Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 104

University of South Florida

Scholar Commons

Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School

2005

The Effect of Conducting Gesture on Expressive-Interpretive


Performance of College Music Majors
Ronald Wayne Gallops
University of South Florida

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd

Part of the American Studies Commons

Scholar Commons Citation


Gallops, Ronald Wayne, "The Effect of Conducting Gesture on Expressive-Interpretive Performance of
College Music Majors" (2005). Graduate Theses and Dissertations.
https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/2885

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has
been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar
Commons. For more information, please contact scholarcommons@usf.edu.
The Effect of Conducting Gesture

on Expressive-Interpretive

Performance of College Music Majors

by

Ronald Wayne Gallops

A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment


of the requirements for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy
School of Music
College of Visual and Performing Arts
University of South Florida

Major Professor: Jack Heller, Ph.D.


David Williams, Ph.D.
Michael Robinson, D.M.A.
Sheila Woodward, Ph.D.

Date of Approval:
June 16, 2005

Keywords: Music, Conducting, Gesture, Interpretation, Perception

 Copyright 2005, R. Wayne Gallops


Dedication

There are numerous friends, mentors, and colleagues who have contributed to my

musical and academic growth. I am indebted to countless music teachers and students of

the Hillsborough County Public Schools, the University of Tampa, Hillsborough

Community College, Florida State University, the University of South Florida, and

Radford University who have offered encouragement and support. In particular, I thank

Jeanine Romano and Dr. Justin Askins for their statistical and editorial assistance. I am

also grateful to the late Dr. Virginia Bridges, who provided an opportunity to pursue

doctoral studies in music education through a fellowship offered in her name. Without the

creative vision of Dr. John Richmond and the diligent guidance of Dr. Jack Heller, Dr.

Sheila Woodward, Dr. David Williams, and Dr. Michael Robinson, this study would not

exist.

I am blessed with wonderful family members who have on many occasions,

provided moral and financial support for my academic endeavors. In particular, my wife

Donna, mother Janice, and grandmother Annie Lois Lee. I wish to dedicate this

dissertation to the memory of my late grandmother Annie Lois Lee, in honor of her

generosity and love of music.


TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES iii

LIST OF FIGURES v

ABSTRACT vi

CHAPTER I: Introduction 1
Perception of Musical Expression and Stylistic Interpretation 4

CHAPTER II: Review of Literature 12


The Perception Contract 12
Conductor Effect on Musician Expression 16
Conducting Gesture Studies 20
Assessing Conducting Gesture 25

CHAPTER III: Research Procedures 29


The Research Question 29
Sampling Procedures 31
Procedure 32
Data Analysis 34
Measurement 36

CHAPTER IV: Data Analysis 38


Introduction 38
Process 38
Participants 39
Data Collection 41
Panel Evaluation 44
Inter-rater Reliability 49
Analysis of Data 50
Other Observations 65
Summary 67

i
CHAPTER V: Discussion 68
Educational Impact 71
Future Research 73
Synthesis of Conducting Domains 75

REFERENCES 77

APPENDICES
Appendix A: Demographics of Conducting Participants i
Appendix B: Musician Nesting With Conductor Videos ii
Appendix C: Unmarked Musical Excerpt iii
Appendix D: Marked Versions A & B of the excerpt iv
Appendix E: Sample of Marked and Evaluated Excerpts A & B
and Description v
Appendix F: Musician Response Scores for each Conductor Nest vi
Appendix G: Evaluation of Post (W/Conductor) Musician Responses ix

ABOUT THE AUTHOR End Page

ii
LIST OF TABLES

Table 1 Mean performance ratings and standard deviations


by body type and evaluator gender 18

Table 2 Mean scores of instructional conditions and dynamic


instruction by group 20

Table 3 Conducting participant demographics 40

Table 4 Conductor video performance evaluations


of the four-member panel 46

Table 5 Evaluations of musician baseline performances 47

Table 6 Musician mean response scores for each conductor 48

Table 7 GRI total scores 49

Table 8 Conductor Ranking: categorized based on GRI gesture score 51

Table 9 Conductor level and GRI response scores 54

Table 10 Conductor level and GRI response scores for each version (A&B) 54

Table 11 GRI Musician Response: ANOVA Procedure for conductor level


and excerpt 55

Table 12 Group means for conductor level and baseline-post


performances for each excerpt version 58

Table 13 ANOVA procedure for baseline-with conductor, conductor level,


and excerpt 58

Table 14 Baseline and Post GRI response scores for high and low level
conductors 59

iii
Table 15 ANOVA procedure for baseline-with conductor, conductor level,
and excerpt 60

Table 16 GRI Gesture: ANOVA procedure for conductor level and


excerpt (A & B) 63

Table 17 GRI Total: rank of conductors high to low 64

Table 18 Means for instrument type and response per excerpt 64

Table 19 ANOVA procedure for instrument type 65

iv
LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. GRI scoring box 27

Figure 2. Nesting procedure 34

Figure 3. Creation of the GRI Scores 36

Figure 4. Melodic excerpt (unmarked) 42

Figure 5. The Evaluation Process: sample of a highlighted copy


of excerpt A 45

Figure 6. Conductor and excerpt interaction for musician GRI response


Score 56

Figure 7. GRI Response score of high and low ability conductors with
excerpt 57

Figure 8. Excerpt A: musician baseline and post performance-responses 61

Figure 9. Excerpt B: musician baseline and post performance-responses 62

v
The Effect of Conducting Gesture
on Expressive-Interpretive
Performance of College Music Majors

Ronald Wayne Gallops

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of non-verbal conducting

gesture on musicians’ stylistic response and whether conducting gestures alone elicit

consistent musical responses from musicians. Through an analysis that utilized a Gestural

Response Instrument (GRI) it was determined that, even if the use of verbal and facial

cues were eliminated, some experienced conductors successfully utilized non-verbal

conducting gestures to communicate specific musical interpretations. It appeared that

musicians responded in specific ways to the musical interpretation of conductors who had

command of a variety of conducting gestures. The results illustrated the existence of a

perceptual contract that facilitates the non-verbal communication expressed through

gestural conducting. As demonstrated through this study, some experienced conductors

lacked the gestural technique and vocabulary necessary to convey prescribed musical

decisions while others were proficient in this area.

vi
The Effect of Gesture

CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION

This study examines how conductors utilize non-verbal gesture to communicate

stylistic interpretation that extends beyond accurate performance of notes and rhythms.

Many factors influence a conductor’s ability to communicate stylistic interpretation and

the musician’s response to the conductor’s communication. Musicians respond to the

musical interpretation of conductors beyond precision of notes and rhythms through a

process that involves nonverbal conducting gestures. This musical response to nonverbal

communication may illustrate a type of social-cultural contract among musicians.

Modern conducting technique, including the use of a baton, is a comparatively

recent musical development. Musical compositions of the late nineteenth century and the

continued increase in complexity and size of symphonic scores created an evolution of

modern conducting technique from mere time-beating to a highly complex and diverse

craft. “The problems that the contemporary conductor has to face are very different from

those of, say, fifty years ago...[there is] general demand for a degree of clarity, precision,

and smoothness of orchestral performance undreamed of even as recently as Richard

Wagner’s time" (Szell in Rudolf, 1950, p.vii).

1
The Effect of Gesture

The baton in its present form is the end result of hundreds of years of
conductorial experimentation. The earliest conducting was done with
gestures of the hand alone, describing melodic contour and pitches,
lengths of notes and phrases. Passing through stages where the leader sat
at the organ with his figured bass part and directed [therefrom]; where the
beat was an audible [prone], thumping along with the music; where the
concertmaster of the orchestra stood up and directed with his bow, and
finally where the silent beat with the baton arrived, conducting has now
become, through the use of the stick and its skilled sign-language, an art of
ultimate refinement (Green, 1961, p.7).

Three important domains that contribute to effective conducting can be identified:

1) knowledge and conceptualization of the score, 2) pedagogical ability to coach toward a

common interpretation, and 3) communication of interpretation through verbal and non-

verbal means (Wilcox, 2003). All three of these domains are perceived through the

mutual understanding of a common musical perception framework, a “social-cultural

contract” (Heller & Campbell, 1981). The historical and declarative elements of the score

contribute to a musical framework of informed intuition. Pedagogical knowledge and the

ability to perceive and shape musical sounds, based on a preconceived notion of the

score, contribute to conducting effectiveness. The conductor’s ability to communicate his

or her desires through a combination of verbal and nonverbal language completes the

conductor’s charge. Strength in all three domains should be the goal of a fine conductor.

Rarely is this goal achieved. The historical and theoretical requirements of score study,

the technical knowledge required of pedagogy, the personality aspects of verbal

communication, and the physical ability to communicate musical meaning through a

vocabulary of gestures are the elements that blend together on the conductor’s podium.

2
The Effect of Gesture

Imagine a clarinetist presenting a lecture/clinic at a professional meeting. After a

lengthy and rigorous discussion of the Mozart Clarinet Concerto K.622, the individual

begins to perform the piece. Given the performer’s vast knowledge of the piece, the

audience anticipates an extraordinary and beautiful performance. Instead, they are treated

to squeaks and squawks indicative of a beginning clarinetist. This clarinetist probably has

not developed the technical and procedural abilities necessary for communicating a

professional musical interpretation.

How typical it is that a performer “knows” much “about” a musical work but

cannot perform it very well. How often are fine musicians subjected to the “frumps” and

“flaps” of an extremely knowledgeable, yet gesturally deficient conductor? Many

suggest that because there are so many “gesturally deficient” conductors, musicians tend

not to look toward the podium (Berlioz, 1970). Some have implied that conducting

gestures have little or no meaning, particularly for professional musicians.

As it is, musicians tend to declare themselves conductors by merely


announcing as much to the world and ironically, sadly, the world generally
accepts the pronouncement without question, regardless of whether the
particular individual has conducting talents, regardless of whether the
individual has technical, intellectual, and emotional capacities to translate
a musical score into an appropriate interpretation via appropriate
conductorial gestures (Schuller, 1997, p.5-6).

An opposing view, as disseminated by many conducting pedagogues, is that a

well-rounded conductor, who possesses a non-verbal gestural vocabulary that is

3
The Effect of Gesture

consistent with an underlying agreement among musicians, is able to energize and bring

to the ensemble performance a heightened and consistent interpretation.

Noted conducting teacher, Elizabeth Green, suggests that when a “conductor's

impulse of will is strong and the technique is secure, then the ensemble truly has a leader

who can unify the musicianship of all into one secure interpretation. Such a conductor

has the finely developed technical skill and the confident drive to convey by his gestures

exactly what he wants without confusion or misunderstanding on the part of the players”

(1961, p.60).

Absent the domains of score study, pedagogy, and the sub-domain of verbal

communication, one can conclude that it is the ability to communicate through nonverbal

gesture that separates the conductor from the music historian, theorist, instrumentalist,

vocalist, or teacher. This study will focus on the effect of non-verbal conducting gestures,

absent facial cues, on the interpretive responses of instrumental performers.

Perception of Musical Expression and Stylistic Interpretation

The common ground that the performer (musician and conductor) and listener

have in terms of perception is the social-cultural contract. This social-cultural contract is

the musical environment in which one is raised. A common perception of musical

sounds within this musical environment is the foundation upon which conductors make

interpretive decisions. It is also the foundation from which conductors can effectively

communicate with other musicians in terms of stylistic interpretation. Absent this

4
The Effect of Gesture

common ground, transmission of musical ideas and the translation of those ideas into

gesture might be impossible. A discussion of conductor ability to convey expressive-

interpretive elements should be framed within an examination of this perception contract.

According to Dowling in Tighe (1993), musical understanding is a procedural

process developed through the cultural-historical context of one’s society. That is to say,

we learn about and come to understand music through acts of listening and performing

(Elliott, 1995). The social-cultural contract determines comprehension of musical

patterns through a set of implicit rules (Heller and Campbell, 1981). These implicit rules

are socially and culturally based, forming a common surface organization. The implicit

rules enable performers and listeners to organize musical sounds in a meaningful way and

collectively perceive nuance and interpret style. “Comprehension of different musical

languages and their respective nuances require listeners who are equipped with different

sets of implicit rules for recognizing and dealing with the surface structure features and

cultural performance conventions unique to each of those languages” (Fiske, 1990, p.6).

Through the social-cultural contract, musicians are able to evoke a musical style. If the

listener is also steeped in the same social-cultural milieu, then perception and musical

style can be realized as nearly the same. The implicit or procedural nature through which

we perform and perceive music is elaborated in the work of Heller and Campbell :

Perceptual learning in music probably involves an ability (similar to that


of speech) to detect those specific properties, patterns, and distinctive
features of the musical environment that are necessary for
communication. As in language perception, the rules necessary for
success of this active content dependent perceptual process are
implicit...and form a social/cultural contract between performer and

5
The Effect of Gesture

listener [that provides the context in which the acoustic signal is decoded]
(1981, p.21).

If the understanding and perception of music is so strongly based in an

internalized (procedural) social-cultural contract, what is the role of score study or

theoretical analysis in the preparation of musical interpretation? Perhaps the general

principles of form, harmonic structure, etc., that consume theoretical analysis are not key,

or as key, as one might think. For Leonard Meyer, “it is style and the musician’s grasp of

style that determines the framework” (1974, p.30). Perhaps it is the experiential knowing

through interaction with musical sounds that develops expression and stylistic intuition in

the musician. The artist “employs the rules and regularities of a prevalent paradigm -- the

grammar, syntax, and formal procedures of an existing style -- in order to create an

original pattern of particulars: a work of art” (Meyer, 1974, p13).

Understanding music is a process that may not be constrained by external (non-

musical) factors. According to Meyer, “the artists need not have conceptualized the rules

he uses let alone be able to explain them. His ‘knowledge’, based on practice and

experience, may be tacit” (1974, p13). Style is therefore predicated through intuition that

is developed within, and is the end product of the social-cultural-historical experience.

Information about form, dynamic contrast, tonal and harmonic relationships, and the

correct combination of pitch and rhythm are deciphered from the musical score.

However, these are not the complete elements of style in performance. How crucial is it

6
The Effect of Gesture

that performers have command of such “declarative knowledge” (Dowling in Tighe,

1993) of the score?

