Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association

2014, Vol. 1, No. 2, 153–167 2326-5523/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cns0000016

Hypnosis and Memory: Two Hundred Years of Adventures


and Still Going!

Giuliana Mazzoni Jean-Roch Laurence


University of Hull Concordia University

Michael Heap
University of Sheffield
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

One of the most persistent beliefs about hypnosis is its ability to transcend mnemonic
abilities. This belief has paved the way to the use of hypnosis in the clinical and legal
arenas. The authors review the phenomena of hypnotic hypermnesia, pseudo-memories,
and amnesia in light of current knowledge of hypnosis and memory. The investigation
of the relation between hypnosis and memory processes has played an important role
in our understanding of memory in action. Hypnosis provides a fertile field to explore
the social, neuropsychological, and cognitive variables at play when individuals are
asked to remember or to forget their past. We suggest promising avenues of research
that may further our knowledge of the building blocks of memories and the mecha-
nisms that leads to forgetfulness.

Keywords: hypnosis, false memory, autobiographical memory, posthypnotic amnesia, clinical


analogs

Preface clinical and forensic consequences associated


with the instantiation of false beliefs. We next
Memory has been the object of scientific in- examine how hypnosis can be used to better
vestigation for more than a century. It might understand autobiographical memories via ana-
come as a surprise to readers, including memory logs of functional amnesia, aberrations and al-
researchers, to discover that some of what we terations in self-identity, and psychological dis-
know today about memory, and the way we orders. Finally, we will explore several research
currently conceptualize it, is the byproduct of venues that promise to enrich our understanding
attempts to understand the effect of hypnosis on of human memory.
memory. In this article, we will focus on two One of the most longstanding beliefs about
major areas in which hypnosis has kindled new hypnosis is that a hypnotized individual is able
and exciting developments for memory re- to remember much more than a nonhypnotized
searchers. The first is the area of false memory, person (Johnson & Hauck, 1999; Legault &
in which hypnosis has played a leading role in Laurence, 2007; Ost, Wright, Easton, Hope, &
alerting researchers to (a) the possibility of cre- French, 2013). The roots of this belief can be
ating false memories in the laboratory, (b) the
traced back to the practitioners of animal mag-
unwarranted confidence hypnotized individuals
netism in the early 1800s, and most notably the
place in the accuracy of their recall, and (c)
Marquis de Puységur, who reported that his
somnambulistic patients could recapture past
events in minute detail, which they promptly
Giuliana Mazzoni, Department of Psychology, University
forgot when “demagnetized.” None of the re-
of Hull; Jean-Roch Laurence, Department of Psychology, membered events seemed available in the pa-
Concordia University; Michael Heap, Department of Psy- tients’ normal state. Hypermnesia (improved
chology, University of Sheffield. net recall over repeated trials), amnesia, and the
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Giuliana Mazzoni, Department of Psychology,
belief that hypnosis is capable of uncovering or
University of Hull, Fenner Building, Cottingham Road, revealing memories were to become intrinsi-
Hull, UK HU6 7RX. E-mail: g.mazzoni@hull.ac.uk cally linked to the history of hypnosis, memory,
153
154 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

and psychological experimentation for the next The trigger for the more recent debate was
200 years (Laurence & Perry, 1988). the enthusiasm of police forces for a “new”
At the end of the 19th century, hypnosis investigative technique, forensic hypnosis, in
found itself at the heart of experimental psy- the wake of which witnesses and victims of
chology. Luminaries in the nascent field of psy- crimes were hypnotized to access crucial, yet
chology, including William James, Wilhelm consciously unavailable, information. Re-
Wundt, Théodule Ribot, and Pierre Janet, to searchers soon challenged the extravagant
name a few, at one time or another, dabbled in claims of success touted by the hypno-
hypnosis. What caught their attention were the investigators, or Svengali squads as they were
amazing memory feats that hypnotized individ- called, on the basis of suspicions that the al-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

uals seemed to perform and the various forms of leged success of hypnosis was a chimera, noth-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

suggested amnesias that they displayed—what ing more than the product of undue suggestion.
Ribot (1882) labeled the “exaltations of mem- As in the 19th century, the matter became more
ory.” It was during this period of hypnotic ef- muddled and complex with the renewal of in-
fervescence that the observations of patients’ terest in multiple personality disorder and the
hypnotic abilities led to theories about dissoci- belief that not only was hypnosis allowing un-
ation, multiple personalities, unconscious cog- conscious memories to surface, but that the re-
nitions, and emotions. Hypnotic hypermnesia, covered memories were often embedded in dif-
posthypnotic amnesia, and posthypnotic sug- ferent “personalities” altogether. Whereas the
gestion were at the vital center of the new police use of hypnosis raised the question of
psychology of the mind. whether suggestion leads to incorporating mis-
Nevertheless, what some patients were re- information, the narratives of multiple person-
membering or apparently forgetting raised ality patients raised the specter that suggestion
questions and doubts in the minds of some could create completely new (and false) mem-
hypnosis experimenters (e.g., Bernheim, 1891/ ories and crystallize imagined or fabricated self-
1973). What if the traumatic memories recalled identities.
during hypnosis were in fact the product of
suggestion? What if the remembrances were
false memories? This latter question wended its The Importance of Beliefs in Shaping
way through the experimental literature and Memory Narratives
eventually to the courts of law, where the debate
thrust the ones who believed in the power of We owe in part to hypnosis the realization
hypnosis to exhume seemingly forgotten mem- that beliefs shape memory reports (see Mazzoni
ories in opposition to those who favored the & Kirsch, 2002; Mazzoni, Scoboria, & Harvey,
idea that suggestions, beliefs, and expectancies 2010). Accordingly, what people remember
shaped the narratives that the patients reported when hypnotized depends, in part, on their be-
as memories (see Laurence & Perry, 1988). liefs about the effect of hypnosis on memory.
Sounds familiar? Indeed it does. One popular belief is that when participants
If the beginning of the 20th century saw the recall events during hypnosis, they recall more
interest in clinical and experimental hypnosis detailed and accurate memories compared with
wane, then beliefs surrounding its purported recall of the same events without hypnosis. This
memory powers did not. Hypnosis continued to belief can probably be traced to the clinical
be used in therapy to explore the past, and work of Janet (1889/1973), who was one of the
techniques such as hypnotic age regression first therapists to use hypnosis to help patients
stirred interest and provoked speculation and recover memories of traumatic events, which he
theory-building attempts in the clinical context assumed caused their pathological conditions,
(Reiff & Scheerer, 1960). Ironically, what had and to modify their memories for these adverse
unfolded as one of the most intense professional events (see, e.g., the well-known case, “Marie”
debates among psychologists at the end of the [Laurence & Perry, 1988, pp. 236 –237]).
19th century in Europe resurfaced at the end of Rooted in the work of early practitioners such as
the 20th century in the United States, again Janet, the belief that hypnosis can recover bur-
fuelled by beliefs surrounding the power of ied memories of traumatic events came to per-
hypnosis on memory. meate many therapeutic approaches.
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 155

