Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 21

Chapter 11

Academic Talent Development:


Theory and Best Practices
Françoys Gagné
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

INTRODUCTION Bloom’s (1985) Developing Talent in Young People.


Soon after, Renzulli and Reis (1991) ended a politi-
For most people, talent development is the imple-
cally oriented article with the following statement:
mentation of specific resources aimed at fostering
“Talent development is the ‘business’ of our field,
the growth of outstanding performances in specific
and we must never lose sight of this goal, regard-
occupational fields: natural and social sciences,
less of the direction that reform efforts might take”
technology, visual and performing arts, health and
(p. 34). Unfortunately, they did not define that
education, commerce, sports, and so forth. Consid-
key term. In the 1990s, the number of publications
ering this handbook focuses on the field of gifted
that included talent development in their title grew
education, this chapter similarly adopts as its focus
steadily. For instance, the administrators of the
the emergence of outstanding school achievements,
Belin-Blank Center at the University of Iowa used
from kindergarten forward. I analyze academic
it in the title of their series of proceedings from
talent development (ATD) from two distinct per-
the biennial Wallace symposia (e.g., Colangelo &
spectives: theoretical and practical. Each of these
Assouline, 2001), and John Feldhusen (1992)
perspectives can be circumscribed by the following
named his theoretical model talent identification
questions. Which personal and contextual causal
and development in education. A cursory look at
influences contribute significantly to the emergence
the tables of contents and subject indexes of recent
of excellence in school subjects? Which educational
handbooks (Balchin, Hymer, & Matthews, 2009;
resources will maximize the transformation of out-
Callahan & Hertberg-Davis, 2013; Colangelo &
standing aptitudes into academic excellence? These
Davis, 2003; Dixon & Moon, 2006; Kerr, 2009;
two questions will guide the contents of this chapter.
MacFarlane & Stambaugh, 2009; Plucker & Callahan,
2008; Renzulli, Gubbins, McMillen, Eckert, &
Circumscribing the Target Concept
Little, 2009; Shavinina, 2009; Sternberg & David-
Talent development is not a new concept in gifted
son, 2005) confirms the term’s more frequent use
education, but it gained some recent popularity as a
in academia. Some scholars have argued that the
label for many conceptual models aiming to explain
growing popularity of the term talent development
the emergence of talents, as well as some program-
marked a major paradigmatic change. For instance,
matic resources. Indeed, a few decades ago, the tal-
Olszewski-Kubilius (2009) stated,
ent development label was not included in the titles
of books or chapters, or in subject indexes (e.g., In 1983, when I entered the field of
Barbe & Renzulli, 1975; Passow, 1979). The term gifted education, there was a paradigm
became increasingly common in the 1980s, helped shift occurring. People were beginning
possibly by the immense popularity of Benjamin to use the term talent development and,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0000038-011
APA Handbook of Giftedness and Talent, S. I. Pfeiffer (Editor-in-Chief)
163
Copyright © 2018 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Françoys Gagné

in fact, my center at Northwestern Uni- targets gifted students and gifted education almost
versity was one of the first to incorporate exclusively. (c) This is the only chapter in
the term into our title—The Center for which talented students appear as a distinct
Talent Development, or CTD. This was circumscribed subgroup. (d) The label talent rarely
not just semantics, although it may have appears outside the term talent development, and
appeared so to outsiders, but indicative those authors who use the term do not specify
of an important conceptual shift in think- what the term talent means within that expression.
ing among leaders in the field of gifted (e) Educational professionals almost never add the
education and those who studied excep- qualifier academic to the expression talent develop-
tional ability. (p. 81) ment, unaware that the label applies equally well
in most occupational fields, especially in arts and
Unfortunately, Olszewski-Kubilius did not spec- sports. (f) The label giftedness covers a large diver-
ify the nature of that conceptual shift. In a similar sity of views and definitions about outstanding
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

vein, Brody (2009) affirmed the following: human abilities.


More recently, we have seen a shift in our If you choose to keep on reading this chapter,
field away from a focus on “gifted educa- you will discover a conceptual distinction that
tion” to one on “talent development,” makes it unique within the handbook. Indeed, this
with the new terminology reflecting a chapter proposes a clear and operational conceptual
growing realization that using a measure differentiation between the labels giftedness and tal-
of general intellectual ability as a sole ent. Furthermore, it serves as the conceptual basis
predictor of achievement is not adequate. for a detailed and comprehensive theory of talent
(pp. 93–94) development, originally named the differentiating
model of giftedness and talent1 (DMGT; Gagné,
Again, we are left in the dark as to the exact rela- 1985), and recently renamed the integrative model
tionship between the two halves of that sentence. of talent development (IMTD; Gagné, 2013). An
Despite its increased use, the long-term develop- overview of the IMTD constitutes the first part of
mental process that leads to academic excellence this chapter.
has remained without clear definitional and descrip-
tive resource parameters until recently (see Gagné,
2011). This is my focus in the second half of this
The Integrative Model of Talent
chapter.
Development: From Genes to
Giftedness, Then to Talent
Importance and Uniqueness of
This Chapter Why do some students excel in school, whereas
The title of this handbook implies that the labels most of their peers obtain average or below-average
giftedness and talent stand for two distinct concepts. performances? Ask a dozen educators, scholars, or
Yet, according to most of its 43 chapters, this is not parents, and you will probably get a dozen distinct
the case. In fact, most contributing authors share answers. Most of us harbor a personal implicit the-
a somewhat common view typified by the following ory about the causal origins of academic talent, and
semantic and conceptual characteristics: one of the main characteristics of these personal
(a) Contributing authors avoid discussion of views is the tendency for each person to privilege
possible differentiated meanings between the labels one key “ingredient” of success over the candidates
giftedness and talent. (b) The label giftedness defended by other people. That key ingredient may
appears much more frequently than the label talent; reside in the family environment or the school
in fact, as shown in many chapter titles, this handbook environment, and it may be identified as an amount

The D in DMGT initially represented the qualifier differentiated (e.g., Gagné, 1985, 2000). The author eventually chose differentiating as a more
1

accurate representation of the model’s goal.

164
Academic Talent Development

of study, determination and will power, motiva- into talents. These five components create the three
tion and passion, cognitive aptitudes, and so forth. blocks on the right in Figure 11.1.
The IMTD aims to relativize the alleged strength
Differentiating giftedness and talent.  Scholars
of these “causal spotlights” of academic success
and practitioners almost unanimously acknowledge
and excellence by proposing a complex interaction
that the concept of giftedness subsumes two distinct
of a diversity of causal factors, whose strength of
realities: early emerging forms with strong biologi-
influence changes not only over the course of the
cal roots and fully developed adult forms, which
educational trajectory, but also from individual
are expressed through associated pairs of terms
to individual. Taken individually, none of these
(e.g., potential/realization, aptitude/achievement,
factors has an overwhelming impact—except in
promise/fulfillment). This dichotomy surfaces in
very special circumstances—on the final educa-
countless popular expressions (e.g., “Education’s
tional outcome, but all play a daily role in the
goal is to maximize each student’s potential,”
complex choreography of influences leading to the
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

“Realizing one’s potential is each person’s lifelong


emergence—or non-emergence—of academic
challenge”). Similarly, the phenomenon of under-
talent. I intend to demonstrate, through the IMTD,
achievement is usually described “as a discrepancy
that cognitive aptitudes, anchored in individuals’
between expected performance (ability or potential)
biological and genetic foundations, act as build-
and actual performance (achievement)” (Siegle &
ing blocks for the numerous academic competen-
McCoach, 2013, p. 377). The distinction between
cies acquired through formal education, and that
potential and achievement is strongly imbedded
this long-term developmental process is continu-
in our views of human abilities. It would reduce
ally modulated by two large groups of influences:
unnecessary conceptual ambiguity if we adopted
intrapersonal catalysts that define individuals’ tem-
distinct labels when referring to aptitudes as
perament, personality, needs, and desires, and envi-
opposed to achievements. I associate each label to a
ronmental catalysts that are present in individuals’
separate concept, adopting the label gifted to convey
family, school, and social environments. The IMTD
the idea of a potential anchored in transmitted—or
evolved (Gagné, 2013) from the DMGT, integrating
given—biological foundations. Two basic definitions
another recent addition, the developmental model
were born that constitute the core of the DMGT
for natural abilities (DMNA). I will examine each of
framework.
these models next according to their chronological
appearance. ■■ Giftedness designates the possession and use of
biologically anchored and informally developed
outstanding natural abilities or aptitudes (e.g.,
Overview of the Differentiating
gifts), in at least one ability domain, to a degree
Model of Giftedness and Talent
that places an individual at least among the top
The DMGT2 defines talent development as the
10% of age peers.
progressive transformation of outstanding natural
■■ Talent designates the outstanding mastery of
abilities (i.e., gifts) into outstanding systematically
systematically developed competencies (knowl-
developed competencies (i.e., talents). It brings
edge and skills) in at least one field of human
together five components: gifts, talents, the talent
activity to a degree that places an individual at
development process, intrapersonal catalysts, and
least among the top 10% of learning peers (those
environmental catalysts. The first three components,
having accumulated a similar amount of learning
called the talent development trio, constitute the core
time from either current or past training).
of the DMGT; their interaction summarizes the
essence of the DMGT’s conception of talent develop- Note how the DMGT clearly separates the con-
ment, namely the progressive transformation of gifts cepts of giftedness, potential, aptitude, and natural

Readers will find more extensive descriptions, specific components, and detailed figures of the IMTD and the DMGT on the author’s web site at http://
2

gagnefrancoys.wixsite.com/dmgt-mddt.

