Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect
Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex

Special issue: Note

Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor


stroke: A more sensitive measure for detecting
subtle memory dysfunction?

Sofie Geurts a,*, Sieberen P. van der Werf b,c,d, Vincent I.H. Kwa e and
Roy P.C. Kessels f,g
a
Canisius Wilhelmina Hospital, Department of Medical Psychology, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
b
University of Amsterdam, Department of Psychology, Brain and Cognition, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
c
OLVG, Department of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
d
Rijnstate Hospital, Department of Medical Psychology, Arnhem, The Netherlands
e
OLVG, Department of Neurology, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
f
Radboud University, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
g
Radboud University Medical Center, Department of Medical Psychology, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

article info abstract

Article history: Cognitive changes after minor stroke or TIA have been reported, with studies describing a
Received 30 March 2017 ‘vascular’ cognitive profile with spared episodic memory. Still, many patients also report
Reviewed 28 September 2017 memory complaints. Studies using long-term forgetting paradigms have detected memory
Revised 4 December 2017 impairment after prolonged intervals in contrast to standard delayed testing in other pa-
Accepted 3 April 2018 tient groups. This study examined whether accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF) is pre-
Published online xxx sent in patients with minor stroke or TIA by comparing one-week delayed recall and
recognition with the performance of a healthy control group. Results revealed that the
Keywords: patients' performance after one week was worse than the controls, in the absence of an
Long-term memory impairment after a short delay. Patients did, however, not report more memory worries
Episodic memory than controls. Possibly, reduced effort, attention or mnemonic strategies may contribute to
Stroke subtle consolidation problems, which go undetected in daily functioning.
Transient ischemic attack © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Vascular cognitive impairment

otherwise good prognosis (Fens et al., 2013). For instance, six


1. Introduction months after minor stroke Dassonville et al. (2016) found high
levels of cognitive complaints and objective cognitive im-
Studies addressing the cognitive consequences after transient
pairments in the domains of memory, executive functioning
ischemic attack (TIA) and minor stroke are scarce, although
and processing speed. Also, Van Rooij et al. (2014) reported
many patients experience cognitive changes despite the

* Corresponding author. Canisius Wilhelmina Hospital, Department of Medical Psychology, Weg door Jonkerbos 100, 6500 GS, Nijmegen,
The Netherlands.
E-mail address: s.geurts@cwz.nl (S. Geurts).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
0010-9452/© 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
2 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7