How prevalent are these beliefs today? Legault and Laurence (2007) reported that
Yapko (1994) conducted a survey of more than 17.5% of women entering therapy without ac-
850 psychotherapists in independent practice tual memories of abuse “recovered” those mem-
that revealed unexpectedly high rates of en- ories during therapy whether hypnosis was used
dorsement of beliefs regarding hypnosis as a or not (Laurence & Freedman, 2009). Thera-
vehicle to enhance memory. Seventy-five per- pists who reported making use of many “recov-
cent of the therapists surveyed agreed that “hyp- ery” techniques (including hypnosis) were the
nosis enables people to accurately remember ones who reported a greater number of recov-
things they otherwise could not” (Yapko, 1994, ered memories. The interplay of beliefs, expec-
p. 234). Poole, Lindsay, Memon, and Bull tancies, and recall of life narratives is still an
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1995) reported comparable rates in an exten- open field in need of investigation. The trans-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sive survey of licensed clinical psychologists in mission of false beliefs (and expectations) and
the United States and Great Britain, revealing their potential consequences is a crucial aspect
that therapy aimed at recovering memories was of our understanding of memory in action.
widespread. Later, Legault and Laurence (2007)
replicated these findings in a sample of more Does Hypnosis Improve Memory?
than 200 psychiatrists, psychologists, and social Empirical Evidence
workers in Canada. Most recently, Ost et al.
(2013) surveyed psychologists and hypnothera- Is the belief that hypnosis enhances recall
pists in the United Kingdom on repressed mem- confirmed by the scientific literature? Although
ories and satanic abuse and concluded, “The re- some studies have indeed found evidence for
sults of this survey indicate that, despite the hypnotic hypermnesia, better controlled studies
extensive literature that has developed in the last have revealed how inaccurate memory reports
20 years, a number of psychological therapists can be in hypnosis. Crucially, these studies con-
hold beliefs that many memory scientists would tributed to the shift in focus of memory research
view as controversial, in short, beliefs about mem- away from the sheer quantity of recollections
ory which fail to be supported by scientific data” toward the quality of memory reports. For de-
(p. 16). cades, researchers (e.g., Gordon Bower, Fergus
Similar beliefs about hypnosis and memory Craik, Robert Lockhart, and Endel Tulving, to
are held by laypersons. For example, White- name a few) had assessed memory in terms of
house, Orne, Orne, and Dinges (1991) reported how many items participants remembered, and
that 93% of college-age participants believed on this basis they established the effectiveness
that hypnosis enhances memory retrieval. John- of encoding and retrieval conditions. Little at-
son and Hauck (1999) found that 88% of the tention was devoted to memory accuracy and
respondents agreed to some degree that “hyp- the errors of memory (Koriat, Goldsmith, &
nosis enables people to accurately remember Pansky, 2000). However, we now know that
things they could not otherwise remember (p. suggestions in hypnosis (see Labelle, Laurence,
17).” Most recently, Green, Page, Rasekhy, Nadon, & Perry, 1990; Green, Lynn, & Mali-
Johnson, and Bernhardt (2006) sampled college noski, 1998), as well as other types of sugges-
students’ beliefs about hypnosis in four coun- tion (Mazzoni & Memon, 2003), including mis-
tries (Australia, German, Iran, and the United information, can modify recall and recognition
States) and reported no cross-cultural differ- in predictable directions (Cox & Barnier, 2013;
ences in response to the latter statement about for a review see Mazzoni & Lynn, 2007; Maz-
hypnosis and memory, but fully 90% of partic- zoni & Scoboria, 2007) and engender complex
ipants, on average, agreed with it. false memories (Mazzoni, Loftus, Seitz, &
The influence of beliefs and expectations on Lynn, 1999).
memory with or without hypnosis has important
clinical and legal ramifications. Belief about Hypnotic Hypermnesia
memory shapes not only personal memory in
hypnosis but (also) waking memory reports Since the early 1970s, researchers have in-
(Mazzoni et al., 2010; Mazzoni, Clark, & Nash, vestigated normal (waking) hypermnesia and
2014) to the point of creating new identities reminiscences (higher cumulative recall over
(Cox & Barnier, 2013). Clinicians surveyed by time). In laboratory investigations of hypnosis
156 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

and memory, the material to be recalled may whether it is with meaningful or meaningless
refer either to autobiographical events— material (e.g., Zelig & Beidleman, 1981).
sometimes from the person’s remote past— or In studies that have measured recall of mean-
to nonautobiographical material, such as filmed ingless (e.g., nonsense syllables) versus mean-
scenes, spoken stories, and word lists. ingful material (e.g., films extracts, meaningful
Age regression is one of the methods of pictures), the outcome is also clear: There is no
choice to uncover otherwise “forgotten” mem- hypermnesia for meaningless stimuli (see, e.g.,
ories. In age regression, participants, including Salzberg & DePiano, 1980). Nevertheless, for
patients, are asked to “go back in time” and meaningful stimuli, most studies did report a
“relive” some past event. Sometimes, notably in hypnotic hypermnesic effect above and beyond
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the clinical setting, regression can involve normal recall (e.g., Stager & Lundy, 1985).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

highly implausible memories in the womb and Consistent with Relinger’s (1984) review, Erde-
even earlier reports of a purported past life. For lyi (1994) concluded that hypnotic hypermnesia
many people, the experience of regression can is evident for the free recall of “high sense”
be profound, vivid, and compelling. True material (e.g., poetry, pictures, films) as op-
(1949) claimed that when participants were age posed to, say, recognition memory for lists of
regressed, they successfully named the day of words or syllables.
the week of childhood birthdays. However, bet- However, there is a caveat. Most studies re-
ter controlled studies failed to replicate these viewed by Relinger (1984) and Erdelyi (1994)
findings (for a summary of this research, see that reported a positive hypermnesia effect did
Nash, 1987). In fact, convincing evidence is not take into account the fact that simply asking
lacking that participants in any real psycholog- participants to repeat the retrieval leads to a
ical or physical sense regress to an earlier stage normal increase in recall. The studies that have
of development or that memories retrieved after used a typical “waking hypermnesia design” in
which participants’ recall is brought to plateau,
regression are uniquely or specifically accurate.
before any hypermnesic technique is used, pro-
As early as 1951, Martin Orne alerted the sci-
duced mixed results, favoring mostly an ab-
entific community about the intrusions of con-
sence of a hypermnesic effect or a very small,
fabulations in hypnotically age-regressed indi-
unreliable one (e.g., Dinges et al., 1992; No-
viduals. Confronted with the fact that the grady, McConkey, & Perry, 1985). For exam-
historical veracity of a past life, memories of ple, Dywan and Bowers (1983) determined that
life in the womb, or an early childhood memory a twofold increase in the number of accurate
can rarely be independently corroborated, re- items reported when memories were elicited in
searchers turned to experimental designs to con- hypnosis was more than offset by a threefold
trol for the memory source and better evaluate increase in the number of errors compared with
the potential influences of hypnosis on memory. the error rate in nonhypnotic controls (see also
In a review of laboratory studies and accounts Dinges et al., 1992; Whitehouse, Dinges, Orne,
of forensic cases in which hypnosis was used to & Orne, 1988 for similar results). In their meta-
enhance eyewitness recall, Relinger (1984) con- analysis of 24 forensically relevant studies,
cluded that hypnosis is more likely to facilitate Steblay and Bothwell (1994) concluded that,
the free recall of meaningful material (e.g., live relative to waking conditions, hypnosis pro-
or filmed scenes such as simulated crimes) than duced overall more recall errors, more intru-
material such as word lists or nonsense sylla- sions of uncued errors, and higher levels of
bles. Researchers then sought to evaluate memories for false information relative to non-
Relinger’s conclusion. As Erdelyi (1994) hypnotic methods. In short, research points at
showed convincingly, when studies are divided best to an ephemeral hypermnesic effect: “now
according to type of memory tests (recall vs. you see it, now you don’t.” At worst, we will
recognition) and types of stimuli used (mean- see that hypnotically enhanced recall poses risks
ingless vs. meaningful), it becomes much easier of false memory creation. Moreover, the in-
to evaluate any hypermnesic effect of hypnosis creased recall after a hypermnesia suggestion
on memory. Examining studies that have tar- during hypnosis may reflect nothing more than
geted recognition, the answer is clear: There is normal (nonhypnotic) hypermnesia (Erdelyi,
no hypnotic hypermnesia in recognition, 1994).
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 157

