Miles A (2014) Addressing The Problem of Cultural Anchoring

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Social Psychology

Quarterly http://spq.sagepub.com/

Addressing the Problem of Cultural Anchoring: An Identity-Based Model of Culture


in Action
Andrew Miles
Social Psychology Quarterly 2014 77: 210 originally published online 23 April 2014
DOI: 10.1177/0190272514524062

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://spq.sagepub.com/content/77/2/210

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

American Sociological Association

Additional services and information for Social Psychology Quarterly can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://spq.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://spq.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - May 28, 2014

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Apr 23, 2014

What is This?

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Social Psychology Quarterly
2014, Vol. 77(2) 210–227
Addressing the Problem of Ó American Sociological Association 2014
DOI: 10.1177/0190272514524062
Cultural Anchoring: An http://spq.sagepub.com

Identity-Based Model of
Culture in Action

Andrew Miles1

Abstract
One of the primary challenges in the study of culture and action is determining which cultural
elements ‘‘anchor’’ (or influence) other elements. Notable attempts to address this problem
include tool kit theory and dual-process models of culture and action. Insightful as these
efforts are, they do not adequately explain action that crosscuts contexts, in part because
they do not detail the specific types of cultural content that matter. This article draws on iden-
tity theory from sociological social psychology and a variety of perspectives from cultural soci-
ology to propose an identity-based model of culture in action that begins to address these
shortcomings. Past research supports the theory’s major claims, although additional work
is required to fully verify the proposed sequence of situated action.

Keywords
identity theory, tool kit theory, action, theory, culture

This article is a synthetic effort to address the cultural content involved unspecified.
a longstanding problem in the sociology of This omission makes it difficult to under-
culture, which Swidler (2001:206) suc- stand cross-context consistencies in
cinctly outlined in her book, Talk of action, in part because such consistencies
Love: ‘‘[T]he biggest unanswered question rely on activation of the same cultural ele-
in the sociology of culture is whether and ments across situations.
how some cultural elements control, I argue that identities are a type of cul-
anchor, or organize others.’’ More simply, tural content that provides traction on the
which cultural elements influence others, problem of cultural anchoring. I propose
either directly or indirectly? With regard an identity-based model of action that
to action, several treatments of this ques-
tion have yielded important insights, 1
Department of Sociology, Duke University,
including the importance of both cultural
Durham, NC, USA
competencies (i.e., knowledge and skills)
Corresponding Author:
and motivations for behavior (Swidler
Andrew Miles, Department of Sociology, Duke
1986; Vaisey 2009). However, these University, Box 90088, 417 Chapel Drive, Durham,
approaches often focus on general mecha- NC 27708, USA.
nisms of cultural influence while leaving Email: andrew.miles@duke.edu

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 211

combines elements from multiple perspec- (see also Bourdieu 1990). At its most basic
tives including ‘‘tool kit’’ theory, work on level, this theory suggests that people
cultural capital, and identity theory (a must act using the cultural competencies
term I use to encompass both the struc- that they already have (Lizardo and
tural work of Stryker and his colleagues Strand 2010; Swidler 2001)—a person
as well as the control model associated cannot become a professional basketball
with Burke and Stets; Bourdieu 1990; player if he has never played the game,
Burke and Stets 2009; Stryker 2008; Swi- nor can a person succeed in academia if
dler 2008). Given the vast amount of she has not learned to speak the intellec-
research these traditions represent, my tual lingo. Swidler argued that people like
synthesis is intentionally selective, aimed to feel competent and tend to seek out
at using the strengths of each to address activities for which they already have
the problem of cultural anchoring. I begin the cultural equipment; after all, using
by discussing prior attempts at addressing the skills one already has is easy and effi-
the anchoring problem and how they fail cient (Swidler 1986, 2008). A tool kit per-
to provide an adequate answer to the spective also implies that cultural knowl-
question of cross-context consistencies in edge is not systematically organized at
action, and then outline the theoretical the individual level and therefore cannot
claims on which my model is built. I then provide coherent reasons for action (see
describe how the model unfolds in concrete also Geertz 1973). Instead, action
contexts and illustrate the plausibility of depends on external structures (e.g., situ-
the model by examining prior empirical ational cues) that activate the cultural
evidence for its claims. competencies appropriate to particular
contexts. This creates consistency of
PAST TREATMENTS OF CULTURAL action in the absence of a coherent cul-
ANCHORING tural model at the micro level (Lizardo
and Strand 2010).
The problem of cultural anchoring—that The tenets of tool kit theory are elegant
is, the issue of which cultural elements in their simplicity, but as Swidler (2008)
control others—is essentially a question noted, they do not fully address the prob-
of causal priority, and different scholars lem of cultural anchoring. The claim that
have proposed different answers to it. At people use cultural ‘‘tools’’ to construct
midcentury, functionalists argued that lines of action suggests how action is cre-
internalized culture in the form of goals ated without specifying why action is
and values motivates action (e.g., Parsons undertaken in the first place. That is,
and Shils 1951), but this perspective came tool kit theory focuses on the cultural
under heavy fire and soon fell to one that means of action without much exploration
emphasized external culture as embodied of the motivations—cultural or other-
in social structures (e.g., Blake and Davis wise—that move actors toward one end
1964; Wrong 1961). or another (Vaisey 2010). Emphasizing
In a 1986 paper, Swidler offered a third the power of external structures to cue
solution, one that blended the internal action provides a partial solution, but
and external elements of culture and has accounting for action that crosscuts con-
come to be known as ‘‘tool kit’’ theory. texts becomes problematic since people
Swidler argued that people have internal- without contexts are generally not seen
ized a wide variety of culturally acquired as motivated actors.
knowledge and competencies that allow Dual-process models offer a different
them to navigate the social landscape solution to the problem of cultural

