1 s2.0 S0191886921002968 Main

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Beauty is in the eye of the beer holder but rarely because of the beer
Tobias Otterbring a, c, *, Kristian Rolschau b
a
School of Business and Law, Department of Management, University of Agder, Universitetsveien 17, 4630 Kristiansand, Norway
b
Department of Management/MAPP, Aarhus University, Fuglesangs Allé 4, 8210 Aarhus V, Denmark
c
Institute of Retail Economics, Kungsgatan 27, 11156 Stockholm, Sweden

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Across three studies, the present research examined beliefs and real-world responses pertaining to whether bar
Time patrons’ self-rated attractiveness would be higher later in the night. Contrary to beliefs held by lay people (Study
Closing time effect 1A) and researchers in relevant disciplines (Study 1B), the results of a field study (Study 2) revealed that patrons
Sex differences
perceived themselves as more attractive at later times, regardless of the amount of alcohol consumed. Rela­
Attractiveness
Relationship status
tionship status moderated this time-contingent finding, which only applied to patrons who were single. However,
Sexual strategies theory consistent with sexual strategies theory, this interplay was further moderated by the patrons’ sex. Men rated
themselves as more attractive later in the night irrespective of their relationship status, whereas this “pretty”
pattern only held for single women. Taken together, the current work highlights the concept of time in forming
consumers’ evaluative judgments and adds to the literature on the closing time effect.

“From a 2 at 10 to a 10 at 2.” perceived value because aspects that are difficult to obtain are seen as
Bar wisdom more attractive (Cialdini, 2009a; Lopes et al., 2019; Lynn, 1991). Mere
exposure effects postulate that people build preferences based on
While the notion that “people get prettier at closing time” might
repeated exposure, partially because familiarity breeds attraction (Fang
seem like an urban legend, several studies support this thesis. The
et al., 2007; Peskin & Newell, 2004; Zajonc, 1968). Yet, Gladue and
closing time effect, as the literature has coined it, proposes that bar
Delaney (1990) found women to be less likely than men to showcase the
patrons rate opposite-sex, but not same-sex, individuals as more
closing time effect, while Madey et al. (1996) found this effect to be
attractive during closing time than during earlier times of a bar’s
restricted to single patrons, thereby bringing forth questions about sex
opening hours (Johnco et al., 2010; Nida & Koon, 1983; Pennebaker
differences and relationship status that existing theories cannot neces­
et al., 1979). With the basis in Brehm’s (1972) reactance theory, Pen­
sarily explain.
nebaker et al. (1979) proposed that time restrictions will threaten the
A common critique of research on the closing time effect is the
freedom to choose a partner, which, in turn, will cause patrons to in­
amount of alcohol consumed. Indeed, another well-known phenome­
crease their attractiveness ratings of potential partners in such con­
non, referred to as the beer goggles effect, suggests that individuals’
sumption contexts. As the threat becomes bigger when the closing time
attractiveness perceptions are influenced by real or imagined alcohol
approaches, so will the magnitude of the reactance.
intoxication (Bègue et al., 2013;Jones et al., 2003; Lyvers et al., 2011).
However, later studies have presented conflicting results and offered
According to the beer goggles effect, people rate themselves (Bègue
other possible explanations for the effect. For instance, Johnco et al.
et al., 2013) and opposite-sex individuals (Jones et al., 2003; Lyvers
(2010) contended that reactance theory is not an adequate explanation,
et al., 2011) as more attractive when they are drunk. As bar patrons are
since they found patrons in relationships show the same effect as single
usually more intoxicated at closing time than earlier in the evening, the
patrons, despite that individuals without a partner should arguably have
confound with the closing time effect seems obvious.
a stronger desire to find one compared to those already in a relationship.
The present research aimed to examine the closing time effect from a
Johnco et al. (2010) contended that better explanations come from
new angle, while simultaneously considering the potential impact of the
commodity theory and mere exposure effects. Commodity theory gives
beer goggles effect. Contrary to previous studies investigating time-
the broader prediction that scarcity of a commodity increases its

