Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232476959

Specific Learning Disabilities: Clarifying, Not Eliminating, a Construct.

Article  in  Professional Psychology Research and Practice · February 2006


DOI: 10.1037/0735-7028.37.1.99

CITATIONS READS
69 2,951

2 authors:

Nancy Mather Noel Gregg


The University of Arizona University of Georgia
87 PUBLICATIONS   4,042 CITATIONS    76 PUBLICATIONS   1,641 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

LRS, SSES/ SLI und Fremdsprachenlernen View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nancy Mather on 02 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Professional Psychology: Research and Practice Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 37, No. 1, 99 –106 0735-7028/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7028.37.1.99

Specific Learning Disabilities: Clarifying, Not Eliminating, a Construct

Nancy Mather Noel Gregg


University of Arizona University of Georgia

Over the past decades, both researchers and practitioners in the field of specific learning disabilities (LD)
have grappled over an appropriate definition as well as the subsequent operational diagnostic criteria. In
a recent article in Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, S. C. Dombrowski, R. W. Kamphaus,
and C. R. Reynolds (2004) proposed a new set of suggested diagnostic criteria that challenges many of
the traditional beliefs about these disorders. This article provides a critique of their proposed criteria as
well as an alternative approach for LD identification that is more aligned with current understanding of
the etiology and characteristics of these disorders.

Keywords: learning disabilities, discrepancy, diagnostic criteria, assessment

Over the years, professionals from several disciplines involved sion of the efficacy of various components of the definition of LD,
with the field of specific learning disabilities (LD) have debated as well as the eligibility criteria used to operationalize these
and struggled to agree on an appropriate conceptual definition and specific disorders. Thus, considerable debate has focused on the
subsequent operational diagnostic criteria for these conditions. roles of general intelligence measures, broad and specific cognitive
Several theories and frameworks have been proposed to help processing measures, and oral language measures in LD identifi-
professionals use assessment information to develop effective in- cation (Fletcher & Reschly, 2005; Hale, Naglieri, Kaufman, &
struction. Theories such as perceptual–motor (Ayres, 1972; Kavale, 2004; Naglieri, Hale, Kaufman, & Kavale, in press).
Barsch, 1967; Kephart, 1960), psycholinguistic (Kirk, McCarthy, In a recent article in Professional Psychology: Research and
& Kirk, 1968), psychoneurological (Johnson & Myklebust, 1967), Practice, Dombrowski, Kamphaus, and Reynolds (2004) proposed
neuropsychological (Gaddes, 1980), and phonological and ortho- a set of suggested diagnostic criteria for LD that challenges many
graphic (Ehri, 1998; Frith, 1985), as well as the Cattell–Horn– of the critical components of the specific LD definition. The
Carroll (CHC) theory (Carroll, 1993; Cattell, 1943; Horn, 1991; Dombrowski et al. diagnostic framework does not take into ac-
Horn & Noll, 1997; McGrew & Flanagan, 1998), have contributed count and incorporate findings from current research or historical
to a better understanding of the cognitive and linguistic dimensions and clinical perspectives regarding identification of individuals
that form the essence of LD. A central component of all of these with LD. In essence, their proposed criteria view LD as a devel-
clinical and statistical theories is that specific cognitive and lin- opmental delay that can be determined by a standard score cutoff
guistic processes are accessed and used by individuals to collect, (i.e., 85 or below). In addition, the evaluation of cognitive and
sort, process, store, and retrieve various types of information. As linguistic processes is seen as unnecessary for accurate LD
new research has provided support for specific theories related to identification.
LD, definitions and eligibility criteria have required adjustments in In response to Dombrowski et al.’s (2004) proposed criteria, we
both terminology and frameworks. (See Kavale & Forness, 2000, begin by addressing a few additional issues related to the points of
for a critical analysis of LD definitions.) agreement with the authors. Next, we discuss specific concerns
The recent reauthorization of the Individuals With Disabilities with components of their proposed LD criteria. In the final section
Education Improvement Act (2004) has fueled additional discus- we present an alternative approach for LD identification that is
more aligned with current understanding of the etiology and the
definition of these specific disabilities.
NANCY MATHER earned her PhD in learning disabilities from the University
of Arizona. She is currently a professor in the Department of Special
Education, Rehabilitation, and School Psychology at the University of Points of Agreement
Arizona. She is a coauthor of the Woodcock–Johnson III (WJ III). Her
primary areas of research interest include factors that affect literacy devel- Several components of Dombrowski et al.’s (2004) proposed
opment as well as issues related to the use and interpretation of the WJ-III. criteria are consistent with what we know about LD.
NOEL GREGG earned her PhD in learning disabilities from Northwestern
University. She is a Distinguished Research Professor at the University of
Georgia in the Department of Special Education and Psychology. She is Overreliance on Ability–Achievement Discrepancy
Director of the Regents Center for Learning Disorders. Her special interests
Individuals with LD typically present an uneven profile of
involve adults with learning disabilities, written language disorders, as-
sessment, and issues related to accommodations.
abilities, demonstrating difficulties with some types of learning but
CORRESPONDENCE CONCERNING THIS ARTICLE should be addressed to Nancy ease with other areas of performance. Unfortunately, with the
Mather, University of Arizona, Department of Special Education, Reha- passage of the Individuals With Disabilities Education Act (1997;
bilitation, and School Psychology, Room 409, Tucson, AZ 85721. E-mail: originally Pub. L. No. 94 –142), a discrepancy between full-scale
nmather@u.arizona.edu intelligence scores and achievement became the hallmark of LD