Memory Enhancement or Memory agery, Mazzoni & Memon, 2003). For example,
Creation? Malinoski, Lynn, and Sivec (1998) interviewed
40 hypnotized and 40 nonhypnotized partici-
Contrary to the lay view that memory accu- pants about their earliest memories. The first
rately portrays experienced events, contempo- time participants were asked to report their ear-
rary cognitive scientists agree that memory is a liest memory, only 3% of the nonhypnotized
fallible and pliable construction, strongly influ- participants recalled a memory earlier than 2
enced by current beliefs, feelings, expectations, years, a finding that contrasts with the 23% of
images, and guesses about past events (Conway hypnotized participants who reported a memory
& Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Loftus, 2003; Maz- earlier than age 2 (and 20% earlier than 18
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

zoni & Kirsch, 2002). Most researchers concur months). After the second interview, only 8% of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that even the most ordinary memories are a nonhypnotized participants reported a memory
composite of accurate and inaccurate recollec- earlier than 2 years, whereas 35% of hypnotized
tions. participants reported memories earlier than 18
In this light, it comes as no surprise that months (30% earlier than a year).
suggestive procedures— hypnosis being just Expectancy, criterion shift, and increased
one of them— can distort the content of recol- confidence. More recent work is aimed at un-
lections. The possibility of creating false mem- derstanding whether expectancy and warnings
ories in hypnosis was identified as early as the moderate the effect of hypnosis on memory.
late 19th century. In a now-classic demonstra- Burgess and Kirsch (1999) found that when the
tion, Bernheim (1891/1973) suggested a false suggestion contained warnings about the cre-
event to a patient (i.e., that she had gone to the ation of false memories in hypnosis, fewer in-
bathroom four times, and on her last visit she accurate memories were reported after hypno-
had fallen and hit her nose). The patient remem- sis. Nevertheless, the effect of the warning
bered the suggested event and reported using during hypnosis was minimal. It is important to
the bathroom, explaining that it was because of note that in no instance did hypnotic induction
diarrhea, and she mentioned falling on the produce an increase in memory accuracy com-
fourth visit and hitting her nose. After hypnosis, pared with performance in controls.
Bernheim apparently could not shake her belief In another study, warnings minimized mem-
in the memory. ory distortions during but not after hypnosis
In line with Bernheim’s findings, by the late (Green et al., 1998). However, Bryant and
1970s and early 1980s, a burgeoning number of Barnier (1999) partly confirmed the Burgess
reports of suggestion-induced memory distor- and Kirsch (1999) findings by showing that
tions emerged (Orne, 1979). Laurence and Perry highly hypnotizable participants retracted mem-
(1983) provided one of the most dramatic em- ories retrieved after a suggestion only when
pirical demonstrations of false memory in hyp- they were not hypnotized. Accordingly, the ex-
nosis. In their study, while hypnotized, highly pectancy for memory enhancement in hypnosis
hypnotizable participants were regressed to a is not easy to challenge, and highly hypnotiz-
night of the previous week and received the able individuals often maintain their belief in
suggestion that a loud noise awakened them. If created memories.
they reported having heard the suggested noise, So far, the evidence reviewed implies that
then they were asked to describe it in detail. increases in correct responses are generally off-
Sixty-three percent of the participants accepted set by increases in incorrect responding, leaving
the suggestion and reported hearing the noise. accuracy unchanged or, at times, decreased.
Of these, 76% subsequently developed false Nevertheless, the most frequently reported
memories of the noise. Confronted 1 week later problem with hypnotically assisted recall is that
with the actual recording of the session, all during or after hypnosis, witnesses may errone-
participants maintained their beliefs that the ously report items that they would normally
memory was genuine. reject on the basis of uncertainty (see, e.g.,
Very early memory reports are also vulnera- Krass, Kinoshita, & McConkey, 1989; Scobo-
ble to suggestive influences through hypnosis ria, Mazzoni, Kirsch, & Milling, 2002). The
and various other suggestive procedures (e.g., observed increase in incorrect responses has
dream interpretation, Mazzoni et al., 1999; im- been most often explained by a lowering of
158 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

participants’ criterion shift (Dinges et al., 1992; that jurors would be compelled by confidently
Erdelyi, 1994). However, several of these stud- held testimony. In conclusion, there is little
ies have shown that hypnosis is associated with theoretical or empirical justification for using
enhanced confidence in responses, regardless of hypnosis to enhance memory.
whether they are correct or erroneous, and that
hypnotizability moderates the effect. This in- Hypnosis and Functional Disorders
crease in confidence has been clearly docu- of Memory
mented in a study in which participants received
misleading questions in and out of hypnosis Memory is at the core of our identity, social
(Scoboria, Mazzoni, & Kirsch, 2008). When relationships, and ability to plan for the future
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

asked in hypnosis to rate confidence in the cor- on the basis of past experiences. It is not sur-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

rectness of their memories, the more highly prising that when memory is impaired, it creates
hypnotizable individuals’ confidence ratings difficulties across many areas of human func-
were inflated relative to confidence expressed tioning. Functional amnesia is arguably one of
without hypnosis. the most compelling and controversial forms of
For a memory researcher, the observed in- forgetting, whether seen in a clinical or a foren-
crease in confidence ratings after multitrial re- sic context, because it involves an apparently
calls is somewhat surprising and is at odds with sudden memory loss typically associated with
the usual interpretation of a lowered criterion psychological trauma rather than with brain
shift leading to a hypermnesic effect. As sug- damage or disease (McNally, 2003). Functional
gested by Dywan (1995), one would expect the amnesia involves three major features: (a) a
new items retrieved to be held with less confi- subjectively compelling apparent inability to
dence if they were the result of a lower criterion consciously access autobiographical memories
shift. As Dywan (1995) proposed, hypnosis and information (i.e., disrupted explicit mem-
may create an illusion of remembering that ory); (b) a continuing influence of the “forgot-
leads to a misattributional report. Consistent ten” information on behavior, thought, and ac-
with other studies on the creation of false mem- tion (i.e., a dissociation between implicit and
ories and consistent with the attributional theo- explicit memory); and (3) reversibility of the
ries of memory (Mazzoni & Kirsch, 2002; effect (see Cox & Barnier, 2003). Functional
Whittlesea & Williams, 2001a, 2001b), visual amnesia is a major feature of the poorly under-
imagination and source monitoring might play a stood, and often dramatic, psychological disor-
major role (Mazzoni & Memon, 2003) because ders of dissociative fugue, dissociative amnesia,
visual mental images with specific perceptual- and dissociative identity disorder (Giesbrecht,
like qualities can be mistaken for memories. Lynn, Lilienfeld, & Merckelbach, 2008), and
Visual images convey a sense of recollection has, for the past 3 decades, figured prominently
that mimics that of true memories (Mazzoni & in controversies about repressed and recovered
Memon, 2003). Whether it be through the use of memories (Erdelyi, 2006).
imagery, perceptual fluency, or the feelings of Functional amnesia is particularly interesting
involuntariness of responses that often accom- because it (a) appears to eliminate detailed and
pany hypnosis, the end result may be that the complex autobiographical memories for a sub-
“new” memories are quite similar to the “old” stantial period of time; (b) the forgetting seems
ones, at least experientially (Laurence, Day, & involuntary, unlike a great deal of everyday
Gaston, 1998). The experience of remembering forgetting in which effort is required to suppress
in these cases has changed, and a criterion shift upsetting memories; and (c) it resolves sponta-
might not be necessary to explain memory en- neously and often dramatically. Accordingly,
hancement (Dywan, 1998). This illusion of re- functional amnesia goes to the heart of theoret-
membering may lead a hypnotized person to ical and applied questions about malingering,
believe that his or her recall has improved, memory accessibility, and the relation between
although some or most of what is recalled is cognition and emotion. However, the spontane-
incorrect. Inflated confidence in hypnotically ous, transient, and complex nature of functional
enhanced eyewitness memories is one of the amnesia has limited its systematic investigation,
issues that led the courts to rule that hypnosis is making laboratory analogs of the phenomenon
inadmissible on the basis of the presumption appealing.
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 159