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


212 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

anchoring—one that gives more scope to detected between stored cultural content
individual motivations (Bourdieu 1990; and the demands of the situation. While
Gross 2009; Vaisey 2009). Although the this may seem to be a simple case of situa-
details of these models vary, all adopt tions cueing culture, in reality, detecting
some form of the argument that cognitive a match is heavily shaped by the charac-
processing occurs at two levels: one fast teristics of the cultural content that a per-
and effortless, the other slow and deliber- son has internalized. In part, this is
ate, and that behavior is primarily guided because content can affect how situations
by culture accessible to the fast system. are perceived (Burke and Stets 2009;
Such culture is automatically activated Mead 1934). Self-definitional values, for
in relevant situations, making it prior to instance, can direct information seeking
observable cultural expressions such as (Verplanken and Holland 2002, study 3),
talk or action. It can include the knowl- and subjectively important identities are
edge and skills highlighted in tool kit the- processed more quickly, guide recall
ory, but it can also encompass motivating more effectively, and are more resistant
constructs like attitudes, values, and to disconfirmation than unimportant
moral worldviews (Gross 2009; Lizardo identities (Markus 1977). This may be
and Strand 2010; Vaisey 2009; Vaisey because subjectively important constructs
and Lizardo 2010). Dual-process models are more cognitively accessible or likely to
thus address the question of cultural be activated from memory (Bizer and
anchoring by fleshing out the details of Krosnick 2001; Fabrigar, MacDonald,
how culture shapes action, but also by pro- and Wegener 2005; Schwartz 2010). Fur-
viding an explanation for why people act. thermore, the more often a construct is
Focusing on cognition allows dual- activated—whether automatically or
process models to attain a high level of through conscious effort—the more acces-
generality and a wide range of applicabil- sible it becomes (Berger and Mitchell
ity, but this comes at the cost of under- 1989; Glasman and Albarracı́n 2006). If
standing the role cultural content plays called up frequently enough, cultural con-
in behavior. By content, I mean specific, structs may grow chronically accessible,
culturally shaped constructs such as val- or nearly always active (Forster and Lib-
ues, attitudes, identities, particular skill erman 2007). In extreme cases, cross-con-
sets (e.g., conversational skills), and so text consistency in action might have lit-
on. Eliding content becomes problematic tle to do with matching internal culture
when trying to understand cross-context to the situation and be guided largely by
consistencies in action, which require us chronically accessible constructs. In
to know not just how or why people act, either case, by neglecting content, dual-
but also when. At first glance, dual- process models necessarily provide only
process models seem promising, for they part of the story and thereby limit explan-
identify an innate processing capacity ations of action.
that operates across situations. However, I argue that identities are a type of cul-
processes must always operate using con- tural content that advances understand-
crete content, and universal machinery ing of cross-context consistency in action.
does little to solve the problem if different Interestingly, Swidler herself proposed
content is activated in each situation. So identity as a possible solution to the prob-
what determines which contents are lem of cultural anchoring, but to my
activated? knowledge neither she nor anyone else
Lizardo and Strand (2010) suggest that within the culture and action tradition
activation occurs when a match is has pursued it (Swidler 2001, 2008).

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 213

Many other scholars in cultural sociology the term identity has taken on many
have discussed identity since Swidler shades of meaning, but most agree that
wrote, and a fair number have referenced identities are self-understandings, mean-
her work, but almost invariably they ings that people incorporate into their
focus on how culture is used to construct conceptions of who they are. Drawing on
identity and not why people go to the trou- the work of early thinkers like James,
ble in the first place (e.g., Tabatabai and Cooley, and Mead, symbolic interaction-
Linders 2011; Tavory and Goodman ists have argued that these self-meanings
2009). That is, they examine how ‘‘cul- are learned through interaction with
tural elements’’ such as cultural scripts others and often widely agreed upon—
and repertoires organize identities, but that is, they are cultural. Identity theorists
they do not adequately consider the possi- assume that people act to maintain identity
bility that identities might themselves be meanings (Burke and Stets 2009). Thus,
cultural elements that can ‘‘control, a mother will try to act in a way consistent
anchor, or organize’’ culture in turn (see with her understanding of how a mother
also Swidler 2001:206). should behave, a Baptist in a way consis-
In what follows, I present an identity- tent with his understandings of what it
based model of culture in action to deal means to be a Baptist, and so forth.
with the problem of cultural anchoring. Evidence suggests that identity pro-
This model draws on identity theory cesses operate as a cybernetic control sys-
from social psychology as well as insights tem. People seek consistency between
from the sociology of culture. It adds con- their identities and their actions, and
crete cultural content to the discussion when inconsistency arises, they alter
(identities) and offers a plausible set of their behavior to try and correct it (Burke
mechanisms for understanding both situ- and Stets 2009; Robinson 2007; Swann,
ationally dependent and cross-contextual Pelham, and Krull 1989). At the most
forms of action. Despite these advantages, basic level this means that when people
I do not pretend that this model is a com- enter situations they enact behaviors
plete description of the connections that are consistent with their identities
between culture and action, nor that it (e.g., professors profess, dentists drill
fully encapsulates past work; my objec- teeth). However, if perceived feedback
tive is more modest. Rather than present from the situation does not confirm their
a comprehensive theory, I aim to inte- identities, they will adjust their behaviors
grate the strengths of multiple research in an attempt to bring situational mean-
traditions to lay out an initial model, ings back in line with the meanings held
without attempting to harmonize all the in their identities. For instance, an engi-
details of the various paradigms on which neer whose project fails might seek out
I draw. another project where she can prove her
technical prowess, and a gang member
COMPONENTS OF THE MODEL who is seen as ‘‘soft’’ might commit acts
of violence to repair his reputation. To
People Have Identities and Try to relate this to the problem of cultural
Behave in Identity-Consistent Ways anchoring, the fact that people act to
Scholars across disciplines have argued verify their identities indicates that they
that identities are important predictors will apply their cultural skills and reper-
of action (Akerlof and Kranton 2000; toires in ways that are consistent with
Hogg, Terry, and White 1995; Stryker their identities, suggesting that self-
1980). Given its use in so many domains, understandings are a motivational force

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


214 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

that organizes other cultural elements, as content also depends on characteristics


Swidler hinted. It also is consistent with of the construct in question, particularly
habitus-based theories that view culture those characteristics that make the con-
as internally organized (in this case into struct more cognitively accessible. In
identities) and motivating (Bourdieu identity theory, the probability that an
1990; Lizardo and Strand 2010; Vaisey identity will be invoked in a given situa-
2009). tion is a function of its salience, which is
defined as ‘‘the readiness to act out an
identity as a consequence of the identity’s
Situations and Identities Interact to properties as a cognitive structure or
Activate Identities schema’’ (Stryker and Serpe 1994:17).
Similar to dual-process models, most (if The fact that salience is a ‘‘readiness’’ to
not all) theorists assume that identities act out an identity suggests some distance
are activated when situational cues with actual behavior—that is, an identity
match meanings held by the actor.1 For can be called up but ultimately fail to
instance, a woman’s doctor identity is have a noticeable influence on action.2
likely to become active when entering Salience, then, is a measure of whether
the hospital where she works, while her an identity is likely to be activated or, in
mother identity will probably be activated other words, cognitively accessible. Iden-
by seeing her son. Whether or not an iden- tity theorists assume that activation can
tity is seen as appropriate to a context occur either automatically or through
(and thus likely to be cued by it) depends conscious effort (Burke and Stets 2009),
on the wider culture in which it is embed- but the interplay of identities, identity
ded. For example, while the rise of the salience, and dual processes has yet to
Religious Right made religious identity receive sustained theoretical or empirical
a viable locus of political action in the attention. Consequently, I do not explic-
United States, for the Yoruba of Nigeria itly incorporate dual-process cognition
religion remained nonpoliticized (Laitin into the model, although I discuss possi-
1986). Still, cultural definitions of con- ble connections in the conclusion.
texts are generally loose enough that Identities are arranged in a salience
they can accommodate many different hierarchy, with the most salient identities
identities—a person can be black, at the top. As with any accessible con-
a woman, and a doctor simultaneously. struct, identities that are higher in the
As I argued previously, detecting salience hierarchy are more likely to be
a match between situations and cultural activated across contexts. In Stryker and
Serpe’s (1994:18) terms, identity salience
1
Symbolic interactionists note that this is ‘‘transsituational’’ and acts as a ‘‘‘per-
‘‘match’’ is a culturally mediated process. Identi- sonality’ variable carried by persons as
ties are cognitive schema that shape how stimuli
they move across situations and respond
from the environment are interpreted and there-
fore what types of matches are likely to be in particular situations.’’ Serpe and
detected (Burke and Stets 2009; Stryker 2008). Stryker (1987), for instance, found that
For clarity in laying out the argument I do not
2
delve into these details, but rather I assume This is an important distinction and allows
that in most circumstances actors share basic identity salience to be conceptualized and mea-
meanings regarding which identities are appro- sured separately from behavior. Identity theo-
priate to different (often institutionalized) set- rists thereby avoid the criticism that they are
tings and the primary behavioral expectations using the same data points (i.e., observed behav-
associated with them. In this way, it makes sense ior) as a measure of both their predictor and their
to talk of ‘‘situational cues’’ generically. outcome.

