Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232736455

Efficacy of interventions to improve motor


performance in children with developmental
coordination disorder: A ....

Article in Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology · October 2012


DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12008 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

130 1,056

7 authors, including:

Bouwien Smits-Engelsman Helene J Polatajko


University of Cape Town University of Toronto
206 PUBLICATIONS 4,891 CITATIONS 258 PUBLICATIONS 6,811 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Peter H Wilson
Australian Catholic University
131 PUBLICATIONS 3,572 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Development of a screening test battery for talent identification of adolescent rugby players in
Zimbabwe. The SCRuM Project View project

Neck-project / Radboud University Holland View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Bouwien Smits-Engelsman on 13 January 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY REVIEW

Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in children


with developmental coordination disorder: a combined systematic
review and meta-analysis
BOUWIEN C M SMITS-ENGELSMAN 1 , 2 | RAINER BLANK 3 | ANNE-CLAIRE VAN DER KAAY 2 | RIANNE
MOSTERD-VAN DER MEIJS 2 | ELLEN VLUGT-VAN DEN BRAND 2 | HELENE J POLATAJKO 4 | PETER H WILSON 5

1 Department of Kinesiology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium. 2 Avansplus University for Professionals, Breda, the Netherlands. 3 University of Heidelberg and Child Centre,
Maulbronn, Germany. 4 Department of Occupational Science and Occupational Therapy, University of Toronto, Canada. 5 School of Psychology, Australian Catholic University,
Melbourne, Australia.
Correspondence to Dr Bouwien Smits-Engelsman at Department of Kinesiology, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101, 3001 Heverlee, Leuven, Belgium. E-mail: bouwiensmits@hotmail.com

PUBLICATION DATA AIM The aim of this study was to review systematically evidence about the efficacy of motor inter-
Accepted for publication 25th July 2012. ventions for children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD), and to quantify treatment
Published online. effects using meta-analysis.
METHOD Included were all studies published between 1995 and 2011 that described a systematic
ABBREVIATIONS review, (randomized) clinical trial, or crossover design about the effect of motor intervention in
CO-OP Cognitive orientation to daily occupa- children with DCD. Studies were compared on four components: design, methodological quality,
tional performance intervention components, and efficacy. Twenty-six studies met the inclusion criteria for the review.
DCD Developmental coordination disorder Interventions were coded under four types: (1) task-oriented intervention, (2) traditional physical
NTT Neuromotor task training therapy and occupational therapy, (3) process-oriented therapies, and (4) chemical supplements.
For the meta-analysis, effect sizes were available for 20 studies and their magnitude (weighted
Cohen’s d [dw]) was compared across training types.
RESULTS The overall effect size across all intervention studies was dw=0.56. A comparison
between classes of intervention showed strong effects for task-oriented intervention (dw=0.89) and
physical and occupational therapies (dw=0.83), whereas that for process-oriented intervention was
weak (dw=0.12). Of the chemical supplements, treatment with methylphenidate was researched in
three studies (dw=0.79) and supplementation of fatty acids plus vitamin E in one study (no effect).
The post hoc comparison between treatment types showed that the effect size of the task-oriented
approach was significantly higher than the process-oriented intervention (p=0.01) and comparison
(p=0.006). No significant difference in the magnitude of effect size between traditional physical
and occupational therapy approaches and any of the other interventions emerged.
INTERPRETATION In general, intervention is shown to produce benefit for the motor performance
of children with DCD, over and above no intervention. However, approaches from a task-oriented
perspective yield stronger effects. Process-oriented approaches are not recommended for improv-
ing motor performance in DCD, whereas the evidence for chemical supplements for children with
DCD is currently insufficient for a recommendation.

Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) ing school continue to have this problem 10 years later.6
are identifiable by the difficulties they have in performing fine These figures indicate that DCD is not a condition that exists
and gross motor tasks, which affect their performance in the only in earlier childhood.
classroom and in activities of daily living.1 According to the Several comorbid problems are common in children with
DSM-IV,2 in DCD the level of motor coordination is below DCD including a substantial overlap with attention-deficit–
that expected given the child’s chronological age and intelli- hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), dyslexia, and autistic spec-
gence, and may lead to problems in activities of daily living trum disorders.7,8 Children with DCD show higher rates of
and ⁄ or academic performance.3 The estimated prevalence of social difficulties, low self-esteem, and associated behavioural
children with DCD is between 6 and 13% of all school-aged problems during childhood and adolescence.9 In particular,
children,4 with some reports finding that males experience a children with combined DCD and ADHD show poorer
higher incidence than females.5 Forty per cent of the children outcomes when evaluated in early adulthood, in terms of
diagnosed as having delayed motor development before start- academic achievement and psychosocial adjustment.10 Indeed,