99
100 MATHER AND GREGG

identification. Much has been written about the limitations of using without other clinical sources of information (Brackett & McPher-
an ability–achievement discrepancy for the identification of LD son, 1996; Hoy et al., 1996).
(e.g., Berninger, 2001; Fletcher et al., 2001; Lyon, 1995; Mather &
Healey, 1990; Vellutino, 2001), as well as the recommendation to Data From Multiple Sources
abandon this type of discrepancy (Flowers, Meyer, Lovato, Wood,
& Felton, 2000; Fuchs, Mock, Morgan, & Young, 2003). Through- We also strongly support Dombrowski et al.’s (2004) call for the
out the past two decades, experts in the field have voiced concerns use of multiple sources of information, or what others have de-
regarding overreliance on an ability–achievement model. Leaders scribed as implementation of a more clinical model (Gregg &
have warned of the theoretical (Kavale, 1987; Scruggs, 1987), Scott, 2000). The National Joint Committee on Learning Disabil-
statistical (V. L. Wilson, 1987), developmental (Parrill, 1987), and ities (NJCLD) provided a well-thought-out critique of the use of
educational (Hessler, 1987) problems inherent in the use of an clinical judgment in the diagnosis of LD (NJCLD, 1997). The
ability–achievement discrepancy. In fact, some have asserted that committee concluded that “significant difficulty cannot be deter-
identifying ability–achievement discrepancies using rigid, quanti- mined solely by a quantitative test score” (p. 6), encouraging
tative formulas has absolutely no meaningful role in LD identifi- professionals to use both descriptive and quantitative information
cation (Bateman, 1992; Kavale, Kaufman, Naglieri, & Hale, during decision making. The key to accurate LD identification is
2005). Furthermore, the use of simple ability–achievement dis- the use of trained professional judgment (Bateman, 1992). Evalu-
crepancies results in overidentification of students with LD (Hoy ators require expertise to compare, contrast, and interpret the
et al., 1996; Kavale, Holdnack, & Mostert, 2005). obtained assessment results. In addition to norm-referenced tests,
Despite the aforementioned criticisms, the ease of using sim- evaluators need to consider such information as informal mea-
plistic discrepancy models has contributed to their overuse and sures; classroom portfolios; student, teacher, and parent reports;
misuse by both public school systems and the psychological com- and feedback from the multidisciplinary team. The decision to
munity (e.g., Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- identify an individual with LD involves the integration of various
ders [4th ed., text revision]; American Psychiatric Association, information sources, including that person’s developmental and
2000). The difficulty in developing a qualitative definition of LD, instructional history, medical and psychological history, family
combined with the need to make funding decisions, prompted and environmental factors, and test scores (Fletcher & Reschly,
school districts to rely on statistical methods to identify students 2005). Tests results can be an aid to judgment, but they should not
with LD (Silver & Hagin, 2002). What we can safely conclude be a substitute.
from past research and practice is that neither the presence nor the Defining and operationalizing LD requires the integration of
absence of an ability–achievement discrepancy is a reliable or several bodies of knowledge. Several years ago, Frith (1999)
valid indicator of LD. Use of a single global IQ score or a single provided a comprehensive three-level framework for integrating
aptitude–achievement discrepancy as the sole identification crite- the paradoxes these neurobiological disorders present. She dis-
rion is too simplistic and contributes to vagueness regarding the cussed the importance of considering the environmental, biologi-
LD concept (Kavale & Forness, 2003). cal, cognitive, and behavioral signs influencing the ability to learn
Under the new Individuals With Disabilities Education Im- specific tasks. Models of eligibility that incorporate multiple
provement Act (2004) regulations, states will no longer be required sources of information will lead toward more accurate diagnoses
to depend on a severe discrepancy between ability and achieve- and the implementation of successful interventions. In this multi-
ment for LD identification. Although states do not need to depend dimensional process, an evaluator must consider dynamics intrin-
on documentation of a severe discrepancy, the use of a discrepancy sic to the individual, as well as the extrinsic factors that affect
formula is not prohibited (see § 614 关b兴 关6兴 关A兴); however, this is performance (Schrank et al., 2005).
subject to change with the final regulations. As an alternative to
ability–achievement discrepancies, the use of intracognitive, intra- Concerns
achievement, and intraindividual discrepancies has been proposed
(Woodcock, 1984). These types of discrepancies have particular In contrast to the aforementioned points, several components of
relevance to LD because they help capture a person’s stronger and Dombrowski et al.’s (2004) proposed criteria are incongruent with
weaker abilities. By analyzing the pattern of strengths and weak- what we know about LD.
nesses that exists within a person or the intraindividual discrepan-
cies, one can begin to determine how specific differences influence LD as Synonymous for Developmental Delay or
functioning and academic performance. The person’s unique pat- Underachievement
tern of abilities then serves as the cornerstone for linking the
results of cognitive ability testing to meaningful instructional plans One major concern is the suggestion that the label developmen-
(Mather & Wendling, 2005). The new Individuals With Disabili- tal learning delay should replace the term LD. Research with
ties Education Improvement Act regulations allow evaluation children and adults has documented that individuals with LD have
teams to use intraindividual differences— explained as a pattern of specific developmental deficits, not delays (Francis, Shaywitz,
strengths and weaknesses in performance, achievement, or both, Stuebing, Shaywitz, & Fletcher, 1996; Shaywitz, 2003). Although
relative to intellectual development—as part of the process for we agree that LD often cause below average academic perfor-
making eligibility decisions for LD placement (see § 300.309 [a] mance, the term delay suggests that the condition is temporary,
[2] [ii]). These within-person discrepancies, however, also have outgrown, and can be overcome. LD are a lifelong condition.
some of the same problems as ability–achievement discrepancies if Although these impairments cause academic difficulties, a person
they are used as the sole criterion for disability determination with strong intellectual and linguistic abilities, motivation, and
LEARNING DISABILITIES 101