In posthypnotic amnesia (PHA), the hypno- plicit measures for high hypnotizable individ-
tist suggests that after hypnosis a person will be uals, but not for low hypnotizable individuals.
unable to recall material (traditionally stimuli For high hypnotizable individuals, their ex-
learned or events experienced during hypnosis plicit recall returned to the unimpaired base-
but extending also to experiences before hyp- line level of low hypnotizable individuals’
nosis) until they receive a reversibility cue. recall after the reversibility cue. Accordingly,
Typically, for high but not low hypnotizable her research provided a useful method for
individuals, PHA leads to disrupted explicit investigating temporary and reversible forget-
memory, discrepancies between implicit and ting of autobiographical memories.
explicit memory, and reversibility when the As Cox and Bryant (2008) noted, Cox and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

hypnotist provides a cue to the participant, such Barnier’s (2003) research provides a useful
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

as “Now you can remember everything” technique for examining shifts in memory ac-
(Barnier, 2002). Researchers have documented cessibility, allowing comparisons with clinical
PHA for items learned or experienced during disorders of memory, including dissociative
hypnosis, such as word lists (e.g., Kihlstrom, identity disorder and posttraumatic stress disor-
1980), for information learned before hypnosis der, in which intrusive memories of trauma are
(e.g., Barnier, Bryant, & Briscoe, 2001), and often suppressed (Brewin, 2001). During hyp-
even for past personal experiences (e.g., nosis, Cox and Barnier (2003) first invited high
Barnier, 2002). and low hypnotizable participants to recall
The similarities between functional amnesia memories of a first romantic relationship in
and PHA (i.e., impaired explicit memory, a response to 10 cue phrases. They then adminis-
dissociation between explicit and implicit mem- tered a PHA suggestion that targeted either the
ory, and reversibility) have spurred researchers entire period or specific memories from that
to use PHA as a laboratory paradigm to model period. After PHA was administered, and again
and explore disorders of personal memory. For after the PHA suggestion was cancelled, ex-
example, Barnier and her colleagues (Barnier, plicit memory was assessed by cued recall (five
2002; Cox & Barnier, 2003) and other investi- cue phrases that had been presented at elicita-
gators (Mendelsohn, Chalamish, Solomonov- tion/“old cues”) and five “new cues” that had
ich, & Dudai, 2008) have argued that PHA can not been presented previously. It is interesting
be used to model functional amnesia. Barnier to note that PHA had the greatest effect on high
has conducted programmatic research on PHA hypnotizable, but not low hypnotizable, individ-
as a model of functional amnesia. Her research uals’ recall (forgetting of old cues) when the
has focused on personal, emotional, everyday suggestion targeted the entire period rather than
life events in contrast with much previous re- specific events. Moreover, high hypnotizable
search that focused on simple material or less individuals exhibited dissociation between ex-
meaningful experiences such as word lists and plicit and implicit memories, as indexed by a
the events of hypnosis (e.g., Kihlstrom & Ev- social judgment task. The authors argued that
ans, 1976; Spanos, Radtke, & Dubreuil, 1982). the pattern of results was consistent with pre-
For example, Barnier (2002) asked high and dictions derived from Conway and Pleydell-
low hypnotizable participants to recall a mem- Pearce’s (2000) model of autobiographical
ory from their first day at high school and their memory, which holds that autobiographical
first day at university. After a hypnotic induc- memory is organized in terms of three intercon-
tion, she provided half of the participants with a nected levels: lifetime periods measured in
PHA suggestion for their first day at high school months, years, decades; general events (e.g.,
and the remainder of the participants with a days, weeks, months); and event-specific
parallel suggestion to forget their first day at knowledge. A more global suggestion to forget
university. Before she administered the re- a particular period would be expected to be
versibility cue to cancel PHA, she assessed, particularly effective in that it would target all
by way of category generation and social memories in a particular period and tag related
judgment tasks, participants’ implicit memory memories in the period with the goal to not
for the events and probed for their explicit remember. Accordingly, the finding that the
recall of the events a second time. Barnier PHA suggestion that targeted the entire period
found a dissociation between explicit and im- had a greater effect on recall performance than
160 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

the PHA suggestion that targeted specific events Hypnotic suggestions can also be used to
is consistent with Conway and Pleydell- model fundamental alterations in identity as
Pearce’s model. core to one’s personality as identification of the
In a fascinating recent study, Mendelsohn self as a man or woman. Burn, Barnier, and
and his colleagues (Mendelsohn et al., 2008) McConkey (2001) selected very high and
used functional magnetic resonance imaging highly hypnotizable participants and suggested
(fMRI) to investigate the effects of suggestions during hypnosis that they would become more
for PHA on brain activity associated with real- and more like the opposite sex in many ways.
life episodic memories of a viewed film and the Most of the very high hypnotizable participants
context in which the film was viewed. The study and many of the highly hypnotizable partici-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

compared high hypnotizable participants who pants responded positively to this rather surpris-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were able to experience PHA with high hypno- ing suggestion. Participants then heard two sto-
tizable participants who were unable to experi- ries: one about a male individual and one about
ence PHA. A week after participants viewed a a female. Participants remembered more of the
documentary movie depicting a routine day in story consistent with their suggested sex
the life of a young Israeli woman, the research- change. Even confronted with contradictory in-
ers administered a hypnotic induction in the formation, most of the participants maintained
fMRI scanner followed by PHA suggestions to their delusional response. Postexperimental in-
forget the movie details until they received a quiries revealed that these participants modified
reversal cue. Participants were tested while per- their information processing and selectively at-
forming a computerized retrieval test and tended to the story consistent with their sex
scanned twice for their recall of the movie and change, perceiving the story as more self-
the context (e.g., during the movie, the door to referential and personally meaningful.
the room was closed): first, after hypnosis was Cox and Barnier (2013) examined the rela-
tion between a suggested self-identity delusion
terminated and while they experienced the post-
and the availability of autobiographical memo-
hypnotic suggestion; and second, after the can-
ries. The researchers provided participants with
cellation of the suggestion.
the suggestion that they would become more
The researchers found that people in the PHA
and more like one of their siblings (or a new
group (who could experience PHA) forgot nonexisting sibling). Highly hypnotizable par-
many more details from the movie than people ticipants produced more specific memories in
in the non-PHA group (who could not experi- line with their new identity, with memories rich
ence PHA). After the suggestion was cancelled, in sensory-perceptual details that often repre-
recall across groups was equivalent and mem- sented a reinterpretation of previous experi-
ory loss reversed. PHA memory loss was highly ences and served to bolster the reality of the
selective: Memories for the context were pre- suggested delusion. The findings supported
served for people in the PHA group despite Conway’s (1996) self-memory system hypoth-
reports of impaired memories for the movie. It esis, which predicts that shifts of identity should
is important to note that the researchers found lead to a corresponding shift in the availability
that activity in the occipital, temporal, and pre- of autobiographical memories.
frontal areas of the brain differed among the In a series of studies aimed at creating labo-
groups and, in the PHA group, between the ratory analogues of actual delusional beliefs
suggestion and reversal conditions. They argued (e.g., mirror self-misidentification, delusion of
that the brain activation seen in PHA reflects a alien control), Barnier and her colleagues
dampening—some form of rapid, early inhibi- (Barnier et al., 2008; Cox & Barnier, 2010)
tion of memory—that is due to increased activ- have shown convincingly that hypnotically sug-
ity in the prefrontal cortex. Although the find- gested delusions can help us understand the
ings reviewed are based on an analog of social and cognitive factors involved in these
functional memory impairments, they point re- delusions and ultimately provide interesting
searchers in interesting directions and beg the clues as to how they can be eliminated or treat-
question of how findings derived from analog ed. These experiments also underscore the role
studies differ from studies of patients with func- that autobiographical memory plays in the
tional amnesia. maintenance of these disorders.
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 161