cognitive impairments after TIA in over a third of their patient


group, the most affected domains being working memory,
2. Method
processing speed and attention, whereas episodic memory
2.1. Participants
was spared. A comparable cognitive profile was found by
Mandzia et al. (2016), p. 90 days after the minor stroke or TIA
We included 30 patients (17 men) who had either experienced
episodic memory performances were within the normal
a TIA or a minor stroke, .5e6 months prior to the assessment.
range, but a lower performance in the of domains attention
The diagnosis of minor stroke or TIA was made by a neurol-
and executive functioning was demonstrated.
ogist in accordance with established criteria (NINDS, 1990).
This ‘vascular’ cognitive profile of attention and executive
The modified Ranking Score (mRS) was used to measure
dysfunction with spared episodic memory function has been
disability in daily activities of the patient group (Sulter, Steen,
related to subcortical brain damage (Zamboni et al., 2017; Van
& de Keyser, 1990). The vast majority of the patient group (27/
Rooij et al., 2017). However, considering the high levels of re-
30) was tested one to three months post-stroke. Other inclu-
ported memory complaints (Dassonville et al., 2016; Fens
sion criteria were that the age- and education-adjusted per-
et al., 2013), the spared episodic memory may also result
formance on the Dutch version of the Rey Auditory Verbal
from the insensitivity of memory tests in detecting subtle
Memory Test (RAVLT; Van der Elst, van Boxtel, van Breukelen,
decrements. Several studies have indeed found that patients
& Jolles, 2005) had to be within the normal range (defined as an
with near-normal performances on standard 30-min delayed-
age- and education-adjusted T-score >35 for the total number
recall tests displayed accelerated long-term forgetting (ALF)
of words recalled in the five learning trials and for the delayed
after a prolonged delay (i.e., days to weeks) (Butler & Zeman,
recall test for which excellent norms are available) and that
2008).
the participant's age was below 75.
ALF has been consistently shown in patients with temporal
Patients were mainly included via a larger study in which
lobe epilepsy (TLE) (Muhlert et al., 2011). Butler et al. (2007)
TIA and minor stroke patients were followed-up after the
compared the performance on the Rey Auditory Verbal
event to monitor changes in cognition, psychological burden
Learning Test (RAVLT) of 50 patients with transient epileptic
and quality of life. This larger cohort existed of 375 patients, 35
amnesia to that of 24 healthy controls after 30-min, one-week
of whom completed the full RAVLT protocol required for this
and three-week delays. After one week, the patients showed a
study. One more patient was included after a regular referral
considerably larger decline on the RAVLT recall scores
for neuropsychological assessment because of concerns about
compared to the controls. In addition to ALF for words and
her cognitive functioning. The diagnosis of minor stroke of
procedural skills, Muhlert, Milton, Butler, Kapur, and Zeman
this one patient was based on the same NINDS criteria as was
(2010) demonstrated faster forgetting of everyday events in
for the other patients. All 36 patients fulfilled the inclusion
patients with transient epileptic amnesia over a period of
criteria of the normal range of T-scores on the RAVLT, but 6
three weeks in comparison to controls. These findings were
patients had to be excluded based on their age (75), leading
subsequently replicated in TLE patients using intervals up to 8
to the remaining group of 30. All patients were living inde-
weeks (see Elliot, Isaac, & Muhlert, 2014, for a review). Two
pendently at home. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression
possible accounts regarding the underlying mechanisms of
Scale (HADS) was used to screen for signs of major mood
ALF have been proposed, that is, a disruption of ‘late’ memory
problems, using the following classifications: 0e7 (normal),
consolidation or subtle (and consequently undetected) early
8e11 (mild), 11e14 (moderate) and 14e21 (severe) (Hung, Liu,
acquisition problems (Cassel, Morris, Koutroumanidis, &
Wang, Yao, & Yang, 2012; Snaith, 2003). See Table 1 for the
Kopelman, 2016).
mean HADS outcomes (note that the HADS was not admin-
A recent review focusing on ALF in other patient groups,
istered in two patients); none of the patients were known to
such as Alzheimer's disease, Multiple Sclerosis and traumatic
have been diagnosed with a major depressive disorder. Two
brain injury, however, showed less consistent evidence for
patients reported moderate complaints of anxiety, one patient
ALF (Geurts, van der Werf, & Kessels, 2015). Furthermore, a
reported moderate depressive symptoms and two patients
recent empirical study in patients with mixed etiologies who
reported moderate to severe complaints on both scales.
reported subjective memory complaints demonstrated
In addition, a control group was selected from a database
steeper long-term (i.e., after one week) forgetting in these
with long-term forgetting data of 101 healthy participants.
patients than in controls (Van der Werf, Geurts, & de Werd,
These data had been collected previously; exclusion criteria
2016). However, the heterogeneity of that patient sample
for these participants were a history of a neurologic disorder
limited further conclusions about specific etiologies.
or a current psychiatric disorder. We used this database to
The present study examines long-term forgetting in minor
group-match 28 healthy participants (15 men) on gender, ed-
stroke and TIA patients, also focusing on whether long-term
ucation level and age. For the demographical data and time
forgetting is related to subjective memory complaints in
since event see Table 1. The two groups did not differ with
these patients. We only included patients who performed
respect to age [t (56) ¼ 1.86, p ¼ .07], educational level
unimpaired on regular 30-min delayed recall and recognition
(U ¼ 450.0, Z ¼ .48, p ¼ .63), or gender distribution [c2 (1) ¼ .06,
memory tests. We hypothesized that even after normal
p ¼ .81]. The study protocol for the patients was evaluated and
acquisition the patient group would show enhanced forget-
approved by the local medical ethics committee of the OLVG
ting compared to a matched healthy control group. Further-
in Amsterdam and the Canisius Wilhelmina Hospital in Nij-
more, we expected to find an association between subjective
megen; collection of control data was approved by the
memory complaints and ALF in these patients.