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 215

students moving to college generally of shared (or at least compatible) mean-


reconstructed social relationships in ings, giving individuals a consistent,
ways that maintained salient prior identi- cross-situational sense of self. I will call
ties. This suggests that those identities this sense of self the core self (cf. MacKin-
remained active even after transitioning non and Heise 2010). That is, the core self
to the college environment. is composed of a person’s most salient
The fact that both situations and identities. Because people occupy differ-
salience influence identity activation sug- ent social positions and have different
gests that people often have multiple life experiences, the identities included
identities active at the same time. Multi- in the core self can vary from person to
ple active identities mean that individu- person (cf. Stryker and Serpe 1982;
als must try and verify several identities Stryker, Serpe, and Hunt 2005).
using just one sequence of behaviors. As The core self has important implica-
identity theorists have argued, this pro- tions for the activation of other, less
cess is facilitated to the extent that identi- salient identities. Situations often leave
ties share meanings and is hampered wide latitude in defining which identities
when identities differ (Burke and Stets are appropriate, so individuals can select
2009). For example, an identity as a ‘‘fol- from a number of identities to find those
lower’’ is likely much easier to verify that are most congruent with their core
simultaneously with being an ‘‘obedient self. In terms of cultural anchoring,
son’’ than is an identity as a ‘‘leader.’’ highly salient identities are cultural ele-
In the former case, both identities could ments that anchor how secondary, more
be verified through similar types of situationally bound identities are
behavior (e.g., obedience to authority deployed.3 The core self therefore offers
figures), while in the latter case identity- a straightforward answer to the question
confirming behaviors diverge (e.g., follow- of cross-context action: people have rela-
ing parental guidance vs. making your tively stable self-perceptions that they
own decisions). The need to verify multi- seek to verify in any situation.
ple identities means that when verifica-
tion conflicts arise, people attempt to
verify their most salient identities (Burke Resources Are Necessary to Verify
and Stets 2009). Identities
To this point, discussion has implicitly
Salient Identities Form a Core Self focused on determining how identities
That Directs and Controls the are selected for activation in behavior
Operation of Less Salient Identities 3
I am indebted to the affect control model of
Because identities are arranged in a hier- the self outlined by MacKinnon and Heise
archy, the most salient identities are at (2010) for suggesting this line of thinking. Their
the top. To my knowledge, the implica- model uses a ‘‘persona’’ (a composite sense of
self) as the key construct that organizes how
tions of this have not been spelled out identities are deployed. I do not use the persona
before, so I take a moment to do so here. model in this article because I am uncomfortable
Logically, hypersalient identities must with the strong separation between personas and
be frequently (if not chronically) accessi- identities that it implies. However, I suspect that
ble, thereby giving continuity to percep- the concepts of persona and salient identities
could be profitably synthesized to give a more
tion and behavior across contexts. complete description of the core self. For the
Because they are activated together, sake of clarity in my argument (and space), I do
these identities are composed primarily not pursue the matter here.

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


216 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

contexts. Here I interrogate the issue of resources to identity processes (but see
whether people have the capacity to enact Stets and Carter 2011; Tsushima and
and maintain identities. While in many Burke 1999).4 Here I am using cultural
cases identity enactment is unproblem- to refer to knowledge and skills that allow
atic, at other times it might be more diffi- a person to navigate social life (Abramson
cult, particularly when people encounter 2012; Swidler 1986). It is here that
novel situations. recourse to cultural sociology can enhance
Identity theorists have proposed the argument.
resources as a way of grappling with The foundational idea needed to link
this issue, and they have demonstrated cultural resources to identities was
that actors with greater resources are bet- expressed by Bourdieu. Bourdieu’s
ter able to confirm their identities. By insight was that culture—whether ideo-
resources, identity theorists mean ‘‘any- logical, material, or institutional—is actu-
thing that supports individuals and the ally a form of capital with real-world
interaction of individuals’’ (Burke and implications (1984, 1986). Possessing
Stets 2009:99). For instance, researchers ‘‘cultural capital’’ allows individuals to
have shown that those with higher sta- signal those in power that they are the
tus—measured as education, occupa- right sort of person and should therefore
tional prestige, income, and nonminority be rewarded (Lamont and Lareau 1988).
racial identification—enjoy higher levels These benefits often take the form of
of identity verification (Cast, Stets, and access to economically or socially advan-
Burke 1999; Stets and Cast 2007; Stets taged positions, or in the parlance of iden-
and Harrod 2004). Presumably, this is tity theories, particular social roles. Thus
because those with higher status are bet- cultural skills are a means of gaining
ter able to control situations in ways that acceptance by elites who act as gatekeep-
facilitate verification (see also Correll and ers for social positions, which are in turn
Ridgeway 2003). Identity verification is required to adopt certain identities
also greater for those who report higher (DiMaggio and Mohr 1985; Lamont
levels of personal or interpersonal resour- 1992; Rivera 2012). Even after passing
ces (e.g., self-efficacy, self-esteem, role- gatekeepers, identity access is uncertain
taking ability, trust, likability; Stets and anytime people act in the presence of
Cast 2007). Taken together, this work others, because each new interaction situ-
suggests the importance of material and ation provides others the opportunity to
psychosocial resources to identity-based accept or reject the identity performances
action. Although consistent with identity being offered. Cultural skills are there-
theory, scholars have paid less attention fore not only required to gain initial
to the crucial contributions of cultural access to identities, but also to continue
to access them in subsequent
4
Identity theorists have found that knowl- interactions.
edge-based resources are tied to the type of iden- Cultural capital is also necessary to
tity one has (Tsushima and Burke 1999) and that
knowledge and skill-based resources (task ability)
successfully enact identities. People can-
interact with how other identities are used (Stets not act in identity-consistent ways if
and Carter 2011). This article adds a model of they do not have the requisite knowledge
how cultural competencies allow a person to or skills to do so. Bourdieu uses the exam-
access and enact identities. The model is consis- ple of art to illustrate this point. Any per-
tent with identity theory’s umbrella definition of
resources but draws on work in cultural sociology
son with sufficient economic resources
to provide a more detailed examination of the pro- can purchase a painting, but money can-
cesses involved and their implications for action. not buy ‘‘the means of ‘consuming’