165
Françoys Gagné

1 2 3 4 5
CATALYSTS NATURAL ABILITIES CATALYSTS
BIOLOGICAL COMPETENCIES
ENVIRONMENTAL (E) GIFTS (G) ENVIRONMENTAL (E)
FOUNDATIONS = top 10% TALENTS (T)
MILIEU (EM) MILIEU (EM) = top 10%
INDIVIDUALS (EI) INDIVIDUALS (EI)
RESOURCES (ER) FIELDS
RESOURCES (ER) DOMAINS
GENOTYPIC INTRAPERSONAL (l) INTRAPERSONAL (I) ACADEMIC (TC)
FOUNDATIONS INTELLECTUAL (GI)

TRAITS
TRAITS
PHYSICAL (IF) PHYSICAL (IF) R TECHNICAL (TT)
MENTAL (IP) MENTAL (IP)

MENTAL
CREATIVE (GC) I SCIENCE &
TECHNOLOGY (TI)

MANAGEMENT
MANAGEMENT

AWARENESS (IW) AWARENESS (IW)


PHENOTYPES SOCIAL (GS)
A ARTS (TA)
MOTIVATION (IM)

GOAL
GOAL

MOTIVATION (IM)
PHYSIOLOGICAL PERCEPTUAL VOLITION (IV) S SOCIAL SERVICE (TP)
VOLITION (IV)
(ENDO) (GP)
E ADMINISTRATION /
PHENOTYPES SALES (TM)

PHYSICAL
DEVELOPMENTAL DEVELOPMENTAL C BUSINESS
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

ANATOMICAL MUSCULAR (GM) OPERATIONS (TB)


PROCESS (D) PROCESS (D)
(EXO) MOTOR CONTROL
PHENOTYPES MATURATION ACTIVITIES (DA) GAMES (TG)
(GR)
INFORMAL LEARNING / INVESTMENT (DI) SPORTS &
ATHLETICS (TS)
EXERCISE PROGRESS (DP)

DMNA

DMGT

Figure 11.1.  Gagné’s integrative model of talent development.

abilities from those of talent, performance, achieve- another unique characteristic, namely a clear
ment, and systematically developed abilities, as well answer to the prevalence question: outstanding
as expertise, eminence, and prodigiousness. The means individuals who belong to the top 10% of the
theory will stand—or fall—on the validity of that relevant reference group in terms of natural ability
basic distinction, especially on the acceptance of the (for giftedness) or achievement (for talent). This
concept of giftedness. Note also that the term ability generous choice for the initial threshold is counter­
serves as an umbrella construct that covers natural balanced by the recognition of levels of giftedness
abilities (e.g., aptitudes) and systematically devel- or talent; the DMGT’s metric-based system of levels
oped abilities (e.g., competencies). Borland (1989) constitutes an intrinsic constituent of the DMGT. It has
was one of the first scholars in the field to recognize five hierarchically superposed levels, with each suc-
the value of this distinction: cessive level including the top 10% of the preceding
level. They are labeled mildly (top 10%), moderately
Gagné’s use of the terms giftedness and
(top 1%), highly (top 0.1%), exceptionally (top
talent appears to be the least arbitrary and
0.01%), and extremely or profoundly (top 0.001%)
the most useful of those proposed thus
gifted/talented. Why 10%? The prevalence ques-
far. The distinction between competence
tion has no absolute answer; nowhere will we find a
and performance is a real and meaning-
magical number that automatically separates those
ful one, and it allows for the building of a
labeled gifted or talented from the rest of the popu-
model that permits the operationalization
lation. The choice of a proper threshold requires
of the concepts (p. 23).
that professionals come to a consensus. Unfortu-
Unfortunately, he didn’t notice that the DMGT nately, no such consensus has yet been achieved in
terminology considers the terms competence and the various fields of talent development. The preva-
performance as synonyms. lence question is crucial for theoretical and practical
How many people are gifted and/or talented? reasons. From a theoretical standpoint, a prevalence
As shown in these definitions, the DMGT offers estimate represents an important contribution

166
Academic Talent Development

toward a more precise definition of any normative Talents progressively emerge from the transfor-
construct (e.g., poverty, tallness, weight, most neu- mation of these outstanding natural abilities or gifts
rotic syndromes) that targets a marginal subgroup into the well-trained and systematically developed
within a population. Practically speaking, adopting competencies that define a particular field of human
a threshold of 10% instead of 1%—a tenfold differ- activity. On the potential–performance continuum,
ence in estimated prevalence—has a huge impact on talents represent the performance end and the out-
selection practices and talent development services come of the talent development process. Talent
(Gagné, 1998). fields can be extremely diverse. Column 5 in
Figure 11.1 shows nine talent subcomponents. Six of
The talent development trio (gifts, talents, and the them have their source in Holland’s (see Anastasi &
talent development process).  The DMGT identi- Urbina, 1997) classification of work-related person-
fies six natural ability domains: four of them belong- ality types: Realistic, Investigative, Artistic, Social,
ing to the mental realm (intellectual gifts, creative Enterprising, and Conventional (RIASEC). Three
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

gifts, social gifts, and perceptual gifts), and the other additional subcomponents complement the RIA-
two belonging to the physical realm (muscular gifts SEC taxonomy: preoccupational academic (K–12)
and motor control gifts). In the field of gifted educa- subjects, games, and sports. A natural ability can
tion, as well as within this handbook, the term gifted express itself in many different ways depending on
refers almost exclusively to outstanding intellectual the field(s) of activity adopted by an individual. For
or cognitive natural abilities. Natural abilities are example, motor control can be modeled into the
not innate (see the following DMNA section); they particular skills of a pianist, a painter, or a video
develop, especially during childhood, through matu- game player. Similarly, cognitive processes can be
rational processes and informal exercise. Yet, that modeled into the scientific reasoning of a chem-
development and the level of expression are sub- ist, the memorization and game analysis of a chess
stantially controlled by individuals’ genetic endow- player, or the strategic planning of an athlete.
ment. Major individual differences can be observed Natural abilities or aptitudes serve as the “raw
in natural abilities in the daily lives of children, at materials” or constituent elements of talents; they
home and at school. For instance, intellectual abili- act through the talent development process. The
ties are needed to learn to read, speak a foreign lan- neologism talentee was created to describe any
guage, or understand new mathematical concepts; individual actively involved in a systematic talent
creative abilities are needed to solve different kinds development process, whatever the field. Talentees
of problems and produce original work in the visual coordinate the various elements of that process,
and performing arts, literature, and science; physical which is subdivided into three subcomponents of
abilities are involved in sports, music, and sculpture; talent development (see Figure 11.1): activities,
and social abilities are essential when interacting investment, and progress. Each of these subcom-
with classmates, teachers, and parents. Gifts can be ponents is subdivided again into multiple facets.
observed more easily and directly in young children Talent development begins when a child or adult
because environmental influences and systematic gains access, through an identification or selection
learning have not yet exerted their moderating influ- process, to a systematic program of activities. These
ence in a significant way. However, they still show activities include a specific content (the curriculum)
themselves in older children, even in adults, through offered within a specific learning environment. The
the facility and speed with which individuals acquire investment subcomponent quantifies the intensity
new competencies in any field of human activity. of the talent development process in terms of time,
Ease and speed in learning are the trademarks of psychological energy, and money. Ericsson’s (2002)
giftedness: They contribute strongly to the learn- concept of deliberate practice combines the time and
ers’ pace of progress, with an extremely rapid pace psychological energy facets. Finally, the progress of
being a key characteristic of prodigies (Gagné & talentees from initial access to peak performance can
McPherson, 2016). be broken down into a series of stages (e.g., novice,

167
Françoys Gagné

advanced, proficient, expert). Its main quantitative component. This partial overlap signals the crucial
representation is pace (e.g., how fast talentees are filtering role that the intrapersonal component plays
progressing, compared with learning peers, toward with regard to environmental influences. The bulk
their predefined excellence goal). The long-term of environmental stimuli must pass through the ana-
developmental course of most talentees will be lytical sieve of individuals’ needs, interests, or per-
marked by a series of more or less crucial turning sonality traits; talentees continually pick and choose
points (e.g., being spotted by a teacher or coach, which stimuli will receive their attention. The
receiving an important scholarship, accidents, death environmental component comprises three distinct
of a family member or close friend). subcomponents: a diversity of environmental influ-
ences, including physical (e.g., climate, rural vs.
The supporting cast.  Two large sets of catalysts, urban living), social, political, financial, and cultural
intrapersonal and environmental (see Figure 11.1) influences; the psychological influence of significant
affect the talent development process, positively or persons in the talentees’ immediate environment,
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

