Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/275032556

Destination Aesthetics and Aesthetic Distance in Tourism Experience

Article  in  Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing · April 2015


DOI: 10.1080/10548408.2014.958608

CITATIONS READS
50 1,439

2 authors:

Ksenia Kirillova Xinran Y. Lehto


Institut Paul Bocuse Purdue University
56 PUBLICATIONS   1,293 CITATIONS    141 PUBLICATIONS   7,129 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Airbnb View project

Hotel-Customer Communications View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ksenia Kirillova on 16 April 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article was downloaded by: [Purdue University]
On: 16 April 2015, At: 08:21
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wttm20

Destination Aesthetics and Aesthetic Distance in


Tourism Experience
Ksenia Kirillova & Xinran Lehto
Published online: 14 Apr 2015.

Click for updates

To cite this article: Ksenia Kirillova & Xinran Lehto (2015): Destination Aesthetics and Aesthetic Distance in Tourism
Experience, Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, DOI: 10.1080/10548408.2014.958608

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10548408.2014.958608

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 2015
© 2015 Taylor & Francis
ISSN: 1054-8408 print / 1540-7306 online
DOI: 10.1080/10548408.2014.958608

DESTINATION AESTHETICS AND AESTHETIC


DISTANCE IN TOURISM EXPERIENCE
Ksenia Kirillova
Xinran Lehto

ABSTRACT. Given the paucity of scholarly attention to the aesthetic component of tourism
experience, this research attempted to empirically explore the roles of destination aesthetic qualities
and “aesthetic distance”, that is, the perceived difference between the aesthetic properties of a
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

destination and those of a tourist’s home environment, in the tourist’s aesthetic judgment and vacation
satisfaction. The results validated a six-factor structure of aesthetic qualities, namely locale character-
istics, scope, upkeep, accord, perceived age, and shape. This research noted that when tourists evaluate
their home environment more positively in terms of upkeep and scope than vacation environment, they
tend to perceive a destination as less beautiful. Only aesthetic distance in scope of experiential features
influences vacation satisfaction. Relevant practical implications are discussed.

KEYWORDS. Tourism aesthetics, aesthetic judgment, satisfaction, experience-based products

1. INTRODUCTION satisfaction. Indeed, among many properties of


tourism destinations, aesthetic qualities represent
The following conversation was overheard that an important component of tourism experience yet
occurred amid scenic Colorado Rocky Mountains it is one of its least studied dimensions (Kirillova,
during a hike. A group of tourists was sharing Fu, Lehto, & Cai, 2014). Prompting tourists to
positive sentiments about the ongoing trip with judge a destination as beautiful or ugly, tourism
one another when a young man admitted that aesthetics could significantly influence tourism
nothing about this outing greatly impressed him. experience, linking it to overall satisfaction
He continued to elaborate that, although landscape (Pizam, Neumann, & Reichel, 1978; Todd,
was quite picturesque, the scenery that he had seen 2009), loyalty (Lee, Jeon, & Kim, 2011), and
thus far was not remarkable enough. Additionally, intention to return (Baloglu, Pekcan, Chen, &
the trails were too crowded, cabins were too rustic, Santos, 2004). As illustrated in the example, in
and, finally, he had seen better views around his addition to aesthetic qualities, the perceived dis-
hometown. Although further details of this tour- tance between the aesthetic properties of a tourist’s
ist’s vacation experience are not known, it is also home and those of a destination environment
possible that the subpar impression of the destina- (hereby referred to as “aesthetic distance”) could
tion aesthetics adversely affected his trip be crucial in tourism experience. For instance,

Ksenia Kirillova is a PhD Candidate, School of Hospitality and Tourism Management, Purdue University, 900
West State St., Marriott Hall, Room 206F, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA (E‑mail:kkirillo@purdue.edu, ph).
Xinran Lehto is a Professor, School of Hospitality and Tourism Management, Purdue University, 900 West
State St., Marriott Hall, Room 257, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA (E‑mail:xinran@purdue.edu).
Address correspondence to: Ksenia Kirillova, School of Hospitality and Tourism Management,
Purdue University, 900 West State St., Marriott Hall, Room 206F, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA
(E‑mail:kkirillo@purdue.edu, ph).
1
2 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

those living in coastal areas could appraise moun- consumer behavior while contributing to con-
tainous destinations more favorably than beach sumptive experience (Bitner, 1992; Hosany &
vacations, simply because they tend to find a Gilbert, 2010; Liu & Jang, 2009; Ryu & Jang,
novel and diversified scenery more beautiful 2007). This view emphasizes the point that pro-
(Kirillova et al., 2014). This sense of novelty, ducts are experienced differently in different
however, could also hinder the vacation experi- contexts. In a restaurant setting, for instance,
ence, increase anxiety, and ultimately hamper consuming food in a fast-food establishment is
vacation satisfaction (Reisinger & Turner, 1998). experientially different from enjoying a meal in
Tourism research has acknowledged the value a fast-casual restaurant. King, Weber,
of aesthetics in tourism experience (Alegre & Meiselman, and Lv (2004, 2007) determined
Garau, 2010; O’Leary & Deegan, 2003; Pizam that varying consumption environments had an
et al., 1978). However, although the multidimen- effect on food evaluation; moreover, adding
sional conceptualization of tourism aesthetics has contextual variables increased acceptance scores
been recently proposed (Kirillova et al., 2014), for side products such as salads and teas. In
understanding of the beauty in tourism is still another study it was shown that fast music
mostly one-dimensional and perfunctory. The promotes fast eating in restaurants and lowers
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

major complication arises from the lack of valid bar purchases while slow classical music
instruments to assess perceived aesthetic qualities increased drinks orders (Milliman, 1986;
of a destination, since previous studies on this North, Shilcock, & Hargreaves, 2003).
subject focused on theory building rather than To explain the mechanism of the environ-
operationalization or empirical validations (e.g. mental influence on consumers, researchers
Kirillova et al., 2014). There is also a paucity of widely adopt Mehrabian and Russell’s (1974)
attention to the role of contrasting environments environmental stimuli, organism or emotional
in tourism experience. The cultural distance states, and responses (SOR). The model implies
research, another area addressing the concept of that a stimulus originated from the environment
distance, recognizes the role of home environ- evokes an emotional response in an individual,
ment in tourism experience; however, it ignores which is then followed by a cognitive or beha-
the aesthetic component (Crotts, 2004). vioral response. To illustrate, Brunner-Sperdin,
Against this background, the current study is Peters, and Strobl (2012) demonstrated that the
a pioneering attempt to empirically examine the ability of the environment to deliver pleasure
role of destination aesthetic qualities as well as and a sense of immersion as well as tangible,
to elaborate on the function of perceived dis- intangible, and social environmental attributes
tance between aesthetic qualities of home and significantly influences consumers’ emotional
destination environments in tourism experience. responses and thus satisfaction with the service
Specifically, we aim to understand how aes- experience. In another instance, Jang and
thetic qualities and aesthetic distance contribute Namkung (2009) found that positive emotions
to tourist aesthetic judgment and vacation satis- mediated the relationship between restaurant
faction. This knowledge will allow tourism atmospherics and behavioral intentions.
practitioners to develop effective tools to man- Specifically to the tourism context, Hosany
age tourism experience through influencing and Gilbert (2010) reported that destination
tourist aesthetic judgment and satisfaction. environments capable of evoking the emotions
of joy, love, and positive surprise contribute to
tourist satisfaction.
2. LITERATURE REVIEW In an emerging stream of research, Lehto
(2013) investigated the role of environment in
2.1 The Role of the Environment in tourists’ perception of vacation as restorative.
Consumptive Experience Conceptually based on Kaplan’s (1995) atten-
tion restoration theory, the study found that
Environment as consisting of tangible and destination environments that fascinate, create
intangible elements is known to influence a sense of mental and physical “away-ness”,
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 3

possess enough context variability yet a mini- to the object of appreciation, which evokes a
mum of confusing and chaotic elements, and loss of self-consciousness and a sense of time.
that are compatible with a tourist’s self-concept Much discussion of aesthetic appreciation
tend to be perceived as restorative and contri- has centered on an object, usually a piece of
bute to vacation satisfaction. In summary, art, with an appreciator acting as an outside
although not directly addressed in the above observer. For instance, it is accepted that
studies, the integral component of a surrounding round and symmetric objects are typically
environment – its aesthetics – could have the judged as more beautiful than angular and
capacity to induce positive emotions and thus asymmetric objects (Jacobsen, Schubotz,
serve as a source of enjoyment during travel. In Höfel, & Cramon, 2006; Silvia & Barona,
this way, tourists’ assessment of a destination 2009). A modern branch of philosophy –
environment as beautiful or ugly becomes an environmental aesthetics – focuses on one’s
important aspect of tourism experience (Todd, physical surroundings. These philosophers
2009). postulate that in order to appreciate an environ-
ment, an individual does not only observe but
he/she is also fully immersed in the object of
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