perseverance can appear to compensate for many of the debilitat- dation, whereas real-life observations of the individual totally
ing aspects. Effort and effective intervention can even result in contradict and negate this conclusion. This is precisely why psy-
adequate performance in some situations (e.g., reading and writing chologists do not diagnose mental retardation without completing
e-mail) but not in others (e.g., completing timed high-stakes tests a measure of adaptive functioning.
within the assigned time limits). In addition, procedures based simply on low achievement as the
The adult population is critical to our understanding of issues criterion would overidentify lower ability students. When compar-
surrounding the construct of LD. If Dombrowski et al. (2004) are ing the ability–achievement, intracognitive, intra-achievement,
correct in assuming that LD is a developmental delay, we should and cutoff models to identify LD, the latter significantly overiden-
see differences between adults with and without LD on measures tified the number of students with low cognitive ability (Brackett
of achievement but not on measures of cognitive and linguistic & McPherson, 1996; Hoy et al., 1996). Therefore, the proposed
processing. For example, adults with LD typically obtain lower cutoff standard score of 85 on an achievement test has the potential
scores than normally achieving peers on measures of phonological of significantly increasing the number of students identified as LD
awareness, rate, memory, and processing speed (Gregg, Bandalos, but not increasing the validity of the diagnoses. Some response-
Coleman, Davis, & Jimenez, in press; Ofiesh, Mather, & Russell, to-intervention (RTI) advocates want to reconstruct LD into a
2005; Shessel & Reiff, 1999). Adults with LD appear to use generic learning problem category instead of attempting to im-
different, often inefficient, cognitive and linguistic processes than prove the sensitivity and specificity of the diagnostic techniques
do their peers without disabilities (Gregg et al., in press). In (Kavale & Forness, 2003; Kavale, Kaufman, et al., 2005). Even
addition, the predictive relationships of cognitive and linguistic within use of an RTI model, to maintain the integrity of the LD
measures to reading and written language differ significantly category and reduce the number of false positives, cognitive as-
across the groups. These adults may learn to compensate, but they sessments are necessary for the students who do not respond to
do not outgrow their problems. By aligning LD with low academic treatments (Fuchs et al., 2003). In addition, a major criticism of the
performance or underachievement, one misstates the nature of the ability–achievement discrepancy criterion has been the unreliabil-
disability and limits an individual’s chances for equal access. ity of the diagnosis. RTI does not resolve the ambiguity. Alterna-
Although LD are a form of underachievement, they are not the tive methods of assessing responsiveness to treatment also produce
same as low achievement (Kavale, 2005; Kavale & Forness, 1995, different subsets of unresponsive children (Fuchs, Fuchs, & Comp-
2003; Scruggs & Mastropieri, 2002) and cannot be equated with ton, 2004).
below average academic functioning. An individual can be under- Furthermore, individuals with LD can obtain standard scores
achieving academically for a myriad of reasons (e.g., lack of within the average to high average range. As noted above, LD is
instruction, poor school attendance, affective factors, or health) not synonymous with underachievement. Through years of special
that have little or nothing to do with LD. education, intensive tutoring, and persistence, individuals with LD
can improve enough academically to appear to have compensated
Use of a Cutoff Score for Eligibility for their disability. This does not mean the disability is no longer
present, only that the person has managed to achieve a certain level
Dombrowski et al. (2004) suggested that the of academic success on some measures. The task demands and
subtest formats can also influence an individual’s performance.
identification of LD shall be based on a dual deficit in academic For instance, an individual with dyslexia might perform above a
achievement as demonstrated by (a) a nationally norm-referenced
standard score of 85 on an untimed reading test of single words or
measure of academic achievement (standard score about 85 or less
text. However, when time is relevant to the score, this same person
reflecting performance one standard deviation below the mean) and
(b) evidence of educational impairment (based on classroom grades, might achieve a score that is significantly below average. Individ-
CBA [curriculum-based assessment], and teacher reports or ratings). uals with LD generally obtain lower scores than typically achiev-
(p. 367) ing individuals on a variety of speeded tasks (Ofiesh et al., 2005).
Or, an individual with working memory problems may struggle on
Although we agree with the need for demonstration of educational a math test involving mental calculations but have little difficulty
impact, we do not support the use of arbitrary cutoffs or the use of on a paper-and-pencil test (Geary, 2003). For this reason, score
only one achievement measure for disability determination. Again, interpretation must always be embedded in knowledge of the type
this assumes that LD is an academic performance deficit and does of task, as well as a thorough clinical history of the individual.
not address the underlying cognitive or linguistic problems or the
person’s clinical history (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2001). In addition, Omission of Cognitive and Linguistic Processes
reliance on a single achievement measure is not supported by best
practice. Instead, focus should be placed on more accurate, mul- Within most definitions, LD is characterized as a disorder within
tidimensional ways of describing academic performance, identify- the basic psychological processes (U.S. Department of Education,
ing learning profiles, and determining appropriate individual in- 2002). Thus, it is important to align the definition with the methods
struction (Hale & Fiorello, 2004; Naglieri & Bornstein, 2003). A used to identify these individuals (Kavale, Kaufman, et al., 2005).
cutoff score with reliance only on achievement measures is cer- The definition has not been changed, so closer adherence to what
tainly not the solution. is stipulated is warranted (Kavale, Holdnack, & Mostert, 2005).
Professionals cannot arrive at valid LD identification through Because the definition describes the essence of the disorder, omis-
the strict interpretation and application of test scores. Clinical sion of documentation of cognitive and linguistic processing from
judgment is an essential component of all types of evaluations. For the eligibility criteria is of great concern. As Kavale, Kaufman, et
example, test scores may suggest that a person has mental retar- al. (2005) stated, “We believe children with ‘true’ SLD [specific
102 MATHER AND GREGG

