3 Organizational Identification A Conceptual and Operational Review 1

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 24

Organizational

ORGANIZATIONAL
74O
© riginal
Blackwell
Oxford,
1460-8545 Article
International
IJMR
Blackwell
UKPublishing
Publishing
Journal of
LtdIDENTIFICATION:
Management
Ltd 2006 Reviews A CONCEPTUAL AND OPERATIONAL REVIEW

identification:
A conceptual and
operational review
Martin R. Edwards

There is a growing body of literature presenting the argument that processes of organizational
identification (OI) are extremely important in helping to ensure that staff work towards the
interests of the organization. There are, however, a number of problems with the way that
the notion of OI has been conceptualized and operationalized in the extant literature. This
paper examines how OI has been defined and measured over a number of decades. A number
of problems are identified with how OI has been conceptualized by researchers, including,
for example, issues about whether there is an affective element to identification and how
the construct relates to organizational commitment. The paper also includes a review of
previous approaches to measuring the concept of OI and raises some key problems with
existing research tools. The paper concludes by arguing for a particular conceptualization of
OI which helps to clarify the complex relationship between identification and organizational
commitment, while at the same time accommodating previous definitions of the construct.

important in helping to ensure that staff work


Introduction
towards the interests of the organization. A
The notion of organizational identification number of authors have suggested, for exam-
(OI) has become a central concept in the area ple, that OI increases the likelihood that staff
of organizational behaviour and is attracting will stay at the organization, be co-operative
increasing attention in management research with other members, and that, when faced
more generally. The reason for this is that OI with choices, they will make decisions that
is seen as a key psychological state reflecting are in the organization’s strategic interest
the underlying link or bond that exists between (Ashforth and Mael 1989; Cheney 1983b;
the employee and the organization and, therefore, Dutton et al. 1994; Elsbach 1999; Rousseau
potentially capable of explaining and predicting 1998; Van Dick 2001; Van Knippenberg and
many important attitudes and behaviours in the Van Schie 2000; Whetton and Godfrey 1998).
workplace. There is a growing literature, presenting Those who identify are also more likely
the argument that processes of OI are extremely to want to go the extra mile on behalf of
© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA

International Journal of Management Reviews Volume 7 Issue 4 pp. 207–230 207


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

Table 1. A brief outline of recent reviews of OI

Author Key focus

Ashforth and Mael (1989) Introduced social identity theory into the area of organizational studies and OI.
Dutton et al. (1994) Developed further the discussion of social identity theory and embedded OI within
the field of management.
Pratt (1998) A broad overview of a number of central questions associated with OI (for example
conceptual issues and a discussion of antecedents and outcomes).
Van Dick (2001) Showed how social identity theory could be used to extend and further understand
the concept of OC.
Van Dick (2004) Discussed issues of OI and organizational change and mergers, linkages with
commitment, dimensions and foci of identification and links with performance.
Riketta (2005) Meta-analysis of research into OI and Attitudinal OC.
Riketta and Van Dick (2005) Meta-analysis of research investigating different foci of OI.

the organization and can help ‘enhance the remain. Additionally, linked to such a discus-
success of firms’ by engaging in ‘coordinated sion, it is also important to review how the
corporate action’ (Rousseau 1998, 218). Further- notion has been operationalized over the years
more, as Cheney (1983a) and Tompkins and and what problems there might be with exist-
Cheney (1985) have argued, OI can be seen as ing measures of the construct.
a mechanism of persuasion. Through identifi- Over the last 15–20 years a number of
cation, it is argued, employees can be influ- reviews of OI have been carried out, many
enced by getting them to buy-in to the with different aims and objectives. The main
organization’s activities. The organization’s reviews, along with a brief summary of their
goals become the individual’s goals, and core focus and contribution, are listed in
those who identify strongly are more likely to chronological order in Table 1.
be motivated to work hard to help achieve Taken together, these various reviews have
these goals. been very useful in advancing our understand-
Despite research indicating the potential ing of the nature of OI, including the meas-
importance of the construct of OI in leading urement and potential conceptual crossovers
to positive outcomes for the organization, the with the notion of organizational commitment
concept itself still remains in a state of (OC). However, the broad range of most of
development, with some authors contesting these contributions has meant that, by and
the essence of its meaning (Kreiner and large, existing reviews in the area have not
Ashforth 2004; Van Dick 2001). As a conse- been able to explore in significant depth the
quence, the way the notion has been and is core theoretical and methodological issues
being operationalized in research is also under relating to the conceptualization and measure-
challenge (Shamir and Kark 2004; Van Dick ment of OI. In addition, it is significant to
et al. 2004). In other words, after over half a note the lack of consensus surrounding the
century of discussion in the academic arena, it notion of OI across the different reviews, with
would seem that there is still considerable key contributions to the area, such as Ashforth
disagreement about the nature, meaning and and Mael (1989), Dutton et al. (1994), Pratt
measurement of OI. Given the range of posi- (1998) and Van Dick (2001), all presenting
tive outcomes commonly associated with OI, slightly different conceptualizations of this
however, it is clearly important to take stock phenomenon as part of their reviews. More-
of this situation. In particular, it is important over, since the publication of the last major
to take a step back and reflect upon the develop- review in the area (Van Dick 2004), there have
ment of OI, the current status of the construct been further developments in the study of OI,
and what key conceptual areas of debate with the publication, for example, of new

208 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

measures of identification (e.g. Shamir and


Problems and Issues in the
Kark 2004).
Conceptualization of Organizational
The aim of the present paper is to address
Identification
this gap in the literature by presenting a
systematic and up-to-date review of the OI
Early Concept Development
construct that covers both conceptual and
operational issues. A detailed review of this In an early conceptual paper, Foote (1951)
kind is particularly important in the light of argued that human beings tend to identify
Riketta’s (2005) recent meta-analysis of OI, with ‘fellows in groups’, that they categorize
showing that many different measures are the social world around them in order to ‘regu-
currently being used to tap identification-based larise their doings’, and that ‘these categorisa-
individual–organizational linkages and that tions of experience motivate behaviour
there seems to be considerable variation in the through the necessary commitment of indi-
relationship between key outcomes and the viduals in all situations’ (p. 21). Furthermore,
different operationalizations of identification Foote suggested that this identification has a
used. In other words, the specific outcomes ‘compelling or inhibitory effect ... on the
found to be associated with OI tend to vary, release of varying kinds of behaviour’ (p. 21).
depending upon how OI is conceptualized and Using this explanation, Foote helps set out
measured, as well as upon the extent to which what the notion of identification consists of
the measure used has crossovers with the when applied to an organization and also
wider notion of OC. This highlights the need presents an argument as to why the notion is
for an in-depth analysis of the conceptual and important within a work context. In summary,
methodological issues surrounding the concept according to Foote, the individual categorizes
of OI that deals with such key issues. Until him/herself as a member of the organization,
these issues are resolved, there remains a risk and this self-categorization then goes on to
of confusion when researchers investigate motivate individuals to act on behalf of the
identification, as it is not always clear what organization.
psychological state is encapsulated by this Writing almost 20 years after Foote’s paper,
very important construct. This is the specific Brown (1969) presented an empirical paper
focus of the present review. investigating identification in organizations.
Specifically, the aims of the present review Brown used Kelman’s (1958) definition as a
are fourfold. First, it aims to review how OI conceptualization. Kelman originally sug-
has been conceptualized over the years, gested that: ‘identification is a self-defining
including important recent developments in response, set in a specific relationship’, and
the area. Second, it examines the overlap that an individual ‘accepts influence because
between OI and OC in order to clarify the he wants to establish or maintain a satisfying
boundaries between the phenomena. Third, it self-defining relationship to another person or
examines how OI has been measured and group’ (p. 52). Key here is the idea that iden-
highlights some of the problems with existing tification involves a form of relationship
measures. Fourth, it presents a conceptual between the employee and the organization
model that can be used to describe sensibly and that this relationship defines the individ-
the notion of OI and to demonstrate how it ual’s self-concept. Furthermore, it also means
can be distinguished from OC. The overall that the organization is able to influence the
aim is to help identify some of the key con- individual’s beliefs in some way.
ceptual and operational problems and chal- Picking up on the potential complexity of
lenges surrounding the concept of OI as a the notion of OI, when operationalizing the
basis for contributing to the further develop- concept, Brown goes on to suggest that a
ment of this important area of study. measure ‘must include four aspects of

