Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

The Journal of Sex Research

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hjsr20

Religiosity, Perceived Effects of Pornography Use


on Personal Sex Life, and Moral Incongruence:
Insights from the German Health and Sexuality
Survey (GeSiD)

Aleksandar Štulhofer, Christian Wiessner, Goran Koletić, Laura Pietras &


Peer Briken

To cite this article: Aleksandar Štulhofer, Christian Wiessner, Goran Koletić, Laura Pietras & Peer
Briken (2021): Religiosity, Perceived Effects of Pornography Use on Personal Sex Life, and Moral
Incongruence: Insights from the German Health and Sexuality Survey (GeSiD), The Journal of Sex
Research, DOI: 10.1080/00224499.2021.1916422

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2021.1916422

Published online: 04 May 2021.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 479

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hjsr20
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH
https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2021.1916422

Religiosity, Perceived Effects of Pornography Use on Personal Sex Life, and Moral
Incongruence: Insights from the German Health and Sexuality Survey (GeSiD)
a b a
Aleksandar Štulhofer , Christian Wiessner , Goran Koletić , Laura Pietrasc, and Peer Briken c

a
Department of Sociology, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Zagreb; bInstitute of Medical Biometry and Epidemiology,
University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf; cInstitute for Sex Research, Sexual Medicine and Forensic Psychiatry, Center for Psychosocial
Medicine, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf

ABSTRACT
Although online pornography use appears to be a mainstream activity, there is little information,
particularly outside of the USA, about how consumers perceive its impact on their sexuality.
Considering increasing concerns about pornography use, this lack of evidence has sociocultural and
clinical ramifications – especially because the recently proposed Moral Incongruence model (MI) suggests
that some individuals may see their pornography use as problematic independently of the frequency of
use. Using data from 4,177 adults from the 2018–2019 national probability-based German Health and
Sexuality Survey, we explored self-perceived impact of pornography use on personal sex life and the role
of MI. Most participants (61.7%) reported no impact of pornography. Women were characterized by
significantly higher odds of reporting positive relative to no impact, while men had higher odds of
reporting mixed/negative compared to no impact. Participants in both the positive and the mixed impact
group reported a significantly higher frequency of pornography use than participants in the no impact
group. In line with the MI model, we observed a significant relationship between participants’ religious
upbringing and self-perceived negative (relative to mixed) impact of pornography use. Social relevance
and clinical implications of the findings are discussed.

Introduction
behavior is not sufficient for a diagnosis nor is distress that is
The prevailing perspective in scholarly literature on pornogra­ based on sexuality-hostile moral or religious reasons or ego-
phy use is one of concerns, with most studies focusing on dystonic experienced sexual behavior. Central to the diagnosis
potential harms and adverse outcomes (Fisher et al., 2019; is a persistent pattern of failure to control intense and repetitive
McKee, 2007). With online use of sexually explicit material sexual impulses or urges, resulting in repetitive sexual behavior
becoming a mainstream behavior among both adults and accompanied by distress (Briken, 2020; Kraus et al., 2018).
young people (Grubbs et al., 2019; Kohut & Štulhofer, 2018; Although it is commonly assumed that problematic pornogra­
Owens et al., 2012; Peter & Valkenburg, 2016), a narrative phy use is a risk primarily for individuals characterized by
about a pornography-induced public health crisis emerged, habitual high-frequency use, it has been recently proposed
with a potential to generate distress and self-labeling among that religious individuals may be especially vulnerable to dis­
some consumers (Ley, 2018; Vaillancourt-Morel & Bergeron, tress and decreased psychological well-being associated with
2019; Webber & Sullivan, 2018). Given the growing pornogra­ personal pornography use (Grubbs, Kraus et al., 2019; Grubbs
phy addiction treatment industry in the Western world, and et al., 2018; Perry, 2018b). Moral incongruence between inter­
a lack of consensus about diagnosing and treating problematic nalized (religion based) beliefs and unacceptable behavior (use
pornography use (Briken, 2020; Briken et al., 2007; Kraus et al., of pornography) has been suggested as a conceptual model to
2016), a rise in the number of self-labeling cases could con­ understand this individual-level vulnerability (Briken, 2020;
tribute to a moral panic about pornography use. The current Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019).
study aimed to explore how congruent are perceptions about To provide more insight about self-perceived effects of
the impact of pornography use on personal sex life with the pornography use and how they resonate with the narrative
societal concerns. about pornography as a public health hazard, the current
For the diagnosis of Compulsive Sexual Behavior Disorder study addressed the prevalence of reported positive, mixed,
(CSBD), which has been included recently into the 11th edition negative, and no impacts of pornography use on personal sex
of International Classification of Diseases and Related Health life, as well as characteristics of adult women and men who
Problems (Briken, 2020; World Health Organization, 2018), report different impact of their pornography use. Further, to
risks for overpathologization of sexual behavior were explicitly explore the utility of the moral incongruence concept, which
named and attempts were made to systematically exclude has been tested almost exclusively in North American samples,
them. Strong sexual interest or a high frequency of sexual we explored its role in reporting different types of self-

CONTACT Goran Koletić gkoletic@hotmail.com Department of Sociology, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences, University of Zagreb, I. Lučića, Zagreb
10000, Croatia
© 2021 The Society for the Scientific Study of Sexuality
2 A. ŠTULHOFER ET AL.

