Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

J Bus Ethics

DOI 10.1007/s10551-014-2292-7

Does it take two to Tangle? Subordinates’ Perceptions


of and Reactions to Abusive Supervision
Gang Wang • Peter D. Harms • Jeremy D. Mackey

Received: 20 August 2013 / Accepted: 11 July 2014


Ó Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Abstract Research on abusive supervision is imbalanced job performance. In addition, the authors investigated the
in two ways. First, with most research attention focused on moderating effect of subordinates’ personality on the
the destructive consequences of abusive supervision, there relationship between perceptions of abusive supervision
has been relatively little work on subordinate-related pre- and subordinates’ interpersonal deviance. Consistent with
dictors of perceptions of abusive supervision. Second, with trait activation theory, subordinates low in both agree-
most research on abusive supervision centered on its main ableness and extraversion were more likely to engage in
effects and the moderating effects of supervisor-related deviant behaviors in response to perceptions of abusive
factors, there is little understanding of how subordinate supervision than subordinates high in either or both
factors can moderate the main effects of perceptions of agreeableness and extraversion.
abusive supervision on workplace outcomes. The current
study aims to advance knowledge of the roles of subordi- Keywords Abusive supervision  Personality 
nates in the formation of and reactions to perceptions of Interpersonal deviance  Task performance
abusive supervision. Specifically, based on victim precip-
itation theory, the authors examined subordinates’ per-
sonality traits and self-reports of task performance as Abusive supervision is defined as ‘‘subordinates’ percep-
antecedents of perceptions of abusive supervision. The tions of the extent to which supervisors engage in the
results show that subordinates high in neuroticism or low in sustained display of hostile verbal and non-verbal behav-
conscientiousness had high levels of perceived abusive iors, excluding physical contact’’ (Tepper 2000, p. 178).
supervision partially through their self-reported deleterious Research suggests that abusive supervision can have severe
consequences on subordinates’ well-being (e.g., Duffy
et al. 2002; Hoobler and Brass 2006), attitudes (e.g., Tep-
G. Wang (&) per et al. 2004), and deviant behavior (e.g., Burton and
Department of Management, College of Business, Florida State Hoobler 2011; Lian et al. 2012; Liu et al. 2010; Tepper
University, 821 Academic Way, P.O. Box 3061110, Tallahassee, et al. 2009; Thau et al. 2009).
FL 32306-1110, USA
Despite the growing body of research on abusive
e-mail: gwang5@business.fsu.edu
supervision in the past decade (Martinko et al. 2013), rel-
P. D. Harms atively little research attention has been paid to predictors
College of Business Administration, University of Nebraska- of abusive supervision, especially predictors related to
Lincoln, CBA 265, P.O. Box 880491, Lincoln, NE 68588-0491,
subordinates (e.g., Henle and Gross 2014; Kiazad et al.
USA
e-mail: pharms2@unl.edu 2010; Kiewitz et al. 2012; Mawritz et al. 2012; Restubog
et al. 2011; Shoss et al. 2013). This is surprising given that
J. D. Mackey victim precipitation theory (e.g., Aquino 2000; Aquino and
Department of Management, College of Business, Florida State
Lamertz 2004; Curtis 1974; Elias 1986) suggests that some
University, 821 Academic Way, P.O. Box 3061110, Tallahassee,
FL 32306-1110, USA subordinates are likely to play a role in the abusive
e-mail: jdm10e@my.fsu.edu supervision process. Moreover, with most research on

123
G. Wang et al.

abusive supervision centered on its main effects and the Second, we examine interpersonal deviance (behaviors
moderating effects of supervisor-level factors (e.g., supervisor that violate organizational norms and harm other employ-
hostile attribution bias, Hoobler and Brass 2006; supervisor ees, such as making fun of others and saying harmful things
management style, Thau et al. 2009), there has been relatively to others; Robinson and Bennett 1995) as a potential con-
little work on the moderating effects of subordinate-level sequence of perceptions of abusive supervision. Past
factors on the relationships between perceptions of abusive research suggests that employees tend to engage in nega-
supervision and workplace outcomes (Burton and Hoobler tive social behaviors (e.g., interpersonal deviance) fol-
2011; Nandkeolyar et al. 2014; Tepper 2007). lowing their supervisors’ ‘‘role modeling’’ (Bandura 1973;
Thus, the purpose of this study is two-fold: to advance our Berkowitz 1993; O’Leary-Kelly et al. 1996). In addition,
understanding of factors that may influence subordinates’ there is evidence that employees direct their dysfunctional
perceptions of and reactions to abusive supervision. First, responses to perceived abusive supervision toward their
based on victim precipitation theory (e.g., Aquino 2000; supervisors and coworkers (e.g., Liu et al. 2010; Mitchell
Aquino and Lamertz 2004; Curtis 1974; Elias 1986), we focus and Ambrose 2007). More importantly, because one pur-
on subordinates’ personality traits (i.e., neuroticism and con- pose of the current research is to study the roles of
scientiousness) and task performance to examine the extent to employees’ interpersonal traits in their potential reactions
which these factors may give rise to perceptions of abusive to perceptions of abusive supervision, interpersonal devi-
supervision. Recently, based on a sample of mostly part-time ance is deemed to be a relevant behavioral outcome of
working undergraduates, Henle and Gross (2014) hypothe- interest.
sized and found that emotional stability (i.e., reverse-scored Although prior research (e.g., Inness et al. 2005;
neuroticism) and conscientiousness were both antecedents Mitchell and Ambrose 2007) reported a positive relation-
negatively associated with perceptions of abusive supervision, ship between perceptions of abusive supervision and sub-
and that negative emotions mediated the negative relation- ordinates’ interpersonal deviance, the boundary conditions
ships of these antecedents with perceptions of abusive of this relationship remain less clear. As an exception, Lian
supervision. Henle and Gross contributed to our initial et al. (2012) found a significant moderating effect of sub-
understanding of the roles of subordinates’ personality in the ordinates’ power distance orientation on the relationship
formation of perceptions of abusive supervision. To extend between abusive supervision and subordinates’ interper-
Henle and Gross’ findings, we use a sample of full-time sonal deviance, such that the relationship was stronger for
employees to examine neuroticism and conscientiousness as subordinates with high-power distance orientation than
antecedents of perceptions of abusive supervision, as well as subordinates with low-power distance orientation. To fur-
the mediating role of task performance in this relationship. ther advance knowledge on the deleterious effects of abu-
Moreover, to complement Henle and Gross, we also study the sive supervision and inform practice, we argue that it is
consequences of abusive supervision (i.e., interpersonal important to examine whether and how subordinates’ Five-
deviance) and the boundary conditions of the consequences Factor Model (FFM) personality traits may moderate the
(i.e., agreeableness and extraversion). relationship between abusive supervision and interpersonal
Also, we extend Tepper et al.’s (2011) study, which deviance. Drawing from trait activation theory (Tett and
examined supervisors’ perceived deep-level dissimilarity Guterman 2000), we expect that subordinates low in
with subordinates, supervisors’ perceived relationship agreeableness and extraversion are more likely to recipro-
conflict with subordinates, and supervisors’ ratings of cate perceptions of abusive supervision with interpersonal
subordinates’ task performance as predictors of perceptions deviance than subordinates high in agreeableness and/or
of abusive supervision. Tepper et al. found that supervi- extraversion. Figure 1 depicts the theoretical model tested
sors’ perceptions of relationship conflict and subordinates’ in this study.
performance both mediated the relationship between This study makes several contributions to the abusive
supervisors’ perceived deep-level dissimilarity and subor- supervision literature. First, this study theoretically identi-
dinates’ reports of abusive supervision, and that deep-level fies and empirically examines two subordinate-level factors
dissimilarity had an indirect effect on perceptions of abu- that may predict perceptions of supervisory abuse. The
sive supervision through relationship conflict for low-per- limited extant research on antecedents of perceived abusive
forming subordinates. Unlike Tepper et al., we focus on the supervision has largely kept supervisors in the center of the
roles of subordinates’ personality and self-reported task stage, but this leader-focused approach is one-sided and
performance in the formation of perceptions of abusive problematic because some followers may contribute to
supervision. We believe that it is important to advance abusive supervision (Aquino and Bradfield 2000; Henle
knowledge of whether and how subordinates high (or low) and Gross 2014; Matthiesen and Einarsen 2001; Tepper
in some personality traits are more (or less) likely to per- 2007). Thus, it is important to examine subordinates’ roles
ceive abusive supervision than others. in driving perceptions of abusive supervision.

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

Subordinates’
Agreeableness

Subordinates’
Extraversion
Subordinates’
Neuroticism

Subordinates’ Perceptions of Interpersonal


Task Performance Abusive Supervision Deviance

Subordinates’
Conscientiousness

Fig. 1 Theoretical model tested in this study

Second, this study contributes to the abusive supervision in hostile and aggressive responses from potential perpe-
literature by examining subordinates’ personality traits as trators (e.g., Aquino 2000; Aquino and Lamertz 2004;
boundary conditions of the relationship between percep- Curtis 1974; Elias 1986). With respect to individual char-
tions of abusive supervision and interpersonal deviance. acteristics, victim precipitation researchers (e.g., Aquino
Finally, this study contributes to trait activation theory and Lamertz 2004; Olweus 1978) argue that victims who
(Tett and Guterman 2000) by applying this theory to the appear to be submissive and vulnerable and victims who
abusive supervision literature and empirically examining appear provocative because of their ‘‘personality driven
its external validity in a field setting. This study represents attitudes, emotions, or behaviors that induce tension and
one of the first attempts to use trait activation theory to conflict’’ (Henle and Gross 2014, p. 463) can become tar-
investigate subordinates’ reactions to perceived abusive gets of abuse. With respect to behaviors, research on victim
supervision with respect to interpersonal deviance. precipitation suggests that behaviors that are disliked,
frustrating, and aggravating tend to give rise to abuse
(Olweus 1978; Tepper et al. 2011). Based on victim pre-
Theoretical Background and Hypotheses cipitation theory (e.g., Aquino 2000; Aquino and Lamertz
2004; Curtis 1974; Elias 1986), we argue that subordinates’
Subordinates’ Personality, Performance, and Abusive personality traits and task performance may play important
Supervision roles in the formation of abusive supervision.

