Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

Social exchange from the supervisor’s perspective: Employee trustworthiness


as a predictor of interpersonal and informational justice
Cindy P. Zapata a,⇑, Jesse E. Olsen b,1, Luis L. Martins c
a
Scheller College of Business, Georgia Institute of Technology, 800 West Peachtree St., NW, Atlanta, GA 30308-1149, United States
b
School of International Studies, Kwansei Gakuin University, 1-1-155 Uegahara, Nishinomiya, Hyogo 662-8501, Japan
c
McCombs School of Business, University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station, B6300, Austin, TX 78712-0210, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Using social exchange theory, we argue that because supervisors tend to value employee trustworthiness,
Received 12 July 2011 they will be more likely to adhere to interpersonal and informational justice rules with trustworthy
Accepted 9 November 2012 employees. Given social exchange theory’s assumption that benefits are voluntary in nature, we propose
Available online 11 December 2012
that the benevolence and integrity facets of trustworthiness will be more likely to engender social
Accepted by Paul Levy
exchange relationships than the ability facet. Specifically, we propose that employees seen as having high
benevolence and integrity engender feelings of obligation and trust from their direct supervisors, increas-
Keywords:
ing the likelihood that these supervisors will adhere to interpersonal and informational justice rules,
Organizational justice
Fairness
which in turn influences employee perceptions of justice. We find partial support for our mediated model
Interpersonal justice using a field sample.
Informational justice Published by Elsevier Inc.
Trustworthiness
Benevolence
Integrity
Social exchange

Introduction Despite the numerous beneficial employee attitudes and behav-


iors associated with justice (Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001;
Understanding the effects of organizational justice has been a Colquitt et al., 2001), there remains an important gap in the justice
popular topic of research for several decades (for a narrative literature: to date, we know little about why supervisors adhere to
review, see Colquitt, Greenberg, & Zapata-Phelan, 2005). Most justice rules in the first place. We should note that our study focuses
scholars agree that organizational justice is multifaceted, consist- on the predominant framework in the organizational justice litera-
ing of procedural justice, which is fostered by adhering to rules like ture by measuring justice indirectly (i.e., measuring adherence or
consistency, accuracy, and bias suppression (Leventhal, 1976, violation of specific justice rules). As it stands, models of organiza-
1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975) and distributive justice, which en- tional justice tend to focus on justice as an independent variable,
tails adhering to rules like equity, equality, and need (Adams, making it exogenous in the causal system. Although important, this
1965; Homans, 1961). In addition, interpersonal justice involves perspective fails to account for antecedents of justice rule adher-
adhering to respect and propriety rules of interpersonal treatment ence. In fact, the need to consider justice as a dependent, rather than
and informational justice captures adherence to truthfulness and an independent, variable has been noted recently by several justice
justification rules (Bies & Moag, 1986; Greenberg, 1993). Past scholars (Colquitt, 2012; Masterson, Byrne, & Mao, 2005; Patient &
research has demonstrated that, when an employee perceives that Skarlicki, 2005; Scott, Colquitt, & Paddock, 2009). From a theoretical
his or her supervisor has adhered to distributive, procedural, inter- perspective, examining why supervisors adhere to justice rules will
personal, and informational justice rules, a number of beneficial provide a more complete picture of justice. From a practical
attitudes and behaviors result (for reviews, see Cohen-Charash & perspective, understanding the reasons why supervisors adhere to
Spector, 2001; Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001; justice rules can help organizations find ways to foster increased
Conlon, Meyer, & Nowakowski, 2005). justice rule adherence on the part of supervisors.
To be clear, there are a few studies that examine direct
measures of fairness perceptions (as opposed to indirect rule-
⇑ Corresponding author. Fax: +1 (404) 894 6030.
based measures) as dependent variables. For example, Skarlicki
E-mail addresses: cindy.zapata@scheller.gatech.edu (C.P. Zapata), jeolsen@
kwansei.ac.jp (J.E. Olsen), luis.martins@mccombs.utexas.edu (L.L. Martins). and Latham (1997) examined the effects of providing fairness
1
Fax: +81 798 54 6082. training to union leaders on employee perceptions of global

0749-5978/$ - see front matter Published by Elsevier Inc.


http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.11.001
2 C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

fairness (see also Skarlicki & Latham, 1996). We should also note increased interpersonal and informational justice rule adherence
that our review of the literature revealed only two studies treating (see Fig. 1).
justice rule adherence as a dependent variable. Using an experi- A secondary purpose of our study was to examine the relation-
mental design, Korsgaard, Roberson, and Rymph (1998) found that ship between supervisor justice rule adherence and employee per-
when confederates behaved assertively (by speaking in a confident ceptions of justice. Unlike the philosophical view of justice, which
manner, maintaining eye contact, listening attentively, and directly attempts to prescribe what should constitute as just behavior,
questioning the feedback received), student participants were more organizational justice is conceptualized as somewhat of a
likely to grant the confederate process and decision control, as well subjective phenomenon (Cohen & Greenberg, 1982; Cropanzano
as provide additional justifications for the feedback provided. & Greenberg, 1997). Thus, the widely accepted operationalization
However, a second field study failed to demonstrate significant of justice is in the eye of the recipient. However, qualitative studies
effects of assertiveness training on perceptions of interactional have demonstrated that certain rules, the rules we now use to de-
justice. More recently, Scott, Colquitt, and Zapata-Phelan (2007) scribe organizational justice, are in fact linked to perceptions of fair
examined the influence of an employee characteristic, namely cha- treatment (e.g., Bies & Moag, 1986; Thibaut & Walker, 1975, 1978).
risma, on supervisor justice rule adherence. Using an approach/ Perhaps because of these studies, the justice literature often tends
avoidance framework, Scott et al. (2007) suggested that charismatic to assume that employee perceptions of supervisory justice are
employees could actually increase positive sentiments and consistent with managerial justice rule adherence. However, we
decrease negative sentiments felt by a supervisor, thus positively know of no empirical study that has actually examined both the
affecting perceptions of interpersonal and informational justice. supervisor’s justice rule adherence behavior (self-reported or
Unlike Scott et al. (2007), which provides an emotional explana- otherwise) and his or her employee’s perceptions of justice.
tion for why managers might choose to adhere to justice rules, we Theoretically, capturing supervisors’ justice rule adherence
use social exchange theory (Blau, 1964) to propose a cognitive demonstrates the exchange of valued benefits critical to social ex-
explanation for managerial justice rule adherence. Thus, the change theory. Put differently, social exchange entails not just the
purpose of our study is to apply what has become the dominant obligation or need to reciprocate, but actual reciprocation. Thus, our
lens in the justice effects literature, namely social exchange (Blau, model attempts to capture the multiple theoretical mechanisms
1964), to a relatively new question: Why do supervisors adhere to involved in social exchange relationships. From a practical per-
justice rules, and what ultimately predicts employee perceptions of spective, establishing the link between supervisor justice rule
justice rule adherence? adherence and employee perceptions of justice increases our abil-
Social exchange theory is arguably the dominant theoretical ity to theoretically propose antecedents to justice, thus better
lens used in the justice literature. However, our approach is unique informing the development of justice interventions. In other
in that we examine the supervisor-employee relationship in re- words, in addition to explaining why employees have strong jus-
verse, focusing on the supervisor’s role in the exchange relation- tice perceptions, one might also ask, why do supervisors adhere
ship, rather than the beneficial employee behaviors that result as to justice rules in the first place? Thus, our model includes super-
a consequence of fair treatment. According to the justice literature, visors’ self-reported justice rule adherence as an antecedent of em-
justice on the part of a supervisor can be viewed as a benefit to ployee perceptions of justice. The following sections provide
employees—a benefit that may obligate employees to reciprocate conceptual support for our model.
with beneficial attitudes and behaviors (e.g., Masterson, Lewis,
Goldman, & Taylor, 2000; Tekleab, Takeuchi, & Taylor, 2005). Our Social exchange and employee trustworthiness
study takes a new perspective by examining this theoretical dy-
namic in reverse, proposing that employees can also provide Beginning in the 1950’s, trust scholars began to document the
supervisors with valued benefits that may obligate them to recip- importance of characteristics of a trustee that foster a willingness
rocate with justice rule adherence. Although both employees and to be vulnerable on the part of the trustor, now commonly referred
supervisors can provide each other with numerous valuable re- to as trustworthiness (Mayer et al., 1995). Despite what appeared
sources, social exchange theory suggests that a particularly impor- to be some disagreement regarding the structure of trustworthi-
tant resource is trustworthiness, which is often conceptualized as ness, Mayer et al. (1995) identified three characteristics that ap-
perceptions of ability, benevolence, and integrity (Mayer, Davis, peared repeatedly throughout the literature: ability, benevolence,
& Schoorman, 1995). In fact, Blau (1964) suggests that ‘‘since social and integrity. With respect to trustworthiness, ability captures
exchange requires trusting others to reciprocate, the initial prob- the trustor’s perception of the trustee’s capabilities and skills re-
lem is to prove oneself trustworthy’’ (p. 98; see also Holmes, quired for success in a particular domain (Mayer et al., 1995). Com-
1981). As a way to reciprocate employee trustworthiness, we pro- petence and expertise are oft-used synonyms of ability (Gabarro,
pose that supervisors may respond with increased interpersonal 1978; Mayer et al., 1995). Benevolence, sometimes termed loyalty,
and informational justice rule adherence. caring, supportiveness, and openness, is typically defined as the
In addition to making a significant contribution to the justice perceived extent to which the trustee wants to do well by the tru-
literature, our study also contributes to the trust literature by stor, excluding self-interested motives (Mayer et al., 1995). Integ-
examining the effects of employee trustworthiness, rather than rity refers to the extent to which the trustor believes the trustee
supervisor trustworthiness. To date, trust scholars have over- adheres to moral and ethically sensible principles (Mayer et al.,
whelmingly referenced the importance of trust in one’s supervisor 1995). We should note that the trust literature tends to conceptu-
and perceptions of supervisor trustworthiness as antecedents of alize trustworthiness as a perception (e.g., Levin, Whitener, &
valuable employee behaviors (for a review, see Colquitt, Scott, & Cross, 2006; Mayer & Davis, 1999; Mayer & Gavin, 2005; Simons,
LePine, 2007; Dirks & Ferrin, 2002). Yet social exchange theory im- 2002). For example, Mayer et al. (1995, p. 720) noted that ‘‘in the
plies that trustworthiness amongst both parties is important for evaluation of trustworthiness it is the perceived level of integrity
effective social exchange relationships (Blau, 1964). Moreover, as that is important rather than the reasons why the perception is
organizations become flatter and supervisors increasingly rely on formed.’’ Therefore we also operationalize trustworthiness as per-
their employees, having trustworthy employees becomes more ceptions of ability, benevolence, and integrity.
critical to workplace success. Our model suggests that employees Many empirical studies have demonstrated the importance of
seen as trustworthy by their supervisors are more likely to engen- trustworthiness. For example, a recent meta-analysis demon-
der social exchange relationships with the supervisors, leading to strated that employees who view their supervisors as trustworthy
C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12 3