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 209


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

involvement: attraction to the organisation, different concepts and suggested that identifi-
consistency of organisational and individual cation involves a sense of belongingness,
goals, loyalty, and reference of self to organi- loyalty or shared characteristics. He goes on
sational membership’ (p. 349). These ‘consti- to suggest that the sense of belongingness can
tute the basic components of organisational result from common goals shared with others
identification’ (p. 349). This approach is one in the organization or as a result of employees
of the most encompassing conceptualizations feeling that their function within the organiza-
of the notion, perhaps with the exception of tion is important in fulfilling their personal
some of the most recent approaches (to be dis- needs. Lee also suggests that, where identifi-
cussed later). By including loyalty, attraction, cation with the organization is in the form of
congruence of goals and reference of self to loyalty, this relates to attitudes and behaviours
organizational membership as basic compo- that support or defend the organization. These
nents, the form of identification that Brown is include ‘supporting the organisational objec-
referring to is fairly broad. tives, taking pride in the tenure in the organi-
At around the same time as Brown, Patchen sation, or defending the organisation to
(1970) conceptualized OI as involving a com- outsiders’ (Lee 1971, 215). The third form of
posite of the following three phenomena: (1) a identification with the organization, shared
perception of shared characteristics, where the characteristics, ‘implies a similarity in quality
individual possesses shared interests and goals between the individual and others within the
with other organizational members; (2) a feel- organisation’ (1971, 215).
ing of solidarity, where the individual feels a Two further definitions of OI presented by
sense of belongingness to that organization; Hall et al. (1970) and Schneider et al. (1971)
and (3) support of the organization where the are worth mentioning in this context. The core
individual supports and defends the organiza- of their two definitions involves the individual
tional goals and policies. On the surface, this accepting the organization’s values and goals,
appears to be a similar definition to that posed to the point that these values and goals become
by Brown (1969). However, there are some the individual’s own. Apparently, this goal and
key differences. Patchen (and subsequently value acceptance, and their integration into
Rotondi 1975) used shared characteristics, the individual’s own value and goal system
support, solidarity and belonging, terms leads to a degree of emotional commitment to
Brown does not refer to. Somewhat problem- the organization. This use of emotional com-
atic here is that such notions are fairly com- mitment distinguishes their approach from
plex in themselves and potentially need that of other writers at the time. In summary,
further explanation. Patchen defines solidarity at around the same time, four separate con-
as a feeling of ‘belonging to, of oneness with, ceptualizations were being presented that
of really being part of some group’ (p. 155) seem broadly similar, although when consid-
also as ‘self-labelling’ (p. 156), while he ered together, they span an extremely wide
defines support as talking up or defending and range of psychological notions.
having loyalty towards the organization. He One of the more influential writers in the
also suggests that a perception of shared char- area of OI that seems to stand out over the
acteristics involves ‘cognitive processes based years is George Cheney. Cheney comes from
on the perception of similarities between a slightly different perspective from the more
one’s self and another person’ (p. 156). As an traditional positivist organizational behaviour
example of the characteristics being referred theorists who have written in the area, as he
to, he suggests ‘shared goals’ (p. 157). writes from a communication or discourse
At around the same time as Patchen, a perspective. Cheney draws heavily on the
related but distinct approach was also posed writings of Burke (1937), who presented the
by Lee (1969, 1971). Lee linked a number of notion of identification as a vehicle or tool for

210 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

persuasion and fostering ‘participation in a suggests that individuals have effectively cate-
collective social role’ (Burke 1937, 144). gorized themselves into a perceived group (in
Cheney (1983a,b) defined OI as ‘an active this case the organization). The writers in this
process by which individuals link themselves approach include Ashforth and Mael (1989),
to elements in the social scene’ (1983a, 342). Dutton et al. (1994), Elsbach (1999) and
To Cheney: ‘a person acts to identify with some many other researchers, such as Van Knippen-
target(s), i.e., persons, families, groups, col- berg and Van Schie (2000) and Van Dick
lectives; and to a lesser extent values, goals, (2001).
knowledge, activities, objects’ (1983a, 145), The main thrust of the social identity theory
thus ‘a person may think of himself as belong- approach is that identification involves the
ing to some special body’ (Burke 1937, 268). assumption that the self-image has two com-
Cheney and Tompkins (1987) argue that ponents: a personal identity and a number of
identification can be seen as a process of the social identities. Proponents of the theory sug-
appropriation of identity involving the ‘devel- gest that human beings need to simplify the
opment and maintenance of an individual’s or social world by categorizing people into
a group’s sameness or substance’ (p. 5). Iden- groups (i.e. gender, race, nationality etc.) and
tification includes ‘the development and main- that people assign themselves (or are assigned
tenance of symbolic linkages salient for the by others) as being members of a particular
individual/group’ (p. 5). As such, Cheney and group or category. Arguably, individuals can
Tompkins present the notion of OI as both a assign themselves membership of a number
process, referring to the process of identifica- of different social groups or categories in the
tion development, and a product or the end construction of their self-concept (specifically
result of the development of identification. the social part of their identity). This social
In discussing the process of identification, categorization is only one particular feature of
Cheney and Tompkins draw heavily on dis- social identity theory (and has been elaborated
cursive perspectives, where talk and discourse within the confines of self-categorization the-
are seen as vehicles for the construction of ory). The other part of the theory is the tenet
identity and, subsequently, identification. This that individuals have a tendency for social
approach can be seen as part of a separate arm comparison in order to make sense of the
of the literature when discussing OI. The world. Furthermore, people often compare
notion that identity is fluid and subject to themselves with other people on the basis of
change and that it is constructed and framed their membership of a particular group. Addi-
in discourse is an approach that is distinct tionally, the individual has self-esteem needs
from the more mainstream positivist involving the need to have positive self-
approach. Despite their potentially conten- regard, and people will try to enhance a posi-
tious ontological position (an area for debate tive self-image either by trying to enhance
which falls outside the scope of this review), their personal identity or by trying to enhance
Cheney’s writings can, as discussed more their social identity. As such, in assigning
fully below, be extremely useful in helping to themselves (either consciously or uncon-
clarify what the notion of OI consists of. sciously) as members of a particular social
group or category, they will often be moti-
vated by the need to ensure that this particular
The Introduction of Social Identity Theory
category is a source of positive identity and
One of the dominant approaches to OI in the that it compares well with other potential
literature over the last 15 years is that associ- social categories. The main tenets of social
ated with social identity theory (the original identity theory have laid the foundation for
proponents of which were Tajfel (1978b) and one of the dominant approaches to OI over the
Tajfel and Turner (1979)). This approach last two decades.

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 211


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

Ashforth and Mael (1989) suggested that state where: ‘a member’s self-concept has
individuals who have OI are effectively cate- incorporated a large part of what he or she
gorizing themselves into a social category, believes is distinctive, central and enduring
that social category being the organization for about the organisation into what he or she
which they work. As such, OI is a specific believes is distinctive, central and enduring
form of ‘social identification’ (p. 22). In their about him or herself’ (p. 242). When referring
key paper, they argued that identification to the notion of shared characteristics, previ-
involves the individual having ‘perceived him ous authors such as Patchen (1970) have sug-
or herself as psychologically inter-twined with gested that the shared characteristics are with
the fate of the group’. They argued that previ- other members rather than the organization.
ous research confused OI with other concepts, Dutton et al. (1994) use the notion of organi-
and ‘this confusion has impeded application zational identity and imbue the organization
of the rich findings of social identification with characteristics that the individual is
to organisations’ (p. 23). Over recent years, expected to share.
many researchers have tried to remedy this A slightly different position on OI, one less
situation. steeped in social identity theory, is that pro-
In an extensive review of the literature, posed by Rousseau (1998). As part of her
Dutton et al. (1994) took social identity the- explanation of what the concept involves, she
ory and applied it to OI with some further suggests that ‘identification refers to a cogni-
refinements. They suggest that OI can be tive state ... Identification is a cognition of
defined as the extent to which an individual’s self in relation to the organisation’ (p. 218).
self-concept contains the same attributes as This in itself is similar to what other writers
those of the perceived organizational identity. include in an explanation of OI (including
Furthermore, Dutton et al. suggest that a per- social identity theorists). However, what
son has a strong OI when: ‘1) his or her iden- makes Rousseau’s conceptualization unique is
tity as an organisation member is more salient that she distinguishes between situated and
than alternative identities; and 2) his or her deep structure identification.
self-concept has many of the same character- According to Rousseau, situated identifica-
istics he or she believes define the organisa- tion is where individuals in the organization
tion as a social group’ (p. 239). To some carry out work that is expected of them in
degree, these further refinements are similar to their role. In such circumstances, situational
those presented by Brown (1969), Lee (1969) cues help encourage a perception of shared
and Patchen (1970), who discuss shared char- interests between the individual and the
acteristics, goals and beliefs between the indi- organization. Often in the work place, organi-
vidual and the organization. Yet, this approach zational members are working towards super-
can be seen as taking the notion of OI one ordinate goals and, when this occurs, the
step further, in that it uses social identity the- individual begins to see him or herself as a
ory and makes an explicit reference to the member of a group working towards a parti-
incorporation of aspects of the organization’s cular goal. The perceived shared interests, where
identity (whether goals, values or characteris- individuals see themselves as part of a larger
tics) into the individual’s own sense of self. organizational identity, is what Rousseau
More generally, according to this approach, refers to as situated identification. This is an
the organization is a big part of the elemental form of identification that can form
employee’s self-concept (the most salient per- fairly quickly and can also disappear once
haps). Furthermore, Dutton et al. (1994) particular super-ordinate situational cues are
believe that individuals share similar charac- removed and particular job roles are no longer
teristics as ‘the organisation as a social group’ carried out. Deep structure identification,
(p. 239). More specifically, OI will involve a however, is identification that has a much

