Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

913192

editorial2020
AUT0010.1177/1362361320913192AutismEditorial

Editorial

Autism

Both sex- and gender-related factors 2020, Vol. 24(3) 539­–543


© The Author(s) 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
should be considered in autism research sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1362361320913192
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320913192

and clinical practice journals.sagepub.com/home/aut

Recently, there has been increasing attention to key inter- women may focus on social information, social learning
sections between sex, gender and autism (Lai et al., 2015), and efforts towards compensatory social coping, which
including the so-called ‘female autism phenotype’ (Mandy may come at a cost in terms of fatigue and poorer mental
et al., 2012), social ‘camouflaging’ (Hull et al., 2017) and health (Hull et al., 2020). The experience of compensatory
experiences of gender diversity in autistic people (van der social coping (e.g. trying to act as ‘non-autistic’) has been
Miesen et al., 2016). This research and related clinical described by some as ‘camouflaging’ or ‘masking’ (Hull
practice have increased awareness of the breadth of autis- et al., 2017); however, these terms are not universally
tic experience. However, sex and gender often have been embraced in the autism advocacy communities. Overall,
conflated in this work, and gender viewed through a binary these lines of research point to multiple factors associated
lens. The term ‘gender’ refers to a range of experiences, with the male preponderance of autism diagnosis preva-
both internal and external (Becker et al., 2017; Ehrtmann lence and patterns of underidentification of autism in girls
et al., 2019), and the intricate interplay between social and women.
gender and inner gender-related experiences has yet to be
articulated in autism (Cooper et al., 2018). In this editorial,
written as a collaboration between clinician researchers
Sex and gender as separate,
and autistic advocates (Fletcher-Watson et al., 2019), we multi-component dimensions
explore key topics in the field and offer recommendations Patterns of differences between females and males, girls and
for advancing this work through more diversified, sex- and boys, and women and men in autism are clearly important
gender-informed lenses, and with a focus on the lived and relevant for research and clinical practice, but current
experiences of autistic people. approaches to this work will be enhanced with a broader
focus on the various facets of gender, as opposed to sex
alone. For example, gendered expectations and social con-
The ‘female autism phenotype’ ditioning, beyond sex-related biological mechanisms, play a
Increased attention to expressions of autism in girls and role in shaping the individual development of autistic girls
women has led to descriptions of a putative ‘female autism and women (Lai & Szatmari, 2020). According to the World
phenotype’ (Hull et al., 2020). Typically, these descrip- Health Organization, sex refers to biological and physiolog-
tions are presented as contrasts to common expressions of ical characteristics, which include sex-related chromosomes
autism in boys and men (Lai et al., 2015). For example, it (http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/health-determi-
has been noted that many autistic girls and women have nants/gender/gender-definitions); sex is often perceived as a
fewer signs of repetitive behaviours and narrow interests binary construct, but in fact, its components can be multi-
than autistic boys and men, and when narrow interests are categorical or dimensional (Joel & McCarthy, 2017).
present, they may be less immediately obvious (Lai & Gender encompasses experiential, social and cultural com-
Szatmari, 2020). There is emerging evidence that individu- ponents including ‘gender norms’ (Barnett, 1997), ‘gender
als with XX sex chromosomes, the sex chromosomes roles’ (Witt, 1997), ‘gender-related interests’ (Davis &
most women have, may be less likely to express character- Hines, 2020; Dittmann et al., 1990; Fast & Olson, 2018),
istics commonly understood to be autistic (the so-called ‘gender-related expressions’ (Becker et al., 2017; Toomey
‘female protective effect’; Werling, 2016), although such et al., 2010) and ‘gender identity’ (i.e. the internal gender
effect does not seem to come from the X chromosome per that a person experiences themselves as; American
se. Autistic girls and women appear to have overall Psychological Association, 2015). Autism-related charac-
stronger social motivation compared to autistic boys and teristics may facilitate greater freedom regarding social gen-
men (Sedgewick et al., 2016, 2019); this could be der roles and societal gender expectations; autistic
driven by female sex-specific neurodevelopmental fac- individuals may feel less pressure to conform to such roles
tors (Chawarska et al., 2016; Werling, 2016) and the (Strang et al., 2018). This may allow for increased expres-
greater social gender expectations placed on girls and sion of gender diversity, such as with gender expressions,
women (Kreiser & White, 2014). Some autistic girls and interests and identities. There is substantial evidence that
540 Autism 24(3)