Fig. 1. Research model: the effect of supervisor perceptions of employee trustworthiness on employee justice perceptions, as mediated by social exchange mechanisms and
supervisor justice rule adherence.

perform better, engage in more frequent citizenship behaviors, and respect: ability, defined as a ‘‘group of skills, competencies, and
refrain from counterproductive behaviors more often than employ- characteristics that enable a party to have influence within some
ees who view their supervisors as less trustworthy (Colquitt et al., specific domain’’ is arguably seen as less discretionary (i.e., within
2007). In their review, Colquitt et al. (2007) speculated that dem- one’s personal control) in nature than either benevolence or integ-
onstrations of ability, benevolence, and integrity could be viewed rity (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 717). In fact, many scholars have noted
as supervisory benefits that invite employee reciprocation in the that ‘‘most people assume that ability is not under the direct voli-
form of beneficial job behaviors. Casting this relationship in tional control of the individual’’ (LePine & Van Dyne, 2001, p. 76;
reverse, one could also reason that supervisors are more likely to see also Weiner, Frieze, Kukia, Reed, Rest, & Rosenbaum, 1971).
develop social exchange relationships with employees who are In contrast, demonstrating loyalty to a supervisor (e.g., benevo-
perceived to be more trustworthy, particularly with regard to the lence) and adhering to moral principles (e.g., integrity) are likely
facets of benevolence and integrity. viewed by others as more discretionary than ability. We expect
Although there are varying perspectives on exchange relation- that the more seemingly discretionary aspects of trustworthi-
ships, scholars agree that one of the core assumptions of social ness—benevolence and integrity—should be more relevant to social
exchange theory is that benefits given are contingent upon the exchange relationships than ability, which the literature suggests
expectation of some future unspecified benefit (Blau, 1964; is less discretionary. This is because social exchange theory relies
Emerson, 1976). Consequently, ‘‘an individual who supplies on the assumption that exchanges are discretionary, and therefore
rewarding services to another obligates him. To discharge this when a supervisor experiences more discretionary aspects of trust-
obligation, the second must furnish benefits to the first in turn’’ worthiness from a subordinate, it should engender greater felt
(Blau, 1964, p. 89). More importantly, social exchange theory dis- obligation than when the supervisor experiences the less discre-
tinguishes between the initial provision of a benefit or service tionary aspect of subordinate trustworthiness.
(i.e., ‘‘rewarding services’’), which if valued, prompts the recipient Indeed, while an employee’s ability might indicate whether or
of the benefit to experience attitudes supportive of reciprocation not that individual has the requisite competencies needed, benev-
(i.e., ‘‘felt obligation’’). Additionally, ‘‘since there is no way to as- olence and integrity are sometimes jointly referred to as character
sure an appropriate return for a favor, social exchange requires (Gabarro, 1978). As such, benevolence and integrity are also
trusting others to discharge their obligations’’ (Blau, 1964, p. 94; important to social exchange relationships because having
i.e., willingness to be vulnerable). Thus, feelings of obligation and employees with strong character should be a valuable asset for
trust are important drivers of the reciprocation of benefits in the supervisors. For example, employees that demonstrate high levels
social exchange process. Because trust entails a ‘‘willingness to of integrity should be viewed as more reliable than their low
be vulnerable to the actions of another party based the expectation integrity counterparts (Simons, 2002). Thus, uncertainty should
that the other will perform a particular action important to the decrease when integrity is high. Benevolent employees, which
trustor, irrespective of the ability to monitor or control that other are those that demonstrate a positive orientation toward their
party’’ (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 712), a lack of trust equals increased supervisor (e.g., displaying loyalty, care, and support) aside from
risk in social exchange relationships. Blau (1964, p. 94) stated that a profit motive, should elicit a positive emotional response from
‘‘only social exchange tends to engender feelings of personal the supervisor (Colquitt et al., 2007). In sum, supervisors benefit
obligation, gratitude, and trust; purely economic exchange as such when employees are seen as demonstrating benevolence and
does not.’’ In keeping with Holmes’s (1981) argument that percep- integrity.
tions of another’s trustworthiness provide the basis for social If employee benevolence and integrity are in fact valuable to
exchange relationships, we argue that one factor likely to prompt supervisors, social exchange theory would suggest that supervisors
the feelings of obligation and trust that are critical to social should develop feelings of obligation toward employees viewed as
exchange is employee trustworthiness. trustworthy on these facets. Perceptions of trust should also follow,
As facets of trustworthiness that inform social exchange, benev- as benevolence and integrity are conceptualized as two central
olence (i.e., ‘‘the extent to which a trustee is believed to want to do antecedents of trust perceptions (Mayer et al., 1995). Therefore,
good to the trustor, aside from an egocentric profit motive’’ Mayer we argue that supervisors experience an increase in perceptions
et al., 1995, p. 718) and integrity (i.e., ‘‘trustor’s perception that the of both trust and obligation toward employees viewed as having
trustee adheres to a set of principles that the trustor finds accept- benevolence and integrity. Ability is also an important facet of
able’’ Mayer et al., 1995, p. 719) differ from ability in an important trustworthiness, but because it is seen as less discretionary in nat-
4 C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

ure, the effects of employee benevolence and integrity should be instance, when a supervisor is open and honest in communications
significantly stronger than those of ability with respect to social with an employee, and treats an employee with respect and propri-
exchange relationships. Therefore, we hypothesize the following: ety, the underlying signal is that the employee is valued by the
supervisor. Taken together, this line of thinking suggests that ac-
Hypothesis 1a. Perceived employee benevolence and integrity are tions indicating adherence to interpersonal and informational jus-
positively related to supervisor felt obligation. tice rules are particularly important reciprocation behaviors that a
Hypothesis 1b. Perceived employee benevolence and integrity are supervisor can supply as a means of discharging obligations and
positively related to supervisor trust. reciprocating the trust engendered by employee benevolence and
integrity. Therefore, we hypothesize the following:
Hypothesis 2a. The effects of perceived employee benevolence
and integrity on supervisor felt obligation are significantly more Hypothesis 3. Felt obligation is positively related to supervisor
positive than the effects of perceived employee ability on supervi- interpersonal and informational justice rule adherence.
sor felt obligation.
Hypothesis 2b. The effects of perceived employee benevolence
and integrity on supervisor trust are significantly more positive Hypothesis 4. Trust is positively related to supervisor interper-
than the effects of perceived employee ability on supervisor trust. sonal and informational justice rule adherence.