learning disabilities] have cognitive deficits and integrities in the trouble translating the written code into the linguistic code that is
basic psychological processes, which often lead to academic fail- a critical process in reading. We have extensive information re-
ure” (p. 17). Thus, to differentiate LD from low achievement, one garding etiology, neurobiology, characteristics, and treatment.
must use cognitive assessments and examine differences within Thus, a diagnosis of dyslexia is rather straightforward when the
cognitive and linguistic functioning. major symptoms are present. In fact, Shaywitz (2003) stated that
Dombrowski et al. (2004) and others have raised concern re- “the diagnosis of dyslexia is as precise and scientifically informed
garding the existence of research to support the need for intellec- as almost any diagnosis in medicine” (p. 165). Standardized tests
tual and cognitive assessments. For example, Fletcher and Reschly of broad and specific cognitive processing abilities can help us
(2005) recently stated that at best, “meager evidence” (p. 15) exists identify individuals with LD if we examine the specific cognitive
to support the assessment of cognitive processes for LD identifi- and linguistic abilities that have established relationships with
cation or intervention. Although additional research is needed, academic performance (Hale & Fiorello, 2004; Mather & Wen-
findings continue to document, clarify, and increase our under- dling, 2005). Frith (1999), however, reminded us that words and
standings of the relationships among various cognitive abilities labels “readily become loaded with ideology while the concepts
and achievement. Dombrowski et al. and Fletcher and Reschly do they refer to may be perfectly non-contentious” (p. 193). There-
not acknowledge the vast amount of current research in measure- fore, whatever definition and/or eligibility models are adopted, the
ment and cognitive psychology that is relevant to the diagnosis of terms used to describe cognitive and linguistic processes must be
LD or the advances in multifactorial intellectual theory that pro- clearly defined.
vide a multidimensional view of intelligence (Kaufman & Kauf- A further consideration is that different cognitive and linguistic
man, 2001; Kavale, Kaufman, et al., 2005; McGrew, Flanagan, deficits impact different types of learning (academic, social, vo-
Keith, & Vanderwood, 1997). cational) depending on the age of the person, as well as the type of
Individuals with LD can be differentiated from low-achieving task (McGrew & Woodcock, 2001). Cutoff models as proposed by
peers on measures of cognitive processing (Kavale, Fuchs, & Dombrowski et al. (2004) do not account for developmental learn-
Scruggs, 1994). Theories such as CHC and Luria’s (1973, 1980) ing curves. For instance, the influence of the linguistic processes
neuropsychological theory are the foundation for several theory- (sublexical and lexical) that influence decoding and reading com-
based tests (e.g., Woodcock–Johnson III [Woodcock, McGrew, & prehension differ depending on the age and specific abilities of an
Mather, 2001]; Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Second individual (Gregg et al., in press). What also has been missing
Edition [Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004]) that also use advanced from many LD definitions and eligibility models is a framework
measurement theory (e.g., item-level response analysis). Results for organizing our thinking (Gregg, in press). Researchers at times
from current research provide evidence for the predictive validity focus on isolated abilities (e.g., phonological awareness, working
of cognitive processes to learning (e.g., Evans, Floyd, McGrew, & memory, reading decoding, written expression) while ignoring the
Leforgee, 2002; Flanagan & Kaufman, 2004; Floyd, Evans, & total system surrounding learning.
McGrew, 2003; Hale, Fiorello, Kavanagh, Hoeppner, & Gaitherer, Johnson and Myklebust (1967) provided one of the first dy-
2001). In addition, many researchers have identified specific cog- namic frameworks to organize the cognitive and linguistic abilities
nitive and linguistic processes as correlates of academic failure. influencing learning, which they labeled the hierarchy of experi-
Considerable research studies exist in the areas of cognition, ence. This early cognitive framework was an information-
language, learning, and measurement regarding the specific char- processing model based primarily on clinical experiences. Other
acteristics and needs of individuals with LD. cognitive models, such as that provided by CHC theory, provide
As examples, phonological and orthographic processes are empirical research based on statistical and logical analyses of
known to be critical for the development of reading and spelling hundreds of data sets, as well as additional sources of validity
skills (Adams, 1990; Gregg et al., in press; Vellutino, Fletcher, evidence, in the form of genetic, neurocognitive, developmental,
Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004); working memory exhibits strong and outcome studies (Horn & Noll, 1997). Without a framework or
connections with many areas of academic performance (Baddeley, blueprint, we may miss relationships directly and indirectly im-
1979, 1986, 2001; Baddeley, Vallar, & Wilson, 1987); both verbal portant for different types of learning.
and visual–spatial working memory tasks appear to be important Historically, the field of LD has been based on the belief that
predictors of math ability (K. M. Wilson & Swanson, 2001); slow individualized educational planning makes a difference because
rapid automatized naming tasks predict poor reading skill (Bowers individuals differ in their abilities and therefore should be taught
& Ishaik, 2003; Wolf & Bowers, 1999); slow cognitive efficiency and treated differently (Kavale & Forness, 1998). A child who has
and cognitive fluency are related to poor reading decoding and a weakness in phonological processing but intact oral vocabulary
spelling (Gregg et al., 2005; Shaywitz, 2003); and verbal abilities, has different instructional needs from the child with a limited
as well as acquired knowledge, have strong relationships with all vocabulary but adequate phonological awareness. Clearly, for stu-
aspects of reading and many aspects of mathematics (Evans et al., dents with LD, differential instruction that addresses the source of
2002; Floyd et al., 2003). the problem and the underlying processing demands is much more
Clearly, we know more about certain types of LD than others. effective than global, generalized approaches that do not (Aaron,
For example, considerable research has been conducted regarding 1997; Hale & Fiorello, 2004; Kavale, Kaufman, et al., 2005).
the relationship between phonological awareness and dyslexia, the Furthermore, Dombrowski et al. (2004) and others (e.g., Siegel,
most common type of LD. Shaywitz (2003) described dyslexia as 1989) suggest that intelligence tests should no longer be required
“unexpected difficulties in phonology and reading in relation to the for LD diagnosis except to rule out mental retardation. They
person’s other cognitive and academic abilities” (p. 335). Because indicate that the elimination of ability testing would also eliminate
of these phonological weaknesses, readers with dyslexia have the practice of diagnosing an individual with “gifted/LD.” The
LEARNING DISABILITIES 103