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 217

a painting,’’ for this requires certain cul- AN IDENTITY-BASED MODEL OF


tural knowledge and taste (Bourdieu CULTURE IN ACTION
1986:50). A person aspiring to the lofty
Taken together, insights from both iden-
positions of collector or art critic needs
tity theory and work on cultural sociology
to have internalized appropriate high-
lay the groundwork for an identity-based
culture reactions to different displays of
model of culture in action. This section out-
style and form; otherwise he will be
lines how the theoretical components pre-
unable to transcend ‘‘the ‘primary stra-
sented previously fit together to shape
tum of the meaning we can grasp on the
action in concrete contexts. The model is
basis of our ordinary experience’ to reach
summarized in Figure 1. I assume that peo-
the ‘stratum of secondary meanings’, i.e.,
ple act to maintain their most salient iden-
the ‘level of the meaning of what is signi-
tities, or in other words, their core selves.
fied’’’ (Bourdieu 1984:2).
These influence the activation of other,
Typically identities and skills go hand-
in-hand—both because identities and more situationally specific identities. Dis-
skills are often learned together and confirmation of identities generates a desire
because people tend to gravitate toward to restore self-relevant perceptions, with
roles for which they are already equipped preference given to those that are most
(Bourdieu 1990; Swidler 2008)—but this salient. This quest for self-confirmation
is not always true. Consider the success- guides people in how they deploy their cul-
ful blue-collar businessman who finds tural knowledge and skills. Consistent with
himself thrust into the ranks of the weal- Swidler’s claim, though, I also assume that
thy, only to discover that he does not people try to make use of the skills and abil-
know how to act at the country club. ities they have and will therefore resist
When there is a mismatch between identi- change that requires them to learn new
ties and skills, skill sets can act back on competencies (Swidler 1986).
motivations to occupy identities, reducing In what follows I use the terms situa-
the desire to enact roles for which people tion and context in a very loose sense,
lack competency (Bourdieu 1990; Swidler applying them to immediate behavioral
1986). Identity enactments that require contexts, which tend to be of relatively
new skills may even seem impossible short duration (e.g., meeting a friend in
(Bourdieu 1990). Note, however, that the the hall), institutional contexts that are
converse is not typically true—that is, occupied for somewhat longer time frames
that possessing cultural competencies (e.g., home, work), as well as longer peri-
motivates action that exploits those skills. ods that accompany life transitions (e.g.,
Just as hammers can be used for many retiring). The moving parts of the model
projects, competencies can be applied in are general enough that they can be
the service of many goals.5 In simple applied at each level of analysis.
terms, cultural skills are necessary but The model picks up when a person
not generally sufficient for action (Vaisey changes situations, as shown in the top
2008). left corner of Figure 1.6 I assume that
6
The motivations for this initial change in sit-
5
The only exception I can think of is a ‘‘novelty uations lie outside the scope of the model. It is
effect.’’ Here acquiring knowledge or skills can probable that the reasons will vary depending
generate enthusiasm for the new possibilities on the type of situation in question. Transitions
that this affords, leading people to pursue action into social situations might be driven by a desire
that they otherwise would not. Typically, this for enjoyment, for instance, while the move to
effect is short-lived as skills become familiar retirement occurs as an inevitable aspect of
and lose their intrinsic interest. work life.

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


218 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

Figure 1. An Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action.


Note: Dotted lines represent transitions that require cultural knowledge, skills, competencies, or other
resources to either (a) perform identity-relevant tasks; and/or (b) have identity performances accepted by
others (e.g., gatekeepers); and/or (c) alter the situation so that the identity can be verified. If resources (cul-
tural skills or other types) are not sufficient for identity enactment in a given situation, then the process fol-
lows the ‘‘no’’ path.

a person can enter a situation with an consideration is whether the identity is


identity already activated, perhaps car- still (or can made to be) appropriate to
ried over from a prior context. Upon the new context. If so, the person contin-
entering the new situation, this identity ues to occupy that identity, provided
is evaluated to see if it can still be used that he has the resources to do so, which
to verify the core self, where the major cannot be taken for granted. For instance,

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 219

a person used to the routines of high If the person does not bring a pre-
school may find it difficult to feel like activated identity into the new context,
a star student after entering college. The or if it is not (or cannot be made to be)
identity to be confirmed is nominally the appropriate, the person will try to transi-
same, but the change in context brings tion to a different identity. This might
with it a change in the skills (and perhaps occur immediately as the person recog-
other resources) needed for verification. nizes the demands of the new situation
Such changes are not restricted to major (e.g., when changing institutional con-
life transitions; they can occur in the texts) or might occur after the person fails
course of daily life as well. Imagine a com- to have her prior identity verified (Smith-
puter programmer who is assigned a pro- Lovin and Robinson 2006). The success of
ject that exceeds his skills or a woman this attempt will depend on two things.
who feels good about her parenting until First, switching identities is conditional
she takes her children to a play group on whether the identity is cognitively
and finds that other mothers value par- available to a person—that is, she must
enting strategies that she does not have prior experience with the identity.
employ. These examples suggest that Second, even if a new identity is available
small changes in situations can alter a person must still have the resources and
the skills needed to enact or access cultural skills to access and enact it in the
identities. immediate context. As noted previously,
Conversely, resources sometimes allow neither process can be taken for granted,
a person to act back on situations or con- particularly when the person is encoun-
trol interactions and therefore can be tering a novel situation. Access can be
used to facilitate identity verification. At facilitated by material, social, and cul-
root, this is because reality is socially con- tural resources that allow the person to
structed, and social or skill-based advan- control the situation, but even then it
tages translate into increased power to depends on the basic ability to enact an
define reality. One way this can occur is identity, which requires cultural knowl-
by altering the definition of what skills edge and skills. To reiterate, ‘‘cultured
are needed for verification. To return to capacities’’ (Swidler 2001:71) are a neces-
the previous example, a high-status sary resource for competently enacting
woman taking her child to a play group any identity.
is more likely to have her parenting tech- Activating a situationally appropriate
niques seen as appropriate than is a low- identity generally proceeds with little dif-
status woman. In some cases, extreme ficulty, but problems can arise when this
levels of resources can also be used to identity differs substantially from other,
redefine even what identities are appro- salient identities that might also have
priate to enact. Barack Obama, for been activated, especially those chroni-
instance, likely gets to play the part of cally accessible identities that are part
president almost anywhere he goes. The of the core self. As noted previously,
important caveat is that the power of when conflicts emerge people prefer to
resources over situations is rarely abso- enact their most salient identities, but if
lute and cannot compensate for a lack these are not appropriate to the situation
of essential cultural skills. A person then nonverification becomes likely. For
with little academic ability is unlikely example, a teenager leaving for school
to live up to even her own (culturally may have little trouble switching from
shaped) standard of what it means to be acting the part of a dependent daughter
a student. to that of the popular socialite, as both