2.2 Aesthetic Judgment appreciation (Hepburn, 1966). Unlike art appre-


ciation, which is contemplative, environmental
Philosophers through the ages pondered the aesthetic experience is built on active and parti-
question of the nature of aesthetic judgment, or cipatory modes of appreciation. Because such
what it is we find attractive. Earlier thinkers an aesthetic process implies stimulation of all
such as Plato, Aristotle, Saint Thomas five human senses, it is experiential in nature
Aquinas, Descartes, and others theorized about (Berleant, 2005). Additionally, Ittelson (1978)
the objective nature of aesthetic judgment. argued that the environment–appreciator rela-
Although their reasoning differed, they agreed tionship is multidimensional as environment
that an object possesses a set of features that could be simultaneously understood as an exter-
prompt any individual to judge it as beautiful or nal object, as representation of self, and as an
ugly (Beardsley, 1975). In contrast, the eight- arena for action. Tourism aesthetics is unique in
eenth–nineteenth century philosophers such as this sense. Because tourism entails traveling a
Kant, Hegel, Hume, and more contemporary certain distance to a vacation destination, the
thinkers argued that because individuals have appreciated environment is often new or less
diverse backgrounds, they process information familiar to tourists than the usual home environ-
about an object’s features distinctly and there- ment. Enhanced by the impression of novelty,
fore their resulting aesthetic judgments may the modes of aesthetic appreciation under these
vary (Beardsley, 1975). These thinkers attested circumstances are potentially more complex.
to the subjective perspective of aesthetic judg-
ment. Modern philosophers consider aesthetic
experience as a process. For instance, Leder, 2.3 Dimensions of Aesthetic Judgment of
Belke, Oeberst, and Augustin (2004) proposed Environments
a theoretical model for aesthetic experience
where aesthetic judgment is viewed as an out- Environmental psychologists have empiri-
come of a cognitive process while aesthetic cally studied how individuals perceive land-
emotion is a result of affective evaluation. scapes. A positive aesthetic judgment in their
These researchers stress that aesthetic judgment research is often equated to aesthetic or envir-
can be assessed by the means of social science onmental preference because the latter is a
but aesthetic emotions can only be measured by strong predictor of the former (Han, 2007).
neuropsychological tools. Markovic (2012) For instance, natural environments are consis-
compares aesthetic experience to the state of tently preferred to urban environments and
“flow” (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975) in a sense judged as more beautiful (Hartig, 1993).
that in that mental state individuals are drawn Because the subjective nature of aesthetic
4 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

judgment implies that aesthetic judgments of an TABLE 1. Dimensions of Tourist Aesthetic


environment could vary among individuals, Judgment
scholars have been particularly interested in
how people realize their judgments, or against Dimensions Dimensions
which dimensions individuals judge a landscape
Scale Diversity
as beautiful or ugly. It was found, for instance, Bright–Dull Diverse–Alike
that physical attributes such as relief lacked Grand–Quaint Novelty
predictive properties for aesthetic judgment Presence of People–Absence Novel–Typical
while perception-based variables such as open- of People
Abundance–Scarcity Shape
ness, smoothness (Kaplan, Kaplan, & Brown, Openness–Narrowness Sophisticated–Simplistic
1989; Rogge, Nevens, & Gulinck, 2007), coher- Time Round–Angular
ence, complexity, and mystery (Van den Berg, Modern–Historic Symmetric–Asymmetric
Vlek, & Coeterier, 1998) were the most power- Young–Old Uniqueness
Condition Unique–Ordinary
ful in determining the preference. Additionally, Clean–Dirty
Real, Arce, and Sabucedo (2000) discovered Well-kept–Run-down
that artificiality, presence of water, roughness Sound
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

of landscape, and human presence were asso- Lively-Peaceful


Human-made-Nature-made
ciated with high beauty scores. Loud–Quiet
While the majority of studies in environment Balance
appreciation considered natural settings, some Human touch–No human touch
researchers focused on aesthetic appreciation Authentic–Non-authentic
Organic–“Eye sore”
of urban environments. Nasar (1984), for
instance, contrasted aesthetic appraisals of Adopted from Kirillova et al. (2014)
urban landscapes among experts, Japanese,
and American students, and found that all
three groups utilize the ratings of order and research in environmental psychology, this qua-
diversity in the environmental appraisal process. litative study did not intend to uncover which
Interestingly, students rated scenes from another landscapes are preferred; rather, it aimed to
country as more beautiful than scenes from their identify the dimensions utilized in aesthetic
own country, also attesting to the dimension of judgments of any landscape. Many themes in
novelty. A variety of other predictors were also this study are supported by the literature outside
empirically shown to influence environmental the tourism domain (e.g. diversity, novelty,
preference in the urban setting: naturalness, shape) while other themes such as uniqueness
maintenance (Galindo & Rodriguez, 2000), and time seem to be exclusive to tourism aes-
vegetation, visual richness, congruence, lumin- thetics. Although this theoretical framework
osity, historicity (Galindo & Hidalgo, 2005), provides a valuable insight into tourists’ “lived
stylistic uniformity, homogeneity of scale, and experiences” of aesthetic qualities of tourism
symmetry (Weber, Schnier, & Jacobsen, 2008). destinations, it lacks empirical validation.
Despite plentiful research on general envir-
onmental preference, tourism aesthetics remains
largely unexplored. Focusing on a specific set- 2.4 Aesthetics and Tourist Satisfaction
ting, Fyhri, Jacobsen, and Tommervik (2009)
discovered that the type of vegetation, degree As a component of tourism experience,
of human influence, and prototypicality affected aesthetics is viewed as an integral element of
tourists’ perceptions of the Norwegian coast as vacation satisfaction (see for example Alegre &
beautiful. In the tourism domain specifically, Garau, 2010; O’Leary & Deegan, 2003; Pizam
Kirillova et al. (2014) conceptualized nine et al., 1978). For example, Meng, Tepanon, and
themes and 21 dimensions against which tour- Uysal (2008) found that natural scenery was the
ists realize aesthetic judgments in both nature- third most important attribute in destination
based and urban settings (Table 1). Unlike attribute importance, and beautiful scenery was
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 5

the fourth critical factor in tourist attribute satis- optimal balance of familiarity–strangeness, sti-
faction. Validating Oliver’s (1989) expectancy– mulation–tranquility, and structure–indepen-
disconfirmation theory in the tourism context, dence (Yiannakis & Gibson, 1992). Lee and
Weber (1997) found that “seeing spectacular Crompton (1992) also showed that the novelty
landscapes” and “watching unique fauna” were motive is influential in the vacation decision-
among the highest in terms of expectation, making process.
demonstrating the importance of these attributes From this perspective, it is possible that the
to overall satisfaction with tourism experience. perception of the distance between aesthetic
Despite the fact that numerous studies have qualities of one’s home environment and of
recognized the importance of aesthetic qualities vacation environment (aesthetic distance) posi-
of a destination in tourism experience, these tively contributes to tourism experience.
qualities have been mostly reduced to a single Similarity-attraction hypothesis (Byrne &
dimensional variable such as “beautiful scen- Nelson, 1965), on the other hand, predicts that
ery” in destination attribute satisfaction scales. familiarity rather than novelty favorably affects
However, considering that beauty is an impor- tourism experience ratings. Reisinger and
tant criterion in environmental preference, it is Turner (1998) showed that cultural differences
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