212 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

greater impact on the individual. Deep struc- cation and commitment. This, as will be dis-
ture identification is where the individual has cussed in greater detail later, can be
created such a link with the organization that problematic, as these two constructs are best
an enduring cognitive schema exists whereby left separate.
the employment relationship has in some way In summary, when reflecting on how the
altered the mental model that the individual notion of OI has developed over the past 50
has of him or herself. The organizational years, the present review shows that even
entity has, in effect, been incorporated into the though there are many overlaps and superfi-
self-concept. In short, the organization becomes cial similarities in the way authors have con-
a part of the individual’s self-concept. ceptualized the phenomenon, there has also
Very recently, Van Dick (2001) and Van been considerable variation in the way OI has
Dick et al. (2004) have extended the debate been defined and approached in the literature.
about exactly what OI consists of, developing The concept of OI has been linked to: belong-
the concept further. Specifically, Van Dick ingness (Lee 1971; Patchen 1970); loyalty
(2001) argues that previous propositions put (Brown 1969; Lee 1971; Patchen 1970);
forward by social identity theory did not go involvement (Brown 1969); attraction to the
far enough in taking advantage of the full organization (Brown 1969); consistency of
scope of the theory and, in a number of papers organizational and individual goals (Brown
(Van Dick 2001, 2004; Van Dick et al. 2004), 1969); reference of self to organizational
extends the explanatory scope of earlier social membership (Brown 1969); shared character-
identity theory tenets to expand the conceptu- istics (Brown 1969; Lee 1971; Patchen 1970);
alization of OI. Van Dick and his colleagues perceived similarity of characteristics (Dutton
(2004) argue that identification consists of et al. 1994); individuals’ acceptance of the
four sub-components: an affective component, organization’s goals and values (Schneider
a cognitive component, an evaluative compo- et al. 1971); integration of the organizational
nent and a behavioural component. goals and values as the individual’s own (Hall
Van Dick’s work significantly extends the et al. 1970); emotional commitment (Schnei-
conceptual boundaries of the notion, while der et al. 1971); emotional attachment (Van
remaining within the confines of social iden- Dick 2001), self-categorization or social iden-
tity theory. In particular, of considerable sig- tification (Ashforth and Mael 1989); self-
nificance is the addition of a behavioural referential or self-defining beliefs (Pratt 1998);
(conative) component which takes the con- a cognition of self in relationship to the organ-
struct beyond a subjective state into one ization (Rousseau 1998); and a feeling of sol-
which includes actual behaviour (more speci- idarity (Patchen 1970; Rotondi 1975). As this
fically ‘participation in action’ p. 276). In illustrates, different authors use different psy-
effect, this extends the boundaries of OI into chological notions when conceptualizing OI.
the conceptual sphere of OC. In the process, These notions undoubtedly share certain sim-
however, Van Dick and his colleagues also ilarities but, fundamentally, the specifics of
succeed in integrating many social psycholog- the definitions involved suggest that the nature
ical and social identity theory processes into of identification differs somewhat from one
the analysis of OC and in showing how social conceptualization to the next. Table 2 briefly
identity theory can help us better understand summarizes how OI has been presented by
commitment in the workplace. This is in itself key contributors to the area (as discussed
an important contribution to the organiza- above). In particular, it identifies some of the
tional behaviour literature. However, by sig- core concepts used and the key definitional
nificantly expanding the conceptualization of features that serve to distinguish the main
OI into the behavioural sphere, it becomes conceptualizations proposed in the literature
more difficult to distinguish between identifi- over the past 30 years.

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 213


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

It is evident from Table 2 and from the


Affective Identification?
review presented above that existing concep-
tualizations of OI seem to cover many differ- There is widespread consensus in the litera-
ent psychological phenomena. This, clearly, ture that identification involves a strong link-
presents problems. As de Vaus (1991) argues age between the individual and the
‘if concepts have no set meaning anyone organization. However, authors disagree with
can define a concept in any way they wish’ regard to the extent to which the linkage
(p. 48), and this in turn makes theoretical should be seen as cognitive or affective in
progress in the area much more difficult to nature. Some researchers argue that there may
achieve. In addition, however, there are some be affective elements to OI (Abrams and de
specific areas of conceptual confusion. There Moura 2001; Bergami and Bagozzi 1996;
are two key issues that stand out in this Schneider et al. 1971; Van Dick 2001), while
respect and which therefore deserve closer others suggest that these affective aspects are
attention. First, as some authors have noted only outcomes of cognitive identification,
(Bergami and Bagozzi 1996; Harquail 1998; which is the core of OI (Ashforth and Mael
Van Dick 2001), there is considerable debate 1989; Bergami and Bagozzi 2000; Rousseau
as to whether identification has an affective 1998). Rousseau, for example, argues that
and /or a cognitive element to it. Second, and ‘identification is a cognition of self in rela-
perhaps most problematic, is the confusion tion to the organisation’, but suggests that
between OC and OI. Each of these issues is ‘identification can also shape and be shaped
examined in greater detail below. by an individual’s affective reaction to the
Table 2. A brief summary of the main conceptualizations of OI

Author Definition – where provided Main sub-concepts/key features:

Brown (1969) Identification is a self-defining Four aspects of involvement:


response, set in a specific Attraction to the organization
relationship between the individual Consistency of organizational and individual
and the organization. goals
Loyalty toward the organization
Reference of self to organizational
membership (p. 349).
Lee (1969, 1971) ‘OI is assumed to be the degree Belongingness resulting from common goals
of the individual’s broad personal shared with others or employees feeling
identification with the that their function fulfils their personal
organisation’ (1971, 215). needs; loyalty, attitudes and behaviours
including ‘supporting the organisational
objectives, taking pride in the tenure in the
organisation, or defending the organisation
to outsiders’ (1971, 215), and shared
characteristics, ‘implies a similarity in quality
between the individual and others within
the organisation’ (1971, 215).
Patchen (1970) – A perception of shared characteristics with
organizational members, shared interests
and goals with other organizational
members; a feeling of solidarity with the
organization, a sense of belongingness to
that organization, and support of the
organization where the individual supports
and defends the organizational goals and
policies.
Hall et al. (1970) and ‘Organisational identification is the Goal and value acceptance.
Schneider et al. (1971) process by which the goals of Emotional commitment to the organization.

214 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

Table 2. Continued

Author Definition – where provided Main sub-concepts/key features:

the organisation and those of


the individual become increasingly
integrated or congruent’ (1970,
pp. 176–177).
Cheney (1983a) ‘Identification – with organisations or ‘A person acts to identify with some target(s),
anything else – is an active process i.e., persons, families, groups, collectives;
by which individuals link and to a lesser extent values, goals,
themselves to elements in the social knowledge, activities, objects. Thus a person
scene’ (p. 342). may think of himself as belonging to some
special body’ (1983a, 145). Cheney and
Tompkins, (1987): Organisational
dentification as a process, of identification
development, and as a product or the end
result of development of identification
where the individual has a strong bond with
the organisation.
Ashforth and Mael (1989) ‘Organisational identification is a Social Identification components: Self-
specific form of social categorization, a perception that an
identification’ and ‘the perception individual is psychologically intertwined
of oneness with, or belongingness with the fate of the organization and an
to the organisation’ (p. 22). incorporation of the organization’s values
and attitudes.
Dutton et al. (1994) ‘The degree to which a member ‘1) His or her identity as an organisation
defines him or herself by the same member is more salient than alternative
attributes that he or she believes identities, and 2) his or her self-concept has
define the organisation’ (p. 239). many of the same characteristics he or she
believes define the organisation as a social
group’ (p. 239).
Pratt (1998) ‘Organisational Identification occurs Involving the integration of ‘beliefs about
when an individual’s beliefs about one’s organisation into one’s identity’ (p.
his or her organisation become self- 172) and that ‘identification explicitly refers
referential or self-defining’ (p.172). to the social aspects of a persons identity’
(p. 173).
Rousseau (1998) ‘Identification is a psychological state ‘Identification is a cognition of self in relation
wherein an individual perceives to the organisation’ (p. 218).
himself or herself to be part of Situated identification: Individuals carry out
a larger whole . . . Organisational work that is expected of them in their role
identification, wherein individuals and situational cues that encourage a
perceive themselves to be part of perception of shared interests.
a larger organisation’ (p. 217). Deep structure identification; an enduring
cognitive schema exists whereby the
employment relationship has in some way
altered the mental model that the
individual has of him or herself.
Van Dick (2001) As with Ashforth and Mael (1989) – Affective component: Emotional attachment
Links to social identity theory and to the group.
self-categorization theory. Cognitive component: Knowledge of being
a member.
Evaluative component:
Positive evaluation of the organization (e.g.
Pride).
Behavioural (conative) component: Including
actual behaviour – more specifically
‘participation in action’.