Hypnosis can also play an important role in the cLean, & Bertrand, 1987). Whether the dis-
investigation of mood, autobiographical memory, rupted retrieval is (a) the product of response
and self-identity. Some researchers (e.g., MacCa- strategies that participants adopt when in-
llum, McConkey, Bryant, & Barnier, 2000) have structed to forget information they would nor-
shown that specific moods induced during hypno- mally retain in memory (e.g., Spanos, 1986) or
sis affect the type of autobiographical memories (b) the consequence of a breakdown in the sub-
reported in response to positive (happy), negative ject’s executive functions is still open to inves-
(sad), and neutral cues. More specifically, whereas tigation.
low hypnotizables were not affected by the mood Suggested amnesia may be highly selective
inductions, high hypnotizables responded differ- (e.g., for particular semantic categories in a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ently to the different cues in the sad condition. word list or a specific memory) or extensive,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Asked to retrieve specific personal memories for representing a “blanket” amnesia. For example,
each cue provided by the experimenter (e.g., the last item of Form C of the Stanford Hyp-
“smile” or “angry”), high hypnotizables had less notic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS:C; Weitzen-
access to specific positive memories while in a sad hoffer & Hilgard, 1962) is the suggestion is to
mood. This finding is consistent with previous forget everything that happened during the hyp-
research indicating that individuals experience notic session. Amnestic responses can be exam-
more difficulty retrieving positive memories when ined in terms of social demands (extrinsic fac-
depressed (Goddard, Dritschel, & Burton, 1996). tors) and the individuals’ cognitive, affective,
In this case, hypnosis helped researchers under- and neurophysiological makeup.
stand the relation between the valence of the emo- Two major forms of intentional forgetting are
tional state and memory output and indicates that directed forgetting (DF; e.g., Basden, Basden,
hypnotic mood induction can be a useful proce- & Gargano, 1993) and intentional suppression
dure to examine the relation between mood and of memories such as in the Think-NoThink pro-
autobiographical memory. Combined, the studies cedure (Anderson & Green, 2001). Few studies
reviewed imply that hypnotically elicited delu- have compared DF with forgetting elicited by
sions can affect information processing and may hypnotic suggestion. So far, results have been
prove to be highly relevant to understanding the mixed, indicating some common, although not
potential links between delusional beliefs and au- identical, retrieval inhibition process. In the
tobiographical memory as well as the subjective most recent study, David, Brown, Pojoga, and
experiences of individuals who suffer from delu- David (2000) concluded that DF and PHA could
sions. implicate different cognitive mechanisms or
One can extrapolate that hypnosis may in the share some similarities through retrieval inhibi-
future provide a means to restructure personal tion. One of the main difficulties in comparing
identity by reshaping memories. Although in DF and PHA is in equating the instructions to
clinical practice hypnosis has been used from forget. In the list-method DF, participants are
time to time during the past 2 centuries to create told after the presentation of the first list that it
punctual positive personal pseudomemories, lit- was a practice run and they can ignore the list
tle empirical work has been done to date in and that the next list presented is the one to
assessing the use of hypnotic suggestions to remember. On the other hand, hypnotized par-
create a network of pseudomemories to shape a ticipants are told that they will forget the first
positive and functional self-identity. list until the amnesic-release cue is provided.
PHA and intentional forgetting. In addi- From the participants’ perspective, these two
tion to providing a valuable analog of functional sets of instructions may be perceived as dispa-
memory disorders, PHA may facilitate our un- rate memory tasks and, in fact, convey very
derstanding of intentional and more seemingly different task demands.
automatic forgetting. PHA appears to be asso- Accordingly, some studies comparing DF
ciated with a breakdown of normal retrieval with PHA procedures have obtained differences
strategies (Kihlstrom & Evans, 1971). Evidence in performance (Kihlstrom, 1983). Compared
for this breakdown derives from the apparent with DF, PHA produced greater amnesia (fewer
lack of structure (serial recall, recall in clusters) items are recalled), and the reversal cue trig-
in the material that participants recall (Coe, gered the return of most memories (in high
1989; Kihlstrom & Wilson, 1984; Spanos, Ma- hypnotizable individuals). In contrast, the DF
162 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

effect was much weaker for forgetting and for lished studies by the first author indicate that
reaccessing memories when the reversal cue highly hypnotizable individuals are better at
was presented (Basden, Basden, Coe, Decker, & inhibiting target words in this latter task com-
Crutcher, 1994). Until the contexts can be pared with medium and low hypnotizable par-
equated, it will be difficult to investigate the ticipants. The suppression effect was obtained
role that hypnosis, hypnotizability, or amnesia without hypnosis, implying that the hypnotic
play in a DF framework. Nonetheless, these induction is not necessary to produce forgetting
studies reveal the intriguing possibility that at due to inhibition. These studies illustrate well
least in high hypnotizable individuals there may how hypnosis, hypnotizability, and specifically
be a forgetting mechanism that has eluded DF PHA can inform memory research and suggest
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

researchers and is worth pursuing. new areas of investigation that may not be read-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Voluntary suppression, exemplified by the ily amenable to study with traditional methods
Think-NoThink task, represents another inter- of directed forgetting.
esting arena of investigation in which the use In the Mendelson et al. (2008) study referred to
of hypnosis and PHA could further knowl- earlier, the authors contended that the simultane-
edge of the mechanisms involved in volun- ous lifting of the amnesia and the restoration of
tarily attempting to not think about some- brain activity in regions crucial to retrieval
thing. The typical intentional suppression strongly imply that memory suppression occurred
procedure (Anderson & Green, 2001) is a at early stages of the retrieval process. The re-
retrieval-induced forgetting task that induces searchers remarked that the amnesia produced by
a nonintentional form of memory inhibition. the posthypnotic suggestion “affects an executive
In a cued-recall task, participants learn a list preretrieval monitoring process, which produces
of word pairs (cue-target) to a certain (usually an early decision on whether to proceed or not on
high) criterion. A subset of the target words is retrieval, and in case of a [question about the
then tagged for “forgetting” (F), most words
movie], aborts the process” (p. 165). Is this exec-
are tagged for “remember” (R), and still other
utive preretrieval monitoring process at play in
words (baseline targets) are not represented
intentional suppression? Is this process responsi-
during this phase. During the Think-NoThink
ble for the difference in retrieval inhibition found
phase, cues for the R and for the F words are
presented, and participants are asked either to when DF and PHA are compared? It is likely that
remember the targets (R targets) or “not to studies similar to that of Mendelson et al. (2007)
think” about the corresponding targets (F tar- could assist in understanding the cognitive pro-
gets). Instructions for the not-thinking condi- cesses involved in DF, PHA, and intentional inhi-
tion request that participants make their bition.
minds “blank.” This task is reminiscent of Memory accuracy. Although researchers
Wegner’s (1989) ironic effects paradigm. have demonstrated that hypnosis should not be
When individuals are asked to voluntarily not used to facilitate recall, some researchers have
think about something, it often triggers the investigated whether hypnosis could be used to
paradoxical emergence of the “forbidden” improve memory accuracy. Wagstaff, Cole,
word or thought. Bowers and Woody (1996) Wheatcroft, Anderton, and Madden (2008) eval-
examined the potential ironic effects of vol- uated the possibility that suggestions provided in
untarily suppressing a specific thought in high hypnosis might attenuate (rather than increase) the
and low hypnotizable individuals, in and out misinformation effect and false memories. The
of hypnosis, with and without suggested study was based on the rationale that expectancies
PHA. Of all of the different groups tested, and demand characteristics established by sugges-
only high hypnotizables, hypnotized partici- tions are responsible for the increase in false mem-
pants who received a PHA suggestion aimed ories and confidence that is typically observed
at suppressing the thought, showed a signifi- during and after hypnosis. Nevertheless, expectan-
cant decrease in the number of intrusions. cies and demand characteristics can be manipu-
Although no published studies to date have lated with the goal of reducing false memories.
compared performance in PHA with intentional When the researchers informed participants that
suppression, as measured by the Think- they could discriminate correct and incorrect to-
NoThink task, the results of a series of unpub- be-recalled items during hypnosis, the typical mis-
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 163