many autistic individuals experience an incongruence meanings of sex and gender in the lives of autistic people
between assigned sex and gender identity (van der Miesen will require an autism-informed framework. Some autis-
et al., 2016). In a recent Dutch study, around 15% of autistic tic people may not communicate about gender-related
adults reported a gender identity different from their sex experiences and needs in ways that are immediately obvi-
assigned at birth (Walsh et al., 2018). The associations of ous (Strang et al., 2018). And for some autistic people,
autism and gender diversity characteristics have also been understanding and integrating abstract social gender
found in a large general population sample of school-age norms and expectations with inner experiences may be
children in Canada (Nabbijohn et al., 2019). Community challenging, for example, ‘what is the difference between
expressions of gender diversity in autism are common. For a boy or a girl?’ (Strang et al., 2019) or ‘understanding
example, the Autistic Women’s Network recently renamed women [my own sex] doesn’t come naturally to me. I
itself the Autistic Women & Nonbinary Network in response needed a book to explain it to me’. (Griffin, 2016). So, to
to its constituency’s gender diversity (Allison, 2018). We better capture gender in autism, it will be important to
still do not know the various gender developmental trajecto- consider those autistic qualities that may affect (1) others’
ries across the lifespan in autistic individuals and how they understanding of the gender-related experiences and
intersect with evolving gender narratives and conceptualiza- needs of the autistic individual and (2) the autistic indi-
tions in societies and cultures. vidual’s own perceptions and comprehension of gender-
related meanings (e.g. how the autistic individual relates
to the neurotypical ‘gendered’ world).
The importance of nonbinary
conceptualizations of gender
Experiences of gender-related
There is an increasing recognition of the prevalence of
gender experiences that fall outside of the ‘male’ and
expectations in autism
‘female’ binary categories (Vincent, 2019), including Amid the complex process of reconciling social gender
experiences of having components of both, neither or flu- norms with one’s inner experiences, some autistic individ-
idity of the binary genders. Emerging evidence suggests uals may feel unique pressure to conform to gender-related
that nonbinary experiences and identities may be particu- expectations. Social intervention approaches often teach
larly common in autism (Dewinter et al., 2017; Stagg & relational skills as a set of fixed rules, which may reinforce
Vincent, 2019; Walsh et al., 2018). We currently have no common gender stereotypes about how a person of a cer-
validated means of measuring nonbinary gender experi- tain sex/gender should act (Tint et al., 2018; Tint & Weiss,
ences in autism, and there is limited information on nonbi- 2017). In particular, compliance-based teaching, in which
nary autistic individuals’ gender-related needs, experiences specific social behaviours are rewarded, while actions
or mental health. Community-based accounts of gender driven by internal guiding factors or motivations are not,
development in autism suggest that societal expectations may contribute to strong adherence to stereotypical gender
for binary gender expressions may lead some autistic peo- norms and expectations, even if they are incongruent with
ple to feel uncomfortable with their gender (Griffin, 2016). inner experience and needs (Jack, 2014). Gender stereo-
types might also be absorbed by autistic people through
the process of ‘camouflaging’ (i.e. internalizing gendered
Understanding gender in autism social conventions to mask differences; Hull et al., 2017).
With the tendency to focus on differences in self-aware- Gendered pressures resulting from structured social inter-
ness in some autistic people (Huang et al., 2017), there vention approaches (many of them being adopted as stand-
has been a general lack of attention to autistic people’s ard care for autistic people currently) or ‘camouflaging’
inner experiences (Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017) or, in might be harmful to autistic people in some instances
some cases, even acknowledgement that autistic people (Cassidy et al., 2019). Although some autistic people can
have an inner experience. Enhancing curiosity about inner overcome this pressure, other autistics may struggle to
experiences in autism is critical as it pertains to issues challenge gender expectations, even if they experience
related to sex and gender. Gender includes elements that these expectations as repressive (Griffin, 2016).
are deeply personal – that another person would not
understand or know without direct communication. And Sex- and gender-related
there are risks (for research, clinical care, personal free- considerations in the identification of
doms) associated with (1) incorrect assumptions about
autism
sex and gender (e.g. concerning a person’s gender iden-
tity, traumatic experiences, comfort with gendered roles) Characteristics that have been associated with a ‘female’
or (2) mistrust of the voiced experience of autistic people versus ‘male’ version of autism are not isolated to one sex
who express gender-related characteristics and needs that or gender. The processes of ‘camouflaging’, though
diverge from the ‘average’ experience. Understanding the described in many ‘female experiences’ of autism (Hull
Editorial 541