worthwhile to zoom in on this aesthetic compo- result in stress and anxiety, eventually leading
nent and to estimate its contribution to tourism to dissatisfaction. Likewise, Ng, Lee, and
experience. Soutar (2007) determined that cultural distance
was negatively correlated with intentions of
Australian residents to visit foreign destinations.
2.5 Novelty in Tourism Experience Based on these findings, perceived aesthetic
distance could hinder rather than enhance tour-
Novelty as well as familiarity has been asso- ism experience. Thus, tourism research on the
ciated with product preferences in consumer effect of contrasting environments on tourist
behavior research where the interplay of both satisfaction is inconclusive and requires further
is known to predict a product choice (Hekkert, exploration.
Snelders, & van Wieringen, 2003) and the for-
mation of consumer loyalty (Toyama &
Yamada, 2012). Unlike a conventional consu- 3. OBJECTIVES
mer choice scenario, in tourism, the notion of
novelty can be linked to exploratory behavior Tourism experience entails a full immersion
(Berlyne, 1966) and thus the desire to travel of an individual into the object of aesthetic
(Lee & Crompton, 1992). Based on Mehrabian appreciation, which is often a novel environ-
and Russell’s (1973) idea that people exhibit ment. Thus, the dimensions of tourists’ aesthetic
arousing-seeking tendencies, Wahlers and Etzel judgment at a destination could be distinct from
(1985) showed that dispositional optimal level that of an object or their routine home environ-
of stimulation contributes to preference for tour- ment. As the literature review demonstrated,
ism experience and environments. For example, aesthetic qualities of vacation destinations are
stimulation seekers prefer vacations that are an important source of satisfaction. Judgment of
different, unique, and unusual while stimulation these qualities as beautiful, conceptualized in
avoiders desire familiar environments and this research as aesthetic judgment, could con-
planned experiences. A related idea is found in tribute to overall satisfaction with vacation
Mannell and Iso-Ahola’s (1987) theory of tour- experience and thus destination loyalty and
ism experience that considers the interplay of future return intentions. In this case, destination
the motives to escape from routine environ- aesthetic qualities as perceived by tourists
ments and to seek recreational opportunities as become of crucial importance in destination
a foundation of tourist motivation. It was further marketing and management. Previous studies
found that tourists tend to enact their preferred also showed that not only destination attributes
roles when a tourism environment provides an themselves influence tourists’ perceptions, the
6 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

perceived distance between home and tourism With appropriate adjustments in wording
environment characteristics could be a catalyst (“vacation destination” was replaced with “the
or deterrent of tourism. Hence, this study had town I live in”), the measure from the second
the following objectives: (1) to explore the section was utilized for home environment eva-
primary dimensions of perceived destination luation, too. The fourth survey section consisted
aesthetic qualities (PDAQ); (2) to understand of the questions addressing aesthetic judgment
how these dimensions contribute to tourist aes- (“overall, I think the vacation destination was
thetic judgment and vacation satisfaction; and beautiful”) and vacation satisfaction (“overall, I
(3) to investigate how aesthetic distance can had a great time at the destination I visited”).
impact tourist aesthetic judgment and ultimately All the questions asked the respondents to
vacation satisfaction. indicate their level of agreeableness on a
7-point Likert scale. The survey concluded
with questions regarding respondents’ socio-
demographics.
4. METHOD
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

4.1 Survey Instrument


The study utilized a survey as a research 4.2 Sampling and Data Collection
instrument. The questionnaire was concerned
with respondents’ most recent vacation experi- Powered by Qualtrics®, an on-line survey was
ence and consisted of five parts. The first section distributed to Amazon Mechanical Turk
contained trip-related questions that asked for (MTurks) members. Amazon MTurks is the web-
information regarding the most recent vacation site that recruits human intelligence workers to
experience such as trip duration, party size, complete various tasks such as sorting and tran-
repeat visit, and a vacation setting (nature- scribing data, and answering surveys. This online
based versus urban). To operationalize destina- organization includes a large participant pool,
tion aesthetic qualities, we adopted the dimen- integrated participant recruitment, and compen-
sionality conceptualization of Kirillova et al. sation systems. It was shown that MTurk parti-
(2014) with a collection of 21 items comprising cipants are demographically more diverse than
nine dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment. conventional Internet samples, obtained data
This conceptualization was not empirically vali- quality is not affected by realistic compensation
dated prior to this research; thus, the starting rates, and data are generally as reliable as infor-
point of our analysis was instrument develop- mation collected via traditional methods
ment. The first version of the measure contained (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). In this
Kirillova et al.’s (2014) 21 items on a 7-point study, each participant was compensated US$
semantic differential scale. Two researchers, 0.70 for a completed survey. An attention filter
both with domain expertise in tourism manage- question was designed to ensure data quality. The
ment and many years of professional experience data were collected in a two-stage process. First,
in the tourism industry, were consulted to ensure a pilot study with 100 Amazon MTurks partici-
the content validity of the instrument, and the pants was conducted to further verify face valid-
anchoring words of some dimensions were ity of the PDAQ measure and explore emerging
reworded as a result. For example, “round–angu- dimensionality. A full-scale survey was con-
lar” was changed to “smooth–rugged” . Then, a ducted in July 2013. The study sample consisted
focus group with 26 graduate students enrolled of individuals aged 18 years and older who had
in the research methods class was conducted to returned from a vacation trip within a year. Of
verify face validity of the measure, after which 500 completed responses of the full-scale survey,
the wording was further refined. 59 questionnaires were excluded from further
The questions in the next section assessed the analysis because of substantial missing data,
perceived aesthetic qualities of a town from leaving 441 completed responses for statistical
which respondents departed to go on vacation. treatment.
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 7

4.3 Data Analysis factor, a dummy variable for setting as predic-


tors and aesthetic judgment and satisfaction as
To statistically validate the measure of dependent variables. A summated mean proce-
PDAQ, this research utilized a split-sample dure was used for factor score calculation.
approach. Using a randomly selected subsample
of 220 respondents as a calibration sample, a
principal component exploratory factor analysis 5. RESULTS
(EFA) with varimax rotation was first run to
reduce the set of variables to a set of compo- The data collection yielded 441 usable ques-
nents of the PDAQ scale. The principal compo- tionnaires. Among those, about 39% of the
nent factor extraction method is seen as respondents were female. The average age of
appropriate when the goal is to reduce the mea- respondents was 31 years old, although it ran-
sured variables to a smaller set of composite ged from 18 to 70 years old. The majority of
components that capture as much information the surveyed have completed a Bachelor’s
as possible in as few components as possible degree (48.6%), followed by high school
(Brown, 2012; Costello & Osborne, 2005; (14.9%), and Master’s degrees (10.4%). Half
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

Hooper, 2012). Given that the current study is of respondents visited a nature-based destina-
the first attempt to empirically treat the frame- tion while the other half took a trip to an
work of tourist aesthetic judgment (Kirillova urban locale. The nature-based destinations
et al., 2014), capturing and retaining as much varied from such domestic landmarks as the
information as possible was the goal of the Grand Canyon and the Catskill Mountains to
analysis. Factors were retained and considered such international destinations as Ooty in
significant if their eigenvalues were located India, and the Cayman Islands. The urban des-
above the elbow of the scree plot and if their tinations ranged from such well-known loca-
items had factor loadings greater than |.40| tions as Chicago and New Orleans in the
(Hair, Black, Babin, Anderson, & Tatham, United States (US) to such global cities as
2005). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) London in the United Kingdom (UK), and
using the remainder of the responses as a vali- Shanghai in China. On average, the respondents
dation sample followed to further verify the traveled with four other people and stayed 5.6
identified dimensional structure. nights at their respective destinations. For 50%
Data were then analyzed by means of multi- of the respondents, this recent vacation was a
ple regressions where the dimensions (as veri- repeat visit.
fied by EFA and CFA procedures) of PDAQ
were independent variables, and aesthetic judg-
ment and vacation satisfaction acted as depen- 5.1 EFA
dent variables. Before implementation of the
statistical analysis, the data were screened to A principal component EFA with varimax
check that all necessary assumptions for a mul- rotation, conducted on 21 items of PDAQ,
tiple regression procedure were satisfied. revealed a six-factor solution, which accounted
Because aesthetic preference could depend on for 66.42% of total variance. The result of the
vacation setting (nature versus urban) (Hartig, Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure of sam-
1993), a dummy variable was included in the pling adequacy indicated an acceptable level of
regressions to control for this effect. Aesthetic .818 (Tabachnik & Fidell, 1989). The Bartlett’s
distance between respondents’ home environ- test of sphericity was also found to be signifi-
ment and vacation destination environment cant (p < .001). As Table 2 shows, there were
was calculated by subtracting the overall score no significant cross-loadings; thus, all items
of the factor of a vacation destination environ- were retained. The six factors explained
ment from the one of home environment. The 26.10%, 15.00%, 8.65%, 6.01%, 5.80%,
analytical procedure included two multiple 4.80% of the variance respectively. The first
regressions with aesthetic distances for each factor, labeled as Locale characteristics signifies
8 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