negatively. The intrapersonal component has five including parents and siblings, but also extended
subcomponents grouped into two main dimensions: family, teachers and trainers, peers, mentors, and
stable traits (physical and mental/personality) and role models; and talent development resources,
goal management processes (self-awareness, motiva- including adapted curricula, special courses or
tion, and volition). Within the mental/personality schools, advanced teams in sports, and so forth.
category, there is an extremely long list of descrip-
tive qualities. Temperament refers to behavioral pre- Dynamic interactions.  All five components
dispositions with strong biological and hereditary of the DMGT entertain a large diversity of com-
underpinnings, whereas personality encompasses a plex dynamic interactions among themselves and
large diversity of positive or negative acquired styles between specific facets within each of them. This
of behavior (Rothbart, 2012). The most widely chapter does not allow the space for a detailed sur-
accepted structure for personality attributes is called vey, but consider that efforts by parents or teachers
the five-factor personality model; research has to modify the characteristics of children and stu-
shown each factor to possess significant biological dents (e.g., interests, personality, beliefs, deviant
roots (McCrae, 2009). The term motivation usually behavior) illustrate environmental to intrapersonal
brings to mind the idea of what motivates us and influences; of course, influences in the opposite
how motivated we are (i.e., volition), that is how direction can also be imagined (e.g., students’ pas-
much effort we are ready to invest to reach a particu- sions influencing the behavior of parents or teach-
lar goal. Within the framework of the action control ers). The most fundamental pattern of interactions
theory, Kuhl and Beckmann (1985; see also Corno, defines the DMGT’s view of the talent development
1993) proposed to differentiate the global goal seek- process, namely the long-term transformation of
ing process into (a) distinct goal-setting activities, outstanding potentialities into equally outstand-
labeled motivation and (b) goal-attainment activi- ing competencies, thanks to the constant mediat-
ties, labeled volition or will power. Talentees first ing effect of both groups of catalysts. Even talent
examine their values and needs, as well as determine can have a motivating impact on students: success
their interests; these will serve to identify the spe- breeds success. It can also influence environmental
cific talent goal for which they will aim. The loftier sources (e.g., parents and teachers). In summary,
the goal, the more efforts talentees will need to no causal component stands alone; they all interact
reach it. Long-term goals placed at a vey high level with each other and with the learning process in
will require intense dedication, as well as daily acts very complex ways, and these interactions will dif-
of will power to maintain investment in practice fer significantly from one person to the next. Even
through obstacles, boredom, and occasional failure. though all four causal components are constantly
The environmental component appears in active, it does not mean that they are equally power-
Figure 11.1 partially hidden behind the intrapersonal ful as agents of talent emergence. This is no doubt a

168
Academic Talent Development

truism at the individual level because each talented “bottom-up” makes clear that such biological
person follows a unique path toward excellence. underpinnings occupy some underground level
But what can be said about averages? Are some fac- under the strictly behavioral DMGT framework. A
tors generally recognized as stronger predictors of brief examination of the literature suggests three
outstanding performance? For those involved in levels constitute an acceptable vertical differentia-
the talent development of gifted individuals, this is tion (see Column 1 in Figure 11.1): The chemical
the ultimate question. Its extensive discussion rep- level is reserved for genotypic foundations (e.g.,
resents another unique characteristic of the DMGT. gene identification, mutations, gene expression,
(See Gagné, 2004, for a further presentation of this epigenetic phenomena, protein production, etc.).
analysis.) The physiological level covers microbiological and
(neuro)physiological processes, which move from
Beyond the Differentiating Model of genotypic to phenotypic phenomena. Their hidden
Giftedness and Talent: Introducing the nature explains the label of endophenotypes; they
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Developmental Model for Natural Abilities correspond to


and the Integrative Model of Talent
physical traits—phenotypes—that are
Development
not externally visible but are measurable.
The DMGT constitutes a strictly behavioral repre-
Endophenotypes can reveal the biological
sentation of the numerous influences facilitating
bases for a disorder better than behav-
or blocking the growth of competencies in general,
ioral symptoms because they represent
including their outstanding manifestations as tal-
a fundamental physical trait that is more
ents. Among this large set of influences, natural abil-
closely tied to its source in a gene variant.
ities play a significant causal role as building blocks
(Nurnberger & Bierut, 2007, pp. 48–49)
of competencies. I defined natural abilities as having
significant biological roots; these roots manifest Finally, the morphological level3 includes anatomical
themselves in many ways, for instance anatomical or characteristics that have been shown to impact abili-
morphological characteristics, neurophysiological ties or intrapersonal catalysts. Most of these charac-
activity in the brain and body, gene expression, and teristics are observable exophenotypes, either directly
countless others. Unfortunately, the DMGT frame- (e.g., height in a basketball player, physique in a
work leaves no room for these distal sources of tal- gymnastist) or indirectly (e.g., brain size, muscle
ent emergence. It became imperative to find a way to type). Endophenotypes and morphological traits are
integrate them. part of the complex hierarchical causal chain joining
genes to physical or mental abilities, and ultimately
Biological foundations of talent development. 
to systematically developed skills.
Science has adopted a hierarchical organization of
explanations, moving progressively from behav- The proper meaning of innate.  The DMNA was
ioral phenomena to physiology, microbiology, conceived first and foremost to integrate these bio-
and chemistry, and then to physics. For instance, logical foundations, and to explain and illustrate
Plomin, DeFries, Craig, and McGuffin (2003) their developmental process. It was also trying (a)
described functional genomics as “a bottom-up to respond to scholars’ questioning of the relevance
strategy in which the gene product is identified of the concept of giftedness and (b) to correct the
by its DNA sequence and the function of the gene misunderstanding of individuals using the DMGT
product is traced through cells and then cell systems who describe gifts as innate and talents as acquired.
and eventually the brain” (p. 14). The expression This simplistic bipolar view is wrong: gifts are not

Anatomy and morphology appear almost synonymous. But, anatomy is considered a subdivision of morphology. “External features such as gross size,
3

shape, color, and other physical features of the biological structures are studied in morphology, whereas anatomy is concerned about the cellular and
tissue level composition of the biological structures” (Difference Between Anatomy and Morphology, 2012). Both perspectives are relevant in the
present context, making it difficult to adopt one term over the other.

169
Françoys Gagné

innate, they develop during childhood, and some- If natural abilities cannot be considered innate,
times continue to do so during adulthood. This where does the “gift” in giftedness reside? It is
developmental view of “natural” abilities has to fight not in the morphological level identified earlier,
its way through a host of common expressions that because these morphological structures require
maintain the ambiguity, like “She is a born musi- extensive development, and most do not achieve
cian,” “It’s God’s gift,” or “Either you have it or you their maturity until adolescence or adulthood. If we
don’t!” If all these uses of innate are incorrect, what go to the physiological level, we might be in a gray
does the term innate really mean? When a child is zone where it becomes difficult to separate innate
said to be a “born” pianist, it is not implied that he processes from those that result from developmen-
or she began playing the piano in the nursery, nor tal activities. For example, genetic agents govern
that he or she was able to play a concerto within most stages of embryogenesis. If the development
weeks of beginning piano lessons. Describing talent was strictly maturational, then we could probably
as innate only makes sense metaphorically. It will speak of innateness. It is clear, however, that the
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

convey the idea that the pianist progressed rapidly chemical level, devoted to gene activity, is almost
and seemingly without effort through his or her completely—but not totally, according to the new
music curriculum, at a much more rapid pace than field of epigenetics—under inborn control.
that of his or her learning peers. The same applies to
any natural ability. Intellectually precocious children Describing the developmental model for natural
do not suddenly manifest an exceptional vocabulary abilities.  How does the development of natural
or highly logical reasoning processes; they develop abilities proceed? The left side of Figure 11.1 shows
these cognitive abilities by going through the same that process through the DMNA. At first glance, it
developmental stages as any other child. The dif- might look similar to the DMGT counterpart on
ference resides in the ease and speed with which the left side of the figure, but a closer look shows
they advance through these successive stages. The major differences between them, at the component
term precocious says it all: they reach a given level and the subcomponent levels. The main difference
of knowledge and reasoning before the majority of is of course a transfer of the gifts component from
their learning peers. the left side (DMGT) to the right side (DMNA);
Researchers in behavioral genetics have given the aptitudes—and their outstanding expression in
term innate a very specific definition. At the behav- gifts—are now the outcome of this particular devel-
ioral level, it implies opmental process. Here, the three levels of biological
underpinnings, structural elements, and processes
hard-wired, fixed action patterns of a
become the building blocks for the phenotypic
species that are impervious to experi-
behavioral abilities. The developmental process spe-
ence. Genetic influence on abilities and
cific to the DMNA is described next, with two macro
other complex traits does not denote the
processes identified. Maturation of course covers a
hard-wired deterministic effect of a single
diversity of biological processes at each of the three
gene but rather probabilistic propensities
basement levels, from the chemical level upward,
of many genes in multiple-gene systems.
that govern the growth of mental and physical abili-
(Plomin, 1998, p. 421)
ties. These maturational processes have no direct
So, when people use the term innate to qualify the relationship with the talent development process
DMGT’s natural abilities, they convey two false itself; their role is to mold the natural abilities that
interpretations: (a) observed individual differences will become the building blocks of talents. As for
are immutable, and (b) they are present at birth the learning subcomponent, it is considered infor-
or, if not, appear suddenly with minimal train- mal because it lacks the structured organization
ing. Because of its restricted meaning, very few (e.g., curriculum, access rules, systematic schedule,
scientists use the term innate to describe any type formal assessment) typical of talent development
of natural ability or temperamental characteristic. activities. It takes the form of spontaneous learning

170
Academic Talent Development

and practice, acquired mostly subconsciously with- learning, to musical activities, or to athletic ones,
out regular attention to its growth. could impact the development of related natural
One cannot imagine a developmental process abilities.
without catalytic influences, intrapersonal and ■■ Resources subcomponent—Government pro-
environmental. These two sets of catalysts appear grams developed to improve the school prepared-
structurally identical to their DMGT counterparts, ness (i.e., cognitive abilities) of at-risk children
although the exact contents within each element will (Nijenhuis, Jongeneel-Grimen, & Kirkegaard,
differ, as well as their relative causal significance. 2014) represent interesting efforts to build up
Two subcomponents, self-awareness and resources, these natural abilities.
play a much more modest causal role in the DMNA
In sum, natural abilities proceed through a devel-
than in the DMGT (they appear in lighter font in
opmental process somewhat similar to the talent
Figure 11.1). For instance, we cannot expect young
development process. The same basic “ingredients”
children to show the same level of awareness toward
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

are involved in fostering or hindering their growth.