What role do subordinates play in the formation of abusive


supervision? As the old saying goes, it takes two to tangle. Main Effects of Subordinates’ Neuroticism and
Although abusive supervision has often been hypothesized Conscientiousness on Abusive Supervision
as a form of supervisors’ retaliation against injustice they
experience from their superiors or the organization (e.g., Personality traits represent individuals’ propensities to
Aryee et al. 2007; Hoobler and Brass 2006; Tepper et al. behave in identifiable ways; these traits are intraindividu-
2006), and the popular press often portrays subordinates as ally stable and interindividually distinct (Digman 1990).
victims of abusive supervision (Jantz and McMurray Although researchers have used different structures to
2009), theoretical and empirical research suggests that capture people’s personality, the FFM has emerged as the
subordinates’ personality and in-role behavior may predict most widely accepted and studied personality framework
perceptions of abusive supervision (Henle and Gross 2014; (e.g., Barrick and Mount 1991; Oh et al. 2011). The FFM
Tepper et al. 2011). Research on harm-doing in various life includes the traits of neuroticism, conscientiousness,
domains provides evidence that some victims of abusive extraversion, agreeableness, and openness to experience
relationships play a role in abusive actions (e.g., Aquino (Costa and McCrae 1992). As explained in detail in the
2000; Aquino and Bradfield 2000; Elias 1986; Murphy and following section, victim precipitation theory (e.g., Aquino
O’Leary 1989). 2000; Aquino and Lamertz 2004; Curtis 1974; Elias 1986)
Specifically, victim precipitation theory posits that some suggests that subordinates high in neuroticism and/or low
individual characteristics and behaviors are likely to result in conscientiousness are more likely to play a role in

123
G. Wang et al.

abusive supervision than subordinates low in neuroticism accomplishment of unit goals (Bass 1990; LePine et al.
and/or high in conscientiousness. 1997; Tepper et al. 2011). According to victim precipita-
Individuals high in neuroticism tend to behave with tion theory, supervisors are likely to be frustrated and
anxiety, impulsiveness, anger, and irritability (Costa and annoyed by subordinates low in conscientiousness, which
McCrae 1992). They also likely experience depression and may make them easy targets of negative supervisory
stress, ruminate about negative events, and express nega- behaviors (e.g., Aquino 2000; Aquino and Bradfield 2000;
tive emotions and attitudes (e.g., Colbert et al. 2012; Die- Aquino and Lamertz 2004; Curtis 1974; Elias 1986). Henle
fendorff and Richard 2003; Henle and Gross 2014; Roelofs and Gross (2014) reported a negative relationship between
et al. 2008; Trierweiler et al. 2002). Because highly neu- subordinates’ conscientiousness and perceptions of abusive
rotic employees consistently show anger, anxiety, and supervision. To replicate Henle and Gross’ findings with a
irritability, supervisors will experience difficulty working full-time adult working sample, we propose the following
with these employees and perceive these employees as hypothesis:
disturbing and annoying (Watson and Clark 1984). In
Hypothesis 2 Subordinates’ conscientiousness will be
addition, neurotic employees’ frequent expressions of
negatively related to their perceptions of abusive
anger and irritability break social norms, which can be
supervision.
detrimental to team cohesion and team organizational cit-
izenship behavior (Chen et al. 2005). As such, supervisors
are likely to perceive neurotic subordinates as demanding, Mediating Roles of Subordinates’ Task Performance
threatening, and ‘‘bad apples’’ or ‘‘downers’’ who drag
down positive group processes (Bradley 2008). The victim precipitation literature suggests that one
Victim precipitation theory suggests that neurotic mechanism through which subordinates’ neuroticism and
employees will appear to be provocative to supervisors conscientiousness may give rise to abusive supervision is
because they induce tension and conflict at work (e.g., their task performance (Olweus 1978; Tepper et al. 2011).
Aquino 2000; Aquino and Lamertz 2004; Curtis 1974; Task performance refers to work behaviors that are stipu-
Elias 1986). In addition to supervisors’ potentially lated by a formal job description (Borman and Motowidlo
increasing negative actions toward neurotic subordinates, 1993; Harrison et al. 2006). As Aquino et al. (1999) noted,
highly neurotic subordinates may be more likely than less individuals high in neuroticism tend to have performance
neurotic subordinates to perceive supervisory behavior as problems because they are more prone to engage in dys-
abusive (Henle and Gross 2014). As previously noted, functional anxiety-driven thought processes and spend
Henle and Gross (2014) used a victim precipitation more time ruminating about negative events than subordi-
framework and a sample of mostly part-time working nates low in neuroticism. Consequently, highly neurotic
undergraduates to hypothesize and find a positive rela- employees usually have less energy and attention to focus
tionship between subordinates’ neuroticism and percep- on their task activities (Colbert et al. 2004) than less neu-
tions of abusive supervision. However, this study will rotic employees. Several meta-analyses have demonstrated
conduct a much needed replication and extension of Henle a significant negative relationship between neuroticism and
and Gross’ findings by utilizing full-time working adults, task performance (e.g., Hurtz and Donovan 2000; Judge
who represent a different population than part-time work- et al. 2013; Mount et al. 1998; Shaffer and Postlethwaite
ing undergraduates (Roth et al. 2011). We propose the 2012). Therefore, we expect that neuroticism will be neg-
following hypothesis: atively related to task performance.
In addition, individuals low in conscientiousness are not
Hypothesis 1 Subordinates’ neuroticism will be posi-
likely to set clear goals or to persistently exert effort to
tively related to their perceptions of abusive supervision.
achieve goals (Mount and Barrick 1995). Conscientious-
whereas neuroticism describes levels of emotional sta- ness was repeatedly reported to be a significant predictor of
bility, conscientiousness describes individuals’ work employee job performance across a number of relevant
motivation, organization and planning, and self-control meta-analyses (e.g., Barrick and Mount 1991; Barrick et al.
(Costa and McCrae 1992). Highly conscientious individu- 2001; Oh et al. 2011; Shaffer and Postlethwaite 2012).
als are dutiful, organized, competent, and goal-oriented. In Further, Judge et al.’s (2013) meta-analytic test of the
contrast, individuals who score low on conscientiousness effects of conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism,
tend to be less organized, more laid back, less driven by openness to experience, and extraversion on performance
success, and less dependable than highly conscientious found that conscientiousness and neuroticism were strong
individuals (Chernyshenko et al. 2011). In the workplace, predictors of task performance, even when controlling for
subordinates low in conscientiousness frequently need the effects of the other personality traits. In sum, the
additional supervision because they tend to impede the aforementioned research suggests that conscientiousness

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

will be positively related to task performance, and that threatens the well-being of an organization, its members, or
neuroticism will be negatively related to task performance. both’’ (Robinson and Bennett 1995, p. 556). In terms of its
Previous research has mainly treated subordinates’ task intended target, workplace deviance is further theoretically
performance as an outcome of perceptions of abusive categorized into interpersonal and organizational deviance
supervision (e.g., Harris et al. 2007). This is understandable (Robinson and Bennett 1995, 1997). Interpersonal deviance
given that job performance is usually regarded as an refers to deviant behaviors directed at individuals (e.g.,
important end-state outcome variable in organizational supervisors and coworkers), whereas organizational devi-
research (Organ and Paine 1999). However, victim pre- ance includes deviant behaviors targeted toward the orga-
cipitation theory suggests that followers’ low-task perfor- nization (e.g., purposefully working slow, destroying
mance may be positively associated with abusive company property, or stealing from the company).
supervision because poor subordinate performance may In this study, we focused on interpersonal deviance as an
threaten the accomplishment of group goals. This tends to outcome of perceptions of abusive supervision for the
influence supervisors’ performance appraisals, pay raises, following reasons. First, prior research suggests that
and/or promotions because supervisors are usually made interpersonal deviance represents a negative social behav-
accountable for their unit performance indicators and their ior that subordinates tend to exhibit when perceiving mis-
followers’ job performance (Tepper et al. 2011). Thus, treatment (Bandura 1973; Berkowitz 1993; O’Leary-Kelly
supervisors are likely to perceive that low-performing et al. 1996). Second, theoretical and empirical research
subordinates break psychological contracts (Rousseau suggests that when experiencing abusive supervision,
1995), are annoying, and are threatening. employees are likely to retaliate against both the harm-doer
According to victim precipitation theory, these subor- (i.e., supervisor; Mitchell and Ambrose 2007) and other
dinates demonstrate profiles of provocative victims and scapegoats (e.g., coworkers; Liu et al. 2010; Restubog et al.
may elicit supervisory behaviors associated with percep- 2011; Tepper et al. 2009; Thau and Mitchell 2010; Wang
tions of abusive supervision. Alternatively, supervisors’ et al. 2012). Third, as we will explain in detail in the next
actions following poor performance may be intended to section, given that we examine how subordinates’ person-
motivate and enhance performance, but may be construed ality traits (i.e., agreeableness and extraversion) that
as abusive (Liu et al. 2010). Consistent with victim pre- clearly distinguish how their behavioral tendencies in
cipitation theory, Tepper et al. (2011) argued and demon- social interactions may influence their responses to per-
strated that supervisor-rated task performance negatively ceptions of abusive supervision (Tett and Guterman 2000),
predicted perceptions of abusive supervision when these it stands to reason that interpersonal deviance is a relevant
constructs were measured at the same point in time. behavioral outcome in such negative social interactions.
Therefore, subordinates’ task performance is likely to be Thus, we believe that a general measure of interpersonal
negatively related to abusive supervision. deviance serves as a comprehensive and distal consequence
Taken together, the foregoing suggests that subordinates of perceptions of abusive supervision in the current study.
high in neuroticism and/or low in conscientiousness are Given that organizational deviance is not interpersonal, we
likely to demonstrate low levels of task performance, and argue that it is unlikely to be an immediate behavioral
by doing so prompt negative supervisory reactions that reaction to subordinates’ perceptions of abusive
result in subordinates’ perceptions of abusive supervision. supervision.
Thus, we predict that the relationships between subordi- Previous research has found a positive association
nates’ neuroticism and conscientiousness and perceptions between perceptions of abusive supervision and subordi-
of abusive supervision will be mediated by subordinates’ nates’ interpersonal deviance (e.g., Lian et al. 2012;
task performance. Mitchell and Ambrose 2007; Thau et al. 2009). However,
little is known about boundary conditions of this relation-
Hypothesis 3 Subordinates’ task performance will
ship with respect to subordinate-level factors (Tepper
mediate the relationship between their neuroticism and
2007). As previously mentioned, Lian et al. (2012) reported
perceptions of abusive supervision.
that subordinates’ power distance orientation served as a
Hypothesis 4 Subordinates’ task performance will boundary condition in the relationship between perceptions
mediate the relationship between their conscientiousness of abusive supervision and interpersonal deviance. In
and perceptions of abusive supervision. addition, Mitchell and Ambrose (2007) found a moderating
effect of subordinates’ negative reciprocity beliefs (indi-
Abusive Supervision and Interpersonal Deviance viduals’ advisability of retribution for unfavorable treat-
ment; Eisenberger et al. 2004) on the relationship between
Workplace deviance refers to ‘‘voluntary behavior that perceptions of active-aggressive abusive supervision (e.g.,
violates significant organizational norms and in so doing ridiculing someone; Tepper 2000) and supervisor-directed