According to social exchange theory, to ensure the continuation Although the importance of justice rule adherence should not
of exchanges, one must discharge obligations by reciprocating in be understated, ultimately it is the recipient’s perceptions of jus-
kind (Blau, 1964). In fact, failing to reciprocate sufficiently places tice rule adherence that acts as a key driver of employee attitudes
future benefits at risk (Blau, 1964). Similarly, trust scholars have and behaviors. In fact, the vast majority of field studies in the jus-
also described a cycle of reciprocity whereby trust begets trust tice literature utilize employee perceptions of supervisors adher-
(e.g., Butler, 1995). Thus, it is to be expected that the felt obligation ence to the various justice rules as the independent variable,
and trust resulting in part from employee trustworthiness will be predicting a number of attitudinal and behavioral variables (for a
followed by actual reciprocation behaviors. We propose that one review, see Conlon et al., 2005). However, we are aware of no stud-
way supervisors can reciprocate is by adhering to interpersonal ies that have examined one of the most obvious antecedents of em-
and informational justice rules. ployee justice perceptions, namely a supervisor’s actual adherence
Because the social exchange dynamic focuses on what Blau to, or violation of, justice rules. Put differently, employee justice
(1964) termed ‘‘voluntary actions’’ (p. 91), we operationalized jus- perceptions presumably result from supervisors’ adherence to, or
tice using interpersonal and informational justice. There are sev- violation of, justice rules. In fact, there is some indirect evidence
eral reasons why managers have more discretion in adhering to suggesting a link between justice rule adherence and perceptions
interpersonal and informational justice rules than with procedural of justice. For example, the rules we now ascribe to interpersonal
and distributive justice rules (for a review, see Scott et al., 2009). and informational justice were identified using open-ended ques-
First, the four justice rules differ in the degree to which character- tions concerning corporate recruiting practices (Bies & Moag,
istics of the organization constrain managerial discretion (Scott 1986). In that study, MBAs identified truthfulness, respect, propri-
et al., 2009). For example, formalized systems typically limit the ety of questions, and justification as ‘‘fairness criteria that they ex-
amount of discretion supervisors have over procedures used to pected recruiters to follow’’ (Bies & Moag, 1986, p. 47, emphasis in
determine rewards (i.e., distributive and procedural justice rules). original). Therefore, we hypothesize the following:
Such constraints are less relevant to informational justice rules,
and not at all relevant to interpersonal justice rules (Scott et al., Hypothesis 5. Supervisor interpersonal justice rule adherence is
2009). In addition, some rules are enacted more frequently than positively related to employee perceptions of interpersonal justice.
others, with increased frequency providing supervisors with more Hypothesis 6. Supervisor informational justice rule adherence is
discretion over them (Scott et al., 2009). For example, exchange- positively related to employee perceptions of informational justice.
based rules, such as distributive and procedural justice, require
an exchange context such as a resource allocation decision (Bies, Summary
2005). In contrast, more encounter-based rules, such as interper-
sonal and informational justice, can occur in daily interactions be- The central thesis of our manuscript is that employee trustwor-
tween supervisors and employees (Bies, 2005). Thus, scholars tend thiness (benevolence and integrity) positively affects interpersonal
to agree that, ‘‘interpersonal and informational justice have a ‘day- and informational justice, through the social exchange mecha-
in day-out’ significance that the other justice dimensions may not nisms of felt obligation and trust. According to social exchange the-
possess’’ (Scott et al., 2007, p. 1597). orists, one can exchange a wide variety of resources, both concrete
According to the multiple needs model (Cropanzano, Byrne, and symbolic (e.g., Blau, 1964; Foa & Foa, 1980). We argue that
Bobocel, & Rupp, 2001), justice is an important concern for individ- trustworthiness is one such resource. For example, integrity, which
uals because it helps to fulfill fundamental human needs such as involves the demonstration of high word-deed alignment, as well
the need for belonging, described as a desire to form and maintain as upholding high moral standards (Mayer et al., 1995), and benev-
lasting, positive interpersonal relationships (Baumeister & Leary, olence, which is characterized by demonstrations of caring and
1995) and the need for positive self-regard, described as a desire concern for the supervisor (Mayer et al., 1995), constitute valuable
to create and maintain a positive view of oneself (Cropanzano resources for supervisors.
et al., 2001). Similarly, the group value/relational models (Lind & If trustworthiness is a valuable resource for supervisors, then
Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992) suggest that justice is important according to social exchange theory, supervisors would want to
to employees partly because it conveys important information provide these employees with resources in an effort to sustain
about their standing or status with their supervisors. Information the relationship. Research supports this line of thinking by demon-
that indicates high status within a group, such as the fact that one’s strating that, in general, individuals that are seen as trustworthy
supervisor adheres to interpersonal and informational justice rules are more likely to receive beneficial resources than individuals
positively validates one’s identity or sense of self-worth, as well as who are not seen as trustworthy (Dirks & Skarlicki, 2009). For
one’s sense of belonging (Festinger, 1954; Tyler & Lind, 1992). For example, when employees are perceived as demonstrating care
C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12 5

and concern (i.e., benevolence), and seen as being reliable and into a drawing for one of five $100 Visa gift cards. Out of 149 stu-
dependable (i.e., integrity), they tend to receive more assistance dents, a total of 117 (79%) agreed to participate. To maintain full
in the form of citizenship behavior (McAllister, 1995). Previous re- control of the data collection process, students were asked to pro-
search has also shown that coworkers are more willing to share re- vide us with business, not personal, email addresses for the poten-
sources with trustworthy employees (Dirks & Skarlicki, 2009). One tial participants. In an effort to ensure full credit, nineteen students
valuable resource that supervisors can provide trustworthy opted to provide us with more than two potential participants (one
employees is justice rule adherence. By providing increased student provided four email addresses, two students provided five
respectful treatment (i.e., interpersonal justice) and increased addresses, and the remaining students provided three addresses).
truthfulness in explanations (i.e., informational justice), supervi- We directly contacted 258 employees. In order to address any po-
sors can discharge the obligations produced by the provision of tential concern regarding the anonymity of participants’ responses,
benevolence and integrity, and thus ensure the continuation of fu- participants were made aware of the fact that the website hosted
ture resource exchanges. In sum, benevolence and integrity should the survey via a Secure Sockets Layer (SSL) protocol, which pro-
affect employee perceptions of interpersonal and informational vides a high level of security for the transmission of survey data.
justice through a supervisor’s feelings of obligation and trust to- Participants were also informed that all identifying information
ward the trustworthy employee, as well as the supervisor’s actual would be removed from the dataset. To increase our response rate,
justice rule adherence. Therefore, we hypothesize the following: we sent out reminder emails one and two weeks after initial con-
tact. In total, 239 employees (93%) responded while 202 supervi-
Hypothesis 7. The relationship between perceived employee sors (85%) responded. Our relatively high response rate is a result
benevolence and employee perceptions of interpersonal justice is of students asking participants to participate before releasing a po-
mediated by felt obligation, trust, and ultimately supervisor tential participant’s email addresses. Once all data were collected,
interpersonal justice rule adherence. we visually inspected the data for a combination of red flags, such
Hypothesis 8. The relationship between perceived employee as personal email addresses, identical IP addresses within each em-
benevolence and employee perceptions of informational justice is ployee-supervisor pair, multiple instances of careless reverse cod-
mediated by felt obligation, trust, and ultimately supervisor ing, and the length of time spent on the survey. Listwise deletion
informational justice rule adherence. resulted in a total of 187 employee-supervisor pairs.