But were explanation a prerequisite for understanding, Bach might not


have been able to compose or Shakespeare to write. For, while both
supremely understood what they were doing, they might not have been
able to explain it. In short, composers, writers, and artists are especially
sensitive, skillful, and imaginative understanders of tonal, verbal, and
visual relationships. But much of their knowledge is tacit rather than
conceptual (Meyer, 1974, p.29).

Historical information might offer helpful anecdotes that contribute to the

performer’s overall knowledge of a particular musical composition. This information has

little to do with style, other than recognizing an already defined style period in which the

composition was constructed.

Style is the artist’s point of departure. It guides, but does not determine the
choices he [the composer] makes. Styles are complex probability
“systems” within which the artist chooses among alternative possibilities -
- that is, in terms of which he invents and combines patterns, develops and
resolves tensions, devises and elaborates formal relationships. For if the
rules specified precisely what was to be done, all choices would be
preordained, and originality would be impossible (Meyer, 1974, p.13).

The question begs, does the score specify precisely what is to be done? Through

analysis, musicians are able to develop declarative (theoretical) knowledge about music.

While Meyer and others assert that this knowledge has some, but not complete impact on

stylistic interpretation, developing declarative knowledge does contribute to refinement

of one’s procedural knowledge. Declarative knowledge develops informed intuition. This

7
The Effect of Gesture

informed intuition enables the musician to perceive complex relationships and elements

within the score and in musical sound.

Theoretical efforts to explain music tend to define the general aspects of

compositional structure and style. “Three kinds of hypotheses, used to explain works of

art, may be distinguished: (a) general laws, which are presumed to be constant over time

and space; (b) restricted principles, derived from and applicable to the norms and

procedures of a specific style; and (c) ad hoc reasons, which are necessary adjuncts to the

first two types when particular works of art are being explained” (Meyer, 1974, p.32).

So, there are general aspects to style that can be readily identified through score study.

Those aspects of the musical score constitute the dimensions that combine to form an

overall impression of the piece, but not the detailed stylistic interpretative dimensions

that create exceptional performance. This general, or macro dimension of the score, does

not in itself propagate the musician / conductor’s stylistic interpretation.

There must be a micro aspect of style that is not found in the score, but rather

through procedural knowledge, the social-cultural contract, and acoustic sound itself. It

is through the micro aspects of the score that conductors seek personal interpretation,

converting those musical notions into physical gesture.

Style is not only manipulated in the patterns that lie on the very surface (macro)

but at a deeper level (micro). “The formulations of style analysis are to general principles

as the taxonomy of species is to an hypothesis about their evolution or a theory of their

physiology” (Meyer, 1974, p.13).

8
The Effect of Gesture

Once a musical work is placed in its general taxonomy, the issues quickly become

micro. Therefore, musicians manipulate the acoustic sounds themselves in order to create

an interpretation that has been formulated through informed intuition. Decisions

musicians make in terms of shaping notes and fragments of phrases create stylistic

interpretations that can be perceived by audiences of similar cultural - stylistic

experiences. Lewis agrees with Meyer’s ideas on how musical style is realized. She

points out that:

A variety of detailed nuance in a musical composition is provided by


looking to the internal sublevels of interpretations and keeping them in
relationship with the whole. These internal sublevels of interpretation are
the minuscule levels of detail inside the phrase -- in the measure. They are
the tiniest levels of stress and restraint: acceleration and hesitation; weight
given a particular entrance or a final consonant; dynamic contour given
the little melodic motif, rhythmic twist, or harmonic detail; and attention
given to salient characteristics (Lewis, 1999, p.79).

The levels of detail discussed in Lewis’ (1999) work (stress, restraint, weight,

contour, etc.), actualize through the manipulation of note entrance, body, and release. The

musicians’ and listeners’ ability to perceive subtle inflections, and denote similar

meanings to those inflections, may be the essence of style.

The perception of microstructures, in the minutest detail, is verified through

Campbell and Heller’s work. Campbell and Heller (1979) demonstrate that the human

mind can quickly classify the timbral elements of sound. They (Campbell and Heller)

utilized this concept of microstructures in assessing listener identification of musical

instruments (timbre). “The traditional segmentation of a musical tone consists of the

9
The Effect of Gesture

attack transient, the steady state [body], and the decay transient [release]. We [Campbell

and Heller] have found it necessary to add the legato transient to this list, so that the

distinct character of the juncture in the two note sequence...can be indicated ” (1979,

p.21). How those fundamental variables of musical sound are manipulated and perceived

is the embodiment of stylistic interpretation. The challenge for the conductor is to

communicate how he or she wishes to manipulate subtle changes in these fundamental

variables.

The communication of musical expression from conductor to performer may be

reliant on the mutual perception of musical meaning as relayed through tension, release,

weight, and contour of note entrance, body, release and legato transient (between notes).

Conductors translate these perceived musical meanings into visual representations that

are, in turn, perceived by performers through an implicit mutual social agreement. This

social agreement may be referred to as a musician-conductor perception contract.

Through this performance agreement between conductors and musicians, a common

perception of musical meaning (style) and visual representation through conducting

gesture consistently manifests itself among trained musicians. The agreement of

conducting gesture may exist at a higher (or lesser) degree than commonly

acknowledged. Conducting pedagogue Elizabeth Green applied the term psychological

conducting to the idea that conducting involves a “transfer of ideas from the conductor’s

mind to the performer’s mind through the medium of correct and precise conductorial

10
The Effect of Gesture

technique without the use of verbal directions or written notation” (1961, p.241).

Through an experimental design, this study will investigate conductor ability to

communicate stylistic interpretation through conducting gestures alone (psychological

conducting) and examine possible commonalities that contribute to effective conducting

gesture.

11
The Effect of Gesture

CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

The transformation of musical meaning into visual representation is the purpose

of the effective conductor. “A simple definition of the art of conducting could be that it

involves eliciting from the orchestra [any ensemble] with the most appropriate minimum

of conductorial (if you will, choreographic) gestures a maximum of accurate acoustical

results” (Schuller, 1997, p.9). This transformation of musical meaning into visual

representation may hinge on the existence of a perception contract that develops out of

the social-cultural contract of the listener. Research has shown that through musical

training, musical / acoustical perception is increased, thereby enabling the development

of a perception contract among musicians (Burnsed, 2001; Dowling, 1993; Fiske, 1990;

Fredrickson, 1997; Holahan & Saunders, 1997).

The Perception Contract

The existence of commonalities in listener perception of the interpretive aspects

of musical performance has been examined through numerous studies. The

communication of interpretive and stylistic decisions through conducting technique may

be reliant on a common music perception framework. Frederickson (1997) explored

questions of music perception in an experiment involving elementary, middle, and high

12
The Effect of Gesture

school students. In particular, do younger and older populations respond similarly to

perceived tension in music? Participants in the study were 2nd, 5th, 8th, 11th and 12th

graders. Participants from grades two and five were selected randomly from intact classes

at a suburban elementary school in a Midwest metropolitan area. Grade eight participants

were randomly selected from a middle school choir of the same metro area. Those from

grades 11 and 12 were randomly selected from a high school band of the same metro

area. The Continuous Response Digital Interface (CRDI), a device for measuring

electromotive forces, mounted on a dial and interfaced with a computer, was used to

collect data. Participants were asked to turn the dial to the right, as they perceived an

increase in degrees of tension in the music listening example.

Mean group responses were calculated for each grade level, once per second.

Once these data were calculated, Pearson correlations were calculated to compare the age

groups to each other. The data from this study were also compared to data of previous

studies that examined perceived tension in music and utilized the same procedures with

adult listeners. The results showed high positive correlations between groups of all age

levels as they responded to tension in music (r =. 86 - .98). When these results were

compared with a previous study (Madsen & Fredrickson, 1993) using adult subjects

(utilizing the same methods and procedures), high correlations were observed. The lowest

correlation was between the 2nd graders and adult musicians (r =. 71, p≤ .001).

13
The Effect of Gesture

While Fredrickson's study does not identify specific variables that elicit

perceptions of tension in music such as dynamic change, harmonic structure, dissonance,

etc., it is interesting to note that even young children have the capability of perceiving

interpretive variables beyond notes and rhythm, and that their ability to do so becomes

more profound with age and musical training. One must examine the results of these

studies with caution. Without reporting the variability of subject responses, the CRDI

Means could be misleading. The standard deviations of scores as well as the means

should always be reported. Without the standard deviations, correlations might give the

impression of much higher relationships than warranted.

Experiments reported by Dowling (1993) focused on ability to perceive tonal

transposition and preserve interval patterns in semitones. Musically experienced (older)

and musically inexperienced (younger) participants were selected and results of

their responses to listening patterns were recorded and compared to age/ability through

ANCOVA. The participants’ ability to identify interval patterns and tones as recorded

tone rows were altered was measured as the dependent variable. It was concluded that

five years of music lessons, at an early age, was sufficient to increase listening ability

in terms of identifying tonality and tonal-scale values. The “Moderately” and

“Professionally” trained group did not display enough difference to be statistically

significant. Dowling’s results suggest that music educators should emphasize procedural

learning so that students can gain acquired listening skills in the domain functions needed

for music cognition.

14
The Effect of Gesture

If a perception contract exists among listeners who hold a similar social-cultural

contract, musicians who grow from within the same contract may develop a refined

perception of stylistic interpretations. If musical training further develops the music

perception contract, conductors might gain access to that perception contract through

targeted methods. As musicians increase their ability to perceive interpretive variables,

conductors may gain stronger influence over the interpretive performance of musicians.

Common interpretive decisions communicated through common gestures that have

meaning to conductors and musicians might be instilled through the music training

process. Dowling’s study (1993) suggests that the ability to interpret subtle expressive

changes in music seems to be enhanced with musical training.

Like a phonetic alphabet, common listening skills enable the conductor to elicit

consistent and subtle responses from musicians. Although fundamental commonalities of

music perception can be demonstrated, Burnsed (2001) also found that it is developed

with age and musical training. The purpose of Burnsed’s study was to determine the

effects of expressive variation in dynamics on the musical preferences of middle school

and elementary school students. The populations from which subjects were sampled

included elementary students, middle school students, and music teachers. Two groups

were created and compared in this study. Group one included 288 students randomly

selected from grades 1-5 in an elementary school in Virginia. Group two

included 78 middle school music students and 22 choral and band directors participating

in a summer music camp.

15
The Effect of Gesture

Data reflecting subject preference for dynamic versus non-dynamic nuance were

reported through reliability coefficients (alpha) and Chi-Square preference significance

tests. These analyses showed that elementary students did not perceive a difference

between subtle expressions and non-expressive versions. The ninth grade students were

much more consistent in their preferences, as were the music teachers. While

specific musical variables were not identified, the results suggest that age and musical

training have impact an on perception of subtle expression in music. It could be,

however, that children may be able to discriminate, but they cannot label or effectively

use the CRDI.

Burnsed’s (2001) study, and the work of Fredrickson (1997), and Dowling (1993)

relate to the degree that conductors are able to control the expressive elements of a

musician’s performance and the degree to which listeners perceive elements of

expression in music performance. A technically skilled conductor may present a fine

visual representation of a musical passage, but if the musician performing the passage has

not developed an ability to perceive and perform subtle expression, the interpretation, as

communicated by the conductor, may not transpire.

Conductor Effect on Musician Expression

VanWeelden (2002) investigated the impact of conductor personal appearance on

the perceived quality of ensemble performance. Undergraduate music majors (N = 163)

were asked to complete a conductor effectiveness questionnaire after viewing a series of

16
The Effect of Gesture

videotaped conductor performances. Sixty-eight of the music major participants

(evaluators) were choral majors and ninety-eight were instrumental majors. The

conductor effectiveness questionnaire used a Likert type scale (1-5 point response) and

asked subjects to evaluate aspects of conductor effectiveness through eye contact, facial

expression, posture and overall impression. Videotapes of six female conductors

representing two different body types, ectomorphic /thin and endomorphic/large,

functioned as the independent variable. These conductors were videotaped in various

performance venues. Each conductor presented her best effort at conducting a

professional recording of a vocal work (The Coolin, by Samuel Barber). An analysis of

variance (ANOVA) of the participant responses determined that there was no significant

difference between endomorphic and ectomorphic conductors as judged by the students.

Table 1 displays the mean performance ratings and standard deviations by body type

found in VanWeelden’s study. These performance ratings reflect scores from the seven

questions that pertain to ensemble conducting effectiveness. Each male and female

evaluator produced scores within the range of 7 to 35 points for each endomorphic or

ectomorphic conductor

17
The Effect of Gesture

Table 1

Mean Performance Ratings and Standard Deviations by Body Type and Evaluator
Gender

Endomorphic Conductors Ectomorphic Conductors

Mean SD Mean SD

Male evaluators 24.12 3.08 24.29 3.36

Female evaluators 23.64 2.51 24.51 2.76

These findings suggest that musicians do not perceive conductor effectiveness based on

appearance, more specifically, body type. With this in mind, the assessment of conductor

communication and effectiveness should move beyond personal appearance and focus on

verbal and non-verbal behaviors.

Skadsem's (1997) study concerning the effect of conductor communication

addressed singers’ responses to verbal, written, and gestural-conducting stimuli. The

populations of interest for this study were choral conductors, college level singers, and

high school level singers. Ninety-six graduate and undergraduate music students and 48

high school music students from three metropolitan high schools surrounding a university

were pooled as participants. Data for the study were recorded on audiotape, videotape,

and on a written response questionnaire.

Singers were asked to perform a folk song (Michael Row Your Boat To Shore)

once with no conductor / written / audio cues, and eight times while viewing a conductor

18
The Effect of Gesture

and choir on a video monitor. Once an initial performance of the song (in its unmarked

version) was recorded, instructions regarding dynamic level were given for the remaining

eight performances. The dynamic level instructions were provided on the video screen

either through written cues, verbal commands, conducting gestures, or increase-decrease

in video audio volume (choir). In an effort to quantify the dynamic contrasts exhibited in

each performance, a panel of three expert conductors used the CRDI (Continuous

Response Digital Interface) to analyze audiotapes of individual vocal performances. An

average score for each excerpt performance was computed across the three judges. CRDI

data for the baseline (first performance with no cues) and the eight cued performances

were then compared in order to calculate a total change score. All of these data were

analyzed using an analysis of variance (ANOVA) and a post-hoc Tukey multiple

comparison test. These procedures of quantitative analysis enabled the researchers to

determine differences among groups and performances.