their strengths and weaknesses as older individuals,
As Angoff (1988) highlighted, the most significant
but intense interests and passions can manifest them-
distinction between gifts and talents remains the
selves very early (see Gagné & McPherson, 2016).
amount of direct genetic contribution. The DMNA
Similarly, within the realm of mental traits, very
makes that point clear in its choice of building
large individual differences appear as soon as they
blocks.
are assessed, either through self, parent, or teacher
ratings. With respect to motivational issues, children Merging the differentiating model of giftedness
express very early their desire—or lack of it—to and talent and the developmental model for natu-
engage in all kinds of daily activities: physical exer- ral abilities into the integrative model of talent
cise, reading, learning to play a musical instrument, development.  As soon as the DMNA was con-
video games, playing with friends, and so forth. To ceived, it became clear that joining the two develop-
some extent, their level of interest will influence the mental models into the IMTD would bring closure
amount of their short-term or long-term investment, to these theoretical musings. Figure 11.1 illustrates
as well as their potential decision to participate in a the result, with the gifts component’s central posi-
talent development program and to maintain their tion ensuring the linkage between the DMNA’s
involvement in it. Environmental catalysts also play build-up of outstanding natural abilities on the left
a significant role in fostering or hindering the devel- side and the DMGT’s talent development process on
opment of human aptitudes; and all three subcom- the right side. IMTD shows how talent development
ponents are involved, except that resources play a has its distal origins in the progressive emergence
lesser causal role because of the informal nature of of natural abilities, as early as through the complex
children’s developmental activities. A few examples process of embryogenesis. The maturation process
are described next. will continue after birth as the various natural abili-
ties, mental and physical, progressively take form
■■ Milieu—Recent studies (e.g., Harden, at different levels of expression from one individual
Turkheimer, & Loehlin, 2007) have shown that to the next, thanks to the contribution of the two
the degree of heritability (H) of cognitive abilities sets of catalysts, as well as innumerable daily occa-
varies with the socioeconomic level of the fami- sions for informal learning and exercise. At some
lies; the H component’s importance decreases point, usually during childhood or early adolescence
significantly in low-income families. In fact, the depending on the talent chosen, some gifted individ-
whole area of gene-by-environment interactions uals will choose a talent field that fits their perceived
belongs to the environmental component. profile of natural abilities and interests, and begin
■■ Individuals subcomponent—Any interventions the long and complex journey that leads to eventual
by the parents to create a specific family environ- top performance, as described in the DMGT model.
ment, propitious either to general knowledge Some individuals will go far beyond the basic 10%

171
Françoys Gagné

threshold of minimal talent, others will not, and the Academic Talent Development:
reasons behind the level of expertise achieved by Programming With Best Practices
talentees will borrow from many of the facets that
The beginning of this chapter posed the question,
comprise the DMGT.
“Which educational resources will maximize the
transformation of outstanding aptitudes into aca-
Summary and Conclusions
demic excellence?” Most school districts in the
This first part of the chapter aimed to introduce a
United States and abroad address that question
theoretical framework that would explain the emer-
with a variety of provisions grouped under the label
gence of outstanding school achievements, called
“gifted program” (see Part I of this handbook). In
academic talents. That framework was the IMTD,
line with this theoretical framework, I use “ATD
which brings together a proximal interpretive model,
program” instead of the more commonly used gifted
the DMGT, and a more distal one, the DMNA (see
program; not only does it identify the desired goal
Figure 11.1). The DMGT brings together, in con-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

of the educational resources to be described, but it


stant complex interactions, four groups of behavior-
allows giving the term “program” a meaning that
ally defined causal influences: outstanding natural
differs in most instances from its use in gifted pro-
abilities or gifts, a long-term developmental process,
grams. Moreover, I find the label gifted program
intrapersonal catalysts, and environmental catalytic
difficult to justify semantically—the programs them-
influences. For its part, the DMNA focuses on the
selves are not gifted, just their target population.4
biological foundations of natural abilities, proposing
three biological levels (chemical, physiological, and
Programs Versus Provisions
morphological), and describing their role as build-
The concept of program used here endorses a semi-
ing blocks for the DMGT’s natural abilities. This dis-
nal distinction proposed over three decades ago by
cussion leads to the following theoretical definition
Abraham Tannenbaum (1983).
of ATD within the IMTD framework.
A program is a comprehensive offering,
Academic talent development cor- sequenced over a long period of time,
responds to the progressive transfor- usually designed as a requirement, and
mation through a long-term learning very much a major part of the total school
process of biologically anchored, infor- curriculum. Thus, the school offers pro-
mally developed, and mostly cognitive grams in mathematics, literature, art,
outstanding natural abilities (gifts) social studies, and the like. A provision,
into equally outstanding systematically on the other hand, is more fragmentary,
developed academic competencies (e.g., an ad hoc offering, relatively brief in
knowledge and skills—talents), thanks duration, often designed by an individual
to constant moderating interactions teacher with special abilities rather than
with two large groups of catalysts, intra- by a curriculum committee, and supple-
personal characteristics and environ- mental to the major offerings, not integral
mental influences. with them. (p. 515)
This definition answers the initial question pro- Borland (1989) built on Tannenbaum’s distinction.
posed as a guiding principle at the beginning of this Although he considered “that there is nothing at
chapter, namely “which personal and contextual all wrong with provisions for the gifted,” and that
causal influences contribute significantly to the they “may be among the most valuable [opportuni-
emergence of excellence in school subjects?” ties] offered to students in their school careers,” he

Under this logic, should programs targeting other special populations be labeled autistic programs, mentally deficient programs, hearing impaired
4

programs, and so forth? Note that the same questioning applies to gifted teachers, gifted resources, gifted education, and other similar labels.

172
Academic Talent Development

judged these provisions to have a major drawback, programs. There is one exception—I use the expres-
namely that they are not “programmatic,” that there sion gifted programs to refer to the same ensemble
is “no guarantee that all gifted students in the sys- of provisions that this label targets in the gifted edu-
tem will be exposed to them” (p. 44). He summa- cation literature, including within this handbook.
rized the main differences as follows. The final construct to be introduced is that of an
ATD pathway. It corresponds within a school dis-
In many respects, programs are every-
trict to an uninterrupted sequence of ATD programs
thing provisions are not. Whereas provi-
covering the whole K–12 educational course.
sions are often temporary expedients,
programs are designed to be permanent
Seven Essential Characteristics
features of school districts’ educational
Moon and Rosselli (2000) proposed to break down
offerings. Whereas provisions are frag-
talent development programs into three main com-
mentary, programs have well-articulated
ponents: (a) the definition of the program’s devel-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

sequences of goals, skills, and content.


opmental goals, (b) the identification of the target
Whereas provisions are extracurricular,
population (e.g., talentees), and (c) the content of
programs consist of activities that con-
the proposed developmental intervention, in terms
stitute a prescribed part of the course
of its curriculum and its administrative parameters.
of study of identified gifted students.
The goal of an IMTD-inspired ATD program is clear:
Whereas provisions are optional, pro-
foster, through the best educational practices avail-
grams are required for all gifted students
able, the maximal transfer of cognitive gifts into
who move through the system. (p. 44)
academic excellence.6 The target population is also
Borland (1989) considered his definition “a clearly identified—students who are best prepared
list of the specifications of an ideal program,” but to profit from the program’s content (curriculum
added: “Even if these specifications are met, there is and format). This leaves the third component to
no assurance that the program will be a good one” be defined: Which resource components can best
(p. 45).5 Note that the two labels do not represent foster the emergence of academic talent? When I
qualitatively distinct categories, but opposite poles first considered this question, I was not looking for
on a continuum. Therefore, some educational small details, but for general characteristics that
resources could possess characteristics that place could apply to most learning situations in general
them somewhere between the two poles. Moreover, education, from kindergarten to high school and
both scholars considered that most existing gifted beyond. I surveyed the professional and scientific
programs at that time belonged much more to the literature, taking note of suggestions from vari-
provision than to the program pole. That observa- ous scholars and professionals. I also examined a
tion probably inspired Tannenbaum’s differentia- diversity of existing gifted programs (e.g., pull-out
tion. I consider this judgment to apply equally well classes, weekend activities, grade skipping, special
to current gifted programs, which are discussed later selective high schools, advanced placement, sum-
in this chapter. All uses of the label program within mer camps); I found a huge diversity of practices,
this chapter will imply the differentiating character- but little homogeneity of components. I examined
istics described by Tannenbaum and Borland (e.g., the best practices acknowledged in other talent
long-term, district planning, part of regular cur- development fields, especially the well-structured
riculum, compulsory for target population). In other fields of music and sport. I found much more con-
words, there is no need to mention them as con- vergence and homogeneity in goals and practices,
stituent characteristics of the IMTD-inspired ATD and they offered plenty of materials that could be

This seminal conceptual distinction between provisions and programs, twice advanced 3 decades ago, has had no impact on the terminological habits
5

of practitioners; terminological fuzziness remains one of the differentiating characteristics between the social and natural sciences.
Choosing this particular program goal does not exclude adopting parallel goals within a given program, for instance developing personal maturity
6

and social conscience, or fostering physical well-being.

