how patients deploy poral lobe beta power in young adults with type 1 diabetes 700

645 additional resources at early processing stages -the WM mellitus. Neuroreport 2002;13:2469—73. 701

646 encoding phase- even in the lower level memory load [11] Broadley MM, White MJ, Andrew B. A systematic review and 702
meta-analysis of executive function performance in type 1 dia- 703
647 condition, whereas it mainly occurs in the healthy con-
betes mellitus. Psychosom Med 2017;79:684—96. 704
648 trols in the higher memory load condition. Indeed, these
[12] Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cog- 705
649 effects differentially affect electrophysiological processing nitive control. Trends Cognit Sci 2014;18:414—21. 706
650 in frontal and posterior brain areas, thus evidencing how [13] Cohen MX. Midfrontal theta tracks action monitoring over mul- 707
651 the metabolic restrictions induced by diabetes can drive tiple interactive time scales. Neuroimage 2016;141:262—72. 708
652 processing alternatives, and probably, the cognitive strate- [14] Cooper PS, Karayanidis F, McKewen M, McLellan-Hall S, Wong 709
653 gies employed. ASW, Skippen P, et al. Frontal theta predicts specific cognitive 710

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
10 F.J. Alvarado-Rodríguez et al.

711 control-induced behavioural changes beyond general reaction [34] Hyllienmark L, Maltez J, Dandenell A, Ludvingsson J, Brismar 778
712 time slowing. Neuroimage 2019;189:130—40. T. EEG abnormalities with and without relation to severe hypo- 779
713 [15] Cromheeke S, Mueller SC. The power of a smile: stronger work- glycaemia in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Diabetologia 780
714 ing memory effects for happy faces in adolescents compared 2005;48:412—9. 781
715 to adults. Cogn Emot 2016;30:288—301. [35] Jensen O, Gelfand J, Kounios J, Lisman JE. Oscillations in 782
716 [16] Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol the alpha band (9-12Hz) increase with memory load dur- 783
717 2013;64:135—68. ing retention in a short-term memory task. Cereb Cortex 784
718 [17] Dolcos F, Iordan AD, Dolcos S. Neural correlates of emotion- 2002;12:877—82. 785
719 cognition interactions: a review of evidence from brain imaging [36] Jin YX, Li XB, Luo YJ. Effects of Emotional Content on 786
720 investigations. J Cogn Psychol 2011;23:669—94. Working Memory: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evi- 787
721 [18] Embury CM, Wiesman AI, Proskovec AL, Heinrichs-Graham dence. In: Liu D, Alippi C, Zhao D, Hussain A, editors. 788
722 E, McDermoth TJ, Lord GH, et al. Altered brain dynam- Advances in Brain Inspired Cognitive Systems. BICS. Springer, 789
723 ics in patients with type 1 Diabetes during working memory Berlin: Heidelberg; 2013 [Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 790
724 processing. Diabetes 2018;67:1140—8. vol 7888.]. 791
725 [19] Franceschi M, Cecchetto R, Minicucci F, Smizne S, Baio G, Canal [37] Kashiwase Y, Matsumiya K, Kuriki I, Shioiri S. Time courses of 792
726 N. Cognitive processes in insulin-dependent diabetes. Diabetes attentional modulation in neural amplification and synchro- 793
727 Care 1984;7:228—31. nization measured with steady-state visual-evoked potentials. 794
728 [20] Fusser F, Linden DE, Rahm B, Hampel H, Haenschel C, Mayer J Cogn Neurosci 2012;24:1779—93. 795
729 JS. Common capacity-limited neural mechanisms of selective [38] Kawasaki M, Yamaguchi Y. Frontal theta and beta synchroniza- 796
730 attention and spatial working memory encoding. Eur J Neurosci tions for monetary reward increase visual working memory 797
731 2011;34:827—38. capacity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2012;8:523—30. 798
732 [21] Fuster JM. The Prefrontal Cortex. 4th ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier; [39] Keitel C, Keitel A, Benwell CSY, Daube C, Thut G, Gross 799
733 2008. J. Stimulus-Driven Brain Rhythms within the Alpha Band: 800
734 [22] Gallardo-Moreno GB, González-Garrido AA, Gudayol-Ferré E, The Attentional-Modulation Conundrum. J Neurosci 801
735 Guàrdia-Olmos J. Type 1 diabetes modifies brain activation in 2019;39:3119—29. 802
736 young patients while performing visuospatial working memory [40] Kizuk SA, Mathewson KE. Power and phase of alpha oscilla- 803
737 tasks. J Diabetes Res 2015:703512. tions reveal an interaction between spatial and temporal visual 804
738 [23] Gaudieri PA, Chen R, Greer TF, Holmes CS. Cognitive function in attention. J Cogn Neurosci 2017;29:480—94. 805
739 children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2008;31:1892—7. [41] Klimesch W, Schimke H, Schwaiger J. Episodic and semantic 806
740 [24] Gejl M, Gjedde A, Brock B, Møller A, van Duikerken E, Haahr memory: an analysis in the EEG theta and alpha band. Elec- 807
741 HL, et al. Effects of hypoglycaemia on working memory and troencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;91:428—41. 808
742 regional cerebral blood flow in type 1 diabetes: a randomized, [42] Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: the 809
743 crossover trial. Diabetologia 2018;61:551—61. inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 2007;53(1):63—88. 810
744 [25] González-Garrido AA, Gallardo-Moreno GB, Gómez-Velázquez [43] LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional mem- 811
745 FR. Type 1 diabetes and working memory processing of emo- ory. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7:54—64. 812
746 tional faces. Behav Brain Res 2019;363:173—81. [44] Li L, Zhang JX, Jiang T. Visual working memory load-related 813
747 [26] González-Garrido AA, López-Franco AL, Gómez-Velázquez changes in neural activity and functional connectivity. PLoS 814
748 FR, Ramos-Loyo J, Sequeira H. Emotional content of One 2011;6:e22357. 815
749 stimuli improves visuospatial working memory. Neurosci Lett [45] Linden DE. The working memory networks of the human brain. 816
750 2015;585:43—7. Neuroscientist 2007;13:257—67. 817
751 [27] Guàrdia-Olmos J, Gallardo-Moreno GB, Gudayol-Ferré E, Peró- [46] Luo J, Yu R. Follow the heart or the head? The interac- 818
752 Cebollero M, González-Garrido AA. Effect of verbal task tive influence model of emotion and cognition. Front Psychol 819
753 complexity in a working memory paradigm in patients with type 2015;6:573. 820
754 1 diabetes. A fMRI study. PLoS ONE 2017;12:e0178172. [47] Magistretti PJ, Allaman I. Brain energy metabolism. In: Neu- 821
755 [28] Guàrdia-Olmos J, Gudayol-Ferré E, Gallardo-Moreno GB, roscience in the 21st Century. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 822
756 Martínez-Ricart M, Peró-Cebollero M, González-Garrido AA. 1591—620. 823
757 Complex systems representing effective connectivity in [48] Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and motivated cognition: the 824
758 patients with Type One diabetes mellitus. PLoS ONE positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci 825
759 2018;13:e0208247. 2005;9:496—502. 826
760 [29] Guimond S, Padani S, Lutz O, Eack S, Thermenos H, Kesha- [49] Maurer U, Brem S, Liechti M, Maurizio S, Michels L, Brandeis D. 827
761 van M. Impaired regulation of emotional distractors during Frontal midline theta reflects individual task performance in a 828
762 working memory load in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res working memory task. Brain Topogr 2015;28:127—34. 829
763 2018;101:14—20. [50] Okon-Singer H, Hendler T, Pessoa L, Shackman AJ. The 830
764 [30] Harmony T. The functional significance of delta oscillations in neurobiology of emotion-cognition interactions: fundamental 831
765 cognitive processing. Front Integr Neurosci 2013;7:83. questions and strategies for future research. Front Hum Neu- 832
766 [31] Harmony T, Fernández T, Silva J, Bernal J, Díaz-Comas L, rosci 2015;9:58. 833
767 Reyes A, et al. EEG delta activity: an indicator of attention [51] Perlstein WM, Elbert T, Stenger VA. Dissociation in human 834
768 to internal processing during performance of mental tasks. Int prefrontal cortex of affective influences on working memory- 835
769 J Psychophysiol 1996;24:161—71. related activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002;99: 836
770 [32] Hou F, Liu C, Yu Z, Xu X, Zhang J, Peng C, et al. Age-related 736—1741. 837
771 alterations in electroencephalography connectivity and net- [52] Pessoa L, Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Attentional control of the 838
772 work topology during n-back working memory task. Front Hum processing of neural and emotional stimuli. Brain Res Cog Brain 839
773 Neurosci 2018;12:484. Res 2002;15:31—45. 840
774 [33] Howorka K, Pumprla J, Saletu B, Anderer P, Krieger M, Sch- [53] Posner MI, Dehaene S. Attentional networks. Trends Neurosci 841
775 abmann A. Decrease of vigilance assessed by EEG-mapping in 1994;17:75—9. 842
776 type I diabetic patients with history of recurrent severe hypo- [54] Pramming S, Thorsteinsson B, Stigsby B, Binder C. Glycaemic 843
777 glycaemia. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000;25:85—105. threshold for changes in electroencephalograms during hypo- 844