The data suggested that singers did respond differently to the four instructional

conditions that were presented: verbal commands, conducting gestures, written cues, and

increase-decrease of the choral audio. It was found that verbal commands made the

largest difference in soliciting dynamic response. Results suggested that singers respond

differently in loud and soft passages, more strongly reacting to instructions regarding

loud dynamics. The data also showed that the three groups (conductors, college students,

and high school students) responded differently to instructions about dynamics. The mean

scores of all instructional conditions by group are illustrated in Table 2.

19
The Effect of Gesture

Table 2

Mean Scores of Instructional Conditions and Dynamic Instruction by Group (Skadsem,


1997)

Condition Conductors College Singers HS Singers


Verbal soft 54.63 49.04 56.48
Verbal loud 51.38 47.38 50.67
Written soft 36.81 21.56 34.58
Written loud 18.54 12.17 20.48
Gesture soft 46.06 29.06 34.44
Gesture loud 39.23 21.10 4.06
Chorus soft 35.04 27.65 46.04
Chorus loud 14.04 24.83 28.71

The Skadsem study highlights the possibility that verbal communication may be

the most effective form of conductor communication. However, for more advanced

musicians, conducting gesture was a close second. The study suggests that ability to

communicate verbally is an important conducting skill and non-verbal behaviors,

specifically conducting gestures, also communicate musical ideas within the framework

of a musical social-cultural contract.

Conducting Gesture Studies

Nonverbal communication has been an area of psychological and educational

study for some time. Bulwer’s (1654) study of gestures used in public speaking is cited as

the earliest documentation of effective nonverbal communication. Numerous

psychological/educational studies affirm the need for effective nonverbal communication

skills in the classroom and music rehearsal. A more focused aspect of music education

20
The Effect of Gesture

research, as it relates to nonverbal communication, attempts to define conducting gesture

beyond verbal communication as nonverbal facial, body, hand and finger movement.

Efforts have been initiated to classify specific nonverbal conducting behaviors.

Sousa (1988) investigated the use of “emblems” in nonverbal conducting gesture. By

asking high school and junior high school students to describe video taped conductor

gestures, Sousa was able to label 38 out of 55 gestures as “emblems.” Sousa labeled a

specific gesture with emblem status if 70% of the subjects were able to effectively match

on a paper-and-pencil test the conducting gesture with its correct musical meaning. The

study determined that the level of gesture recognition increased with experience. High

school music students successfully recognized 37 of the 55 gestures while junior high

school students recognized only 19 of 55 gestures. The study found that in most cases,

musical experience increased ability to classify conducting gestures.

Cofer (1998) extended the work of Sousa (1988) by investigating the effect of

limited instruction in conducting gestural meaning on the musical response of seventh

grade band students. Participants were band students drawn from an urban public school

district (N = 60) who had two to three years of wind instrument performance experience.

Through random selection, an experimental group (n = 30) and a control group (n = 30)

were established. Participants in the experimental group received instruction on the

meanings of eighteen conducting gestures identified as emblems in the Sousa (1988)

study: piano, forte, subito piano, subito forte, two types of crescendo, two types of

decrescendo, staccato, legato, tenuto, two types of marcato, accelerando, ritardando, and

two types of fermata.


21
The Effect of Gesture

A four bar melody was constructed through which all of the musical expressions

could be demonstrated. Participants in the control group rehearsed the four bar melody

with an instructor who used no conducting gestures. The instructor of the control group

also reviewed with student’s terms that corresponded with specific musical expressions

identified for the study. During the preparation process, the instructor of the control

group tapped his baton on a music stand while students attempted to perform the melody

using the prescribed musical expressions. Participants of the treatment group were

instructed in the terminology of the musical expressions and corresponding conducting

emblems were demonstrated.

Conducting-gesture recognition of both groups (treatment and control) was

assessed through individual performance of the melodies while simultaneously following

the baton of a conductor on a television monitor, and a paper-and-pencil test. Results of

an independent sample t-test showed a significant difference between groups on the

paper-and-pencil measure of conducting gesture recognition (t = 6.97,p ≤ .001) and the

individual performance measure (F = 39.26, p ≤ .0001). The mean difference between the

treatment group (M = 16.40, SD = 2.68) and the control group (M = 12.50, SD = 2.68) on

the paper-and-pencil test reflects this result. The Bonferroni method, a statistical

adjustment that effectively raises the standard of proof needed when examining multiple

hypotheses simultaneously, verified that subjects in the treatment group performed nine

of the eighteen conducting gestures significantly better than subjects in the control group

(p < .05). Based on these results, Cofer concluded that seventh-grade band students of the

22
The Effect of Gesture

treatment group were able to consistently respond to nine of eighteen conducting

“emblems” after short-term conducting-gesture instruction while those of the control

group were only able to respond to three of the emblems. It is interesting to note that even

without instruction, the control group participants were intuitively able to correctly

respond to three of the 18 emblems.

Sidoti (1991) also explored the effect of expressive and non-expressive

conducting on the performance accuracy of high school students. Subjects in the

experimental group were asked to perform melodies while viewing a videotape of a

conductor utilizing expressive conducting gestures that visually reinforced expression

markings on the page. Subjects in the control group performed the same melodies while

watching a conductor who used non-expressive (beating time) conducting gestures.

Results indicated a significant difference between subject groups (p < .0001). Students in

the experimental group (M = 2.99, SD = .88) performed with greater accuracy, in terms

of selected expression markings, than did students of the control group (M = 2.78, SD =

.80). It is interesting to note that the Sidoti study utilized an upper body video of the

conductors with no isolation of facial or body language cues, and that participants

performed from copy that included expressive markings.

House (1998) approached the question of conductor influence on the expressive

performance of musicians in a similar way. College trumpet students were instructed to

perform an excerpt while following a conductor on video. A leading instrumental

conductor (John Whitwell) was video taped while conducting the excerpt in an expressive

23
The Effect of Gesture

way and once again in a non-expressive way. The expressive approach made an attempt

to convey the interpretive markings of the excerpt. In contrast, the non-expressive

conducting approach utilized simple beat patterns. Musicians performed the excerpt

while viewing the conductor videos. A three-member panel then evaluated the

expressiveness of those performances.

Even though the musicians performed from a copy of the excerpt which contained

expressive-articulative and phrase markings, a significant yet small difference in the

expressiveness between musician performances recorded while viewing the non-

expressive conducting (M = 4.896, SD = 1.606) versus the expressive conducting

(M = 5.287, SD = 1.732) was evident (p < .01).

The use of videotaped conducting as a two-dimensional medium for isolating

conducting gesture presentations has been implemented not only in the House study, but

also in many other studies (Cofer, 1996; Fredrickson, 1992; Sidoti, 1990; Skadsem, 1997;

Sousa, 1988). Even though responding to a live conductor is optimal in terms of

ecological validity, the use of videotaped conducting ensures that participants are

exposed to a consistent independent variable.

Graves (1984) isolated verbal communication and facial/body language cues from

technical conducting gestures. The Graves study applied six experimental conditions in a

choral rehearsal context. An unusual, and interesting, dependent measure was used in the

Graves study. The performances of a Bach chorale were recorded under the different

conducting conditions and then presented to high school students to determine if they

24
The Effect of Gesture

heard any differences in the performances. Conductors were asked to prepare the Bach

chorale using verbal and nonverbal conducting techniques, nonverbal techniques alone,

and nonverbal techniques while wearing a facial mask. Results indicated that there

were no significant differences between the nonverbal conditions and the nonverbal

/masked conditions. This outcome suggests that, while facial expression is a valuable

element of nonverbal communication, it is not a crucial variable on its own or when

removed from nonverbal conducting gesture.

Assessing Conducting Gesture

Herbert Karpicke’s study (1987) investigated conductors through the development

of an instrument that measures the performance response of musicians to a variety of

nonverbal conducting gestures. Karpicke defines conducting gesture as “an observable

movement of the hands or hands and arms, or a change in visual direction, in addition to

or outside of an ongoing beat pattern which results in observable musical response in

performance context”(1987, p.3). The purpose of his study was to develop a reliable and

valid instrument for evaluating and measuring conductor gesture and subsequent

performance (musician) response.

25
The Effect of Gesture

Two subject groups were selected for experiments that would validate Karpicke’s

Gestural Response Instrument (GRI). Twenty-five high school orchestra conductors from

the Houston and Dallas, Texas areas were contacted for participation. Twenty (n = 20)

conductors agreed to participate in the experiments. Although this was a convenience

sample, the researcher was able to obtain a great deal of demographic and performance

related information on these subjects. Each conductor had a record of participation in

Texas performance evaluation festivals. Their respective music supervisors verified this

information. Conducting subjects were placed into two categories, “strong” or “weak,”

based on previous festival ratings and music supervisor knowledge of the subject’s

conducting performance ability. The conductors were then rank ordered (strongest to

weakest) by their music supervisors.

Conducting subjects conducted a volunteer orchestra through a series of musical

excerpts. Prior to the experiment, the researcher rehearsed the piece that included these

excerpts.

The conductors were instructed to use no verbal cues or communication during

the performance/recording sessions. Only facial and hand/arm conducting gestures were

utilized. Participants conducted the piece while simultaneous video recording took place.

Excerpts were cued at consistent places in the music. Six of these excerpts were selected

from each conductor-orchestra performance.

Four expert observers were trained in using the GRI and were asked to view a

random mix of video excerpts from each conductor’s performance, totaling 560

26
The Effect of Gesture

observations. Using a 1-5 Likert scale, observers were asked to rate the perceived

effectiveness of each conductor gesture (top box of the GRI) and the corresponding

musical response of the orchestra (Figure1).

Figure 1. GRI scoring box

4
gesture
3
response

Gesture (top) and response (bottom) scores for each excerpt were multiplied and

averaged, creating an overall Gestural Response Score. These scores were then aligned

with their respective conductors. ANOVA (Analysis of Variance) was used to verify

consistency among conductors, observers, musical excerpts, gestures, responses, and

gesture/response variance.

According to Karpicke, this scoring method “allows both the gesture and its

associated response to be scored as contingencies” (1987, p.59). When four observers are

used, the GRI Total scores yield a substantial reliability (r =. 80). It was also noted that

observer consistency remained stable even though musical content (excerpts) varied.

Because the same orchestra performed the same excerpts with each conductor, it is

important to know which facet was having a stronger effect, the conductor or maturation

of the orchestra. In this study, no systemic orchestra response was found: however, it

27
The Effect of Gesture

would seem that consistency of musician response and learning effects (history,

maturation, practice effect, etc.) should be accounted for with use of the GRI in a group

performance setting.

Data suggest that with four or more observers, the GRI is considered a reliable

instrument for assessing conducting gesture/musician response events. The GRI is also

able to discriminate between stronger and weaker conductors' ability to elicit musical

responses through gesture. It should be noted that response score reliability (r =. 50) was

lower than gesture score reliability (r =. 85). This discrepancy suggests that observers

should receive further training in how to consistently assess the aural/response aspect of

the GRI instrument. Perhaps audio of the musical response should be evaluated

separately rather than simultaneously with video of the corresponding gesture.

The use of a full ensemble brings a number of confounding variables into

Karpicke’s study. Certain variables may be controlled (history, maturation, bias, ability,

following the principal player, etc.) using individual musicians and recording individual

responses.

Karpicke’s Gestural Response Instrument (GRI) study is a substantive effort to

develop a subjective assessment of conductor performance. Use of the GRI and other

methods of conductor assessment may enhance conductor training and examine musician

perception of conducting gestures.

28
The Effect of Gesture

CHAPTER III
RESEARCH PROCEDURES

The Research Question


For musicians, it is widely believed that the ability to understand and manipulate
segments of musical sound, such as entrance, body, and release, are crucial for musical
expression (Lewis, 1999; Gallops, 1999). Stylistic interpretive decisions are made
through the musician’s informed intuition of appropriate musical shape and ability to
“control the beginning, middle, and end of notes” (Gallops, 1999, p.35). For conductors,
perception and manipulation of these segments of musical tone (entrance, body, release)
may be crucial when translating musical interpretation into visual representation. Elliott
articulates the procedural process of informed intuition involved in music making as an
action involving “nonverbal forms of thinking and knowing in and of themselves”
(Elliott, 1995, p.55). As discussed in previous chapters, stylistic expressive-interpretive
decisions are generated through the conductor’s informed intuition of appropriate musical
shape.
The ability to translate musical interpretation into visual representations, the
perception and ability to manipulate entrance-body-release, and the informed intuition
developed through experience and knowledge, are tools that engage conductors in a
perception contract. This perception contract among musicians may allow nonverbal
communication of very specific interpretive decisions. According to this view, the
conductor’s intuition and learned ability to communicate those stylistic-interpretive
decisions through gesture is what ultimately determines conducting success (Graves,
1984; Grechesky, 1985; Green, 1961; Lewis, 1999; Mayne, 1992; Miller, 1988;
29
The Effect of Gesture

Strouse, 1987). In order to establish the existence of a perception contract between


musicians and conductors, this study investigated differences between performances of
musical excerpts (baseline-post w/conductor) through an experimental design using a
Gestural Response Instrument - GRI (Karpicke, 1987) as the dependent measure.
In an effort to minimize confounding and extraneous variables, a Gestural
Response Instrument (GRI) format was used in this study with three modifications; 1)
individual musicians performed the musical excerpts rather than a full orchestra, 2) each
conductor’s performance was video taped, focusing only on the hands/arms, 3) panels of
experts evaluated the conducting videos separately from the audio record of musician
responses, and 4) a nested sampling procedure was utilized in an effort to increase
observation responses while minimizing order effect and practice effect.
Descriptive data were collected and examined. Effect sizes between performances
demonstrated the degree to which communication occurred through nonverbal
conducting gestures. Demographic information pertaining to each conductor was
gathered. This information included gender, age, conducting background (band or
orchestra), experience, type of training, major instrument, and level of professional
involvement (high school, college, university, and professional). After data analysis,
trends appeared in individual conductors. These trends will be discussed in Chapter IV
and Chapter V, and may warrant further research.
Instrumentalists gather their musical experiences from varied ensemble traditions.
Thus the string musician might be more responsive to expressive-interpretive conducting
than the brass player. Moreover, the wind player might create clearer interpretive
articulations. If this is the case, differences in brass, woodwind, and string musician
response scores may appear. The sample size of brass, woodwind, or string

30
The Effect of Gesture

musicians for this experiment was not large enough to generalize significance from an
ANOVA in nested sampling (Kromrey & Dickenson, 1996). However, results of this
study might display a trend that warrants further research with a larger generalizable
sample.