Hypothesis 9. The relationship between perceived employee


Measures
integrity and employee perceptions of interpersonal justice is
mediated by felt obligation, trust, and ultimately supervisor
To minimize potential order effects, we randomized the order of
interpersonal justice rule adherence.
the scales. Each participant was then randomly assigned to a
Hypothesis 10. The relationship between perceived employee slightly different version of the survey. Any scales that were
integrity and employee perceptions of informational justice is adapted for this study are presented in the Appendix.
mediated by felt obligation, trust, and ultimately supervisor
informational justice rule adherence. Supervisor perceptions of employee trustworthiness
Method The three facets of trustworthiness were measured with items
from Mayer and Davis’s (1999) trustworthiness measure, adapted
Sample to reference the supervisor’s employee. Supervisors were asked
to indicate the extent to which they agreed with each statement,
A total of 187 supervisor-employee dyads participated in the with response scales ranging from 1 (‘‘Strongly Disagree’’) to 5
study. All participants were full-time employees from a variety of (‘‘Strongly Agree’’). The perceived ability scale consisted of six
industries, including financial services, information technology, items, including: ‘‘My subordinate is very capable of performing
engineering, healthcare, banking, telecommunications, and phar- his/her job’’ and ‘‘My subordinate is well qualified.’’ This scale
maceuticals. The participants (employees and supervisors) were demonstrated a coefficient alpha of .91. Sample items from the
on average 38 (SD = 13.06) and 45 (SD = 11.33) years old, and had perceived benevolence scale included: ‘‘My subordinate really
been with their organizations for an average of 5.9 (SD = 6.99) looks out for what is important to me’’ and ‘‘My needs and desires
and 3.5 (SD = 3.64) years, respectively. The sample was 79% Cauca- are very important to my subordinate.’’ This scale demonstrated a
sian, with other ethnicities as follows: Hispanic (5%), Asian/Pacific coefficient alpha of .90. The perceived integrity scale consisted of
Islander (9%), and African American (3%). Some participants chose six items including: ‘‘I never have to wonder whether my subordi-
‘‘other’’ as their ethnicity (4%). The gender breakdown was as nate will stick to his/her word’’ and ‘‘Sound principles seem to
follows: females (45%) and males (55%). guide my subordinate’s behaviors.’’ This scale resulted in a coeffi-
cient alpha of .77.
Procedure
Felt obligation
To recruit a wide variety of occupations, we relied on the often- We measured felt obligation with three items from the Eisen-
used snowball sampling technique (Eddleston, Veiga, & Powell, berger, Armeli, Rexwinkle, Lynch, and Rhoades (2001) scale,
2006; Martins, Eddleston, & Veiga, 2002; Mayer, Kuenzi, adapted to reference the supervisor’s felt obligation toward his or
Greenbaum, Bardes, & Salvador, 2009; Morgeson & Humphrey, her employee. Supervisors were asked to indicate the extent to
2006). We asked undergraduate students enrolled in organiza- which they agreed with each statement, with response scales
tional behavior classes at a large southeastern university in the ranging from 1 (‘‘Strongly Disagree’’) to 5 (‘‘Strongly Agree’’). The
United States to each recruit two potential research participants items included: ‘‘I feel a personal obligation to do whatever I can
as one potential option to fulfill a course requirement. In order to to help my subordinate achieve his/her goals,’’ ‘‘I owe it to my
meet the eligibility requirements, participants had to be willing subordinate to do what I can to ensure that s/he is well-served
to fill out a brief online survey, as well as have an immediate super- and satisfied,’’ and ‘‘I would feel an obligation to take time from
visor that was also willing to fill out an online survey. In exchange my personal schedule to help my subordinate if s/he needed my
for their participation, all employees and supervisors were entered help.’’ The coefficient alpha for this scale was .72.
6 C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

Trust Results
To measure trust, we used five items from a scale reported by
Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis (2007). Supervisors were asked to Descriptive statistics
indicate the extent to which they agreed with each statement, with
response scales ranging from 1 (‘‘Strongly Disagree’’) to 5 Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and zero-order
(‘‘Strongly Agree’’). The items included: ‘‘I would be willing to let correlations among the variables in the study, with the coefficient
my subordinate have complete control over a task that was impor- alphas for each scale shown on the diagonal.
tant to me,’’ ‘‘If my subordinate asked why a problem occurred, I
could speak freely even if I were partly to blame,’’ and ‘‘It is impor-
tant for me to have a good way to keep an eye on my subordinate Analysis
(R).’’ The coefficient alpha for this scale was .67.
To provide support for the construct validity of our measures,
Employee perceptions of justice we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis. All 45 factor loadings
We measured the employee’s perception of interpersonal and were statistically significant. Fit statistics indicated acceptable fit
informational justice using Colquitt’s (2001) scale. For interper- for the hypothesized ten-factor model and were as follows: v2
sonal justice, employees were asked to reflect on the interpersonal (900, N = 187) = 1,363.91; CFI = .90; IFI = .90; SRMR = .06. As ex-
treatment received from their supervisors. Sample items from the pected, the ten-factor measurement model fit the data significantly
four-item scales include: ‘‘Has he/she treated you in a polite man- better than alternative measurement models as judged by a chi-
ner?’’ and ‘‘Has he/she refrained from improper remarks or com- square difference test, including a model that combined employee
ments?’’ For informational justice, employees were asked to perceptions of interpersonal and informational justice rule adher-
reflect on the explanations and communications received from ence into one interactional justice rule adherence factor (v2 diff
their supervisors during decision-making procedures. Sample [9, N = 187] = 353.92, p < .05; CFI = .82; IFI = .83; SRMR = .08), a
items from the five-item scales include: ‘‘Has he/she been candid model that combined supervisor perceptions of interpersonal and
when communicating with you?’’ and ‘‘Were his/her explanations informational justice rule adherence into one interactional justice
regarding procedures reasonable?’’ The coefficient alphas for these rule adherence factor (v2 diff[9, N = 187] = 192.15, p < .05;
scales were .89 and .87, respectively. CFI = .86; IFI = .86; SRMR = .08), a model that combined benevo-
lence and integrity into one character factor (v2 diff[9,
N = 187] = 151.89, p < .05; CFI = .87; IFI = .87; SRMR = .07), and a
Supervisor self-reported justice rule adherence model that combined supervisor perceptions of employee ability,
We measured supervisor interpersonal and informational jus- benevolence, and integrity into one perceived trustworthiness fac-
tice rule adherence using an adaptation of Colquitt’s (2001) scale. tor (v2 diff[17, N = 187] = 610.72, p < .05; CFI = .77; IFI = .77;
Supervisors were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed SRMR = .08). We therefore proceeded using the hypothesized ten-
with each statement, with response scales ranging from 1 (‘‘To a factor model.
Very Small Extent’’) to 5 (‘‘To a Very Large Extent’’). For interper- We should note that in order to adequately reduce the possibil-
sonal justice rule adherence, supervisors were asked to reflect on ity of nonconvergence or improper solutions in a LISREL (Jöreskog
the interpersonal treatment of his or her employee. The four-item & Sörbom, 1996) analysis, Kline (2005) recommends obtaining at
scale included the following: ‘‘Do you treat this subordinate in a least ten (and certainly no fewer than five) observations per free
polite manner?’’ ‘‘Do you treat this subordinate with dignity?’’ parameter to be estimated. We report the results of a traditional
‘‘Do you treat this subordinate with respect?’’ and ‘‘Have you re- confirmatory factor analysis in order to confirm the relationships
frained from improper remarks or comments toward this subordi- between the manifest and latent variables. Although we use previ-
nate?’’ Unexpectedly, the scale demonstrated an unacceptable ously validated measures, these results should be interpreted with
alpha of .66 due to the impropriety item. In hindsight, supervisors caution given our sample size and number of parameters. In testing
might view the term ‘‘improper remarks’’ as vague, and perhaps as our hypotheses, however, we opted to use a ‘‘partially latent’’ ap-
less relevant to justice. Thus, dropping this item results in a scale proach, in which scale scores were used as single indicators of
that better captures the justice phenomenon of interest in this the latent variables with error variances set to (1-alpha)  variance
manuscript, and has the added benefit of reducing random error (Kline, 2005, p. 229) In light of Kline’s (2005) above mentioned rec-
(coefficient alpha = .92). For informational justice rule adherence, ommendations, a ‘‘fully latent’’ approach in cases such as ours
supervisors were asked to reflect on the explanations and commu- would reduce the likelihood of achieving acceptable fit, even if
nications given to the employee during decision-making proce- the hypothesized model were an accurate representation of reality
dures. Sample items from the five-item scale are: ‘‘Are you (Williams, Vandenberg, & Edwards, 2009). This use of single indica-
candid when communicating with this subordinate?’’ and ‘‘Are tors reduces the number of paths to be freely estimated in our final
your explanations to this subordinate regarding procedures rea- path model and alleviates concerns about such potentially impro-
sonable?’’ The coefficient alpha for this scale was .74. per solutions.
All exogenous variables were allowed to covary in our tests. We
Control variables also allowed the disturbance terms for employee interpersonal and
To control for individuals’ response tendencies on the depen- informational justice rule adherence to covary, and for supervisor
dent variables (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003), we interpersonal and informational justice rule adherence to covary,
controlled for employee neuroticism, because a general disposition in order to represent higher-order ‘‘interactional justice’’ factors.
toward negative moods may bias perceptions of interpersonal and Covarying disturbance terms are used to recognize that the endog-
informational justice (Barsky & Kaplan, 2007). We used three items enous variables likely share other common causes that are not in-
from Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, and Lucas’ (2006) scale to measure cluded in the present model (Kline, 2005; Preacher & Hayes, 2008).
this trait. Employees were asked to indicate the extent to which Scholars also point out, however, that prior research should be
they agreed with each statement, with response scales ranging used to determine whether such covariances are justified (Kline,
from 1 (‘‘Strongly Disagree’’) to 5 (‘‘Strongly Agree’’). Sample items 2005). Interpersonal and informational justice were first conceptu-
include: ‘‘I have frequent mood swings,’’ and ‘‘I get upset easily.’’ alized as a single interactional justice facet (Bies & Moag, 1986).
The coefficient alpha for this scale was .73. Scholars have recently argued on both conceptual and empirical
C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12 7

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations.