information and inflated confidence effects were in hypnotic and nonhypnotic contexts and to ex-
not evident. plore the extent to which similar or different rest-
In a similar vein, Oeberst and Blank (2012) ing state conditions characterize retrieval of hyp-
have shown that the misinformation effect can be notic and awake involuntary memories. Recent
completely reversed through what the authors call studies have shown that hypnosis activates ante-
an “enlightenment” procedure, in which the re- rior default mode areas in the brain (e.g., McG-
searchers ask participants to search for two con- eown, Mazzoni, Venneri, & Kirsch, 2009), and
tradictory items in memory, rather than for the the retrieval of involuntary memories seems to be
typically single “correct” answer. This procedure facilitated during mind wandering states in which
apparently enhances source monitoring (Johnson, the default mode is also active (Vannucci et al., in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993) by testing sepa- press; for a review on mind wandering, see
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

rately the contribution to the final memory of what Schooler et al., 2011). The recent development of
is remembered about the items and what is re- new methodologies that elicit involuntary memo-
membered about the source of the item. The Wag- ries in the laboratory (see Schlagman & Kvavilas-
staff et al. (2008) and Oeberst and Blank (2012) hvili, 2008; Vannucci et al., in press; Berntsen et
studies raise the intriguing possibility that hypno- al., 2013) imply that future studies in this area
sis might produce a change in the way participants might provide insight into the nature of involun-
interpret the memory task, enhance comparison of tary memory retrieval.
what is remembered and from what source, and Prospective memory? It is intriguing to
facilitate which mental contents are reported contemplate that posthypnotic suggestion (PHS)
among the various mental contents activated. could be considered a form of prospective mem-
Accordingly, hypnosis holds potential to en- ory (Einstein & McDaniel, 1996), which is the
hance the metacognitive monitoring system, type of memory that makes it possible for us to
which is responsible during retrieval for assess- remember what to do in the future (e.g., ask a
ing the source and adequacy of memories (Ko- question or take a pill at a certain predefined time
riat et al., 2000; Johnson et al., 1993; Mazzoni during the day). People seemingly set up their
& Hanczakowski, 2011). Suggestions might be own cues as reminders for the time and place
phrased to enhance scrutiny of the material re- where a particular action is to occur. PHS is a
trieved and to direct participants’ attention spe- suggestion given during hypnosis to take place
cifically toward memory characteristics (e.g., after hypnosis is terminated. It is conceived as a
sensory details, self-referentials) that research way to remember to do something at a certain
on source monitoring has shown to discriminate time; that is, when a cue is presented. How do
true and false memories, hypothetically engen- people remember to open a window when they do
dering more successful monitoring of accurate not remember the instructions (given in hypnosis)
memories and rejection of false memories. The but do so at the specified time or in the specified
possibility of using hypnosis to discriminate context? How do people set up the cues that will
true and false memories would not only repre- serve as reminders to perform the action? It would
sent a major contribution to memory research, be quite interesting to use PHS as a method to
but it would also represent a welcome develop- study prospective memory. These are questions
ment in the forensic and clinical domains (see, that can animate research for some years to come.
e.g., Wagstaff et al., 2011).
Involuntary autobiographical memories. Concluding Remarks
Involuntary autobiographical memories “pop up”
in everyday life during undemanding monotonous We have proffered but a few ideas regarding
tasks with no intention or directive to retrieve how hypnosis can be used to further our knowl-
them (Berntsen, Staugaard, & Sørensen, 2013; see edge of memory. We foresee a productive in-
Vannucci, Mazzoni, & Batool, in press). One of teraction between hypnosis and memory re-
the markers of these memories is the feeling of search. Hypnosis represents an excellent model
involuntariness, which people also often experi- for understanding social, cognitive, and neuro-
ence when retrieval occurs in hypnosis. A poten- logical processes involved in remembering and
tially productive line of research would be to forgetting personal events. More specifically,
address the frequency and comparability of mem- hypnosis provides a fruitful methodology to
ories that emerge on a seemingly involuntary basis model memory disorders and their relation to
164 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

self-identity, a welcome venue to differentiate Burgess, C. A., & Kirsch, I. (1999). Expectancy
voluntary from involuntary forgetting and a information as a moderator of the effects of hyp-
broad and fertile field in the investigation of nosis on memory. Contemporary Hypnosis, 16,
memories in everyday life. 22–31. doi:10.1002/ch.146
Burn, C., Barnier, A., & McConkey, K. M. (2001).
Information processing during hypnotically sug-
gested sex change. International Journal of Clin-
References ical and Experimental Hypnosis, 49, 231–242. doi:
Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing 10.1080/00207140108410073
unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, Coe, W. C. (1989). Posthypnotic amnesia: Theory
410, 366 –369. doi:10.1038/35066572 and research. In N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Barnier, A. J. (2002). Posthypnotic amnesia for au- (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral per-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tobiographical episodes: A laboratory model of spective (pp. 110 –148). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus
functional amnesia? Psychological Science, 13, Books.
232–237. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00443 Conway, M. A. (1996). Autobiographical memories
Barnier, A. J., Bryant, R. A., & Briscoe, S. (2001). and autobiographical knowledge. In D. C. Rubin
Posthypnotic amnesia for material learned before (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobio-
or during hypnosis: Explicit and implicit memory graphical memory (pp. 67–93). Cambridge, United
effects. International Journal of Clinical and Ex- Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. doi:
perimental Hypnosis, 49, 286 –304. doi:10.1080/ 10.1017/CBO9780511527913.003
00207140108410079 Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000).
Barnier, A. J., Cox, R. E., O’Connor, A., Coltheart, The construction of autobiographical memories in
M., Langdon, R. A., Breen, N., & Turner, M. the self-memory system. Psychological Review,
(2008). Developing hypnotic analogues of clinical 107, 261–288. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.261
delusions: Mirrored-self misidentification. Cogni- Cox, R. E., & Barnier, A. J. (2003). Posthypnotic
tive Neuropsychiatry, 13, 406 – 430. doi:10.1080/ amnesia for a first romantic relationship: Forget-
13546800802355666 ting the entire relationship versus forgetting se-
Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., Coe, W. C., Decker, S., lected events. Memory, 11, 307–318. doi:10.1080/
& Crutcher, K. (1994). Retrieval inhibition in di- 09658210244000108
rected forgetting and posthypnotic amnesia. Interna- Cox, R. E., & Barnier, A. J. (2010). Hypnotic illu-
tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypno- sions and clinical delusions: Hypnosis as a re-
sis, 42, 184 –203. doi:10.1080/00207149408409351 search method. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry (Spe-
Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J. cial Issue on Delusion and Confabulation), 15,
(1993). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicit 202–232. doi:10.1080/13546800903319884
memory tests: A comparison of methods. Journal Cox, R. E., & Barnier, A. J. (2013). Shifting self,
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, shifting memory: Testing the self-memory system
and Cognition, 19, 603– 616. doi:10.1037/0278- model with hypnotic identity delusions. Interna-
7393.19.3.603 tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp-
Bernheim, H. (1891/1973). Hypnotisme, suggestion, nosis, 61, 416 – 462. doi:10.1080/00207144.2013
psychothérapie: Etudes nouvelles (N. Y. Aronson, .810479
Trans.). Paris, France: Octave Douin. Cox, R. E., & Bryant, R. A. (2008). Advances in
Berntsen, D., Staugaard, S. R., & Sørensen, L. M. T. hypnosis research: Methods, designs and contribu-
(2013). Why am I remembering this now? Predicting tions of intrinsic and instrumental hypnosis. In M. R.
the occurrence of involuntary (spontaneous) episodic Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The Oxford handbook
memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: of hypnosis: Theory, research, and practice (pp.
General, 142, 426 – 444. doi:10.1037/a0029128 311–336). New York, NY: Oxford University
Bowers, K. S., & Woody, E. Z. (1996). Hypnotic Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198570097.013
amnesia and the paradox of intentional forgetting. .0012
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 381–390. David, D., Brown, R., Pojoga, C., & David, A.
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.381 (2000). The impact of posthypnotic amnesia and
Brewin, C. R. (2001). Memory processes in post-traumatic directed forgetting on implicit and explicit mem-
stress disorder. International Review of Psychiatry, 13, ory: New insights from a modified process disso-
159–163. doi:10.1080/09540260120074019 ciation procedure. International Journal of Clini-
Bryant, R. A., & Barnier, A. J. (1999). Eliciting auto- cal and Experimental Hypnosis, 48, 267–289. doi:
biographical pseudomemories: The relevance of hyp- 10.1080/00207140008415246
nosis, hypnotizability, and attributions. International Dinges, D. F., Whitehouse, W. G., Orne, E. C.,
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 47, Powell, J. W., Orne, M. T., & Erdelyi, M. H.
267–283. doi:10.1080/00207149908410037 (1992). Evaluating hypnotic memory enhancement
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 165