173
Françoys Gagné

applied in educational settings. A synthesis of that and academic excellence expresses itself as out-
search first took form as the “10 commandments” standing mastery of the official K—12 curriculum.
for ATD (Gagné, 2007). That initial inventory was This is the curriculum that must be enriched for
subsequently reduced to six (Gagné, 2011), then academic talentees to experience regular learning
finalized (Gagné, 2015) into seven constituent ele- challenges. The term curriculum covers the content
ments judged essential to ensure the effectiveness of of specific subject matters at a particular grade level,
an ATD program: and their integrated structure within and between
grade levels; it also includes instructional strategies.
1. enriched K–12 curriculum A service that does not have as its mission to imple-
2. systematic daily enrichment ment this keystone characteristic cannot receive the
3. full-time ability grouping IMTD-inspired ATD label. This is also the key ele-
4. customized/accelerated pacing ment in Tannenbaum’s (1983) definition of a proper
5. personal excellence goals program for gifted students. As he pointed out,
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

6. highly selective access “enrichment for the gifted is as much an educational


7. early introduction imperative as is the ‘common core’ for the general
The first four characteristics target Moon and school population” (p. 424). The recently proposed
Rosselli’s (2000) content/format component, the advanced academics model (Peters, Matthews,
fifth characteristic targets the program’s goals, and McBee, & McCoach, 2014) recommends a similar
the last two characteristics target the talentee popu- curricular priority. I use the term enriched with the
lation. An enriched K–12 curriculum is a keystone clear awareness that I was “delinquently” rejecting
characteristic—grouping all the others according to the politically correct custom of my colleagues, who
program components solved a conundrum, namely have adopted the term differentiation (e.g., Borland,
trying to create some hierarchy among them. Except 1989; French, 2009; Kaplan, 2009; Renzulli, 2009;
for the early introduction characteristic, which VanTassel-Baska & Little, 2003). It is a very sad
targets the point of departure of a structured ATD thing that perceived political pressures or public ste-
pathway, I consider the six other constituent charac- reotypes (e.g., a nonenriched curriculum is a “poor”
teristics as necessary components. These seven curriculum) force professionals to put aside proper
characteristics lead to the following formal defini- terminology. I have argued repeatedly (e.g., Gagné,
tions of ATD as either a program offered by a school 2007) for the rehabilitation of the concept of enrich-
system or a developmental process followed by ment, for the simple reason that it best describes the
academic talentees. type of differentiation specifically appropriate for
fast learners.
An IMTD-inspired ATD program is a What does an enriched curriculum look like? At
customized long-term sequence of struc- the broadest level, that of a structured set of subject
tured learning activities anchored in a matters, it does not differ substantially from stan-
consistantly enriched and challenging dard curriculum; most adaptations appear to target
academic curriculum directed toward the specific contents at particular grade levels, as well
attainment of high-level excellence goals. as instructional strategies (e.g., Hertberg-Davis &
An (ATD) process refers to the pursuit Callahan, 2013; Tomlinson, 2009; VanTassel-
by academic talentees of personal long- Baska & Little, 2003). For instance, Rogers (2009)
term excellence goals within an IMTD- identified seven research-based content—and
inspired ATD program. instructional—modifications that provide “signifi-
cant academic benefits for gifted learners” (p. 264):
Each of the seven constituent characteristics are
abstract concepts, complex contents, multidisci-
examined in more detail next.
plinary themes, sequence reorganization, links with
1. An enriched K–12 curriculum.  By definition, human and social issues, introduction of profes-
ATD programs aim to foster academic excellence, sional inquiry methods, and subject acceleration

174
Academic Talent Development

(see Chapter 23, this handbook). With respect to force them to idle regularly nor too fast to create
instructional strategies, I proposed (Gagné, 2007) feelings of helplessness. In the case of academic
four different types of enrichment, called the four talentees, teachers must look out regularly for signs
Ds: (1) density, (2) difficulty, (3) depth, and of unchallenging content; high-achieving students
(4) diversity. This sequence reflects a decreasing often struggle to face, day after day, the consistently
order of relevance, giving priority to enrichment in slow and repetitious pace in standard classrooms.
density. Also called curriculum condensation or com- This particular problem rarely surfaces in sports
pacting (Reis, Burns, & Renzulli, 1992), it serves or arts, where talent development practices almost
as the pedagogical core of a properly enriched cur- automatically maintain a cutting edge teaching
riculum. ATD specialists should prioritize it over strategy.
other forms of enrichment because it offers the
most relevant response to giftedness’s trademark, 3. Full-time ability grouping.  This next charac-
namely ease and speed in learning. Moreover, the teristic corresponds with the preceding one: how
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

school time liberated through faster mastery of can we best deliver daily enrichment to talentees, if
subject matter creates learning space for additional not by grouping them with a specially trained ATD
enrichment. teacher?7 Yet, this administratively sensible solution,
especially its full-time variety, touches a sensitive
2. Systematic daily enrichment.  This second chord, probably even more sensitive than the sub-
characteristic might look almost tautological ject of academic acceleration (see the Customized/
because of similarities with the first characteristic Accelerated Pacing section). Commonly discussed
(an enriched K–12 curriculum) with its enrichment in gifted education handbooks 20 years ago (e.g.,
focused on condensing the standard curriculum, Colangelo & Davis, 1997; Davis & Rimm, 1985;
which implies its implementation on a daily basis. Heller, Mönks, & Passow, 1993), the subject of abil-
I perceived a need for its inclusion because many ity grouping has almost disappeared from recent
teachers or school administrators are worried about handbooks, as a separate chapter (e.g., Balchin et al.,
the (mythic) cataclysmic impact of accelerative mea- 2009; Callahan & Hertberg-Davis, 2013; Dixon &
sures; these unfounded fears lead them to refuse that Moon, 2006; Heller, Mönks, Sternberg, & Subotnik,
their talentees progress too far ahead while remain- 2000; MacFarlane & Stambaugh, 2009; Shavinina,
ing in standard classrooms. Accordingly, after allow- 2009) and as an entry in encyclopedia-type hand-
ing a short burst of enrichment in density, talentees books (e.g., Kerr, 2009; Plucker & Callahan, 2008).
switch to other types of enrichment, like enrich- I defended the role of grouping early in my career
ment in depth (long-term projects) or enrichment (Gagné, 1987), and the arguments I invoked then
in diversity (noncurricular short-term activities). are as valid today as they were three decades ago:
Talentees will progress in brief rapid spurts followed Opposition to the full-time grouping of talentees
by pauses occupied with “lateral” enrichment, end- remains hard to understand in view of the research
ing their school year more or less at the same level evidence on the positive academic impacts of
of subject matter mastery as their well-performing grouping (Kulik, 2003; Rogers & Span, 1993) and
nongifted learning peers. Appropriate enrichment the almost total lack of enrichment activities in
must propose intellectual challenges on a daily standard classrooms that specifically target aca-
basis. Vygotsky’s (1978) concept of zone or proximal demically talented students (Archambault et al.,
development, as well as Brody and Stanley’s (2005) 1993). At all levels of the K–12 educational system,
talent search instructional approach, aptly convey teachers prioritize students with learning difficul-
the need to maintain students’ pace at the cutting ties. Moreover, the curriculum of most preservice
edge of their learning capacity, neither too slow to teacher training programs reflects the low priority of

ATD teacher is more appropriate and clear than gifted teacher. We could similarly substitute academic talentee for gifted student, ATD for gifted
7

education, and National Association for the Development of Academic Talent for National Association for Gifted Children.

175
Françoys Gagné

talented students’ needs; courses on special popu- in support of all forms of accelerative enrichment
lations give only lip service to the characteristics (Colangelo, Assouline, & Gross, 2004; Rogers,
and educational needs of academically talented 1991). Borland (1989) summarized the problem as
students (Croft, 2003). In that context, responding follows: “Acceleration is one of the most curious
adequately to the special educational needs of fast phenomena in the field of education. I can think of
learners becomes a mission impossible. no other issue in which there is such a gulf between
This leads directly to the generalization of full- what research has revealed and what most practitio-
time grouping as the only effective way to create ners believe” (p. 185). Similar statements abound in
appropriate classroom conditions for sustained the gifted education literature, including a remark-
daily enrichment; by grouping thirty or so students able metaphor involving medical innovation pro-
around a single ATD teacher, it also provides an posed by Durr (1964).
efficient use of limited specialized resources. In a
nutshell, full-time grouping answers a full-time 5. Personal excellence goals.  Four qualifiers (per-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

need with a full-time solution, facilitates the enrich- sonal, excellence, challenging, long-term) describe
ment of all subject matters in the standard curricu- the educational goals that talentees are invited to
lum, and contrary to most pull-out services, does set for themselves, however only the first two are
not require adding a teacher to the school faculty. included in the heading of this section. Excellence
Recent evidence gives additional strength to that goals must be understood normatively, which means
solution. An important evaluation study (VanTassel- in relationship with the expected achievements of
Baska et al., 2008) confirmed the enormous time learning peers. As members of a highly selective
and financial resources required to train regular group (see Full-Time Ability Grouping and Highly
elementary school teachers to implement language Selective Access sections), talentees’ reference
arts enrichment modules in their classroom. A team base differs from that of standard education stu-
of university specialists had to invest hundreds of dents. They are no longer “big fish in a little pond”
hours of professional time over a period of 2 years (Marsh & Hau, 2003; Plucker et al., 2004), but have
to train just 12 elementary school teachers to an become smaller fish in the bigger pond of talented
acceptable level in the proper use of these enrich- classmates. Therefore, these goals should far exceed
ment materials, which covered about a third of a the level of academic excellence typically expected
school year in a single subject matter. within the standard curriculum. Obtaining high
marks in a standard classroom has nothing to do
4. Customized/accelerated pacing.  Grouping with ATD; most academically talented students can
talentees to offer an enriched curriculum does not reach such goals easily. Their normative status also
mean that all individual differences in learning distinguishes them from personal bests, which can
pace have disappeared; these individual differences apply to the academic goals of all students. The per-
produce an increasing gap over time between slow sonal qualifier refers to talentees not only choosing
and fast learners, which has been called a fan spread these educational goals themselves, but also revising
effect (Gagné, 2005). Analyses of achievement test them periodically; they have full ownership.
scores, as well as results from talent searches, show The challenging qualifier means that these per-
a large gap in knowledge and skills between mildly sonal excellence goals should incite talentees to
talented students and their exceptionally talented leave the security formerly offered by their big
peers (Gagné, 2005; Lupkowski-Shoplik, Benbow, fish status, and accept to test their learning limits,
Assouline, & Brody, 2003). Consequently, those not only in cognitive terms, but also with respect
who progress significantly faster than other talentees to their motivation and volition. The final quali-
should be allowed to move ahead at an accelerated fier, long-term, refers to a goal-setting process that
pace. Unfortunately, most accelerative measures looks beyond a few weeks or months, trying to
face strong resistance from most administrators, encompass at least a full segment (e.g., elementary,
teachers, and parents despite scientific evidence middle school, high school) of the K–12 educational