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Perceived employee ability 4.53 .53 (.91)
2. Perceived employee benevolence 4.14 .64 .44* (.90)
3. Perceived employee integrity 4.35 .51 .49* .61* (.77)
4. Felt obligation 4.45 .56 .28* .35* .29* (.72)
5. Trust 4.04 .51 .40* .56* .54* .38* (.67)
6. Supervisor interpersonal justice 4.62 .49 .33* .32* .36* .35* .26* (92)
7. Supervisor informational justice 4.14 .44 .30* .30* .39* .26* .29* .34* (.74)
8. Employee interpersonal justice 4.50 .63 .15* .13  .26* .14  .21* .26* .25* (.89)
9. Employee informational justice 4.03 .71 .16* .16* .23* .08 .19* .12 .46* .48* (.87)
10. Employee neuroticism 2.24 .71 .05 .10 .08 .09 .01 .05 .01 .11 .01 (.73)

Notes: N = 187. Coefficient alphas appear in parentheses on the diagonal.


*
p < .05, two-tailed.
 
p < .10, two-tailed.

grounds that this construct be separated into two correlated facets across all possible subset regression models) using Dominance
(e.g., Bies, 2005; Colquitt et al., 2001), an assertion supported fur- Analysis 4.4 (James LeBreton). To provide an index of average
ther by tests of our data, discussed below. Similarly, we allowed importance for each predictor, we rescaled the general dominance
the disturbance terms for the mediators of felt obligation and trust statistic by dividing it by the total variance explained in the out-
to covary, in accordance with Preacher and Hayes’s (2008) recom- come (note that the columns representing rescaled dominance
mendation for such multiple mediator models. sum to 100). As Table 2 indicates, perceived ability accounts for
We controlled for employee neuroticism by including paths to 26.32% of the predictable criterion variance of felt obligation, com-
employee perceptions of interpersonal and informational justice. pared to the 48.05% accounted for by perceived benevolence and
To test our mediation predictions, we also included direct effects 25.63% accounted for by perceived integrity. Thus, Hypothesis 2a
from supervisor perceptions of employee trustworthiness (i.e., was partially supported. With trust as a dependent variable, we
ability, benevolence, and integrity) to employee perceptions of found that perceived ability accounts for 17.58% of the predictable
interpersonal and informational justice (MacKinnon, Lockwood, criterion variance of trust, compared to the 44.66% accounted for
Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002). Fit statistics indicated good by perceived benevolence and the 37.76% accounted for by
model fit: v2 (16, N = 187) = 53.97, p < .05; CFI = .96; IFI = .96; perceived integrity. Hypothesis 2b was therefore supported.
SRMR = .08.1 Fig. 2 illustrates all paths included in this model, noting Hypothesis 3 predicts a positive relationship between supervi-
estimates resulting from our tests. sor felt obligation and supervisor-reported interpersonal and
Hypothesis 1a predicted that perceived employee benevolence informational justice rule adherence. As Table 3 demonstrates, felt
and integrity were positively related to supervisor felt obligation. obligation was significantly related to supervisor reported inter-
As expected, our results demonstrated significant effects of per- personal and informational justice (b = .36 and b = .16, respec-
ceived benevolence on felt obligation (b = .26) while perceived tively), providing support for Hypothesis 3. Similarly, Hypothesis
integrity did not have a significant effect on felt obligation 4 predicts a positive relationship between supervisor-reported
(b = .11, ns). Thus, Hypothesis 1a was partially supported. Hypoth- trust and supervisor-reported interpersonal and informational
esis 1b predicted that perceived employee benevolence and integ- justice rule adherence. This hypothesis was also supported, as both
rity were positively related to supervisor trust. As expected, paths were significant (b = .21 and b = .26, respectively).
benevolence and integrity demonstrated significant effects on trust Hypothesis 5 predicts that supervisor-reported interpersonal
(b = .35 and b = .30, respectively). Thus, Hypothesis 1b was justice is positively related to employee perceptions of informa-
supported. tional justice, while Hypothesis 6 predicts that supervisor-reported
Hypothesis 2a predicted that the effects of perceived employee informational justice is positively related to employee perceptions
benevolence and integrity on supervisor felt obligation would be of informational justice. As indicated in Table 3, supervisor-re-
significantly more positive than the effects of perceived employee ported interpersonal justice was significantly related to employee
ability while Hypothesis 2b made the same prediction for supervi- perceptions of interpersonal justice (b = .32), thus providing sup-
sor trust. As expected, employee ability was not significantly port for Hypothesis 5. Supervisor-reported informational justice
related to either felt obligation or trust (b = .13 and b = .11, respec- was significantly related to employee perceptions of informational
tively) providing indirect support for Hypotheses 2a and b. To more justice (b = .49), thus providing support for Hypothesis 6.
directly compare perceived employee ability, benevolence, and We tested our four mediation hypotheses using the product of
integrity, we used dominance analysis (Azen & Budescu, 2003; coefficients approach discussed by MacKinnon et al. (2002). The
Budescu, 1993), a method that is particularly useful when evaluat- product of coefficients approach requires modeling both direct
ing the relative importance of correlated predictors (LeBreton, and indirect effects, with mediation resulting in a statistically sig-
Ployhart, & Ladd, 2004). Specifically, we computed the general nificant indirect effect. Therefore, we report the direct, indirect,
dominance statistic (the average increase in R2 for each predictor and total effects for our model in Table 4. Hypothesis 7 predicts
that felt obligation, trust, and ultimately supervisor interpersonal
justice mediate the relationship between supervisor perceptions
1
We also conducted the analysis including a path from supervisor informational of employee benevolence and employee perceptions of interper-
justice rule adherence to employee perceptions of interpersonal justice and a path sonal justice. Hypothesis 8 makes the same prediction in terms
from supervisor interpersonal justice rule adherence to employee perceptions of of informational justice. As seen in Table 4, LISREL’s effect decom-
informational justice (v2 [14, N = 187] = 49.12; CFI = .96; IFI = .96; SRMR = .07), but it position demonstrates that the indirect effects of supervisor per-
did not alter our findings. The path for supervisor informational justice rule
adherence to employee perceptions of interpersonal justice was significant (b = .15,
ceptions of employee benevolence on employee perceptions of
p < .05), but the path between supervisor interpersonal justice rule adherence and interpersonal and informational justice were statistically signifi-
employee perceptions of informational justice was not (b = .10, ns). cant (.05 and .06, respectively), supporting Hypotheses 7 and 8.
8 C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

Fig. 2. The hypothesized model and results. v2 (16, N = 187) = 53.97; CFI = .96; IFI = .96; SRMR = .08. Values shown are unstandardized regression coefficients. p < .05, one-
tailed. Hypothesized paths are illustrated with thick lines. Significant paths are illustrated with solid lines, while non-significant paths are illustrated with dotted lines.

Table 2 of interpersonal justice. Hypothesis 10 makes the same prediction


Dominance analysis results. for informational justice. As seen in Table 4, LISREL’s effect decom-
Predictable criterion variance explained position demonstrates that the indirect effects of perceived
Predictors Felt obligation (%) Trust (%)
integrity on interpersonal and informational justice were statisti-
cally significant (.03 and .05, respectively). However, as we did
Perceived employee ability 26.32 17.58
not previously find a significant effect of perceptions of employee
Perceived employee benevolence 48.05 44.66
Perceived employee integrity 25.63 37.76 integrity on felt obligation in our test of Hypothesis 1, this indirect
effect is likely transferred primarily through the trust mediator.
Notes: Predictable criterion variance explained was computed by dividing the
Therefore, we interpret the results of this test in conjunction with
general dominance statistic for each predictor (or the average increase in R2 for each
predictor across all possible subset regression models) by the total variance in the the results of our tests of Hypothesis 1 as partial support for
outcome explained by all predictors. Hypotheses 9 and 10.

Discussion
Hypothesis 9 predicts that felt obligation, trust, and ultimately
supervisor-reported interpersonal justice mediate the relationship In addition to proposing employee trustworthiness, conceptu-
between perceived employee integrity and employee perceptions alized as perceived benevolence and integrity (Mayer et al., 1995),

Table 3
Structural equation modeling results.