et al., 2020) and reportedly more intense overall in autistic who may or may not find their own life journeys
women, are present also in autistic men and nonbinary concordant with them. Do they create new expecta-
autistic adults (Hull et al., 2019). Similar processes have tions and stereotypes that will need to be decon-
been described outside of autism research, such as among structed and contextualized? And how is the
gender-diverse people and LGBTQ+ people more broadly, specific language developed so far experienced by
who may learn to ‘pass’, ‘code-switch’ or ‘blend in’ for autistic people (e.g. ‘female protective effect’)?
social or safety reasons (Fuller et al., 2009; Spradlin, When new terms are coined, we must work with
1998). The sex-based phenotypic descriptions as they per- the autistic communities to both understand the
tain to autism identification and diagnostics, as well as tai- lived experiences underlying the concepts and
loring of accommodations and supports, have insufficiently avoid gender and/or autism-related stereotypes
integrated gender as an independent and critical second set which may be demeaning or lacking in precision.
of variables (Kreiser & White, 2014; Lai & Szatmari, 3. The lived experiences of autistic people who
2020). Consider, for example, an assigned male at birth express distance from gender conventions or stere-
with female gender identity who has affirmed (formerly otypes and/or their sex assigned at birth reflect a
known as ‘transitioned to’) a female gender role. How phenomenon that is more than just ‘social aware-
does an informed clinician consider the question of autism ness differences’. The ability of some autistic peo-
in such a gender-diverse individual? What gender norms ple to exist more readily outside of social gen-
are to be used on normed autism screening tools (e.g. the der expectations and boldly express their inner
Social Responsiveness Scale)? Is the emerging putative gender needs has already positively impacted gen-
‘female autism phenotype’ to be expected and impactful der-related social justice, as many gender diversity
on diagnostic considerations? And what of the autistic cis- advocates are autistic (e.g. Abeni, 2015; Brown,
gender boy who is subthreshold on so-called ‘gold-stand- 2016; Callahan, 2018; McCool, 2017; National
ard’ autism diagnostic measures, possibly due to factors Autistic Society, 2018). Take for example, Martine
related to camouflaging? If researchers and clinicians are Stonehouse, an autistic transgender woman who
broadening their understanding of what autism looks like drew upon her gender diversity and autistic neuro-
in girls and women based on emerging studies that support divergence to help change the law in Canada to pro-
such conceptualization, should the same be applied to vide gender-related medical interventions for
boys, men or gender-diverse people who embody the puta- transgender people (Alon, 2016). Research should
tive ‘female autism phenotype’ as well? focus not only on the differences in autism as ‘chal-
lenges’ or ‘symptoms’ but also on the ways in which
Initial recommendations for autistic characteristics and their overlap with gen-
der diversity may help to advance the understand-
embracing a sex- and gender- ing of gender. And clearly, respect should be
informed framework in autism afforded to autistic people who are experiencing
research and clinical practice and/or exploring gender diversity. We increasingly
1. Sex and gender should be included and measured as provide neurotypical youth the room to explore and
separate constructs, comprising multiple compo- express their gender identities, and autistic people
nents that can be operationalized as continuous or deserve the same rights and opportunities.
multi-categorical variables. New, autism-friendly
measures need to be developed, and these measures
Conclusions
should capture both binary and nonbinary experi-
ences of gender. Assessment and diagnosis should Autistic accounts of gender experiences and gender diver-
also consider the impact of the gendered world and sity encourage greater attention to the ways in which sex
gendered social milieus on autistic people (e.g. how and gender in autism must be examined separately and
are social and cultural gender norms and expecta- dimensionally, and through autism-informed characteriza-
tions experienced by autistic people and how do tion methods. Such work will allow for better understand-
these experiences impact them developmentally?) ing of how autistic people experience gender, and how this
2. Concepts such as the ‘female autism phenotype’, impacts their experiences of being autistic. As a field, we
‘camouflaging’ and ‘female protective effect’ are just beginning to recognize the inner experiences of
reflect important findings and constructs that autistic people. With regard to sex and gender, such recog-
advance the field, but the terms and conceptualiza- nition is important for science as well as for the respect of
tions will be enriched with the engagement of the individual, and in some cases may be lifesaving (White,
autistic people in defining the fullness of the con- 2016). Inner experience is too commonly forgotten in
cepts. Further, we need to understand how these autism research and clinical practice. Yet, autistic people
concepts are experienced by autistic individuals who are less attached to, aware of or concerned with
542 Autism 24(3)