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 215


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

organisation’ (p. 218). Drawing on the self- engages individuals’ feelings and emotions.
categorization elements of social identity the- This is in line with core social identity theory
ory, Ashforth and Mael (1989) adopt an even arguments emphasized by Tajfel (1972,
more explicitly cognitive approach to OI, sug- 1978b) who, as noted by Bergami and
gesting that previous researchers have ‘con- Bagozzi (1996), explicitly suggested that
fused the concept ... with affect’ (p. 23). social identity involves both the individual
More generally, as a number of writers have having knowledge that they belong to certain
noted (e.g. Bergami and Bagozzi 1996; Har- groups as well there being an emotional sig-
quail 1998), the main focus of the cognitive nificance that goes hand in hand with this
approaches tends to be on self-categorization process. More generally, central to affective
and goal and value congruence, which are approaches to OI is the idea that OI empha-
related to the thinking aspects of identification sizes both emotional and cognitive elements
rather than the feeling aspects that are com- and that, as a result, it cannot be properly
monly associated with affective identification. understood without taking into account both
Not surprisingly, cognitively oriented authors components. This more comprehensive view
often present quite a dry and computational of OI is reflected in a number of recent con-
picture of the phenomenon, reducing it to tributions to the area (Bergami and Bagozzi
quasi-information processing and storage. At 1996; Harquail 1998; Van Dick 2001). Com-
the same time though, they explicitly discuss bining the cognitive and affective elements,
OI in terms of a self-organizational linkage Harquail (1998) argued that ‘OI engages more
that involves an attachment with the organiza- than our cognitive self-categorization and our
tion and a relationship or a sense of belong- brains, it engages our hearts’ (p. 225). She
ingness (e.g. Ashforth and Mael 1989; firmly presents the argument that affective
Rousseau 1998). In other words, even identification can be seen as conceptually dis-
researchers who view OI as mainly involving tinct from cognitive identification, although
a cognitive process of self-categorization write she recognizes that the two components may
about OI using potentially quite emotive terms. be difficult to separate in practice. In a similar
Affectively oriented authors do not neces- vein, Bergami and Bagozzi (1996) suggest
sarily deny the importance of cognitive factors that there are both cognitive and emotional
and, in particular, of cognitive processes of aspects to OI. Specifically, they argue that
self-categorization for OI. Rather, they tend to when one identifies with an organization, a
emphasize the fact that this sense of attach- mental schema exists, which has a cognitive
ment and belonging that is found when some- element as well as an emotional aspect, and
body identifies is likely to be emotionally that, when this schema is triggered, emotions
laden and to carry significant emotional con- attached to it may be excited at the same time.
notation, particularly since the process of Finally, in one of the most recent contribu-
identification itself involves the individual’s tions to the area already mentioned, Van Dick
self-concept becoming linked to the organiza- (2001) also suggests that previous authors
tion in a deep and meaningful way. Thus, as have mainly drawn a cognitive element from
Dutton et al. (1994) argue, the stronger a per- social identity theory when applying it to an
son’s OI, the more salient the organization is organizational arena and that it is important
over alternative identities. Similarly, Van Dick to include an affective element as a key part
(2001) suggests that, for an individual, realiz- of the construct. Indeed, Van Dick presents
ing that he or she is a member of a certain cat- affective identification as a central component
egory is a necessary first step to identifying of a broader model of OI. In summary, despite
with that category, but that there is more to some continuing debate as to whether OI has
identification than just self-categorization. an affective component, it seems difficult to
Identification also directly involves and maintain the position that OI is purely cognitive

216 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

in nature, but this is a view that is reflected in identification, involvement and loyalty. Their
the more comprehensive conceptualizations of conceptualization of the term identification
OI that have appeared in recent years. here involved ‘Pride in the organisation; inter-
nalisation of the organisation’s goals and values’
(p. 40). Involvement consisted of: ‘willingness
Organizational Identification and
to invest personal effort as a member of the
Commitment Linkages
organisation, for the sake of the organisation’
A major problem that exists when trying to (p. 41) and loyalty was defined as ‘affection
clarify the meaning of OI is that of conceptual for and attachment to the organisation; a sense
overlap with other individual–organizational of belongingness manifesting as a wish to
constructs, in particular with OC. There are stay’ (p. 40). Again, we can see considerable
important similarities in fact in the way that crossovers between OC and OI.
OI and OC have not only been conceptual- Most recently, Meyer and Allen (1991) sug-
ized, but also in the way they have been oper- gested that OC consists of three components:
ationalized over the years (operationalization affective commitment, continuance commit-
issues will discussed later). These similarities ment and normative commitment. They define
are partly due to the fact that both concepts affective commitment as: ‘the employee’s
are meant to tap into and describe very similar emotional attachment to, identification with,
psychological states, to the extent that some and involvement in the organisation’ (p. 67).
conceptualizations of OC actually include Continuance commitment refers to: ‘an
identification as a sub-concept. Organizational awareness of the costs associated with leaving
commitment research dates back to around the the organization, employees whose primary
same time as the earlier studies into OI (e.g. link to the organization is based on continu-
Buchanan 1974; Hrebiniak and Allutto 1972; ance commitment remain because they need
Porter et al. 1974). If anything, however, OC to do so’. Normative commitment refers to: ‘a
has been even more heavily researched than OI feeling of obligation to continue employment,
(see the meta-analysis of Mathieu and Zajoc those with a high level of normative commit-
(1990), Meyer et al. (2002) and a recent review ment feel that they ought to remain with the
of the concept by Swailes (2002)), resulting in organisation’ (p. 67). As can be seen in the
a plethora of different definitions and concep- Meyer et al. (2002) meta-analysis, the most
tualizations being presented over the years. used measure of commitment of these three
There are, however, three dominant conceptu- sub-components is affective commitment, as it
alizations of OC that can be identified in the goes to the heart of what authors tend to refer
literature. These are summarized below. to when discussing commitment.
First, Mowday et al. (1979) proposed what When one looks at how OI and OC are
can be considered to be the first of these described, it is apparent that authors are often
dominant conceptualizations, where OC is using the same terms to describe the different
defined as: ‘the strength of an individual’s concepts. The notion of involvement was used
identification-with and involvement-in a particular in the conceptualization of OC by Cook and
organisation’ (p. 604). They suggested that Wall (1980), Mowday et al. (1979) and also
this involved: (a) a strong belief in and accept- Meyer and Allen (1991). Involvement was
ance of the organization’s goals and values; also used in the conceptualization of OI by
(b) a willingness to exert considerable effort Brown (1969) and more recently by Van Dick
on behalf of the organization; and (c) a desire (2001). Similarly, loyalty was used as part
to maintain membership. Immediately, we can of the conceptualization of OC by Cook
see some major similarities with how identifi- and Wall (1980) as well as being used to con-
cation has been conceptualized. Secondly, ceptualize OI by Brown (1969), Lee (1969,
Cook and Wall (1980) defined OC in terms of: 1971) and Patchen (1970). All authors have

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 217


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

suggested that OC involves some form of con- notions of loyalty, attachment or solidarity
gruence between the individual’s goals and often used in relation to OC, as well as over-
values and those of the organization. This is lapping with more general notions of emo-
also presented as a key aspect of OI by almost tional links and affective commitment. Many
all the researchers in the area. Additionally, (if not all) of these concepts are actually used
the idea that there is a feeling of membership, to define the others as well as the more gen-
belonging and attachment is also a common eral constructs of OC and OI. As such, it is
theme in how both OI and OC have been perhaps not surprising that, conceptually, OI
conceptualized. and OC have often been confused over the
Owing to the apparent risk of concept years. Moreover, disentangling the two con-
redundancy (an issue raised specifically with structs is made all the more difficult by the fact
the notion of OC by Morrow (1983)), it is an that they both refer to almost the same broad
important exercise to clarify the crossovers phenomenon, that of a strong individual–
between the two notions. This is especially organizational linkage.
important when certain researchers refer to
the two notions as if they are totally separate
Disentangling Organizational
concepts. For example, Rousseau (1998)
Identification from Organizational
states that: ‘Identification is a cognition-of-
Commitment: A Proposed Model
self in relationship to the organisation while
citizenship is a behaviour and commitment is While recognizing that a large number of
an affective response’ (p. 218). Similarly, Ber- partly different and partly overlapping con-
gami and Baggozzi (2000) argue that the two structs (and sub-constructs) have been used
concepts are separate and that ‘organisational to define both OI and OC, there are two key
identification and organisational commitment points worth emphasizing in this context.
are two components of one’s social identity in First, most of the definitions of OC, as we
the organisation’ (p. 556). While Pratt sug- have seen, have at least three aspects to them.
gests that the two concepts can be distin- Second, OI is usually one of, or part of, these
guished on the basis that ‘identification sub-concepts. As such, it seems more sensible
explains the individual–organisation relation- to distinguish between the two by saying that
ship in terms of an individual’s self-concept, OC includes, in some form, OI. OC seems,
organisational commitment does not’ (p. 178). almost deliberately, to have a number of com-
The extent to which authors see OI and ponents that make it a global measure where,
commitment as distinct clearly varies with the if present, a number of positive employee and /
particular definitions and measures used to or organizational outcomes can be reasonably
determine the actual degree of conceptual and expected to follow.
operational overlap involved. More generally, To a degree, one of the most successful
though, problems of overlap in this area are attempts at distinguishing between identifica-
made worse by two main factors. First, as we tion and commitment is that by Cheney and
have seen, there is no real agreement in the Tompkins (1987). These writers are able to
literature as to the definition of either OI or distinguish between the two notions by sug-
OC (Swailes 2002). Second, researchers in gesting that identification refers to a process
this area are, in any case, attempting to differ- and positing that it covers the substance of
entiate concepts that fundamentally overlap, action patterns, while commitment refers to
in particular on the basis of overlapping sub- the form these action patterns take. Identifica-
concepts (like a busy Venn diagram). For tion is the ‘appropriation of identity and com-
example, concepts that have been linked to mitment is the binding to action’ (p. 9).
OI, such as membership or belongingness, Cheney and Tompkins (1987) draw on Kanter
would seem to be inextricably associated with (1972) in presenting this distinction: ‘in