176
Academic Talent Development

trajectory. Consequently, these goals cannot apply 2001; Cox, Daniel, & Boston, 1985; Johnsen, 2009)
to popular activities like summer camps, once a have shown that two identification instruments
week pull-out classes, or weekend enrichment activ- outrank all others in terms of their prevalence:
ities; they need to target main academic objectives (a) IQ scores from group-administered cognitive
relevant to the enriched curriculum. Goals must also ability tests, and (b) scores from local subject exams
involve a substantial investment in time and effort. and/or standardized achievement tests (SATs).
On the other hand, these goals do not need to be Indeed, the ubiquity of these two measures led me
ultimate or peak achievement goals (e.g., complet- to propose the acronym IGAT—intellectually gifted
ing a PhD), at least not before entering high school. and academically talented—to describe the typi-
However, if some young talentees entertain long- cal population of students in most gifted programs
term career plans, so much the better. But such (Gagné, 2007). In other words, being bright is rarely
passionate involvements remain quite rare. sufficient to deserve the gifted label and gain access
to local programs; students must also show high
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

6. Highly selective access.  This sixth character- academic performance. The IGAT acronym conveys
istic follows directly from the first two defining that idea of “bright achievers.”
characteristics: an enriched curriculum offered daily. If both sources of information dominate iden-
ATD requires not only outstanding natural learning tification criteria, which of the two should receive
abilities, but also, as with any other developmental priority, IQ scores or SATs? At first glance, indices
program, demonstrated probability of future suc- of academic talent appear simple, at the data col-
cess. To best assess this probability, we look for cri- lection and interpretation levels. Yet, that easy
teria that have been shown to significantly predict metric and straightforward meaning hides a much
achievement in the program; programs with many more complex interpretive power. According to the
years of activity should have gathered that kind of DMGT, talents result from the progressive trans-
information. These predictors can be found among formation of high natural abilities through a long
the dozens of variables included in the four causal developmental process, with the catalytic help of
components (gifts, talent development, intraper- personal characteristics and environmental influ-
sonal catalysts, and environmental catalysts) of the ences. Consequently, measures of talent incorporate
DMGT. There is of course a limited pool of scien- the combined influences of all these distinct sources;
tifically proven predictors, crowned by intellectual it gives them very complex roots. They have roots in
aptitudes (Macintosh, 2011), which includes, as the genetics of high natural abilities, roots in passion
most powerful predictors, intrapersonal characteris- and interest for a field’s knowledge and skills, roots
tics like conscientiousness, deliberate practice, love in unfailing perseverance and will power, roots in
of learning, will power, and grit (e.g., Duckworth, parental and teacher support, and, roots in chance
Peterson, Matthews, & Kelly, 2007; Ericsson, 2006; (i.e., good and bad luck). This is no doubt why
Gagné, 2004; von Stumm, Hell, & Chamorro- achievement measures predict future achievement
Premuzic, 2011). The relative predictive power of so well, much better by far than any aptitude mea-
these variables will certainly vary to some extent sure. For instance, Marques, Pais-Ribeiro, and Lopez
from one ATD program to the next. (2011) found correlations above .90 between con-
A partial digression relevant to the DMGT gifts/ secutive aggregated subject matter achievements in
talents differentiation seems appropriate with Grades 6 to 8. For his part, Muijs (1997) observed
respect to the identification or selection process: an “extremely strong relationship [between] school
The relative importance of measures of cognitive achievement in Wave 1 [Grade 4] with school
abilities (with intellectual giftedness as its outstand- achievement in Wave 2 . . . a fact born out by a Pear-
ing manifestation) as opposed to measures of school son correlation of .88 (p < .001) over time” (p. 272).
achievement (and academic talent as its outstand- Talent scouts usually identify future talentees by
ing manifestation). Various surveys of identification observing the noncompetitive learning activities of
practices in school districts (e.g., Coleman & Cross, a mixed group of learners (e.g., standard schooling,

177
Françoys Gagné

music lessons, playful sport participation); they look have never become popular, research evidence has
for outstanding and precocious achievements (i.e., shown their numerous benefits. After examining
emerging talent), as well as signs of strong intrinsic all 68 evaluative studies of early entrance, Rogers
motivation and volition. If forced to choose between (1991) concluded that it constitutes a very desirable
intellectual giftedness and academic talent measures, initiative for the majority of children. In summary,
I would prioritize academic talent, even if it meant this seventh characteristic strongly invites school
that some selected students would not reach the administrators to make this initial service the cor-
minimum giftedness threshold of top 10%. Other nerstone of their school district’s talent development
scientifically confirmed significant predictors, like program. Of course, qualifying early entrance as a
those mentioned previously, can easily compensate cornerstone implies that it should be followed by
for below threshold natural cognitive abilities. the other building blocks of a comprehensive ATD
pathway, from kindergarten to college.
7. Early introduction.  The final characteristic of
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

IMTD-inspired ATD programs questions a common Summary and Conclusions


administrative practice in school districts—delay The second part of this chapter aimed to identify the
structured enrichment until at least Grades 3 or 4. characteristics of ATD programs that best encour-
The justifications given appear associated with age an academic talentee’s outstanding potential.
worries about (a) a less reliable selection of pro- I described seven constituent elements that were
cedures with younger children, (b) a still fragile essential to reach that goal. These characteristics led
development in younger children, and (c) a too to formal definitions of IMTD-inspired programs and
rapid move from the playful early school environ- processes. ATD programs could in turn be sequen-
ment to the more achievement-oriented classroom tially structured into a comprehensive K–12 ATD
treadmill (Rogers, 1991). That postponement policy pathway. Concretely, it would begin in kindergarten
contradicts a fundamental law of individual differ- or first grade with an early entrance policy for intel-
ences in development: precocity can manifest itself lectually precocious children. Beyond that initial cor-
precociously. Indeed, the popularity of the Wechsler nerstone, academic talentees would follow a parallel,
Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence constantly enriched pathway through primary and
(Wechsler, 2003) confirms that intellectual precoc- high school. This pathway would be available to all
ity becomes easily noticeable by ages 3 or 4. Indeed, children manifesting clear indices of future outstand-
many children who enter kindergarten already ing academic achievement, and it would invite these
know the alphabet, can write their name, read some academic talentees to set for themselves challenging
words, and do simple arithmetic. Their intellectual academic excellence goals. Full-time ability grouping
precocity makes them better prepared than the aver- would not necessarily mean enforcing an enriched
age first grader to tackle the Grade 1 curriculum. age-grade lockstep; educators would still occasionally
Dozens of studies (see Gagnier, 1999) have shown allow further acceleration because of remaining large
that the level of cognitive development, as measured individual differences in learning pace within the tal-
by IQ and/or school readiness tests, predicts aca- entee population. This comprehensive programming
demic achievement in the first grades of elementary pathway would introduce more relevant designa-
school much better than students’ chronological tions, replacing the labels gifted children and gifted
age. The correlation between chronological age and education with the more relevant terms talentee, aca-
academic achievement among cohorts of first grad- demically talented, and ATD. Educators would still
ers ranges between .10 and .25 (Gagné & Gagnier, use the gifted label, but in a more specific context;
2004), whereas the predictive power increases to .50 it would refer to natural abilities (e.g., when talking
or more when using school readiness tests (Jensen, about gifted learners) as proposed within the DMGT
1980). In terms of explained variance (r2), the dif- framework. But the term academically talented stu-
ference between the two predictors amounts to at dent would become the more common expression, if
least a 6:1 ratio. Although early entrance provisions only because it represents the main criterion of access