evaluated impact of pornography use using the data collected Such a conclusion seems supported by qualitative research,
in a large-scale population-based German sample. It should be particularly by a larger Canadian mixed-methods study that
noted that our study did not address problematic pornography found “no negative impact” to be the most common theme in
use, a clinical construct conceptualized, measured, and diag­ participants’ responses to open-ended survey questions (Kohut
nosed by experts (Bőthe et al., 2019; Kor et al., 2014; Kraus & et al., 2017). However, it should be noted that qualitative
Sweeney, 2019). Instead, we focused on laymen’s perceptions research in this area paints a more complex picture, perhaps
of how, if at all, pornography affects their sexuality. because it is based on developing adolescents’ and emerging
adults’ narratives (Harvey, 2020; Löfgren-Mårtenson &
Månsson, 2010; McCormack & Wignall, 2017).
Self-Perceived Impact of Pornography Use on Personal Sex
Life
Moral Incongruence as an Explanation for Perceiving
In one of the first systematic explorations of the self-perceived Personal Pornography Use as Problematic?
effects of pornography use on personal sex lives, Hald and
In a series of recent papers, Grubbs and colleagues proposed
Malamuth (2008) found little evidence of negative self-
and advanced a conceptual model focusing on a specific psy­
perceived effects in a national probability sample of young
chosocial mechanism, moral incongruence (MI), that underlies
Danes. Overall, they observed moderate positive effects,
self-perceived pornography addiction and problematic porno­
which were more prevalent among male than female
graphy use (Grubbs, Kraus et al., 2020; Grubbs, Lee et al., 2020;
participants.1 Importantly, the authors also noted a significant
Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019; Grubbs et al.,
and positive relationship between the frequency of pornogra­
2018). The authors described the mechanism through the
phy use and reported self-perceived positive, but not negative,
following set of propositions: (1) religiosity, which is reflected
effects (Hald & Malamuth, 2008). In discussing the findings,
in the internalization of religious norms and values, leads to (2)
the authors suggested that the gender difference is likely due to
moral disapproval of pornography. When a person uses por­
gender-specific evaluation of and interest in sexually explicit
nography in spite of his/her disapproval, which seems to be
material. They contrasted the finding that frequency of porno­
relatively common (Perry, 2017, 2018a; Perry & Whitehead,
graphy use was positively linked to self-perceived positive
2018), moral incongruence – the experience of incompatibility
effects of using pornography with an expectation, based on
between the person’s behavior and their highly regarded values
the desensitization model (Landripet et al., 2019; Seigfried-
and beliefs about such behavior – leads to self-blame and self-
Spellar & Rogers, 2013), that pornography use will escalate
perceived inability to control the behavior (hence, self-labeling
over time – leading to adverse outcomes. In conclusion, Hald
as “sexual addict”) (Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019). This conceptual
and Malamuth warned against the generalization of their find­
model helps to explain findings that more religious users of
ings across cultures, proposing that Danish culture may be
pornography are likely to report problematic pornography use
characterized by lower risks of sexual aggression compared
and self-proclaimed addictive pornography use at substantially
to, for example, the U.S.A. (Hald & Malamuth, 2008).
lower levels of pornography consumption than less religious
In more than a decade since the publication of the Hald and
participants (Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs et al., 2018).
Malamuth study, limited additional information about how
Similar dynamics were found for the association between por­
men and women evaluate their pornography use has been
nography use and reporting symptoms of depression (Perry,
made available, despite an increase in the availability of free
2018b).
online pornography and mainstream acceptance of pornogra­
The MI model has been criticized for prioritizing MI over
phy (Lykke & Cohen, 2015). Three studies that used full or
other factors and personal characteristics relevant for either
a brief version of the Pornography Consumption Effects Scale,
dysregulated or just self-stigmatized use of pornography, such
originally developed by Hald, consistently found that self-
as media narratives about pornography addiction, partner’s
percieved positive effects were more prevalent than negative
objections and associated relationship friction, a feminist per­
effects in convenience samples of men who have sex with men
spective on pornography industry and its products, etc. (Fisher
(Hald et al., 2013), heterosexual men (Miller et al., 2018), as
et al., 2019; Vaillancourt-Morel & Bergeron, 2019; Willoughby,
well as Canadian (Hesse & Pedersen, 2017) and Indonesian
2019). In addition, the conceptualization of MI is not comple­
university students (Wijaya Mulya & Hald, 2014). In another
tely clear on the distinction between subjective (i.e., the experi­
earlier study that was carried out in a large-scale non-
ence of MI) and objective factors (i.e., high frequency of
probability-based sample of Australians to explore self-
pornography use) (Grubbs, Kraus et al., 2019). While in an
perceived effects on pornography use on personal attitudes
earlier paper the authors seemed to believe that the frequency
toward sexuality, 35% of participants reported no impact,
of use was of secondary importance to MI (Grubbs & Perry,
59% of participants reported positive impact and only 7%
2018), in a later paper they seem to have adopted a less hier­
negative impact of pornography use (Mckee, 2007).
archical viewpoint, in which MI and highly frequent use of
In conclusion, the literature suggests that self-perceived
pornography are conceptualized as independent predictors of
impact of pornography use on personal sex life, when any is
problematic pornography use (Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019).
reported, is more often perceived as positive than negative.
However, meta-analytic findings reported in the later study
1 indicated that the association between MI and problematic use
It should be noted that the authors explored levels of positive relative to
negative impact. They did not assess the absolute prevalence of reporting (rz = .66) was substantially greater than the association between
positive, negative, and no impact of pornography use. the frequency of pornography use and problematic outcomes
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 3

(rz = .28), strengthening the theoretical emphasis on the role of Using a large-scale national probability-based sample of
MI (Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019). German adults, we explored differences in self-perceived
It should be stressed that most of the evidence in support of impact of pornography use on personal sex life focusing on
the MI model has been collected in North American, primarily the following two research questions:
U.S., samples (Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019).
This prompts a question about the extent to which the model RQ1 – Which characteristics differentiate participants who
may be culture-specific (see Vaillancourt-Morel & Bergeron, reported positive and those who reported mixed or negative
2019) or substantially more relevant for a society characterized self-perceived impact of pornography use on their sex life from
by high religiosity (including more fundamental branches of peers who reported no effects?
Christianity) and rising moral panic related to pornography
use, not only among young people, as mirrored in an increasing RQ2 – Does MI, as suggested by the recent literature, account
number of U.S. states declaring pornography a public health for some of these differences in the German context?
crisis (Ley, 2018; McKay et al., 2021; Webber & Sullivan, 2018).
The scant evidence from other countries, mostly European, is We believe that the phenomenon of self-perceived impact of
largely indirect – with the exception of a recent Polish study pornography use and the role of MI have both clinical and
(Lewczuk et al., 2020). More precisely, two earlier studies, also social relevance (Grubbs, Perry, et al., 2019). Self-perceived
based on a large-scale convenience sample as the Polish one, harm is distressing and, apart from reduction in psychological
reported significant associations between religiosity indicators, well-being, may lead to adverse health outcomes – particularly
on the one hand, and problematic sexual use of Internet (Ross in an environment in which the narrative about harms of
et al., 2012) and hypersexuality (Štulhofer et al., 2016), on the pornography use is gaining mainstream traction. In such socio­
other hand. Unlike the Lewczuk et al. study, the two studies (one cultural climate, self-stigmatization among pornography users
sampling Swedish and another Croatian adults) did not include may contribute to a more negative public view of sexuality and/
an indicator of moral disapproval of pornography. The Polish or a moral panic regarding pornography use. In exploring the
study (Lewczuk et al., 2020), which directly explored the MI role of MI, the current study also has direct ramifications for
model, found that moral disapproval of pornography and reli­ clinical management of individuals who report problematic use
giosity predicted self-perceived addiction and problematic por­ of sexually explicit material (Briken, 2020; Grubbs et al., 2017;
nography use, respectively. In contrast to small effect sizes of Kraus et al., 2018).
these links, the authors reported strong associations between the
frequency of pornography use and the two indicators of proble­
matic pornography use – reiterating the dilemma about the Method
contribution of high levels of pornography use to self-
Participants and Data Collection
perceived problematic pornography use independently of MI
(Bőthe et al., 2020; Štulhofer et al., 2016). Considering that To test the research questions, we used data from the first
Poland is one of the most religious European societies (Luijkx German national sex survey (GeSiD: German Health and
et al., 2016), systematic exploration of the MI model in less Sexuality Survey). The analytical sample for the current
religious societies is notably missing. study included 4,177 participants who reported any lifetime
pornography use (44.2% were women; 648 individuals
answered that they never used pornography). All partici­
Study Aims pants were German-speaking residents, aged between 18
and 75 years (M = 45.6, SD = 15.2). The study used a two-
There were two main rationales for this study. First, considering
stage stratified register-based sampling, with 178 municipa­
the scant literature on self-perceived impact of pornography use,
lities (200 sampling points) selected in the first stage, and
we wanted to provide information about how perceptions about
a random proportional to size sample of residents per
the self-perceived impact of pornography use on personal sex life
sampling point selected in the second stage. Young adults
resonate with the emerging narrative on pornography as a public
aged 18 to 35 years, the part of the population that is most
health crisis risk. Second, the current study aimed to empirically
sexually active and most vulnerable to sexually transmitted
assess the validity of the MI conceptual model in a less religious
infections, were oversampled to enable more detailed
society compared to the U.S.A.,2 where most of the evidence in
insights into sexual risk taking and the associated repro­
support of the model has been collected (Grubbs, Perry et al.,
ductive and sexual health outcomes. The dataset was
2019). This seems important, because it provides insights about
weighted for age, gender, education, nationality, and region
the model’s utility in different cultural settings – which has
to be broadly representative of the adult German popula­
scientific, but also clinical implications. Are faith-based moral
tion (for a detailed description of the GeSiD study metho­
beliefs and values linked to self-perceived negative impact of
dology, see Matthiesen et al., 2018). Calculated according to
pornography use on person’s sexuality in a similar fashion, and
the 2016 guidelines of the American Association for Public
independently of the frequency of pornography use, in more
Opinion Research (AAPOR) adapted for use in the context
religious and less religious societies?
of German public opinion surveys (Stadtmüller et al., 2019),
the study response rate was 30.2%.
2
According to the World Values Survey data (see http://www.worldvaluessurvey. Fieldwork was carried out between October 2018 and
org/WVSOnline.jsp). September 2019 by 257 trained interviewers employed by the
4 A. ŠTULHOFER ET AL.