123
G. Wang et al.

deviance. To build on these previous findings, we believe just social interactions. The dominant framework in per-
that it is important to examine the moderating effects of sonality psychology is the FFM (Costa and McCrae 1992;
subordinate characteristics other than power distance ori- Digman 1990). A general consensus among social and
entation and negative reciprocity beliefs. personality psychologists is that dominance and nurturance
Given the dominant role of FFM personality traits in correspond to extraversion and agreeableness (e.g., Barry
individual differences research (e.g., Barrick and Mount and Friedman 1998; Caldwell and Burger 1997; McCrae
1991), we find it surprising that the potential moderating and Costa 1989). Ample evidence shows that agreeableness
effects of subordinates’ FFM personality traits on the rela- and extraversion are particularly relevant to individuals’
tionship between perceived abusive supervision and inter- social motives and behaviors (e.g., Carlo et al. 2005;
personal deviance have not been adequately examined. Thus, Graziano and Eisenberg 1997; Koole et al. 2001; Snyder
one of the major contributions of this investigation is to 1983). McCrae and Costa (1989, p. 586) argued that
advance our understanding of the effects of FFM personality extraversion and agreeableness ‘‘determine directly the
traits on perceptions of and reactions to abusive supervision. amount of social stimulation preferred and the prevailing
Trait activation theory (Tett and Guterman 2000) sug- quality of social interaction,’’ whereas neuroticism, open-
gests that followers’ personality traits may affect their ness, and conscientiousness ‘‘are not intrinsically inter-
responses to perceived abusive supervision. As we describe personal’’ in that ‘‘one can feel unhappy, respond to art, or
below, we expect that followers who score low on both accomplish a task regardless of the presence or absence of
agreeableness and extraversion will be more likely to other people.’’ Therefore, given that trait activation theory
engage in interpersonal deviance associated with perceived (Tett and Guterman 2000) suggests that subordinates who
abusive supervision than followers who are high in one or differ in interpersonal traits may react differently to per-
both of these traits. ceived abusive supervision, we argue that agreeableness
and extraversion are the two FFM personality traits of
Trait Activation Theory and the Activation of particular relevance to the understanding of negative social
Disagreeableness and Introversion interactions and their effects.
Supervisory behaviors such as yelling, screaming, and
Integrating trait and situationist perspectives (Kenrick and publicly ridiculing subordinates likely break social interaction
Funder 1988), Tett and Guterman (2000) proposed trait norms in many different work environments, signify negative
activation theory, which posits that in order to activate interpersonal relationships, discourage cooperation, and fos-
behavioral expression of a trait, situations need to provide ter an unfriendly and cold group climate (Mitchell and
trait-relevant cues. Trait-relevant cues can come from three Ambrose 2007). This negative environment is likely to acti-
sources or levels: task, social, and organizational (Tett and vate the expression of disagreeableness and introversion.
Burnett 2003). For example, goal-focused leadership could Agreeableness reflects one’s interpersonal orientation. Dis-
supply social cues that trigger highly conscientious sub- agreeable people are characterized as antagonistic, vengeful,
ordinates to behave conscientiously (Colbert and Witt hostile, self-centered, and ruthless (Costa and McCrae 1992).
2009). In the current study, given that leadership is a social Meta-analytic results have shown that among the FFM,
influence process (Yukl 2010), we focus on perceptions of agreeableness has the strongest negative relationship with
abusive supervision in employees’ work environment as workplace deviance (Berry et al. 2007).
one social source of trait-relevant cues that may activate Further, according to social learning theory (Bandura
the expression of their personality traits that distinctly 1973), which posits that employees gain an understanding
differentiate them in social (i.e., interpersonal) interactions: of what constitutes acceptable behavior by observing oth-
disagreeableness and introversion. ers in the workplace, supervisors who engage in negative
Evidence that agreeableness and extraversion represent behaviors may serve as ‘‘role models’’ who signal that
interpersonal traits that clearly distinguish how individuals positive social interactions are not valued and that hostility
behave in social interactions stem from two independent and ruthlessness are part of normal work life. Numerous
lines of research (Koole et al. 2001; McCrae and Costa studies examining abusive supervision have used social
1989). In social psychology, researchers specifically focus learning theory to explain the process by which employees
on personality in social interaction. The dominant general learn how and when to engage in particular behaviors (e.g.,
structure that organizes interpersonal traits in social psy- Brown et al. 2005; Kiewitz et al. 2012; Mawritz et al. 2012;
chology is the interpersonal circumflex, which is defined by Mitchell and Ambrose 2012). Accordingly, employees who
dominance and nurturance (e.g., Freedman et al. 1951; experience supervisory abuse perceive that their supervi-
Wiggins 1979, 1980). sors engage in interpersonal mistreatment, and therefore
In contrast, researchers in personality psychology gen- learn that interpersonal mistreatment is acceptable in the
erally summarize personality traits across situations, not workplace.

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

In fact, research on workplace aggression has shown that likely to focus on attaining revenge for the mistreatment
having abusive models helps relieve victims’ inhibitions to they receive and to ruminate over the harm for a longer
act similarly (Berkowitz 1993; O’Leary-Kelly et al. 1996). time than their disagreeable but extraverted counterparts.
Thus, perceptions of abusive supervision may provide cues In contrast, disagreeable employees who are also extra-
that trigger disagreeableness. When perceiving supervisory verted may vent their anger by talking to others (e.g.,
abuse, disagreeable subordinates are likely to be cued to coworkers, family members); consequently, they may
fully express their antagonism, vengefulness, and hostility experience decreased stress levels and divert their attention
against the harm-doers or innocent scapegoats with fewer from the mistreatment they received (Bies and Tripp 1996).
concerns about further escalating the incivility spiral Thus, we expect that disagreeable extraverts will be less
(Anderson and Pearson 1999; Tepper et al. 2001). Thus, we motivated to engage in retaliating behaviors than dis-
expect that perceptions of abusive supervision will have a agreeable introverts.
stronger relationship with interpersonal deviance for dis- In sum, the forgoing suggests a three-way interaction
agreeable subordinates than with agreeable subordinates. between perceptions of abusive supervision, disagreeable-
Extraversion reflects the extent to which people are ness, and introversion. Specifically, we predict that intro-
sociable, talkative, energetic, and active. Individuals who version will strengthen the moderating effect of
are low in extraversion (i.e., introverts) prefer to be alone disagreeableness on the relationship between perceptions
and are characterized as reserved, quiet, and independent of abusive supervision and interpersonal deviance, such
(Costa and McCrae 1992). Perceptions of abusive super- that when subordinates high in disagreeableness and
vision are likely to activate introversion because they introversion are harmed, they are most likely to retaliate
create situations in which victims are likely to be cued to with antagonism and hostility. Formally, we advance the
avoid aggressors. One way to do this is to try not to be following hypothesis:
noticed by staying quiet and avoiding open communication
Hypothesis 5 Subordinates’ agreeableness and extraver-
with others. When perceiving supervisory abuse, introverts
sion will jointly moderate the relationship between per-
may have difficulty venting their anger and desire for
ceptions of abusive supervision and subordinates’
revenge because they do not want to share their feelings
interpersonal deviance, such that when subordinates are
(e.g., anger and resentment) with others. Thus, introverts
low in both agreeableness and extraversion, perceptions of
tend to experience negative emotions for an extended
abusive supervision and subordinates’ interpersonal devi-
period of time. In addition, introverts who perceive
ance will have the strongest relationship.
supervisory abuse are likely to have a narrow thought-
action repertoire (Fredrickson 1998). Consequently, intro-
verts may ruminate over the harm they experience for an
extended period of time and develop a deep desire to get Method
even. This suggests that introverts may not immediately
react to perceptions of abusive supervision, but might react Sample and Procedure
to them if perceived supervisory abuse accumulates over
time or other personality traits are activated that interact Data for this study were collected in two waves approxi-
with introversion to prompt behavioral reactions. Thus, mately 3 weeks apart. At Time 1, seven hundred and nine
introversion alone may not manifest in an expression of working adults were recruited as part of a class exercise to
interpersonal deviance, but its effects may interact with the teach students in an introductory management course how
effects of disagreeableness to cue interpersonal deviance. research is conducted, analyzed, and reported. Students
Taken together, the above arguments suggest that dis- received bonus points in their course for each person they
agreeableness and introversion may jointly moderate the recruited, up to a maximum of 10. At Time 1, participants
relationship between perceptions of abusive supervision were asked to complete measures of their FFM personality
and subordinates’ interpersonal deviance. Specifically, traits and demographics. They also provided responses to
perceptions of supervisory abuse are likely to activate the least preferred coworker (LPC) scale, which was used
disagreeable tendencies in subordinates. Because interper- as a marker variable in the common method variance
sonal workplace deviance can be thought of as a behavioral (CMV) analysis. At Time 2, participants completed mea-
manifestation of disagreeableness, we expect disagreeable sures of perceptions of abusive supervision, task perfor-
subordinates to engage in more interpersonal deviance than mance, and interpersonal deviance. Three hundred and
their agreeable counterparts. If disagreeable subordinates seventy-six participants from various organizations (e.g.,
are also introverted, they may also be cued to express their health care institutions, federal agencies, and law firms)
introversion and engage in interpersonal deviance to an completed the online surveys in both waves, for a response
even greater extent than extraverts because they are more rate of 53 %.