ª The Authors. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology ª 2012 Mac Keith Press DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12008 1
the persistent nature of DCD in around one-half of individuals What this paper adds
first diagnosed in childhood emphasizes the importance of tar- • Children with DCD benefit from some form of motor intervention.
geted intervention.11–13 • Task-oriented interventions and traditional motor-based skill training yield
Therapeutic approaches are drawn from occupational ther- significant effects.
apy,14–20 physiotherapy,21–30 medicine31–33 (e.g. methylpheni- • Task-oriented interventions improve motor performance more than process-
oriented interventions.
date), diet34 (e.g. fatty acids plus vitamin E supplementation,), • Few intervention studies capture changes in the level of participation.
and education21,35,36 (teachers, parents, physical education). • Higher-quality studies are needed on optimal intensity and treatment duration.
Although different terms can be found in the literature to
describe interventions for DCD, approaches can be grouped developed in a sort of hierarchical fashion. Basic abilities
under three main types: process-oriented, task-oriented, and (such as postural control, in-hand manipulation, visual–per-
conventional physical and occupational therapy. Chemical ceptual skills) need to be refined in conjunction with teaching
treatments (e.g. fatty acids) represent a newer, fourth type. complex motor skills.
Process-oriented approaches target the components or body Although many different intervention strategies have been
functions needed to perform activities. Sensory integration, used and studied, it remains unclear which best improve motor
kinaesthetic training, perceptual training, or combinations are performance or activities in children with DCD and alleviate
examples of these bottom-up approaches. For DCD, the the associated problems. To determine the most effective
hypothesis is that the improvement of body functions such as intervention strategy, we searched the literature on interven-
sensory integration, kinaesthesia, muscle strength, core stabil- tions in children with DCD published between 1995 and
ity, visual–motor perception, and so on leads to better skill December 2011. The longer an intervention method has
performance.37,38 existed, the larger the possibility that it has been investigated
By comparison, task-oriented approaches tend to focus on for its treatment effects. This is certainly true for sensory inte-
motor performance, i.e. on learning particular motor skills, gration and kinaesthetic training.5,26 Studies investigating sen-
with attention given to specific aspects of task performance sory integration and kinaesthetic training occurred mainly
that are causing the child difficulty. Leading examples are neu- between the early 1970s and 1996, with very few since 1996.
romotor task training (NTT),26,35,39,40 the cognitive orienta- Several studies published after 1996 have investigated task-
tion to daily occupational performance (CO-OP) oriented approaches like CO-OP and NTT, both of which
approach,37,38,41 and imagery training.29 In reality, these task- are relatively new. No meta-analysis has been conducted so far
oriented approaches are all based on a combination of current that includes this work.
motor control or motor learning and ecological principles, The main aim of this meta-analysis was to synthesize the
with the relative contribution of these frameworks varying literature from 1995 to December 2011, a period during
from one method to another.42 which DCD gained greater recognition as a diagnosis in its
NTT can be considered a true hybrid: from motor learning own right. In 1994 the London Consensus group43 pro-
theory, it draws strongly on the notion that task structure and duced a more detailed description of DCD compared with
scheduling are fundamental to the way skills are assembled the DSM-III-R (1987)44 and DSM-IV (1994).2 This was an
over repeated learning trials and sessions; from the ecological important milestone in the research into DCD. It meant
approach, it considers how task and environmental constraints the beginning of a uniform approach to children with
can be manipulated to provide some leverage for the individual motor impairment, not otherwise specified by a medical
child whose motor impairment does not enable direct and diagnosis. From 2004 to 2006, several professionals from
seamless movement in a particular workspace.20,35–38 different countries met to obtain a new agreement about
CO-OP can be considered the most pure example of a top- the diagnosis, research, and intervention characteristics and
down approach. It focuses particularly on the use of cognitive comorbidities of DCD, resulting in the Leeds Consensus
strategies to facilitate skill acquisition, and uses a collaborative, statement in 2006.45 From these meetings, several issues
problem-solving approach adapted from cognitive-behavioural emerged: there was no consistency in the interpretation and
therapy, particularly the work of Meichenbaum. The child is application of diagnostic tests, and no uniformity in the
encouraged to form a mental model of how to attack a move- application of the various evidence-based intervention meth-
ment task; they are led to generate a movement goal, plan its ods. The Leeds Consensus set the following guidelines that
implementation, and reflect on how their performance was or intervention approaches should meet: (1) activities should be
was not successful (goal, plan, do, check).37,38,42 functional, based on goals that are relevant to daily living
From a traditional physical therapy or occupational ther- and meaningful to the child; (2) they should enhance gener-
apy training perspective, individuals with DCD are trained in alization and application in the context of everyday life; and
the most important fundamental gross motor and fine motor (3) interventions must be evidence-based and grounded in
skills (hopping, jumping, throwing, and catching; cutting, theories that are applicable to understanding children with
drawing, writing), and in the basic motor abilities that are DCD.38,45
thought to be prerequisite for skills (e.g. trunk stability for Thereafter, various (inter-)national professionals inside and
certain fine-motor skills). These approaches combine under- outside Europe, some of whom participated in the seminars in
lying process-oriented approaches with direct skill training; Leeds, decided to write a directive for diagnosis, prognosis,
the underlying assumptions are that motor skills are and intervention for children with DCD. This resulted in the