218 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

Kanter’s view, then, commitment becomes a the evaluative and conative forms of identifi-
profound and expressive outcome of an indi- cation included in his construct can, from the
vidual’s linkage of the self to a collective over point of view of the present conceptualization,
time’ (p. 8). Importantly here, Cheney and be considered to be outcomes which are sec-
Tompkins emphasize commitment as an ondary to, rather than defining characteristics
expression of an individual’s identification of, OI. Secondary states, such as positive evalu-
through behavioural (or intentional) pledges. ations of the organization and feelings of
Fundamentally though, it is somewhat prob- pride by members, are presented here as sec-
lematic to discuss the two notions as com- ondary consequences of identifying with the
pletely separate constructs, especially because organization, in the sense that identification
when defining the two notions, authors, as we potentiates these states.
have seen, are seemingly talking about a very The rationale for this is that, although
similar thing – a strong linkage between the social identity theory suggests that identifica-
individual and the organization. Despite this, tion involves an evaluative component, such
however, the Cheney distinction can help us to an element may not be necessary for some-
differentiate between the two notions and body actually to identify with an organization,
clarify what can reasonably be seen to fall whereas, without a cognitive or affective ele-
within the boundaries of OI. ment, it would be difficult to argue that a per-
More generally, it can be argued that con- son did indeed identify with the organization.
ceptual boundaries around identification can In addition to this, as Jackson (2002) shows,
be considered to be limited to a specific sub- identification can lead to an exaggerated pos-
jective state of the individual. This subjective itive evaluation of the in-group. Hence, it
state does not include individuals’ evaluative could be argued that positive evaluations,
reactions to the organization, such as having rather than being a constituent or core part of
pride in the organization that can be consid- OI, are actually likely to follow or be an out-
ered secondary to this primary subjective come of cognitive and affective identification.
state. Nor does it include individuals’ inten- The model presented here does not radi-
tions to act or actual behaviours, such as their cally depart from the boundaries of social
intention to stay in the organization or various identity theory. What it does do, however, is
forms of organizational citizenship behavi- integrate many of the ways in which identifi-
ours. In contrast, OC is a more encompassing cation has been conceptualized over the years
construct which includes the subjective state and, in the process, it helps to clarify the
of OI plus certain psychological states that boundaries between identification and com-
would be considered to follow if a person mitment, drawing on many of the psycholo-
identified with an organization. These include gical processes and foundations of social
states such as willingness or intention to parti- identity theory.
cipate in activities which benefit the organi-
zation, a positive evaluation of the organization
Problems and Issues in the
and intention to stay at the organization.
Measurement of Organizational
These would be further along the path struc-
Identification
ture of the nomological model relating to OI.
Figure 1 illustrates what can reasonably be
Validity Issues and Evaluation Criteria
expected to fall within the conceptual
domain of OI, what psychological states Where particular psychological constructs are
might follow from this and how this might of central interest to a research programme,
compare with OC. great care must be applied when considering
Importantly, with respect to the most recent how to operationalize and measure these con-
model of OI presented by Van Dick (2001), structs. Moving on from conceptual issues,

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 219


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

Figure 1. Conceptual nomological model of OI.

this section provides a critical review of how The first two approaches to measurement
a number of researchers have operationalized reviewed below, which represent some of the
the notion of OI. In particular, five approaches earliest operationalizations of the construct,
to the operationalization of OI are considered are those of Brown (1969) and Hall et al.
here. Taken together, these approaches pro- (1971). The third is the Organizational Identi-
vide a good representation of the main ways fication Questionnaire (OIQ) constructed by
in which OI has been operationalized by Cheney (1982). This is presented because it
researchers in this area. Note that the aim of has been used by a number of researchers.
this review is not necessarily to provide a The fourth is the measure used in a study
detailed critique of each measure but rather by Mael and Ashforth (1992), which has
to demonstrate particular problems that exist become the main scale used to measure OI.
with some of the main measures of OI used Finally, a single item graphic measure of
by researchers in this area. identification is also considered. This last
Before examining the relevant measures, measure has been selected because it is one of
however, it is worth noting that there is a wide the most recent measures to be presented in
range of research that has been carried out the literature.
investigating various forms of identification There are a number of ways in which the
(for details, see Haslam 2004; Hogg and measure or scale of a given construct might be
Abrams 1988; Van Dick et al. 2005), and that evaluated. One way to is to consider its valid-
there are many measures that have been used ity. There are many ways in which the validity
to tap different targets of identification, of a measure can be tested. The various forms
Haslam (2004) presents examples of many of of validity include: face validity, content
these. However, the review below will con- validity, construct, concurrent, discriminant,
centrate explicitly on key measures of OI. convergent and criterion validity. The first two

220 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

forms of validity, face and content validity, work commitment and suggested that research
are the main focus considered in the present carried out where there are deficient or con-
review. Face validity refers to whether, upon taminated measures tends to contribute to the
simple inspection, the measures can be con- formation of redundant concepts. The issues
sidered to be a sensible instrument to tap the raised by Morrow are equally relevant to the
construct at hand. Content validity, in con- notion of OI.
trast, assesses whether particular items on a Five main concerns are raised in the follow-
scale actually tap or relate to specific domains ing review of measures. First, identification
of the construct as defined. The evaluation of measures used by researchers are often lack-
existing measures below will mainly be limited ing in content validity, since they do not
to these two forms of validity, since these are always seem to correspond to how the notion
the most relevant from the point of view of the is conceptualized and, as such, may not
present discussion. always be measuring exactly what authors had
It is of course essential when operationaliz- intended. Second, some of the operationaliza-
ing a mental phenomenon such as OI that tions risk contamination in that certain meas-
there is a clear relationship between how the ures are so broad that they are likely to be
phenomenon is conceptualized and how it is tapping a wide variety of psychological con-
then measured. Without this clear link, structs over and above OI (including possible
research that attempts to test certain aspects of outcomes of identification). Third, related to
either the psychological or the social world the second issue, some authors may be bor-
around us can be viewed with some scepti- rowing scales originally designed to measure
cism, as there will be a lack of construct other psychological constructs without too
validity. As de Vaus (1991) argued: ‘When much critical reflection. Fourth, because of
developing indicators for concepts, the task the conceptual confusion between the notions
is not to find indicators which match some of OI and OC, some operationalizations seem
concept which has a set definition, it is to to be measuring aspects which might equally
first define the concept and then develop indi- be expected to be part of the broader concept
cators for the concept as it has been defined’ of OC rather than identification. All of these
(p. 48). four issues challenge the general validity of the
As argued by Morrow (1983) in a concep- measures involved and, as such, raise serious
tual and operational analysis of the notion of questions about the instruments under review.
work commitment, when operationalizing a In addition to this, in places, the face validity
mental phenomenon, it is essential that of some of the scales will also be questioned.
researchers show clear linkages between a
conceptual definition and a measurement pro-
Early Measures of Organizational
cedure. The strength of this epistemic correla-
Identification
tion is a key methodological concern that the
researcher needs to heed when approaching One of the earliest attempts at measuring OI
the operationalization of a construct. If there (Brown 1969) can be seen as an example of a
fails to be a close relationship between how a low epistemic correlation between an opera-
concept is defined and how it is measured, tionalization and a conceptualization. Brown
there is a risk of deficiency (when aspects of, conceptualized OI as attraction to the organi-
or variability in, the concept are not measured zation, consistency of organizational and situ-
by a particular operational tool) and/or con- ational goals, loyalty and reference of self to
tamination (where there is variability in the organizational membership. However, the
measure not relating to the concept it is actual operationalization of this construct
attempting to measure). Morrow raised these measures a number of notions not necessarily
points with respect to the measurement of related to the conceptualization. For example,

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 221


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

the second phase of Brown’s measure asks been widely used by researchers in the area
questions relating to management and over the past 20 years. These are the scales
employee interests or goals, and reference is developed by Cheney (1982) and Mael and
also made to how such relations compare with Ashforth (1992), respectively. It also considers
organizations in the private sector. It is not a less traditional graphic scale of OI more
clear, however, what sub-component, if any, recently proposed by Bergami and Bagozzi
this part of the scale is linked to. More gener- (2000) and Shamir and Kark (2004).
ally, Brown’s scale as a whole does not seem Cheney’s (1982) original OIQ included 25
to be measuring the construct as defined and, items ‘designed to reflect three identifiable,
therefore, risks deficiency and contamination but not analytically distinct “components”
due to the lack of epistemic correlation. In [of OI] isolated by Patchen (1970) as mem-
addition, there are also possible problems of bership, loyalty and similarity’ (p. 349). The
face validity with the scale. Some of the items items involved, however, seem to cover many
in fact seem to be very hypothetical and/or potential concepts over and above the various
complex, making it difficult to interpret. Over- sub-dimensions of identification included in
all, therefore, Brown’s OI scale is an example the outlined conceptualization. Specifically,
of a measure that tends to suffer from a lack Cheney’s questionnaire seems to be measur-
of both construct and face validity. ing: value congruence; pride in the organiza-
Similar problems apply to the OI scale used tion; perceived homogeneity of organizational
in a series of studies published in the early member values; self-description as a member;
1970s, Hall et al. (1970) and Schneider et al. citizenship behaviour; loyalty; sharing in the
(1971). The first part of the measure com- successes and failures of the organization; a
prises a set of seven items borrowed and perception that the organization cares about
modified from Lodahl and Kejner (1965), the respondent; a tendency to talk about the
originally a 20-item job involvement scale. organization to others; a feeling that the
Although the face validity of this scale is organization is a family; concern for the fate
reasonable, there seems to be very little con- of the organization; having warm feelings about
nection between how Hall et al. (1970) con- the organization; and a recognition of the
ceptualize the notion of OI and these seven organization’s success. Such a wide range of
items included in the scale, hence raising items will almost definitely be accessing con-
problems of content validity. In particular, the cepts beyond the particular definition or con-
authors conceptualize OI as an integration or ceptualization presented and therefore raises
congruence between the values and goals of the problem of a lack of content validity.
the organization and the individual. However, In addition, important questions need to be
none of the items in the scale refers specifi- asked of the likely discriminant validity of
cally or directly to the goals or the values of Cheney’s OIQ. Despite a high level of relia-
either the individual or the organization. In bility being reported (alpha = 0.95, Cheney
fact, most of the items are broadly phrased, to 1983b) the wide coverage of his measure
the extent that it is likely that there will be means that the items are almost certainly tap-
variance in the final construct measured that ping into antecedents of OI and also potential
will not just be due to the extent to which outcomes of identification. It would be sur-
respondents identify with the organization. prising if the Cheney measure were not in
Thus, contamination is likely to occur. some way related to a large number of wider
psychological notions. Despite this, Cheney’s
questionnaire has been used by a number of
Selected Later Operationalizations
researchers over the years (e.g. Apker and Fox
This section reviews two later influential con- 2002; Russo 1998; Scott 1997; Scott et al.
tributions to the measurement of OI that have 1999).