123
G. Wang et al.

Of the 376 participants, 40.4 % were female. Partici- Marker Variable


pants’ average age was 37.66 years (SD = 13.58), and the
average organizational tenure was 6.68 years (SD = 8.11). The 18-item LPC measure (Fiedler 1967) was used as a
At the time of the study, all participants were working as marker variable to test for the presence and biasing effect
non-supervisory front-line employees. In terms of ethnic- of CMV. The participants were asked to think about one
ity, 85.3 % were Caucasian, 8 % Asian, 2.1 % African coworker with whom they had the most difficulty getting a
American, 2.1 % Hispanic, and 2.4 % other. About half of job done and rate that person on an 8-point scale ranging
participants (48.8 %) had bachelors’ degrees or above, from 1 = ‘‘unpleasant’’ or ‘‘unfriendly’’ to 8 = ‘‘pleasant’’
42.1 % received associate’s degree or some college edu- or ‘‘friendly.’’
cation, and 9.1 % had completed secondary school.
Control Variables1
Measures
When testing Hypotheses 1–4, we controlled for subordi-
nates’ agreeableness, extraversion, and openness given that
Abusive Supervision
prior research has shown that they tend to be correlated
with the substantive variables, which may confound study
We used Tepper’s (2000) 15-item abusive supervision
results (e.g., Berry et al. 2007; Henle and Gross 2014;
scale. Respondents were asked to indicate the frequency
Mount et al. 2005; Oh et al. 2011; Tepper et al. 2001; Wu
with which they perceived their immediate supervisor
and Hu 2013). Similarly, when testing Hypothesis 5, we
exhibited abusive behaviors. Sample items are ‘‘expresses
controlled for subordinates’ neuroticism, conscientious-
anger at me when he/she is mad for another reason’’ and
ness, and openness. The results with and without the con-
‘‘lies to me.’’ We used a 5-point scale ranging from
trols remained essentially unchanged.2 Following Becker’s
1 = ‘‘never’’ to 5 = ‘‘very often.’’
(2005) recommendations, we reported results without any
controls.
Personality

Participants’ FFM personality traits were assessed with a Results


20-item short form of the International Personality Item
Pool (IPIP)—FFM measure (Goldberg 1999) that was Table 1 shows means, standard deviations, reliability esti-
developed by Donnellan et al. (2006). Sample items are mates, and correlations among study and marker variables.
‘‘get upset easily’’ (neuroticism), ‘‘sympathize with others’ Reliability estimates (i.e., a) fell within an acceptable range
feelings’’ (agreeableness), ‘‘talk to a lot of different people for most study variables, but the estimates for conscien-
at parties’’ (extraversion), and ‘‘like order’’ (conscien- tiousness (a = .62), neuroticism (a = .69), and openness
tiousness). A 5-point scale ranging from 1 = ‘‘strongly to experience (a = .69) fell just below Nunnally’s (1978)
disagree’’ to 5 = ‘‘strongly agree’’ was used. recommended standard for acceptable internal consistency
estimates of a C .70. The pattern of zero-order correlations
Task Performance provided some preliminary support for the study hypothe-
ses: subordinates’ neuroticism was negatively related to
Participants assessed their task performance using Wil- self-reported task performance and positively related to
liams and Anderson’s (1991) 7-item scale. Sample items perceptions of abusive supervision, and subordinates’
are ‘‘fulfill responsibilities in job description’’ and ‘‘ade- conscientiousness was positively related to self-reported
quately complete assigned duties.’’ Respondents used a 5-
1
point scale ranging from 1 = ‘‘strongly disagree’’ to Thanks to an anonymous reviewer’s thoughtful suggestion, we
retested our hypotheses using age, gender, and organizational tenure
5 = ‘‘strongly agree.’’
as demographic control variables. The inclusion of these additional
control variables did not substantively affect the results of hypothesis
testing, but significantly reduced the sample size, so they were
Interpersonal Deviance
excluded from the final results.
2
It is noted that the slope difference test that compares the
Bennett and Robinson’s (2000) 7-item interpersonal devi- relationship (i.e., simple slope) between perceived abusive supervi-
ance scale was used. Sample items include ‘‘make fun of sion and interpersonal deviance among subordinates low in agree-
someone at work’’ and ‘‘play a mean prank on someone at ableness and extraversion with the slope of subordinates high in both
traits was not significant (p \ .07, two-tailed test) when the control
work.’’ Respondents used a 5-point scale ranging from variables were included, and was significant (p \ .05, two-tailed test)
1 = ‘‘never’’ to 5 = ‘‘very often.’’ when the control variables were not included.

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and variable intercorrelations


Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1 Conscientiousness 3.59 .68 (.62)


2 Neuroticism 2.71 .74 -.15** (.69)
3 Extraversion 3.23 .79 -.01 -.08 (.75)
4 Agreeableness 3.89 .62 .14** -.04 .27*** (.73)
5 Openness 3.51 .70 -.07 -.10* .19*** .02 (.69)
6 Task performance 4.15 .63 .27*** -.15** .05 .12* -.01 (.89)
7 Abusive supervision 1.39 .62 -.11* .14** .05 -.07 -.01 -.27*** (.95)
8 Interpersonal deviance 1.92 1.00 -.13* .03 .06 -.18*** .03 -.20*** .38*** (.84)
9 Least preferred 3.57 1.51 .07 -.03 .03 -.07 .03 -.05 .00 .07 (.96)
coworker
n = 376. The reliability (coefficient alpha) estimates are presented along the diagonal
* p \ .05, ** p \ .01, *** p \ .001. Statistical tests are based on two-tailed tests

task performance and negatively related to perceptions of latent marker variable, demonstrated good fit, suggesting
abusive supervision. Also, subordinates’ self-reports of that the expected factor structure captured the relationships
task performance were negatively related to perceptions of among observed variables fairly well. The Baseline model
abusive supervision, which were positively related to sub- with the marker variable being orthogonal to the substan-
ordinates’ interpersonal deviance. Before testing the study tive study variables also demonstrated good fit, providing
hypotheses, we assessed the potential impact of CMV on evidence that the marker variable was not related to the
study variables and relationships among them. study variables, as expected. The v2 difference test further
indicated that constraining the correlations between the
Assessing the Impact of CMV on Study Variables marker variable and study variables to zero did not worsen
the model fit (CFA vs. Baseline: Dv2 = 7.61, df = 12,
Because all of the variables were collected from the same p [ .10).
source (albeit at two different points in time), we first Next, the Method-C model with equal loadings of sub-
conducted a test for CMV using a marker variable tech- stantive variable indicators on the marker variable also
nique (Williams et al. 2010) with LPC as a marker variable. demonstrated good fit, suggesting the assumption that the
Given the focus and the scope of Fiedler’s (1967) contin- substantive indicators as a set were not significantly related
gency model of leadership, which focused on the LPC, to the marker variable was supported. The Model-U, in
none of the study variables used in the current study could which the assumption of equality of substantive item
be considered central to Fiedler’s approach. Thus, based on loadings on the marker variable was relaxed, did not
the previous theoretical and empirical work on Fiedler’s demonstrate a decrease in fit (Method-C vs. Method-U:
LPC approach (e.g., Ayman et al. 1995; Fiedler 1972; Roch Dv2 = 63.89, df = 44, p [ .05), suggesting that the more
et al. 2005), we had no reason to expect that LPC would parsimonious Model-C could be retained for further ana-
strongly relate to any of the variables used in the current lysis as the best model representing the effects of method
study, and therefore selected LPC as a marker variable. As variance. The results of testing Method-C and Method-U
shown in Table 1, LPC displayed non-significant correla- models showed that substantive items were not contami-
tions with the study variables. nated by CMV. In line with this conclusion, the Method-R
The CMV analysis was conducted using LISREL ver- model with interfactor correlations fixed to be equal to the
sion 8.80 (Jöreskog and Sörbom 2006). The test for the corresponding correlations from the Baseline model dem-
presence and influence of CMV was conducted in the three onstrated that CMV had no meaningful biasing effect on
phases described by Williams et al. (2010). At Phase 1, the relationships among the study variables (Method-C vs.
multiple factor models were compared to each other with Method-R: Dv2 = 0.12, df = 21, p [ .10).
the purpose of determining whether method effects were At Phase 2, we decomposed the observed reliabilities of
present and equal across items measuring study variables. study variables into substantive and method components.
The results of model comparison are presented in Table 2. The method components comprised between 0.1 and 0.2 %
As seen in the table, the Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the total variable reliabilities shown in Table 1. This
(CFA) model with all items loading on their corresponding suggests that the influence of CMV on variable reliabilities
factors, including marker variable indicators loading on the was negligible. Finally, at Phase 3, we conducted sensitivity