* Corresponding author at: School of Business and Law, Department of Management, University of Agder, Universitetsveien 17, 4630 Kristiansand, Norway.
E-mail addresses: tobias.otterbring@uia.no (T. Otterbring), krro@mgmt.au.dk (K. Rolschau).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110921
Received 6 November 2020; Received in revised form 9 April 2021; Accepted 10 April 2021
Available online 10 May 2021
0191-8869/© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
T. Otterbring and K. Rolschau Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

related effects on opposite-sex attractiveness, we test predictions on self- that since people in a relationship should be less inclined than single
rated attractiveness. The closing time effect has focused on bar patrons’ people to focus on finding a mate, the threat of not finding one due to the
attractiveness perceptions of potential partners (commonly conceptu­ closing time, as postulated by reactance theory, should not impact this
alized as opposite-sex individuals) but has neglected investigations of former group with respect to their attractiveness ratings of opposite-sex
individuals’ self-rated attractiveness. For example, Johnco et al. (2010) others.
concluded that the closing time effect only occurs for potential partners, Self-perceived attractiveness is positively linked to psychological
since increases in attractiveness were found for ratings of opposite-sex, well-being (Umberson & Hughes, 1987), even when altered surgically
but not same-sex, individuals. However, such a result does not rule (Margraf et al., 2013), and higher self-rated attractiveness is correlated
out potential effects on self-ratings. Clearly, one’s own attractiveness with multiple self-view-relevant traits, such as lower anxiety and higher
plays a prominent role in attracting a partner (Buss, 1985; Jonason et al., self-esteem (Baumeister et al., 2005; Diener et al., 1995; Gupta et al.,
2009; Schmitt et al., 2012; Walter et al., 2020), so positive changes in 2015; Longo & Ashmore, 1995). This suggests that the “shaken self” of
self-rated attractiveness might be beneficial in attracting a mate. Thus, being single may be meaningfully mitigated by amplifying one’s self-
to expand on the literature, the current work investigated whether the rated attractiveness as a means of restoring self-view confidence (Gao
closing time effect applies to self-rated attractiveness rather than et al., 2009; Otterbring, 2020). Moreover, individuals in relationships
attractiveness ratings for opposite-sex individuals. have been shown to unconsciously engage in defensive mechanisms
meant to maintain their current mate (Karremans & Verwijmeren,
1. Theory and hypotheses 2008), indicating that people who are single, as opposed to in a rela­
tionship, should be more motivated to perceive themselves in a favor­
Pennebaker et al. (1979) was the first to document the closing time able fashion as a function of time. Hence, we hypothesize:
effect. Since then, several studies have attempted to replicate the orig­
Hypothesis 2. Relationship status will moderate the link between time
inal findings (Gladue & Delaney, 1990; Johnco et al., 2010; Madey et al.,
and self-rated attractiveness, with patrons who are single being more
1996; Nida & Koon, 1983; Sprecher et al., 1984). For instance, Nida and
inclined to rate themselves as attractive at later versus earlier times of a
Koon (1983) partially replicated the effect by showing that male patrons
bar’s opening hours compared to patrons who are in a relationship.
rated the attractiveness of “the women here tonight” more favorably
over the course of the night at a Country and Western bar, but not at a While relationship status should have implications for self-rated
campus bar. However, other studies have muddied the water by finding attractiveness, so should bar patrons’ sex (Gladue & Delaney, 1990).
conflicting results or offering alternative explanations. Sprecher et al. Sexual strategies theory postulates that men and women display sex-
(1984) found no evidence for the effect, and alcohol provides a specific responses in short-term mating contexts, with men typically
competing explanation as studies have shown alcohol to exert a com­ exhibiting more promiscuous sexual attitudes and behaviors (Buss &
parable impact as time in similar settings (Jones et al., 2003; Lyvers Schmitt, 1993). For example, men are more likely to accept sexual in­