Sampling Procedures
Two pools of participants were sampled for this study: experienced instrumental
conductors and college musicians who performed musical excerpts while watching the
conductors. Conductors had equal opportunity to be selected from a pool of 50
experienced high school, college, university, and professional instrumental conductors.
Through simple random sampling, fifteen conductors were selected from the pool of 50.
This study focused on experienced instrumental conductors, particularly those who have
demonstrated success as a high school, college, or professional conductor. In order to
examine trends for future research and to clarify results as related to individual
conductors, preliminary information was gathered from each conductor that included
gender, specific age, conductor background (band or orchestral conducting), type of
conducting experiences, training, major instrument, and current area of professional
involvement: high school, college, university or professional (see Appendix A).
Woodwind, brass, and string instrumentalists were selected from a pool of fifty
college/university level music majors. A music major was defined as someone who is
pursuing an undergraduate course of study in music performance, music education, or
composition at a college or university. Twenty-five participants were selected through
stratified random sampling, providing numbers of woodwind-brass-string instrumentalists
(brass n=10 / woodwind n=10 / string n=5).

31
The Effect of Gesture

Procedure
One melodic excerpt was selected. The excerpt did not contain extremely
complex rhythms or technical challenges and the excerpt was not easily recognized from
standard classical literature. That is to say, it was not familiar to the performing
musicians as commonplace among instrumental performance literature or numerous
recordings. Once an appropriate excerpt was identified, it was duplicated three times.
One copy of the excerpt contained no expressive-interpretive markings. Distinct
expressive-interpretive markings were created for each of the two remaining copies of the
excerpt. These marked versions of the melodic excerpt were labeled as version ‘A’ and
version ‘B’ respectively. An expressive-interpretive marking was defined as staccato,
tenuto, accent, slur, dynamic, crescendo, decrescendo, accelerando, ritardando, and
tempo. (Appendix D)
Conductors selected for participation practiced and conducted each of the marked
excerpt versions (‘A’ & ‘B’). In preparation, conductors used whatever amount of time
they needed to prepare and practice each version of the excerpt. Conducting
performances were video recorded while participants conducted, using gestures believed
to represent the expressive-interpretive markings of each excerpt. While recording the
gestures, conductors were allowed to sing as they conducted and were asked to use a
baton and both hands as they saw fit. Through use of digital video technology, the face
was blurred and any vocalization was removed from the recording. Only hands/upper
chest were visible on the video, thereby eliminating overt body language and facial or
vocal cues. Each excerpt was duplicated with a fifteen-second pause between repeated
conductor performances of the excerpt. All videos were coded and timed, identifying the

32
The Effect of Gesture

conductor and the excerpt (‘A’ & ‘B’) that was on the video. A separate VHS videotape
was generated for each conductor.
From the selected musicians (n=25), randomly selected nests of 5 participants
watched and performed individually to mixed sets of conducting videos. Each nest
included 2 brass, 2 woodwind, and 1 string musicians. Nests were pre-assigned to sets of
six video-conducting excerpts. This process was duplicated, thereby creating enough
nests to perform all of the video excerpts. Through the nesting procedure, an increased
number of responses was generated with limited duplication of personnel. Limiting the
number of times that a participant performed the excerpts, yet generating a large set of
responses was an important result of nesting. VanWeelden (2002) found that conductor
performance ratings were affected when participants viewed conductors in a certain
order. Suppression of practice effect and order effect in participant response was an
important design aspect of this study (Appendix B). The nesting procedure applied in this
study controlled for suppression of multiple response (practice effect) and created a
mixed order of videos (Dunham, Lemke & Moran, 1991; Holahan & Saunders, 1997;
VanWeelden, 2002). Care was taken in determining the unit of measure as the mean of
each nest. Some research has shown that power is lost when nested sampling is applied,
particularly when individual scores are the unit of measure (Kromrey, 1996).
Participants received and practiced the unmarked copy of the excerpt, focusing
simply on accuracy of notes and rhythms. The musicians performed individually in
consistent acoustical environments, with no other participants present. Each musician was
instructed to expressively perform the melody using a personal interpretation. This initial
performance was labeled as that particular musician’s baseline response. The musicians
were then asked to follow the conductor’s video taped gestures as he/she performed each

33
The Effect of Gesture

excerpt. The musician had an opportunity to perform each excerpt twice while viewing
the conductor video (pausing fifteen seconds between each performance). These two
performances created a pre-post (with conductor) set of performance recordings for each
version of the excerpt (Figure 2). Figure 2 illustrates the mixed assignment of conductor
videos as they were assigned to randomly selected nests of musicians.

Figure 2. Nesting Procedure

Pre

A b1
B b5
B w6
A w10
A s8

11 B b6
15 A b2
A w2
A w5
B s6

b1= brass 1; w6= woodwind 6; s8=string 8

Data Analysis
Using the format of Karpicke’s Gestural Response Instrument (GRI), panels of
experts evaluated the conducting gesture effectiveness and the musician performance
responses (baseline and post w/conductor) as compared to the expression markings of the

34
The Effect of Gesture

‘A’ and ‘B’ versions of the excerpt. For the conductor evaluators, an expert was defined
as a musician with graduate training in instrumental conducting and professional
performance experience as a musician and conductor. For the musician response
evaluators, an expert was defined as a musician with undergraduate training in
instrumental music and professional performance experience. Each panelist received
training and practice in highlighting (marker pen) perceived expressive-interpretive
performance, using a five point Likert Scale, and utilizing the GRI gesture or response
scoring boxes.
The conductor evaluation panelists applied a Likert Scale to evaluate each
conductor video recording (N=15, 30 excerpts) in terms of gestural conducting
effectiveness (GRI Gesture). The Likert Scale categories were (5) definite gestures, (4)
somewhat definite gestures, (3) inconsistent gestures, (2) not very definite gestures, and
(1) poor gestures. The task of each panelist was to rate the conducting gestures as they
attempt to express through their performance of the phrase the expressive and
interpretive markings of the excerpt. These GRI ratings were assigned without listening
to any performance. The decisions of the panelists were based on how well the
conducting gestures seemed to represent the two written versions of the excerpt.
Audio recordings were collected as musicians performed the excerpts. The audio
recordings were labeled and sorted according to excerpt (‘A’ & ‘B’), baseline or post
performance (w/conductor), and conductor (N=15). When comparing the performances to
the marked excerpt, the musician response panelists evaluated the performances in terms
of expressive-interpretive accuracy. The panelists were asked to mark with a highlighter
pen while listening to the recordings, those interpretive markings that the musicians
adhered to in the performance. A five point Likert Scale was used to quantify each

35
The Effect of Gesture

panelist’s evaluation of the musician responses (GRI Response). The Likert Scale
categories for musician response were defined as (5) definite responses, (4) somewhat
definite responses, (3) inconsistent responses, (2) not very definite responses, and (1)
poor responses.

Measurement
Gesture and Response scores were observed individually and were multiplied to
create a GRI score for each performance (as recommended by Karpicke). GRI scores
were aligned with conductors and a total GRI score was calculated (averaged) for each of
the excerpts. A Conductor GRI score was calculated (averaged) from the excerpt scores
to give each conductor an overall GRI gesture, response, and total score (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Creation of the GRI Scores

GRI Gesture GRI Response GRI TOTALS overall totals


conductor excerpt score BASELINE POST BASELINE POST Baseline Post
(w/conductor) (w/conductor)

1 1a 4 w3 b3 s4 w4 b4 s5 4 / 3.33 4/4 13.33 16

1b 3 w3 b2 s3 w3 b3 s3 3 / 2.67 3/3 8.00 9

N=15 conductors w=woodwind b=brass s=string

Once these descriptive data were acquired, differences in GRI scores between
performances of the excerpts (baseline-post w/conductor) were examined. If non-verbal
communication took place, GRI response scores should increase each time a musician

36
The Effect of Gesture

performed an excerpt with a specific conductor. An ANOVA calculated differences in the


average GRI scores between baseline and post performances (w/conductor) of the
excerpts. Chapter IV displays those differences, concluding that strong and specific non-
verbal gestural communication can take place between conductor and performer.

37
The Effect of Gesture

CHAPTER IV

DATA ANALYSIS

Introduction

This chapter will illustrate and elucidate data that were collected from conductor

videos, musician responses, and the evaluation of those performances-responses. The

analysis will be guided through a discussion of process, participants, collection of data,

results, and reliability of the results.

Process

The Gestural Response Instrument (GRI), as described in the previous chapter,

was utilized in this study. The product of a gesture score and response score creates a

total score that reflects the ability of a conductor to communicate a musical interpretation

through conducting gesture, and the corresponding response of musicians to those

gestures. It is important to understand what each of these scores represents.

The GRI gesture score is the average score of a four-member panel evaluation of

the conductor as he/she conducts a passage that is marked with specific interpretive

markings. Evaluators rated each conductor’s ability to communicate the interpretive

markings of the passage with a 1-5 numerical Likert score. Two contrasting versions of

the excerpt were created in order to verify results were not obtained due to musician

intuition or innate properties of any particular expressive-interpretive approach. With this

38
The Effect of Gesture

in mind, two GRI gesture scores were calculated: one for an ‘A’ version of a melodic

excerpt and one for a ‘B’ version of the same excerpt. Those two scores were averaged,

creating a GRI gesture total.

The GRI response score is the average score of five musician performances

(responses). A three-member panel evaluated these performances. Each evaluator

assigned a Likert Score of 1-5 based on how closely the performance matched the

specific interpretive markings of the ‘A’ or ‘B’ version of the melodic excerpt.

The gesture score and response score for each conductor was multiplied, creating

a GRI total score. For example, if a conductor received an overall gesture score of 4 and

a musician response score of 3, the GRI total score for that conductor was 12. As

described in the Karpicke (1987) study, the GRI total score can therefore range from 1 to

25. If the two scores were added instead of multiplied, the total possible score would be

10. The multiplied score allows a greater range (25 instead of 10).

Participants

The participants involved in this study were categorized as conductors and

musicians. As discussed in the previous chapter, conductor participants were randomly

selected from a pool of experienced conductors. In order to determine the effect of

gesture on the performance of musicians, it was important to control for experience and

training of conductor, therefore all of the conductors represented successful, trained,

instrumental conductors. The conductors who actually participated in this study reflect

the following demographics (Table 3).

39
The Effect of Gesture

Table 3

Conducting Participant Demographics (N=15)

Number of Conductors out of N=15


GENDER
Female 3
Male 12
EXPERIENCE
High School Conducting 14
College-University Conducting 9
Community Ensemble Conducting 7
Professional Conducting 3
TRAINING
Undergraduate Degree in Music 15
Masters Degree in Music 9
Doctorate-Post Masters Degree or study 3
Numbers reflect how many of the conductors (N=15) fit into each category.
For detailed information on each conductor: see appendix A

The musicians who performed the melodic excerpts were college-university

undergraduate music majors representing brass, woodwind, and string instrumentalists

( N=10 brass / 10 woodwind / 5 string). The GRI response score assigned to each

conductor was an average of five musician response scores.

Musicians were assigned specific conductors and excerpts through a nesting

procedure. A nest of five musicians representing two brass, two woodwind, and one

string player viewed each conducting performance. The randomness of musician

assignment limited order effect and created 75 recordings for each version of the excerpt,

plus 25 baseline performances, totaling 175 recorded performances. For a detailed listing

of musician assignments, see Appendix B.

40
The Effect of Gesture

A four-member panel of expert evaluators rated the video taped performances of

each conductor. The conductor evaluators were expert musicians who have extensive

experience as conductors, professional performers, and university music professors.

Due to the large number of performance recordings generated, two three-member

panels evaluated the recorded responses of the musicians. Each panel was comprised of

experienced professional musicians. Panel ‘A’ rated the performances of the first half of

recordings and panel ‘B’ evaluated the remaining half.

Data Collection

As discussed in Chapter Three, this study utilized digital video and audio

technology throughout the data collection phase. A few days before they were to perform

for the camera, conductor participants were provided with two distinct versions of the

melodic excerpt selected for the study (Der Jungling Am Bache, by Schubert): version

‘A’ and version ‘B’ (Appendix D).

Each conductor was instructed to review and practice both versions of the melodic

excerpt, keeping in mind that the goal was to conduct in such a way to elicit an

interpretive response from a musician as close as possible to the interpretative markings

on the written excerpt. The conductor was asked to use a baton and was informed that

facial cues would be digitally removed from the video. A blank wall was used as a

background and the digital video camera was placed approximately 12-15 feet away. The

camera view was framed above the conductor’s waist so that all of the beat patterns and

41
The Effect of Gesture

gestures could be recorded. Conducting participants were also instructed that they could

sing, whistle, or hum as they conducted if they wished, and that the audio would be

turned off when the musicians viewed the video. A conductor code (1 to 15) and an

excerpt code (‘A’ or ‘B’) were verbally cued at the beginning of each videotaped

conducting performance. Once each conductor rendered performances of both versions of

the excerpt, a digital “video blur” was placed over the face of each conductor. The digital

blur prohibited musician participants from deciphering facial gestures that might relate to

the music. This process also inhibited the identification of the conducting participants.

The product was a master video of fifteen conductors conducting both versions of the

excerpt in a way that, at least in the mind of the conductor, communicated as much of the

interpretive details as possible.

Musician participants were recorded while performing from an unmarked copy of

the melodic excerpt (Figure 4). Although it retains rhythmic and melodic elements, the

unmarked copy of the excerpt is void of any interpretive-phrase markings.

Figure 4. Melodic Excerpt (unmarked)

42
The Effect of Gesture

The recording equipment consisted of a dual cassette recorder, two omni-

directional microphones, and a 20” television monitor with VCR playback. The

microphones were placed at the optimum distance appropriate for each instrument. Each

musician was given the option of performing while seated or standing with the television

monitor placed at eye level, two and one half feet away.