TABLE 2. Exploratory Factor Analysis for PDAQ

Factors Factor loadings

F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 F6

Locale characteristics
Quaint – Massive .523 .240 −.090 .025 .391 .207
Not crowded – Crowded .753 .049 .048 .003 .193 .155
Tightly-spaced – Open-spaced −.601 −.081 −.057 .384 .275 −.004
Peaceful – Lively .842 .073 −.050 .124 .119 −.080
Nature-made – Man-made .793 .061 .039 −.319 −.121 .030
Quiet – Loud .896 .005 −.117 −.040 .143 −.020
Presence of nature – Presence of people .859 −.017 .095 −.155 −.011 .194
Simplistic – Sophisticated .737 .094 .134 −.064 −.015 .083
Scope
Nothing to see – Lots of things to see .139 .728 .084 .095 −.022 .154
All alike – Diverse .214 .649 .187 .236 .050 −.162
Familiar – Novel −.030 .677 −.210 −.152 .057 −.201
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

Ordinary – Unique .003 .874 .083 .015 .009 .088


Upkeep
Dirty – Clean −.086 .053 .873 .065 .047 .225
Run-down – Well-maintained .128 .064 .906 .121 .109 .039
Accord
Dark – Bright .102 −.149 .245 .551 .356 −.237
Unbalanced – Balanced −.162 .176 .119 .736 −.122 .318
Artificial – Authentic −.390 .212 .038 .634 −.272 .075
Perceived age
Historic – Modern .172 −.118 .085 −.053 .749 .170
Old – Young −.036 .179 .074 .010 .739 .160
Shape
Rugged – Smooth .141 .139 .275 .066 .198 .686
Asymmetric – Symmetric .171 −.187 .047 .074 .214 .728

Note. PDAQ: perceived destination aesthetic qualities.

the distinction between human-influenced and a destination. The factor Perceived age illus-
nature environments. This factor attests to the trates the apparent age of a destination and
destination appearance of size, degree of human people within the locale while Shape attests to
influence, sound, and the sophistication of fea- classic dimensions of aesthetic judgment, as
tures. The second factor, Scope, represents more elaborated in the art appreciation studies.
experiential dimensions of aesthetic qualities of
tourism destinations and largely attests to the
richness of a sense of novelty, uniqueness, 5.2 CFA
diversity, and abundance of visual and auditory
cues at a destination. The factor Upkeep refers Further verification of the six-factor solution
to the perceived hygienic conditions at a desti- with CFA performed on the remainder of the
nation and the degree of maintenance of its sample initially indicated a poor fit
attributes. Although consisting of similar items (χ2 = 266.32, df = 121, p < .001; χ2/df = 3.97;
as Kirillova et al.’s (2014) theme “condition”, comparative fit index (CFI) = .794; adjusted
the factor was named Upkeep to better reflect goodness of fit index (AGFI) = .790; root
the rewording of the items. The factor Accord mean square error of approximation
includes the items that attest to the degree of (RMSEA) = .093); however, after three items
authenticity and balance of physical features of with loadings less than |.4| were removed
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 9

TABLE 3. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for PDAQ

Factor Standardized factor Cronbach Composite Average variance


loading alpha reliability extracted

Factor 1: Locale characteristics (7


items)
Not crowded–Crowded .66 .75 .84 .54
Open-spaced–Tightly-spaced .43
Peaceful–Lively .88
Nature-made–Man-made .78
Quiet–Loud .90
Presence of nature–Presence of .81
people
Simplistic–Sophisticated .48
Factor 2: Scope (3 items)
Nothing to see-Lots of things to see .60 .53 .47 .24
All alike-Diverse .44
Ordinary-Unique .62
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

Factor 3: Upkeep (2 items)


Run-down–Well-kept .82 .78 .76 .62
Dirty–Clean .78
Factor 4: Accord (2 items)
Artificial–Authentic .60 .54 .53 .38
Unbalanced–Balanced .67
Factor 5: Perceived age (2 items)
Historic–Modern .76 .68 .66 .53
Old–Young .69
Factor 6: Shape (2 items)
Rugged–Smooth .77 .75 .71 .55
Asymmetric–Symmetric .51

Notes. PDAQ: perceived destination aesthetic qualities; Open-spaced–Tightly-spaced was reverse-coded to align its direc-
tionality with the rest of the items; fit indices: X2 = 278.32, df = 119, p < .000; X2/df = 2.34; comparative fit index (CFI) = .887;
adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI) = .816; Tucker-Lewis index = .858; goodness of fit index (GFI) = .897; root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .08.

(quaint–massive, dark–bright, and familiar – The standardized loadings for all 18 items
novel), the fit notably improved (Table 3). ranged from |.43| to |.90|. Cronbach alphas for
Although the overall chi-square statistics indi- four factors were above a recommended cut-
cated that the data did not fit the model well off point of .6 (Mueller, 2004), indicating
(χ2 = 278.32, df = 119, p < .001), given the acceptable internal consistency. Factors Scope
sensitivity of this statistics to the sample size and Accord, however, exhibited reliability
(Bentler & Bonett, 1980), other fit indices were coefficients slightly below the recommended
examined. The normed chi-squared, a statistics threshold. Convergent validity was also some-
that reduces the sensitivity of the chi-square what compromised for Scope and Accord as
statistics to sample size, indicated that the the value for composite reliability was less
model fits the data well with χ2/df = 2.34, than a minimum criteria of .6 (Bagozzi & Yi,
which is lower than a recommended threshold 1988). Discriminant validity was estimated by
of 3 (Bentler, 1990). Additionally, an acceptable comparing the average variance extracted
fit is achieved when CFI is greater than .90, (AVE) with the correlation between the six
AGFI is greater than .80, and RMSEA is less factors. If the variance explained by a factor
or equal to.08 (Hair et al., 2005). Although CFI is higher than correlation among the factors,
for the model was slightly below the threshold the factors are considered as different from
(CFI = .887), other indices indicated an ade- one another (Fornell & Larcker, 1981).
quate fit with AGFI = .816 and RMSEA = .08. Generally, discriminant validity was
10 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

TABLE 4. Factor Correlations significant (F = 24.36, p < .001) predicting


28.00% of variance in tourists’ aesthetic judg-
Factors 1 2 3 4 5 6 ment. Of this portion, Scope (t = 5.347,
p < .001), Upkeep (t = 4.328, p < .001), and
Locale 1
characteristics
Accord (t = 5.120, p < .001) were all significant.
Scope .167 1 It is important to remember that two of the three
Upkeep −.160 .618 1 significant factors, namely Scope and Accord,
Accord −.477 .473 .636 1 exhibit low reliability. Since the most likely pro-
Perceived age .475 .157 .064 −.173 .1
Shape .284 .118 .271 −.033 .470 1
blem resulting from low reliability is the reduction
in the statistical power necessary to detect the
effect (e.g. Nelson, Rosenthal, & Rosnow,
1986), the significant effects from these factors
confirmed. However, because AVE for Scope attest to their more dramatic influence on depen-
and Accord were below a recommended dent variables than if reliability and statistical
threshold of .5 (Fornell & Larcker, 1981), power were at accepted levels. The result provides
discriminate validity was compromised for support for the claims that, unlike object apprecia-
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

these factors as well. Table 4 depicts bivariate tion, tourism environment appreciation is unique
correlations between the factors. in that tourists consider destinations with salient
experiential aesthetic features that are satisfactory
in Scope and are in Accord with overall surround-
5.3 PDAQ, Aesthetic Judgment, and ings, and are well-maintained,. A similar pattern is
Vacation Satisfaction evident in relation to vacation satisfaction. Scope
(t = 5.601, p < .001), Upkeep (t = 3.119, p < .001),
Two separate multiple regression analyses and Accord (t = 3.213, p < .001) were significant
were conducted on the PDAQ factors as indepen- predictors of vacation satisfaction, explaining
dent variables, and aesthetic judgment and vaca- 17.6% of variance (F = 13.822, p < .001). This
tion satisfaction as dependent variables, while is an expected result considering that the aesthetic
controlling for the effect of setting. The results aspect of destinations is known to be an important
are presented in Table 5. The multiple regression determinant of satisfaction (e.g. Kozak &
model with aesthetic judgment as an outcome was Rimmington, 2000).