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7 3

Table 1 e Demographic characteristics for all participants, as well as depressive symptoms, anxiety level, stroke details and
cerebrovascular risk factors for the patients.

Patients (N ¼ 30) Controls (N ¼ 28)


Mean (SD) or N Range Mean (SD) Range
Sex distribution (men/women) 17/13 15/13
Age (years) 60.2 (10.5) 28e74 55.6 (8.0) 39e77
Education (level)a 5 2e7 6 1e7
HADS
Anxiety 4.1 (4.8) 0e18
Depression 5.1 (4.2) 0e17
Time since event (days) 67.8 (30.5) 16e182
TIA/minor stroke 4/26
Modified Rankin Scale (mRS)b
0 e No symptoms 3
1 e No significant disability 11
2 e Slight disability 8
3 e Moderate disability 3
First-ever stroke or TIA (yes/no/unknown) 21/1/8
Stroke locationc
Left hemisphere 12
Right hemisphere 8
Bilateral 1
Infratentorial 4
Arteryc
Middle cerebral artery 17
Posterior cerebral artery 2
Vertebrobasilar arterial system 6
Unknown 1
Type 2 diabetes (yes/no) 4/26
Hypertension (yes/no) 16/14
Hypercholesterolemia (yes/no) 6/24
Smoking (yes/no/unknown) 10/19/1

Note:
a
Mode; education level was classified using a 7-point scale based on the Dutch educational system, with ‘1’ reflecting less 6 years of primary
school and ‘7’ university degree. HADS ¼ Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale: 0e7 (normal), 8e11 (mild), 11e14 (moderate) and 14e21
(severe); note that HADS scores were missing for two patients.
b
mRS was not available for 5 patients.
c
For the minor stroke patients only.

research ethical committee of the faculty of social sciences of controls underwent memory testing with the RAVLT and
Radboud University in Nijmegen. completed the Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire (MMQ). It
was agreed upon that they would be contacted by telephone to
2.2. Materials and procedure provide feedback about their baseline results. After one week
the same testing procedure was followed as described for the
The Dutch version of the RAVLT was used to assess verbal patients. At baseline neither the patients nor the controls
learning, recall and recognition performances. This version of were informed about the one-week follow-up assessment of
the RAVLT consists of a list of fifteen non-associated words the word list.
that is presented orally over five trials. The total learning score The MMQ was used to assess subjective memory func-
is the sum of these five trials, with a possible score ranging tioning. In this study, we used two of its three subscales, that
from 0 to 75. The standard testing procedure involves a 30-min is, MMQ Contentment, reflecting the degree of concern and
delayed-recall test (score range: 0e15), followed by a delayed contentment regarding memory functioning (18 items, score
recognition test. In the recognition test, the 15 target words range 0e72) and MMQ Ability, assessing daily forgetfulness (20
were presented among 15 distracters (scoring range: 0e30, i.e., items, score range 0e80). The MMQ Contentment and Ability
hits þ correct rejections). subscales are positively defined, with lower scores indicating
In addition to the standard protocol, we added a long-term less memory contentment and reflecting more daily forget-
delayed recall and recognition test. In the long-term delayed fulness (Troyer & Rich, 2000; Van der Werf & Vos, 2011).
recognition condition, the original 15 distracters were
replaced with 15 alternative distracters that were chosen from 2.3. Data analysis
a parallel RAVLT version (with matching word frequencies for
the distractor items). After one week, all patients were asked First, we examined whether the groups differed on the RAVLT
to come back to discuss the results; during that visit the standard outcome measures (i.e., total number of words
additional recall and recognition tests were administered. The recalled during the 5 learning trials, the 30-min delayed recall