2 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2012


European Academy for Childhood Disability recommenda- ment’, and the more specific treatment approaches (connected
tions for diagnosis and treatment of DCD, issued in January with OR) sensory integration, Neuro-Developmental Treat-
2012.46 In support of these recommendations for intervention, ment (NDT): Neuromotor Task Training (NTT), Cognitive
a systematic review on treatment efficacy was initiated. It was Orientation to daily Occupational Performance (CO-OP),
the aim of this review to rate both the scientific quality of the Perceptual Motor Training (PMT), motor imagery training
studies and compare the magnitude of differences across treat- (MIT), sensory integration training (SIT), task-specific train-
ment types, highlighting the most effective. ing, cognitive training, timing control, kinaesthetic training.
The following exclusions (‘NOT’) were used to obtain
METHOD more pure DCD studies: cerebral palsy, stroke, traumatic
Criteria for inclusion and exclusion brain injury, leukodystrophia or muscular disorders. Addition-
In advance of the actual literature search, selection criteria ally, references of all selected articles were checked for further
were set for the abstracts. Abstracts of meta-analysis, system- papers suitable for inclusion.
atic reviews, randomized controlled trials (RCTs), clinical tri- Each abstract resulting from the search was individually
als, and narrative reviews were included. The papers had to be screened for inclusion by at least two of four independent
written in English, German, or Dutch and published between experts (BS-E, A-CvdK, RM-vdM, EV-vdB). When consensus
1995 and 2011. existed, the full-text articles were evaluated.
The populations included in the studies were children of Each full-text article was again rated by two independent
any age, diagnosed with DCD or with possible DCD. DCD experts who needed to reach a consensus to include the paper
was defined either according to the criteria of the DSM-IV2 in the study. If consensus was not reached, a third expert was
or as motor impairment not otherwise specified by a medical consulted.
diagnosis, but examined with standardized motor tests con- Next, for each of the included studies, the experts had to
firming motor impairment. Studies reporting on children with agree on the level of evidence rated according to levels of evi-
poor motor performance, which was defined as 1SD below the dence (Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine,47 or in
mean, were also included. Studies investigating a sample of case of an RCT, the PEDro scale;48 Tables I and II). If con-
children with a syndrome (like ADHD or dyslexia), were sensus was not reached by the two experts, a third was con-
included in the descriptive part of the study but excluded from sulted. Authors did not rank their own studies. Moreover, all
the meta-analysis. conclusions and recommendations were reviewed by the inter-
The included studies had to have motor outcome measures national DCD guideline group in several meetings.46
with standardized and internationally accepted assessments,
for example, the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, Data analysis
the Concise Assessment Method for Children’s Handwriting, All abstracted study characteristics are shown in Table SI
or the Test of Gross Motor Development-2. (online supporting information). These include population
description, number of participants, age and relevant baseline
Literature search testing results, type of intervention, description of the inter-
The literature search spanned the period January 1995 to vention, frequency, intensity and duration, outcome measures,
December 2011. The following databases were consulted: description of results of the populations, short description of
Medline, the Cochrane Library, PubMed, CINAHL, PsychI- the conclusion and limitations, main conclusions.
nfo, PsychLit, OTDBase, OTseeker, PEDRO, ERIC, Included articles were assessed by members of project group
Embase, and HealthSTAR. The search terms were agreed for methodological quality using the instruments mentioned,
upon at a meeting of the international working group for the and results were added to Table SI results. To classify their
EACD recommendations for DCD in 2008. efficacy, the results were characterized by either a ‘+’ for sig-
The following search terms were used (connected with nificant improvement in the experimental group(s) or ‘0’ for
‘OR’): motor skills disorder, developmental coordination no significant change.
disorder, clumsiness, clumsy, clumsy child syndrome, clumsy Estimates of effect size were calculated for each comparison
child, in-coordination, dys-coordination, minimal brain of pre- and post-test results of a given treatment. The pre-
dysfunction, minor neurological dysfunction ⁄ disorder, motor ferred estimate was Cohen’s d.49 To calculate estimates of
delay, perceptual-motor impairment, motor coordination dif- effect size, means and standard deviations for pre- and post-
ficulties ⁄ problems, motor learning difficulties ⁄ problems, mild test results were used whenever they were reported.
motor problems, non-verbal learning disability ⁄ disorder ⁄ The inclusion of multiple dependent measures (and associ-
dysfunction, sensorimotor difficulties, sensory integrative ated statistics) was allowed for each study. Mean effect sizes
dysfunction, physical awkwardness, physically awkward, psy- are not unduly affected by non-independence in most
chomotor disorders, motor control and perception, apraxias, instances,50 and many authors choose not to weight studies
developmental dyspraxia, perceptual motor dysfunction, mini- according to the number of effect sizes. Pseudo-independence
mal cerebral dysfunction. of effect size was assumed here. Outcome measures were
These search terms above where combined (with ‘AND’) therefore analysed at the level of each finding; there were 54
with the general terms (connected with OR) ‘physical therapy’, effect sizes, distributed among four treatment types, plus one
‘physiotherapy’, ‘occupational therapy’, ‘intervention’, ‘treat- for comparison groups, pre- and post test.

Review 3
Table I: Levels of evidence (modified according to Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine), March 200947

Level of evidence Grade Oxford level Oxford definition (intervention studies)

1 (high) Evidence from a meta-analysis of randomized Ia Evidence from a meta-analysis or systematic


controlled trials review of randomized controlled trials (with
homogeneity).
Evidence from at least one randomized Ib Evidence from at least one randomized controlled trial.
controlled trial (intervention study) or
well-controlled trial with well-described sample
selection (diagnostic study); confirmatory data
analysis. QUADAS rating >10.
2 (moderate) Evidence from at least one controlled study IIa Evidence from systematic review of cohort
without randomization or QUADAS rating >7. studies (with homogeneity); or evidence from
at least one controlled study without randomization.
Evidence from at least one other type of IIb Individual cohort study (including low-quality
quasi-experimental study. randomized studies, e.g. <80% follow-up).
Evidence from at least one other type of
quasi-experimental study.
3 (low) Evidence from observational studies. QUADAS III Evidence from case–control studies; or evidence
rating >4. from observational studies
4 (very low) Evidence from expert committee reports or IV ⁄ V Evidence from expert committee reports or
experts. experts.

QUADAS, Quality Assessment Tool for Diagnostic Accuracy Studies.

Table II: Definitions of the PEDro scale48

1. Eligibility criteria were specified (not rated). No Yes


2. Subjects were randomly allocated to groups (in a crossover study, subjects were randomly allocated another in No Yes
which treatments were received).
3. Allocation was concealed. No Yes
4. The groups were similar at baseline regarding the most important prognostic indicators. Yes
5. There was blinding of all subjects. No Yes
6. There was blinding of all therapists who administered the therapy. No Yes
7. There was blinding of all assessors who measured at least one key outcome. No Yes
8. Measures of at least one key outcome were obtained from more than 85% of the subjects initially allocated to groups. No Yes
9. All subjects for whom outcome measures were available received the treatment or control condition as allocated No Yes
or, whether this was not the case, data for at least one key outcome was analysed by ‘intention to treat’.
10. The results of between-group statistical comparisons are reported for at least one key outcome. No Yes
11. The study provides both point measures and measures of variability for at least one key outcome. No Yes

The PEDro scale is a 10-point scale with dichotomous (1 ⁄ 0, referring to yes ⁄ no) responses that account for key quality aspects of an experimental
study. A score of 7 or higher indicates a good, reliable study, and a score of 5 or 6 as acceptable. No, no points; yes, one point for questions 2–11.
Total score ‡7, good; 5–6, moderate; <5, weak.