TABLE 5. Multiple Regressions for PDAQ Predicting Aesthetic Judgment and Vacation Satisfaction

Model Aesthetic judgment Vacation satisfaction

Standardized Standardized
Unstandarzied coefficients coefficients Unstandarzied coefficients coefficients

B SE β B SE β

(Constant) 2.659*** .397 3.107*** .398


Urban −.055 .123 −.023 .191 .051 .085
Locale characteristics −.077 .051 −.086 −.014 .054 −.017
Scope .287*** .054 .252 .301*** .049 .282
Upkeep .213*** .049 .207 .154*** .049 .160
Accord .248*** .049 .262 .156*** .038 .176
Perceived age −.059 .038 −.072 −.064 .041 −.083
Shape −.044 .123 −.023 −.035 .123 −.043
Adjusted R2 .280 .176
F-value 24.235*** 13.822***

Notes. PDAQ: perceived destination aesthetic qualities; SE: standard error; ***p < .001.
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 11

5.4 Aesthetic Distance, Aesthetic negative aesthetic distance, the more beautiful
Judgment, and Vacation Satisfaction the destination appears to tourists. When vaca-
tion satisfaction is treated as a predicted vari-
Two separate multiple regressions were run able (F = 5.479; p < .001; adj. R2 = .067), only
with aesthetic distance for each factor of PDAQ the distance in Scope was significant
as independent variables, and aesthetic judg- (t = −3.784, p < .001), affecting vacation satis-
ment and vacation satisfaction as dependent faction in a similar manner as it did aesthetic
variables, while controlling for the effect of judgment. In general, the results showed that
setting (Table 6). Insightful findings emerged more experiential components enhanced by the
from regressing aesthetic distance on tourists’ contrasting sense of novelty constitute a critical
aesthetic judgment (F = 8.53; p < .001; adj. source of satisfaction in vacation experience.
R2 = .112). Controlling for the setting effect,
which was significant (t = −3.58, p < .001), only
the distances in Scope (t = −2.820, p = .005) 6. DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
and Upkeep (t = −3.036, p = .003) were influ-
ential in tourists’ aesthetic judgment of a desti- Previous research mainly treated tourism aes-
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

nation. Since the distance score was calculated thetics as a one-dimensional destination attri-
by subtracting the overall score of the factor of bute whereas the particular content, or which
a vacation destination environment from the one aspects of a destination comprise its beauty, has
of home environment, negative coefficients not received much analysis. Moreover, a con-
mean that a one-unit increase in distance, or trast in aesthetic qualities between one’s home
when home environment is rated more posi- and the vacation environments, or aesthetic dis-
tively than tourism environment, decreases tour- tance, has not been considered. The current
ist aesthetic judgment and vacation satisfaction research, motivated by the need to systemati-
by the value of the coefficient. It is shown that cally and empirically assess the function of the
when tourists evaluate their home environment aesthetic component in tourism experience, has
more positively in terms of Upkeep and Scope attempted to link destination aesthetic qualities
of its aesthetic featured than the vacation envir- to aesthetic judgment and vacation satisfaction.
onment, they tend to perceive a destination as It has also uncovered associations between aes-
less beautiful. In other words, the farther the thetic qualities of a home environment, tourists’

TABLE 6. Multiple Regressions for Aesthetic Distance Predicting Aesthetic Judgment and Vacation
Satisfaction

Model Aesthetic judgment Vacation satisfaction

Standardized Standardized
Unstandarzied coefficients coefficients Unstandarzied coefficients coefficients

B SE β B SE β

(Constant) 6.010*** .087 5.858*** .089


Urban −.424*** .120 −.177 −.001 .116 −.001
Locale characteristics .084 .046 .122 .060 .044 .095
Scope −.101** .036 −.156 −.134*** .035 −.222
Upkeep −.144** .047 −.206 −.049 .038 −.076
Accord −.009 .047 −.013 −.045 .042 −.069
Perceived age .063 .040 .085 −.015 .038 −.022
Shape .039 .036 .056 .016 .034 .024
Adjusted R2 .112 .067
F-value 8.530*** 5.479***

Notes. SE: standard error; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
12 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

satisfaction, and their perception of a destina- 1989; Nasar, 1984; Rogge et al., 2007) and abun-
tion as beautiful, thus providing a practical dance (Ohta, 2001) were noted as important in
device for tourism destination marketers in mar- individuals’ aesthetic appreciation. However, this
ket segmentation, selection, and promotion investigation revealed that this component is not
efforts. The results of this research deliver simply important; in fact, it is the second most
important insights for both destination man- salient dimension of tourists’ aesthetic judgment,
agers and consumers in search of satisfying and, as such, deserves further scrutiny. While
vacation experiences. upkeep (also previously elaborated on in environ-
mental psychology and tourism research) was also
a prominent dimension of tourism aesthetics, such
6.1 The Role of PDAQ in Tourism factors as accord, perceived age, and shape were
only incrementally salient. This result is consis-
The findings suggest a six-dimensional struc- tent with Kirillova et al.’s (2014) qualitative
ture of PDAQ, and they are: locale characteris- assessment of tourists’ aesthetic judgment in
tics, scope, upkeep, accord, perceived age, and that, although still influential, classic dimensions
shape. A relatively low variance explained of aesthetics such as symmetry and round shape
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

when predicting tourist aesthetic judgment is are not as critical as its more experiential compo-
consistent with previous theorizations of aes- nents such as diversity, abundance, and
thetic judgment as people often link “the beauty authenticity.
of a tourism destination to other domains of life This study has also established that the
such as religion, history, poverty, safety, life- dimensions of scope, accord, and upkeep posi-
style …” (Kirillova et al., 2014, p. 288). Thus, tively affect tourists’ aesthetic judgment and
as is evident from the findings, purely aesthetics vacation satisfaction. The abundance of authen-
aspects of tourism experience, which were the tic, well-maintained, diverse, and unique fea-
focus in this research, account for only a portion tures that are in harmony with natural and
of tourists’ aesthetic judgment. Even though cultural surroundings significantly contribute to
destination aesthetic qualities are important in tourists’ judgments of a destination as beautiful.
prompting tourists to classify a destination as The latter particularly coincides with Lehto’s
beautiful or ugly, it appears that they are not the (2013) conceptualization of perceived restora-
only constituents of aesthetic judgment. tive qualities of vacation destinations where
Aesthetic qualities that characterize the vacation the compatibility dimension stresses the impor-
locale accounted for most of the variation tance of a destination to be perceived as “true to
(26.10%), which is not surprising considering itself”, holistic, and in harmony with natural
that people are cognizant of the type of setting surroundings.
(urban versus nature) in which their vacation Furthermore, similar to abundance and diver-
takes place and thus judge a destination against sity, Lehto’s extent dimension attests to the
the dimensions most appropriate for the setting. content variability and the sense of environmen-
The items comprising this dimension corrobo- tal depth at a destination. These concurrences
rate the previous studies in environmental psy- suggest the possible interrelationship between
chology as naturalness (Galindo & Rodriguez, perceived aesthetic and restorative qualities of
2000; Nasar, 1984; Real et al., 2000), natural vacation destinations, warranting future
sounds (Yu & Kang, 2010), openness (Rogge research in this direction.
et al., 2007), cultivatedness (Van den Berg The findings invite a variety of opportunities
et al., 1998), and a degree of human influence for destination planners and managers. Because
(Fyhri et al., 2009) were all found to influence the dimension of scope in tourism aesthetics is
environmental preference. critical in providing aesthetic pleasure and deli-
The experiential aspects of tourism aesthetics vering an enjoyable tourism experience, a des-
that attest to its scope are also supported by past tination should strive to create a sense of
literature to a certain extent, for such criteria as complete immersion into the destination envir-
diversity (Galindo & Hidalgo, 2005; Kaplan et al., onment in order to prompt tourists to
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 13