Musicians were given the unmarked copy of the excerpt in advance and were able

to practice the excerpt until it was well under hand. Once warmed-up and familiarized

with the recording surroundings, the musician was asked to perform the excerpt utilizing

his/her own personal interpretation. This initial, or baseline performance, was recorded to

provide a reference from which changes in future performances could be measured. The

baseline performance demonstrated a musician’s intuitive interpretation prior to the

conductor-initiated performances.

Once the baseline performance was recorded, the musician was instructed to

perform the excerpt while viewing six video recordings selected from the thirty conductor

video performances. The musician was asked to interpret the excerpt in the way that

he/she felt the conductor was communicating musical interpretation through conducting

gesture. The musician’s performance response was recorded on audio cassette tape and

labeled with the corresponding conductor-excerpt code. This process created a baseline

and six conductor initiated performances for each musician (N=25). A detailed listing of

musician codes and the corresponding nesting of conductor videos can be found in

Appendix B.

43
The Effect of Gesture

It is important to note that musicians performed along with each conducting video

twice with fifteen-second intervals between each; first as a practice recording (pre), and

secondly as a “post” (w/Conductor) recording. The post (w/Conductor) recording was

used as data for the evaluation procedure. The recording process was not halted, even in

those cases where the musician could not follow the conductor’s gestures.

Panel Evaluation

Panels of qualified professional musicians evaluated the video and audio

recordings produced by the conductors and musicians. The evaluation process utilized a

1-5 Likert scale scoring system and a procedure that involved highlighting interpretive-

articulative markings on copies of the melodic excerpt.

The conductor evaluation panel was instructed to view each conductor conduct

each version of the excerpt (A & B) while following along with the matching marked

version of the excerpt. As the conductor utilized patterns and gestures that the evaluator

regarded as effective communication of an interpretative mark on the score, the evaluator

was to highlight (with a marker pen) the specific interpretive-articulative markings

(Figure 5).

44
The Effect of Gesture

Figure 5. The Evaluation Process: Sample of a Highlighted Copy of Excerpt ‘A’

Once the evaluator viewed the conductor’s videotaped performances and

highlighted the interpretive-articulative markings that, in the view of the evaluator, were

effectively communicated, a 1-5 Likert score was assigned. As discussed in Chapter

Three, the Likert scoring system was defined in the following way: (5) definite gestures,

(4) somewhat definite gestures, (3) inconsistent gestures, (2) not very definite gestures,

(1) poor gestures. The task of the evaluator was to rate the conducting gestures as they

attempted to express the articulative and interpretive markings of the excerpt. As

illustrated in Table 4, this scoring process assigned four ratings to each conductor (N=15)

for each version of the excerpt (A & B). A mean score for each conductor was calculated

and termed as the GRI Gesture score.

45
The Effect of Gesture

Table 4

Conductor Video Performance Evaluations of the four-member panel: GRI Gesture Score
Evaluator (I-IV) – A or B version of the excerpt
Mean=
GRI Gesture
IA IB IIA IIB IIIA IIIB IVA IVB score
CONDUCTOR
1 3 3 3 3 4 3 4 3 3.25
2 2 2 2 2 1 1 3 3 2.00
3 4 4 2 2 4 4 4 4 3.50
4 2 2 4 4 4 3 3 3 3.12
5 4 5 3 3 3 3 3 3 3.37
6 3 3 3 2 3 1 1 1 2.12
7 2 2 1 2 3 2 4 4 2.50
8 3 3 2 2 3 2 3 3 2.62
9 2 1 2 2 2 1 2 2 1.75
10 4 4 3 3 4 4 2 3 3.37
11 2 2 2 2 3 2 3 2 2.25
12 3 2 4 3 3 2 2 2 2.62
13 2 2 4 3 3 3 3 4 3.00
14 1 1 1 1 2 1 2 2 1.37
15 5 4 5 4 4 4 4 5 4.37

A similar process was used to evaluate musician responses to the conducting

gestures. Due to the number of recordings created (175), two three-member panels of

musician evaluators listened to the musician performances. These evaluators were

instructed to track the performances with the corresponding version of the excerpt,

marking the interpretive-articulations that were perceived by the evaluator.

A 1-5 Likert score was assigned to the musician response (performance),

reflecting the evaluator’s perception of the degree to which the musician performed the

specific interpretive-articulative markings of the excerpt. The Likert Scale categories for

musician response were; (5) definite responses, (4) somewhat definite responses, (3)

inconsistent responses, (2) not very definite responses, (1) poor responses.

46
The Effect of Gesture

The Baseline performances created by each musician were scored against the A

and B versions of the excerpt. Since the baseline performance was the musician’s own

personal interpretation of the excerpt, and because the baseline performance was

compared to the ‘A’ and ‘B’ marked version of the excerpt, the evaluator’s Likert rating

was typically low (1.0 < 2.66). The Likert ratings for baseline performances as they were

evaluated against the ‘A’ and ‘B’ versions of the excerpt, are displayed in Table 5.

Table 5

Evaluations of Musician Baseline Performances


Musician Code Excerpt ‘A’ / ‘B’ Mean score
B1 (1) 1.33 / 1.33
B2 (2) 2.0 / 1.66
B3 (3) 1.66 / 1.66
B4 (4) 1.0 / 1.0
B5 (5) 1.66 / 1.33
B6 (6) 2.33 / 2.0
B7 (7) 1.66 / 1.66
B8 (8) 2.66 / 2.0
B9 (9) 2.33 / 2.66
B10 (10) 2.0 / 2.0
W1 (11) 2.0 / 2.0
W2 (12) 2.33 / 2.33
W3 (13) 2.33 / 1.33
W4 (14) 1.66 / 1.33
W5 (15) 3.0 / 3.0
W6 (16) 2.0 / 2.0
W7 (17) 2.0 / 2.0
W8 (18) 1.66 / 1.66
W9 (19) 2.0 / 2.33
W10 (20) 2.33 / 3.0
S1 (21) 1.66 / 1.66
S2 (22) 2.0 / 1.33
S3 (23) 2.33 / 2.0
S4 (24) 1.33 / 2.33
S5 (25) 2.0 / 2.0
B=Brass Musician Code / W=Woodwind Musician Code / S=String Musician Code
47
The Effect of Gesture

The musician response scores were aligned with each conductor. The mean for

the Post (w/Conductor) musician responses of each nest was calculated in order to create

a GRI Response score for each conductor. Table 6 displays the musician mean response

scores for each conductor’s nest of musicians.

Table 6

Musician Mean Response Scores for Each Conductor

CONDUCTOR GRI Response Score


1 3.15
2 2.53
3 2.87
4 2.97
5 3.20
6 2.66
7 2.60
8 2.77
9 2.23
10 3.4
11 2.43
12 2.97
13 2.70
14 2.23
15 3.43
The GRI Response score is the mean of the musician response score of the nest that view the conductor.

Table 7 reflects the GRI TOTAL scores for each conductor. This score is the product of

the GRI Response and GRI Gesture score. The GRI TOTAL score quantifies the effect of

the conductor’s gestures on the musician’s responses (Karpicke, 1987).

48
The Effect of Gesture

Table 7

GRI Total Scores

CONDUCTOR GRI Gesture Score GRI Response Score GRI TOTAL


(GRIG) (GRIR) (GRIGxGRIR)
1 3.25 3.15 10.24
2 2 2.53 5.07
3 3.5 2.87 10.03
4 3.12 2.97 9.27
5 3.37 3.20 10.8
6 2.12 2.66 5.65
7 2.15 2.60 6.5
8 2.62 2.77 7.26
9 1.75 2.23 3.91
10 3.37 3.40 11.47
11 2.25 2.43 5.47
12 2.62 2.97 7.78
13 3 2.70 8.1
14 1.37 2.23 3.07
15 4.37 3.43 15.02
Range = 3.07 < 15.02

Inter-rater Reliability

Threats to validity can arise in studies that involve subjective assessment if inter-

rater reliability is not estimated and accounted for. This study involved the evaluation of

conductors by one panel of raters, and the evaluation of musician responses by two other

panels of evaluators. It is important to remember that subjective rating, especially for

musical performance, will typically generate medium-high reliability estimates (r =.55 -

.80). It is important to examine reliability quotients as they relate to literature in the field.

This particular study utilizes the Gestural Response Instrument (GRI) developed in the

Karpicke study of 1987. Reliability quotients of the Karpicke study ranged from r =.50 to

r =.80.

49
The Effect of Gesture

The first examination of inter-rater reliability involved an evaluation of the

conductors as they conducted the two versions of the excerpt. The estimated reliability of

the panel when they evaluated all conductors on the ‘A’ version of the excerpt was

r =.38. The reliability estimate for the evaluations of the ‘B’ version was r =.55. The

reliability of the ‘A’ version (r =.38) evaluations is quite low. The ‘B’ evaluations fall

within the medium range of reliability (r =.55).

Since there was no significant difference found between versions of the excerpt

and conductor GRI Gesture scores, and given the results of the initial reliability estimates

for the GRI Gesture scores, the researcher extracted the Mid-ability conductors and

calculated reliability estimates for the High and Low conductor evaluations. The

estimated reliability of the panel when they evaluated High and Low ability conductors

on the ‘A’ version of the excerpt was r =.56. The reliability estimate for the evaluations

of High and Low conductors on the ‘B’ version was r =.68. Extracting the mid-ability

conductors increased the inter-rater reliability estimates.

Reliability was also measured between raters of the musician responses. Once

again, these estimates reflect the evaluation of responses to the ‘A’ version and the ‘B’

version of the etude. The reliability for the evaluations of musician responses for the ‘A’

version of the etude was r =.51. Evaluations of musician responses for the ‘B’ version of

the etude was in the medium-high range (r =.67) . Once again, estimated reliability scores

were higher for the ‘B’ version than that of the ‘A’ version.

50
The Effect of Gesture

Given that these reliability coefficients were gathered from small groups (n = 5)

of conductor evaluations, the r = .51 – .67 range might be considered acceptable

reliability.

Analysis of Data

As described above, the evaluators created a highlighted representation of the

interpretive aspects of the performances as they studied the corresponding markings on

the ‘A’ or ‘B’ version of the excerpt. A 1-5 Likert Score was assigned for each

performance. The conductor scores (GRI Gesture) and musician response scores (GRI

Response) were matched and listed in an excel file.

When examining Hierarchical Linear Models, the unit of measure must remain

independent so as not to violate assumptions. With this in mind, the mean score of the

panel evaluations was used as the unit of measure for GRI Gesture and GRI Response

scores. Each conductor’s GRI Gesture score reflects the average score of the four-

member panel that evaluated conductor video performances (Table 4). These GRI

Gesture scores were used to separate the conductors into Low (n = 5), Medium (n = 5), or

High (n = 5) categories. Table 8 displays this ranking.

51
The Effect of Gesture

Table 8

Conductor Ranking: categorized based on GRI Gesture score.

GRI gesture score


CONDUCTOR MEAN
15 4.375
3 3.5
10 3.375
5 3.375
1 3.25
4 3.125
13 3
12 2.625
8 2.625
7 2.5
11 2.25
6 2.125
2 2
9 1.75
14 1.375
High Conductor Group n=5 (M 3.58, SD .46)
Low Conductor Group n=5 (M 1.98, SD .35)

Five null hypotheses were constructed in order to apply an Analysis of Variance

(ANOVA) for these data:

H1: There is no difference between corresponding GRI Musician mean


Response scores of conductors who score high on GRI Gesture and those
conductors who score low on GRI Gesture.

H2: There is no interaction between Baseline musician mean response


scores and corresponding Post (w/Conductor) musician mean response
scores for conductors with High and Low GRI Gesture scores.

H3: There is no interaction between Baseline musician mean response


scores and corresponding Post (w/Conductor) mean response scores for
each excerpt (A&B).

52
The Effect of Gesture

H4: There is no difference between GRI conductor mean gesture scores for
the (A) and (B) versions of the excerpt.

H5: There is no difference between brass, woodwind, and string musician


mean response scores for the (A) or (B) versions of the excerpt.

The null hypotheses were developed in order to examine the question of gesture

influence on musician performance. Each null hypothesis was examined using the

appropriate ANOVA design.

The first question examined whether conductors who received higher GRI

Gesture scores were actually able to solicit higher GRI Response scores from the

musicians than those conductors who received lower GRI Gesture scores. A significant

difference between high and low conductor groups indicates that conducting gestures

alone can elicit consistent musical responses from musicians. The first null hypothesis is

stated as follows:

H1: There is no significant difference between corresponding GRI


Musician mean Response scores of conductors who score high on GRI
Gesture and those conductors who score low on GRI Gesture.

Conductors were categorized into high-mid-low levels based on their GRI Gesture

scores. Table 9 displays the main effect means for musician responses sorted by

conductor level.

53
The Effect of Gesture

Table 9

Conductor Level (CONLEVEL) and GRI Response scores

CONLEVEL GRI Response


(Hi-Mid-Low) Mean (SD)
Hi (n=5) 3.10, (.47)
Mid (n=5) 2.85, (.44)
Lo (n=5) 2.47, (.36)
Sample mean of all responses & standard deviations (SD) sorted by conductor level

Two versions of the excerpt were utilized. Table 10 displays the means of

musician nests that performed for each conductor group.

Table 10
Conductor Level and GRI Response scores for each version (A&B).

CONLEVEL GRI Resp 'A' GRI Resp 'B'


(Hi-Mid-Lo) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Hi (n=5) 2.86, (.54) 3.34, (.41)
Mid (n=5) 2.96, (.46) 2.74, (.42)
Lo (n=5) 2.26, (.40) 2.68, (.33)
Sample mean (M) & standard deviation (SD)

The ANOVA procedure that examined conductor level (high, middle and low)

and excerpt is illustrated in Table 11. Results indicate that conductors of the High

classification (M 3.10) were able to elicit significantly higher GRI Response scores from

musicians than conductors of the Low classification (Mean Difference = 2.47). There is an

interaction between Conductor level and whether the conductor was attempting to

interpret the ‘A’ or ‘B’ version of the excerpt. Conductors of both the Low and High

54
The Effect of Gesture

classifications elicited higher GRI Response scores from musicians when attempting to

interpret the ‘B’ version of the excerpt (Table 10).