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
4 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7

performance and the 30-min delayed recognition) using Stu- analysis on the free-recall scores, the sphericity assumption
dent's t-tests. Subsequently, we performed a mixed-design was violated [W ¼ .658, c2 (2) ¼ 23.0, p < .0005], so Greenhouse-
GLM analysis using the recall scores of the RAVLT as within- Geisser corrections were applied. The results showed a sig-
subject factor (three levels: trial 5, 30-min and 1-week recall) nificant main effect of Time [F (1.49, 83.5) ¼ 142.1, p < .0005,
and group (patients vs controls) as between-subject factor. h2p ¼ .717], a Time  Group interaction [F (1.49, 83.46) ¼ 5.3,
Next, we performed a mixed-design GLM analysis on the p ¼ .013, h2p ¼ .086], but no main effect of Group [F (1,56) ¼ 1.4,
recognition data (two levels: 30-min and 1-week recognition), p ¼ .291, h2p ¼ .020]. Follow-up t-tests showed that while the
again with group (patient vs controls) as between-subject free recall performance did not differ between the two groups
factor. The assumption of normality of the data was exam- after 30 min (see above, d ¼ .04), the patients showed
ined using the skewness and kurtosis statistics for all vari- a worse free-recall compared to the controls after 1 week
ables and Mauchly's W was computed to examine the [t (56) ¼ 2.318, p < .024; d ¼ .61]. With respect to the recog-
sphericity assumption. nition performance (see Table 2), the GLM analysis showed a
Using the two delayed recall scores (i.e., after 30 min and significant main effect of Time [F (1, 56) ¼ 63.9, p < .0005,
one week) we calculated the percentage of information loss h2p ¼ .533], but neither a main effect of Group [F (1, 56) ¼ 1.0,
(Recall Decline) as follows: [100  (standard 30-min recall p ¼ .334, h2p ¼ .017] nor a Time  Group interaction
score e one-week recall score)/standard 30-min recall score]. [F (1, 56) ¼ .3, p ¼ .606, h2p ¼ .005].
The same formula was used with the delayed recognition For the decline scores (see Table 2), a significant group
scores for the percentages of information loss on the recog- difference was found on Recall Decline [t (56) ¼ 3.286, p < .002]
nition condition (Recognition Decline). The Recall and Recog- but not on Recognition Decline [t (56) ¼ .614, p ¼ .542], indi-
nition Decline scores as well as the MMQ scores were cating that the percentage of information loss after one week
compared across the groups using Student's t-tests. Effect was significantly higher for the patients than the controls on
sizes were computed (h2p for the GLM factors and Cohen's d for the recall measures but not on the recognition scores.
t-tests). Pearson correlation coefficients (r) were computed for With respect to the MMQ (see Table 2), three patients did
the patients and controls separately to examine the associa- not fully complete the questionnaire. MMQ analyses were
tions between the Recall and Recognition Decline measures, performed with the remaining 27 patients and all controls. No
the MMQ subscales, and the time post-event. significant differences were found between patients and
controls on MMQ Ability [t (53) ¼ .974, p ¼ .34] or MMQ
Contentment [t (56) ¼ 1.221, p ¼ .23]. A significant positive
3. Results correlation was found in the patient group between the MMQ
Ability score and Recognition Decline (r ¼ .39, p ¼ .04). Higher
Skewness and kurtosis statistics showed all variables were subjective memory ability scores were associated with more
normally distributed. The two groups did not differ signifi- long-term recognition decline. A significant negative correla-
cantly with respect to the total score on the five learning trails tion was found in the control group between the MMQ
[Mcontrol ¼ 47.9, SD ¼ 7.4; Mpatients ¼ 47.2, SD ¼ 9.5; t (56) ¼ .31, Contentment score and Recall Decline (r ¼ .39, p ¼ .04). In
p ¼ .76], the 30-min delayed recall score [t (56) ¼ .165, p ¼ .87, this group higher memory contentment scores were associ-
see Fig. 1] and the 30-min delayed recognition score ated with less decline on long-term recall. Time after stroke or
[t (56) ¼ .953, p ¼ .35, see Table 2] of the RAVLT. TIA was not significantly correlated with Recall Decline
Fig. 1 shows the RAVLT free recall scores immediately after (r ¼ .317) or Recognition Decline (r ¼ .247).
trial 5, the 30-min delay and after 1-week. For the GLM