123
G. Wang et al.

Table 2 Test for the common Model v2 df p value RMSEA CFI TLI SRMR
method variance: model
comparisons CFA 2,471.74 1,052 \.001 0.063 0.94 0.94 0.057
Baseline 2,479.35 1,064 \.001 0.063 0.94 0.94 0.057
Method-C 2,476.11 1,063 \.001 0.063 0.94 0.94 0.058
Method-U 2,412.22 1,019 \.001 0.063 0.94 0.93 0.054
Method-R 2,476.23 1,084 \.001 0.062 0.94 0.94 0.058
n = 376

analysis to determine whether the impact of CMV observed Table 3 Summary of regression results of testing Hypotheses 1–4
in the current analysis could be due to sampling error. To Variable Model 1: Model 2: Task Model 3:
conduct the analysis, we first fixed the loadings of the sub- Abusive performance Abusive
stantive indicators on the marker variable to be equal to the supervision supervision
value at the upper end of the 95 % confidence intervals b SE b SE b SE
(Model-S(.05)), then fixed those loadings to be equal to the
value at the upper end of their 99 % confidence intervals Conscientiousness -.09 .05 .26*** .05 -.02 .05
(Model-S(.01)). The correlations among the factors in both Neuroticism .13* .04 -.12* .04 .10* .04
Model-S(.05) and Model-S(.01) remained unchanged and Task performance -.25*** .05
were virtually identical (with the maximal difference of .01) R2 .03** .09*** .09***
to correlations obtained in the Baseline model and Method- n = 376
C model. This suggests that the obtained estimates of the * p \ .05, ** p \ .01, *** p \ .001. Statistical tests are based on
impact of method variance do not appear to be a function of two-tailed tests
sampling error. In sum, the test for method effects using the
marker variable approach indicated that study variable items abusive supervision was positive and significant, which
were neither contaminated by CMV, nor did it bias rela- supported Hypothesis 1. Further, the relationship between
tionships among study variables or affect reliability conscientiousness and abusive supervision was negative
estimates. but not significant, failing to support Hypothesis 2.
In Model 2 of the mediation analysis, we examined the
Tests of Study Hypotheses3 relationships between neuroticism and conscientiousness
(predictors) and self-reported task performance (mediator).
Table 3 presents the results of testing Hypotheses 1–4. We Both relationships were significant and in the predicted
followed the steps provided in the recent literature for direction. Specifically, neuroticism was negatively related
testing mediation (LeBreton et al. 2009), which pertained to self-reported task performance and conscientiousness
to the relationships among neuroticism, conscientiousness, was positively related to self-reported task performance
self-reported task performance, and perceptions of abusive (see Table 3, Model 2). In Model 3 of the mediation ana-
supervision. In Model 1 of the mediation analysis (Baron lysis, we tested the relationships between self-reported task
and Kenny 1986), we examined the effects of neuroticism performance (mediator) and perceptions of abusive super-
and conscientiousness (predictors) on perceptions of abu- vision (outcome) in the presence of neuroticism and con-
sive supervision (outcome). As seen in Table 3 (Model 1), scientiousness (predictors). As expected (see Table 3,
the relationship between neuroticism and perceptions of Model 3), the relationship between task performance and
3
Thanks to an anonymous reviewer’s thoughtful suggestion, we
retested our hypotheses using the 5-item short measure of abusive Footnote 3 continued
supervision, which was called active-aggressive abusive supervision high extraversion was not significant (Db4–1 = .05, p = .645, two-
(Mitchell and Ambrose 2007). The results showed that neuroticism tailed test). The change suggests that when perceiving active-
had a non-significant total effect (p = .23, two-tailed test) on aggressive abusive supervision, highly agreeable and extraverted
perceptions of active-aggressive abusive supervision, and conscien- employees tended to engage in more interpersonal deviant behaviors
tiousness had a non-significant total effect (p = .06, two-tailed test). than when perceiving a broad range of abusive supervisory behavior.
The indirect effects of neuroticism and conscientiousness through task This may be because perceptions of active-aggressive abusive
performance were both significant. Moreover, there was still a supervision forms a stronger situation than perceptions of a broad
significant three-way interaction (p = .038, two-tailed test) among range of abusive supervision, such that highly agreeable and extra-
extraversion, agreeableness, and perceptions of active-aggressive verted employees perform more interpersonal deviance under the
abusive supervision. When plotted, the three-way interaction showed stronger situation (Bowers 1973). In summary, our study shows that
a similar pattern of relationships among the simple slopes as shown in findings based on the 5-item measure of active-aggressive abusive
Fig. 2. The only change was that the simple slope difference between supervision are similar to, but slightly different from, those based on
low agreeableness and low extraversion and high agreeableness and the broad 15-item measure of abusive supervision.

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

perceptions of abusive supervision was negative and Table 4 Summary of regression results of testing Hypothesis 5
significant. Variable Interpersonal
The indirect effects of neuroticism and conscientious- deviance
ness on perceptions of abusive supervision via self-reported
b SE
task performance were tested for significance using two
methods—the asymmetric confidence limits approach Step 1 (main effects)
(MacKinnon et al. 2004, 2007) and the bootstrap method Extraversion .08 .05
(1,000 samples; Shrout and Bolger 2002)—designed to Agreeableness -.18*** .05
account for the non-normality of the distribution of the Abusive supervision .32*** .05
indirect effects. The indirect effect of neuroticism on per- DR2 .17***
ceptions of abusive supervision via self-reported task per- Step 2 (two-way interactions)
formance was positive and significant: .02, asymmetric Extraversion 9 abusive supervision -.09 .05
95 % CI: (.003; .049), bootstrap 95 % CI (.004; .040). The Agreeableness 9 abusive supervision -.03 .05
indirect effect of conscientiousness on perceptions of Extraversion 9 agreeableness .05 .04
abusive supervision via self-reported task performance was DR2 .02
negative and significant: -.06, asymmetric 95 % CI Step 3 (three-way interactions)
(-.094; -.029); bootstrap 95 % CI (-.071; -.018). Taken Extraversion 9 agreeableness 9 abusive .13* .05
together, the results of the mediation analysis show that supervision
self-reported task performance partially rather than fully DR2 .01*
mediated the effect of neuroticism on perceptions of abu- 2
R for the total equation .20***
sive supervision because the effect of neuroticism on per-
n = 376
ceptions of abusive supervision in the presence of self-
* p \ .05, ** p \ .01, *** p \ .001. Statistical tests are based on
reported task performance (see Table 3, Model 3) was two-tailed tests
smaller but still significant (Baron and Kenny 1986). Thus,
Hypothesis 3 was partially supported.
However, the results of the mediation analysis failed to difference test to examine differences between pairs of
support Hypothesis 4 because the total effect of conscien- slopes (Dawson and Richter 2006). This test revealed that
tiousness on perceptions of abusive supervision was not the relationship between perceived abusive supervision and
significant (see Table 3, Model 1). According to Mathieu interpersonal deviance was stronger when subordinates
and Taylor (2006), there should be a significant total effect were low in both traits than when subordinates were only
of an independent variable on a dependent variable for a low in one trait (Db4–2 = 0.29, p \ .05; Db4–3 = 0.42,
mediating effect to exist. When the total effect is not sig- p \ .01) or high in both traits (Db4–1 = 0.24, p \ .05).
nificant, the independent variable could have an indirect Slopes in the remaining pairs did not significantly differ
effect on the dependent variable through a third variable. from each other (Db1–2 = 0.11, p [ .10; Db1–3 = 0.34,
Following Mathieu and Taylor’s distinction between p [ .10; Db2–3 = 0.23, p [ .10).
mediation and indirect effect, we concluded that consci- Thus, Hypothesis 5 received support: perceived abusive
entiousness had a significant indirect effect on perceptions supervision interacted with extraversion and agreeableness
of abusive supervision through self-reported task to predict interpersonal deviance, such that when perceiv-
performance. ing supervisory abuse, subordinates low in both agree-
Table 4 summarizes the hierarchical regression results ableness and extraversion were more likely to engage in
we used to test Hypothesis 5. As predicted, the three-way interpersonal deviance than subordinates high in agree-
interaction among agreeableness, extraversion, and per- ableness and low in extraversion, low in agreeableness and
ceived abusive supervision in predicting interpersonal high in extraversion, or high in both traits.
deviance was significant (b = .13, p \ .05). To graphically
examine the three-way interaction, we constructed an
interaction plot (see Fig. 2) and conducted the simple Discussion
slopes test (Aiken and West 1991). After establishing that
three of the four simple slopes were significantly different Drawing upon extant theoretical and empirical research, we
from zero [b1(high agreeableness, high extraver- examined subordinates’ personality and self-reports of task
sion) = 0.32, p \ .01; b2 (high agreeableness, low extra- performance as antecedents of perceptions of abusive
version) = 0.27, p \ .01; b3(low agreeableness, high supervision, as well as the moderating effects of subordi-
extraversion) = 0.14, p [ .10; b4(low agreeableness, low nates’ personality traits in the relationship between subor-
extraversion) = 0.56, p \ .001], we conducted a slope dinates’ perceived abusive supervision and interpersonal