A random-effects approach to data analysis was used, based The magnitudes of the mean effect size estimates (d) were
on the method of Lipsey and Wilson.51 Here it is assumed then interpreted according to the convention of Cohen:
that variation between effect sizes is due not only to random d=0.30 (small effect size), d=0.50 (moderate effect size), and
sampling error but also to other factors within studies (e.g. d=0.80 (large effect size).
methodological differences). The random-effects model is
preferable when heterogeneity of effect sizes is present, RESULTS
indexed by Q statistics. Although less powerful than a fixed- The total number of studies matching the search terms was
effects approach, it does allow greater generalization. Effect 3708. Each abstract was then reviewed by the members of the
sizes were weighted by the inverse of their variance. For each project group (BS-E, A-CvdK, RM-vdM, EV-vdB) and
treatment type, a mean weighted Cohen’s d (dw) value was cal- assessed to determine inclusion or exclusion in the review.
culated using the SPSS (SPSS Inc, Chicago IL, USA). Eighty-five full-text studies were evaluated independently by
Homogeneity of effect sizes was investigated using the Q two individual experts. Of these, 72 papers were deemed as
statistic.52,53 Heterogeneity indicated that the variability in the candidates for this review. From these 72 studies, 44 were
effect sizes might be due to outliers or a moderator variable. A excluded after reading the full paper because the outcomes of
group of effect sizes was deemed homogeneous when the value the study did not include motor skills, were descriptive, did
of Q obtained was not significant at p<0.01. Outliers were also not describe any intervention method, or were a single-case
considered, if necessary. study. The final complement of eligible studies in the

4 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2012


systematic review was 26 (Fig. 1). For the meta-analysis CO-OP, NTT, and motor imagery were mentioned; (2)
proper, there were 20 eligible studies. under process-oriented intervention, sensory integrative train-
ing and kinaesthetic training were mentioned; (3) under tradi-
Level and quality of evidence tional therapy (physical therapy and occupational therapy),
Twenty-six studies met the inclusion criteria. The dates of perceptual motor training and contemporary treatment
publication ranged from 1995 to 2011. Of those 26 articles, approach, contemporary physical therapy based on fundamen-
one was a systematic review, one was a meta-analysis, and the tal motor training and occupational therapy, psychomotor
remainder were primary studies. The primary studies were training, and therapist-guided parent or teacher intervention
scored with the PEDro scale to evaluate all RCTs and clinical were mentioned.
trials. The overall quality was variable, with a mean of 6.0 The Movement Assessment Battery for Children was the
(range 4–10). measure most frequently used for diagnostic purposes (12
The methodological quality of these studies was scored to studies), followed by the Concise Assessment Method for
determine the strength of the evidence according to the guide- Children’s Handwriting (4), the Test of Gross Motor Devel-
lines of the Oxford Centre for Evidence-based Medicine - opment (second edition) (2), the Kinesthetic Sensitivity Test
Levels of Evidence.47 (2), and the Test Of Motor Impairment (2). Diagnostic tests
A total of 912 children with DCD or motor impairment that featured for a specific intervention were the following:
classified with a test score of at least 1SD below the mean, The Tennessee Self-Concept Scale (1), the Child Anxiety
with no other medical diagnoses, participated in the studies Scale, the Perceived Motor Competence scale (1), the Devel-
included in this review. The average sample size was 44 partic- opmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (1), the Southern
ipants. Seventeen studies had fewer than 44 partici- California Sensory Integration Test (1), the Finger Identifica-
pants,2,5,14,16,17,20,22,24,26–28,30,31,33,35,36,65one had over a 100.34 tion test (1) and the Evaluation Tool of Children’s Handwrit-
The duration of the intervention ranged from 2 weeks to ing (1), the Bruininks–Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency
6 months, and the amount of instruction ranged from 4 to (1), the Canadian Occupational Performance Measure (2), the
26 hours. Performance Quality Rating Scale (1), the Vineland Adaptive
In the 26 studies, the following terms for the intervention Behavior Scale (VABS) (1), the Self-Perception Profile for
were most often found: (1) under task-oriented intervention, Children (1), the Harter’s Scale of Perceived Compe-

Initial number of articles for this review with matching titles, in


databases (N = 3708)

Exclusion for this review based upon population or study other than intervention:
Abstract information (n = 3576)
Full text version (n = 85)

Number of related articles based on references (n = 5)

Absolute number of included articles for review (n = 72)

Final number of included articles with complete data


available for systematic review (n = 26)
Final number of studies for the meta-analysis (n = 20)

Figure 1: Flowchart of the literature search.