222 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

Another key operationalization is the short sions of the scale have also been used by
six-item OI scale proposed by Mael and Ash- Shamir and his colleagues (Shamir and Kark
forth (1992). Despite the fact that Mael and 2004; Shamir et al. 2000) and, in a large sur-
Ashforth define organisational identification vey, by Dukerich et al. (2002). The scale
as ‘the perception of oneness with an organi- relies on a graphical representation of the
sation’ (p. 104), their OI scale is based on six merging of identities between the individual
items taken directly from a pre-existing scale and the organization and is an interesting
of Identification with a Psychological Group development with regard to the measurement
developed by Mael and Tetrick (1992) and of OI. However, despite the graphics, this is
mainly designed to measure the extent to ultimately a single-item scale and will require
which people feel that they share experiences an assumption that the individual respondent
with their psychological group. Although indi- has the same idea of what it means to identify
viduals who identify with an organization with their work unit as the researcher does. In
may well indicate that they feel that they other words, the question risks some misinter-
share experiences with a psychological group, pretation. Without other items to construct a
this is not necessarily what the essence of OI reliable measure, this scale may well be prob-
consists of (especially as defined by Mael and lematic in terms of validity. Shamir and Kark
Ashforth 1992). Other authors have also (2004) accept this, arguing that their scale ‘is
raised doubts about the content validity of the not superior to verbal scales of organisational
scale, with Abrams and de Moura (2001), for identification, only that it may be as useful as
example, arguing that ‘Mael and Ashforth’s the verbal scales’ (p. 121), suggesting that
scale is predominantly concerned with public further work is needed to judge the worth of
expressions of identification rather than its this new scale.
subjective meaning’ (p. 137).
Mael and Ashforth’s (1992) short scale has
Organizational Commitment and
become one of the most widely used in the OI
Organizational Identification
literature (e.g. Bamber and Iyer 2000; Moye
Contamination
and Bartol 2001; Van Knippenberg and Van
Schie 2000; Van Knippenberg et al. 2002; Having reviewed some of the main measures
Wiesenfeld et al. 1998) and shows good levels of OI used by researchers over the past 30– 40
of reliability, with an average alpha of 0.85 years and having noted some of the key prob-
across six reported studies (Mael and Ashforth lems associated with these measures, this sec-
1992). Despite this, the Mael and Ashforth tion concludes by considering the extent to
scale does not necessarily correspond closely which the conceptual overlap between OI and
to their original conceptualization of the con- OC noted above also reproduces at the level
struct. Moreover, despite arguing that OI is a of the measurement of the two concepts.
cognitive construct, Mael and Ashforth (1992) When one looks at the indicators used to
include items in their measurement tool that measure OI and OC item by item, it does
are very likely to be tapping into affective seem that there is considerable crossover in
states, a point also made by Van Dick (2001), measurement. As examples, the item referring
who suggested that the emphasis of the meas- to the respondent feeling proud to be a mem-
ure is on evaluative and affective identification ber of the organization is included in the
and that the cognitive element of the Mael and identification measurement presented by Hall
Ashforth scale ‘is totally neglected’ (p. 271). et al. (1970) and Cheney (1982), as well as in
Finally, an interesting alternative operation- the OC measures presented by Mowday et al.
alization of OI that has been proposed (1970) and by Cook and Wall (1980). Other
recently is the single-item graphic scale devel- indicators that seem to be used in both scales
oped by Bergami and Bagozzi (2000). Ver- include items asking whether the respondent

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 223


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

feels as though he/she shares the fate of the cation measures do not pick up. For example,
organization (either success or failure). These many AOC scales include some element of
items appear in Cheney’s questionnaire and in intentions to stay/leave, not commonly con-
Mael and Ashforth’s (1992) identification sidered to be a part of OI.
measure, as well as in the Mowday et al. Interestingly, Riketta (2005) found that dif-
(1979) and in the Meyer and Allen (1991) ferent measures of identification have differ-
commitment scales. Also included in both sets ent levels of overlap with AOC. He also shows
of measures are indicators relating to the that OI in general tends to correlate less with
respondent’s feeling a sense of belonging and absenteeism, intentions to leave and job satis-
membership or being a part of the organiza- faction than does AOC. This potentially indi-
tion. Items of this type are included in the cates that AOC is generally a broader notion
Cheney (1982) measure of identification, as than OI, as it is more likely to pick up on a
well as in the Cook and Wall (1980) and in the range of attitudes that are likely to indicate
Meyer and Allen (1991) commitment scales. how happy or satisfied the individual is with
Many other examples of scale overlap could the organization (a point made by Pratt in
be given. 1998). Organizational identification, however,
Miller et al. (2000), based on their evalu- tends to correlate to a greater extent with
ation of Cheney’s measure (1982), concluded OCB than with AOC. The explanation pre-
that, because the items were developed from sented for this is that OI measures have more
job involvement commitment scales, the OIQ specific items that include sharing the goals of
‘essentially provides a broad measure of com- the organization and, as such, are more likely
mitment’ (p. 648). More generally, the issue to have a stronger (and more specific) rela-
of overlap in measures used for identification tionship with OCB or extra-role behaviour
and commitment has been explicitly than would a broader measure such as AOC.
addressed by Riketta (2005) in a recent meta- In summary, the above review suggests that
analysis. Riketta collected data from some 96 there are significant limitations in the way OI
research studies that investigated OI, many of has been operationalized over the last few
which also had some measures of attitudinal decades. In particular, the main measures of
organizational commitment (AOC). In total, OI that have been used by researchers in this
62% of shared variance was found between area would appear to suffer from significant
the measures of commitment and those of problems of face, and more fundamentally, of
identification. Immediately, this suggests a content validity. More generally, these problems
very high overlap in the two constructs. There highlight an important underlying methodo-
is, however, 38% of the variance that is not logical issue faced by researchers attempt-
shared between the two types of measure, and ing to operationalize a latent psychological
Riketta suggests that this indicates that ‘OI as construct such as OI. This is the fact that it is
a measure in the typical OI study may well difficult to measure such an internal psycholo-
have unique features as compared with AOC gical state with questionnaire items. The con-
in its common operational form’ (p. 368). struction of questions designed to measure
Indeed, this does suggest that there is not such a psychological state is a key step in the
complete operational overlap between the two research process. If the questions are not care-
measures. This could well be explained by the fully worded, they may not measure the psycho-
fact that any composite measure of AOC will logical construct they are intended to tap,
invariably have elements that are being and the content validity of the measure will be
accessed which are beyond what is being correspondingly low.
measured by OI (rather than vice versa). Importantly, the review has also highlighted
Indeed, it is likely to be the broader notion of the substantial overlap that exists between
OC that has unique features that the identifi- main measures of OI and OC commonly used

224 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

by researchers and the difficulty, therefore, in that do not incorporate intention to stay as
trying to make a clear distinction between part of the definition of the OI construct. Simil-
these two phenomena in practice. Signific- arly, given that the Meyer and Allen model
antly, this overlap in terms of measurement includes involvement as a key constituent of
reflects, as we have seen, a more fundamental the commitment construct, one would expect
conceptual overlap between the two con- employee participation in organizational
structs. More generally, therefore, the present activities to correlate more strongly with this
review directs attention to the fact that, in the conceptualization of OC than with identifica-
process of clarifying the meaning of OI and tion. The model presented in Figure 1 clearly
developing an appropriate measure of the con- separates involvement and intentions to stay
struct, it is centrally important that the from the core identification construct. Identi-
researcher also addresses the problem of con- fication is indeed treated as a more specific
ceptual and operational overlap between OI construct than is commitment in this model
and OC. and, as such, is explicitly expected to have
fundamentally different sets of antecedents
and outcomes than OC. It is not, therefore,
Summary and Conclusion
just an academic exercise to ensure that the
In the current review, the confusion in the boundaries between OI and OC are clarified,
literature concerning where the conceptual as managers might find that having a clear
boundaries of OI lie, in particular in relation view of what these two constructs actually
to its sister concept of OC, is set out. An entail can help them better to understand the
important contribution of this review is that it specific implications of these two phenomena
firmly demonstrates that OI and OC are not for different key employee attitudes and
the same concept. Figure 1 indicates how con- behaviour in the workplace.
ceptual boundaries between the two con- Furthermore, if managers are interested in
structs might be clarified. Organizational trying to foster commitment or identification,
commitment is a broader, more general con- they may have to focus on encouraging differ-
struct than OI, which has a more specific ent activities, given the expected differences
focus. Clarifying these boundaries is an in antecedents across the two constructs.
important task, particularly given that there Traditionally, the set of antecedents that are
are likely to be different antecedents associ- presented by social identity authors in relation
ated with each of the two constructs and to identification are different from those
indeed different outcomes. Assuming that presented by commitment authors. For example,
identification and commitment are indeed Ashforth and Mael (1989, 1996) proposed
important phenomena, with different anteced- that workers are more likely to identify with
ents and outcomes, it is essential that an organization if it is distinctive from other
researchers and managers are able to under- organizations, with recognizable values, recog-
stand clearly the distinction between the two. nizable goals and if it has a strong and
When examining how the two notions have enduring identity. Additionally, if the organi-
been conceptualized (see above), it is clear zation is seen as prestigious with a positive
that the causes and outcomes of OI and OC can image, individuals are more likely to identify
be expected to be different, depending on how with it. These antecedents, although likely to
they are conceptualized. For example, actual be important in the development of OC, are
turnover might be expected to be more strongly much more specific than those presented as
related to conceptualizations of OC (e.g. Cook important by commitment authors. The iden-
and Wall 1980) that include intentions to tification antecedents seem to focus, on the
stay as a key part of the loyalty sub-construct, one hand, on what the organization is; the
than to more specific conceptualizations of OI commitment antecedents, on the other hand,