et al., 2011). A few studies have tried to remedy this issue by simulta­ vitations, engage in casual sex, and have extradyadic sex (Baranowski &
neously investigating attractiveness perceptions as a function of both Hecht, 2015; Buunk & Dijkstra, 2004; Clark & Hatfield, 1989; Conley,
time and alcohol. Gladue and Delaney (1990) found no support for the 2011; Hald & Høgh-Olesen, 2010; Otterbring, 2021). These and other
thesis that increased attractiveness ratings of opposite-sex patrons is due sex differences are well-established, cross-culturally valid (Buss, 1989;
to alcohol consumption, but did find evidence of a closing time effect, Lippa, 2009; Schmitt, 2003; Walter et al., 2020), and have been shown
while Johnco et al. (2010) found opposite-sex, but not same-sex, to exert downstream effects on consumer responses (Griskevicius et al.,
attractiveness to increase over three time periods at a pub, with 2007; Otterbring et al., 2018; Sundie et al., 2011). Furthermore,
alcohol only explaining a small portion of the variance. although men and women are more similar in their preferences for short-
Undoubtedly, the closing time effect on opposite-sex ratings has term versus long-term mates and generally prioritize physical attrac­
received a fair share of attention from researchers, perhaps causing time- tiveness in short-term mating contexts, the relative importance of
contingent examinations on self-rated attractiveness to be overlooked. physical attractiveness still carries more weight for the short-term mate
Given the importance of attractiveness in mating contexts (Buss & appeal of women rather than men (Li & Kenrick, 2006). Women’s
Schmitt, 2019; Rhodes et al., 2005), especially in short-term ones (Buss relatively less promiscuous sexual attitudes and behaviors, combined
& Schmitt, 1993; Li & Kenrick, 2006), a boost in one’s self-rated with findings documenting that physical attractiveness is more impor­
attractiveness should likely improve self-perceived mate value and, tant for the mate appeal of women even in short-term mating contexts,
consequently, should be adaptive in that it may facilitate approach be­ suggest that relationship status should play a more prominent role in
haviors towards potential mates. Indeed, positive self-perceived mate shaping women’s self-rated attractiveness. Accordingly, single women
value tends to be related to one’s attractiveness to others (Zeigler-Hill & should be more likely to perceive themselves positively on the attrac­
Myers, 2011) and has been shown to facilitate intimate interactions tiveness dimension around closing time than partnered women. In
(Nezlek, 1999). Therefore, in the bar context, an increase in self-rated contrast, considering men’s relatively more promiscuous sexual atti­
attractiveness should improve one’s confidence of finding a mate. tudes and behaviors, the impact of time on their self-rated attractiveness
Together with research demonstrating that a temporary increase in self- should arguably be less influenced by their relationship status. In other
perceived mate value can shift preferences towards short-term mating words, such time-contingent attractiveness ratings should be less
(Surbey & Brice, 2007), this suggests that the closing time effect on self- differentiated between men who are single and men who are in a rela­
rated attractiveness should mirror the previously documented effects on tionship, although men, regardless of their relationship status, should
ratings of opposite-sex individuals. Accordingly, we hypothesize: draw certain mating-relevant benefits by boosting their self-perceived
beauty. Therefore, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 1. Bar patrons will rate their own attractiveness more
favorably at later versus earlier times of a bar’s opening hours. Hypothesis 3. The moderating role of relationship status on the link
between time and self-rated attractiveness will be contingent on pa­
Beyond the original findings by Pennebaker et al. (1979), Madey
trons’ sex and should be weaker for male than for female patrons.
et al. (1996) reported relationship status as a moderator of the closing
time effect, suggesting that the strength of association between time and
attractiveness ratings depends on whether the rater is single or in a
relationship. Specifically, Madey et al. (1996) hypothesized and found