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


220 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

are regularly reinforced and therefore practical purposes I believe it is useful to


cognitively available. However, her core instead think of the core self as stable in
self will remain active in both settings, the face of disconfirmation. People do
and any identities closely associated not immediately begin to change in
with it will persist across contexts. At response to disconfirming situations;
both home and school, for instance, the rather, they initially apply coping strate-
teenager will likely be guided by her gies to resist the perceived threat to the
sense of gender identity. She cannot self (cf. Burke 1996). These might include
turn this off, which might lead to nonver- directing attention to verifying feedback
ification if she is required to participate in or ignoring or counteracting nonverifying
activities that she does not perceive as feedback (e.g., ‘‘I really am a good stu-
sufficiently feminine (e.g., playing foot- dent, no matter what grade I got’’). Stav-
ball in P.E. class). ing off nonverification depends on having
If the core self is unable to be con- both access to and the ability to apply
firmed by changing to a different iden- these strategies, which again highlights
tity—either because the person is unable the importance of cultural resources for
or unwilling to change—then the person identity processes. It is only when these
will try to exit the situation. This, too, strategies fail (or require too much effort
can require material resources or cultural to continue) that the environment begins
skills, as when a person starts a job, discov- to shift the meanings held in the identity,
ers he does not like it, and wants to change and through it the meanings in the core
employment but lacks either the funds to self. The key consideration therefore
relocate or the needed qualifications to becomes whether a situation can be exited
obtain a better position. If the situation before this occurs. If so, the model cycles
cannot be exited, then the core self will back to the beginning and the verification
exist in a state of tension with the environ- process begins anew (see Figure 1).
ment, and the person will experience emo- A person who cannot exit a disconfirm-
tional pressure to resolve the discrepancy.7 ing situation will next try to adapt by
Identity theory suggests that identities adopting a new, situationally appropriate
are always changing in response to the identity that can be used to confirm the
environment, though typically at a slow core self. This response comes late in the
rate, and would predict that the longer process because, as Swidler noted, people
a nonverifying situation persists, the tailor their action to the skills they already
more the self would change to match con- possess, and acquiring a new identity also
textual meanings (Burke 2006). I do not means learning the cultural competences
know whether this model of perpetual needed to enact it, and potentially acquir-
change is technically accurate, but for ing additional material resources to sup-
port it (Swidler 1986, 2001). For example,
7
The terms tension and emotional pressure are a couple that has their first child might
intentionally ambiguous. This is because evi- find it difficult to fully maintain their pre-
dence is mixed regarding the particular emotions
(or even emotional valences) that lead one to seek
vious spousal and personal identities, and
identity verification. Identity theorists predict so they compensate by adopting parental
that any disconfirmation will lead to negative identities. Again, this step depends both
affect, but affect control theorists predict that on the salience of prior identities and on
only negative nonverification has this effect and having (or being able to learn) the skills
positive nonverification leads to positive emotion.
However, both theories agree that people try to
needed for parenting. It also requires
verify their identities regardless of the direction material resources; children, after all, are
of disconfirmation. expensive. Adopting a new identity is

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 221

typically time-consuming, so persons interviews and observational data, Lar-


might experience repeated nonverification eau argued that both the middle-class
until the process is complete. People rarely and working/lower-class adults in her
feel that mastering the art of parenting is study wanted to be good parents—that
a short-term affair. is, they wanted to enact the parent iden-
As noted by identity theorists, identi- tity—but that they differed in their ability
ties do change, often unintentionally to translate their intentions into benefi-
(Burke 2006; Burke and Stets 2009, chap- cial outcomes for their children because
ter 9). In the current framework, this their cultural skills were differentially
occurs only if the previous steps cannot valued by dominant institutions. With
verify the core self. That is, a person gen- regard to education, for instance, lower-
erally will try to operate using one of the class parents tended to trust the judg-
identities she has and, failing that, will ment of school personnel and generally
try to exit the disconfirming context. If complied with school recommendations.
neither of these is an option, she will try Middle-class parents more often saw the
to verify her core self by adopting a new, system as malleable and worked with
situationally relevant identity, even if school teachers and administrators to
this takes time. It is only when this too secure optimal outcomes for their chil-
proves prohibitively difficult that her core dren.8 As Lareau pointed out, only the
self begins to change. This will occur as middle-class approach was well adapted
she adopts and verifies contextually appro- for extracting results from the educa-
priate identities that are at odds with her tional system. Furthermore, school per-
core self. As posited by identity theory, sonnel often viewed parental involvement
the direction of the change is toward situ- positively and responded accordingly. The
ational meanings, which narrows the dis- present model suggests that middle-class
tance between the core self and the situa- adults would therefore be able to have
tional feedback and therefore reduces their preferred parental identities veri-
nonverification (Burke 2006). fied in interactions with school personnel,
but this would be more challenging for
EVIDENCE FOR THE IDENTITY-BASED lower-class parents—that is, middle-class
MODEL OF CULTURE IN ACTION adults have greater access to the parental
identity. This is exactly what Lareau
Evidence for the model can be culled from found. She reported that many lower-
many places. As the basic operations of class parents had difficulty interacting
identities are already well established in with schools and felt ineffectual in their
the social psychological literature (e.g., efforts to procure advantages for their
Burke and Stets 2009; Robinson and children. Because many parents were
Smith-Lovin 2006), I will focus on two unwilling or unable to exit the parent
studies that demonstrate the crucial role identity, this lack of cultural skills led to
played by cultural skills in accessing and repeated nonverification.
enacting identities, though I will also Wacquant’s (2004) ethnography of
note where they lend support to other Southside Chicago provides a second
aspects of the model. example. In an effort to build social ties
The first example comes from Lareau’s needed for his study of life in the ghetto,
(2003) Unequal Childhoods, a rich study
of American families that highlights the 8
See the examples of Ms. Marshall (middle
connections between class and the strati- class) and Ms. Driver (lower class) to illustrate
fication of cultural capital. Drawing on these points.