gender conventions may in fact be providing an important Sexual Behavior, 49, 373–394. https://doi.org/10.1007/
window into what gender is and can be without the strict s10508-019-01624-7
overlays of gender-related expectations and social mean- Dewinter, J., De Graaf, H., & Begeer, S. (2017). Sexual orienta-
ings. Some autistic people may be seeing beyond the con- tion, gender identity, and romantic relationships in adoles-
cents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
ventions that bind neurotypical genders and expressions.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(9), 2927–2934.
We encourage an openness to what autistic people have to
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3199-9
tell us and teach us about gender – about their own gender Dittmann, R. W., Kappes, M. H., Kappes, M. E., Borger, D.,
experiences and journeys, and maybe even about the ways Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Stegner, H., . . . Wallis, H. (1990).
the world can open to a more expansive view and accept- Congenital adrenal hyperplasia. II: Gender-related behavior
ance of gender-related experiences, less confined by neu- and attitudes in female salt-wasting and simple-virilizing
rotypical social and gender codes. patients. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15(5-6), 421–434.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4530(90)90066-i
Ehrtmann, L., Wolter, I., & Hannover, B. (2019). The interrelat-
References edness of gender-stereotypical interest profiles and students’
Abeni, C. (2015, November 21). Watch: D.C. trans activist gender-role orientation, gender, and reasoning abilities.
arrested on trans day of resilience. Advocate. https://www. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1402. https://doi.org/10.3389/
advocate.com/transgender/2015/11/21/watch-dc-trans- fpsyg.2019.01402
activist-arrested-trans-day-resilience Fast, A. A., & Olson, K. R. (2018). Gender development in
Allison, C. (2018). Autistic Women & Nonbinary Network transgender preschool children. Child Development, 89(2),
(AWN). https://awnnetwork.org/ 620–637. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12758
Alon, K. (Writer). (2016). Transfixed. https://www.youtube. Fletcher-Watson, S., Adams, J., Brook, K., Charman, T., Crane,
com/watch?v=Oz9S5yjzTGc L., Cusack, J., . . . Pellicano, E. (2019). Making the future
American Psychological Association. (2015). Guidelines for together: Shaping autism research through meaningful par-
psychological practice with transgender and gender noncon- ticipation. Autism, 23(4), 943–953. https://doi.org/10.1177
forming people. American Psychologist, 70(9), 832–864. /1362361318786721
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039906 Fuller, C. B., Chang, D. F., & Rubin, L. R. (2009). Sliding
Barnett, B. (1997). Gender norms affect adolescents. Network under the radar: Passing and power among sexual minori-
(Research Triangle Park, N C), 17(3), 10–13. ties. Journal of LGBT Issues in Counseling, 3(2), 128–151.
Becker, I., Ravens-Sieberer, U., Ottova-Jordan, V., & Schulte- https://doi.org/10.1080/15538600903005334
Markwort, M. (2017). Prevalence of adolescent gender Gillespie-Lynch, K., Kapp, S. K., Brooks, P. J., Pickens,