2
T. Otterbring and K. Rolschau Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

2. Overview of studies Participants in Study 1B completed a paper-and-pencil survey


distributed by the first author during the welcome reception of
Across three studies, we investigate the impact of time on bar pa­ RARCS2019, which is an academic conference for scholars in retailing,
trons’ self-rated attractiveness. Given that people’s common sense and hospitality, consumer behavior, and service research. Because no
their roles as naïve or intuitive psychologists have been stressed as great explicit comparison existed for participants in Study 1A regarding the
assets for theory building (Friestad & Wright, 1995; Furnham, 1988; attractiveness question and given that the estimated number of scholars
Heider, 1958; Ross, 1977), we initially combine two pools of partici­ showing up at the welcome reception was estimated to be well below
pants to examine lay people’s (Study 1A) and researchers’ (Study 1B) 100 according to the conference organizers (making a between-subjects
beliefs pertaining to such a time-specific effect, along with its attributed design under-powered), Study 1B used a within-subjects design. Thus,
cause. Specifically, we test whether people generally believe patrons to the conference attendees received both attractiveness questions, with
rate themselves as more attractive later in the night and, if so, whether the order of these questions (time: earlier, later) randomized through
this is mainly attributed to the closing time effect or the beer goggles two different sets of surveys that had been mixed arbitrarily prior to data
effect. Next, we examine the validity of these beliefs at a bar (Study 2). collection. This alternative design should have made it easier for par­
Moreover, we test whether the presumed closing time effect is contin­ ticipants to make their attractiveness assessments, as they could
gent on bar patrons’ relationship status and sex, and whether alcohol compare their beliefs on how bar patrons would rate their own attrac­
intoxication may impact this interplay. tiveness across two distinct time points.3 Following the two attractive­
ness items, participants then replied to the other items from Study 1A
3. Studies 1A–B: beliefs about the closing time effect using the same response formats.

3.1. Purpose and participants


3.3. Results
Studies 1A–B sought to shed light on individuals’ beliefs corre­
sponding to the impact of time on bar patrons’ self-rated attractiveness. Across Studies 1A–B, participants thought that bar patrons would
In addition, we aimed to investigate whether people mainly attribute the rate their own level of attractiveness more favorably during later versus
effect to time or alcohol consumption, while also examining their level earlier times of a bar’s opening hours; see Fig. 1. Indeed, one-way
of certainty in such beliefs. To this end, we conducted one study (Study ANOVAs, with time as a between-subjects factor in Study 1A and as a
1A; N = 117; 64% male)1 on a mixed student/community sample rep­ within-subjects factor in Study 1B, consistently revealed a significant
resenting lay people (cf. Heider, 1958; Molden & Dweck, 2006; difference between conditions in attractiveness responses among lay
Otterbring & Mitkidis, 2018). These responses were subsequently com­ people (Study 1A: F(1, 115) = 4.96, p = .028, η2 = 0.04) as well as re­
plemented with the beliefs held by researchers in relevant disciplines searchers (Study 1B: F(1, 52) = 11.75, p = .001, η2 = 0.18).
through another study (Study 1B; N = 55; 51% male, Mage = 44.93, SD Participants also agreed with the statement postulating that alcohol
= 11.87) on attendees at the academic conference “Recent Advances in would play a larger role than time in influencing bar patrons’ self-rated
Retailing and Consumer Services” (RARCS2019). The combination of attractiveness, as evidenced by one-sample t-tests, which showed
these studies enabled us to examine the robustness of the results across significantly higher agreement scores than the scale midpoint of 4
sample types, settings, and study designs, as recommended in prior (Study 1A: M = 5.51, SD = 1.43, t(116) = 11.44, p < .001; Study 1B: M
research contrasting common-sense psychology with scientific psy­ = 5.10, SD = 1.42, t(49) = 5.49, p < .001). Finally, participants
chology (Friestad & Wright, 1995; Kelley, 1992). expressed certainty in these predictions, with one-sample t-tests
demonstrating significantly higher certainty ratings than the neutral
response option indicated by the scale midpoint of 4 (Study 1A: M =
3.2. Materials and procedure
5.32, SD = 1.32, t(116) = 10.75, p < .001; Study 1B: M = 4.80, SD =
1.59, t(49) = 3.56, p < .001), with 76.1% (Study 1A) and 62.0% (Study
Studies 1A-B used identical measures but implemented them in
slightly different ways. Participants in Study 1A completed an online
survey through Qualtrics and were randomly assigned to one of two 7
conditions using a single-factor between-subjects design (time: earlier,
later). Specifically, they received the following question, with the text in 6
the brackets representing the “earlier” condition and the contrasting
words outside the brackets representing the “later” condition: 5
How do you think bar patrons rate their own level of attractiveness at
later [earlier] times of a bar’s opening hours; in other words, around [far 4
away from] closing time?
Next, participants indicated their response on a single-item scale (1 3
= unattractive; 7 = attractive) and subsequently replied to the state­
ment: “The amount of alcohol consumed by bar patrons has a stronger 2
impact on how they rate their own level of attractiveness than the spe­
cific time at which they are present in the bar” (1 = strongly disagree; 7 1
= strongly agree).2 Finally, participants indicated their level of certainty Lay People Researchers
in their reply (1 = very uncertain; 7 = very certain).
Early Late