Variables Felt obligation Trust Supervisor self-reported justice rule adherence Employee perceptions of justice
Interpersonal Informational Interpersonal Informational
Perceived employee ability .13 .11 – – .08 .11
Perceived employee benevolence .26* .35* – – .11 .06
Perceived employee integrity .11 .30* – – .26* .09
Felt obligation – – .36* .16* – –
Trust – – .21* .26* – –
*
Neuroticism – – – – .22 .01
Supervisor interpersonal – – – – .32* –
Supervisor informational – – – – – .49*
R2 .17* .43* .24* .13* .19* .24*

Notes: The model included all indirect paths described and direct paths from perceived trustworthiness (ability, benevolence, and integrity) to employee perceptions of
interpersonal and informational justice. v2 (16, N = 187) = 53.97; CFI = .96; IFI = .96; SRMR = .08. The path coefficients shown are unstandardized path coefficients.
*
p < .05, one-tailed.
C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12 9

Table 4 more directly targeted at the supervisor. That difference may ac-
Effect Decomposition for Justice Perceptions. count for the stronger effects of benevolence on felt obligation than
Effect decomposition Total effect Indirect effect Direct effect of integrity on felt obligation.
On employee perceptions of interpersonal justice While our primary focus was on the benevolence and integrity
Supervisor perceptions of employee trustworthiness dimensions of trustworthiness, we also contrasted them to the
Ability .06 .02 .08 effects of ability. As expected, benevolence had significantly stron-
Benevolence .06 .05* .11 ger positive effects on supervisor felt obligation and trust than did
*
Integrity .29 .03* .26*
Employee neuroticism .22* – .22*
ability. However, while integrity had a significantly stronger posi-
tive effect on trust than did ability, the two did not differ signifi-
On employee perceptions of informational justice
Supervisor perceptions of employee trustworthiness
cantly in strength of effect on felt obligation. Although integrity
Ability .09 .02 .11 and ability are certainly valuable resources, benevolence, by defini-
Benevolence .00 .06* .06 tion, seems likely to evoke stronger supervisory feelings of obliga-
Integrity .13 .05* .09 tion and trust. Given that benevolence entails demonstrating
Employee neuroticism .01 – .01
support for one’s supervisor, trust and feelings of obligation should
Notes: Values shown are unstandardized path coefficients. naturally result on the part of the supervisor when benevolence is
*
p < .05, one-tailed. perceived to be high. After all, as mentioned previously, benevo-
lence is target-specific (e.g., demonstrating loyalty), while integrity
and ability are more general in nature.
In support of our assertions, Colquitt, LePine, Piccolo, Zapata,
as a new predictor of supervisor justice rule adherence, our study and Rich (2012) recently argued that although both affect and cog-
used the dominant lens in the justice effects literature, namely nition-based trust demonstrate beneficial effects on outcomes such
social exchange theory (Blau, 1964), to provide a cognitive expla- as employee performance, they likely do so through different
nation for why supervisors adhere to more discretionary justice mechanisms. Specifically, their results demonstrated that affect-
rules. For social exchange relationships to flourish, each party based trust (benevolence) led to increased obligations in the form
must feel obligated to reciprocate the benefits provided by the of normative commitment whereas cognition-based trust (ability
other and trust the other to reciprocate in kind (Blau, 1964). and integrity) decreased perceptions of uncertainty. Given our fo-
According to Blau (1964), felt obligation and trust result from cus on social exchange and felt obligation in particular, our results
the receipt of valued benefits. We reasoned that benevolence demonstrating a stronger effect of benevolence on obligation and
and integrity were two potential benefits employees can provide trust are consistent with Colquitt et al.’s (2012) results. It is also
to their supervisors (Blau, 1964). Therefore, we predicted that important to note that while our assessments of these constructs
supervisors would be more likely to trust and feel obligated to- are supervisor-reported and thus same-source, these differential
ward employees that they perceived to have higher benevolence results should not be attributed to common method variance
and integrity, and subsequently to reciprocate with increased (see Podsakoff et al., 2003).
adherence to interpersonal and informational justice rules. Fur- Our findings regarding the links between supervisory assess-
ther, we proposed that supervisory adherence to justice rules ments of justice rule adherence and employee perceptions of
would affect employee perceptions of interpersonal and informa- justice contribute a test of a common assumption in the justice
tional justice. literature. Up until this point, there has been an assumed link be-
As expected, our results demonstrated that supervisor percep- tween actual justice rule adherence and employees’ subsequent
tions of employee benevolence were positively associated with perceptions of justice. Our data corroborate this link, by demon-
employee perceptions of interpersonal and informational justice strating a significant effect between supervisors’ self-perceived
through supervisor feelings of obligation and trust, as well as interpersonal and informational justice rule adherence and em-
supervisor adherence to interpersonal and informational justice ployee perceptions of interpersonal and informational justice.
rules. Thus, when employees are seen as demonstrating caring Though we are unaware of any studies measuring perceptions
and concern for the supervisor (Mayer et al., 1995), supervisors’ of justice rule adherence from both perspectives (i.e., the employ-
feelings of obligation and trust in the employee do in fact lead to ee and the supervisor), our results are consistent with past re-
increased respect, propriety, truthfulness, and justification (i.e., search in related literatures. For example, Heidemeier and
interpersonal and informational justice), which in turn result in Moser (2009) demonstrated an uncorrected meta-analytic corre-
higher employee perceptions of interpersonal and informational lation of .22 between self-reported and supervisory ratings of
justice. job performance, which is consistent with previous meta-analyses
We also found similar results for employee integrity, in that a (see also Harris & Schaubroeck, 1988). In addition, meta-analytic
supervisor’s perceptions of employee’s integrity were related to data on leader-member exchange relationships suggests moder-
employee perceptions of interpersonal and informational justice ate agreement (r = .29 uncorrected) between employee and super-
through trust and supervisor justice rule adherence. However, we visor perceptions of leader-member exchange quality (Gerstner &
did not find a significant effect of integrity on felt obligation. Day, 1997).
Although we certainly expected the results for integrity to mirror Interestingly, our results demonstrate stronger effects for
the results found for benevolence, in hindsight there may be rea- informational justice than interpersonal justice. This result is rea-
sons why trust was a more powerful mediator of the effects of sonable if one considers the nature of interpersonal and informa-
integrity than felt obligation. Often, adherence to informational tional justice rule adherence. When compared to interpersonal
or interactional justice rules requires supervisors to share sensitive justice, informational justice may be less subjective in nature. For
information with their employees. This sharing is facilitated when example, informational justice rules should demonstrate a stron-
supervisors trust the employees concerned, and perceived integrity ger effect than interpersonal justice rules because a supervisor
is a strong predictor of trust (Mayer & Davis, 1999; Mayer & Gavin, objectively either provides or withholds explanations from his or
2005). Whereas demonstrations of integrity and benevolence are her employees, whereas a supervisor’s demonstrated level of
discretionary, the former is directed at a wider range of targets, sensitivity and concern toward his or her employees (i.e., interper-
including the task and the supervisor, whereas benevolence is sonal justice) is more subjective in nature.
10 C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

Practical implications trustworthiness as predictors of trust and ultimately risk-taking