experience the destination in its richest and 6.2 Aesthetic Distance in Tourism
most expansive scope. One strategy could be Experience
to provide tourists with many things to see and
to do that are unique to a destination. While The findings indicated that the contrast in
some destinations already have a number of aesthetic qualities between tourists’ home set-
unique features in their inventories (e.g. the ting and destination environment has an effect
Eiffel Tower in Paris, France, or the Chicago on tourists’ aesthetic judgment and satisfaction.
Bean in Chicago, US), others, especially emer- Specifically, when home environment is evalu-
gent destinations, may lack the attributes easily ated more favorably in terms of upkeep and the
identifiable with the locale, and, in this case, abundance of diverse and unique aesthetic cues,
such features should be designed. For instance, a destination tends to be judged as less beauti-
a town could consider building an exclusively ful. In the case when destination aesthetics is
projected monument or a fountain with which it perceived as superior on these dimensions, tour-
could potentially be uniquely associated. In ists consider a destination as more attractive.
addition to abundance of unique attributes, des- Interestingly, although a similar relationship
tination managers should also ensure that these exists between aesthetic distance and satisfac-
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

features are also diverse and captivating enough tion, only the contrast in features reflecting the
in order for tourists to sustain their admiration scope of destination aesthetics influences tour-
for the duration of a trip. In other words, these ists’ satisfaction. The prediction of how per-
features must provide an optimal level of aes- ceived aesthetic distance affects tourists’
thetic stimuli for favorable aesthetic judgment satisfaction and aesthetic judgment, therefore,
to occur. depends not on the absolute distance between
Besides the overall level of upkeep at a des- the two points of comparison but rather on the
tination, which is widely recognized as essential direction of this comparison. As a whole, these
to tourist satisfaction (see for example Baloglu results support previous discussions on the
& McCleary, 1999; Kozak, 2003), destinations’ value of novelty in tourism (Lee & Crompton,
perceived integrity to its core character and 1992) and provide insights into why, under
surrounding environment also appears to be certain conditions, the sense of novelty may be
crucial to tourists’ aesthetic evaluations and counterproductive to tourists’ enjoyment, as
satisfaction. Destination managers should make evidenced by Reisinger and Turner (1998) and
any effort to present destination features as Ng et al. (2007).
authentic and fitting the surroundings. In order In general, the experiential aspect of tourism
to design and deliver a satisfying experience, as exemplified in the criticality of the dimen-
Pine and Gilmore (1998), for example, suggest sions of scope has been extensively discussed in
that positive cues fulfilling the theme should be the literature (see for example Cohen, 1979;
maximized while negative cues, or those not Curtin, 2005; Otto & Ritchie, 1996; Uriely,
directly related to the theme, must be reduced. 2005); however, the extent of its influence on
In the tourism context, the theme signifies the tourists was not determined. Focusing on aes-
core essence of a destination, and destination thetics, the current study demonstrated that
aesthetic qualities should be assessed on the experiential aspects of tourism are pervasive in
degree of their compatibility with the core. tourism consumption, influencing how a desti-
Destination attributes should also appear to be nation is perceived, admired, and evaluated.
in harmony with natural and cultural surround- However, besides destination aesthetics, experi-
ings. For instance, planners should avoid pla- ential components of aesthetic qualities of a
cing an ultra-modern skyscraper next to a home environment are also relevant to tourists’
historic building and vice versa to preserve the satisfaction and aesthetic judgment as they con-
distinct architecture. Thus, instituting and enfor- stitute a benchmark against which all tourism
cing zoning ordinances becomes essential to environments are judged. It is then a natural
protecting an authentic character of a place outcome that if tourism aesthetics exceed the
and the integrity of its aesthetic features. baseline level of what a tourist is used to at
14 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

home, a destination is considered as beautiful. tourism experience. Thus, if the aesthetic dis-
Then, aesthetic judgment and satisfaction in tance between destination and home environ-
tourism is not just a function of destination ment is large and negative, the distinction
attributes and tourists’ personal characteristics between ordinary and extraordinary is clearly
but also of their living environments. demarcated and tourists are more likely to
Emphasizing the importance of home envir- experience “flow”. Taken as a whole, when
onment, the idea that aesthetic distance could considered in marketing plans, a properly
define tourists’ satisfaction and the perception defined aesthetic distance has the potential to
of a destination as beautiful offers guidance for benefit destinations as well as tourists.
destination marketers. First, the strategic mar-
keting process should begin with compiling a
destination’s aesthetic inventory, in which aes- 7. CONCLUSION
thetic qualities are to be classified into the six
dimensions as suggested in this research. Given Tourism environment implies a unique process
the criticality of scope in tourism aesthetics, of aesthetic appreciation. This study is a pioneer-
destinations should strive to acquire as many ing effort to empirically address the role of desti-
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

aesthetic features as possible in this dimension. nation aesthetics in tourism experience.


Second, in their market segmentation efforts, Conceptually, it has expanded the notion of beau-
destination marketing organizations (DMOs) tiful scenery to be understood as a multidimen-
should emphasize geographic segmentation sional construct, encompassing traditional
that is based not purely on the distinction dimensions (e.g. shape) as well as dimensions
between geographic areas but rather on a type unique to landscape aesthetics (e.g. accord). The
and quality of aesthetic features contained six dimensions provide the baseline measure for
within these locales. For example, although destination aesthetic qualities as perceived by
Moscow and St. Petersburg are geographically tourists. Having introduced the concept of aes-
close and located within one country, Russia, thetic distance, this research further demonstrated
they possess distinct aesthetic qualities and, for the criticality of contrasting environments in tour-
this reason, their residents would not constitute ism experience and the relevance of home envir-
a homogeneous market segment. Third, destina- onment in aesthetic evaluation of destinations and
tion aesthetic inventory should be matched with tourist satisfaction. The measure of perceived
that of targeted locales to determine the seg- aesthetic qualities of a tourism destination could
ments with the highest likelihood of engender- allow the aesthetic components to be linked to
ing favorable aesthetic judgments of destination such tourism concepts as motivation, loyalty, and
and tourists being satisfied with the entire tour- behavioral intentions, further solidifying our
ism experience. Therefore, the optimal target understanding of the value of aesthetics in tour-
market would include individuals from locales ism experience.
with the farthest negative aesthetic distance to a Aside from the conceptual value, the study
tourism destination, and DMOs should specifi- also bears practical significance. It empirically
cally channel their efforts into maximizing the verified how tourists evaluate aesthetic proper-
distance in terms of upkeep and experiential ties of vacation destinations, and thus it invites
aspects. opportunities to influence tourists’ aesthetic
Maximizing a negative aesthetic distance is judgment and assists destination planners in
also instrumental in facilitating extraordinary the evaluation of destinations’ aesthetic inven-
experiences at tourism destinations. These deep- tory. The developed instrument could serve as a
ly intense positive experiences stand in opposi- useful tool in destination planning and market-
tion to everyday life events and include “peak”, ing as it allows for a more comprehensive eva-
transformative, or “flow” moments. When com- luation of destination aesthetic components.
bined with physical surroundings, such experi- Practitioners may be able to inventorize the
ences may be particularly powerful, aesthetic qualities and assess the relative impor-
contributing to tourists’ evaluation of the entire tance of aesthetic dimensions in tourists’
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 15