Fig. 1. Assumed time effects on patrons’ self-rated attractiveness.


1
Data on participants’ age were not collected in Study 1A. Note – Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean.
2
An anonymous reviewer raised concerns regarding this item due to its
somewhat ambiguous nature, indicating that the results for this item should be
interpreted with appropriate caution. However, participants in Study 1B were
encouraged to ask questions to the first author distributing the surveys if they
found some items difficult to understand, and these participants did not express
3
any difficulties in understanding the item in question. We thank an anonymous reviewer for making this remark.

3
T. Otterbring and K. Rolschau Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

1B) of participants indicating some level of certainty (i.e., slightly were classified as “evening” responses, and the remaining responses,
certain, certain, or very certain).4 made from 9 pm to closing time (2.00 am) were categorized as “night”
responses (n = 141; 30%).5 The nature and significance of our results
3.4. Discussion remain unchanged if we analyze the data on a minute-by-minute basis in
a multiplicative moderation analysis (PROCESS Model 3), indicating
Studies 1A–B demonstrate that people believe bar patrons to rate robustness of the findings.
their own attractiveness higher at later times of the bar’s opening hours. We conducted a 3 (time: afternoon, evening, night) × 2 (relationship
This result holds across study settings and designs (between-subjects status: single, in a relationship) × 2 (patron sex: male, female) between-
versus within-subjects) and independent of whether lay people or re­ subjects ANOVA on self-rated attractiveness to test our hypotheses. This
searchers in relevant disciplines are asked. Furthermore, the attributed analysis revealed a significant main effect of time on bar patrons’ self-
cause for this effect, expressed with certainty, is the amount of alcohol rated attractiveness (F(2, 463) = 4.93, p = .008, η2p = 0.02). Consis­
consumed rather than the specific time at which patrons are present in tent with Hypothesis 1, planned contrasts revealed that patrons rated
the bar, suggesting that people generally perceive a “beer goggles”-ac­ themselves as more attractive during the night than during the afternoon
count as more plausible than a “closing time”-account. and evening (t(472) = 2.20, p = .028), whereas self-rated attractiveness
did not differ significantly between the afternoon and evening (t(472) =
4. Study 2: field study on the closing time effect 1.60, p = .111); see Fig. 2.
In line with Hypothesis 2, a two-way interaction between time and
4.1. Purpose and participants relationship status emerged (F(2, 463) = 3.53, p = .030, η2p = 0.02). For
patrons in a relationship, time did not impact self-rated attractiveness (F
Study 2 aimed to examine whether the beliefs and the attributed < 1). However, among single patrons, time had a significant impact on
cause concerning bar patrons’ self-rated attractiveness, as displayed by self-rated attractiveness (F(2, 173) = 4.44, p = .013, η2p = 0.05). Planned
participants in Studies 1A-B, could be validated under real-world con­ contrasts revealed that single patrons evaluated themselves as more
ditions in a bar. The study included 475 bar patrons (67% male; Mage = attractive during the night than during the afternoon and evening (t
28.74, SD = 8.07), of whom 176 were single and 299 were in a (173) = 2.72, p = .007), whereas self-rated attractiveness did not differ
relationship. significantly among single patrons between the afternoon and evening (t
(173) = 1.13, p = .261); See Fig. 3.
Corroborating Hypothesis 3, the three-way interaction between time,
4.2. Materials and procedure
relationship status, and patron sex was also significant (F(2, 463) = 4.12,