Review 5
tence ⁄ Harter and Pike’s Pictorial Scale of Perceived Compe- Task-oriented approach
tence (1), the Motor Teaching Principles Taxonomy (1), the For the task-oriented approaches, individual and group pro-
Visual Analogue Scales (1), the Rhythm Integrated Test (1), grammes were both effective ways of teaching motor skills in
and the Sensory Integrated Praxis Test (1). DCD. Task-oriented approaches work on teaching essential
The outcome measures are all standardized, general assess- activities of daily living and, thereby, stimulate participation at
ments for gross- and ⁄ or fine-motor function. All studies home, school, in leisure, and sports.22,26,30,36–38,55 Whether
included in our systematic review are presented in Table SI. the training effects are attributable mainly to flow-on effects
Twenty-two of the 24 primary studies were effective for participation is an issue for continued research. On current
in improving motor performance. Of these, most were understanding, it is recommended that NTT be used for chil-
longer than 10 weeks, and the frequency of intervention dren with DCD, with training yielding positive (task-specific)
was mostly once a week, with home exercises in three change on measures of gross- and fine-motor skill.21,22,26,27
studies.24,28,55 In ten studies treatment was given in a group The efficacy of CO-OP was also evident but work here
setting.15,18,20,21,23–25,28,30,54 requires more robust metrics. Children with DCD are shown
Based on the data in Table SI, there is strong evidence that to generate more effective strategies than those receiving other
children with DCD benefit from task-oriented approaches approaches consisting most commonly of sensory-integration
(see also Pless and Carlsson55). In addition, there was sufficient and non-problem-related general fine- and gross-motor activi-
evidence that motor-training-based interventions, as used in ties.14,16 Importantly, children with better verbal ability tend
traditional physical or occupational therapies, were generally to make better gains in approaches where language plays an
effective for children with DCD.14,17,19,22–26,28–30,36 Third, important mediating role; their ability to interpret cues and
evidence for the efficacy of process-oriented approaches (e.g. prompts and to implement responses to them may be a predic-
sensory integrative training and kinaesthetic training) was con- tive factor here.14 In short, we recommend that task-oriented
flicting. intervention methods like NTT and CO-OP should be pre-
scribed with some confidence to children with DCD who are
Meta-analysis of intervention studies in need of intervention to improve their motor performance.
Characteristics of the treatment and comparison samples are
shown in Table SI. The total number of effect sizes for all Motor imagery training
studies combined was 54. Q statistics revealed homogeneity Caution is needed when considering the efficacy of motor
for each treatment category (p>0.01). The mean effect size imagery training because, first, there is only one study available
across all treatment types was dw=0.56, indicating that treat- (Wilson et al.29). In addition, of the total referred sample, 36%
ment generally had a moderate effect. When the different had a test score within the low normal range on the Movement
treatment types were compared, the effect sizes were signifi- Assessment Battery for Children. Therefore, conclusions
cantly different (F4,49=5.04, p=0.002); the task-oriented about motor imagery training for severe DCD should be inter-
approach had the highest mean value (dw=0.89; 95% CI preted with care. However, the average change of scores on the
0.64–1.14), followed by the traditional motor-training-based Movement Assessment Battery for Children showed that per-
therapy (d=0.83; 95% CI 0.46–1.20), process-oriented inter- ceptual motor training and motor imagery training improved
vention (dw=0.12; 95% CI )0.10–0.35), and comparison motor performance to a similar degree.29 More high-quality
(dw=0.23; 95% CI 0.04–0.43). Chemical treatments contrib- research is needed to clarify the conditions under which motor
uted only four effect sizes, but yielded a moderate to large imagery training is best implemented for children with DCD.
value (dw=0.79; 95% CI 0.15–1.42). The post-hoc Bonfer-
roni comparison between treatment types showed that the Traditional physical therapy and occupational therapy
effect size of the task-oriented approach was significantly There is sufficient evidence that traditional physical or occupa-
higher than the process-oriented approach (p=0.01) and tional therapies (either individual or group) are also effective in
comparison (p=0.006). No significant difference in the teaching motor skills to children with DCD.14,17,19,22–26,28–30,36
magnitude of effect size between traditional physical and Individual therapy was studied in two good-quality RCTs. In
occupational therapy approaches and any of the other the RCT by Wilson et al.,29 it was evident that perceptual
interventions emerged. The effect size of the methylpheni- motor training results in improved motor performance
date was 0.81 and not significantly different from any of the (d=0.76). Here, 13 of the 17 children in the perceptual motor
other treatments. training group exceeded the upper limit of the 95% CI for
comparisons. Moreover, Peens et al.23 showed that a combina-
DISCUSSION tion of motor- and psychological-based intervention improved
The results of meta-analysis of recent research support the motor proficiency and self-concept in DCD. The findings
view that task-oriented (dw=0.89) and traditional motor-train- from our meta-analysis showed that children with DCD bene-
ing-based therapies (dw.83) show strong treatment effects in fit the most from task-oriented approaches; we recommend
children with DCD, whereas that for process-oriented therapy that perceptual motor training adopts a task-specific approach.
is weak (dw=0.12). The implications of these numbers and of For example, this integrated approach is effective in improving
the other findings of the reviewed studies for theory and prac- the motor performance of children with combined
tice are discussed. DCD ⁄ ADHD. Fifty per cent of these children obtained nor-

6 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2012


mal scores on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children tate the involvement of parents and teachers, for example the
immediately after training, while 35% reached normal limits use of user-friendly online resources that model good training
after 4 weeks.28 practices.62–64

Process-oriented training Methylphenidate


Although the sensory integration method is the most com- Methylphenidate had a positive effect on both behavioural
monly investigated approach, evidence is mixed. It is qualified ADHD symptoms and fine-motor performance (i.e. hand-
by Hillier54 as being an effective treatment method in children writing) in children with combined DCD and ADHD.
with DCD. However, in the meta-analysis of Pless and Carls- Additional motor therapy was indicated for around one-half of
son,55 sensory integration was not as effective as functional the combined group. We do not know whether methylpheni-
skill-specific interventions, and in the older analysis of Vargas date can be used to improve motor performance in DCD
and Camilli19 results for sensory integration did not show a gen- without comorbidities. Moreover, there are some important
erally positive effect for studies published between 1983 and issues with longer-term side effects.31–33 Finally, although
1993. This conflicting report warrants deeper consideration. fatty acids with vitamin E can improve academic skills like
The meta-analysis by Vargas and Camilli19 evaluated 16 stud- reading and spelling as well as behaviour, there is no evidence
ies published between 1972 and 1994. Overall, the average effect for a positive effect on motor performance.34
size of sensory integration was d=0.29. Larger effect sizes were
found in psycho-educational (d=0.39) and motor categories Handwriting
(d=0.40). Sensory integration methods were effective overall, Schoolchildren having handwriting problems are often pre-
but were not shown to differ from alternative ones. In studies sented for treatment; this is between 25 and 50% of all the
comparing sensory integration with no treatment, a significant referred children in the Netherlands.65 Prewriting exercises
effect for sensory integration was replicated in earlier studies (and fine motor training, if required) has been shown to
(1972–1982), but not in more recent studies (1983–1993). What improve handwriting.27 Three different studies using a task-
must also be kept in mind is that sensory integration methods oriented approach based on NTT all showed significant
are relatively long in duration by normal standards.19 improvement in handwriting.21,26,27 More specifically, task-
In the systematic review by Hillier,54 six studies reported oriented self-instruction involving knowledge of results had a
positive effects for sensory integration,57–61 which was taken as positive effect on the quality of handwriting in children; impor-
support for this approach. Included among these were two tantly, this occurred both in regular and special schools.21
very old studies by Wilson et al.61 and Humphries et al.57–59
Kaplan et al.60 investigated two papers in which all groups Limitations and implications for future research
receiving intervention improved, but no one method was more After evaluating 3708 abstracts and 77 full-text papers, we
effective than another. Leemrijse et al.,52 in a crossover study, were able to locate 24 primary studies addressing the research
showed a positive effect for sensory integration, although the questions posed in this paper. Of these studies, 12 were cate-
sample was small (n=6). Finally, the meta-analysis of Pless and gorized as ‘physical therapy and occupational therapy’, 10
Carlsson55 showed that specific skill interventions in children were categorized as ‘task-oriented’, and three as methylpheni-
with DCD had stronger treatment effects than sensory inte- date. Of the 24 primary studies, seven used a research design
gration. The highest effect was noted for skill-specific inter- that produced definite evidence (level Ib). However, most of
ventions (1.46), the lowest for sensory integration (d=0.21). the studies (17) used a research design (seven at level IIa and
Watemberg et al. concluded in his RCT that sensory inte- 10 at level IIb) that did not control for all other factors that
gration is an effective treatment for DCD ⁄ ADHD; however, it could have produced the observed results, and are considered
was given in combination with other interventions and used in to have given moderate levels of evidence. This should be
a task-specific manner.28 In addition, the RCT of Sudsawad taken into account when interpreting the results. However,
et al.18 showed that although kinaesthetic training did improve the advantage of a systematic review is that the consistency of
kinaesthetic acuity, it did not assist handwriting or motor per- the results adds to the grading of evidence. Moreover, analysis
formance in children aged between 6 and 7 years.18 Taken of the combined data set in the meta-analysis adds information
together and in light of our review, there is no recent evidence not available in the isolated studies.
of well-designed studies in support of sensory integration or The scheduling of treatment was variable, from once a
kinaesthetic training. week14,16,22,24–27,29,31 to every (school-)day.15,17,31,32,34 As well,
the amount of instruction ranged from 4 to 26 hours. The
Parent- and teacher-guided intervention meta-analysis had too few effect sizes (n=54) to dissect the
The complement of studies showed that parents and teachers effects of frequency and duration. Moreover, few studies had a
were able to provide effective intervention for children with sufficient sample size to enable designs that would allow com-
DCD provided this was tuned to their specific movement parison between different forms of intervention.15–17,23,28,29
problems, and monitored by a professional. Involvement of Most studies compared one intervention with no interven-
parents and teachers is likely to ensure that the learned tion.14,18,21,22,24–27,30–32,34,43
skills will continue to be used after the formal interven- It was also alarming that many studies did not describe the
tion.21,37,56 We need more studies to clarify how best to facili- intervention in sufficient detail to help researchers and clini-