Kantar GmbH social research institute. Prior to an inter­ Attitudes toward Sexuality
viewer’s visit (interviewer’s biological sex was matched with Liberal vs. non-liberal attitudes about sexuality were measured
participant’s), all selected participants received a personal letter with a single-item indicator (“Many people use terms ‘liberal’
with brief study information, an endorsement letter from the and ‘conservative’ to describe their attitude toward sexuality
German Federal Center for Health Education, and the research (. . .) to which group do you belong?”). A 7-point scale, ranging
team’s contact information. Written informed consent was from 1 = liberal (open-minded) to 7 = conservative (traditional),
obtained from all participants; 30 EUR (about 34 USD) was was used to record responses.
offered as a token of appreciation.
The questionnaire, which was extensively pretested in 2016/ Moral Issues with Pornography
2017 using a sample of 1,555 individuals (Matthiesen et al., Only participants who reported either mixed or negative self-
2018), was a combination of computer assisted personal inter­ perceived impact on pornography use on their sex life were
viewing (CAPI) and, for more sensitive questions (including asked a question about possible negative consequences of their
a module about pornography use), computer assisted self- pornography use (“Has your pornography consumption ever
interviewing (CASI). During the CASI phase, the interviewer resulted in: (a) Relationship problems; (b) termination of
stayed in the same room with the participant, but was a relationship; (c) lack of sexual desire for your partner; (d)
instructed to position themselves so that they could not see problems at work or at school; (e) bad consciousness of a moral
what the participant was typing. The interviewers’ role was to or religious practice; and (f) none of the above?”) to which
provide support, if needed. The questionnaire consisted of 260 participants were to respond with yes or no. Checking the item
questions, which were divided into 18 modules that focused on “A bad consciousness of a moral or religious nature” indicated
different aspects of sexual health. On average, the interview a dissonance or moral incongruence between personal beliefs
took about 51 minutes (median completion time was 48 min­ or values and practice.
utes). All GeSiD study procedures were approved by the Ethical
Board of the State Psychotherapy Chamber in Hamburg. Religiosity Indicators
Apart from the standard public opinion measure of religiosity
(“How often do you practice your faith (visit service, pray,
Measures comply with religious rules)?”), with four response categories
Pornography Use ranging from 1 = never to 4 = often,3 the survey also asked
In the questionnaire, pornography was defined as: “When we about being raised religiously. The latter indicator was formu­
refer to ‘watching pornography’ (in this section) we refer to all lated as: “I experience a strictly religious upbringing in my
media depicting primarily erotic and sexual acts (texts, draw­ childhood and youth”, with a corresponding scale ranging
ings, photos, films, clips, streams, and online material); porno­ from 1 = does not apply to me at all to 5 = totally applies to me.
graphic images are used for sexual arousal.” Frequency of
pornography use in the past 12 months was measured on Analytical Strategy
a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = daily. Self-
perceived impact of pornography use was assessed by the To explore the two research questions, multinomial logistic
following question: “Did your consumption of pornography regression was used to identify predictors and correlates of self-
have an impact on your sex life? Four choices were offered: perceived impact of pornography use, with the no impact
1 = no impact, 2 = a positive impact, 3 = both a positive and group serving as reference category. Taking into account the
negative impact, and 4 = a negative impact. complex sampling design used in the GESID study, the multi­
variate regression analysis was carried out with Stata’s 15 svy
command to adjust for stratification and clustering of the data,
Sociodemographic Characteristics as well as for post-hoc weighting (by sex, age, education,
Apart from age and gender, we also asked about education nationality, and region) based on the 2016 census data. The
(no completed school, primary, secondary, and tertiary edu­ analysis was carried out sequentially. Sociodemographic char­
cation), monthly household income (ranging from 1 = up to acteristics were included in the first step (model A), the fre­
200 EUR (≈ US$ 237) to 10 = 6,000 EUR (≈ US$ 7,101) or quency of pornography use was added in the second step
more), urban vs. rural place of residence (living in (model B), and attitudes toward sexuality and religiosity indi­
a settlement of up to 5,000 inhabitants), and current relation­ cators were added in the final step (model C). Model fit indices
ship status (dichotomized into single and married or in (log-likelihood, AIC, and BIC) were estimated using raw data
a relationship). In addition, the following question was used (i.e., without adjustment for data nestedness and weighting).
to address sexual identity: “Please select the answer which To assess possible differences between the mixed impact
describes best how you see yourself at present?” Answers were and the negative impact group, we employed binomial logistic
anchored on a 7-point scale. The first five points ranged from regression analysis, with the larger group (mixed impact) ser­
1 = exclusively heterosexual to 5 = exclusively homosexual/gay, ving as reference. Due to the negative impact group’s small size
while the final two points denoted asexual and other identi­ (n = 87), the number of independent variables in this logistic
ties. Due to a small number of participants in all but the regression model was limited to seven. In addition to basic
exclusively heterosexual category, the indicator was dichoto­
mized into 0 = exclusively heterosexual identity and 1 = a 3
Participants who had previously reported that they were not religious were
sexual minority identity. assigned “never” as their frequency of practicing faith.
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 5