123
G. Wang et al.

Fig. 2 Three-way interactive


effects among perceived
abusive supervision,
agreeableness, and extraversion
on subordinates’ interpersonal
deviance

deviance. We found that subordinates’ self-reports of task The robust relationship we found between neuroticism
performance partially mediated the relationship between and perceptions of abusive supervision corresponds to
neuroticism and perceptions of abusive supervision, and findings in other research areas that victims with certain
that conscientiousness had an indirect effect on perceptions personality traits are more likely to be abused or perceive
of abusive supervision through their self-reported task that they are abused than others (Elias 1986). Specifically,
performance. In addition, we found that subordinates’ we were able to replicate Henle and Gross’ (2014) finding
extraversion and agreeableness jointly moderated the that emotional stability had a significant negative total
effects of perceived abusive supervision on their interper- effect on perceptions of abusive supervision. Extending
sonal deviant behavior. Specifically, when perceiving Henle and Gross, our findings demonstrate that in addition
supervisory abuse, subordinates who scored low on both to negative emotions, self-reports of task performance
agreeableness and extraversion were more likely to engage could also transmit the indirect effects of emotional sta-
in interpersonal deviance than other subordinates who bility and conscientiousness.
scored high on either or both of these two personality traits. Although not hypothesized, subordinates’ agreeableness
This study’s findings reveal subordinates’ roles in the in our study did not have a significant total or indirect
formation of and reactions to abusive supervision, and effect (via perceptions of task performance) on perceptions
contribute to the expanding literature on the dark side of of abusive supervision. This finding is consistent with
organizational behavior. First, previous studies on ante- Henle and Gross’ (2014) findings. Unlike Henle and Gross,
cedents of abusive supervision have mainly focused on we found a non-significant total effect of subordinates’
supervisor perceptions of injustice (e.g., Tepper et al. 2006) conscientiousness on perceptions of abusive supervision,
and dissimilarity (Tepper et al. 2011). Our findings suggest which may stem from the relatively low reliability estimate
that alternative mechanisms through which perceptions of of conscientiousness in the current study. A future meta-
abusive supervision are formed may exist. Because abusive analysis (Hunter and Schmidt 2004) likely will reveal a
supervision represents a specific form of perceived inter- population relationship between subordinates’ conscien-
personal mistreatment within supervisor–subordinate tiousness and perceptions of abusive supervision.
interactions, and because research on workplace harass- Further, this study replicates Tepper et al.’s (2011)
ment posits that victims’ personality and behavior may finding that subordinates’ self-reports of task performance
contribute to aggression (Matthiesen and Einarsen 2001), it can be an antecedent of perceptions of abusive supervision.
is reasonable to expect that subordinates may also give rise Adding to Tepper et al., this study shows that subordinates’
to negative supervisory actions through their negative personality traits (i.e., neuroticism and conscientiousness)
emotional expressions, attitudes, and less conscientious could be viable antecedents of perceptions of abusive
behaviors (Henle and Gross 2014), as well as through supervision in addition to supervisors’ perceived dissimi-
inadequate fulfillment of job responsibilities. In line with larity and relationship conflict with subordinates. Overall,
this expectation, we found that neurotic and less consci- these findings reveal the important role played by subor-
entious subordinates are more likely to experience abusive dinates in the formation of perceived abusive supervision.
supervision partly due to their poor performance of job Our findings suggest that acknowledging the role of fol-
duties. lower characteristics when examining antecedents of

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

abusive supervision may further our understanding of this personality related to social interactions (i.e., disagree-
important phenomenon. ableness and introversion). Thus, our findings provide
Second, complementing prior studies (e.g., Henle and initial support for the external validity of trait activation
Gross 2014; Tepper et al. 2011), our findings show that theory in abusive supervision research.
subordinates’ agreeableness and extraversion jointly mod-
erated the relationship between perceptions of abusive Implications for Practice
supervision and interpersonal deviance. The significant
moderation effect provides a more fine-grained picture of The findings of this study suggest several important
the negative consequences of perceived abusive supervi- implications for practice. First, given the negative conse-
sion than previously available and contributes to the quences of perceived abusive supervision on subordinates’
growing literature on abusive supervision. Tepper (2007) psychological well-being and the spillover effect on group
called for more research on the boundary conditions of the climate and cooperation (Mitchell and Ambrose 2007), we
effects of abusive supervision on workplace outcomes. To believe that managing subordinates’ performance problems
our knowledge, this study represents the first attempt to in an abusive way should be avoided because it is a practice
investigate the simultaneous effects of subordinates’ that harms the organization and its members. We recom-
agreeableness and extraversion as boundary conditions of mend that organizations hire employees who possess the
the relationship between subordinates’ perceived abusive necessary knowledge, skills, and abilities to perform their
supervision and interpersonal deviance. tasks, and that organizations provide coaching, training,
Our study shows that subordinates can differ in their and feedback so that employees can continuously improve
reactions to perceived abusive supervision based on their their levels of performance.
personality characteristics. For example, disagreeable and Second, from an ethical perspective, we suggest that
introverted subordinates tend to react to perceived abusive organizations provide training or consultation for supervi-
supervision with higher levels of interpersonal deviance sors regarding how to treat subordinates with particular
than agreeable and/or extroverted subordinates. Overall, personality traits, especially those high in neuroticism or
our study illustrates the significance of examining bound- low in conscientiousness. For example, organizations can
ary conditions of negative effects of abusive supervision. train supervisors to engage in constructive conflict man-
Consistent with Tepper et al. (2001), our study shows that agement that addresses concerns and provides feedback in
subordinates’ personality traits could jointly moderate their a timely and appropriate manner that considers subordi-
responses to perceived abusive supervision. nates’ personality traits (Sutton 2007). Further, organiza-
Adding to Burton and Hoobler (2011), Nandkeolyar tions can adopt and promote strict policies against
et al. (2014), Mitchell and Ambrose (2007), and Tepper interpersonal mistreatment in the workplace (Sutton 2007),
et al. (2001), our study theoretically contributes to the and establish HR hotlines, as well as other complaint
abusive supervision literature by introducing trait activa- mechanisms (Krasikova et al. 2013) that enable employees
tion theory as a means to identify agreeableness and to report interpersonal mistreatment. Not only would these
extraversion as two joint moderators of the relationship policies make it clear to supervisors that they need to be
between perceived abusive supervision and interpersonal sensitive toward whether or not subordinates perceive
deviance. One theoretical implication offered by our study supervisory treatment, but they also would provide subor-
and prior research is that different personality traits tend to dinates with the opportunity to identify when they perceive
be involved in moderating different consequences of per- supervisory mistreatment.
ceived abusive supervision. Thus, future researchers are Third, organizations can assess employees’ personality
prompted to theoretically derive boundary conditions of so that they can identify which applicants are most likely to
different consequences of perceived abusive supervision. perceive abusive supervision and subsequently take
Third, the significant interaction among subordinates’ appropriate measures to prevent perceptions of abusive
agreeableness, extraversion, and perceived abusive super- supervision if they decide to hire these applicants. For
vision is in line with the predictions of trait activation example, employers could train employees in conflict
theory (Tett and Guterman 2000). Complementing prior management and coping skills, which might help reduce
research suggesting that positive leadership (e.g., goal- levels of perceived supervisory abuse, as well as the
focused leadership; Colbert and Witt 2009) could be a prevalence of interpersonal deviance.
social cue that triggers employees to express their per-
sonality traits (e.g., conscientiousness), our study demon- Limitations and Implications for Future Research
strates that perceived negative supervisory behaviors (e.g.,
perceptions of abusive supervision) could also serve as a Admittedly, this study has several limitations. First, all of
social cue that triggers the expression of employees’ the constructs were measured from the same source.

123
G. Wang et al.

However, the CMV analysis results indicated that the abusive supervision is consistent with prior research, future
relationships among the variables likely were not attribut- research could examine the extent to which perceptions of
able to common source bias (Spector 1987). In addition, abusive supervision are associated with actual abusive
our two-wave research design further helps mitigate supervisory behavior, as well as how results differ
against the problem of common method bias (Podsakoff depending on how abusive supervision is measured. Sim-
et al. 2003). Nonetheless, we suggest future research uses ilarly, our measure of performance reflected subordinates’
multi-source data collection designs to replicate our find- perceptions of their own performance. Future research
ings. For example, task performance and/or interpersonal could explore whether or not subordinates’ and supervi-
deviance could be measured from supervisors’ perspec- sors’ ratings of performance are differentially associated
tives. Employees with certain combinations of personality with perceptions of and reactions to abusive supervision.
traits may be more likely to perceive abusive supervision
than others, regardless of actual levels of supervisory
abuse. Due to the self-report nature of the data examined in Conclusion
this study, it is difficult to determine the extent to which
supervisors actually engaged in abusive actions. One major In summary, our results show that subordinates’ personal-
advantage of a multi-source approach is that it would be ity traits play an important role in perceptions of and
possible to determine the degree to which the reported reactions to abusive supervision. Specifically, subordinates
abusive supervision was a real consequence of neuroticism high in neuroticism or low in conscientiousness are more
and low conscientiousness or simply a perceptual bias. likely to experience perceptions of abusive supervision
Second, the reliability estimates (i.e., a) for conscien- through their deleterious job performance than subordi-
tiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience fell nates low in neuroticism or high in conscientiousness. In
below Nunnally’s (1978) recommended standard of .70. addition, subordinates low in both agreeableness and
This is not surprising, considering that each construct was extraversion tend to respond to perceived supervisory
measured using 4 items validated in Donnellan et al.’s abuse with more interpersonal deviance than subordinates
(2006) mini-IPIP measure. Highly shortened measures of who score high on either or both of the two personality
personality suffer from a number of well-known problems, traits. We hope our study spurs additional research exam-
including a lack of breadth in item content, which can ining the role of employees’ personalities in the formations
undermine reliability and reduce validity (see Credé et al. of and reactions to abusive supervision.
2012). We recommend that studies that attempt to con-
structively replicate this study’s findings use more com- Acknowledgments The authors are grateful to Dina Krasikova and
Liam Patrick Maher for their help.
prehensive measures of personality than those included in
the mini-IPIP.
Third, because data on perceptions of abusive supervi-
References
sion, task performance, and interpersonal deviance were
collected at the same point in time, the causal relationships Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
between these variables are tentative. Longitudinal or interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
experimental designs are needed to infer causality among Anderson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling
the study variables. For example, it is likely that subordi- effect of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management
Review, 24, 452–471.
nates’ interpersonal deviance leads to supervisors’ attempts Aquino, K. (2000). Structural and individual determinants of work-
to correct deviant employees’ misbehavior by engaging in place victimization: The effects of hierarchical status and
negative behaviors that could be construed as abusive. conflict management style. Journal of Management, 26, 171–
However, considering the power and status asymmetry 193.
Aquino, K., & Bradfield, M. (2000). Perceived victimization in the
between supervisors and subordinates (Tepper et al. 2008), workplace: The role of situational factors and victim character-
we suggest it is not likely that subordinates direct deviant istics. Organization Science, 11, 525–537.
behaviors at supervisors in the first place. Aquino, K., Grover, S. L., Bradfield, M., & Allen, D. G. (1999). The
Finally, we used Tepper’s (2000) widely used measure effects of negative affectivity, hierarchical status, and self-
determination on workplace victimization. Academy of Manage-
of abusive supervision, which measures subordinates’ ment Journal, 42, 260–272.
perceptions of supervisors’ abusive behavior, rather than Aquino, K., & Lamertz, K. (2004). A relational model of workplace
actual supervisory behavior. Thus, our findings reflect the victimization: Social roles and patterns of victimization in
extent to which subordinates perceive they are abused by dyadic relationships. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1023–
1034.
their supervisors, as well as how they react to their per- Aryee, S., Chen, Z. X., Sun, L., & Debrah, Y. A. (2007). Antecedents
ceptions of supervisory abuse, as opposed to reactions to and outcomes of abusive supervision: Test of a trickle-down
actual supervisory abuse. Although our examination of model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 191–201.