problem is that gifted individuals with LD really do exist. These deficient cognitive and linguistic processes that underlie the dis-
students may have high verbal aptitudes and reasoning abilities, order (Kavale, Holdnack, & Mostert, 2005; Kavale, Kaufman, et
but their performance is often compromised by their ease and rate al., 2005; Robinson, Menchetti, & Torgesen, 2002). Furthermore,
of production. A learning disability can be found in people of any valid diagnoses can be accomplished only by individuals who have
age who are at any level of intellectual functioning (Cruickshank, specialized training in LD, can analyze multiple data sources, and
1983; Shaywitz, 2003). To fail to identify these “otherwise gifted” can exercise clinical judgment.
individuals accurately can result in inappropriate treatments, lack Thus, the LD diagnostic process would include these
of understanding by parents and teachers, and denial of appropriate procedures:
accommodations and interventions. The end result is a restriction 1. Observe a limitation in one or more of the following areas of
of educational and vocational opportunities for the individual. achievement: reading (basic skills, fluency, or comprehension);
Clearly, use of a cutoff for low achievement obfuscates the iden- written language (basic skills, fluency, or expression); or mathe-
tification of gifted individuals with LD (Kaufman & Kaufman, matics (basic skills, fluency, or application). Rule out alternative
2001). In addition, the implementation of other models, such as explanations for the limitation (e.g., mental retardation, lack of
RTI, ignores students with academic aptitude in the top 10% who opportunity).
are able to remain on grade level in one area while excelling in 2. Document the limitation using multiple sources of data (e.g.,
most other academic areas (Reynolds, 2005). standardized or curriculum-based measurements using multiple
Norm-referenced cognitive assessments are needed to document test formats; response to intervention; teacher, student, and parent
the presence, nature, and severity of the disability (Schrank et al., reports; class work samples; and educational history).
2005), as well as the cognitive assets that can be used to improve 3. Identify the specific cognitive and/or linguistic correlates that
educational performance. For example, if an individual has aver- appear to be related to the identified area of underachievement or
age or above average verbal reasoning abilities but poor written relative difficulty. Rule out alternative explanations for the cogni-
expression, accommodations, such as oral testing, are likely to be tive or linguistic difficulties.
useful. This type of accommodation would not be needed, how- Within the above criteria, a couple of points need further ex-
ever, for an individual whose verbal reasoning abilities are in line planation. We have not included oral language as an area of
with his or her written language skills. In fact, it is virtually achievement. We think that the role of oral language processes
impossible to make an accurate diagnosis of LD without test data requires reconsideration in current LD definitions and eligibility
from a comprehensive evaluation that reveals a person’s strengths criteria because oral language encompasses an array of linguistic
and weaknesses (Kavale, Holdnack, & Mostert, 2005). processes that both support and impact the development of aca-
Critical to any criteria operationalizing the construct of LD is demic performance. In both the Individuals With Disabilities Ed-
the degree to which empirical research and theory support the ucation Act (1997) and the Individuals With Disabilities Education
validity of the sources. The Standards on Educational and Psycho- Improvement Act (2004), as well as the Dombrowski et al. (2004)
logical Testing (American Educational Research Association et al., proposal, oral language is placed as an achievement area similar to
1999) identify substantive, internal, and external validity as im- reading, writing, and mathematics.
perative to determining construct validity. A strong psychological Linguistic processes, as with cognitive processes, influence
theory enhances substantive (content) validity through the identi- functioning in achievement areas both directly and indirectly. In
fication of a well-bounded construct domain (Benson, 1998; fact, the boundaries between some cognitive and linguistic pro-
Sireci, 2005). The Dombrowski et al. (2004) model suggested for cesses are often ambiguous. Vygotsky (1962) referred to this
LD diagnosis provides no empirical research or theory to support interdependence of thought and language by using the term “verbal
the criteria, suggesting weak substantive, internal, and external thought” (p. 65). From a measurement perspective, recent research
validity. Current research investigating the cognitive, linguistic, has identified a high correlation between many cognitive and
and achievement performance of individuals with LD across the language measures, suggesting an artifact of shared variance of
life span is providing the substantive, internal, and external valid- underlying language processes present across achievement and
ity necessary for a diagnostic model. The outcome of this research cognitive measures (Floyd, Gregg, & Keith, 2005). Therefore, we
is identification of assessment information that can be even more suggest that oral language should be one of the constructs used to
predictive in determining specific interventions and accommoda- explain an individual’s difficulties in achievement, and that
tions effective with the different profiles subsumed under the achievement in turn refers to specific problems in aspects of
rubric of LD. reading, writing, or mathematics.
We have also separated academic areas into the subcomponents
Proposed Diagnostic Criteria of basic skills, fluency, and application. Some individuals with LD
have difficulty on timed tests, whereas others have more difficulty
As Dombrowski et al. (2004) have indicated, a need exists for with the acquisition of basic skills, such as decoding and spelling.
diagnostic criteria to help inform and guide diagnostic decision When these types of data are aggregated, the specific problems
making. Several researchers have developed operational defini- may be masked. Within this framework, a limitation is not equated
tions of LD that include documentation of both academic and with any set cutoff point of performance. One can determine a
cognitive difficulties and provide a comprehensive framework for limitation only by examining a person’s educational history and
LD diagnosis (e.g., Flanagan, Ortiz, Alfonso, & Mascolo, 2002; opportunities, all pertinent assessment results, the intensity and
Kavale & Forness, 2000). The first part of an LD evaluation is then duration of services that he or she has received, genetic factors, the
to determine an initial domain-specific classification in reading, unique pattern of strengths and weaknesses, and the types of
writing, or math (Stanovich, 1999); the next part is to identify the accommodations and interventions that have been used to maxi-
104 MATHER AND GREGG