4. Discussion

In this study we investigated whether abnormal long-term


forgetting occurs in patients with minor stroke or TIA, who
performed in the normal range on standard memory testing.
Results revealed indeed more long-term forgetting in the TIA
and minor stroke patients in comparison to a healthy control
group on free recall. The one-week recognition performance,
however, did not differ between the groups, in line with pre-
vious findings (see Geurts et al., 2015).
Cued recall (as in recognition tests) demands less cognitive
effort, less attention and less mnemonic strategies in com-
parison to free recall, making recognition tests typically less
sensitive to subtle changes (Reas & Brewer, 2013). It could be
hypothesized that in the case of subtle memory impairment
effort, attention and mnemonic strategies do not affect the
Fig. 1 e Mean number of words recalled (þS.E.M.) on the performance after 30 min, but have an increasing effect when
RAVLT after immediate testing (i.e., after trial 5), a 30-min more time has passed, possibly resulting in a sharper decline
delay and a one-week delay for the TIA and minor stroke of long-term recall abilities as compared to long-term recog-
patients and the healthy controls (max. ¼ 15); *p < .05. nition. Furthermore, recognition is thought to rely on two

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7 5

Table 2 e Results for RAVLT delayed recognition and long-term decline as well as MMQ scores.

Patients (N ¼ 30) Controls (N ¼ 28) Effect size (Cohen's d)


Mean (SD) Range Mean (SD) Range
RAVLT Delayed Recognition
Recognition 30 min 29.1 (.9) 27e30 29.3 (1.1) 27e30 .20
Recognition 1 week 26.7 (2.9) 19e30 27.2 (2.0) 23e30 .20
RAVLT decline scores (% long-term decline; 1-week vs 30-min)
Free recall 60.4 (28.9) 0e100 30.8 (39.3) 116.7e81.8 .86*
Recognition 8.4 (8.5) 3.45e34.5 7.2 (6.2) 3.7e20 .16
MMQ (N ¼ 27)
Ability 60.7 (10.7) 35e78 58.2 (8.4) 42e75 .26
Contentment 54.3 (12.2) 31e72 50.5 (10.9) 28e71 .33

RAVLT ¼ Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Dutch version); MMQ ¼ Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire; *p < .05.