(Mayer et al., 1995). In addition, felt obligation and trust are
Taken together, our results offer a number of practical implica- conceptualized as one’s own perceptions about another, in this
tions. Because of the importance of employees’ perceptions of case, the supervisor’s feelings of obligation toward and trust in
justice to a wide variety of outcomes, justice scholars have begun his or her employee. Because social exchange assumes unspeci-
to examine ways to increase those perceptions. For instance, a fied obligations, it is important to measure felt obligations from
handful of studies have demonstrated that justice training results the reciprocator’s perspective. Similarly, since trust is in the eyes
in an increase in employees’ perceptions of justice (for a review, of the beholder, it is important to measure it from the supervi-
see Skarlicki & Latham, 2005). However, this area of research sor’s perspective in our model. To minimize concerns regarding
focuses primarily on the role that supervisors play in employees’ common method bias, we used different sources when theoreti-
perceptions of justice. As stated previously, there are very few cally appropriate. More importantly, our main independent and
studies that have examined the effects of employees’ characteris- dependent variables, supervisor perceptions of employee trust-
tics and behaviors on justice perceptions. For example, Korsgaard worthiness and employee perceptions of interpersonal and infor-
et al. (1998) evaluated employee assertiveness, while Scott et al. mational justice, were not same source. Therefore, the mediation
(2007) examined employee charisma. However, assertiveness effects demonstrated in our data should not be an artifact of
and charisma may be less mutable than benevolence and integrity. method variance. In addition, we should also note that we theo-
Our findings suggest that employees may be able to influence rized and found significant differences between perceptions of
their supervisors’ adherence to interpersonal and informational ability and benevolence on supervisor felt obligation and trust.
justice rules by demonstrating high levels of benevolence and These results are also not attributable to common method
integrity. variance.
Scholars suggest that perceptions of benevolence take time to A second limitation of our study is that we did not include the
develop through employees’ demonstrations of loyalty and altru- supervisor’s trust propensity (i.e., a general tendency to trust
ism toward their supervisors (Mayer et al., 1995; Schoorman [Mayer et al., 1995]) in our controls. Given its relationship with
et al., 2007). Further, such demonstrations must not be perceived perceptions of trustworthiness and trust, it should be included
to be motivated by extrinsic rewards, but rather by a personal in future research on the effects of trust-related constructs on so-
attachment and a genuine desire to help the supervisor (Mayer cial exchange processes. Another potential limitation concerns
et al., 1995). Also, research in the area of integrity has proposed the cross-sectional nature of our data. According to Pedhazur
specific behaviors that can increase perceptions of one’s level of and Schmelkin (1991), causation cannot be inferred without
integrity (e.g., Whitener, Brodt, Korsgaard, & Werner, 1998). For manipulating the independent variable. In a similar vein, Stone-
example, Simons (2002) has argued that the alignment of one’s Romero and Rosopa (2004) have asserted that longitudinal or
words and deeds is critically important to building a reputation experimental data is needed to confirm mediation. Although con-
of behavioral integrity. Similarly, Dasgupta (1988) has emphasized ceptualizing employee ability, benevolence, and integrity as ante-
the importance of keeping promises as a way of increasing percep- cedents to justice is more theoretically appropriate than
tions of one’s integrity. Thus, by increasing supervisory perceptions suggesting the reverse, future research should utilize either
of their integrity by acting reliably and consistently (e.g., meeting experimental or cross-lagged data to establish causation
agreed-upon deadlines, keeping your word, etc.), employees may empirically.
be able to influence the kind of treatment they receive from their
supervisors (i.e., increased interpersonal and informational justice Suggestions for future research
rule adherence).
Importantly, our findings suggest that rather than being passive While the current study suggests that employees are not
recipients of justice from supervisors, employees can influence their merely passive recipients of justice, future research should also
own justice climate at work through demonstrations of benevolence examine the effects of supervisor characteristics on justice.
and integrity. While this does not absolve supervisors from a moral Although we know of no published studies examining supervisor
obligation to adhere to justice rules, it suggests that employees may characteristics as antecedents of justice, justice scholars have be-
be more empowered than previously assumed in effectuating their gun to propose some potentially important traits. For example,
experience of justice. While employees usually cannot choose their Patient and Skarlicki (2005) suggested supervisor empathy as
supervisors, they can choose to engage in behaviors that foster per- an important characteristic that might influence interpersonal
ceptions of their benevolence and integrity. justice. Specifically, supervisors who are sensitive to the emo-
tions of others should be more likely to treat employees with re-
Limitations spect and propriety. In addition to trustworthiness, other
employee characteristics and behaviors, such as proactivity,
As with all studies, there are some limitations which should may also influence employees’ experiences of justice through
be noted. First, our measures of employee trustworthiness, trust, their effects on supervisory perceptions, and should be explored
and felt obligation were same-source, raising concerns about in future research.
potential common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2003). However, Although it is likely that both supervisor and employee
using the supervisor as the source for both constructs seemed characteristics have independent effects on justice, it may prove
most appropriate, and is consistent with prior research. For fruitful to examine supervisor-employee characteristics concur-
example, past research tends to evaluate trustworthiness from rently. Research in the area of person-supervisor (PS) fit—or the
the perspective of the trustor rather than self-reported by the congruence between the goals, values, or personalities of an
trustee (e.g., Levin et al., 2006; Mayer & Gavin, 2005). Of course, employee and his or her supervisor—suggests that employee-
one might argue that ability and integrity can be measured supervisor congruence positively affects one’s satisfaction with
objectively, such as with IQ or integrity tests. However, the his or her supervisor (Kristof-Brown, Zimmerman, & Johnson,
trust literature emphasizes the importance of perceptions of 2005). Research in the area of relational demography has resulted
C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12 11

in similar conclusions. In fact, similarity on a wide variety of References


factors, such as age, race (for a review, see Riordan, 2000),
personality (Liao, Joshi, & Chuang, 2004), and functional Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–299). New York: Academic Press.
background (Randel & Jaussi, 2003) can affect work attitudes
Azen, R., & Budescu, D. V. (2003). The dominance analysis approach for comparing
and behaviors (e.g., satisfaction with one’s supervisor; Vecchio predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Methods, 8, 129–148.
& Bullis, 2001). Researchers could examine how justice-related Barsky, A., & Kaplan, S. A. (2007). If you feel bad, it’s unfair: A quantitative synthesis
of affect and organizational justice perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92,
processes may explain these findings.
286–295.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
Appendix A. Adapted scales used in this study attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117,
497–529.
Bies, R. J. (2005). Are procedural and interactional justice conceptually distinct? In J.
Felt obligation (items drawn from Eisenberger et al., 2001; Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The handbook of organizational justice
a = .72) (pp. 85–112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
1. I feel a personal obligation to do whatever I can to help my Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. F. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of
fairness. In R. J. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.). Research on
subordinate achieve his/her goals. negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43–55). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
2. I owe it to my subordinate to do what I can to ensure that Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley.
s/he is well-served and satisfied. Budescu, D. V. (1993). Dominance analysis: A new approach to the problem of
relative importance of predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Bulletin,
3. I would feel an obligation to take time from my personal
114, 542–551.
schedule to help my subordinate if s/he needed my help. Butler, J. K. (1995). Behaviors, trust, and goal achievement in a win-win negotiating
Trust (items drawn from Schoorman et al., 2007) role play. Group Organ Manage, 20, 486–501.
1. My subordinate keeps my interests in mind when making Cohen, R. L., & Greenberg, J. (1982). The justice concept in social psychology. In J.
Greenberg & R. L. Cohen (Eds.), Equity and justice in social behavior (pp. 1–41).
decisions. New York: Academic Press.
2. I would be willing to let my subordinate have complete Cohen-Charash, Y., & Spector, P. E. (2001). The role of justice in organizations: A
control over a task that was important to me. meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86,
287–321.
3. If my subordinate asked why a problem occurred, I could Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct
speak freely even if I were partly to blame. validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386–400.
4. It is important for me to have a good way to keep an eye on Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice
at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice
my subordinate. research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425–445.
5. Increasing my vulnerability to criticism by my subordinate Colquitt, J. A., Greenberg, J., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2005). What is organizational
would be a mistake. justice? A historical overview. In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The
handbook of organizational justice (pp. 3–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Supervisor justice rule adherence (items adapted from Colquitt, J. A. (2012). Organizational justice. In S. W. J. Kozlowski (Ed.), The Oxford
handbook of industrial/organizational psychology (Vol. 1) (pp. 526–547). New
Colquitt, 2001) York: Oxford University Press.
Interpersonal (a = .92) Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., Piccolo, R. F., Zapata, C. P., & Rich, B. L. (2012). Explaining
The questions below refer to the way you treat this particular the justice-performance relationship: Trust as social exchange or trust as
uncertainty reduction? Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 1–15.
subordinate when carrying out decision-making
Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., & LePine, J. A. (2007). Trust, trustworthiness, and trust
procedures. To what extent: propensity: A meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking
1. Do you treat this subordinate in a polite manner? and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 909–927.
2. Do you treat this subordinate with dignity? Conlon, D. E., Meyer, C. J., & Nowakowski, J. M. (2005). How does organizational
justice affect performance, withdrawal, and counterproductive behavior? In J.
3. Do you treat this subordinate with respect? Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The handbook of organizational justice
4. Has he/she refrained from improper remarks or (pp. 301–328). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
comments? Cropanzano, R., Byrne, Z. S., Bobocel, D. R., & Rupp, D. R. (2001). Moral virtues,
fairness heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organizational justice.
Informational (a = .74) Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 164–209.
Cropanzano, R., & Greenberg, J. (1997). Progress in organizational justice: Tunneling
The questions below refer to the explanations and through the maze. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.). International review of
communications you give this particular subordinate about industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 317–372). New York: Wiley.
decision making procedures. To what extent: Dasgupta, P. (1988). Trust as a commodity. In D. Gambetta (Ed.), Trust: Making and
breaking cooperative relations (pp. 49–72). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
1. Are you candid when communicating with this Dirks, K. R., & Ferrin, D. L. (2002). Trust in leadership: Meta-analytic findings and
subordinate? implications for research and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87,
2. Have you thoroughly explained decision-making proce- 611–628.
Dirks, K. R., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2009). The relationship between being perceived as
dures to this subordinate? trustworthy by coworkers and individual performance. Journal of Management,
3. Are your explanations to this subordinate regarding proce- 35, 136–157.
dures reasonable? Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). The mini-IPIP
scales: Tiny yet effective measures of the big five factors of personality.
4. Have you communicated details to this subordinate in a
Psychological Assessment, 18, 192–203.
timely manner? Eddleston, K. A., Veiga, J. F., & Powell, G. N. (2006). Explaining sex differences in
Have you tailored communications to meet this subordinate’s managerial career satisfier preferences: The role of gender self-schema. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 91, 437–445.
individuals’ needs?
Eisenberger, R., Armeli, S., Rexwinkel, B., Lynch, P. D., & Rhoades, L. (2001).
Neuroticism (items drawn from Donnellan et al., 2006; Reciprocation of perceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology,
a = .73) 86, 42–51.
1. I have frequent mood swings. Emerson, R. M. (1976). Social exchange theory. Annual Review of Sociology, 2,
335–362.
2. I am relaxed most of the time. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7,
3. I get upset easily. 117–140.
Foa, E. B., & Foa, U. G. (1980). Resource theory: Interpersonal behavior as exchange.
Note: The coefficient alphas (a) listed were computed from the present study’s In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, & R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in
sample. theory and research (pp. 77–94). New York: Plenum Press.
12 C.P. Zapata et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 121 (2013) 1–12