mize performance. The essence of LD is documentation of the Evans, J. J., Floyd, R. G., McGrew, K. S., & Leforgee, M. H. (2002). The
cognitive and linguistic integrities in the presence of processing relations between measures of Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) cognitive
deficits that result in unexpected learning failure (Kavale, Kauf- abilities and reading achievement during childhood and adolescence.
man, et al., 2005). Clearly, the critical factor of accurate LD School Psychology Review, 31, 246 –262.
diagnosis rests with trained, competent professionals who are able Flanagan, D., & Kaufman, A. S. (2004). Essentials of WISC-IV assessment.
New York: Wiley.
to integrate quantitative and qualitative information to arrive at an
Flanagan, D. P., Ortiz, S. O., Alfonso, V. C., & Mascolo, J. T. (2002). The
accurate diagnosis. achievement test desk reference (ATDR): Comprehensive assessment
and learning disabilities. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
References Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Barnes, M., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J.,
Olson, R. K., et al. (2001, August). Classification of learning disabili-
Aaron, P. G. (1997). The impending demise of the discrepancy formula. ties: An evidence-based evaluation. Paper presented at the U.S. Depart-
Review of Educational Research, 67, 461–502. ment of Education LD Summit, Washington, DC.
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about Fletcher, J. M., & Reschly, D. J. (2005). Changing procedures for identi-
print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. fying learning disabilities: The danger of perpetuating old ideas. School
American Educational Research Association, American Psychological As- Psychologist, 59, 10 –15.
sociation, & National Council on Measurement in Education. (1999). Flowers, L., Meyer, M., Lovato, J., Wood, F., & Felton, R. (2000). Does
Standards for educational and psychological testing (2nd ed.). Wash- third grade discrepancy status predict the course of reading develop-
ington, DC: American Educational Research Association. ment? Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 49 –71.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
Floyd, R. G., Evans, J. J., & McGrew, K. S. (2003). Relations between
ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
measures of Cattell–Horn–Carroll (CHC) cognitive abilities and math-
Ayres, A. J. (1972). Southern California Sensory Integration Tests. Los
ematics achievement across the school-age years. Psychology in the
Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Schools, 40, 155–171.
Baddeley, A. D. (1979). Working memory and reading. In P. Kolers, E.
Floyd, R., Gregg, N., & Keith, T. (2005). Reading comprehension across
Wrolstad, & H. Boruma (Eds.), Processing of visible language (Vol. 1,
the lifespan. Unpublished manuscript.
pp. 32–56). New York: Plenum Press.
Francis, J. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher,
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University
J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading dis-
Press.
ability: A longitudinal individual growth curves analysis. Journal of
Baddeley, A. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psy-
Educational Psychology, 88, 3–17.
chologist, 56, 851– 864.
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K.
Baddeley, A. D., Vallar, G., & Wilson, B. (1987). Sentence comprehension
Patterson, J. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia, neuro-
and phonological memory: Some neuropsychological evidence. In M.
psychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–
Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of
330). London: Erlbaum.
reading (pp. 509 –529). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Frith, U. (1999). Paradoxes in the definition of dyslexia. Dyslexia: An
Barsch, R. H. (1967). Achieving perceptual motor efficiency. Seattle, WA:
Special Child Publications. International Journal of Research and Practice, 5, 192–214.
Bateman, B. (1992). Learning disabilities: The changing landscape. Jour- Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L., & Compton, D. (2004). Identifying reading disabil-
nal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 29 –36. ities by responsiveness-to-instruction: Specifying measures and criteria.
Benson, J. (1998). Developing a strong program of construct validation: A Learning Disability Quarterly, 27, 216 –227.
test anxiety example. Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice, Fuchs, D., Mock, D., Morgan, P. L., & Young, C. L. (2003).
34, 10 –16. Responsiveness-to intervention: Definitions, evidence, and implications
Berninger, V. W. (2001). Understanding the “lexia” in dyslexia: A multi- for the learning disabilities construct. Learning Disabilities Research
disciplinary team approach to learning disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, and Practice, 18, 157–171.
51, 23– 48. Gaddes, W. (1980). Learning disabilities and brain function: Neuropsy-
Bowers, P. G., & Ishaik, G. (2003). RAN’s contribution to understanding chological approach. New York: Springer-Verlag.
reading disabilities. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Geary, D. C. (2003). Learning disabilities in arithmetic: Problem-solving
Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 140 –157). New York: Guilford differences and cognitive deficits. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S.
Press. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 199 –212). New
Brackett, J., & McPherson, A. (1996). Learning disabilities diagnosis in York: Guilford Press.
post-secondary students: A comparison of discrepancy-based diagnostic Gregg, N. (in press). Thinking beyond: Learning disabilities and psycho-
models. In N. Gregg, C. Hoy, & A. Gay (Eds.), Adults with learning logical assessment. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment.
disabilities: Theoretical and practical perspectives (pp. 68 – 84). New Gregg, N., Bandalos, D., Coleman, C., Davis, M., & Jimenez, J. (in press).
York: Guilford Press. The dimensionality of phonological and orthographic awareness tasks
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic for college students with and without dyslexia and AD/HD. Journal of
studies. New York: Cambridge University Press. Developmental Neuropsychology.
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological Gregg, N., Hoy, C., Flaherty, D. A., Norris, P., Coleman, C., Davis, M., et
Bulletin, 40, 153–193. al. (2005). Decoding and spelling accommodations for postsecondary
Cruickshank, W. M. (1983). Learning disabilities: A neurophysiological students with dyslexia: It’s more than processing speed. Learning Dis-
dysfunction. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 16, 27–29. abilities: A Contemporary Journal, 3(2), 1–17.
Dombrowski, S. C., Kamphaus, R. W., & Reynolds, C. R. (2004). After the Gregg, N., & Scott, S. (2000). Definition and documentation: Theory,
demise of the discrepancy: Proposed learning disabilities diagnostic measurement, and the court. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 5–13.
criteria. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 35, 364 –372. Hale, J. B., & Fiorello, C. A. (2004). School neuropsychology: A practi-
Ehri, L. C. (1998). Grapheme–phoneme knowledge is essential for learning tioner’s handbook. New York: Guilford Press.
to read words in English. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word Hale, J. B., Fiorello, C. A., Kavanagh, J. A., Hoeppner, J. B., & Gaitherer,
recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 3– 40). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. R. A. (2001). WISC-III predictors of academic achievement for children
LEARNING DISABILITIES 105