Table 11

GRI Musician Response: ANOVA Procedure for Conductor Level and Excerpt

Source DF F Value Pr > F


CONLEVEL 2 26.57 < .0001
EXCERPT 1 10.10 .0018
CONLEVEL*EXCERPT 2 10.09 < .0001

Conductors of the mid-ability group (n=5) scored slightly higher when attempting

to interpret the ‘A’ version of the excerpt. A post hoc Tukey test examined pairwise

comparisons between conductor groups and indicates s a significant (p < .05) and large

effect size (.79) between the Low Conductor group and the Middle and High groups on

version ‘A’ of the excerpt. There was also a significant (p < .05) and large effect size (.61)

displayed between the Low Conductor group and the High Conductor group on version

‘B’ of the excerpt. It is important to note that the difference between mean differences of

GRI Response scores for the mid-ability conductors as they conducted the ‘A’ version

(M = 2.96, SD .46) or ‘B’ version (M = 2.74, SD = .42) is small (M = .22) and led to an

interaction between level of conductor and excerpt. (F = 10.09, p < .0001).

Figure 6 presents the interaction effect for GRI Responses of High-Medium-Low

rated conductors as they interpreted the ‘A’ or ‘B’ version of the excerpt.

55
The Effect of Gesture

Figure 6. Conductor & Excerpt Interaction for GRI Response Scores

4.0

3.5 Hi
Med
3.0
Low
2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

‘A’ Version ‘B’ Version

The GRI Response scores of mid-ability conductors, with respect to excerpt (‘A’

and ‘B’), display an opposite effect. The mid-ability level conductors scored slightly

higher while attempting to interpret the ‘A’ version of the excerpt (2.79 vs. 2.78). In

Figure 7, mid-ability level conductors are classified as a moderating variable (Jaccard,

1998) and are removed from the interaction graph. Figure 7 illustrates the conclusion that

there exist a significant difference between the responses of musicians to the high rated

conductors (High GRI Gesture) and those conductors who were classified in the low

rated group (Low GRI Gesture) (F=26.57,p < .0001). The differences in Means, while

relatively small, are not due to chance. The high conductor group elicited higher musician

response scores, on both versions of the excerpt, than the low conductor group.

56
The Effect of Gesture

Figure 7. GRI Response Scores of High and Low Ability Conductors with Excerpt

4.0

3.5 Hi

3.0
Low
2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

‘A’ Version ‘B’ Version

An examination of the GRI Response scores of the musicians displayed a

significant difference between the baseline performance (each musician’s personal

interpretation) and the post performance (while watching the conductor). The null

hypothesis was stated as follows:

H2: There is no interaction between Baseline musician mean response scores and
corresponding Post (w/conductor) musician mean response scores for conductors
with High and Low GRI Gesture scores

Table 12 displays the mean scores of baseline and post performances with Conductors

(w/Conductor) as they relate to the Low, Medium, and High category conductors. The

data suggest that the conducting gestures changed the performance of the musicians and

57
The Effect of Gesture

that the changed performance corresponded to the conductor’s pre-determined

interpretation (F=188.18, p < .0001).

Table 12

Group means for Conductor Level (CONLEVEL) and Baseline-Post performances


(w/Conductor) of musicians for each excerpt version (A & B).

CONLEVEL Baseline 'A' w/Conductor 'A' Baseline 'B' w/Conductor 'B'


(Hi-Mid-Lo) mean (SD) mean (SD) mean (SD) mean (SD)
Hi (n=5) 1.79, (.48) 2.99, (.99) 1.99, (.50) 3.42, (.78)
Mid (n=5) 2.14, (.33) 2.79, (.81) 1.78, (.54) 2.78, (.64)
Lo (n=5) 1.97, (.42) 2.15, (.42) 2.05, (.65) 2.63, (.72)
Sample means & standard deviation (SD)

Table 13 displays results of the ANOVA procedure applied to conductor level

(CONLEVEL) and change scores between the baseline (BL) and post performances with

conductors (w/Conductor).

Table 13

ANOVA procedure for Baseline - with Conductor (BL-w/Conductor), Conductor Level


(CONLEVEL), and Excerpt

Source DF F Value Pr > F


BL-w/Conductor (Time) 1 188.18 < .0001
BL-w/Conductor *CONLEVEL 2 19.02 < .0001
BL-w/Conductor *EXCERPT 1 7.00 .00091
Error (BL-w/Conductor) 146

BL-w/Conductor *CONLEVEL refers to the interaction between baseline (BL)

and post performance scores with conductors (w/Conductor) as musicians performed

while viewing High and Low ability level conductors. ANOVA found that there was a
58
The Effect of Gesture

significant difference between the baseline performance (BL) and the post performance

response (w/Conductor) while watching the conductor videos (F 19.02, p < .0001). A

post-hoc Tukey test determined that a significant difference (p < .05) and large effect size

(.64) existed between the Low Conductor group and the Mid-ability conductor group and

also between the Low and High conductor group (.84) for the ‘A’ version of the excerpt.

A significant difference (p < .05) and large effect size (.79) was also evident between the

Low conductor group and the High conductor group for version ‘B’. The interaction

between the Mid-ability and High conductor group was also significant (p < .05) and

displayed a large effect size (.64).

Hypothesis three examined if a significant interaction existed between

conductor’s ability to elicit specific musician responses and each excerpt (A & B).

H3: There is no interaction between Baseline musician mean response scores


and corresponding Post (w/Conductor) mean response scores for each excerpt
(A&B).

The musician’s w/Conductor (Post) response scores increased significantly


depending on whether High, Middle or Low ability conductors were attempting the ‘A’
or ‘B’ version of the excerpt (Table 14).

Table 14

Baseline and POST GRI Response Scores of High and Low Level Conductors

CONDUCTOR LEVEL Baseline GRI Response w/Conductor GRI Response


‘A’ version / ‘B’ version ‘A’ version / ‘B’ version
High 1.79 1.99 2.99 3.42
Middle 2.14 1.78 2.79 2.78
Low 1.97 2.05 2.15 2.64

59
The Effect of Gesture

As reflected through the BL-w/Conductor*Excerpt interaction analysis (Table

15), there was a significant difference between excerpts: that is to say, some conductors

elicited higher GRI Response scores while attempting to conduct the ‘B’ version (F = 7.0,

p = .00091).

Table 15

ANOVA procedure for Baseline - with Conductor (BL-w/Conductor), Conductor Level


(CONLEVEL), and Excerpt

Source DF F Value Pr > F


BL-w/Conductor (Time) 1 188.18 < .0001
BL-w/Conductor *CONLEVEL 2 19.02 < .0001
BL-w/Conductor *EXCERPT 1 7.00 .00091
Error (BL-w/Conductor) 146

As previously discussed, A post-hoc Tukey test compared the between group

means of the High, Middle and Low ability conductors for significance, finding that the

High and Low group conductors elicited significantly higher GRI Response scores while

conducting the ‘B’ version of the excerpt. The Middle ability conductor group elicited

slightly higher GRI Response scores while conducting the ‘A’ version of the excerpt.

As illustrated in Figure 8 and Figure 9, it is interesting to note that there was a

significant difference between the baseline and post (w/Conductor) performance of

musicians as they performed both versions of the excerpt with High, Medium, and Low

GRI Gesture classified conductors (F = 19.02, p < .0001). Even though the musicians

created only one baseline performance against which all of their subsequent performance-

60
The Effect of Gesture

responses were measured, the nested means for the baseline performances differed

between conductors and excerpts. Since the nesting procedure produced varied

combinations of musicians, mean scores for the baseline performances of the nests also

varied. A post-hoc Tukey test revealed a significant (p < .05) yet small effect size (.34)

between the nested Baseline scores, when compared to the ‘A’ version of the excerpt, for

the High and Middle conductor groups (Figure 8). Baseline scores, when compared to

the ‘B’ version of the excerpt, displayed a significant (p < .05) yet small effect size (.26)

between the nested Baseline scores for the Middle and Low conductor groups (Figure 9).

Figure 8. Excerpt ‘A’: Musician Baseline and Post Performance-Responses

BL POST

61
The Effect of Gesture

For excerpt A, the largest increase between baseline and post performances

occurred with musician nest that had the lowest baseline score and performed for the high

rated conductors.

Figure 9. Excerpt ‘B’: Musician Baseline and Post Performance-Responses

BL POST

If a significant difference was found between GRI Gesture scores of the two

versions of the excerpt (A & B), it might be concluded that one version was much easier

to conduct or interpret. For the purposes of this study, it was important that both versions

of the etude were similar in terms of interpretive demand, yet required very different

interpretive approaches. In order to determine if there was a difference in difficulty

between versions of the excerpt, the following null hypothesis was examined:
62
The Effect of Gesture

H4: There is no difference between GRI conductor mean gesture scores of


the (A) and (B) versions of the excerpt.

As displayed in Table 16, ANOVA concluded that there was no significant difference

between versions ‘A’ and ‘B’ of the etude (EXCERPT) in terms of GRI Conductor

Gesture scores (CONLEVEL).

Table 16

GRI Gesture: ANOVA procedure for Conductor Level and Excerpt (A & B)

Source DF F Value Pr > F


CONLEVEL 2 5.51 .0080
EXCERPT 1 2.40 .1238
Error 146

There was no significant difference between the GRI Gesture scores assigned to

each conductor by the panel of conductor evaluators and conductor’s attempts to conduct

the ‘A’ or ‘B’ versions of the excerpt. There was, as already discussed, significant

differences between GRI Response scores of musicians as they performed with

conductors (F = 5.51, p = .008). As discussed earlier, the High and Low level conductor’s

attempts to interpret the ‘B’ version elicited a higher GRI Response from the musicians

(Figure 7).

Table 17 displays the ranked order for all of the conductors based on the product

of the GRI Gesture and GRI Response scores (GRI TOTAL)

63
The Effect of Gesture

Table 17

GRI TOTAL: Rank of Conductors High to Low

GRI Gesture GRI Response GRI


CONDUCTOR Gesture score Response score TOTAL
15 4.37 3.43 15.02
10 3.37 3.4 11.47
5 3.37 3.2 10.8
1 3.25 3.15 10.24
3 3.5 2.86 10.03
4 3.12 2.97 9.27
13 3 2.7 8.1
12 2.62 2.97 7.78
8 2.62 2.77 7.26
7 2.5 2.6 6.5
6 2.12 2.66 5.65
11 2.25 2.43 5.47
2 2 2.53 5.07
9 1.75 2.23 3.91
14 1.37 2.23 3.07

An examination of differences between the responses of brass, woodwind, and

string instrumentalists was investigated, either as a confounding variable or as fodder for

future research. Using the following null hypothesis as a directive, no significant

difference between the responses of the three instrument types was found.

H5: There is no difference between brass, woodwind, and string musician


response scores for the (A) or (B) versions of the etude.

Table 18

Means for Instrument type and response per excerpt.

INSTR Excerpt 'A' Excerpt 'B'


mean (SD) mean (SD)
brass 2.73, (1.0) 3.11, (.87)
woodwind 2.54, (.88) 2.86, (.83)
string 2.70, (.75) 2.79, (.41)
Sample mean & standard deviation (SD)

64
The Effect of Gesture

Table 18 displays the means and standard deviations for musician responses

sorted by instrument. Results of the ANOVA procedure (Table 19) indicate that there was

no interaction between instrument Type (INSTR) and excerpt A or B (EXCERPT). As

discussed earlier, Low and High group conductors elicited a higher response from

musicians while attempting the ‘B’ version of the excerpt (F = 4.71, p = .0318).

Table 19

ANOVA procedure for Instrument Type

Source DF F Value Pr > F


INSTR 2 1.16 .3169
EXCERPT 1 4.71 .0318
INSTR*EXCERPT 2 .32 .7209

Other Observations

Substantial quantities of visual data have been generated through this study.

Video recordings of conductor performances for both versions of the excerpt are quite

revealing, especially comparisons of the High group (top 5) to the Low group (bottom 5).

Highlighted evaluations of the etude displayed the interpretive markings that evaluators

heard musicians bring forth in their performances. There were certain trends that can be

discussed.

65
The Effect of Gesture

The vertical and horizontal planes utilized by the High GRI rated conductors

seemed to correspond with rhythmic and lyrical passages. The patterns of the High group

tended to become more vertical while conducting rhythmic passages and more horizontal

while conducting lyrical passages. The lateral plane of the High group conductors tended

to shift as the music increased and decreased in volume and tempi. When there was an

increase in volume or tempi, the lateral conducting plane seemed to move forward. When

there was a decrease in volume or tempi, the plane appeared pulled back.

The High group conductors tended to shift in and out of traditional beat patterns,

conducting more of a general phrase in space and time. This economy of gesture was

evident in the right hand beat pattern and in left hand cues. The Low group conductors

tended to maintain the basic beat patterns throughout the performance. Some

consistencies seemed evident and should be analyzed through future research designed to

appropriately deal with the qualitative data generated through this study.

The highlighted evaluations of the musician responses revealed that certain

articulation patterns, and dynamic tendencies might be intuitive. These intuitive

tendencies were evident in the baseline performances of the musicians, none of whom

collaborated in any way. A research design developed for the analysis of the intuitive

interpretive tendencies of musicians might reveal strong consistencies.

66
The Effect of Gesture

Summary

It is important to remember that the musicians had no expressive-interpretive

markings on their excerpt copy. Even so, the results indicate that when musicians

performed the excerpt while viewing high rated conductors, they were able to realize

substantially more of the expressive-interpretive markings than when they performed

while viewing lower rated conductors.

67
The Effect of Gesture

CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of non-verbal conducting

gestures on musicians’ stylistic response and determine if conducting gesture alone can

elicit consistent and accurate musical responses from musicians. It was determined that

some experienced instrumental conductors successfully utilize non-verbal conducting

gestures to communicate specific musical interpretations. It appeared that undergraduate

instrumental music majors (musicians) responded in favorable ways to the musical

interpretation of conductors who had command of a variety of conducting gestures. It

also appeared that those conductors were able to illicit specific musical responses from

the musicians. The results illustrated the existence of a type of social-cultural contract

among musicians: a contract that facilitates non-verbal communication expressed through

gestural conducting.