sociodemographic characteristics (gender, age, education, and Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample (Only participants who
reported lifetime pornography use; n = 4,177).
sexual orientation), only the indicators that were consistently
associated with the outcome in the multinomial testing (fre­ % (unweighted n; weighted
n)
quency of pornography use and religious upbringing) were Age
included in this final analysis. To further explore the MI con­ 18–25 12.0 (652; 496)
ceptual model, the indicator of moral disapproval of porno­ 26–35 18.2 (966; 750)
36–45 17.3 (716; 713)
graphy use – for which only participants who reported negative 46–55 22.8 (752; 940)
or mixed impact of pornography use provided information – 56–65 18.1 (698; 746)
was also added to the analysis. 66–75 11.5 (393; 473)
Gender
Less than 6% of participants who reported any lifetime Male 55.8 (2,174; 2,296)
pornography use did not answer the question about the self- Female 44.2 (2,003; 1,822)
perceived impact of pornography use on their sex life. There In a relationship/married
Yes 76.4 (3,206; 3,146)
was less than two percent of missing information about the No 23.6 (971; 973)
frequency of pornography use in the past 12 months and <0.5% Residence
of missing responses to the question about religious upbring­ Rural (up to 5,000 inhabitants) 14.2 (582; 586)
Urban 85.8 (3,595; 3,532)
ing. Similarly rare was missing information regarding the fre­ Education
quency of attending religious ceremonies (<1.5%). No completed formal education 1.5 (40; 63)
Primary education 27.8 (727; 1,137)
Secondary education 50.9 (2,372; 2,082)
Results Tertiary education 19.7 (1,005; 806)
Income
Basic sociodemographic characteristics, including the frequency Less than 1,299 EUR (≈ US$ 1,542) 13.1 (477; 505)
1,300 to 2,599 EUR (≈ US$ 1,543–3,084) 33.3 (1,208; 1,278)
of pornography use in the past 12 months, of the current study’s 2,600 to 4,499 EUR (≈ US$ 3,086–5,339) 35.9 (1,460; 1,377)
sample are presented in Table 1. A great majority of participants 4,500 EUR or more (≈ US$ 5,341) 17.7 (749; 681)
reported no impact of pornography use on their sex life (61.7%). Sexual identity
Exclusively heterosexual 88.1 (3,528; 3,534)
Of the rest, most noted a positive impact of using pornography Sexual minority 11.9 (562; 478)
(24.9%). Mixed impact was reported by 11.1% and a negative Religious practice
impact by 2.5% of participants (n = 87). In the negative impact Never 46.8 (1,962; 1,910)
Seldom 24.0 (1,007; 980)
group, 57.5% of individuals were men (n = 50). Sometimes 16.1 (671; 658)
Often 13.0 (502; 530)
Religious upbringing
RQ1 – Differences between Participants Who Reported No 1 (Does not apply to me at all) 45.3 (1,974; 1,864)
2 14.7 (645; 604)
Impact and Those Who Reported Positive and Mixed/ 3 18.1 (737; 746)
Negative Impact of Pornography Use, Respectively 4 11.4 (438; 469)
5 (Completely applies to me) 10.4 (377; 429)
The findings of sequential multinomial logistic regression ana­ Frequency of pornography use (past
lysis are shown in Table 2. With only sociodemographic indi­ 12 months)
Never 38.7 (1,476; 1,556)
cators included (see Model A), the odds of reporting a positive Up to several times a year 31.6 (1,315; 1,271)
impact compared to reporting no impact were increased by Up to several times in a month 17.8 (766; 716)
a sexual minority identity (AOR = 1.77, p < .001) and decreased Several times a week 10.4 (467; 419)
Daily 1.5 (70; 59)
by age (AOR = 0.99, p < .001). In the case of mixed/negative
Note. Numbers do not always add up due to missing information. Percentages are
impact, the odds of belonging to this group were also increased based on weighted analyses.
by reporting a sexual minority identity (AOR = 2.73, p < .001)
and decreased by age (AOR = 0.96, p < .001), female gender
(AOR = 0.29, p < .001), and being married or in a relationship pornography use was controlled for, suggesting gender-
(AOR = 0.63, p = .006). The odds of reporting mixed/negative specific perceptions of the impact among individuals with
impact of pornography use on personal sex life, compared to more frequent pornography use. This interaction effect (fre­
reporting no effects, were over 70% lower among female than quency of pornography use was mean centered before building
male participants. We also observed a role of education, with the interaction term) was confirmed in the case of positive
completed tertiary education – when compared to completed (AOR = 1.63, p < .001), but not negative impact of pornography
secondary education – increasing the odds of reporting mixed/ use (AOR = 0.82, p = .277) (not shown in tables). An inspection
negative impact (AOR = 1.69, p = 001). of the non-parametric interaction effect indicated that the odds
Adding the frequency of pornography use (Model B) some­ of reporting positive, relative to neutral, impact of pornogra­
what affected the previously reported findings. After this inclu­ phy use on personal sex life were substantially higher among
sion, age and sexual identity ceased to significantly predict more frequent female pornography users than among more
reporting positive and mixed/negative impact of pornography frequent male pornography users. We observed no differences
use, respectively. In addition, female participants were charac­ in the odds between male and female participants characterized
terized by significantly higher odds of belonging to the positive by less frequent pornography use.
impact group than male participants (AOR = 1.62, p < .001). With practically identical effect size, frequency of pornogra­
The difference became significant once the frequency of phy use increased the odds of reporting both positive
6 A. ŠTULHOFER ET AL.

Table 2. Predictors and correlates of self-perceived impact of pornography use on personal sexual life (Reference category = no impact).
Positive impact Mixed or negative impact
Model A AOR 95% CI AOR 95% CI
Age 0.99*** 0.98–0.99 0.96*** 0.96–0.97
Female gender 0.84 0.70–1.01 0.29*** 0.22–0.38
In a relationship/married 1.19 0.92–1.53 0.63** 0.46–0.87
Sexual minority 1.77*** 1.36–2.31 2.73*** 2.07–3.59
Education (reference = secondary education)
No completed school 1.09 0.43–2.80 0.20 0.03–1.38
Primary education 0.84 0.65–1.09 0.64* 0.42–0.98
Tertiary education 1.05 0.83–1.32 1.69** 1.24–2.30
Income 1.02 0.97–1.07 0.99 0.94–1.05
Urban place of residence 0.93 0.73–1.20 1.39 0.88–2.19
Observations = 3,565; Log likelihood = −3086.78, AIC = 6213.55, BIC = 6337.13; pseudo R2 = .056
Model B AOR 95% CI AOR 95% CI
Age 1.00 0.99–1.00 0.98*** 0.97–0.98
Female gender 1.62*** 1.28–2.06 0.57*** 0.43–0.76
In a relationship/married 1.36* 1.06–1.76 0.75 0.53–1.06
Sexual minority 1.48** 1.11–1.97 2.27*** 1.68–3.08
Education (reference = secondary education)
No completed school 1.48 0.46–4.81 0.29 0.04–2.07
Primary education 0.99 0.75–1.30 0.76 0.50–1.16
Tertiary education 1.04 0.82–1.32 1.65** 1.19–2.28
Income 1.01 0.96–1.05 0.99 0.93–1.05
Urban place of residence 0.92 0.73–1.16 1.46 0.97–2.31
Frequency of pornography use 1.92*** 1.69–2.17 1.94*** 1.69–2.23
Observations = 3,505; Log likelihood = −2912.32, AIC = 5868.65, BIC = 6004.26; pseudo R2 = .096
Model C AOR 95% CI AOR 95% CI
Age 1.00 0.99–1.00 0.98*** 0.97–0.98
Female gender 1.58*** 1.25–2.00 0.58*** 0.44–0.78
In a relationship/married 1.40* 1.08–1.81 0.73 0.52–1.03
Sexual minority 1.41* 1.05–1.89 2.36*** 1.74–3.20
Education (reference = secondary education)
No completed school 1.69 0.52–5.46 0.27 0.04–1.90
Primary education 1.01 0.77–1.34 0.74 0.48–1.13
Tertiary education 1.03 0.80–1.32 1.65** 1.18–2.30
Income 1.01 0.96–1.06 0.99 0.93–1.06
Urban place of residence 0.96 0.77–1.21 1.44 0.93–2.24
Frequency of pornography use 1.86*** 1.64–2.11 1.96*** 1.69–2.28
Liberal/conservative sex attitudes 0.93 0.87–1.00 1.05 0.97–1.14
Religious upbringing 0.96 0.89–1.03 1.15** 1.05–1.25
Religious practice (reference = never)
Rarely 1.01 0.79–1.29 0.73* 0.54–0.99
Sometimes 0.97 0.73–1.30 0.79 0.57–1.09
Often 1.00 0.69–1.44 0.66 0.42–1.03
Observations = 3,461; Log likelihood = −2862.35, AIC = 5788.69, BIC = 5985.47; pseudo R2 = .100
Note. CI = confidence interval around adjusted odds ratio (AOR); * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