p = .017, η2p = 0.02). Male patrons rated themselves as more attractive
Study 2 was conducted during four consecutive weekends at a Danish
later in the night, regardless of their relationship status, as evidenced by
bar that accommodates approximately 100 patrons at peak hours. A
the significant main effect of time (F(2,311) = 3.15, p = .044, η2p = 0.02).
confederate posing as a bartender asked patrons to report in a brief
Planned contrasts revealed that male patrons’ self-rated attractiveness
paper-and-pencil survey how many beers they had consumed, their age,
was marginally higher during the night than during the afternoon and
sex, relationship status, and the time at which they completed the sur­
evening (t(314) = 1.82, p = .070), and marginally higher during the
vey. The patrons also rated how attractive (1 = unattractive; 7 =
evening than during the afternoon (t(314) = 1.90, p = .059). There was
attractive) and drunk (1 = sober; 7 = drunk) they perceived themselves
no interaction between relationship status and time on male patrons’
to be at that point in time (Brown et al., 1992; Cornil et al., 2017).
self-rated attractiveness (F < 1) and the main effect of relationship status
Single-item scales were used to reduce the intrusiveness of the survey
was also non-significant (F < 1). For female patrons, the main effect of
per wishes of the bar manager, and with the validity of such scales in
time on self-rated attractiveness was marginally significant (F(2, 152) =
mind (Bergkvist & Rossiter, 2007). Attractiveness was not specified as
2.90, p = .058, η2p = 0.04). Crucially, although the main effect of rela­
physical attractiveness as in, for instance, Johnco et al. (2010) and
tionship status was not significant (F(1, 152) = 2.07, p = .153, η2p =
Sprecher et al. (1984), but in a more generic sense (Dunn & Hill, 2014;
0.01), the interaction between time and relationship status emerged (F
Dunn & Searle, 2010). This was done to capture variance from more
than just physical appearance in the measure and because physical
attractiveness, viewed in isolation, is not always a good predictor of 7
mate value (Fisher et al., 2008).
6
4.3. Results
5
Responses were classified as a function of whether they were made
during afternoon, evening, or night according to common definitions for 4
these time-specific daily events (Cambridge English Dictionary, 2019;
Learner’s Dictionary, 2019). To this end, and to have a roughly equal 3
number of responses per time category, all responses made from 12.00
am to 5.59 pm (n = 182; 38%) were categorized as “afternoon” re­ 2
sponses, all responses made from 6.00 pm to 8.59 pm (n = 152; 32%)
1
Afternoon Evening Night
4
Interestingly, the lay people involved in Study 1A reported significantly
Fig. 2. Time effects on patrons’ self-rated attractiveness.
higher certainty levels in their predictions than the researchers involved in
Note – Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean.
Study 1B (F(1, 165) = 4.71, p = .031, η2 = 0.03), supporting the general
conclusion of the “Dunning-Kruger effect,” with unskilled people typically
being more inclined to overestimate their abilities (Kruger & Dunning, 1999).
Due to missing data in Study 1B, we got 53 usable responses for the later versus
earlier questions and 50 usable responses for the other two items (i.e., whether
5
alcohol was thought to have a stronger impact than time on bar patrons’ self- The proportion of male and female patrons who were single versus in a
rated attractiveness and the degree of certainty in this belief). relationship did not differ for any of these time categories (all χ 2 < 1).

4
T. Otterbring and K. Rolschau Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

1
Afternoon Evening Night

Single In a relationship

Fig. 3. Time × relationship status effects on patrons’ self-rated attractiveness.


Note – Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean.