(hypermnesia and reminiscence) using multi-trial lic: A systematic evaluation. American Journal of
forced recall. Journal of Experimental Psychol- Clinical Hypnosis, 42, 10 –20. doi:10.1080/
ogy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1139 – 00029157.1999.10404241
1147. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.1139 Kihlstrom, J. F. (1980). Posthypnotic amnesia for re-
Dywan, J. (1995). The illusion of familiarity: An alter- cently learned material: Interactions with ‘episodic’
native to the report-criterion account of hypnotic and ‘semantic’ memory. Cognitive Psychology, 12,
recall. International Journal of Clinical and Experi- 227–251. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90010-9
mental Hypnosis, 43, 194 –211. doi:10.1080/ Kihlstrom, J. F. (1983). Instructed forgetting: Hyp-
00207149508409961 notic and nonhypnotic. Journal of Experimental
Dywan, J. (1998). Toward a neurophysiological Psychology: General, 112, 73–79. doi:10.1037/
model of hypnotic memory effects. American 0096-3445.112.1.73
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 40, 217–230. doi: Kihlstrom, J. F., & Evans, F. J. (1971). Posthypnotic
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

10.1080/00029157.1998.10403428 amnesia as disorganized retrieval. Proceedings of


Dywan, J., & Bowers, K. S. (1983). The use of the Annual Convention of the American Psycho-
hypnosis to enhance recall. Science, 222, 184 –185. logical Association, 6, 775–776.
doi:10.1126/science.6623071 Kihlstrom, J. F., & Evans, F. J. (1976). Recovery of
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996). Retrieval memory after posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of
processes in prospective memory: Theoretical ap- Abnormal Psychology, 85, 564 –569. doi:10.1037/
proaches and some new empirical findings. In M. 0021-843X.85.6.564
Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel Kihlstrom, J. F., & Wilson, L. (1984). Temporal
(Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applica- organization of recall during posthypnotic amne-
tions (pp. 115–141). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 200 –
Erdelyi, M. H. (1994). The empty set of hypermnesia. 208. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.93.2.200
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Koriat, A., Goldsmith, M., & Pansky, A. (2000).
Hypnosis, 42, 379–390. doi:10.1080/002071494084 Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. An-
09366
nual Review of Psychology, 51, 481–537. doi:
Erdelyi, M. H. (2006). The return of the repressed.
10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.481
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 535–543. doi:
Krass, J., Kinoshita, S., & McConkey, K. M. (1989).
10.1017/S0140525X06479115
Hypnotic memory and confident reporting. Applied
Giesbrecht, T., Lynn, S., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Merck-
Cognitive Psychology, 3, 35–51. doi:10.1002/acp
elbach, H. (2008). Cognitive processes in dissoci-
.2350030105
ation: An analysis of core theoretical assumptions.
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 617– 647. doi: Labelle, L., Laurence, J., Nadon, R., & Perry, C. (1990).
10.1037/0033-2909.134.5.617 Hypnotizability, preference for an imagic cognitive
Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., & Burton, A. (1996). Role style, and memory creation in hypnosis. Journal of
of autobiographical memory in social problem Abnormal Psychology, 99, 222–228. doi:10.1037/
solving and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psy- 0021-843X.99.3.222
chology, 105, 609 – 616. doi:10.1037/0021-843X Laurence, J. R., Day, D., & Gaston, L. (1998). From
.105.4.609 memories of abuse to the abuse of memories. In
Green, J. P., Lynn, S., & Malinoski, P. (1998). Hypnotic S. J. Lynn & K. M. McConkey (Eds.), Truth in
pseudomemories, prehypnotic warnings, and malle- memory, New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 323–
ability of suggested memories. Applied Cognitive 346.
Psychology, 12, 431– 444. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099- Laurence, J. R., & Freedman, S. (2009). Research
0720(199810)12:5⬍431::AID-ACP516⬎3.0.CO; brief: Number of clients at risk for developing
2-R false memories of abuse: Addendum to Legault
Green, J. P., Page, R. A., Rasekhy, R., Johnson, and Laurence (2007). Crime Scene, 16, 15–16.
L. K., & Bernhardt, S. E. (2006). Cultural views Laurence, J. R., & Perry, C. (1983). Hypnotically
and attitudes about hypnosis: A survey of college created memory among highly hypnotizable sub-
students across four countries. International Jour- jects. Science, 222, 523–524. doi:10.1126/science
nal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 54, .6623094
263–280. doi:10.1080/00207140600689439 Laurence, J. R., & Perry, C. (1988). Hypnosis, will,
Janet, P. (1889/1973). L’automatisme psychologique. and memory: A psycho-legal history. New York,
Paris, France: Société Pierre Janet. NY: Guilford Press.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. Legault, E., & Laurence, J. R. (2007). Recovered
(1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulle- memories of childhood sexual abuse: Social
tin, 114, 3–28. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3 worker, psychologist, and psychiatrist reports of
Johnson, M. E., & Hauck, C. (1999). Beliefs and beliefs, practices, and cases. Australian Journal of
opinions about hypnosis held by the general pub- Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 35, 111–133.
166 MAZZONI, LAURENCE, AND HEAP

Loftus, E. F. (2003). Make-believe memories. Amer- Cognition: An International Journal 18, 848 – 855.
ican Psychologist, 58, 867– 873. doi:10.1037/ doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.001
0003-066X.58.11.867 McNally, R. J. (2003). Remembering trauma. Cam-
MacCallum, F., McConkey, K. M., Bryant, R. A., & bridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University
Barnier, A. J. (2000). Specific autobiographical Press.
memory following hypnotically induced mood Mendelsohn, A., Chalamish, Y., Solomonovich, A.,
state. International Journal of Clinical and Exper- & Dudai, Y. (2008). Mesmerizing memories:
imental Hypnosis, 48, 361–373. doi:10.1080/ Brain substrates of episodic memory suppression
00207140008410366 in posthypnotic amnesia. Neuron, 57, 159 –170.
Malinoski, P., Lynn, S. J., & Sivec, H. (1998). The doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.022
assessment, validity, and determinants of early Nash, M. (1987). What, if anything, is regressed
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

memory reports: A critical review. In S. J. Lynn & about hypnotic age regression? A review of the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