(AOR = 1.92, p < .001) and mixed/negative impact effects. As presented in Table 3, the odds of belonging to the
(AOR = 1.94, p < .001) in comparison to no effects. The latter group were substantially increased by both religious
addition of sex attitudes and religiosity indicators (Model C) upbringing (AOR = 1.30, p = .031) and moral issues with
did not change the pattern of observed predictors and corre­ pornography (AOR = 2.11, p = .025). It should also be noted
lates. Religious upbringing significantly increased the odds that the frequency of pornography use significantly decreased
(AOR = 1.15, p = .003) of belonging to the mixed/negative the odds (AOR = 0.48, p < .001) of reporting negative, in
group. Fit indices associated with the three multinomial regres­
sion analyses indicated a continuous improvement in model fit,
pointing to the explanatory importance of the frequency of Table 3. Predictors and correlates of self-perceived negative impact of pornogra­
pornography use and religious upbringing. phy use on personal sexual life (Reference category = mixed impact; n = 507).
AOR 95% CI
Age 1.01 0.99–1.04
RQ2 – the Role of MI Female gender 0.88 0.42–1.87
Sexual minority 1.28 0.60–2.70
The significant role of religious upbringing in explaining dif­ College/university education 0.57 0.30–1.07
ferences in reporting the impact of pornography use on perso­ Frequency of pornography use 0.48*** 0.36-.65
Moral issues with pornography 2.11* 1.17–3.83
nal life provided some support for the MI. Additional evidence Religious upbringing 1.30* 1.03–1.64
was observed in the analysis that directly compared partici­ Note. CI = confidence interval around adjusted odds ratio (AOR); * p < .05, **
pants who reported mixed and those who reported negative p < .01, *** p < .001.
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 7

contrast to mixed impact of pornography use on personal sex personal sex life (Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs, Perry et al.,
life. 2019). However, they distinguish between current religiosity
levels, as indicated by the frequency of attending religious
ceremonies, and religious upbringing or growing up sur­
Discussion
rounded by faith- and religion-based values and beliefs. We
The current study aimed to address the issue of self-perceived would argue that the latter is likely to affect a person’s moral
impact of pornography use on personal sex life in a large-scale perspective considerably stronger than reasons underlying the
national study of health and sexuality in Germany. Attempting person’s current religious attendance – which may reflect social
to understand characteristics underlying different evaluations, conformism, the importance of maintaining personal network
we focused on the theoretical model of MI that links religious ties, and/or the need for social support. Acknowledging that
values and beliefs to a negative evaluation of personal porno­ the evidence we obtained is only suggestive of the proposed
graphy use. Considering that this association has predomi­ primacy of the exposure to religious moral notions and beliefs
nantly been explored in studies carried out in the U.S.A. during childhood and, likely, early adolescence, as well as
(Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019), we revisited potentially culture-specific, we nonetheless find the insight
the relation using data from an overall less religious and more important for working with individuals experiencing poten­
sexually permissive European society. The findings of our tially negative effects of pornography use on their sexual life. It
multivariable regression analyses can be summarized in the is our hope that this proposition will be further tested in the
following points. First, we found consistent evidence of the future.
role of MI in reporting negative impact of pornography use on The substantial gender difference found in the current
personal sex life. Second, compared to participants who study – compared to male participants, women were charac­
reported no impact of pornography use on personal sex life, terized by substantially higher odds of reporting positive and
women were characterized by substantially higher odds of lower odds of reporting mixed/negative impact, relative to no
reporting positive impact, while men had higher odds of impact – is at odds with the results of the 2008 Danish study.
reporting mixed and negative impact. Third, sexual minority Hald and Malamuth (2008) found reports of positive self-
identity was substantially linked to both positive and mixed/ perceived effects of pornography use on personal sex life
negative impact. Fourth, the frequency of pornography use more prevalent among male, compared to female, participants.
distinguished those who reported positive impact from the no In contrast, in the Indonesian study, female students reported
impact group in the same way that it distinguished between a larger difference between positive and negative self-perceived
participants characterized by mixed/negative impact and those effects than their male peers (Wijaya Mulya & Hald, 2014).
who reported no impact. Fifth, we observed a robust and Taking into account that the gender difference in the current
negative role of college/university education in reporting study was observed with the frequency of pornography use
mixed/negative impact. Finally, lower (not higher) frequency controlled for, one possible explanation would be the media
of pornography use, moral issues with pornography, and reli­ accounts about addiction to pornography and pornography-
gious upbringing were linked to perceiving pornography hav­ induced sexual dysfunctions, which have been almost invari­
ing a negative effect on personal sex life, compared to mixed ably focused on men. It has been suggested that such dramatic
impact of pornography use. This central finding, which we later narratives can influence some men to self-label as pornography
discuss in more detail, may appear counter-intuitive: indivi­ addicts (Cantor et al., 2013; Ley et al., 2014), as well as influence
duals who linked their pornography use to adverse sexual out­ the views of mental health professionals (Klein et al., 2019).
comes reported substantially less frequent use than participants We also observed that a sexual minority identity, in
who reported a positive role of pornography. However, the comparison to exclusively heterosexual identity, was linked
finding is in line with the findings that the majority of users to both positive and mixed/negative impact, relative to no
do not find their pornography use harmful (Hald & Malamuth, impact of pornography use on personal sex life. In the
2008) and that in some cases it may contribute to higher present study, larger effect sizes were associated with report­
personal (Štulhofer et al., 2010) and dyadic sexual satisfaction ing mixed/negative impact, which is at odds with the Hald
(Vaillancourt-Morel et al., 2019), as well as with the recently et al. (2013) findings that only 3% of MSM in a large-scale
proposed MI model (Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019). North American survey reported any negative effects. Self-
Similarly to what was reported in a number of studies perceived positive effects are likely linked to the educational
carried out in the North American context (Grubbs et al., and identity-supporting role of pornography, as indicated in
2014; Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs, Perry et al., 2019; recent qualitative studies (Harvey, 2020; Nelson et al., 2014).
Grubbs et al., 2018), MI also had a significant role in our The reporting of mixed and negative impact points to
national sample from Germany – a country characterized by a diversity of experiences with pornography among sexual
relatively low religiosity levels. Although we did not directly minority individuals, with some of them likely affected by
assess MI (i.e., the GeSiD questionnaire did not include an unrealistic expectations promoted by pornographic material
operationalization of this psychosocial mechanism), the cur­ (Harvey, 2020) or by genital and body appearance compar­
rent study suggests a novel analytical contribution to the con­ isons with actors (Whitfield et al., 2018) that generally seem
ceptual model, which will require further assessment to to affect men more than women (Cranney, 2015).
confirm (or refute). Our findings confirm the notion that In line with previous findings (Hald & Malamuth, 2008;
religiosity, broadly speaking, increases the risk of reporting Kvalem et al., 2014), the frequency of pornography use was
(self-perceived) negative effects of pornography use on higher in participants who reported positive impact than those
8 A. ŠTULHOFER ET AL.