experiences and gender expression in Germany. Journal of J., & Schwartzman, B. (2017). Whose expertise is it?
Adolescent Health, 61(1), 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Evidence for autistic adults as critical autism experts.
jadohealth.2017.02.001 Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 438. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Brown, E. (Producer). (2016). Martine Stonehouse oral history. fpsyg.2017.00438
Digital Transgender Archive. https://www.digitaltransgen- Griffin, A. (2016). Gender identity issues and females on the
derarchive.net/files/pk02c9792 spectrum. https://the-art-of-autism.com/gender-identity
Callahan, M. (2018). The autistic, non-binary, queer, law stu- -issues-and-females-on-the-spectrum/
dent fighting for disability justice. Northeastern University. Huang, A. X., Hughes, T. L., Sutton, L. R., Lawrence, M.,
https://news.northeastern.edu/2018/04/19/the-autistic-non- Chen, X., Ji, Z., & Zeleke, W. (2017). Understanding the
binary-queer-law-student-fighting-for-disability-justice/ self in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD):
Cassidy, S. A., Gould, K., Townsend, E., Pelton, M., Robertson, A review of literature. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1422.
A. E., & Rodgers, J. (2019). Is camouflaging autistic https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01422
traits associated with suicidal thoughts and behaviours? Hull, L., Lai, M. C., Baron-Cohen, S., Allison, C., Smith,
Expanding the interpersonal psychological theory of sui- P., Petrides, K. V., & Mandy, W. (2019). Gender dif-
cide in an undergraduate student sample. Journal of Autism ferences in self-reported camouflaging in autistic and
and Developmental Disorders. Advance online publication. non-autistic adults. Autism, 24(2), 352–363. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3 org/10.1177/1362361319864804
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., Powell, K., DiNicola, L., & Shic, Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., Allison, C., Smith, P., Baron-Cohen,
F. (2016). Enhanced social attention in female infant sib- S., Lai, M. C., & Mandy, W. (2017). ‘Putting on my best
lings at risk for autism. Journal of the American Academy normal’: Social camouflaging in adults with autism spec-
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(3), 188.e1–195.e1. trum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.11.016 Disorders, 47(8), 2519–2534. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Cooper, K., Smith, L. G. E., & Russell, A. J. (2018). Gender s10803-017-3166-5
identity in autism: Sex differences in social affiliation Hull, L., Petrides, K. V., & Mandy, W. (2020). The female
with gender groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental autism phenotype and camouflaging: A narrative review.
Disorders, 48(12), 3995–4006. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
s10803-018-3590-1 Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Davis, J. T. M., & Hines, M. (2020). How large are gender s40489-020-00197-9
differences in toy preferences? A systematic review and Jack, J. (2014). Autism and gender: From refrigerator mothers to
meta-analysis of toy preference research. Archives of computer geeks. University of Illinois Press.
Editorial 543