aesthetic judgment and satisfaction with a des- caution. Therefore, future research is needed
tination. By locating the dimensions on which a to provide additional support for the dimen-
destination has the highest and lowest scores, sionality of the measure proposed in the cur-
destination marketers can develop appropriate rent study. Nonetheless, it represents an initial
marketing strategies to capitalize on strong attempt to operationalize tourists’ perceptions
aspects of destination aesthetics. Furthermore, of destination aesthetic qualities, providing
the method to measure the aesthetic distance insights into how aesthetic judgments are rea-
enables marketers to identify the market seg- lized in the tourism context. Although every
ments with aesthetic distances that optimally effort was made to sample vacationer popula-
increase the likelihood of positive aesthetic tion with as varied experiences and destina-
judgment and satisfaction. tions as possible, it should be acknowledged
that by asking respondents to recall the most
recent trip, a recollection bias could occur.
7.1 Limitations and Future Research The current study also used a non-probability
sampling technique that could have further
Despite the contributions, it is important to biased the results thus limiting their general-
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

acknowledge the limitations of this study. izability. Given the experiential nature of tour-
Having focused on the relationship between aes- ism, future studies should adopt a
thetics and outcomes of tourism experience such phenomenological perspective to examine the
as aesthetic judgment and vacation satisfaction, aesthetic judgment in tourism and its role in
this study neither considered tourists’ motivations tourists’ satisfaction with a destination.
beyond a vacation nor accounted for the types of
vacations undertaken by respondents. Because
certain types of tourism experiences (e.g. cultural
or heritage tourism) would imply greater attention REFERENCES
to destination aesthetic qualities than others, this
information could be an important source of var- Alegre, J., & Garau, J. (2010). Tourist satisfaction and
iation in responses. Future research is encouraged dissatisfaction. Annals of Tourism Research, 37(1),
to relate motivation, aesthetics, and type of tour- 52–73. doi:10.1016/j.annals.2009.07.001
ism experience to tourism outcomes. In this Bagozzi, R., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of struc-
tural equation models. Journal of the Academy of
research, aesthetics has been discussed and illu- Marketing Science, 16(1), 74–94. doi:10.1007/
strated from a Western perspective, obscuring the BF02723327
relevance of the findings to cross-cultural travel Baloglu, S., & McCleary, K. (1999). A model of destination
and travel within the non-Western world. Further image formation. Annals of Tourism Research, 26(4),
research is needed to frame and validate the find- 868–897.
ings in a cross-cultural and non-Western context: Baloglu, S., Pekcan, A., Chen, S., & Santos, J. (2004). The
how does aesthetic judgment in tourism differ relationship between destination performance, overall
satisfaction, and behavioral intention for distinct seg-
among tourists of various cultural backgrounds
ments. Journal of Quality Assurance in Hospitality and
and how is destination beauty evaluated in a Tourism, 4(3–4), 149–165. doi:10.1300/J162v04n03_10
non-Western world? Further research is invited Beardsley, M. C. (1975). Aesthetics from classical Greece to
to explore the role of perceived aesthetic distance the present (Vol. 13). Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama
in destination choice and destination image eva- Press.
luation. It would also be of practical significance Bentler, P. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural
to verify whether people from the same locale models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
arrive at similar aesthetic judgments of a
Bentler, P., & Bonett, D. (1980). Significance tests and
destination. goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures.
Validation of the measure used in this Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 588–606.
research raised the issue of construct validity Berleant, A. (2005). Aesthetics and environment:
of two factors – scope and accord – indicating Variations on a theme. Burlington, VT: Ashgate
that the results should be interpreted with Publishing Company.
16 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

Berlyne, D. (1966). Curiosity and exploration. Science, Han, K. T. (2007). Responses to six major terrestrial
153(3731), 25–33. doi:10.1126/science.153.3731.25 biomes in terms of scenic beauty, preference, and
Bitner, M. (1992). Servicescapes: The impact of physical restorativeness. Environment and Behavior, 39(4),
surroundings on customers and employees. Journal of 529–556. doi:10.1177/0013916506292016
Marketing, 56, 57–71. doi:10.2307/1252042 Hartig, T. (1993). Nature experience in transactional per-
Brown, T. (2012). Confirmatory factor analysis for spective. Landscape and Urban Planning, 25(1–2),
applied research. New York, NY: Guilford Press. 17–36. doi:10.1016/0169-2046(93)90120-3
Brunner-Sperdin, A., Peters, M., & Strobl, A. (2012). It is Hekkert, P., Snelders, D., & van Wieringen, P. (2003).
all about the emotional state: Managing tourists’ experi- ‘Most advanced, yet acceptable’: Typicality and
ences. International Journal of Hospitality Management, novelty as joint predictors of aesthetic preference in
31(1), 23–30. doi:10.1016/j.ijhm.2011.03.004 industrial design. British Journal of Psychology, 94,
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). 111–124. doi:10.1348/000712603762842147
Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, Hepburn, R. (1966). Contemporary aesthetics and the neglect
yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological of natural beauty. In B. Williams & A. Montefiore (Eds.),
Science, 6(1), 3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980 British analytical philosophy (pp. 285–310). London:
Byrne, D., & Nelson, D. (1965). Attraction as a linear Routledge & Kegan Paul.
function of proportion of positive reinforcements. Hooper, D. (2012). Exploratory factor analysis. In H. Chen
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, (Ed.), Approaches to quantitative research - theory and
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

659–663. doi:10.1037/h0022073 its practical application: A Guide to dissertation stu-


Cohen, E. (1979). A phenomenology of tourist experi- dents (pp. 1–33). Cork: Oak Tree Press.
ences. Sociology, 13(2), 179–201. doi:10.1177/ Hosany, S., & Gilbert, D. (2010). Measuring tourists’
003803857901300203 emotional experiences toward hedonic holiday destina-
Costello, A., & Osborne, J. (2005). Best practices in tions. Journal of Travel Research, 49(4), 513–526.
exploratory factor analysis: Four recommendations for doi:10.1177/0047287509349267
getting the most from your analysis. Practiceal Ittelson, W. (1978). Environmental perception and urban
Assessment, Research, and Evaluation, 10(7), 1–9. experience. Environment and Behavior, 10(2), 193–
Crotts, J. (2004). The effect of cultural distance on over- 213. doi:10.1177/0013916578102004
seas travel behaviors. Journal of Travel Research, Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R., Höfel, L., & Cramon, D.
43(1), 83–88. doi:10.1177/0047287504265516 (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxi- beauty. Neuroimage, 29(1), 276–285.
ety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Jang, S., & Namkung, Y. (2009). Perceived quality, emo-
Curtin, S. (2005). Nature, wild animals and tourism: An tions, and behavioral intentions: Application of an
experiential view. Journal of Ecotourism, 4(1), 1–15. extended Mehrabian–Russell model to restaurants.
doi:10.1080/14724040508668434 Journal of Business Research, 62(4), 451–460.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2008.01.038
models with unobservable variables and measurement Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., & Brown, T. (1989).
error: Algebra and statistics. Journal of Marketing Environmental preference: A comparison of four
Research, 18(3), 382–388. doi:10.2307/3150980 domains of predictors. Environment and Behavior,
Fyhri, A., Jacobsen, J., & Tømmervik, H. (2009). Tourists’ 21(5), 509–530. doi:10.1177/0013916589215001
landscape perceptions and preferences in a Scandinavian Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature:
coastal region. Landscape and Urban Planning, 91(4), Toward an integrative framework. Journal of
202–211. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2009.01.002 Environmental Psychology, 15, 169–182. doi:10.1016/
Galindo, M., & Hidalgo, M. (2005). Aesthetic preferences 0272-4944(95)90001-2
and the attribution of meaning: Environmental categor- King, S., Meiselman, H., Hottenstein, A., Work, T., &
ization processes in the evaluation of urban scenes. Cronk, V. (2007). The effects of contextual variables
International Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 19–27. on food acceptability: A confirmatory study. Food
doi:10.1080/00207590444000104 Quality and Preference, 18, 58–65. doi:10.1016/j.
Galindo, M., & Rodriguez, J. (2000). Environmental aes- foodqual.2005.07.014
thetics and psychological wellbeing: Relationships King, S., Weber, A., Meiselman, H., & Lv, N. (2004). The
between preference judgments for urban landscapes effect of meal situation, social interaction, physical
and other relevant affective responses. Psychology in environment and choice on food acceptability. Food
Spain, 4(1), 13–27. Quality and Preference, 15, 645–653. doi:10.1016/j.
Hair, J., Black, B., Babin, B., Anderson, A., & Tatham, R. foodqual.2004.04.010
(2005). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Boston, Kirillova, K., Fu, X., Lehto, X., & Cai, L. (2014). What
MA: Prentice Hall. makes a destination beautiful? Dimensions of tourist
Ksenia Kirillova and Xinran Lehto 17