As discussed in Chapter One, effective conducting requires extensive knowledge

of the musical score combined with an ability to make musical decisions. According to

the results of this study, some of the experienced conductor participants lacked the

gestural technique and vocabulary necessary to convey musical decisions without verbal

instruction. There were however, conducting participants who possessed the technical

ability required to elicit a high degree of targeted response from musicians without the

use of verbal or facial cues.


68
The Effect of Gesture

The study of conducting has focused primarily on effective score study

techniques, verbal communication, basic conducting patterns, and facial cues. Few

researchers have examined the effects of gestural technique on performance (Sousa,

1988; Cofer, 1998; Graves, 1984; House, 1998; Sidoti, 1991). As discussed in Chapter

Two, one study developed an instrument that assesses the effect of conducting gestures

on the interpretive response of musicians (Karpicke, 1987). Using the Gestural Response

Instrument developed by Karpicke, this study has confirmed that there exists an unspoken

vocabulary that fine conductors possess. This unspoken vocabulary, when effectively

applied, may have precise influence on the interpretive response of musicians.

As described in the previous chapter, this study involved two groups of

performing participants: conductors and musicians. Conductors were randomly selected

from a pool of fifty experienced conductors, all residing in Central Florida. The

experiences of these instrumental conductors ranged from successful high school

conducting, college-university conducting, to professional band/orchestra conducting.

Quantitative data collected from the assessment of the conductor and musician groups

were analyzed.

Fifteen conductors (N=15) agreed to participate in this study. Two distinct

interpretations of a melodic excerpt were created. Each conductor was asked to conduct

both interpretive versions of the excerpt. These conducting performances were

videotaped and coded with conductor number and excerpt version (‘A’ or ‘B’).

69
The Effect of Gesture

Digital technology was used to blur the face of each conductor, thereby eliminating facial

expression and identification.

A panel of four experts evaluated the conductor video performances. These

performances were assessed in two ways. First, the evaluators used a marker pen to

highlight those interpretive items that the conductor conveyed adequately. This process

created a visual representation of the gestural effectiveness of each conductor on each

version of the excerpt. Secondly, the evaluators assigned a 1-5 Likert Score for each

conducting performance. This score was termed as the GRI Gesture score (GRI: Gesture

Response Instrument).

College Musicians from among four college/university music departments

participated in this study. The ability of the musicians varied and represented brass,

woodwind, and string instruments (N=25: 10 brass / 10 woodwind / 5 string).

The musicians were asked to perform from an unmarked edition of the Schubert

melody, Der Jungling Am Bache. Each musician first recorded a performance that

represented a personal interpretation of the excerpt. That initial recording was labeled as

the baseline performance. While viewing selected conductor videos, the musicians were

asked to perform the etude, as they believed the conductor interpreted it. Each

performance was recorded and coded with a musician number-code and the

corresponding conductor-excerpt code. This process created a baseline and post-with

conductor set of performances that corresponded with specific conductor video

performances.

70
The Effect of Gesture

Two panels of three evaluators listened to the musician responses (performances

with conductors). The musicians generated a total of 175 recordings of the excerpt. One

panel (Panel A) reviewed 84 recordings; the other panel (Panel B) reviewed 91

recordings. The evaluators used a marker pen to highlight those interpretive items that

they felt the musician conveyed adequately. This process created a visual representation

for the response of each musician, on each version of the etude. The evaluators then

assigned a 1-5 Likert Score for each of these musician responses. This score was termed

as the GRI Response score. Data were subjected to an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

technique to determine whether or not there were differences between baseline

performances and the musician responses generated while viewing conductors.

Educational Impact

Based on substantial accomplishments within their performance and conducting

realms, all of the conductor participants involved in this study were experienced

musicians-conductors. However, some were not able to consistently convey precise

musical messages. The conductor participants were given predetermined interpretations

of the melodic excerpt and were asked to communicate those predetermined musical

decisions. The high rated conductors, who possessed a highly developed gestural

vocabulary as it functions within an apparent conductor-musician contract, were able to

convey those predetermined articulations, dynamics, and phrase markings. Even with the

removal of verbal and facial cues, it appears that these conductors had developed the

71
The Effect of Gesture

ability to communicate musical decisions through conducting-gestural technique alone.

The existence of a social-cultural contract among musicians and conductors,

involving non-verbal communication may be supported through the results of this study.

Conductors who continually examine, develop, and exploit this conductor-musician

contract may become equipped to manage rehearsal time wisely and gain a higher and

efficient degree of interpretive response from the musicians under his/her baton.

Through conductor and music teacher training curricula, methods designed to

enhance the conductor-musician contract should be developed. With results of this study

in mind, the Elizabeth Green (1961) method of psychological conducting may be a most

appropriate component for conductor training and curricula. The Gestural Response

Instrument (Karpicke, 1987) utilized in this study was a reasonable measure of conductor

effectiveness and musician response. Use of the GRI as a suitable instructional tool in

conductor training is encouraged.

Trends that appeared in the video and evaluative process of this study may

suggest that the isolation of specific “emblems” (Sousa, 1988), the development of a

wide-ranging gestural vocabulary, and proper use of the horizontal, vertical, and lateral

planes appear key in the effective use of the conductor-musician contract.

For the music educator/conductor, the exploitation of conducting technique paired

with strong non-verbal communication may enhance a teacher’s ability to manage music

rehearsal behaviors and develop the conductor-musician contract in young

72
The Effect of Gesture

musicians. The research of Cofer (1998) and Sousa (1988) confirm that instruction in the

recognition of specific conducting gestures may enhance the interpretive performance of

young musicians. The conductor-musician contract may be the milieu from which

musical interpretation is communicated non-verbally. Methods that enhance

the development of this conductor-musician contract should be incorporated as a

dimension of ensemble training.

Future Research

As discussed in Chapter Four, the visual data generated though this study is ripe

for further research. Examination of the highlighted evaluations for the musician baseline

performances and responses displayed interpretive trends. It is possible that musicians

have developed intuitive interpretive approaches consistent with certain types of passages

and motives. If this is the case, one can surmise that the interpretive intuition of

musicians is also an aspect of the conductor-musician contract. A study designed to

examine the intuitive approaches of musicians would certainly add to research in music

perception, interpretation, and conducting.

This particular study categorized conductors as High, Medium, and Low gestural

communicators. Consistent differences seemed to exist in terms of pattern, use of plane,

gravity, and “emblems.” A study designed to examine the gestural tendencies of high

scoring conductors could aid in the development of effective methods for training

conductors in the use of gesture and exploitation of the conductor-musician contract.

73
The Effect of Gesture

This study focused on non-verbal communication through the baton and hands of

conductors. A study, which focuses on facial cues as non-verbal communication, could

utilize a similar research design and the Gestural Response Instrument (GRI). A

comparison of GRI scores generated through this study and the GRI scores generated

through a facial cues study would aid in the development of research based conducting

curricula and further exploration of the conductor-musician contract.

Although the results of this study are important, caution in the interpretation of

these outcomes is recommended. Researchers who might replicate this design are advised

to prepare the evaluators of conductor videos and musician responses through a more

rigorous procedure. Evaluators involved in this study were given written and verbal

instructions on the highlighting and Likert Scale scoring process. The implementation of

a more specific rubric that corroborates the number of highlighted responses with a Likert

Score rating is recommended (Ex: three highlights = Likert rating of ‘1’; eight highlights

= Likert rating of ‘3’; etc.). Although the reliability quotients of this study are acceptable

given the number of subjects, inter-rater reliability coefficients may rise if evaluators are

exposed to actual practice sessions.

The melodic excerpt used in this study was marked with two different

interpretations: however, the nature of both interpretations of the melody (Der Jungling

Am Bache) was lyrical. Future studies might utilize a wider variety of lyrical and

rhythmic interpretations. The implementation of this research design with individual

musicians, who were instructed to pay careful attention to the conductor’s gestures, may

74
The Effect of Gesture

exaggerate the ecological validity of the results. Individual musician performances were

evaluated (rather than ensemble performances) so that the influence of other musicians on

individual interpretation could be controlled. In order to increase ecological validity,

future designs might utilize pairings or small ensembles with one-on-a-part.

No significant difference was found between the responses of brass, woodwind,

and string instrumentalists. Even so, one should be reminded that the sample size of

respective instrument types were not large. A study designed to examine the question of

interpretive response and instrumentalist type might yield different results.

The suggestions outlined in this chapter are not inclusive of all possible related

areas of research that could add to the development of more effective instrumental

conducting methods.

Synthesis of Conducting Domains

The three domains that contribute to effective conducting must function in a

unified way: 1) knowledge and conceptualization of the score, 2) pedagogical ability to

coach toward a common interpretation, and 3) communication of interpretation through

verbal and non-verbal means (Wilcox, 2003). Conductors who excel in all three of these

domains have tremendous impact on the musical effect of ensembles under their baton.

This study has focused on one variable of the communication domain; non-verbal

conducting gestures. It appears that, through gesture alone, certain experienced

conductors possess an extraordinary ability to direct the expressive-interpretive

75
The Effect of Gesture

performance of musicians. While all of the conductors involved in this study were able to

perform basic conducting patterns and communicate simple changes in tempi and

dynamics, there were those who communicated far more. The continued study of

effective conducting techniques and gestures, and the development of methods that

exploit the apparent conductor-musician contract, will develop fine conductors who

effectively communicate broad and appropriate musical decisions.

76
The Effect of Gesture

References

Berlioz, H. (1970 republished). The conductor: the theory of his art. London: William

Reeves.

Bulwer, J. (1974 republished). Chirologia: or, the natural language of the hand, and

chironomia. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

Burnsed, V. (2001). Differences in preference for subtle dynamic nuance between

conductors, middle school music students, and elementary school students.

Journal of Research in Music Education 49(2), 49-56.

Campbell, W. & Heller, J. (1979). Convergence procedures for investigating music

listening tasks. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 59.

Cofer, R. S. (Fall 1998). Effects of conducting-gesture instruction on seventh-grade band

students’ performance response to conducting emblems. Journal of Research in

Music Education 46(3), 360-73.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum Associates.

Dunham, P., Lemke, M. & Moran, P. (1991). Effect of equal and random amounts of

varied practice on transfer task performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills 73,

673-674.

Elliott, D. (1995). Music matters. New York: Oxford University Press.

Fiske, H. (1990). Music and mind. Lewiston, NY: Mellon Press.

77
The Effect of Gesture

Fredrickson, W.E. (1992). An exploratory study of band musicians’ eye contact and

performance as influenced by loss of visual and/or aural stimuli. Ph.D. diss.,

Florida State University.

Fredrickson, W. E. (1997). Elementary, middle, and high school student perceptions of

tension in music. Journal of Research in Music Education 45(4), 626-635.

Gallops, W. (1999). Taking note of notes. Teaching Music 7(1), 32-35.

Graves, D. (1984). The choral conductor’s communication of musical interpretation.

Ph.D. diss. University of Connecticut.

Grechesky, R. (1985). An analysis of nonverbal and verbal conducting behaviors and

their relationship to expressive musical performance. Ph.D. diss., University of

Wisconsin. .

Green, E. (1961). The modern conductor. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

Heller, J. & Campbell, W. (1981). A theoretical model of music perception and talent.

Bulletin of the Council of Research in Music Education. 21-22.

Holahan, J. & Saunders, T.C. (1997). Children’s discrimination of tonal patterns: pattern

contour, response time, and item difficulty level. Bulletin of the Council for

Research in Music Education 132, 85-101.

House, R. (1998). Effects of expressive and non-expressive conducting on the

performance and attitudes of advanced instrumentalists. D.M.A. diss., Arizona

State University.

78
The Effect of Gesture

Jaccard, J. (1998). Interaction effects in factorial analysis of variance. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage.

Karpicke, H. A. (1987). Development of an instrument to assess conducting gesture and

validation of its use in orchestral performance. Ed.D. diss., University of Houston.

Lewis, D. (1999). Conducting musical shape. Ph.D. diss., Oklahoma State University.

Kromrey, J. & Dickenson, W. (1996). Detecting unit of analysis problems in nested

designs: statistical power and rates of the F test for groups-within-treatments

effects. Educational and Psychological Measurement 56, 215-231.

Madsen, C. & Fredrickson, W.E. (1993). The experience of musical tension: a replication

of Nielsen’s research using the continuous response digital interface. Journal of

Music Therapy 30, 46-63.

Madsen, C. & Madsen, C. (1978). Experimental research in music. Raleigh, NC:

Contemporary Publishing Co..

Madsen, C. & Moore, R. S. (1978). Experimental research in music: workbook in

design and statistical tests. Raleigh, NC: Contemporary Publishing Co..

Mayne, R. (1992). Musical conducting gestures and their interpretation. Ph.D. diss.,

Ohio State University.

Meyer, L. B. (1991). A pride of prejudices; or, delight in diversity. Music Theory

Spectrum 13(2), 241-51.

79
The Effect of Gesture

Meyer, L. B. (1976). Grammatical simplicity and relational richness. Critical Inquiry

2(2): 693-761.

Meyer, L.B. (2000). The spheres of music: a gathering of essays. Chicago: University of

Chicago Press.

Miller, S. (1988). The effect of labian movement theory on the ability of student

conductors to communicate musical interpretation. Ph.D. diss., University of

Wisconsin.

Moerbeek, M.,van Breukelen, J & Berger, M. (2000). Design issues for experiments in

multi-level populations. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics 25(3),

271-284.

Rudolf, M. (1950). The grammar of conducting. NewYork: Schirmer.

Schuller, G. (1997).The complete conductor. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Skadsem, J. A. Effect of conductor verbalization, dynamic markings, conductor gesture,

and choir dynamic level on singer’s dynamic response. Journal of Research in

Music Education 45 n4, (Winter 1997): 508-519.

Sidoti, V.J. (1990). The effects of expressive and non-expressive conducting on the

performance accuracy of selected expression markings by individual high school

instrumentalists. Ph.D. diss., Ohio State University. Dissertation Abstracts

International 51 (3270A).

80
The Effect of Gesture

Sousa, G. (1988). Musical conducting emblems: An investigation of the use of specific

conducting gestures and their interpretation by instrumental performers., Ph.D.

diss., Ohio State University. Dissertation Abstracts International 49 (2143A).