Review 7
cians alike to understand fully what it was about the treatment gress.21,36 More studies are needed to research the most
that made it successful. In other words, it was not always obvi- effective way that parents and teachers can be engaged in
ous that treatments using the same term were identical in nat- instruction.38
ure (see Table SI). Most papers provided only a global
description of the intervention, for example ball skills and bal- CONCLUSION
ance activities.14–16,18,22–24,26–28,30,43 We conclude that, in general, task-oriented (e.g. NTT and
Additionally, there is a need to profile those children who CO-OP) and motor-training-based intervention (physical
do respond to treatment, as opposed to those who do not, therapy, occupational therapy) both show strong treatment
including factors like age and comorbidity. Finally, only three effects (dw=0.89 and dw=0.83 respectively), whereas that for
studies17,25,27 investigated follow-up effects of 3 months or process-oriented therapy is weak (dw=0.12). Moreover, thera-
longer. Hence, no statements can be made about the longer- pies should ideally have some task-oriented elements to them
term effect of the treatment approaches. Next to more rigid to promote transfer. Of the more top-down approaches, NTT
scientific designs, this is a prime issue for future research. could be the therapy of choice for younger children and those
Importantly, the costs of intervention for DCD are enormous. with lower verbal or learning capabilities. Children with well-
For instance, in Germany, the public insurance system is pay- developed verbal skills may well be the ones who benefit most
ing about 400 million euros a year for occupational therapy from CO-OP. For handwriting problems, a task-oriented
for children with sensorimotor problems (mainly children with approach based on NTT is the best option. It is too early to
DCD, of whom the largest part are referred for fine motor state with confidence that motor imagery training is effective
and handwriting problems). A next step would be to conduct for treating DCD.29 Process-oriented approaches (e.g. sensory
cost-effectiveness analysis, once these evidence-based recom- integration and kinaesthetic training) show only a weak effect,
mendations are implemented. the same level as no-treatment comparison, and are not rec-
ommended for improving motor performance in DCD. More-
Recommendations for practice over, they do not follow current thinking on motor control or
Our literature review largely substantiates the Leeds Consen- motor learning.
sus for intervention.45 These guidelines stipulate the follow- Finally, chemical supplements (like fatty acids and vitamin
ing: treatment activities should be task-oriented, functional, E) did not improve motor performance. However, daily doses
and relevant to daily living; be child-centred in the process of of methylphenidate may be useful in children with comorbid
therapy; involve significant others including parents and teach- ADHD and DCD, particularly for fine-motor issues like
ers; and be evidence-based and grounded in theories that are handwriting.
applicable to understanding children with DCD.
Although training effects are commonly shown at a task- SUPPORTING INFORMATION
specific level, it is not always apparent how they transfer The following additional material may be found online.
across related and disparate areas of function. This refers to Table SI: Levels of evidence (modified according to Oxford Centre for
the classic issue of horizontal versus vertical transfer. Because Evidence-Based Medicine), March 2009.46
the purpose of treatment is to achieve participation in mean- Please note: This journal provides supporting online information sup-
ingful life areas as independently as possible and with high plied by the authors. Such materials are peer reviewed and may be re-orga-
quality of life, measures that capture the level of participation nized for online delivery, but may not be copy-edited or typeset.
should also be used to evaluate treatment effects. Involvement Technical support issues or other queries (other than missing files) should
of parents, teachers, and physical education is needed to max- be addressed to the authors.
imize transfer into daily life and to ensure longer-term pro-