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003
+Model
NEUCLI 2671 1—11 ARTICLE IN PRESS
Type-1 diabetes affects cognitive strategies 11

845 glycaemia in patients with insulin dependent diabetes. Br Med [59] Sommerfield AJ, Deary IJ, McAulay V, Frier BM. Short-term, 861
846 J (Clin Res Ed) 1988;296:665—7. delayed, and working memory are impaired during hypo- 862
847 [55] Pu M, Yu R. Post-encoding frontal theta activity predicts inci- glycemia in individuals with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 863
848 dental memory in the reward context. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2003;26:390—6. 864
849 2019;158:14—23. [60] Tallroth G, Lindgren M, Stenberg G, Rosén I, Agardh CD. Neuro- 865
850 [56] Romo-Vazquez R, Velez-Perez H, Ranta R, Louis-Dorr V, Maquin physiological changes during insulin-induced hypoglycaemia 866
851 D, Maillard L. Blind source separation, wavelet denoising and and in the recovery period following glucose infusion in type 867
852 discriminant analysis for EEG artefacts and noise cancelling. 1 (insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus and in normal man. 868
853 Biomed Signal Process Control 2012;7:389—400. Diabetologia 1990;33:319—23. 869
854 [57] Roux F, Uhlhaas PJ. Working memory and neural oscillations: [61] Tribl G, Howorka K, Heger G, Anderer P, Thoma H, Zeitl- 870
855 alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM infor- hofer J. EEG topography during insulin-induced hypoglycemia 871
856 mation? Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 2014;18:16—25. in patients with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Eur Neu- 872
857 [58] Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Pecherstorfer T, Fre- rol 1996;36:303—9. 873
858 unberger R, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha synchronization and
859 functional coupling during top-down processing in a working
860 memory task. Hum Brain Mapp 2005;26:148—55.

Please cite this article in press as: Alvarado-Rodríguez FJ, et al. Type-1 diabetes shapes working memory processing
NEUCLI 2671 1—11
strategies. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology (2019), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2019.09.003

You might also like