(2, 152) = 6.60, p = .002, η2p = 0.08). For female patrons in a relation­ 4.4. Discussion
ship, time did not influence their self-rated attractiveness (F < 1).
However, time had a strong and significant impact on self-rated Study 2 verified the main prediction made by participants in Studies
attractiveness among female patrons who were single (F(2, 55) = 1A-B, as bar patrons rated their own level of attractiveness more
6.93, p = .002, η2p = 0.20). Indeed, planned contrasts revealed that they favorably at later times of a bar’s opening hours. However, while par­
rated themselves as significantly more attractive during the night than ticipants in Studies 1A–B attributed such self-centered attractiveness
during the evening and afternoon (t(55) = 3.48, p < .001), although perceptions to increased alcohol consumption rather than the specific
their self-rated attractiveness did not differ between the afternoon and time at which patrons are present in a bar, Study 2 found that time, but
evening (t(55) = 1.46, p = .151); see Fig. 4. not alcohol intake or the subjective sense of alcohol intoxication, pre­
Controlling for the number of beers consumed and/or how drunk the dicted bar patrons’ self-rated attractiveness. Therefore, the findings
patrons felt when completing the survey did not change the nature and from Study 2 provide stronger support for the closing time effect than
significance of these findings, and none of these alcohol-linked variables the beer goggles effect. Importantly, although patrons showed a general
were significantly associated with self-rated attractiveness. However, increase in their self-rated attractiveness around closing time, this ten­
the more objective measure of alcohol intake (i.e., the number of beers dency was moderated by their relationship status and, in turn, their sex.
consumed) showed a stronger association with self-rated attractiveness Female patrons perceived themselves as more attractive at later times
(r = 0.08, p = .091) than the subjective sense of alcohol intoxication (r = only if they were single, while such a pattern applied more generally
0.02, p = .599). This leaves some, albeit relatively weak, support for the among men, irrespective of their relationship status.
beer goggles effect.
Generally, both male (M = 5.36, SD = 1.25) and female patrons (M = 5. General discussion
5.27, SD = 1.18) rated their own attractiveness as significantly above the
scale midpoint of 4 (tmale(316) = 19.42, p < .001; tfemale(157) = 13.48, p As far as can be ascertained, this is the first scientific work to
< .001), consistent with a self-serving bias account (Kruger & Dunning, investigate the validity of the closing time effect with respect to self-
1999). rated attractiveness. Studies 1A–B found that lay people and scholars

Panel A - Men Panel B - Women

7
7
6 6
5 5
4 4
3 3

2 2

1 1
Afternoon Evening Night Afternoon Evening Night

Single In a relationship Single In a relationship

Fig. 4. Sex-specific time × relationship status effects on patrons’ self-rated attractiveness.


Note – Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean.