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

Ayman, R., Chemers, M. M., & Fiedler, F. (1995). The contingency Chernyshenko, O. S., Stark, S., & Drasgow, F. (2011). Individual
model of leadership effectiveness: Its levels of analysis. The differences: Their measurement and validity. In S. Zedeck (Ed.),
Leadership Quarterly, 6, 147–167. APA handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. New 2, pp. 117–151). Washington, DC: American Psychological
York: Prentice Hall. Association.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator Colbert, A. E., Judge, T. A., Choi, D., & Wang, G. (2012). Assessing
variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, the trait theory of leadership using self and observer ratings of
strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality personality: The mediating role of contributions to group
and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. success. The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 670–685.
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality Colbert, A. E., Mount, M. K., Harter, J. K., Witt, L. A., & Barrick, M.
dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel R. (2004). Interactive effects of personality and perceptions of
Psychology, 44, 1–26. the work situation on workplace deviance. Journal of Applied
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). The FFM Psychology, 89, 599–609.
personality dimensions and job performance: Meta-analysis of Colbert, A. E., & Witt, L. A. (2009). The role of goal-focused
meta-analyses. International Journal of Selection and Assess- leadership in enabling the expression of conscientiousness.
ment, 9, 9–30. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 790–796.
Barry, B., & Friedman, R. A. (1998). Bargainer characteristics in Costa, P. T., Jr, & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality
distributive and integrative negotiation. Journal of Social and Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-
Personality Psychology, 74, 345–359. FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: PAR.
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass and Stogdill’s handbook of leadership (3rd Credé, M., Harms, P. D., Nierhorster, S., & Gaye-Valentine, A.
ed.). New York: Free Press. (2012). An evaluation of the consequences of using short
Becker, T. E. (2005). Potential problems in the statistical control of measures of the Big Five personality traits. Journal of Person-
variables in organizational research: A qualitative analysis with ality and Social Psychology, 102, 874–888.
recommendations. Organizational Research Methods, 8(3), 274– Curtis, L. A. (1974). Victim precipitation and violent crime. Social
289. Problems, 21, 594–605.
Bennett, R. J., & Robinson, S. L. (2000). Development of a measure Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way
of workplace deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(3), interactions: The development and application of a slope
349–360. difference test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926.
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and Diefendorff, J. M., & Richard, E. M. (2003). Antecedents and
control. New York: McGraw-Hill. consequences of emotional display rule perceptions. Journal of
Berry, C. M., Ones, D. S., & Sackett, P. R. (2007). Interpersonal Applied Psychology, 88, 284–294.
deviance, organizational deviance, and their common correlates: Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-
A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417–440.
410–424. Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006).
Bies, R. J., & Tripp, T. M. (1996). Revenge in organizations: The The mini-IPIP scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five
good, the bad, and the ugly. In R. W. Griffin, A. O’Leary-Kelly, factors of personality. Psychological Assessment, 18, 192–203.
& J. M. Collins (Eds.), Dysfunctional behavior in organizations: Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D., & Pagon, M. (2002). Social undermining in
Violent and deviant behavior. Stamford, CT: JAI Press. the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 331–351.
Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1993). Expanding the criterion Eisenberger, R., Lynch, P., & Aselage, J. (2004). Who takes the most
domain to include elements of contextual performance. In N. revenge? Individual differences in negative reciprocity norm
Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in endorsement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30,
organizations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 787–799.
Bowers, K. S. (1973). Situationism in psychology: An analysis and a Elias, R. (1986). The politics of victimization: Victims, victimology,
critique. Psychological Review, 80, 307–336. and human rights. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bradley, B. H. (2008). The bad apple spoils the bunch: How a Fiedler, F. E. (1967). A theory of leadership effectiveness. New York:
disagreeable person damages team performance and what can McGraw-Hill.
be done about it. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Fiedler, F. E. (1972). Personality, motivational systems, and behavior
University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA. of high and low LPC persons. Human Relations, 25(5), 391–412.
Brown, M. E., Treviño, L. K., & Harrison, D. A. (2005). Ethical Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review
leadership: A social learning perspective for construct develop- of General Psychology, 2, 300–319.
ment and testing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Freedman, M. B., Leary, T. F., Ossorio, A. G., & Coffrey, H. S.
Processes, 97, 117–134. (1951). The interpersonal dimension of personality. Journal of
Burton, J. P., & Hoobler, J. M. (2011). Aggressive reactions to Personality, 20, 143–161.
abusive supervision: The role of interactional justice and Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, person-
narcissism. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 52, 389–398. ality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several
Caldwell, D. F., & Burger, J. M. (1997). Personality and social Wave-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F.
influence strategies in the workplace. Personality and Social Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp.
Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1003–1012. 7–28). Tilburg: Tilburg University Press.
Carlo, G., Okun, M. A., Knight, G. P., & de Guzman, M. R. T. (2005). Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. H. (1997). Agreeableness: A
The interplay of traits and motives on volunteering: Agreeable- dimension of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs
ness, extraversion and prosocial value motivation. Personality (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 795–824).
and Individual Differences, 38(6), 1293–1305. London: Academic Press.
Chen, X. P., Lam, S. S. K., Naumann, S. E., & Schaubroeck, J. Harris, K. J., Kacmar, K. M., & Zivnuska, S. (2007). An investigation
(2005). Group citizenship behavior: Conceptualization and of abusive supervision as a predictor of performance and the
preliminary tests of its antecedents and consequences. Manage- meaning of work as a moderator of the relationship. The
ment and Organization Review, 1, 273–300. Leadership Quarterly, 18, 252–263.