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


222 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

Wacquant enrolled in a local boxing gym could have exited the situation at any
but quickly learned that adopting the time—indeed, the identity incumbents
identity of a boxer required physical skills expected him to. Doing so would have
and deep-seated reactions that could only removed the necessity of learning new
be acquired through intense training. Yet skills and protected his core self from fur-
these physical competencies could only ther nonverification. To reiterate, Wac-
provide him with the tools to enact the quant’s ability to overcome these chal-
identity in a general, technical way. His lenges and remain at the gym depended
access to the boxer identity also required on his learning a particular set of iden-
him to gain acceptance in the eyes of the tity-specific skills, which enabled him to
other boxers in the particular gym where both perform the behaviors expected of
he trained, which required additional cul- a boxer and gain legitimacy in the eyes
tural skills. He had to master the ‘‘shop- of the identity gatekeepers.
talk’’ that revolved around topics such as
‘‘the maintenance of the body, . . . ‘making CONCLUSION
weight’ . . . [and] technical subtleties of the
Over a decade ago, Swidler posed a crucial
game’’ (38), as well as talk that reflected
problem for scholars interested in linking
life in the ghetto. Only in the combination
culture to action: which elements of cul-
of the physical and the social could he find
ture anchor (or cause) others? A number
‘‘the cultural capital proper to the group’’
of scholars have offered useful responses,
(39) and come to fully occupy the pugilist
but their work does not adequately
identity. Developing the necessary cul-
address action that crosscuts contexts, in
tural competencies took time, and during
part because they do not detail which cul-
the process Wacquant faced repeated dis-
tural contents matter. The model outlined
confirmation—in his words, his early
previously uses identity to answer these
‘‘technical ineptitude was equalled only
questions. In short, it suggests that peo-
by [his] feeling of frustration and some-
times discouragement’’ (x). ple are motivated to act in self-consistent
According to his reports, Wacquant ways and that the most important self-
eventually earned his place in the gym, perceptions are those salient identities
but his reflections on the process make that have been incorporated into the
it painfully evident (in his case, literally) core self. This core self manages which
that enacting and accessing identities other identities are activated in different
are fraught affairs that can fail if the situations, favoring identities that are
needed skills are not acquired. As empha- both consistent with its own meanings
sized by the present model, the exact and situationally appropriate. Identities
skills needed for identity enactment vary in turn guide how cultural competencies
by context—Wacquant needed knowledge and other resources are deployed. At the
of the ghetto, but this requirement would same time, cultural skills and knowledge
certainly be different in a gym set in a dif- can limit the identities that can be veri-
ferent location and frequented by a differ- fied and the contexts in which verification
ent clientele.9 Note, too, that Wacquant can occur. Fundamentally, this is because
relevant cultural competencies are
9
In fact, a quick search on Google brought up needed to perform identity-consistent
a boxing facility in Massachusetts called the actions at all (that is, to enact identities).
‘‘Gentlemen’s Gym Boxing Club,’’ and the pic- When action occurs in the presence of
tures posted on the website make it obvious
that it is very different from the gym described others, skills (and other resources) also
by Wacquant. allow a person to have their identity

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 223

performances accepted, thereby providing more likely across situations that do not
access to identities. With only minimal require interaction.
violence to nuance, we might summarize Of course, patching holes in past argu-
the answer to the problem of cultural ments is insufficient to demonstrate util-
anchoring presented here as follows: ity. Does the theory-building demon-
core selves anchor less salient identities, strated here have practical import, or is
which in turn anchor cultural competen- it simply an exercise in seeking logical
cies, provided that a person possesses coherency for the sake of coherency (see
the requisite competencies for enacting also Wrong 1961)? I can think of at least
an identity in a given context. two ways that the current model can
This model is not a complete solution to make significant real-world contributions
the problem of cultural anchoring, but it to empirical research.
does advance the discussion in several First, the model has implications for
ways. First, it retains important insights stratification researchers. Adopting iden-
offered by prior theories. From tool kit tities in a given situation requires having
theory it incorporates skills and other cul- the cultural competencies to both perform
tural resources, and from dual-process the behaviors associated with the identi-
theories (e.g., Vaisey 2009) the idea ties and to have the identity performances
that culture can be motivating, but adds accepted by others. The two need not be
to these specific cultural contents— overlapping sets, but both are required
identities—that are motivating in for identity verification. This means that
known, predictable ways. Second, the those aspiring to privileged positions
model accounts for the activation of cul- must learn the culture of their incum-
tural content in a way that retains sensi- bents. Failing this, they will either experi-
tivity to context but provides greater ence emotional arousal and an eventual
insight into the mechanisms that under- shift in their core selves and/or will exit
lie the activation process (e.g., identity the disconfirming situations, effectively
salience). Third and perhaps most impor- leaving the privileged positions to those
tant, it explains action that occurs both with the needed cultural capacities. As
within and across situations. Actions Bourdieu (1986) noted, acquiring these
sometimes crosscut contexts because competencies can require prolonged expo-
the identities that motivate them are sure, and opportunities for this type of
active in many situations—likewise, learning are unequally distributed across
actions can be bound to certain contexts the social landscape (see also Khan 2011).
when shaped by situation-specific identi- On a more positive note, personalized
ties. In all cases, action depends on hav- meanings located in the core self may pro-
ing appropriate cultural knowledge, vide individuals with tools to survive
skills, and other resources. Consistent within and even resist dominant struc-
with Abramson (2012), this implies that tures. Institutional diversity in modern
the power of identities to generate society means that people have multiple
cross-context consistency can be limited forums for seeking self-verification.
in interaction settings in which actors Because the core self is slow to change,
come from different class backgrounds. individuals can resist disconfirming sit-
Identities are more likely to promote con- uations provided that they have regular
sistency across situations in which inter- access to social support or coping strate-
actants are of the same class, where cul- gies. Self-verification becomes easier as
tural resources are presumably held freedom to choose situations and inte-
(relatively) constant. Consistency is also raction partners increase. Interaction

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


224 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

contexts are particularly important is both because it is not essential to the


because they allow meanings to be trans- current argument and because identity
mitted from person to person, which can researchers do not yet agree on how the
lead to changes in dominant meaning two relate (Smith-Lovin and Robinson
structures. Smith-Lovin (2007), for 2006). Adding an affective component
instance, argued that local changes in would add additional depth and richness,
identity meanings exert pressure to particularly if it could be shown that dis-
change general identity meanings, which tinct emotions alter the verification pro-
in turn allows them to distill into addi- cess in unique ways. For example, we
tional institutional domains (cf. Foy et might expect that a person who feels
al. forthcoming). In this way, situated shame will be more apt to immediately
attempts at self-verification can alter exit a disconfirming situation than a per-
established meaning structures. son who merely feels embarrassed.
There are several aspects of the model The proposed model also raises a num-
that could benefit from additional empiri- ber of intriguing questions. First and fore-
cal work. First, the model would benefit most, the notion of a core self bears
from verifying the hypothesized sequence a strong resemblance to Bordieu’s habitus
of events. For instance, when do people (cf. Vaisey 2009), but the exact contours of
enter situations with active identities, this relationship are unclear, suggesting
and when do they deactivate identities that further attention could be profitably
before switching contexts? Switching directed to fleshing out the connections
identities takes time, perhaps propor- between the two. Of particular interest
tional to the level of investment in the is whether the core self, like the habitus,
prior identity. Imagine the parent who, operates largely through fast, automatic
after a hard day of work, needs a few cognitive processes. The assumption of
minutes before engaging with the chil- chronically accessibility suggests that it
dren. Attention should also be given to does, but to date identity theory has
whether people can ‘‘skip’’ steps in the largely been silent on the relationship
process and how this occurs. Those pur- between identities (and by extension the
posely aspiring to identities, for instance, self generally) and dual-process models
often seem to bypass the first several of cognition.10 Testing these links will
phases of the process—that is, they both enhance identity theory and facili-
directly enter disconfirming situations to tate connections between the present
leverage themselves into a desired iden- model and current work on culture in
tity (e.g., Wacquant 2004; cf. Vaisey and action. I also argued that identity activa-
Lizardo 2010). Empirical work is also tion depends on the salience of at least
needed to bolster the claim that the core two identities (the prior and the one
self is chronically accessible. Relatedly, needed for the new situation), as well as
scholars might ask how other mecha- possession of cultural tools appropriate
nisms of identity activation fit into the to the context, but the question of how
model. For instance, individuals some- these constructs jointly affect identity
times disingenuously invoke identities enactment—that is, in what combinations
for instrumental gain—what does this
10
imply about the core self and how the con- Several recent studies suggest that people
hold implicit identities that can guide their
cept might be expanded? Finally, the cur-
behavior (e.g., Srivastava and Banaji 2011). I
rent model (intentionally) does not am currently working on a project that explores
address the issue of emotional reactions whether identities primarily operate using auto-
to the identity verification process. This matic or deliberative processes.