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 225


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

tend to draw attention more to what the organi- employees, while those of identification have
zation does. As the OC antecedents are based more to do with what the organization stands
on social exchange arguments, in particular, for and with its image. Given this, it is essen-
this will direct attention to highlight the par- tial that the overlap and differences between
ticular human resource practices that an the two constructs be clarified, as this review
organization has in place that are likely to has attempted to do.
encourage the development of OC amongst
employees (Guest 1987; Pfeffer 1998).
Directions for Future Research
Examples of specific antecedents that have
been linked theoretically and empirically to This review has tracked the development of
commitment are perceived organizational sup- the concept of OI over the last fifty years or so
port (Eisenberger et al. 1986, 1990) and also and highlighted some key issues that remain
the exchange mechanisms operating within to be resolved in terms of how the notion is
the psychological contract (Guest and Con- conceptualized and measured. Importantly,
way 1997). It is argued that the organization the review shows that the key areas remaining
will, through its practices and treatment of contentious relate to the extent to which the
employees, encourage or discourage staff to notion of OI includes an affective element as
be organizationally committed. In contrast, in well as to its the exact conceptual crossover
relation to identification, a number of studies with OC. Furthermore, how OI has been oper-
have shown a link between the perceived ationalized has been reviewed, and many of
image of an organization and the extent to the main measures of OI seem similar to the
which employees identify with it (Mael and tools used to measure OC. To a degree, when
Ashforth 1992; also Smidts et al. 2001). Most looking at the two literatures on identification
recently, in one of the largest research projects and commitment, it would appear that the two
to be carried out in the area, Dukerich et al. psychological constructs have been operating
(2002) showed that attributes associated with concurrently for many years, almost to the
the organization, and how attractive these point that they are competing for the same
attributes were perceived to be by the employ- research ground. It is clear from the review
ees, predicted the extent to which respondents that the overlap of the two concepts is consid-
identified with the organization, as did the erable, and more recent conceptualizations of
attractiveness of the construed external image. identification (e.g. that presented by Van Dick
This study is an example of research into 2001) continue this by extending the range of
antecedents of identification as theorized by psychological states that the construct is
proponents of social identity theory and sup- argued as covering. It does seem clear from
ports some of the propositions put forward recent discussions that OI will necessarily
in the Dutton et al. (1994) review article, involve some affective component. It can
including in particular the idea that the more definitely be argued that it would be a major
attractive the image of the organization is, omission to ignore such an affective element
or the more positive are the perceptions of as a key part to identification, particularly
the organizations and its characteristics, the as it is suggested that this psychological state
more likely people are to identify with that has such a big impact on an individual’s
organization. self-concept.
In summary, what the above studies suggest The model of OI presented in Figure 1 is
is that OC, both theoretically and empirically, designed to clarify where the conceptual
can be expected to have an impact on a boundaries lie between the two notions. OI
broader set of outcomes than can identifica- can be considered to be a core sub-component
tion. They also suggest that the antecedents of of commitment. It involves a cognitive ele-
OC relate more to how the organization treats ment, where individuals effectively categorize

226 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

themselves as a member of the organization, given to these constructs in the organizational


and they share its values and goals. It also behaviour and management literature, such
includes an affective element where the indi- clarification provides a valuable contribution
vidual feels a sense of membership and to the field. Further research is needed to pro-
belonging with an emotional link or attach- vide a rigorous operationalization of the con-
ment to the organization. Aspects such as cept, where the items link specifically to the
loyalty and involvement can be seen as notions two core sub-components of affective and
separate from, yet related to, OI, while being cognitive identification and to present meas-
part of the wider notion of OC. Even one of ures that are also linked to the three separate
the most used conceptualizations of commit- components that are potentiated by this state
ment, that of affective commitment, proposed of identification.
by Meyer and Allen (1991), can be seen to be
broader than the conceptualization of identifi-
References
cation presented here. The notion of identifi-
cation presented here involves a significant Abrams, D. and de Moura, R. (2001). Organisational
psychological linkage between the individual identification: psychological anchorage and turn-
and the organization, whereby the individual over. In Hogg, M.A. and Terry, D.J. (eds), Social
feels a deep, self-defining affective and cogni- Identity Processes in Organisational Contexts.
tive bond with the organization as a social Philadelphia: Psychology Press, pp. 131–148.
Apker, J. and Fox, D.H. (2002). Communication:
entity. Affective commitment, however, is said
improving RNs’ organisational and professional
to involve ‘the employee’s emotional attach- identification in managed care hospitals. Journal of
ment to, identification-with, and involvement- Nursing Administration, 32(2), 106 –114.
in the organisation’ (Meyer and Allen 1991, Ashforth, B.E. and Mael, F.A. (1989). Social identity
67). The definition of identification presented theory and the organisation. Academy of Manage-
here may involve an emotional attachment, but ment Review, 14(1), 20 –39.
involvement is deemed to be outside the scope Ashforth, B.E. and Mael, F.A. (1996). Organisational
of what identification is considered to include. identity and strategy as a context for the individual.
Importantly, aspects of identification that In Baum, J.A.C. and Dutton, J.E. (eds), Advances
include behaviours and some form of evalu- in Strategic Management, Vol. 13. Greenwich, CT:
ation of one’s membership of the organization JAI Press, pp. 19 – 64.
Bamber, E.M. and Iyer, V. (2002). Big 5 Auditors’
may well be expected to follow when some-
professional and organisational identification:
body identifies with an organization, but these consistency or conflict? Auditing Journal of Practice
are potentiated by an initial cognitive and and Theory, 21(2).
affective linkage. Importantly, psychological Bergami, M. and Bagozzi, R.P. (1996). Organisational
states such the evaluation of membership and identification: conceptualisation measurement, and
intentions to act (or actual behaviours) are nomological validity. Working Paper 9608-10,
aspects that fall outside the core construct of University of Michigan Business School.
identification. In using Cheney and Tompkins’ Bergami, M. and Bagozzi, R.P. (2000). Self categori-
(1987) distinction between identification and sation, affective commitment and group self-esteem
commitment, commitment involves an addi- as distinct aspects of social identity in the organi-
tional step, as it encapsulates intentions to act sation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39,
555–577.
as well as potential actions and reactions to
Brown, M.E. (1969). Identification and some condi-
perceived organizational performance. The tions of organisational involvement. Administrative
model presented here, as well providing a par- Science Quarterly, 14, 346 –355.
simonious conceptualization of OI, also helps Buchanan, B. II (1974). Building organisational com-
show how the notion fits with other cognate mitment: the socialization of managers in work
concepts, such as OC and organizational citi- organisations. Administrative Science Quarterly,
zenship behaviour. In view of the importance 19(4), 533 –546.