distinct processes, that is, familiarity (a sense of having seen There are some limitations that should be addressed. First,
an item before) and recollection (remembering that a partic- we analyzed the minor stroke and TIA patients together, as the
ular item have been previously seen, as well as the context in sample size was too small to perform subgroup analyses.
which it appeared) (Merkowa, Burke, & Kahanac, 2015). These Future studies should include a larger group so that TIA and
processes combined may also have facilitated the recognition minor stroke patients can be analyzed separately, to investigate
performance after 1 week, resulting in a performance similar to what extent each diagnosis contributes to long-term forget-
to controls in the patients. Alternatively, this discrepancy may ting. Also, we only included patients below 75 years of age, as a
also be the result of a retrieval deficits due to subtle executive result of which the ALF cannot be generalized to minor stroke at
impairments (Unsworth, 2015; Davidson, Troyer, & older age. Future studies should also investigate the clinical
Moscovitch, 2005). significance of ALF in minor stroke and TIA, for example using a
Furthermore, we investigated the association between longitudinal approach, to identify those who are at risk for
subjective memory complaints and the actual memory per- further cognitive decline. Furthermore it would be interesting to
formance. The results demonstrated comparable levels of relate ALF in patients with TIA or minor stroke to neuroimaging
memory contentment and complaints of forgetfulness in the findings, for instance diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI), as
patient group compared to the controls. In addition, the as- DWI abnormalities have been found in minor stroke and TIA
sociations between subjective memory functioning and the patients (Brazzelli et al., 2014; Kate et al., 2015; Schulz, Briley,
memory performances were in general low. However, the two Meagher, Molyneux, & Rothwell, 2003; Van Rooij et al., 2017).
significant correlations are interesting. That is, patients who In sum, this study is the first to investigate long-term
reported less memory complaints demonstrated more forget- forgetting after cerebrovascular disease, showing that accel-
ting over time on the recognition task. In contrast, healthy erated forgetting may be a more sensitive measure in detecting
participants who were more content with their memory per- memory difficulties after minor stroke and TIA than traditional
formance showed less recall decline over time. This suggests memory assessment. Based on our results, the delay interval in
that healthy controls may be better at rating their own memory testing in clinical practice (typically between 15 and
memory functioning in comparison to the patients. Only a few 30 min) may be too short. This is also in agreement with
previous studies have addressed subjective cognitive func- neurobiological findings on episodic memory formation. That
tioning in minor stroke and TIA. Two studies reported the is, there is evidence that consolidation takes place over days
presence of memory complaints after minor stroke or TIA and weeks rather than minutes, possibly mediated by sleep
(Dassonville et al., 2016; Fens et al., 2013), while one study (Chambers, 2017; Dudai, Karni, & Born, 2015). For instance, ALF
reported that TIA patients did not experience cognitive com- for context location was found in thalamic stroke patients after
plaints (Van Rooij et al., 2014). In (major) stroke, subjective 24 h, despite normal context memory performances after one
cognitive functioning has been studied more extensively, but hour (Tu, Miller, Piguet, & Hornberger, 2014).
conflicting results on the association between subjective Future neuroimaging studies combined with more exten-
memory complaints and performances on memory tests have sive cognitive testing (also including measures of executive
been found (e.g., Van Rijsbergen, Mark, de Kort, & Sitskoorn, function and mental effort) may provide further insights into
2014). A lack of insight or emotional distress are possible the underlying mechanisms of ALF in TIA and minor stroke
causes. An alternative explanation for this discrepancy is that patients.
the subtle memory decline after one week may not have
resulted in noticeable daily problems in our sample, especially
given the relatively short time since the vascular event for Acknowledgements
most of the patients. Both assumptions could contribute to the
lack of concordance in this study and even explain why pa- The authors would like to thank the BAC team consisting of
tients who reported less complaints showed more long-term I. Bekker, J. Kramer, and B. Notz for their assistance in the data
forgetting on recognition. collection.

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
6 c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7

references the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A, 112, 14378e14383.