178
Academic Talent Development

to, and progress within, ATD programs. Teachers selective high schools in New South Wales, Australia
responsible for guiding talentees through the various [List of Selective High Schools in New South Wales,
components of that ATD pathway would be called 2017]). When systematically implemented with a
ATD teachers. truly enriched curriculum, self-contained honors
I am not aware that such a pathway exists any- classes also represent potentially appropriate exam-
where. Indeed, most school systems in developed ples of ATD (Kulik, 2003). This limited sample of
countries do not even succeed in putting into existing programs demonstrates that the IMTD’s ATD
practice the first two key characteristics. Indeed, as model can be implemented in our field, if not as a full
discussed in more detail elsewhere (Gagné, 2011), ATD pathway, at least through partial ATD programs.
the two more popular prototypes currently found in On the other hand, their small number, especially
elementary classrooms (Archambault et al., 1993; their almost total absence in elementary and middle
Cox et al., 1985) are pull-out classes and regular schools, suggests that extensive dissemination lies far
classroom enrichment. Both practices ignore most in the future. Most school systems fall very short of
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

of the key characteristics described previously, espe- answering the educational needs of their academically
cially the crucial principle of daily enrichment of talented high school students; they have planned as
the regular school curriculum. In the specific case their unique pathway an age-grade lockstep coupled
of regular classroom enrichment, major evaluation with a slow-paced curriculum that covers the 13 years
studies (e.g., Archambault et al., 1993; Robinson, from kindergarten to twelfth grade. And that harsh
1998) have shown that the majority of these provi- judgment of academic monotony extends to almost
sions offer little more than a lip service response every developed country. Such slow dissemination
to talented students’ needs. The results revealed, should surprise no one; ATD promoters face numer-
among other things, that teachers offered these ous obstacles. The specter of elitism hangs constantly
activities no more than two or three times a month. over their heads; the low priority in most schools of
Even worse, the activities usually targeted the whole talented students’ educational needs remains a serious
classroom, leaving little specific enrichment for obstacle to increased public investment; the ambiva-
talented students. The authors concluded that their lent attitudes of many teachers and administrators
survey had painted have deep roots; and resistance toward the two main
administrative provisions needed to fully implement
a disturbing picture of the types of
the ATD model (full-time grouping and acceleration)
instructional services gifted students
will not disappear easily. Changes in terminology will
receive in regular classrooms across the
also happen very slowly. The gifted label is too deeply
United States. It is clear from the results
embedded in our professional lexicon to expect a
that teachers in regular third and fourth
rapid increase in use for the terms academically tal-
grade classrooms make only minor
ented or talentee. In summary, just as students do
modifications in the curriculum and their
with regard to their educational goals, we should split
instruction to meet the needs of gifted
our ultimate objective into a coordinated series of
students. (Archambault et al., 1993, p. 5)
more modest intermediate goals. If we believe in the
From these results, one can understand the busy- ATD model, we must maintain constant pressure on
work label Julian Stanley (1979) used with disdain educational authorities and the school community.
to describe most of what passes for regular class- As stated in my 11th commandment (Gagné, 2008):
room enrichment. “Thou shalt advocate . . . unremittingly!” (p. 237).
If we encounter virtually no IMTD-based ATD
programs in primary schools, we can observe inter- References
esting examples of ATD-style academic enrichment Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing
at the high-school level (e.g., 165 highly selective (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
public high schools in 30 U.S. states—less than 1%— Angoff, W. H. (1988). The nature–nurture debate,
identified by Finn & Hockett, 2012, or the 45 or so aptitudes, and group differences. American

179
Françoys Gagné

Psychologist, 43, 713–720. http://dx.doi.org/ Educational Researcher, 22, 14–22. http://dx.doi.org/


10.1037/0003-066X.43.9.713 10.3102/0013189X022002014
Archambault, F. X., Jr., Westberg, K. L., Browns, S. W., Cox, J., Daniel, N., & Boston, B. O. (1985). Educating able
Hallmark, B. W., Emmons, C. L., & Zhang, W. learners: Programs and promising practices. Austin:
(1993). Regular classroom practices with gifted University of Texas Press.
students: Results of a national survey of classroom
Croft, L. J. (2003). Teachers of the gifted: Gifted teachers.
teachers. Storrs, CT: The National Research Center of
In N. Colangelo and G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of
the Gifted and Talented.
gifted education (3rd ed., pp. 558–571). Boston, MA:
Balchin, T., Hymer, B., & Matthews, D. J. (Eds.). (2009). Allyn & Bacon.
The Routledge international companion to gifted
Davis, G. A., & Rimm, S. B. (1985). Education of the gifted
education. London, England: Routledge.
and talented. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Barbe, W. B., & Renzulli, J. S. (Eds.). (1975). Psychology
Difference between anatomy and morphology. (2012).
and education of the gifted (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Retrieved from http://www.differencebetween.com/
Halsted Press.
difference-between-anatomy-and-vs-morphology
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Bloom, B. S. (Ed.). (1985). Developing talent in young


Dixon, F. A., & Moon, S. M. (Eds.). (2006). The handbook
people. New York, NY. Ballantine Books.
of secondary gifted education. Waco, TX: Prufrock
Borland, J. H. (1989). Planning and implementing programs Press.
for the gifted. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., &
Brody, L. E. (2009). Personalized programs for talent Kelly, D. R. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion
development: The Johns Hopkins model for meeting for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social
individual needs. In B. MacFarlane & T. Stambaugh Psychology, 92, 1087–1101. http://dx.doi.org/
(Eds.), Leading change in gifted education: The 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087
festschrift of Dr. Joyce VanTassel-Baska (pp. 93–105).
Durr, W. K. (1964). The gifted student. New York, NY:
Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
Oxford University Press.
Brody, L. E., & Stanley, J. C. (2005). Youths who reason
Ericsson, K. A. (2002). Attaining excellence through
exceptionally well mathematically and/or verbally:
deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert
Using the MVT:D4 model to develop their talents. In
performance. In M. Ferrari (Ed.), The pursuit of
R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions
excellence in education (pp. 4–55). http://dx.doi.org/
of giftedness (2nd ed., pp. 20–37). http://dx.doi.org/
10.1002/9780470690048.ch1
10.1017/CBO9780511610455.003
Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and
Callahan, C. M., & Hertberg-Davis, H. L. (2013).
deliberate practice on the development of superior
Fundamentals of gifted education. New York, NY:
expert performance. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness,
Routledge.
P. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cambridge
Colangelo, N., & Assouline, S. (Eds.). (2001). Talent handbook of expertise and expert performance
development IV: Proceedings from the 1998 Henry B. (pp. 683–706). http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
and Jocelyn Wallace national research symposium on CBO9780511816796.038
talent development. Scottsdale, AZ: Great Potential.
Feldhusen, J. F. (1992). Talent identification and
Colangelo, N., Assouline, S., & Gross, M. U. M. (2004). development in education (TIDE). Sarasota, FL: Center
A nation deceived: How schools hold back America’s for Creative Learning.
brightest students. Iowa City, IA: Connie Belin and
Finn, C. E., Jr., & Hockett, J. A. (2012). Exam schools:
Jacqueline N. Blank International Center for Gifted
Inside America’s most selective public high schools.
Education and Talent Development.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/9781400844579
Colangelo, N., & Davis, G. A. (Eds.). (1997). Handbook
French, H. M. (2009). Curriculum differentiation. In
of gifted education (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &
B. MacFarlane & T. Stambaugh (Eds.), Leading
Bacon.
change in gifted education: The festschrift of Dr. Joyce
Colangelo, N., & Davis, G. A. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook VanTassel-Baska (pp. 351–360). Waco, TX: Prufrock
of gifted education (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Press.
Bacon.
Gagné, F. (1985). Giftedness and talent: Reexamining
Coleman, L. J., & Cross, T. L. (2001). Being gifted in a reexamination of the definitions. Gifted Child
school: An introduction to development, guidance, and Quarterly, 29, 103–112. http://dx.doi.org/
teaching. Waco, TX: Prufrock Press. 10.1177/001698628502900302
Corno, L. (1993). The best-laid plans: Modern Gagné, F. (1987). Doit-on regrouper les élèves doués
conceptions of volition and educational research. ou talentueux? [Should we group gifted or talented

180
Academic Talent Development

students?]. Revue Canadienne de Psycho-Education, Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., & Loehlin, J. C. (2007).
16, 57–75. Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents’
cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics, 37, 273–283.
Gagné, F. (1998). A proposal for subcategories within
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4
the gifted or talented populations. Gifted Child
Quarterly, 42, 87–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Heller, K. A., Mönks, F. J., & Passow, A. H. (Eds.).
001698629804200203 (1993). International handbook of research and
Gagné, F. (2000). Understanding the complex development of giftedness and talent. Oxford, England:
choreography of talent development through Pergamon Press.
DMGT-based analysis. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Mönks, Heller, K. A., Mönks, F. J., Sternberg, R. J., & Subotnik,
R. J. Sternberg, & R. Subotnik (Eds.), International R. (Eds.). (2000). International handbook for research
handbook for research on giftedness and talent of giftedness and talent (2nd ed.). Oxford, England:
(2nd ed., pp. 67–79). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Pergamon Press.
B978-008043796-5/50005-X
Hertberg-Davis, H. L., & Callahan, C. M. (2013).
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: Defensible curriculum for gifted students: An
The DMGT as a developmental theory. High
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