who reported no impact of their pornography use. participant characteristics, the key indicators, and analytical
Interestingly, the same was observed for those who reported approaches, all three studies observed the effect of MI4 –
a mixed/negative impact, relative to no impact. The novel regardless of the fact that the U.S.A. and Poland are highly
insight provided by the current study is that the frequency of religious countries, unlike Germany. A task for future studies is
pornography use was higher among men and women who to enable comparative assessments of the links between MI, on
reported mixed (i.e., both positive and negative) impact than the one hand, and faith and religiosity intensity levels, different
in participants who reported negative impact. Considering that types of faith- and religion-based beliefs, and specific religious
it does not align with what should be expected based on the denominations, on the other hand. Such evidence might be of
pornography as a public health hazard thesis, the observation substantial use in clinical settings.
has policy ramifications (Nelson & Rothman, 2020). However,
potential implications of the finding need to be weighed against
the fact that we did not assess specific content of pornographic Clinical Implications
material used, which may constitute an additional difference The results of our study also indicate that MI is an important
between some of the groups explored in the current study. concept both for clinical assessment, as well as for counseling
Finally, we should also note that tertiary education was and therapy. Keeping in mind that we did not address proble­
consistently related to increased odds of reporting mixed/nega­ matic, compulsive, or dysregulated pornography use, possible
tive impact of pornography use on personal sex life, relative to distress linked to self-evaluation of pornography use and its
no impact. Considering that the current study did not include perceived impact on personal sex life should be clinically
any indicators that can assist in pinpointing a mechanism approached in a differentiated way. If the experience of distress
underlying this relationship, at this point we can only speculate is mainly caused by an anti-sexual or, more specifically, an
that the association reflects a higher likelihood of encountering anti-pornography moral attitude, compulsive sexual behavior
narratives about problematic use of pornography, pornography disorder should not be diagnosed. In such cases, sex-positive
addiction, and sexual health problems allegedly related to the counseling with the aim of reducing MI should be offered.
consumption of sexually explicit material. It may also be that What is interesting and new about our study is that MI appears
more educated people are more easily disturbed, compared to to be better predicted by religious upbringing and deeply
their less educated peers, by sexually explicit material depicting rooted religious attitudes and beliefs (which may or may not
sexual acts they find aggressive or degrading – regardless of be very explicit) than by current religious practice. Thus, it may
whether they encountered it unintentionally or were searching make sense to focus on such attitudes in psychotherapy, but
for it. Future studies, particularly qualitative ones, should not in the context of sexual addiction or dysregulated sexuality
address these plausible mechanisms. interventions. It should also be noted that some therapeutic
Overall, what can be concluded about self-perceived effects approaches, either individual- or couple-focused, might even
of pornography use on sex life and as the role of MI as the basis increase MI by reinforcing moralistic, sex-negative attitudes.
of existing cross-cultural evidence? Across post-industrial Instead, a sex-positive approach focused on resolving interna­
Western societies, there is consistent quantitative and qualita­ lized sociocultural conflicts (Coleman et al., 2018), which uti­
tive evidence that most people perceive their pornography use lizes techniques that bring balance between sexual inhibition
as having no negative effects on personal sex life (Hald & and excitation and assists in improving the client’s understand­
Malamuth, 2008; Miller et al., 2018; Löfgren-Mårtenson & ing of his/her sexuality, is more promising (Briken, 2020).
Månsson, 2010; McKee, 2007). What is less clear is whether
they believe that their pornography use has no effect or
Study Limitations and Strengths
a positive effect. In contrast, the finding that only a small
minority of participants report negative effects of pornography Although pornography use can affect both personal and rela­
is consistent in the literature. A similar finding was reported in tional (dyadic) sex life, the current study focused exclusively on
a non-Western country, but the study included only college a particular aspect of the former phenomenon. In addition to
students, who are likely the least conservative segment of the standard limitations related to the use of self-reported mea­
national population (Wijaya Mulya & Hald, 2014). The intent sures, which is unavoidable in research on sensitive topics such
to which such findings, as noted by Hald and Malamuth as sexual behavior, our outcome was operationalized as
(2008), may be biased by an ingrained optimism or the ten­ a single-item indicator. This precluded an analysis of its metric
dency to describe personal happenings in more positive terms characteristics. However, the indicator, which was also used in
than (objectively) deserved, remains unknown. Taking into a recent Norwegian sexuality and health national study
account that the majority of participants in the current study (Koletić, et al., 2021), appears to have solid face validity.
reported no impact, this bias appears to be of limited concern Although Hald proposed a more complex operationalization
here. of self-perceived effects of personal pornography use (i.e., the
In the case of MI, the evidence collected in the U.S.A., Pornography Consumption Effects Scale; Miller et al., 2019;
Poland, and Germany supports the conceptual model in Hald & Malamuth, 2008; Hald et al., 2013), their approach is
which personal faith and religiosity contribute to self- not feasible for large-scale national sex studies, which require
perceived negative effects of pornography use independently relatively brief measures to enable the assessment of a wide
from or even contrary to frequent use of sexually explicit range of topics of interest. Similarly, following Grubbs, Kraus
material. Although a direct comparison of effect sizes is not
4
feasible due to differences in sampling strategy, basic The term “effect” does not refer to any causal relationship.
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 9