https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1513145112.
Muhlert, N., Grünewald, R. A., Hunkin, N. M., Reuber, M.,
Howell, S., Reynders, H., et al. (2011). Accelerated long-term
Brazzelli, M., Chappell, F. M., Miranda, H., Shuler, K., Dennis, M.,
forgetting in temporal lobe but not idiopathic generalised
Sandercock, P. A. G., et al. (2014). Diffusion-weighted imaging
epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2417e2426.
and diagnosis of ischemic attack. Annals of Neurology, 75,
Muhlert, N., Milton, F., Butler, C. R., Kapur, N., & Zeman, A. Z.
67e76. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24026.
(2010). Accelerated forgetting of real-life events in transient
Butler, C. R., Graham, K. S., Hodges, J. R., Kapur, N., Wardlaw, J. M.,
epileptic amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3235e3244. https://
& Zeman, A. Z. J. (2007). The syndrome of transient epileptic
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.001.
amnesia. Annals of Neurology, 61, 587e598. https://doi.org/
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
10.1002/ana.21111.
Committee. (1990). Special report from the national institute
Butler, C. R., & Zeman, A. Z. (2008). Recent insight into the
of neurological disorders and stroke: Classification of
impairment of memory in epilepsy: Transient epileptic
cerebrovascular diseases III. Stroke, 21, 637e676. https://
amnesia, accelerated long-term forgetting and remote
doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.21.4.637.
memory impairment. Brain, 131, 2243e2263. https://doi.org/
Reas, E. T., & Brewer, J. B. (2013). Retrieval search and strength
10.1093/brain/awn127.
evoke dissociable brain activity during episodic memory
Cassel, A., Morris, R., Koutroumanidis, M., & Kopelman, M. (2016).
recall. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 219e233. https://
Forgetting in temporal lobe epilepsy: When does it become
doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00335.
accelerated? Cortex, 78, 70e84. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Schulz, U. G. R., Briley, D., Meagher, T., Molyneux, A., &
j.cortex.2016.02.005.
Rothwell, P. M. (2003). Abnormalities on diffusion weighted
Chambers, A. M. (2017). The role of sleep in cognitive processing:
magnetic resonance imaging performed several weeks after a
Focusing on memory consolidation. Wiley Interdisciplinary
minor stroke or transient ischaemic attack. Journal of
Reviews Cognitive Science, e1433. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 74, 734e738. https://
wcs.1433.
doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.6.734.
Dassonville, P., Nash, S., Servajean, V., Sanchez, M., Mazza, S.,
Snaith, R. P. (2003). The hospital anxiety and depression scale.
Colliot, P., et al. (2016). Cognitive impairments and impact on
Health and Quality of Life Outcome, 1, 29. https://doi.org/10.1186/
activities of daily living after minor stroke. Annals of Physical
1477-7525-1-29.
and Rehabilitation Medicine, 59S, e67ee79. https://doi.org/
Sulter, G., Steen, C., & de Keyser, J. (1990). Use of the Barthel index
10.1016/j.rehab.2016.07.165.
and modified Rankin Scale in acute stroke trials. Stroke, 30,
Davidson, P. S. R., Troyer, A. K., & Moscovitch, M. (2005). Frontal
1538e1541.
lobe contributors to recognition and recall: Linking basic
Troyer, A. K., & Rich, J. B. (2000). Psychometric properties of a new
research with clinical evaluation and remediation. Journal of
metamemory questionnaire for older adults. Journals of
the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 210e223. https://
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, 19e27.
doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060334.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/57.1.P19.
Dudai, Y., Karni, A., & Born, J. (2015). The consolidation and
Tu, S., Miller, L., Piguet, O., & Hornberger, M. (2014). Accelerated
transformation of memory. Neuron, 88, 20e32. https://doi.org/
forgetting of contextual details due to focal medio-dorsal
10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004.
thalamic lesion. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 320.
Elliot, G., Isaac, C. L., & Muhlert, N. (2014). Measuring forgetting: A
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00320.
critical review of accelerated long-term forgetting studies.
Unsworth, N. (2015). Working memory capacity and recall from
Cortex, 54, 16e32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.001.
long-term memory: Examining the influences of encoding