Joel, D., & McCarthy, M. M. (2017). Incorporating sex as a bio- in Pediatric Psychology, 7(4), 396–404. https://doi.
logical variable in neuropsychiatric research: Where are we org/10.1037/cpp0000310
now and where should we be? Neuropsychopharmacology, Strang, J. F., Powers, M. D., Knauss, M., Sibarium, E.,
42(2), 379–385. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.79 Leibowitz, S. F., Kenworthy, L., .  .  . Anthony, L. G. (2018).
Kreiser, N. L., & White, S. W. (2014). ASD in females: Are we “They thought it was an obsession”: Trajectories and per-
overstating the gender difference in diagnosis? Clinical spectives of autistic transgender and gender-diverse ado-
Child and Family Psychology Review, 17(1), 67–84. https:// lescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
doi.org/10.1007/s10567-013-0148-9 48(12), 4039–4055. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018
Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., -3723-6
& Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Sex/gender differences and Tint, A., Hamdani, Y., Sawyer, A., Desarkar, P., Ameis, S. H.,
autism: Setting the scene for future research. Journal of Bardikoff, N., & Lai, M. C. (2018). Wellness efforts for
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, autistic women. Current Developmental Disorders Reports,
54(1), 11–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.003 5(4), 207–216. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-018-0148-z
Lai, M. C., & Szatmari, P. (2020). Sex and gender impacts on Tint, A., & Weiss, J. A. (2017). A qualitative study of the service
the behavioural presentation and recognition of autism. experiences of women with autism spectrum disorder. Autism,
Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 33(2), 117–123. https://doi. 22(8), 928–937. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317702561
org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000575 Toomey, R. B., Ryan, C., Diaz, R. M., Card, N. A., & Russell,
Mandy, W., Chilvers, R., Chowdhury, U., Salter, G., Seigal, A., S. T. (2010). Gender-nonconforming lesbian, gay, bisexual,
& Skuse, D. (2012). Sex differences in autism spectrum dis- and transgender youth: School victimization and young
order: Evidence from a large sample of children and adoles- adult psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology,
cents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 46(6), 1580–1589. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020705
1304–1313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0 van der Miesen, A. I. R., Hurley, H., & de Vries, A. L. C. (2016).
McCool, M. A. (2017). Passing cars: The internal monologue Gender dysphoria and autism spectrum disorder: A narra-
of a neurodivergent trans girl. CreateSpace Independent tive review. International Review of Psychiatry, 28(1), 70–
Publishing Platform. 80. https://doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2015.1111199
Nabbijohn, A. N., van der Miesen, A. I. R., Santarossa, A., Vincent, B. (2019). Breaking down barriers and binaries in
Peragine, D., de Vries, A. L. C., Popma, A., .  .  . VanderLaan, trans healthcare: The validation of non-binary people.
D. P. (2019). Gender variance and the autism spectrum: An International Journal of Transgenderism, 20(2-3), 132–137.
examination of children ages 6-12 years. Journal of Autism Walsh, R. J., Krabbendam, L., Dewinter, J., & Begeer, S. (2018).
and Developmental Disorders, 49(4), 1570–1585. https:// Brief report: Gender identity differences in autistic adults:
doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3843-z Associations with perceptual and socio-cognitive profiles.
National Autistic Society. (2018). Stories from the spectrum: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(12),
Wenn Lawson. https://www.autism.org.uk/about/stories/ 4070–4078. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3702-y
wenn-lawson.aspx Werling, D. M. (2016). The role of sex-differential biology in risk
Sedgewick, F., Hill, V., & Pellicano, E. (2019). ‘It’s different for for autism spectrum disorder. Biology of Sex Differences, 7,
girls’: Gender differences in the friendships and conflict of 58. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-016-0112-8
autistic and neurotypical adolescents. Autism, 23(5), 1119– White, B. (2016, November 15). The link between autism and
1132. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318794930 trans identity. The Atlantic. https://www.theatlantic.com/
Sedgewick, F., Hill, V., Yates, R., Pickering, L., & Pellicano, health/archive/2016/11/the-link-between-autism-and-trans-
E. (2016). Gender differences in the social motivation and identity/507509/
friendship experiences of autistic and non-autistic adoles- Witt, S. D. (1997). Parental influence on children’s socialization
cents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), to gender roles. Adolescence, 32(126), 253–259.
1297–1306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2669-1
Spradlin, A. L. (1998). The price of ‘passing’: A lesbian per- John F Strang1,2, Anna IR van der Miesen3, Reid
spective on authenticity in organizations. Management Caplan4, Cat Hughes5, Sharon daVanport6
Communication Quarterly, 11(4), 598–605. https://doi. and Meng-Chuan Lai7,8
org/10.1177/0893318998114006 1
Children’s National Hospital, USA
Stagg, S. D., & Vincent, J. (2019). Autistic traits in individuals self- 2
The George Washington University, USA
defining as transgender or nonbinary. European Psychiatry, 3
61, 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.06.003 Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands
4
Strang, J. F., Klomp, S. E., Caplan, R., Griffin, A. D., Anthony, Autistic Self Advocacy Network, USA
5
L. G., Harris, M. C., . . . van der Miesen, A. I. R. (2019). Autistica, UK
6
Community-based participatory design for research that Autistic Women & Nonbinary Network (AWN), USA
7
impacts the lives of transgender and/or gender-diverse University of Toronto, Canada
8
autistic and/or neurodiverse people. Clinical Practice Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Canada

You might also like