introduction. In C. M. Callahan & H. L. Hertberg-


Ability Studies, 15, 119–147. http://dx.doi.org/ Davis (Eds.), Fundamentals of gifted education
10.1080/1359813042000314682 (pp. 259–262). New York, NY: Routledge.
Gagné, F. (2005). From noncompetence to exceptional Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. New York,
talent: Exploring the range of academic NY: Free Press.
achievement within and between grade levels.
Gifted Child Quarterly, 49, 139–153. http:// Johnsen, S. K. (2009). Identification. In B. Kerr (Ed.),
dx.doi.org/10.1177/001698620504900204 Encyclopedia of giftedness, creativity, and talent
Gagné, F. (2007). Ten commandments for academic talent (pp. 439–443). http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/
development. Gifted Child Quarterly, 51, 93–118. 9781412971959.n194
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0016986206296660 Kaplan, S. N. (2009). The grid: A model to construct
Gagné, F. (2008). Talent development: Exposing the differentiated curriculum for the gifted. In J. S. Renzulli,
weakest link. Revista Española de Pedagogía, LXVI, E. J. Gubbins, K. McMillen, R. D. Eckert, & C. A. Little
221–240. (Eds.), Systems and models for developing programs for
the gifted and talented (2nd ed., pp. 235–251). Mansfield
Gagné, F. (2011). Academic talent development and the Center, CT: Creative Learning Press.
equity issue in gifted education. Talent development
and excellence, 3, 3–22. Kerr, B. (2009). Encyclopedia of giftedness, creativity, and
talent. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412971959
Gagné, F. (2013). The DMGT: Changes within, beneath, and
beyond. Talent Development and Excellence, 5, 5–19. Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (Eds.). (1985). Action control:
From cognition to behavior. http://dx.doi.org/
Gagné, F. (2015). Academic talent development 10.1007/978-3-642-69746-3
programs: A best practices model. Asia Pacific
Education Review, 16, 281–295. http:// Kulik, J. A. (2003). Grouping and tracking. In
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12564-015-9366-9 N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of
gifted education (3rd ed., pp. 268–281). Boston, MA:
Gagné, F., & Gagnier, N. (2004). The socio-affective and
Allyn & Bacon.
academic impact of early entrance to school. Roeper
Review: A Journal on Gifted Education, 26, 128–138. List of selective high schools in New South Wales. (2017).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02783190409554258 Retrieved from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_
Gagné, F., & McPherson, G. E. (2016). Analyzing selective_high_schools_in_New_South_Wales
musical prodigiousness using Gagné’s Lupkowski-Shoplik, A., Benbow, C. P., Assouline, S. G., &
Integrative Model of Talent Development. In Brody, L. E. (2003). Talent searches: Meeting
G. E. McPherson (Ed.), Musical prodigies: the needs of academically talented youth. In N.
Interpretations from psychology, education, musicology, Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted
and ethnomusicology (pp. 3–114). Oxford, England: education (3rd ed., pp. 204–218). Boston, MA:
Oxford University Press. Allyn & Bacon.
Gagnier, N. (1999). L’adaptation socio-affective et scolaire MacFarlane, B., & Stambaugh, T. (Eds.). (2009). Leading
des élèves admis à une entrée précoce au préscolaire change in gifted education: The festschrift of Dr. Joyce
[The socio-affective and academic adjustment VanTassel-Baska. Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
of students admitted early in kindergarten]
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Macintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and human intelligence (2nd
Quebec, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

181
Françoys Gagné

Marques, S. C., Pais-Ribeiro, J. L., & Lopez, S. J. (2011). Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P.
The role of positive psychology constructs in (2003). Behavioral genetics. In R. Plomin, J. C. DeFries,
predicting mental health and academic achievement I. W. Craig, & P. McGuffin, P. (Eds.), Behavioral genetics
in children and adolescents: A two-year in the postgenomic era (pp. 3–15). Washington, DC:
longitudinal study. Journal of Happiness Studies, American Psychological Association.
12, 1049–1062. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
Plucker, J. A., & Callahan, C. M. (Eds.). (2008). Critical
s10902-010-9244-4
issues and practices in gifted education: What the
Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (2003). Big-fish-little- research says. Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
pond effect on academic self-concept. A cross- Plucker, J. A., Robinson, N. M., Greenspon, T. S.,
cultural (26-country) test of the negative effects of Feldhusen, J. F., McCoach, D. B., & Subotnik,
academically selective schools. American Psychologist, R. F. (2004). It’s not how the pond makes you
58, 364–376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ feel, but rather how high you can jump. American
0003-066X.58.5.364 Psychologist, 59, 268–269. http://dx.doi.org/
McCrae, R. B. (2009). The Five-Factor Model of 10.1037/0003-066X.59.4.268
personality traits: Consensus and controversy. Reis, S. M., Burns, D. E., & Renzulli, J. S. (1992).
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Curriculum compacting: The complete guide to
Cambridge handbook of personality psychology modifying the regular curriculum for high ability
(pp. 148–161). http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ students. Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning
CBO9780511596544.012 Press.
Moon, S. M., & Rosselli, H. C. (2000). Developing Renzulli, J. S. (2009). The multiple menu model for
gifted programs. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Mönks, developing differentiated curriculum. In J. S.
R. J. Sternberg, & R. Subotnik (Eds.), International Renzulli, E. J. Gubbins, K. McMillen, R. D. Eckert, &
handbook for research on giftedness and talent (2nd C. A. Little (Eds.), Systems and models for developing
ed., pp. 499–521). Oxford, England: Pergamon. programs for the gifted and talented (2nd ed., pp.
Muijs, R. D. (1997). Predictors of academic achievement 353–381). Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning
and academic self-concept: A longitudinal Press.
perspective. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Renzulli, J. S., Gubbins, E. J., McMillen, K. S., Eckert,
67, 263–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ R. D., & Little, C. A. (2009). Systems and models
j.2044-8279.1997.tb01243.x for developing programs for the gifted and talented
Nijenhuis, J., Jongeneel-Grimen, B., & Kirkegaard, E. (2nd ed.). Mansfield Center, CT: Creative Learning
O. W. (2014). Are Headstart gains on the g factor? Press.
A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 46, 209–215. http:// Renzulli, J. S., & Reis, S. M. (1991). The reform
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.07.001 movement and the quiet crisis in gifted education.
Gifted Child Quarterly, 35, 26–35. http://dx.doi.org/
Nurnberger, J. I., Jr., & Bierut, L. J. (2007). Seeking the
10.1177/001698629103500104
connections: Alcoholism and our genes. Scientific
American, 296(4), 46–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ Robinson, G. J. (1998). Classroom practices with high
scientificamerican0407-46 achieving students: A national survey of middle school
teachers (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
Olszewski-Kubilius, P. (2009). The idea of talent University of Connecticut, Storrs.
development: How we got there and where we are
going. In B. MacFarlane & T. Stambaugh (Eds.), Rogers, K. B. (1991). A best evidence synthesis of the
Leading change in gifted education: The festschrift of research on types of accelerative programs for gifted
Dr. Joyce VanTassel-Baska (pp. 81–91). Waco, TX: students (Doctoral dissertation). Available from
Prufrock Press. Dissertation Abstracts International (UMI no.
9122206).
Passow, A. H. (Ed.). (1979). The gifted and talented: Their
education and development. Chicago, IL: University of Rogers, K. B. (2009). What we now know about
Chicago Press. appropriate curriculum and instruction for gifted
learners. In B. MacFarlane & T. Stambaugh (Eds.),
Peters, S. J., Matthews, M. S., McBee, M. T., & McCoach, Leading change in gifted education: The festschrift of
D. B. (2014). Beyond gifted education: Designing and Dr. Joyce VanTassel-Baska (pp. 263–269). Waco, TX:
implementing advanced academic programs. Waco, Prufrock Press.
TX: Prufrock Press.
Rogers, K. B., & Span, P. (1993). Ability grouping
Plomin, R. (1998). Genetic influence and cognitive with gifted and talented students: Research and
abilities. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 420–421. guidelines. In K. A. Heller, F. J. Mönks, & A. H.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98381236 Passow (Eds.), International handbook of research and

182
Academic Talent Development

development of giftedness and talent (pp. 585–592). high-ability learners. In J. S. Renzulli, E. J. Gubbins,
Oxford, England: Pergamon Press. K. McMillen, R. D. Eckert, & C. A. Little (Eds.),
Rothbart, M. K. (2012). Advances in temperament: Systems and models for developing programs for the
History, concepts, and measures. In M. Zentner & gifted and talented (2nd ed., pp. 571–597). Mansfield
R. L. Shiner (Eds.), Handbook of temperament Center, CT: Creative Learning Press.
(pp. 3–20). New York, NY: Guilford Press. VanTassel-Baska, J., Feng, A. X., Brown, E., Bracken, B.,
Shavinina, L. (Ed.). (2009). International handbook Stambaugh, T., French, H., . . . Bai, W. (2008).
on giftedness. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ A study of differentiated instructional change over
978-1-4020-6162-2 3 years. Gifted Child Quarterly, 52, 297–312.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0016986208321809
Siegle, D., & McCoach, D. B. (2013). Underachieving
gifted students. In C. M. Callahan and H. L. VanTassel-Baska, J., & Little, C. A. (Eds.). (2003).
Hertberg-Davis (Eds.), Fundamentals of gifted Content-based curriculum for high-ability learners.
education: Considering multiple perspectives Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
(pp. 377–387). New York, NY: Routledge. von Stumm, S., Hell, B., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T.
(2011). The hungry mind: Intellectual curiosity is
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Stanley, J. C. (1979). Educational non-acceleration:


An international tragedy. In J. J. Gallagher (Ed.), the third pillar of academic performance. Perspectives
Gifted children: Reaching their potential (pp. 16–43). on Psychological Science, 6, 574–588. http://
Jerusalem, Israel: Kollek & Sons. dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691611421204

Sternberg, R. J., & Davidson, J. E. (Eds.). (2005). Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society: The development
Conceptions of giftedness (2nd ed.). http:// of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511610455 University Press.

Tannenbaum, A. J. (1983). Gifted children: Psychological and Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for
educational perspectives. New York, NY: Macmillan. children (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
Tomlinson, C. A. (2009). The parallel curriculum model:
A design to develop potential and challenge

183

You might also like