Fens, M., van Heugten, C. M., Beusmans, G. H. M. I., Limburg, M.,
strategies, study time allocation, search efficiency, and
Haeren, R., Kaemingk, A., et al. (2013). Not as transient:
monitoring abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42,
Patients with transient ischaemic attack or minor stroke
50e61. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000148.
experience cognitive and communication problems: An
Van Rijsbergen, M. W., Mark, R. E., de Kort, P. L., &
exploratory study. European Journal of General Practice, 19,
Sitskoorn, M. M. (2014). Subjective cognitive complaints after
11e16. https://doi.org/10.3109/13814788.2012.715147.
stroke: A systematic review. Journal of Stroke and
Geurts, S., van der Werf, S. P., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2015). An
Cerebrovascular Diseases, 23, 408e420. https://doi.org/10.1016/
evaluation on the use of long-term forgetting rates in patients
j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2013.05.003.
with memory problems. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 752. https://  raj, B. M.,
Van Rooij, F. G., Plaizier, N. O., Vermeer, S. E., Go
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00752.
Koudstaal, P. J., Richard, E., et al. (2017). Executive function
Hung, C. I., Liu, C. Y., Wang, S. J., Yao, Y. C., & Yang, C. H. (2012).
declines in the first 6 months after a transient ischemic attack
The cut-off points of the Depression and Somatic Symptoms
or transient neurological attack. Stroke, 48, 3323e3328. https://
Scale and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale in
doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.018298.
detecting non-full remission and a current major depressive
Van Rooij, F. G., Schaapsmeerders, P., Maaijwee, N. A. M., van
episode. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 16,
Duijnhoven, D. A. H. J., De Leeuw, F. E., Kessels, R. P. C., et al.
33e40. https://doi.org/10.3109/13651501.2011.617456.
(2014). Persistent cognitive impairment after transient
Kate, M. P., Riaz, P., Gioia, L., Sivakumar, L., Jeerakathil, T., Buck, B.,
ischemic attack. Stroke, 45, 2270e2274. https://doi.org/10.1161/
et al. (2015). Dynamic evolution of diffusion-weighted imaging
STROKEAHA.114.005205.
lesions in patients with minor ischemic stroke. Stroke, 45,
Van der Elst, W., van Boxtel, M. P. J., van Breukelen, J. P., &
2318e2321. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.009775.
Jolles, J. (2005). Rey's verbal learning test: Normative data
Mandzia, J. L., Smith, E. E., Horton, M., Hanly, P., Barber, P. A.,
for 1855 healthy participants aged 24-81 years and the
Godzwon, C., et al. (2016). Imaging and baseline predictors of
influence of age, sex, education, and mode of
cognitive performance in minor ischemic stroke and
presentation. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
patients with transient ischemic attack at 90 days. Stroke, 47,
Society, 11, 290e302. https://doi.org/10.1017/
726e731. https://doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.011507.
S1355617705050344.
Merkowa, M. B., Burke, J. F., & Kahanac, M. J. (2015). The human
Van der Werf, S. P., Geurts, S., & de Werd, M. M. W. (2016).
hippocampus contributes to both the recollection and
Subjective memory ability and long-term forgetting in
familiarity components of recognition memory. Proceedings of

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002
c o r t e x x x x ( 2 0 1 8 ) 1 e7 7

patients referred for neuropsychological assessment. Frontiers Zamboni, G., Griffanti, L., Jenkinson, M., Mazzucco, S., Li, L.,
in Psychology, 7, 605. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00605. Küker, W., et al., Oxford Vascular Study. (2017). White matter
Van der Werf, S. P., & Vos, S. H. (2011). Memory worries and self- imaging correlates of early cognitive impairment detected by
reported daily forgetfulness: A psychometric evaluation of the the Montreal Cognitive Assessment after transient ischemic
Dutch translation of the multifactorial memory questionnaire. attack and minor stroke. Stroke, 48, 1539e1547. https://doi.org/
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25, 244e268. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 10.1161/STROKEAHA.116.016044.
13854046.2010.543290.

Please cite this article in press as: Geurts, S., et al., Accelerated long-term forgetting after TIA or minor stroke: A more sensitive measure
for detecting subtle memory dysfunction? Cortex (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.04.002

You might also like