123
G. Wang et al.

Harrison, D. A., Newman, D. A., & Roth, P. L. (2006). How revenge cognitions. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
important are job attitudes? Meta-analytic comparisons of Psychology, 83, 835–856.
integrative behavioral outcomes and time sequences. Academy MacKinnon, D. P., Fritz, M. S., Williams, J., & Lockwood, C. M.
of Management Journal, 49, 305–325. (2007). Distribution of the product confidence limits for the
Henle, C. A., & Gross, M. A. (2014). What have I done to deserve indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behavior Research Meth-
this? Effects of employee personality and emotion on abusive ods, 39(3), 384–389.
supervision. Journal of Business Ethics, 122, 461–474. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004).
Hoobler, J., & Brass, D. (2006). Abusive supervision and family Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the
undermining as displaced aggression. Journal of Applied Psy- product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral
chology, 91, 1125–1133. Research, 39(1), 99–128.
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Martinko, M. J., Harvey, P., Brees, J. R., & Mackey, J. (2013). A
Correcting error and bias in research findings (2nd ed.). review of abusive supervision research. Journal of Organiza-
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc. tional Behavior, 34, S120–S137.
Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job Mathieu, J. E., & Taylor, S. R. (2006). Clarifying conditions and
performance: The Big Five revisited. Journal of Applied decision points for mediational type inferences in Organizational
Psychology, 85(6), 869–871. Behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 1031–1056.
Inness, M., Barling, J., & Turner, N. (2005). Understanding super- Matthiesen, S. B., & Einarsen, S. (2001). MMPI-2 configurations after
visor-targeted aggression: A within-person, between-jobs design. persistent bullying at work. European Journal of Work and
Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 731–739. Organizational Psychology, 10, 467–484.
Jantz, G. L., & McMurray, A. (2009). Healing the scars of emotional Mawritz, M. B., Mayer, D. M., Hoobler, J., Wayne, S. J., & Marinova,
abuse. Grand Rapids, MI: Revell. S. V. (2012). A trickle-down model of abusive supervision.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL 8.8 for Windows Personnel Psychology, 65(2), 325–357.
[computer software]. Skokie, IL: Scientific Software Interna- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1989). The structure of
tional, Inc. interpersonal traits: Wiggins’s circumplex and the five-factor
Judge, T. A., Rodell, J. B., Klinger, R. L., Simon, L. S., & Crawford, model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 586–
E. R. (2013). Hierarchical representations of the five-factor 595.
model of personality in predicting job performance: Integrating Mitchell, M. S., & Ambrose, M. L. (2007). Abusive supervision and
three organizing framework with two theoretical perspectives. workplace deviance and the moderating effects of negative
Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(6), 875–925. reciprocity beliefs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1159–
Kenrick, D. T., & Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: 1168.
Lessons from the person–situation debate. American Psycholo- Mitchell, M. S., & Ambrose, M. L. (2012). Employees’ behavioral
gist, 43, 23–34. reactions to supervisor aggression: An examination of individual
Kiazad, K., Restubog, S. L. D., Zagencyzk, T. J., Kiewitz, C., & and situational factors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(6),
Tang, R. L. (2010). In pursuit of power: The role of authoritarian 1148–1170.
leadership in the relationship between supervisors’ Machiavel- Mount, M. K., & Barrick, M. R. (1995). The Big Five personality
lianism and subordinates’ perceptions of abusive supervisory dimensions: Implications for theory and practice in human
behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 512–519. resource management. Research in Personnel and Human
Kiewitz, C., Restubog, S. L. D., Zagenczyk, T. J., Scott, K. L., Garcia, Resource Management, 13, 153–200.
P. R. J., & Tang, R. L. (2012). Sins of the parents: The role of Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., Scullen, S. M., & Rounds, J. (2005).
supervisors’ prior experience of family undermining in predict- Higher-order dimensions of the big five personality traits and the
ing subordinates’ perceptions of abusive supervision. The big six vocational interest types. Personnel Psychology, 58(2),
Leadership Quarterly, 23, 869–882. 447–478.
Koole, S. L., Jager, W., van den Berg, A. E., Vlek, C. A., & Hofstee, Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., & Stewart, G. L. (1998). Five-factor
W. K. (2001). On the social nature of personality: Effects of model of personality and performance in jobs involving inter-
extraversion, agreeableness, and feedback about collective personal interactions. Human Performance, 11, 145–165.
resource use on cooperation in a resource dilemma. Personality Murphy, C. M., & O’Leary, K. D. (1989). Psychological aggression
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(3), 289–301. predicts physical aggression in early marriage. Journal of
Krasikova, D. V., Green, S. G., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Destructive Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 579–582.
leadership: A theoretical review, integration, and future research Nandkeolyar, A. K., Shaffer, J. A., Li, A., Ekkirala, S., & Bagger, J.
agenda. Journal of Management, 39(5), 1308–1338. (2014). Surviving an abusive supervisor: The joint roles of
LeBreton, J. M., Wu, J., & Bing, M. N. (2009). The truth(s) on testing for conscientiousness and coping strategies. Journal of Applied
mediation in the social and organizational sciences. In C. E. Lance Psychology, 99(1), 138–150.
& R. J. Vandenberg (Eds.), Statistical and methodological myths Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-
and urban legends: Doctrine, verity, and fable in the organiza- Hill.
tional and social sciences (pp. 107–142). New York: Routledge. O’Leary-Kelly, A., Griffin, R., & Glew, D. (1996). Organization-
LePine, J. A., Hollenbeck, J. R., Ilgen, D. R., & Hedlund, J. (1997). motivated aggression: A research framework. Academy of
Effects of individual differences on the performance of hierar- Management Review, 21, 225–253.
chical decision making teams: Much more than g. Journal of Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer
Applied Psychology, 82, 803–811. ratings of Five-Factor Model of personality traits: A meta-
Lian, H., Lance Ferris, D., & Brown, D. J. (2012). Does power analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 762–773.
distance exacerbate or mitigate the effects of abusive supervi- Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in schools: Bullies and whipping boys.
sion? It depends on the outcome. Journal of Applied Psychology, Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
97, 107–123. Organ, D. W., & Paine, J. B. (1999). A new kind of performance for
Liu, J., Kwong, K. H., Wu, L. Z., & Wu, W. (2010). Abusive industrial and organizational psychology: Recent contributions
supervision and subordinate supervisor-directed deviance: The to the study of organizational citizenship behavior. In C. L.
moderating role of traditional values and the mediating role of Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of

123
Subordinates and Abusive Supervision

industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 337–368). Tepper, B. J., Duffy, M. K., Hoobler, J. M., & Ensley, M. D. (2004).
New York: Wiley. Moderators of the relationship between coworkers’ organiza-
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. tional citizenship behavior and fellow employees’ attitudes.
(2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 455–465.
critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Tepper, B. J., Duffy, M. K., & Shaw, J. D. (2001). Personality
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903. moderators of the relationships between abusive supervision and
Restubog, S. L. D., Scott, K. D., & Zagenczyk, T. J. (2011). When subordinates’ resistance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,
distress hits home: The effects of contextual factors and 974–983.
psychological distress in predicting employee responses to Tepper, B. J., Henle, C. A., Lambert, L. S., Giacalone, R. A., &
abusive supervision. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 713– Duffy, M. K. (2008). Abusive supervision and subordinates’
729. organization deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 721–
Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. (1995). A typology of deviant 732.
workplace behaviors: A multidimensional scaling study. Acad- Tepper, B. J., Moss, S., & Duffy, M. K. (2011). Predictors of abusive
emy of Management Journal, 38, 555–572. supervision: Supervisor perceptions of deep-level dissimilarity,
Robinson, S. L., & Bennett, R. J. (1997). Workplace deviance: Its relationship conflict, and subordinate performance. Academy of
definition, its manifestations, and its causes. Research on Management Journal, 54, 279–294.
Negotiations in Organizations, 6, 3–27. Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based
Roch, S. G., Ayman, R., & Harris, M. (2005). Effect of identifiability, interactionist model of job performance. Journal of Applied
rating audience, and conscientiousness on rating level. Interna- Psychology, 88, 500–517.
tional Journal of Selection and Assessment, 13, 53–62. Tett, R. P., & Guterman, H. A. (2000). Situation trait relevance, trait
Roelofs, J., Huibers, M., Peeters, F., Arntz, A., & van Os, J. (2008). expression, and cross-situational consistency: Testing a principle
Rumination and worrying as possible mediators in the relation of trait activation. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 397–
between neuroticism and symptoms of depression and anxiety in 423.
clinically depressed. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, Thau, S., Bennett, R. J., Mitchell, M. S., & Marrs, M. B. (2009). How
1283–1289. management style moderates the relationship between abusive
Roth, P. L., Switzer, F. S, III, Van Iddekinge, C. H., & Oh, I. S. supervision and workplace deviance: An uncertainty manage-
(2011). Toward better meta-analytic matrices: How input values ment perspective. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
can affect research conclusions in human resource management Processes, 108, 79–92.
simulations. Personnel Psychology, 64(4), 899–935. Thau, S., & Mitchell, M. S. (2010). Self-gain or self-regulation
Rousseau, D. M. (1995). Psychological contracts in organizations: impairment? Tests of competing explanations of the supervisor
Understanding written and unwritten agreements. Thousand abuse and employee deviance relationship through perceptions
Oaks, CA: Sage. of distributive justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 1009–
Shaffer, J., & Postlethwaite, B. E. (2012). A matter of context: A 1031.
meta-analytic investigation of the relative validity of contextu- Trierweiler, L. I., Eid, M., & Lischetzke, T. (2002). The structure of
alized and noncontextualized personality measures. Personnel emotional expressivity: Each emotion counts. Journal of
Psychology, 65, 445–494. Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 1023–1040.
Shoss, M., Eisenberger, R., Restubog, S. L. D., & Zagenczyk, T. J. Wang, W., Mao, J., Wu, W., & Liu, J. (2012). Abusive supervision
(2013). Blaming the organization for abusive supervision: The roles and workplace deviance: The mediating role of interactional
of perceived organizational support and supervisor organizational justice and the moderating role of power distance. Asia Pacific
embodiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98, 158–168. Journal of Human Resources, 50, 43–60.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The
nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psycholog-
Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422–445. ical Bulletin, 96, 465–490.
Snyder, M. (1983). The influence of individuals on situations: Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait descriptive
Implications for understanding the links between personality and terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and
social behavior. Journal of Personality, 51(3), 497–516. Social Psychology, 37, 395–412.
Spector, P. E. (1987). Method variance as an artifact in self-reported Wiggins, J. S. (1980). Circumplex models of interpersonal behavior.
affect and perceptions at work: Myth or significant problem? In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology
Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 438–443. (Vol. 1, pp. 265–294). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Sutton, R. I. (2007). The no asshole rule: Building a civilized Williams, L. J., & Anderson, S. E. (1991). Job satisfaction and
workplace and surviving one that isn’t. New York: Warner organizational commitment as predictors of organizational
Business Books. citizenship and in-role behaviors. Journal of Management, 17,
Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy 601–617.
of Management Journal, 43, 178–190. Williams, L. J., Hartman, N., & Cavazotte, F. (2010). Method
Tepper, B. J. (2007). Abusive supervision in work organizations: variance and marker variables: A review and comprehensive
Review, synthesis, and research agenda. Journal of Manage- CFA marker technique. Organizational Research Methods, 13,
ment, 33, 261–289. 477–514.
Tepper, B. J., Carr, J. C., Breaux, D. M., Geider, S., Hu, C., & Hua, W. Wu, T.-Y., & Hu, C. (2013). Abusive supervision and subordinate
(2009). Abusive supervision, intentions to quit, and employees’ emotional labor: The moderating role of openness personality.
workplace deviance: A power/dependence analysis. Organiza- Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 956–970.
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 156–167. Yukl, G. (2010). Leadership in organizations (7th ed.). Upper Saddle
Tepper, B. J., Duffy, M. K., Henle, C. A., & Lambert, L. S. (2006). River, NJ: Pearson Education Inc.
Procedural injustice, victim precipitation, and abusive supervi-
sion. Personnel Psychology, 59, 101–123.

123

You might also like