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 227


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

Burke, K. (1937). Attitudes Toward History. New Hall, D.T., Schneider, B. and Nygren, H.T. (1970).
York: The New Republic. Personal factors in organisational identification.
Cheney, G. (1982). Organisational identification as a Administrative Science Quarterly, 15, 176 –190.
process and product: a field study. Unpublished Harquail, C.V. (1998). Organisational identification
master’s thesis, Purdue University. and the ‘whole person’: integrating affect, beha-
Cheney, G. (1983a). The rhetoric of identification and viour, and cognition. In Whetten, D.A. and God-
the study of organisational communication. Quar- frey, P.C. (eds), Identity in Organisations: Building
terly Journal of Speech, 69, 143 –158. Theory Through Conversations. London: Sage, pp.
Cheney, G. (1983b). On the various and changing 223–231.
meanings of organisational membership; a field Haslam, S.A. (2004). Psychology in Organizations; a
study of organisational identification. Communica- Social Identity Approach, 2nd edition. London:
tion Monographs, 50, 342 –362. Sage.
Cheney, G. and Tompkins, P.K. (1987). Coming to Hogg, M.A. and Abrams, D. (1988). Social Identifi-
terms with organisational identification and cations; a Social Psychology of Intergroup Rela-
commitment. Central States Speech Journal, 38, tions and Group Processes. London: Routledge.
1–15. Hrebiniak, L.G. and Alutto, J.A. (1972). Personal and
Cook, J. and Wall, T. (1980). New work attitude role-related factors in the development of organisa-
measures of trust, organisational commitment and tional commitment. Administrative Science Quar-
personal need fulfilment. Journal of Occupational terly, 17(4), 555 –573.
Psychology, 53, 39–52. Jackson, J. (2002) Intergroup attitudes as a function
De Vaus, D.A. (1991). Surveys in Social Research, of different dimensions of group identification and
3rd edition. Sydney: Allen & Unwin. perceived intergroup conflict. Self and Identity, 1,
Dukerich, J.M., Golden, B.R. and Shortell, S.M. 11–33.
(2002). Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: the Kanter, R.M. (1972). Commitment and Community.
impact of organizational identification identity and Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
image on the cooperative behaviours of physicians. Kelman, H.C. (1958). Compliance, identification, and
Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 507–533. internalization: three processes of attitude change.
Dutton, J.E., Dukerich, J.M. and Harquail, C.V. (1994). Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 51–60.
Organisational images and member identifica- Kreiner, G.E. and Ashforth, B.E. (2004). Evidence
tion. Administration Science Quarterly, 39, 239– toward an expanded model of organizational iden-
263. tification. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25,
Eisenberger, R., Huntington, R., Hutchinson, S. and 1–27.
Sowa, D. (1986). Perceived organisational support. Lee, S.M. (1969). Organisational identification of
Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 500 –507. scientists. Academy of Management Journal, 12,
Eisenberger, R., Fasolo, P. and Davis-LaMastro, V. 327–337.
(1990). Perceived organisational support and Lee, S.M. (1971). An empirical analysis of organisa-
employee diligence, commitment, and innovation. tional identification. Academy of Management
Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 51–59. Journal, 14, 213 –226.
Elsbach, K.D. (1999). An expanded model of organi- Lodahl, T.M. and Kejner, M. (1965). The definition
sational identification. Research in Organisational and measurement of job involvement. Journal of
Behaviour, 21, 163 –200. Applied Psychology, 49, 24 –33.
Foote, N.N. (1951). Identification as the basis for a Mael, F.A. and Ashforth, B.E. (1992). Alumni and
theory of motivation. American Sociological their alma mater; a partial test of a reformulated
Review, 16, 14–21. model of organisational identification. Journal of
Guest, D. (1987). HRM and industrial relations. Jour- Organisational Behaviour, 13(2), 103 –123.
nal of Management Studies, 24(5), 503–21. Mael, F.A. and Tetrick, L.E. (1992). Identifying
Guest, D.E. and Conway, N. (1997) Motivation and organisational identification. Educational and Psy-
the Psychological Contract. London: CIPD. chological Measurement, 52, 813–824.
Hall, D.T. (1971). A theoretical model of career sub- Mathieu, J.E. and Zajac, D.M. (1990). A review and
identity development in organisational settings. meta-analysis of the antecedents, correlates, and
Organisational Behaviour and Human Perform- consequences of organisational commitment. Psy-
ance, 6, 50–76. chological Bulletin, 108(2), 171–194.

228 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005


December 2005

Meyer, J. and Allen, N. (1991). A three component with organisations. Journal of Organisational
conceptualisation of organisational commitment. Behaviour, 19, 217–233.
Human Resource Management Review, 1, 61–89. Russo, T.C. (1998). Organisational and professional
Meyer, J.P., Stanley, D.J., Herscovitch, L. and Topol- identification. Management Communication Quar-
nytsky, L. (2002). Affective, continuance and terly, 12(1), 72–111.
normative commitment to the organisation; a meta- Schneider, B., Hall, D.T. and Nygren, H.T. (1971).
analysis of antecedents, correlates and consequences. Self image and job characteristics as correlates of
Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 61, 20 –52. changing organisational identification. Human
Miller, V.D., Allen, M., Casey, M.K. and Johnson, J.R. Relations, 24, 397–416.
(2000). Reconsidering the organisational identifica- Scott, C.R. (1997). Identification with multiple targets
tion questionnaire. Management Communication in a geographically dispersed organisation. Man-
Quarterly, 13, 626 – 658. agement Communication Quarterly, 10, 491–522.
Morrow, P.C. (1983). Concept redundancy in organi- Scott, C.R., Connaughton, S.L., Diaz-Saenz, H.,
sational research: the case of work commitment. Maguire, K., Ramirez, R., Richardson, B., Morgan,
Academy of Management Review, 8, 486 –500. D. and Shaw, S.P. (1999). The impacts of com-
Mowday, R., Steers, R. and Porter, L. (1979). The munication and multiple identifications on intent
measurement of organisational commitment. Jour- to leave; a multi-methodological exploration.
nal of Vocational Behaviour, 14, 224 –247. Management Communication Quarterly, 12(3),
Moye, N.A. and Bartol, K.M. (2001). Re-framing the 400–435.
psychological contract: social exchange-based Shamir, B. and Kark, R. (2004) A single item graphic
dimensions and their relationship to organizational scale for the measurement of organisational identi-
identification and performance. Paper presented at fication. Journal of Occupational and Organiza-
the 2001 Annual Conference of the Society for tional Psychology, 77, 115–123.
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Shamir, B., Breinin, E., Zakay, E. and Popper, M.
Diego, CA. (2000). Leadership and social identification in
Patchen, M. (1970). Participation, Achievement and military units: direct and indirect effects. Journal of
Involvement in the Job. Englewood Clifs, NJ: Pren- Applied Social Psychology, 30(3), 612– 640.
tice Hall. Smidts, A., Pruyn, A.T.H. and Van Riel, C.B.M.
Pfeffer, J. (1998) The Human Equation. Boston: (2001). The impact of employee communication
Harvard Business School Press. and perceived external prestige on organisational
Porter, L.W., Steers, R.M., Mowday, R.T. and identification. Academy of Management Journal,
Boulian, P. (1974).Organisational commitment, 49(5), 1051–1062.
job satisfaction, and turnover among psychiatric Swailes, S. (2002) Organisational commitment; a cri-
technicians, Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, tique of the construct and measures. International
603 – 609. Journal of Management Reviews, 4, 155 –178.
Pratt, M.G. (1998). To be or not to be? Central ques- Tajfel, H. (1972). Experiments in a vacuum. In Isreal,
tions in organisational identification. In Whetton, J. and Tajfel, H. (eds), The Context of Social Psy-
D.A. and Godfrey, P.C. (eds), Identity in Organisa- chology. London: Academic Press.
tions: Building Theory Through Conversations. Tajfel, H. (ed.) (1978a). Differentiation Between
London: Sage, pp. 171–207. Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of
Riketta, M. (2005). Organizational identification; a Inter-Group Relations. London: Academic Press.
meta-analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66, Tajfel, H. (1978b). Social categorisation, social
358 –384. identity and social comparison. In Tajfel, H. (ed.),
Riketta, M. and Van Dick, R. (2005). Foci of attach- Differentiation Between Social Groups: Studies in
ment in organizations; a meta-analysis comparison the Social Psychology of Inter-Group Relations.
of the strength and correlates of work-group versus London: Academic Press, pp. 61–76.
organizational commitment and identification. Tajfel, H. and Turner, J.C. (1979). An integrative the-
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 67, 490 –510. ory of social conflict. In Austein, W. and Worchel,
Rotondi, T. (1975). Organisational identification: S. (eds), The Social Psychology of Inter-Group
issues and implications. Organisational Behaviour Relations, 2nd edition. Chicago: Nelson Hall.
and Human Performance, 13, 95 –109. Tompkins, P.K. and Cheney, G. (1985) Communica-
Rousseau, D.M. (1998). Why workers still identify tion and unobtrusive control in contemporary

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005 229


Organizational identification: A conceptual and operational review

organizations. In McPhee, R.D. and Tompkins, P.K. Foci and correlates of organisational identification.
(eds), Organizational Communication – Traditional Journal of Occupational and Organisational Psy-
Themes and New Directions. New York: Sage, chology, 73, 137–147.
pp. 179–210. Van Knippenberg, D., Van Knippenberg, B., Monden, L.
Van Dick, R. (2001). Identification in organisational and de Lima, F. (2002). Organisational identifica-
contexts: linking theory and research from social tion after a merger; a social identity perspective.
and organisation psychology. International Journal British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 233–
of Management Reviews, 3(4), 265–283. 252.
Van Dick, R. (2004). My job is my castle: identifica- Whetton, D.A. and Godfrey, P.C. (eds) (1998). Iden-
tion in organizational contexts. International tity in Organisations: Building Theory Through
Review of Industrial and Organizational Psycho- Conversations. London: Sage.
logy, 19, 171–203. Wiesenfeld, B.M., Raghuram, S. and Garud, R.
Van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Stellmacher, J. and Christ, (1998). Communication patterns as determinants of
O. (2004). The utility of a broader conceptualisa- organisational identification in a virtual organisa-
tion of organisational identification: which aspects tion. Journal of Computer Mediated Communica-
really matter? Journal of Occupational and Organi- tion, 3(4).
sational Psychology, 77, 171–191.
Van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Stellmacher, J. and Christ, O. Martin R. Edwards is from the Department of
(2005). Category salience and organizational iden- Management, King’s College London, Franklin
tification. Journal of Occupational and Organiza- Wilkins Building, 150 Stamford Street, London
tional Psychology, 78, 273–285. SE1 9NH, UK.
Van Knippenberg, D. and Van Schie, E.C.M. (2000).

230 © Blackwell Publishing Ltd 2005

You might also like