Strouse, L. (1987). From analysis to gesture: A comprehensive approach to the score.

Ph.D. diss., Ball State University.

Tighe, T. & Dowling, J. (1993). Procedural and declarative knowledge in music

cognition and education. Psychology and music: the understanding of melody

and rhythm. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum Press.

VanWeelden, Kimberly (2002). Relationships between perceptions of conducting

effectiveness and ensemble performance. Journal of Research in Music Education

50(2), 165-76.

Wilcox, D. (2003). Unpublished paper presented at the Midwest Band and Orchestra

Conducting Clinic. Chicago, IL.

Zucker, D. (1990). An analysis of variance pitfall: the fixed effects analysis in a nested

design. Educational and Psychological Measurement 50 (Winter), 731-735

81
APPENDICE

i
APPENDIX A: Demographics of Conducting Participants

Conductor education experience gender instrument(s) level exper.

I BME Beg/Adv Male trp HS

II BA Beg/Adv Male tromb/tba C


Sem/Pvt

III BME/MM Beg/Adv/Sem Male tba/pno MS/HS/C/CM/Pro


DMA

IV BME/MM Beg/Sem/Pvt Male cl MS/HS/C

V BME/MME Beg/Adv Female fl MS/HS

VI MME/MA Beg/Adv Male cl/sx/fl MS/HS/C/CM

VII BME/MA Beg/Adv/Sem/Pvt Female tromb HS/C/CM

VIII BME Beg/Adv Male vla/vln MS/HS

IX BME Beg/Adv Male tromb MS/HS

X BME Beg/Adv/Sem/Pvt Female hn MS/HS

XI BA/MM Beg/Sem/Pvt Male trp MS/HS/C/CM

XII BA/MM Beg/Adv/Sem/Pvt Male pno/voc HS/C/CM/Pro

XIII BME/MM Beg/Adv/Sem/Pvt Male tromb MS/HS/C/CM


Ph.D.

XIV BA Music Beg/Adv/Sem/Pvt Male fl/hn HS

BA Music
XV /MME/post Beg/Adv/Sem/Pvt Male clar/pno/voc MS/HS/C/CM/Pro
MME/post
Education: BA/BM/BME=Bachelors Degree in Music or Music Education
MA/MME/MM= Masters Degree in Music or Music Education
Experience: Beg= beginning conducting course / Adv= advanced conducting course / Sem= seminar in
conducting / Pvt=private instruction in conducting
Level Experience: MS=middle school band-orchestra / HS=high school band-orchestra /
C= college band-orchestra / CM=community band-orchestra / Pro= professional band-orchestra

i
APPENDIX B: Musician Nesting With Conductor Videos

Conductor Excerpt Musicians nested to each conductor video


I A B1 B3 W1 W3 S1
B B5 B7 W5 W7 S5
II A B2 B9 W2 W9 S2
B B4 B6 W4 W6 S4
III A B8 B10 W8 W10 S3
B B1 B3 W1 W3 S1
IV A B5 B7 W5 W7 S5
B B2 B9 W2 W9 S2
V A B4 B6 W4 W6 S4
B B8 B10 W8 W10 S3
VI A B1 B3 W1 W3 S1
B B5 B7 W5 W7 S5
VII A B2 B6 W2 W6 S2
B B4 B6 W4 W6 S4
VIII A B8 B10 W8 W10 S3
B B1 B3 W1 W3 S1
IX A B5 B7 W5 W7 S5
B B2 B9 W2 W9 S2
X A B4 B6 W4 W6 S4
B B8 B10 W8 W10 S3
XI A B1 B3 W1 W3 S1
B B5 B7 W5 W7 S5
XII A B2 B9 W2 W9 S2
B B4 B6 W4 W6 S4
XIII A B8 B10 W8 W10 S3
B B1 B3 W1 W3 S1
XIV A B5 B7 W5 W7 S5
B B2 B9 W2 W9 S2
XV A B4 B6 W4 W6 S4
B B8 B10 W8 W10 S3

ii
APPENDIX C: Unmarked Musical Excerpt used in the study.

iii
APPENDIX D: Marked Versions ‘A’ and ‘B’ of the excerpt used in the study.

iv
APPENDIX E: Sample of Marked and Evaluated Excerpts ‘A’ & ‘B’ and Description

musician code evaluator


response rating conductor number
and excerpt

highlight of perceived response

v
APPENDIX F: Musician Response Scores for Each Conductor Nest

Version ‘A’ Version ‘B’


GRI
Musician Response
CONDUCTOR MUSICIAN IA IIA IIIA IB IIB IIIB Response mean Score
1 1 2 4 3 5 5 4 3.428571429
1 2 2 3 2 3 3 4 2.833333333
1 3 2 4 3 3 5 3 3.333333333
1 4 2 3 3 3 3 3 2.833333333
1 5 3 4 2 3 4 4 3.333333333
RespTOTAL
Cond 1 3.152
2 1 2 3 2 1 2 2 2
2 2 2 3 3 4 2 3 2.833333333
2 3 3 5 3 1 2 2 2.666666667
2 4 2 2 2 3 4 4 2.833333333
2 5 1 2 2 3 2 4 2.333333333
RespTOTAL
Cond 2 2.533
3 1 3 5 5 1 4 3 3.5
3 2 3 3 3 2 4 2 2.833333333
3 3 1 2 2 1 3 3 2
3 4 3 3 3 3 3 3 3
3 5 2 4 2 3 4 3 3
RespTOTAL
Cond 3 2.866
4 1 2 4 2 3 4 2 2.833333333
4 2 1 3 2 3 3 4 2.666666667
4 3 1 4 2 5 4 3 3.166666667
4 4 2 3 2 4 3 4 3
4 5 3 4 4 1 4 3 3.166666667
RespTOTAL
Cond 4 2.966
5 1 1 1 1 4 4 5 2.666666667
5 2 5 4 4 4 4 4 4.166666667
5 3 1 3 2 2 2 3 2.166666667
5 4 4 4 3 3 3 4 3.5
5 5 3 4 4 3 4 3 3.5
RespTOTAL
Cond 5 3.2
6 1 2 4 3 2 3 2 2.428571429
6 2 2 4 2 2 3 3 2.571428571
6 3 2 3 3 2 3 2 2.571428571
6 4 1 1 2 4 4 3 2.714285714
6 5 2 4 2 2 3 3 3
RespTOTAL
Cond 6 2.657

vi
APPENDIX F (continued)
7 1 3 5 3 1 2 2 2.666666667
7 2 2 3 2 2 3 4 2.666666667
7 3 2 3 2 2 3 2 2.333333333
7 4 1 3 3 1 3 3 2.333333333
7 5 2 3 2 4 3 4 3
RespTOTAL
Cond 7 2.6
8 1 4 4 4 2 4 3 3.5
8 2 1 4 3 2 4 3 2.833333333
8 3 3 5 3 2 3 4 3.333333333
8 4 1 2 2 2 2 2 1.833333333
8 5 2 3 2 2 3 2 2.333333333
RespTOTAL
Cond 8 2.766
9 1 1 1 1 4 5 3 2.5
9 2 1 2 2 3 3 3 2.333333333
9 3 2 1 1 5 4 2 2.5
9 4 1 2 2 1 1 1 1.333333333
9 5 3 2 3 2 3 2 2.5
RespTOTAL
Cond 9 2.233
10 1 1 2 2 5 4 5 3.166666667
10 2 4 4 4 3 5 4 4
10 3 1 4 3 1 2 3 2.333333333
10 4 4 5 4 5 3 5 4.333333333
10 5 4 3 4 2 4 2 3.166666667
RespTOTAL
Cond 10 3.4
11 1 3 3 2 2 3 2 2.5
11 2 2 3 1 2 3 2 2.166666667
11 3 1 3 3 4 3 2 2.666666667
11 4 1 3 2 3 3 2 2.333333333
11 5 2 4 2 1 3 3 2.5
RespTOTAL
Cond 11 2.433
12 1 5 5 3 2 3 1 3.166666667
12 2 2 2 2 4 4 5 3.166666667
12 3 4 4 3 1 3 3 3
12 4 4 2 3 2 4 3 3
12 5 1 3 2 3 2 4 2.5
RespTOTAL
Cond 12 2.966
13 1 3 5 4 3 3 2 3.333333333
13 2 3 3 4 3 4 2 3.166666667
13 3 2 2 1 1 2 2 1.666666667
13 4 3 4 3 2 3 3 3
13 5 1 3 2 2 4 2 2.333333333

vii
APPENDIX F (continued)
RespTOTAL
Cond 13 2.7
14 1 1 2 2 2 4 2 2.166666667
14 2 1 3 2 2 3 2 2.166666667
14 3 2 2 2 5 3 3 2.833333333
14 4 2 2 2 1 2 3 2
14 5 1 2 2 2 3 2 2
RespTOTAL
Cond 14 2.233
15 1 1 2 2 4 4 5 3
15 2 5 4 4 4 4 4 4.166666667
15 3 1 3 3 3 3 3 2.666666667
15 4 3 4 4 4 5 4 4
15 5 4 4 4 2 3 3 3.333333333
RespTOTAL
Cond 15 3.433

viii
APPENDIX G: Evaluation of POST (w/Conductor) Musician Responses

Panel A: Evaluation of Musician Responses

conductor-
excerpt MUSICIAN evaluator I evaluator II evaluator III

1a 1 2 4 3
11a 1 2 4 3
16a 1 3 3 2
3a 2 2 3 2
12a 2 4 5 3
17a 2 5 5 3
1a 3 2 3 2
11a 3 2 4 2
16a 3 2 3 1
20a 4 1 2 2
10a 4 1 1 1
15a 4 1 2 2
9a 5 2 4 2
14a 5 1 1 1
19a 5 1 2 2
8b 1 2 4 3
13b 1 2 4 3
18b 1 3 3 2
9b 2 3 4 2
14b 2 5 5 3
19b 2 2 4 2
8b 3 2 4 2
13b 3 2 4 3
18b 3 3 4 2
3b 4 1 2 2
12b 4 1 2 2
17b 4 2 3 1
1b 5 5 5 4
11b 5 2 3 2
16b 5 2 3 2
1a 11 2 4 3
11a 11 2 3 3
16a 11 1 3 3
3a 12 3 5 3
12a 12 3 5 3
17a 12 4 4 3
1a 13 2 3 3
11a 13 1 1 2

ix
APPENDIX G (continued)
16a 13 1 3 2
20a 14 1 3 3
10a 14 1 3 2
15a 14 1 4 3
9a 15 1 4 2
14a 15 2 1 1
19a 15 2 2 2
8b 11 1 3 3
13b 11 2 3 4
18b 11 1 2 2
9b 12 5 4 3
14b 12 5 4 2
19b 12 5 3 3
8b 13 3 3 3
13b 13 2 2 2
18b 13 2 3 3
3b 14 1 2 2
12b 14 2 3 2
17b 14 1 3 3
1b 15 3 5 3
11b 15 2 3 2
16b 15 4 3 2
1a 21 3 4 2
11a 21 2 4 2
16a 21 2 4 2
3a 22 1 2 2
12a 22 2 3 2
17a 22 1 3 2
18a 23 1 3 2
8a 23 2 4 2
13a 23 2 3 2
8b 21 3 4 3
13b 21 2 3 2
18b 21 2 4 2
9b 22 1 4 3
14b 22 2 3 2
19b 22 2 3 2
20b 23 2 3 3
10b 23 3 4 3
15b 23 2 4 2

x
APPENDIX G (continued)
Panel B: Evaluation of Musician Responses

cond-excerpt MUSICIAN evaluator IV evaluator V evaluator VI

20a 6 4 3 5
10a 6 4 4 5
15a 6 4 4 4
9a 7 2 3 1
14a 7 2 2 1
19a 7 2 3 1
18a 8 4 5 3
8a 8 5 5 3
13a 8 4 4 4
3a 9 3 3 2
12a 9 2 3 2
17a 9 2 2 2
18a 10 4 3 3
8a 10 3 3 2
13a 10 3 4 1
3b 6 3 2 4
12b 6 4 3 2
17b 6 5 4 4
1b 7 4 3 3
11b 7 3 3 2
16b 7 2 3 2
20b 8 5 4 4
10b 8 5 4 4
15b 8 5 4 5
9b 9 4 3 3
14b 9 3 3 3
19b 9 3 2 1
20b 10 4 4 4
10b 10 4 4 4
15b 10 4 5 3
20a 16 4 4 3
10a 16 3 4 4
15a 16 4 5 4
9a 17 2 3 2
14a 17 2 2 1
19a 17 2 2 2
18a 18 1 2 2
8a 18 2 1 1
13a 18 3 2 1

xi
APPENDIX G (continued)
3a 19 2 2 2
12a 19 3 3 3
17a 19 3 2 4
18a 20 3 4 3
8a 20 3 3 3
13a 20 2 2 1
3b 16 4 4 3
12b 16 3 3 1
17b 16 3 4 2
1b 17 3 3 3
11b 17 3 4 4
16b 17 2 3 3
20b 18 3 3 3
10b 18 3 2 2
15b 18 3 2 1
9b 19 4 3 4
14b 19 1 1 1
19b 19 3 2 1
20b 20 4 5 4
10b 20 4 3 3
15b 20 5 3 5
20a 24 4 4 4
10a 24 4 4 3
15a 24 4 3 4
9a 25 4 4 3
14a 25 3 2 3
19a 25 2 2 1
3b 24 4 2 3
12b 24 4 3 4
17b 24 4 2 3
1b 25 4 4 3
11b 25 3 3 2
16b 25 3 3 1

xii
About the Author

R. Wayne Gallops serves as Director of Instrumental Music Education and Jazz

Studies at Radford University (Virginia). His responsibilities include courses in music

education, conducting, jazz piano, and direction of the RU Jazz Ensembles. Prior to his

appointment at Radford University, Professor Gallops served on the music faculties of

the University of Tampa and Hillsborough Community College. He performs and

conducts throughout the United States and his K-12 public school teaching experience

spans sixteen years. Mr. Gallops holds degrees from Hillsborough Community College,

the University of Tampa, Florida State University, and entered the Ph.D. program at the

University of South Florida in 2001.

61

You might also like