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 225

and in what circumstances—requires edited by R. E. L. Faris. Chicago: Rand


additional attention. Finally, it is worth McNally & Company.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1984. Distinction: A Social
considering how long people can resist sit- Critique of the Judgement of Taste. Cam-
uational disconfirmation in cases where bridge: Harvard University Press.
exiting a situation is unfeasible or impos- Bourdieu, Pierre. 1986. ‘‘The Forms of Capi-
sible. Does the process differ when the tal.’’ Pp. 46–58 in Handbook of Theory of
nonverification is the result of intentional Research for the Sociology of Education,
edited by J. E. Richardson. Westport, CT:
entrance into the situation in pursuit of Greenwood Press.
an aspirational identity, compared to Bourdieu, Pierre. 1990. The Logic of Practice.
when the context is unwanted but exter- Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
nally imposed? Are there strategies that Burke, Peter J. 1996. ‘‘Social Identities and
allow a person to resist changes to the Psychosocial Stress.’’ Pp. 141–74 in Psycho-
social Stress: Perspectives on Structure, The-
self, perhaps indefinitely? ory, Life Course, and Methods, edited by H.
The identity model of culture in action Kaplan. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
should be seen as a next step in address- Burke, Peter J. 2006. ‘‘Identity Change.’’
ing the perpetually vexing problem of Social Psychology Quarterly 69(1):81–96.
the role of culture in action and as an Burke, Peter J. and Jan E. Stets. 2009. Identity
Theory. New York: Oxford University
illustration of the benefits of combining Press.
multiple research traditions in pursuit of Cast, Alicia D., Jan E. Stets, and Peter J.
more satisfying theoretical answers. Burke. 1999. ‘‘Does the Self Conform the
Views of Others?’’ Social Psychology Quar-
terly 62(1):68–82.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Correll, Shelley J. and Cecilia L. Ridgeway.
2003. ‘‘Expectation States Theory.’’ Pp.
I’d like to thank Steve Vaisey, Lynn Smith-Lovin, 29–51 in Handbook of Social Psychology,
the guest editors of SPQ (Omar Lizardo and edited by J. Delamater. New York: Kluwer
Jessica Collett), and several anonymous reviewers Academic and Plenum Publishers.
for helpful feedback on earlier drafts of this DiMaggio, Paul and John Mohr. 1985. ‘‘Cul-
article. tural Capital, Educational Attainment,
and Marital Selection.’’ American Journal
REFERENCES of Sociology 90(6):1231–61.
Fabrigar, Leandre R., Tara K. MacDonald, and
Abramson, Corey M. 2012. ‘‘From ‘Either-Or’ to Duane T. Wegener. 2005. ‘‘The Structure of
‘When and How’: A Context-Dependent Attitudes.’’ Pp. 79–124 in The Handbook of
Model of Culture in Action.’’ Journal for Attitudes, edited by D. Albarracin, B. T.
the Theory of Social Behaviour 42(2):155–80. Johnson and M. P. Zanna. New York: Psy-
Akerlof, George A. and Rachel E. Kranton. chology Press.
2000. ‘‘Economics and Identity.’’ Quarterly Forster, Jens and Nira Liberman. 2007.
Journal of Economics 115(3):715–53. ‘‘Knowledge Activation.’’ Pp. 201–31 in
Berger, Ida E. and Andrew A. Mitchell. 1989. Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Prin-
‘‘The Effect of Advertising on Attitude ciples, edited by A. W. Kruglanski and E. T.
Accessibility, Attitude Confidence, and the Higgins. New York: Guilford Press.
Attitude-Behavior Relationship.’’ Journal Foy, Steven, Robert Freeland, Andrew Miles,
of Consumer Research 16(3):269–79. Kimberly B. Rogers, and Lynn Smith-Lovin.
Bizer, George Y. and Jon A. Krosnick. 2001. Forthcoming. ‘‘Emotions and Affect as
‘‘Exploring the Structure of Strength- Source, Outcome and Resistance to Inequal-
Related Attitude Features: The Relation ity.’’ In The Handbook of the Social Psychol-
Between Attitude Importance and Attitude ogy of Inequality. New York: Springer.
Accessibility.’’ Journal of Personality and Geertz, Clifford. 1973. ‘‘Religion as a Cultural
Social Psychology 81(4):566–86. System.’’ Pp. 87–125 in The Interpretation
Blake, Judith and Kingsley Davis. 1964. of Cultures. New York: Basic Books.
‘‘Norms, Values, and Sanctions.’’ Pp. Glasman, Laura R. and Dolores Albarracı́n.
456–84 in Handbook of Modern Sociology, 2006. ‘‘Forming Attitudes that Predict

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


226 Social Psychology Quarterly 77(2)

Future Behavior: A Meta-Analysis of the Robinson, Dawn T. 2007. ‘‘Control Theories in