K. M. McConkey (Eds.), Truth in memory (pp. empirical literature. Psychological Bulletin, 102,
109 –136). New York, NY: Guilford Press. 42–52. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.42
Mazzoni, G., Clark, A., & Nash, R. A. (2014). Dis- Nogrady, H., McConkey, K. M., & Perry, C. (1985).
owned recollections: Denying true experiences un- Enhancing visual memory: Trying hypnosis, trying
dermines belief in occurrence but not judgments of imagination, and trying again. Journal of Abnor-
remembering. Acta Psychologica, 145, 139 –146. mal Psychology, 94, 195–204. doi:10.1037/0021-
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.007 843X.94.2.195
Mazzoni, G., & Hanczakowski, M. Metacognitive Oeberst, A., & Blank, H. (2012). Undoing suggestive
processes before and during retrieval. In P. A. influence on memory: The reversibility of the eye-
Higham & J. P. Leboe (Eds.), Construction of witness misinformation effect. Cognition, 125,
remembering and metacognition. Essays in honor 141–159. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.009
of Bruce Whittlesea (pp. 91–108). Basingstoke, Orne, M. T. (1951). The mechanisms of hypnotic age
United Kingdom: Palgrave Macmillan. regression: An experimental study. The Journal of
Mazzoni, G. A. L., & Kirsch, I. (2002). Autobio- Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 213–225.
graphical memories and beliefs: A preliminary doi:10.1037/h0059971
metacognitive model. In T. J. Perfect & B. L. Orne, M. T. (1979). The use and misuse of hypnosis
Schwartz (Eds.), Applied metacognition (pp. 121– in court. International Journal of Clinical and
145). New York, NY: Cambridge University Experimental Hypnosis, 27, 311–341. doi:10.1080/
Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489976.007 00207147908407571
Mazzoni, G. A., Loftus, E. F., Seitz, A., & Lynn, S. J. Ost, J., Wright, D. B., Easton, S., Hope, L., & French,
(1999). Changing beliefs and memories through C. C. (2013). Recovered memories, satanic abuse,
dream interpretation. Applied Cognitive Psychol- dissociative identity disorder and false memories
ogy, 13, 125–144. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099- in the UK: A survey of clinical psychologists and
0720(199904)13:2⬍125::AID-ACP560⬎3.0.CO; hypnotherapists. Psychology, Crime & Law, 19,
2-5 1–19. doi:10.1080/1068316X.2011.598157
Mazzoni, G., & Lynn, S. J. (2007). Using hypnosis in Poole, D. A., Lindsay, D. S., Memon, A., & Bull, R.
eyewitness memory: Past and current issues. In (1995). Psychotherapy and the recovery of mem-
M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross, & R. C. L. ories of childhood sexual abuse: U.S. and British
Lindsay (Eds.), The handbook of eyewitness psy- practitioners’ opinions, practices, and experiences.
chology: Vol. I. Memory for events (pp. 321–338). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 63, 426 – 437. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.63.3.426
Mazzoni, G., & Memon, A. (2003). Imagination can Reiff, R., & Scheerer, M. (1960). Memory and hyp-
create false memories. Psychological Science, 14, notic age regression. Oxford, United Kingdom:
186 –188. doi:10.1046/j.1432-1327.2000.01821.x International University Press.
Mazzoni, G., & Scoboria, A. (2007). False memories. Relinger, H. (1984). Hypnotic hypermnesia: A criti-
In F. T. Durso, R. S. Nickerson, S. T. Dumais, S. cal review. American Journal of Clinical Hypno-
Lewandowsky, & T. Perfect (Eds.), Handbook of sis, 26, 212–225. doi:10.1080/00029157.1984
applied cognition (2nd ed., pp. 787– 812). Hobo- .10404165
ken, NJ: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9780470713181.ch30 Ribot, T. A. (1882). Diseases of memory: Essays in
Mazzoni, G., Scoboria, A., & Harvey, L. (2010). the positive psychology. London, United Kingdom:
Nonbelieved memories. Psychological Science, Kegan Paul, Trench & Company. doi:10.1037/
21, 1334 –1340. doi:10.1177/0956797610379865 12818-000
McGeown, W. J., Mazzoni, G., Venneri, A., & Salzberg, H. C., & Depiano, F. A. (1980). Hypnotiz-
Kirsch, I. (2009). Hypnotic induction decreases ability and task motivating suggestions: A further
anterior default mode activity. Consciousness and look at how they affect performance. International
HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY 167

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, Vannucci, M., Mazzoni, G., & Batool, I. (in press).
28, 261–271. doi:10.1080/00207148008409851 Modifying the frequency and characteristics of in-
Schlagman, S., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2008). Invol- voluntary memories. PLoS One.
untary autobiographical memories in and outside Wagstaff, G. F., Cole, J., Wheatcroft, J., Anderton,
the laboratory: How different are they from vol- A., & Madden, H. (2008). Reducing and reversing
untary autobiographical memories? Memory & pseudomemories with hypnosis. Contemporary
Cognition, 36, 920 –932. doi:10.3758/MC.36.5 Hypnosis, 25, 178 –191. doi:10.1002/ch.366
.920 Wagstaff, G. F., Wheatcroft, J. M., Burt, C. L., Pilking-
Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, ton, H. J., Wilkinson, K., & Hoyle, J. D. (2011).
T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Enhancing witness memory with focused meditation
Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the and eye-closure: Assessing the effects of misinfor-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Science, 15, mation. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

319 –326. 26, 152–161. doi:10.1007/s11896-010-9082-7


Scoboria, A., Mazzoni, G., & Kirsch, I. (2008). Wegner, D. M. (1989). White bears and other un-
“Don’t know” responding to answerable and un- wanted thoughts. New York, NY: Viking.
answerable questions during misleading and hyp- Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Stan-
notic interviews. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C. Palo
ogy: Applied, 14, 255–265. doi:10.1037/1076- Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
898X.14.3.255 Whitehouse, W. G., Dinges, D. F., Orne, E. C., &
Scoboria, A., Mazzoni, G., Kirsch, I., & Milling, Orne, M. T. (1988). Hypnotic hypermnesia: En-
L. S. (2002). Immediate and persisting effects of hanced memory accessibility or report bias? Jour-
misleading questions and hypnosis on memory nal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 289 –295. doi:
reports. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.3.289
Applied, 8, 26 –32. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8 Whitehouse, W., Orne, E. C., Orne, M. T., & Dinges,
.1.26 D. F. (1991). Distinguishing the source of memo-
Spanos, N. P. (1986). Hypnotic behavior: A social- ries reported prior waking and hypnotic recall at-
psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, tempts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 51–59.
and ‘trance logic.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences, doi:10.1002/acp.2350050105
9, 449 – 467. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00046537 Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001a).
Spanos, N. P., MacLean, J. M., & Bertrand, L. D. The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: I. The
(1987). Serial organisation during hypnotic amne- heuristic basis of feelings and familiarity. Jour-
sia under two conditions of item presentation. nal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 361–374. Memory, and Cognition, 27, 3–13. doi:10.1037/
doi:10.1016/0092-6566(87)90017-1 0278-7393.27.1.3
Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H., & Dubreuil, D. L. (1982). Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001b). The
Episodic and semantic memory in posthypnotic discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: II. Expecta-
amnesia: A reevaluation. Journal of Personality tion, uncertainty, surprise, and feelings of famil-
and Social Psychology, 43, 565–573. doi:10.1037/ iarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
0022-3514.43.3.565 Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 14 –33. doi:
Stager, G. L., & Lundy, R. M. (1985). Hypnosis and 10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.14
the learning and recall of visually presented mate- Yapko, M. D. (1994). Suggestions of abuse: True and
rial. International Journal of Clinical and Exper- false memories of childhood sexual trauma. New
imental Hypnosis, 33, 27–39. doi:10.1080/ York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
00207148508406633 Zelig, M., & Beidleman, W. B. (1981). The investiga-
Steblay, N. M., & Bothwell, R. K. (1994). Evidence tive use of hypnosis: A word of caution. Interna-
for hypnotically refreshed testimony: The view tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypno-
from the laboratory. Law and Human Behavior, sis, 29, 401– 412. doi:10.1080/00207148108409173
18, 635– 651. doi:10.1007/BF01499329
True, R. M. (1949). Experimental control in hypnotic Received April 1, 2014
age regression states. Science, 110, 583–584. doi: Revision received April 7, 2014
10.1126/science.110.2866.583 Accepted April 8, 2014 䡲

You might also like