aesthetic judgment. Tourism Management, 42, 282–293. Nelson, N., Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1986).
doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006 Interpretation of significance levels and effect sizes by
Kozak, M. (2003). Measuring tourist satisfaction with psychological researchers. American Psychologist,
multiple destination attributes. Tourism Analysis, 7(3/ 41(11), 1299–1301. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.41.11.1299
4), 229–240. doi:10.3727/108354203108750076 Ng, S., Lee, J., & Soutar, G. (2007). Tourists’ intention to
Kozak, M., & Rimmington, M. (2000). Tourist satisfaction visit a country: The impact of cultural distance.
with Mallorca, Spain, as an off-season holiday destina- Tourism Management, 28(6), 1497–1506.
tion. Journal of Travel Research, 38(3), 260–269. doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2006.11.005
doi:10.1177/004728750003800308 North, A., Shilcock, A., & Hargreaves, D. (2003). The
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). effect of musical style on restaurant customers’ spend-
A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judg- ing. Environment and Behavior, 35(5), 712–718.
ments. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 489–508. O’Leary, S., & Deegan, J. (2003). People, pace, place:
doi:10.1348/0007126042369811 Qualitative and quantitative images of Ireland as a tour-
Lee, S., Jeon, S., & Kim, D. (2011). The impact of tour ism destination in France. Journal of Vacation Marketing,
quality and tourist satisfaction on tourist loyalty: The 9(3), 213–226. doi:10.1177/135676670300900302
case of Chinese tourists in Korea. Tourism Ohta, H. (2001). A phenomenological approach to natural
Management, 32(5), 1115–1124. doi:10.1016/j. landscape cognition. Journal of Environmental
tourman.2010.09.016 Psychology, 21(4), 387–403. doi:10.1006/
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

Lee, T., & Crompton, J. (1992). Measuring novelty seek- jevp.2001.0233


ing in tourism. Annals of Tourism Research, 19(4), Oliver, R. (1989). Processing of the satisfaction response
732–751. doi:10.1016/0160-7383(92)90064-V in consumption: A suggested framework and research
Lehto, X. (2013). Assessing the perceived restorative quali- propositions. Journal of Consumer Satisfaction,
ties of vacation destinations. Journal of Travel Research, Dissatisfcation and Complaining Behavior, 2, 1–16.
52(3), 325–339. doi:10.1177/0047287512461567 Otto, J., & Ritchie, J. (1996). The service experience in
Liu, Y., & Jang, S. (2009). The effects of dining atmo- tourism. Tourism Management, 17(3), 165–174.
spherics: An extended Mehrabian–Russell model. doi:10.1016/0261-5177(96)00003-9
International Journal of Hospitality Management, Pine, B., & Gilmore, J. (1998). Welcome to the experience
28(4), 494–503. economy. Harvard Business Review, 76, 97–105.
Mannell, R. C., & Iso-Ahola, S. E. (1987). Psychological Pizam, A., Neumann, Y., & Reichel, A. (1978).
nature of leisure and tourism experience. Annals of Dimentions of tourist satisfaction with a destination
Tourism Research, 14(3), 314–331. doi:10.1016/0160- area. Annals of Tourism Research, 5(3), 314–322.
7383(87)90105-8 doi:10.1016/0160-7383(78)90115-9
Markovic, S. (2012). Components of aesthetic experience: Real, E., Arce, C., & Sabucedo, M. (2000). Classification
Aesthetic fascination, aesthetic appraisal, and aesthetic of landscapes using quantitative and categorical data,
emotion. I-perceptions, 3, 1–17. doi:10.1068/i0450aap and prediction of their scenic beauty in north-western
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. (1974). An approach to Spain. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20(4),
environmental psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT 355–373. doi:10.1006/jevp.2000.0184
Press. Reisinger, Y., & Turner, L. (1998). Cross-cultural differ-
Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1973). A measure of ences in tourism: A strategy for tourism marketers.
arousal seeking tendency. Environment and Behavior, Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 7(4), 79–
5(3), 315–333. doi:10.1177/001391657300500303 106. doi:10.1300/J073v07n04_05
Meng, F., Tepanon, Y., & Uysal, M. (2008). Measuring Rogge, E., Nevens, F., & Gulinck, H. (2007). Perception
tourist satisfaction by attribute and motivation: The case of rural landscapes in Flanders: Looking beyond aes-
of a nature-based resort. Journal of Vacation Marketing, thetics. Landscape and Urban Planning, 82, 159–174.
14(1), 41–56. doi:10.1177/1356766707084218 doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2007.02.006
Milliman, R. E. (1986). The influence of background Ryu, K., & Jang, S. S. (2007). The effect of environmental
music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. Journal perceptions on behavioral intentions through emotions:
of Consumer Research, 13, 286–289. doi:10.1086/ The case of upscale restaurants. Journal of Hospitality
209068 & Tourism Research, 31(1), 56–72. doi:10.1177/
Mueller, C. (2004). Reliability. In M. Lewis-Beck (Ed.), 1096348006295506
The Sage encyclopedia of social science research meth- Silvia, P., & Barona, C. (2009). Do people prefer curved
ods (Vol. 3). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. objects? Angularity, expertise, and aesthetic preference.
Nasar, J. L. (1984). Visual preferences in urban street scenes: Empirical Studies of the Arts, 27(1), 25–42.
A cross-cultural comparison between Japan and the doi:10.2190/EM.27.1.b
United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Tabachnik, B., & Fidell, L. (1989). Using multivriate
15(1), 79–93. doi:10.1177/0022002184015001005 statistics (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Harper & Row.
18 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL & TOURISM MARKETING

Todd, C. (2009). Nature, beauty, and tourism. In J. Tribe of the German travel market in Australia. Pacific
(Ed.), Philosophical issues in tourism (pp. 154–170). Tourism Review, 1(1), 35–45.
Bristol: Channel View Publications. Weber, R., Schnier, J., & Jacobsen, T. (2008). Aesthetics
Toyama, M., & Yamada, Y. (2012). The relationships of streetscapes: Influence of fundamental properties on
among tourist novelty, familiarity, satisfaction, and des- aesthetic judgments of urban space. Perceptual and
tination loyalty: Beyond the novelty-familiarity conti- Motor Skills, 106, 128–146. doi:10.2466/pms.106.1.
nuum. International Journal of Marketing Studies, 4, 6. 128-146
Uriely, N. (2005). The tourist experience: Conceptual Yiannakis, A., & Gibson, H. (1992). Roles tourists play.
developments. Annals of Tourism Research, 32(1), Annals of Tourism Research, 19(2), 287–303.
199–216. doi:10.1016/j.annals.2004.07.008 doi:10.1016/0160-7383(92)90082-Z
Van den Berg, A., Vlek, C., & Coeterier, J. (1998). Group Yu, L., & Kang, J. (2010). Factors influencing the sound
differences in the aesthetic evaluation of nature devel- preference in urban open spaces. Applied Acoustics, 71
opment plans: A multilevel approach. Journal of (7), 622–633. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2010.02.005
Environmental Psychology, 18, 141–157. doi:10.1006/
jevp.1998.0080
Wahlers, R., & Etzel, M. (1985). Vacation preference as a SUBMITTED: March 11, 2014
manifestation of optimal stimulation and lifestyle experi- FINAL REVISION SUBMITTED:
ence. Journal of Leisure Research, 17(4), 283–295. July 21, 2014
Downloaded by [Purdue University] at 08:21 16 April 2015

Weber, K. (1997). The assessment of tourist satisfaction ACCEPTED: August 12, 2014
using the expectancy disconfirmation theory: A study REFEREED ANONYMOUSLY

View publication stats

You might also like