REFERENCES
1. Dewey D, Wilson B. Developmental coordination disorder. 6. Losse A, Henderson E, Elliman D, Hall D, Knight E, Jong- 10. Rasmussen P, Gillberg C. Natural outcome of ADHD with
What is it?. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 2001; 20: 5–27. mans M. Clumsiness in children-do they grow out of it? A DCD at age 22 years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical 10-year follow-up study Dev Med Child Neurol 1991; 33: 55– 2000; 39: 1424–31.
Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn, text revision). Wash- 68. 11. Cantell MH, Smyth MM, Ahonen TP. Clumsiness in adoles-
ington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000. 7. Kadesjö B, Gillberg C. Developmental coordination disorder cence: educational, motor, and social outcomes of motor delay
3. Niemeijer AS, Smits-Engelsman BCM, Reynders K, Schoe- in Swedish 7-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy- detected at 5 years. Adapt Phys Activ Q 1994; 11: 115–29.
maker MM. Verbal actions of physiotherapists to enhance chiatry 1999; 38: 820–8. 12. Christiansen AS. Persisting motor control problems in 11-
motor learning in children with DCD. Hum Mov Sci 2003; 8. Dewey D. Subtypes of developmental coordination disorder. to-12-year old boys previously diagnosed with deficits in
22: 567–81. In: Cermak SA, Larkin D, editors. Developmental Coordina- attention, motor control and perception (DAMP). Dev Med
4. Hadders-Algra M. The neural group selection theory: prom- tion Disorder. Albany, NY: Delmar Thomson Learning, Child Neurol 2000; 42: 4–7.
ising principles for understanding and treating developmental 2002: 40–53. 13. Geuze RH, Borger H. Children who are clumsy: 5 years
motor disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol 2000; 42: 707–15. 9. Ward A, Rodger S. The application of cognitive orientation later. Adapt Phys Activ Q 1993; 10: 10–21.
5. Miller LT, Polatajko HJ, Missiuna C, Mandich AD, Macnab to daily occupational performance (CO-OP) with children 5– 14. Green D, Chambers ME, Sugden DA. Does subtype of
JJ. A pilot trial of a cognitive treatment for children with 7 years with developmental coordination disorder. Br J Occup developmental coordination disorder count: is there a differ-
developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci 2001; Ther 2004; 67: 257–64. ential effect on outcome following intervention? Hum Mov
20: 183–210. Sci 2008; 27: 363–82.