et al. (2019), we used a single-item indicator of the use of problematic. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 17(4), 793–811. https://
pornography being morally reprehensible to the user. To doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.01.007
reduce this limitation, we also assessed MI indirectly by explor­ Briken, P. (2020). An integrated model to assess and treat compulsive
sexual behaviour disorder. Nature Reviews. Urology, 17(7), 391–406.
ing links between a single-item religiosity measure (i.e., the https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/s41585-020-0343-7
frequency of attending religious ceremonies) and a single-item Briken, P., Habermann, N., Berner, W., & Hill, A. (2007). Diagnosis and
measure of religious family socialization, on the one hand, and treatment of sexual addiction: A survey among German sex therapists.
self-perceived problems associated with one’s pornography Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 14(2), 131–143. https://doi.org/10.
use, on the other hand. To the best of our knowledge, no 1080/10720160701310450
Cantor, J. M., Klein, C., Lykins, A., Rullo, J. E., Thaler, L., & Walling, B. R.
validated multi-item measure of MI has been introduced in (2013). A treatment-oriented typology of self-identified hypersexuality
the literature – which should be prioritized in future research referrals. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(5), 883–893. https://doi.org/
in this area. Finally, the fact that we did not assess preferred 10.1007/s10508-013-0085-1
pornographic content (i.e, “genre”; (Hald et al., 2018), Coleman, E., Dickenson, J. A., Girard, A., Rider, G. N., Candelario-Pérez,
a possible confounder of identified differences between mixed L. E., Becker-Warner, R., Kovic, A. G., & Munns, R. (2018). An
integrative biopsychosocial and sex positive model of understanding
and negative impact groups, is a limitation to be avoided in and treatment of impulsive/compulsive sexual behavior. Sexual
future studies. Addiction and Compulsivity, 25(2–3), 125–152. https://doi.org/10.
Among the current study’s strengths, such as probability- 1080/10720162.2018.1515050
based sampling, design-adjusted statistical analysis, and statis­ Cranney, S. (2015). Internet pornography use and sexual body image in
tical power, we should also underscore the use of multiple a Dutch sample. International Journal of Sexual Health, 27(3), 316–323.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19317611.2014.999967
indicators of religiosity. Each of these indicators tapped into Fisher, W. A., Montgomery-Graham, S., & Kohut, T. (2019). Pornography
a different facet (upbringing, current practice, and moral eva­ problems due to moral incongruence. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48
luation) of the construct. (2), 425–429. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1291-7
Grubbs, J. B., Hook, J. P., Griffin, B. J., Cushman, M. S., Hook, J. N., &
Penberthy, J. K. (2017). Treating hypersexuality. In Z. D. Peterson (Ed.),
The Wiley handbook of sex therapy (pp. 115–128). Wiley Blackwell.
Conclusions Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Hook, J. N., & Carlisle, R. D.
In this large-scale national probability-based sample of adult (2014). Transgression as addiction: Religiosity and moral disapproval
as predictors of perceived addiction to pornography. Archives of Sexual
Germans we addressed self-perceived impact of pornography Behavior, 44, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0257-z
use on personal sex life and the role of MI – the concept that Grubbs, J. B., Kraus, S. W., & Perry, S. L. (2019). Self-reported addiction to
was recently proposed to explain why some individuals, mostly pornography in a nationally representative sample: The roles of use
men, report problematic pornography use and adverse mental habits, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Journal of Behavioral
health outcomes despite relatively moderate levels of pornogra­ Addictions, 8(1), 88–93. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.134
Grubbs, J. B., Kraus, S. W., Perry, S. L., Lewczuk, K., & Gola, M. (2020).
phy use. Two key findings, which may be specific to the more Moral incongruence and compulsive sexual behavior: Results from
sexually permissive and less religious European context, were cross-sectional interactions and parallel growth curve analyses.
obtained. First, the majority of surveyed participants reported Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(3), 266–278. https://doi.org/10.
that pornography did not impact their sex life. Second, we 1037/abn0000501
observed a significant relationship between religious upbringing Grubbs, J. B., Lee, B. N., Hoagland, K. C., Kraus, S. W., & Perry, S. L.
(2020). Addiction or transgression? Moral incongruence and
and self-perceived negative impact of pornography use, as pre­ self-reported problematic pornography use in a nationally representa­
dicted by the MI conceptual model. Both findings are relevant tive sample. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(5), 936–946. https://doi.
for sexuality and relationship therapy. org/10.1177/2167702620922966
Grubbs, J. B., & Perry, S. L. (2018). Moral incongruence and pornography
use: A critical review and integration. Journal of Sex Research, 56(1),
29–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2018.1427204
Funding
Grubbs, J. B., Perry, S. L., Wilt, J. A., & Reid, R. C. (2019). Pornography
The study was funded by the German Federal Centre for Health Education problems due to moral incongruence: An integrative model with
[Grant numbers: Z2/25.5.2.1/18; Z2/25.5.2.1/20]. a systematic review and meta-analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48
(2), 397–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1248-x
Grubbs, J. B., Wilt, J. A., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., & Kraus, S. W.
(2018). Moral disapproval and perceived addiction to internet porno­
ORCID graphy: A longitudinal examination. Addiction, 113(3), 496–506.
https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14007
Aleksandar Štulhofer http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5138-3644 Grubbs, J. B., Wright, P. J., Braden, A. L., Wilt, J. A., & Kraus, S. W. (2019).
Christian Wiessner http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7943-9881 Internet pornography use and sexual motivation: A systematic review
Goran Koletić http://orcid.org/0000-0003-0976-1685 and integration. Annals of the International Communication
Peer Briken http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1360-014X Association, 43(2), 117–155. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2019.
1584045
References Hald, G. M., & Malamuth, N. M. (2008). Self-perceived effects of porno­
graphy consumption. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(4), 614–625.
Bőthe, B., Tóth-Király, I., Potenza, M. N., Griffiths, M. D., Orosz, G., & https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9212-1
Demetrovics, Z. (2019). Revisiting the role of impulsivity and compul­ Hald, G. M., Smolenski, D., & Rosser, B. R. S. (2013). Perceived effects of
sivity in problematic sexual behaviors. Journal of Sex Research, 56(2), sexually explicit media among men who have sex with men and psy­
166–179. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2018.1480744 chometric properties of the Pornography Consumption Effects Scale
Bőthe, B., Tóth-Király, I., Potenza, M. N., Orosz, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (PCES). The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 10(3), 757–767. https://doi.
(2020). High-frequency pornography use may not always be org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02988.x
10 A. ŠTULHOFER ET AL.