Attitude-Behavior Relation.’’ Psychological Sociology.’’ Annual Review of Sociology
Bulletin 132(5):778–822. 33(1):157–74.
Gross, Neil. 2009. ‘‘A Pragmatist Theory of Robinson, Dawn T. and Lynn Smith-Lovin.
Social Mechanisms.’’ American Sociological 2006. ‘‘Affect Control Theory.’’ Pp. 137–59 in
Review 74(3):358–79. Contemporary Social Psychological Theories.
Hogg, Michael A., Deborah J. Terry, and Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Katherine M. White. 1995. ‘‘A Tale of Two Schwartz, Shalom. 2010. ‘‘Basic Values: How
Theories: A Critical Comparison of Identity They Motivate and Inhibit Prosocial Behav-
Theory with Social Identity Theory.’’ Social ior.’’ Pp. 221–42 in Prosocial Motives, Emo-
Psychology Quarterly 58(4):255–69. tions, and Behavior: The Better Angels of
Khan, Shamus Rhaman. 2011. Privilege: The Our Nature, edited by M. Mikulincer and
Making of an Adolescent Elite at St. Paul’s P. R. Shaver. Washington, DC: American
School. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Psychological Association.
Press. Serpe, Richard T. and Sheldon Stryker. 1987.
Laitin, David D. 1986. Hegemony and Culture: ‘‘The Construction of Self and the Recon-
Politics and Religious Change among the struction of Social Relationships.’’ Pp. 41–
Yoruba. Chicago: University of Chicago 66 in Advances in Group Processes. Green-
Press. wich, CT: JAI Press.
Lamont, Michèle. 1992. Money, Morals, and Smith-Lovin, Lynn. 2007. ‘‘The Strength of
Manners: The Culture of the French and Weak Identities: Social Structural Sources
American Upper-Middle Class. Chicago: of Self, Situation and Emotional Experi-
University of Chicago Press. ence.’’ Social Psychology Quarterly
Lamont, Michèle and Annette Lareau. 1988. 70(2):106–24.
‘‘Cultural Capital: Allusions, Gaps and Smith-Lovin, Lynn and Dawn T. Robinson.
Glissandos in Recent Theoretical Develop- 2006. ‘‘Control Theories of Identity, Action,
ments.’’ Sociological Theory 6(2):153–68. and Emotion: In Search of Testable Differ-
Lareau, Annette. 2003. Unequal Childhoods: ences between Affect Control Theory and
Class, Race, and Family Life. Berkeley: Identity Control Theory.’’ Pp. 163–88 in
University of California Press. Purpose, Meaning and Action: Control Sys-
Lizardo, Omar and Michael Strand. 2010. tems Theories in Sociology, edited by K. A.
‘‘Skills, Toolkits, Contexts and Institutions: McClelland and T. J. Fararo. New York:
Clarifying the Relationship between Differ- Palgrave MacMillan.
ent Approaches to Cognition in Cultural Srivastava, Sameer B. and Mahzarin R. Banaji.
Sociology.’’ Poetics 38(2):205–28. 2011. ‘‘Culture, Cognition, and Collaborative
MacKinnon, Neil J. and David R. Heise. 2010. Networks in Organizations.’’ American
Self, Identity, and Social Institutions. New Sociological Review 76(2):207–33.
York: Palgrave Macmillan. Stets, Jan E. and Michael J. Carter. 2011. ‘‘The
Markus, Hazel. 1977. ‘‘Self-Schemata and Pro- Moral Self: Applying Identity Theory.’’
cessing Information about the Self.’’ Jour- Social Psychology Quarterly 74(2):192–215.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology Stets, Jan E. and Alicia D. Cast. 2007.
35(2):63–78. ‘‘Resources and Identity Verification from
Mead, George H. 1934. Mind, Self and Society: an Identity Theory Perspective.’’ Sociologi-
From the Standpoint of a Social Behavior- cal Perspectives 50(4):517–43.
ist. edited by C. W. Morris. Chicago: Uni- Stets, Jan E. and Michael M. Harrod. 2004.
versity of Chicago Press. ‘‘Verification across Multiple Identities:
Parsons, Talcott and Edward A. Shils. 1951. The Role of Status.’’ Social Psychology
‘‘Values, Motives, and Systems of Action.’’ Quarterly 67(2):155–71.
Pp. 47–275 in Toward a General Theory Stryker, Sheldon. 1980. Symbolic Interaction-
of Action, edited by T. Parsons and E. A. ism: A Social Structural Version. Menlo
Shils. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings Publishing
Press. Company.
Rivera, Lauren A. 2012. ‘‘Hiring as Cultural Stryker, Sheldon. 2008. ‘‘From Mead to a Struc-
Matching: The Case of Elite Professional tural Symbolic Interactionism and
Service Firms.’’ American Sociological Beyond.’’ Annual Review of Sociology
Review 77(6):999–1022. 34(1):15–31.

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014


Identity-Based Model of Culture in Action 227

Stryker, Sheldon and Richard T. Serpe. 1982. Identity.’’ Social Psychology Quarterly
‘‘Commitment, Identity Salience, and Role 62(2):173–89.
Behavior: A Theory and Research Exam- Vaisey, Stephen. 2008. ‘‘Reply to Ann Swi-
ple.’’ Pp. 199–218 in Personality, Roles, dler.’’ Sociological Forum 23(3):619–22.
and Social Behavior, edited by W. Ickes Vaisey, Stephen. 2009. ‘‘Motivation and Justi-
and E. S. Knowles. New York: Springer- fication: A Dual-Process Model of Culture
Verlag. in Action.’’ American Journal of Sociology
Stryker, Sheldon and Richard T. Serpe. 1994. 114(6):1675–715.
‘‘Identity Salience and Psychological Cen- Vaisey, Stephen. 2010. ‘‘What People Want:
trality: Equivalent, Overlapping, or Com- Rethinking Poverty, Culture, and Educa-
plementary Concepts?’’ Social Psychology tional Attainment.’’ The ANNALS of the
Quarterly 57(1):16–35. American Academy of Political and Social
Stryker, Sheldon, Richard T. Serpe, and Mat- Science 629(1):75–101.
thew O. Hunt. 2005. ‘‘Making Good on Vaisey, Stephen and Omar Lizardo. 2010.
a Promise: The Impact of Larger Social ‘‘Can Cultural Worldviews Influence Net-
Structures on Commitments.’’ Pp. 93–123 work Composition?’’ Social Forces
in Advances in Group Processes, edited by 88(4):1595–618.
S. R. Thye and E. J. Lawler. Bradford, Verplanken, Bas and Rob W. Holland. 2002.
UK: Emerald Group Publishing. ‘‘Motivated Decision Making: Effects of
Swann, William B. Jr., Brett W. Pelham, and Activation and Self-Centrality of Values
Douglas S. Krull. 1989. ‘‘Agreeable Fancy on Choices and Behavior.’’ Journal of Per-
or Disagreeable Truth? Reconciling Self- sonality and Social Psychology 82(3):434–
Enhancement and Self-Verification.’’ Jour- 47.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology Wacquant, Loic. 2004. Body and Soul: Note-
57(5):782–91. books of an Apprentice Boxer. New York:
Swidler, Ann. 1986. ‘‘Culture in Action: Sym- Oxford University Press.
bols and Strategies.’’ American Sociological Wrong, Dennis H. 1961. ‘‘The Oversocialized
Review 51(2):273–86. Conception of Man in Sociology.’’ American
Swidler, Ann. 2001. Talk of Love: How Culture Sociological Review 26(2):183–93.
Matters. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press. BIO
Swidler, Ann. 2008. ‘‘Comment on Stephen
Vaisey’s ‘Socrates, Skinner, and Aristotle:
Andrew A. Miles is a PhD student in the
Three Ways of Thinking about Culture in
Action.’’’ Sociological Forum 23(3):614–18. Department of Sociology at Duke Univer-
Tabatabai, Ahoo and Annulla Linders. 2011. sity. The goal of his research is to under-
‘‘Vanishing Act: Non-Straight Identity Nar- stand the processes underlying human
ratives of Women in Relationships with behavior and to synthesize them into
Women and Men.’’ Qualitative Sociology
more complete models of action. His
34(4):583–99.
Tavory, Iddo and Yehuda C. Goodman. 2009. work is interdisciplinary, often drawing
‘‘‘A Collective of Individuals’: Between Self on literature from sociology, psychology,
and Solidarity in a Rainbow Gathering.’’ anthropology, and neuroscience. He is
Sociology of Religion 70(3):262–84. also interested in morality, social psychol-
Tsushima, Teresa and Peter J. Burke. 1999.
ogy, and quantitative methods.
‘‘Levels, Agency, and Control in the Parent

Downloaded from spq.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITY OF SASKATCHEWAN LIBRARY on May 29, 2014

You might also like