with learning disabilities: Are global and factor scores comparable? Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychol-
School Psychology Quarterly, 16(1), 31–35. ogy. London: Penguin Books.
Hale, J. B., Naglieri, J. A., Kaufman, A. S., & Kavale, K. A. (2004). Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed.). New York:
Specific learning disability classification in the new Individuals With Basic Books.
Disabilities Education Act: The danger of good ideas. School Psychol- Lyon, G. R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia,
ogist, 58, 6 –13. 45, 3–27.
Hessler, G. L. (1987). Educational issues surrounding severe discrepancy. Mather, N., & Healey, W. C. (1990). Deposing aptitude–achievement
Learning Disabilities Research, 3, 43– 49. discrepancy as the imperial criterion for learning disabilities. Learning
Horn, J. L. (1991). Measurement of intellectual capabilities: A review of Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1(2), 40 – 48.
theory. In K. S. McGrew, J. K. Werder, & R. W. Woodcock, WJ–R Mather, N., & Wendling, B. (2005). Linking cognitive assessment results
technical manual (pp. 197–232). Itasca, IL: Riverside. to academic interventions for students with learning disabilities. In D. P.
Horn, J. L., & Noll, J. (1997). Human cognitive abilities: Gf-Gc theory. In Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assess-
D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft, & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary ment: Theories, tests, and issues (2nd ed., pp. 269 –294). New York:
intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 53–91). New Guilford Press.
York: Guilford Press. McGrew, K. S., & Flanagan, D. P. (1998). The intelligence test desk
Hoy, C., Gregg, N., Wisenbaker, J., Bonham, S., King, M., & Moreland, C. reference (ITDR): Gf-Gc cross-battery assessment. Boston: Allyn &
(1996). Clinical model versus discrepancy model in determining eligi- Bacon.
bility for learning disabilities services at a rehabilitation setting. In N. McGrew, K. S., Flanagan, D. P., Keith, T. Z., & Vanderwood, M. (1997).
Gregg, C. Hoy, & A. Gay (Eds.), Adults with learning disabilities: Beyond g: The impact of Gf-Gc specific cognitive abilities research on
Theoretical and practical perspectives (pp. 55– 67). New York: Guilford the future use and interpretation of intelligence tests in the schools.
Press. School Psychology Review, 26, 177–189.
Individuals With Disabilities Education Act Amendments of 1997, Pub. L. McGrew, K., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). WJ III technical manual. Itasca,
No. 105–117, 20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq. IL: Riverside.
Individuals With Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004, Pub. L. Naglieri, J. A., & Bornstein, B. T. (2003). Intelligence and achievement:
No. 108 – 446, 20 U.S.C. § 1462 (h). Just how correlated are they? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,
Johnson, D. J., & Myklebust, H. R. (1967). Learning disabilities: Educa-
21, 244 –260.
tional principles and practices. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Naglieri, J. A., Hale, J. B., Kaufman, A., & Kavale, K. A. (in press).
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2001). Assessment of specific learning
Implementation of IDEA: Using RTI and cognitive assessment methods.
disabilities in the new millennium: Issues, conflicts, and controversies.
Psychology in the Schools.
In A. S. Kaufman & N. L. Kaufman (Eds.), Specific learning disabili-
National Joint Committee on Learning Disabilities. (1997). Operational-
ties: Psychological assessment and evaluation (pp. 433– 461). Cam-
izing the NJCLD definition of learning disabilities for ongoing assess-
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
ment. Retrieved January 4, 2004, from http://www.ldonline.org/njcld/
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Assessment Battery
operatonalizing.html
for Children (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Ofiesh, N., Mather, N., & Russell, A. (2005). Using speeded cognitive,
Kavale, K. A. (1987). Theoretical issues surrounding severe discrepancy.
reading, and academic measures to determine the need for extended test
Learning Disabilities Research, 3, 12–20.
time among university students with learning disabilities. Journal of
Kavale, K. A. (2005). Identifying specific learning disability: Is respon-
Psychoeducational Assessment, 23, 35–52.
siveness to intervention the answer? Journal of Learning Disabilities,
Parrill, M. (1987). Developmental issues surrounding severe discrepancy.
38, 553–562.