Gabarro, L. (1978). The development of trust, influence, and expectations. In A. G. McAllister, D. J. (1995). Affect and cognition-based trust as foundations for
Athos & J. J. Gabarro (Eds.), Interpersonal behavior: Communication and interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal,
understanding in relationships (pp. 290–303). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 38, 24–59.
Gerstner, C. R., & Day, D. V. (1997). Meta-analytic review of leader-member Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. (2006). The work design questionnaire (WDQ):
exchange theory: Correlates and construct issues. Journal of Applied Psychology, Developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design
82, 827–844. and the nature of work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1321–1339.
Greenberg, J. (1993). The social side of fairness: Interpersonal and informational Patient, D. L., & Skarlicki, D. P. (2005). Why managers don’t always do the right thing
classes of organizational justice. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: when delivering bad news: The role of empathy, self-esteem, and moral
Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. 79–103). Hillsdale, NJ: development in interactional fairness. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, D. P.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Skarlicki, & Van den Bos, K. (Eds.), What motivates fairness in organizations? (pp.
Harris, M. M., & Schaubroeck, J. (1988). A meta-analysis of self- supervisor, self-peer, 149–178). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
and peer-supervisor ratings. Personnel Psychology, 48, 151–160. Pedhazur, E., & Schmelkin, L. P. (1991). Measurement, design, and analysis: An
Heidemeier, H., & Moser, K. (2009). Self-other agreement in job performance integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
ratings: A meta-analytic test of a process model. Journal of Applied Psychology, Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method
94, 353–370. biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and
Holmes, J. G. (1981). The exchange process in close relationships: Microbehavior recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903.
and macromotives. In M. J. Lerner & S. C. Lerner (Eds.), The justice motive in social Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for
behavior (pp. 261–284). New York: Plenum. assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior
Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behaviour: Its elementary forms. London: Routledge & Research Methods, 40, 879–891.
Kegan Paul. Randel, A. E., & Jaussi, K. S. (2003). Functional background identity, diversity, and
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8 user’s reference guide. Moorseville, IN: individual performance in cross-functional teams. Academy of Management
Scientific Software, Inc.. Journal, 46, 763–774.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Riordan, C. M. (2000). Relational demography within groups: Past developments,
Guilford Press. contradictions, and new directions. In G. R. Ferris (Ed.). Research in personnel
Korsgaard, M. A., Roberson, L., & Rymph, R. D. (1998). What motivates fairness? The and human resources Management (Vol. 19, pp. 131–173). New York: JAI Press.
role of subordinate assertive behavior on managers’ interactional fairness. Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (2007). An integrative model of
Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 731–744. organizational trust: Past, present, and future. Academy of Management Review,
Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. (2005). Consequences of 32, 344–354.
individuals’ fit at work: A meta-analysis of person-job, person-organization, Scott, B. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Paddock, E. L. (2009). An actor-focused model of justice
person-group, and person-supervisor fit. Personnel Psychology, 58, 281–342. rule adherence and violation: The role of managerial motives and discretion.
LeBreton, J. M., Ployhart, R. E., & Ladd, R. T. (2004). A Monte Carlo comparison of Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 756–769.
relative importance methodologies. Organizational Research Methods, 7, Scott, B. A., Colquitt, J. A., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2007). Justice as a dependent
258–282. variable: Subordinate charisma as a predictor of interpersonal and
LePine, J. A., & Van Dyne, L. (2001). Voice and cooperative behavior as contrasting informational justice perceptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1597–1609.
forms of contextual performance. Evidence of differential relationships with big Simons, T. (2002). Behavioral integrity: The perceived alignment between
five personality characteristics and cognitive ability. Journal of Applied managers’ words and deeds as a research focus. Organization Science, 13, 18–35.
Psychology, 86, 326–336. Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1996). Increasing citizenship behavior within a labor
Leventhal, G. S. (1976). The distribution of rewards and resources in groups and union: A test of organizational justice theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81,
organizations. In L. Berkowitz & W. Walster (Eds.). Advances in Experimental 161–169.
Social Psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 91–131). New York: Academic Press. Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (1997). Leadership training in organizational justice
Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to increase citizenship behavior within a labor union: A replication. Personnel
to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg, & R. Psychology, 50, 617–633.
Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27–55). New Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (2005). How can training be used to foster
York: Plenum Press. organizational justice? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The handbook of
Levin, D. Z., Whitener, E. M., & Cross, R. (2006). Perceived trustworthiness of organizational justice (pp. 3–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
knowledge sources: The moderating impact of relationship length. Journal of Stone-Romero, E. F., & Rosopa, P. J. (2004). Inference problems with hierarchical
Applied Psychology, 91, 1163–1171. multiple regression-based tests of mediating effects. In J. Martocchio (Ed.).
Liao, H., Joshi, A., & Chuang, A. C. (2004). Sticking out like a sore thumb: Employee Research in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 23, pp. 249–290).
dissimilarity and deviance at work. Personnel Psychology, 57, 969–1000. Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Tekleab, A. G., Takeuchi, R., & Taylor, M. S. (2005). Extending the chain of
Plenum Press. relationships among organizational justice, social exchange, and employee
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A reactions: The role of contract violations. Academy of Management Journal, 48,
comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. 146–157.
Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale,
Martins, L. L., Eddleston, K. A., & Veiga, J. F. (2002). Moderators of the relationship NJ: Erlbaum.
between work-family conflict and career satisfaction. Academy of Management Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1978). A theory of procedure. California Law Review, 66,
Journal, 45, 399–409. 541–566.
Masterson, S. S., Byrne, Z. S., & Mao, H. (2005). Interpersonal and informational Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. P.
justice: Identifying the differential antecedents of interactional justice Zanna (Ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115–191).
behaviors. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, & D. P. Skarlicki (Eds.), What San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
motivates fairness in organizations? (pp 79–103). Greenwich, CT: Information Vecchio, R. P., & Bullis, R. C. (2001). Moderators of the influence of supervisor-
Age Publishing. subordinate similarity on subordinate outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Masterson, S. S., Lewis, K., Goldman, B. M., & Taylor, M. S. (2000). Integrating justice 86, 884–896.
and social exchange: The differing effects of fair procedures and treatment on Weiner, B., Frieze, L. H., Kukia, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. (1971).
work relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 738–748. Perceiving the causes of success and failure. Morristown, NJ: General Learning
Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (1999). The effect of the performance appraisal system on Press.
trust for management: A field quasi-experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, Whitener, E. M., Brodt, S. E., Korsgaard, M. A., & Werner, J. M. (1998). Managers as
84, 123–136. initiators of trust: An exchange relationship framework for understanding
Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of managerial trustworthy behavior. The Academy of Management Review, 23,
organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20, 709–734. 513–530.
Mayer, R. C., & Gavin, M. B. (2005). Trust in management and performance: Who Williams, L. J., Vandenberg, R. J., & Edwards, J. R. (2009). Structural equation
minds the shop while the employees watch the boss? Academy of Management modeling in management research: A guide for improved analysis. The Academy
Journal, 48, 874–888. of Management Annals, 3, 543–604.
Mayer, D. M., Kuenzi, M., Greenbaum, R., Bardes, M., & Salvador, R. (2009). How low
does ethical leadership flow? Test of a trickle-down model. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108, 1–13.

You might also like