8 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2012


15. Polatajko HJ, Macnab JJ, Anstett B, Malloy-Miller T, 32. Flapper BCT, Schoemaker MM. Effects of methylphenidate 48. PEDro: Physiotherapy Evidence Database. Available from:
Murphy K, Noh S. A clinical trial of the process-oriented on quality of life in children with both developmental coordi- http://www.pedro.org.au/english/downloads/pedro-scale
treatment approach for children with developmental co- nation disorder and ADHD. Dev Med Child Neurol 2008; (accessed 10th March 2010).
ordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol 1995; 7: 310–9. 50: 294–9. 49. Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, editors. The Hand-
16. Sangster CA, Beninger C, Polatajko HJ, Mandich A. Cogni- 33. Bart O, Podoly T, Bar-Haim Y. A preliminary study on the book of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis (2nd edn).
tive strategy generation in children with developmental coor- effect of methylphenidate on motor performance in children New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 2009.
dination disorder. Can J Occup Ther 2005; 72: 67–77. with comorbid DCD and ADHD. Res Dev Disabil 2010; 31: 50. Dindia K, Allen M. Sex differences in self disclosure. Psychol
17. Sims K, Henderson SE, Hulme C, Morton J. The remedia- 1443–7. Bull 1992; 112: 106–24.
tion of clumsiness I: an evaluation of Laszlo’s kinaesthetic 34. Richardson AJ, Oxon DP, Montgomery P. The Oxford-Dur- 51. Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical Meta-Analysis. (Applied
approach. Dev Med Child Neurol 1996; 38: 976–87. ham Study: a randomised, controlled trial of dietary supple- social research methods series 2001, volume 49.) Thousand
18. Sudsawad P, Trombly CA, Henderson A, Tickle-Degnen L. mentation with fatty acids in children with developmental Oaks, CA: Sage, 2001.
Testing the effect of kinesthetic training on handwriting per- coordination disorder. Pedriatrics 2005; 115: 1360–6. 52. Leemrijse C, Meijer OG, Vermeer A, Adèr HJ, Diemel S.
formance in first grade students. Am J Occup Ther 2002; 56: 35. Niemeijer AS, Schoemaker MM, Smits-Engelsman BCM. The efficacy of Le Bon Départ and sensory integration treat-
26–33. Are teaching principles associated with improved motor per- ment for children with developmental coordination disorder:
19. Vargas S, Camilli G. A meta-analysis of research on sensory formance in children with developmental coordination disor- a randomized study with six single cases. Clin Rehabil 2000;
integration treatment. Am J Occup Ther 1999; 53: 189–98. der? A pilot study Phys Ther 2006; 86: 1221–30. 14: 247–59.
20. Dunford C. Goal-oriented group intervention for children 36. Sugden DA, Chambers ME. Intervention in children with 53. Wilson PH, McKenzie BE. Information processing deficits
with developmental coordination disorder. Phys Occup Ther developmental coordination disorder: the role of parents and associated with developmental coordination disorder: a meta-
Pediatr 2011; 31: 288–300. teachers. Br J Educ Psychol 2003; 73: 545–61. analysis of research findings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998;
21. Jongmans MJ, Linthorst-Bakker E, Westenberg Y, Smits- 37. Barnhart RC, Davenport MJ, Epps SB, Nordquist VM. 39: 829–40.
Engelsman BCM. Use of a task-oriented self-instruction Developmental coordination disorder. Phys Ther 2003; 83: 54. Hillier S. Intervention for children with developmental coor-
method to support children in primary school with poor hand- 722–30. dination disorder: a systematic review. J Allied Health 2007;
writing quality and speed. Hum Mov Sci 2003; 22: 549–66. 38. Sugden DA. Current approaches to intervention in children 5: 1–11.
22. Niemeijer AS, Smits-Engelsman BCM, Schoemaker MM. with developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child 55. Pless M, Carlsson M. Effects of motor intervention on devel-
Neuromotor task training for children with developmental Neurol 2007; 49: 467–71. opmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis. Adapt Phys
coordination disorder: a controlled trial. Dev Med Child Neu- 39. Smits-Engelsman BCM, Tuijl ALT. Toepassing van cogni- Activ Q 2000; 17: 381–401.
rol 2007; 49: 406–11. tieve motorische controle theorieën in de kinderfysiotherapie: 56. Sigmundson H, Pedersen AV, Whiting HTA, Ingvaldsen
23. Peens A, Pienaar AE, Nienaber AW. The effect of different het controleren van vrijheidsgraden en beperkingen. (Appli- RP. We can cure your child’s clumsiness! A review of inter-
intervention programmes on the self-concept and motor pro- cation of cognitive motor control theories in pediatric physi- vention methods. Scand J Rehabil Med 1998; 30: 101–6.
ficiency of 7- to 9-year-old children with DCD. Child Care cal therapy practice: controlling degrees of freedom and 57. Humphries T, Wright M, McDoughall B, Vertes J. The effi-
Health Dev 2008; 34: 316–28. constraints). In: Smits-Engelsman BCM, Ham van I, Vaes P, cacy of sensory integration therapy for children with learning
24. Pless M, Carlsson M, Sundelin C, Persson K. Effects of Aufdemkampe G, Dekker den JB, editors. Jaarboek Fysio- disability. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 1990; 10: 1–17.
group motor skill intervention on 5- to 6-year-old children therapie. Houten: Bohn Stafleu Van Loghum, 1998: 202–29. 58. Humphries T, Wright M, Snider L, McDougall B. A com-
with developmental coordination disorder. Pediatr Phys Ther 40. Schoemaker MM, Smits-Engelsman BCM. Neuromotor Task parison of the effectiveness of sensory integrative therapy and
2000; 12: 183–9. Training: a new approach to treat children with DCD. In: perceptual-motor training in treating children with learning
25. Pless M, Carlsson M, Sundelin C, Persson K. Pre-school Sugden DA, Chamers M, editors. Children with Developmen- disabilities. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1992; 13: 31–40.
children with developmental coordination disorder: self-per- tal Coordination Disorder. London: Whurr, 2005: 212–27. 59. Humphries T, Snider L, McDougall B. Clinical evaluation of
ceived competence and group motor skill intervention. Acta 41. Sugden DA, Henderson SE. Ecological Intervention for the effectiveness of sensory integrative and perceptual
Paediatr 2001; 90: 532–8. Children with Movement Difficulties. London: Pearson motor therapy in improving sensory integrative function in
26. Schoemaker MM, Niemeijer AS, Reynders K, Smits-Engels- Assessment, 2007. children with learning disabilities. Occup Ther J Res 1993; 13:
man BCM. Effectiveness of neuromotor task training for 42. Sudgen D, Dunford C. Intervention and the role of theory, 163–82.
children with developmental coordination disorder: a pilot empirism and experience in children with motor impairment. 60. Kaplan BJ, Polatajko HJ, Wilson BN, Faris PD. Reexamina-
study. Neural Plast 2003; 10: 155–63. Disabil Rehabil 2007; 29: 3–11. tion of sensory integrative treatment. A combination of two
27. Smits-Engelsman BCM, Niemeijer AS, Galen Van GP. Fine 43. Polatajko HJ, Fox AM, Missiuna C. An international consen- efficacy studies. J Learn Disabil 1993; 26: 342–7.
motor deficiencies in children diagnosed as DCD based sus on children with developmental coordination disorder. 61. Wilson NW, Kaplan BJ, Fellowes S, Gruchy C, Faris P. The
on poor grapho-motor ability. Hum Mov Sci 2001; 20: 161– Can J Occup Ther 1995; 62: 3–6. efficacy of sensory integration treatment comparing to tutor-
82. 44. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical ing. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 1992; 12: 1–37.
28. Watemberg N, Waiserberg N, Zuk L, Lerman-Sagie T. Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, text revision). Wash- 62. Wilson PH. Practitioners review: approaches to assessment
Developmental coordination disorder in children with atten- ington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1987. and treatment of children with DCD: an evaluative review.
tion deficit-hyperactivity disorder and physical therapy inter- 45. Sugden D, Chambers M, Utley A. Leeds Consensus State- J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005; 46: 806–23.
vention. Dev Med Child Neurol 2007; 49: 920–5. ment. Leeds, 2006. Available from: http://www.dcd-uk.org/ 63. Missiuna C, Cairney J, Pollock N, et al. A staged approach
29. Wilson PH, Patrick MD, Thomas MA, Maruff P. Motor consensus.html (accessed 8th August 2012). for identifying children with developmental coordination
imaginery training ameliorates motor clumsiness in children. 46. Blank R, Smits-Engelsman B, Polatajko H, Wilson P, Euro- disorder from the population. Res Dev Disabil 2011; 32:
J Child Neurol 2002; 17: 491–8. pean Academy for Childhood Disability. European Academy 549–59.
30. Tsai CL. The effectiveness of exercise intervention on inhib- for Childhood Disability (EACD): recommendations on the 64. Developmental Coordination Disorder. CanChild. [Inter-
itory control in children with developmental coordination definition, diagnosis, and intervention of developmental net]. Available from: http://dcd.canchild.ca/en/ (accessed
disorder: using a visuospatial attention paradigm as a model. coordination disorder (long version). Dev Med Child Neurol 20th December 2011).
Res Dev Disabil 2009; 30: 1268–80. 2012; 54: 54–93. 65. Bosga-Stork I, Overvelde A, Van Bommel-Rutgers I, et al.
31. Flapper BCT, Houwen S, Schoemaker MM. Fine motor 47. Oxford Centre for Evidence-based Medicine - Levels of Evi- [Inventory of referral patterns, research and treatment of chil-
skills and effects of methylphenidate in children with atten- dence. Available from: http://www.cebm.net/ (accessed 8th dren with writing problems. A digital survey]. Ned Tijdschr
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordi- August 2012). Kinderfysiother 2009; 21: 14–8. [In Dutch].
nation disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol 2006; 48: 165–9.

Review 9

View publication stats

You might also like