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1995). The nature of learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research, 3, 32– 41.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Reynolds, C. R. (2005, August). Considerations in RTI as a method of
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1998). The politics of learning disabilities. diagnosis of learning disabilities. Paper presented at the 12th Annual
Learning Disability Quarterly, 21, 245–273. Institute for Psychology in the Schools of the American Psychological
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (2000). What definitions of learning Association, Washington, DC.
disabilities say and don’t say: A critical analysis. Journal of Learning Robinson, C. S., Menchetti, B. M., & Torgesen, J. K. (2002). Toward a
Disabilities, 33, 239 –256. two-factor theory of one type of mathematics disabilities. Learning
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (2003). Learning disabilities as a disci- Disabilities Research & Practice, 17, 81–90.
pline. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook Schrank, F. A., Teglasi, H., Wolf, I. L., Miller, J. A., Caterino, L. C., &
of learning disabilities (pp. 76 –93). New York: Guilford Press. Reynolds, C. R. (2005). American Academy of School Psychology reply
Kavale, K. A., Fuchs, D., & Scruggs, T. E. (1994). Setting the record to response-to-intervention perspective. School Psychologist, 59, 30 –33.
straight on learning disability and low achievement: Implications for Scruggs, T. E. (1987). Theoretical issues surrounding severe discrepancy:
policy-making. Learning Disability Research and Practice, 9, 70 –77. A discussion. Learning Disabilities Research, 3, 23–25.
Kavale, K. A., Holdnack, J. A., & Mostert, M. P. (2005). Responsiveness Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (2002). On babies and bathwater:
to intervention and the identification of specific learning disability: A Addressing the problems of identification of learning disabilities. Learn-
critique and alternative proposal. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28, ing Disability Quarterly, 25, 155–168.
2–16. Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia: A new and complete science-
Kavale, K. A., Kaufman, A. S., Naglieri, J. A., & Hale, J. (2005). Changing based program for reading problems at any level. New York: Knopf.
procedures for identifying learning disabilities: The danger of poorly Shessel, I., & Reiff, H. B. (1999). Experiences of adults with learning
supported ideas. School Psychologist, 59, 16 –25. disabilities: Positive and negative impacts and outcomes. Learning Dis-
Kephart, N. C. (1960). The slow learner in the classroom. Columbus, OH: ability Quarterly, 22, 305–316.
Charles E. Merrill. Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabil-
Kirk, S. A., McCarthy, J. J., & Kirk, W. D. (1968). Illinois Test of ities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 618 – 629.
Psycholinguistic Abilities (Rev. ed.). Urbana: University of Illinois Silver, A. A., & Hagin, R. A. (2002). Disorders of learning in childhood
Press. (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
106 MATHER AND GREGG

Sireci, S. (2005). Unlabeling the disabled: A perspective on flagging scores to a domain-general or a domain-specific working memory deficit?
from accommodated test administrations. Educational Researcher, Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 237–248.
34(1), 3–12. Wilson, V. L. (1987). Statistical and psychometric issues surrounding
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The sociopsychometrics of learning disabilities. severe discrepancy. Learning Disabilities Research, 3, 21–23.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 350 –361. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the
U.S. Department of Education. (2002). Specific learning disabilities: Find- developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–
ing common ground. Washington, DC: Author. 438.
Vellutino, F. R. (2001). Further analysis of the relationship between Woodcock, R. W. (1984). A response to some questions raised about the
reading achievement and intelligence: A response to Naglieri. Journal of Woodcock–Johnson: Efficacy of the aptitude clusters. School Psychol-
Learning Disabilities, 34, 306 –310. ogy Review, 13, 355–362.
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). The Woodcock–
Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past Johnson III. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2– 40.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Received March 2, 2005
Press. Revision received September 7, 2005
Wilson, K. M., & Swanson, H. L. (2001). Are mathematics disabilities due Accepted October 14, 2005 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like