Hald, G. M., Štulhofer, A., & Lange, T. (2018). Sexual arousal and sexually Luijkx, R., Halman, L., Sieben, I., Brislinger, E., & Quandt, M. (2016).
explicit media (SEM): Comparing patterns of sexual arousal to SEM European values in numbers(Vol. 16). Brill. https://doi.org/10.1163/
and sexual self-evaluations and satisfaction across gender and sexual 9789004328525
orientation. Sexual Medicine, 6(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.esxm. Lykke, L. C., & Cohen, P. N. (2015). Widening gender gap in opposition to
2017.11.001 pornography, 1975–2012. Social Currents, 2(4), 307–323. https://doi.
Harvey, P. (2020). Let’s talk about porn: The perceived effect of online org/10.1177/2329496515604170
mainstream pornography on LGBTQ youth. In D. N. Ferris, Matthiesen, S., Dekker, A., & Briken, P. (2018). Pilot study on adult
D. R. Compton, & A. P. Herrera (Eds.), Gender,sexuality and race in sexuality in Germany - First results on feasibility and comparison of
the digital age (pp. 31–52). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978- methods. Zeitschrift Fur Sexualforschung, 31(3), 218–236. https://doi.
3-030-29855-5_3 org/10.1055/a-0663-4354
Hesse, C., & Pedersen, C. L. (2017). Porn sex versus real sex: How sexually McCormack, M., & Wignall, L. (2017). Enjoyment, exploration and edu­
explicit material shapes our understanding of sexual anatomy, physiol­ cation: Understanding the consumption of pornography among young
ogy, and behaviour. Sexuality & Culture, 21(3), 754–775. https://doi. men with non-exclusive sexual orientations. Sociology, 51(5), 975–991.
org/10.1007/s12119-017-9413-2 https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038516629909
Klein, V., Briken, P., Schröder, J., & Fuss, J. (2019). Mental health profes­ McKay, K., Poulin, C., & Muñoz-Laboy, M. (2021). Claiming public health
sionals’ pathologization of compulsive sexual behavior: Does clients’ crisis to regulate sexual outlets: A critique of the state of Utah’s declara­
gender and sexual orientation matter? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, tion on pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 50(2), 401–405.
128(5), 465–472. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000437 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-020-01884-8
Kohut, T., Fisher, W. A., & Campbell, L. (2017). Perceived effects of McKee, A. (2007). Positive and negative effects of pornography as attributed
pornography on the couple relationship: Initial findings of open-ended, by consumers. Australian Journal of Communication, 34(1), 87–104.
participant-informed, “bottom-up” research. Archives of Sexual https://eprints.qut.edu.au/14575/
Behavior, 46(2), 585–602. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0783-6 Miller, D. J., Hald, G. M., & Kidd, G. (2018). Self-perceived effects of
Kohut, T., & Štulhofer, A. (2018). Is pornography use a risk for adolescent pornography consumption among heterosexual men. Psychology of Men
wellbeing? An examination of temporal relationships in two indepen­ & Masculinity, 19(3), 469–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000112
dent panel samples. Plos One, 13(8), e0202048. https://doi.org/10.1371/ Miller, D. J., Kidd, G., & Hald, G. M. (2019). Measuring self-perceived
journal.pone.0202048 effects of pornography: A short-form version of the Pornography
Koletić, G., Štulhofer, A., Hald, G.M. & Træen, B. (2021). Self-Assessed Consumption Effects Scale. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48(3),
effects of pornography use on personal sex life: Results from a large- 753–761. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1327-z
scale study of norwegian adults. International Journal of Sexual Health. Nelson, K. M., Leickly, E., Yang, J. P., Pereira, A., & Simoni, J. M. (2014).
Kor, A., Zilcha-Mano, S., Fogel, Y. A., Mikulincer, M., Reid, R. C., & The influence of sexually explicit online media on sex: Do men who
Potenza, M. N. (2014). Psychometric development of the Problematic have sex with men believe they “do what they see”? AIDS Care, 26(7),
Pornography Use Scale. Addictive Behaviors, 39(5), 861–868. https:// 931–934. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540121.2013.871219
doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.027 Nelson, K. M., & Rothman, E. F. (2020). Should public health professionals
Kraus, S. W., Krueger, R. B., Briken, P., First, M. B., Stein, D. J., Kaplan, M. S., consider pornography a public health crisis? American Journal of Public
Voon, V., Abdo, C. H. N., Grant, J. E., Atalla, E., & Reed, G. M. (2018). Health, 110(2), 151–153. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2019.305498
Compulsive sexual behaviour disorder in the ICD-11. World Psychiatry, 17 Owens, E. W., Behun, R. J., Manning, J. C., & Reid, R. C. (2012). The
(1), 108–109. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20499 impact of internet pornography on adolescents: A review of the
Kraus, S. W., & Sweeney, P. J. (2019). Hitting the target: Considerations research. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 19(1–2), 99–122. https://
for differential diagnosis when treating individuals for problematic use doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2012.660431
of pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48(2), 431–435. https:// Perry, S. L. (2017). Does viewing pornography diminish religiosity over
doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1301-9 time? Evidence from two-wave panel data. Journal of Sex Research, 54
Kraus, S. W., Voon, V., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Should compulsive (2), 214–226. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1146203
sexual behavior be considered an addiction? Addiction. 11(12), 2097– Perry, S. L. (2018a). Not practicing what you preach: Religion and incon­
2106. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13297 gruence between pornography beliefs and usage. Journal of Sex Research,
Kvalem, I. L., Træen, B., Lewin, B., & Štulhofer, A. (2014). Self-perceived 55(3), 369–380. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2017.1333569
effects of Internet pornography use, genital appearance satisfaction, Perry, S. L. (2018b). Pornography use and depressive symptoms:
and sexual self-esteem among young Scandinavian adults. Examining the role of moral incongruence. Society and Mental
Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 8(4), Health, 8(3), 195–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/2156869317728373
Article 4. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2014-4-4 Perry, S. L., & Whitehead, A. L. (2018). Only bad for believers? Religion,
Landripet, I., Buško, V., & Štulhofer, A. (2019). Testing the content pornography use, and sexual satisfaction among American men. Journal
progression thesis: A longitudinal assessment of pornography use and of Sex Research, 56(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2017.
preference for coercive and violent content among male adolescents. 1423017
Social Science Research, 81, 32–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch. Peter, J., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2016). Adolescents and pornography:
2019.03.003 A review of 20 years of research. Journal of Sex Research, 53(4–5),
Lewczuk, K., Glica, A., Nowakowska, I., Gola, M., & Grubbs, J. B. (2020). 509–531. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1143441
Evaluating pornography problems due to moral incongruence model. Ross, M. W., Månsson, S.-A.-A., & Daneback, K. (2012). Prevalence,
Journal of Sexual Medicine, 17(2), 300–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. severity, and correlates of problematic sexual internet use in Swedish
jsxm.2019.11.259 men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(2), 459–466. https://
Ley, D., Prause, N., & Finn, P. (2014). The emperor has no clothes: A review of doi.org/10.1007/s10508-011-9762-0
the ‘pornography addiction’ model. Current Sexual Health Reports, 6(2), Seigfried-Spellar, K. C., & Rogers, M. K. (2013). Does deviant pornogra­
94–105. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11930-014-0016-8 phy use follow a Guttman-like progression? Computers in Human
Ley, D. J. (2018). The pseudoscience behind public health crisis legislation. Porn Behavior, 29(5), 1997–2003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.04.018
Studies, 5(2), 208–212. https://doi.org/10.1080/23268743.2018.1435400 Stadtmüller, S., Silber, H., Daikeler, J., Martin, S., Sand, M., Schmich, P.,
Löfgren-Mårtenson, L., & Månsson, S.-A. (2010). Lust, love, and life: Schröder, J., Struminskaya, B., Weyandt, K. W., & Zabal, A. (2019).
A qualitative study of Swedish adolescents’ perceptions and experiences Adaptation of the AAPOR final disposition codes for the German survey
with pornography. Journal of Sex Research, 47(6), 568–579. https://doi. context. GESIS – Leibniz-Institute for the Social Sciences. https://doi.
org/10.1080/00224490903151374 org/10.15465/gesis-sg_en_026
THE JOURNAL OF SEX RESEARCH 11

Štulhofer, A., Buško, V., & Landripet, I. (2010). Pornography, sexual Webber, V., & Sullivan, R. (2018). Constructing a crisis: Porn panics and
socialization, and satisfaction among young men. Archives of Sexual public health. Porn Studies, 5(2), 192–196. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Behavior, 39(1), 168–178. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9387-0 23268743.2018.1434110
Štulhofer, A., Jurin, T., & Briken, P. (2016). Is high sexual desire a facet of Whitfield, T. H. F., Rendina, H. J., Grov, C., & Parsons, J. T. (2018).
male hypersexuality? Results from an online study. Journal of Sex & Sexually explicit media and condomless anal sex among gay and bisex­
Marital Therapy, 42(8), 665–680. https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X. ual men. AIDS and Behavior, 22(2), 681–689. https://doi.org/10.1007/
2015.1113585 s10461-017-1952-x
Vaillancourt-Morel, M.-P., & Bergeron, S. (2019). Self-perceived proble­ Wijaya Mulya, T., & Hald, G. M. (2014). Self-perceived effects of pornography
matic pornography use: Beyond individual differences and religiosity. consumption in a sample of Indonesian university students. Media
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48(2), 437–441. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Psychology, 17(1), 78–101. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2013.850038
s10508-018-1292-6 Willoughby, B. J. (2019). Stuck in the porn box. Archives of Sexual
Vaillancourt-Morel, M.-P., Daspe, M.-È., Charbonneau-Lefebvre, V., Behavior, 48(2), 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1294-4
Bosisio, M., & Bergeron, S. (2019). Pornography use in adult mixed-sex World Health Organization. (2018). International classification of diseases
romantic relationships: Context and correlates. Current Sexual Health for mortality and morbidity statistics (11th ed.). https://icd.who.int/
Reports, 11(1), 35–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11930-019-00190-2 browse11/l-m/en

You might also like