5
T. Otterbring and K. Rolschau Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

in relevant disciplines believe bar patrons rate their own level of offers the most parsimonious explanation for precise pattern of results
attractiveness more favorably at later times of a bar’s opening hours due seems to be sexual strategies theory (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). Future
to increased alcohol consumption. Contrary to this belief, Study 2 found research should try to replicate the current findings, while simulta­
time, but not alcohol intoxication, explained patrons’ responses in an neously investigating if the psychological mechanism underlying the
actual field setting. Specifically, patrons perceived themselves as more closing time effect differs as a function of whether the attractiveness
attractive at later times of a bar’s opening hours, regardless of the ratings relate to opposite-sex individuals or self-evaluations.
amount of alcohol consumed or how drunk they felt. Relationship status
moderated this time-contingent finding, which only applied to patrons CRediT authorship contribution statement
who were single. However, consistent with sexual strategies theory
(Buss & Schmitt, 1993), this interaction was further moderated by the Tobias Otterbring: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal anal­
patrons’ sex. Men rated themselves as more attractive the later they ysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Project
responded, regardless of their relationship status, whereas this pattern administration, Funding acquisition. Kristian Rolschau: Conceptuali­
held only for single women. Notably, the present article offers a novel zation, Methodology, Investigation, Data curation, Writing – original
approach of testing the closing time effect on self-rated attractiveness draft, Writing – review & editing.
rather than the previous focus on opposite-sex ratings.
At a general level, the current research highlights the concept of time Acknowledgement
in forming consumers’ evaluative judgments. In particular, the results
suggest that merely spending time in consumption contexts associated This work was supported by a grant awarded to the first author from
with a time-restricted market for mating may boost certain individuals’ the Aarhus University Research Foundation (AUFF). The authors are
perceptions of their own attractiveness in a way that likely facilitates grateful to Jessica Li, Jill Sundie, and Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair for
approach behaviors towards potential mates and mirrors sex differences helpful comments on an earlier version of this article.
in short-term mating strategies (Buss, 1989; Buss & Schmitt, 2019;
Schmitt, 2003). References
While we found support for our theorizing under ecologically valid
conditions, as called for by scholars in psychology and consumer Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2010). Why are narcissists so charming at first
sight? Decoding the narcissism–popularity link at zero acquaintance. Journal of
research (Cialdini, 2009b; Otterbring et al., 2020), other factors cannot
Personality and Social Psychology, 98(1), 132–145.
be explicitly ruled out as alternative accounts. It could be that patrons Baranowski, A. M., & Hecht, H. (2015). Gender differences and similarities in receptivity
entering bars at later hours simply constitute more attractive crowds, to sexual invitations: Effects of location and risk perception. Archives of Sexual
according to themselves or others, but they may also represent a Behavior, 44(8), 2257–2265.
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2005). Exploding the self-
segment that wears fancier clothes and more alluring accessories than esteem myth. Scientific American, 292(1), 84–91.
patrons entering at earlier hours, given that such style-based strategies Bègue, L., Bushman, B. J., Zerhouni, O., Subra, B., & Ourabah, M. (2013). “Beauty is in
seem to play a prominent role in mating contexts (Hill et al., 1987; the eye of the beer holder”: People who think they are drunk also think they are
attractive. British Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 225–234.
Townsend & Levy, 1990). Furthermore, “night owls” (i.e., individuals Bergkvist, L., & Rossiter, J. R. (2007). The predictive validity of multiple-item versus
with an eveningness disposition) are more likely to have narcissistic single-item measures of the same constructs. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(2),
personality traits (Jonason et al., 2013), which is positively (and meta- 175–184.
Brehm, J. W. (1972). Responses to the loss of freedom: A theory of psychological reactance.
analytically) related to physical attractiveness (Holtzman & Strube, Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
2010) as well as short-term mate appeal, including interest as a short- Brown, J. D., Novick, N. J., Lord, K. A., & Richards, J. M. (1992). When Gulliver travels:
term romantic and sexual partner (Dufner et al., 2013). People with Social context, psychological closeness, and self-appraisals. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 62(5), 717–727.
narcissistic tendencies also perceive themselves more positively and are
Buss, D. (1985). Human mate selection: Opposites are sometimes said to attract, but in
particularly prone to display self-presentation tactics, such as altering fact we are likely to marry someone who is similar to us in almost every variable.
their appearance to obtain social gains, wearing expensive, conspicuous American Scientist, 73(1), 47–51.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses
clothing, and, among women, wearing makeup and showing cleavage
tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12(1), 1–14.
(Back et al., 2010; Fox & Rooney, 2015; Vazire et al., 2008). Therefore, Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary
future research on the closing time effect should collect data on patrons’ perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100(2), 204–232.
dress style and level of narcissism, since these factors may partially Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (2019). Mate preferences and their behavioral
manifestations. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 77–110.
explain the findings reported herein. Buunk, B. P., & Dijkstra, P. (2004). Men, women, and infidelity: Sex differences in
A final suggestion for future research is to uncover the specific psy­ extradyadic sex and jealousy. In The state of affairs: Explorations in infidelity and
chological process explaining our results. The present findings appear to commitment (pp. 103–120).
Cambridge English Dictionary. (2019). Meaning of afternoon in English Accessed at: htt
contradict the predictions proposed by Johnco et al. (2010) stemming ps://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/afternoon. (Accessed 6
from mere exposure effects and commodity theory. Mere exposure ef­ November 2020).
fects can theoretically explain increases in opposite-sex ratings over time Cialdini, R. B. (2009a). Influence: Science and practice (Vol. 4). Boston: Pearson Education.
Cialdini, R. B. (2009b). We have to break up. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(1),
(Halberstadt et al., 2003). However, such exposure effects are unlikely 5–6.
to explain the current findings on self-rated attractiveness. Similarly, Clark, R. D., & Hatfield, E. (1989). Gender differences in receptivity to sexual offers.
commodity theory can explain a general increase in attractiveness rat­ Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 2, 39–55.
Conley, T. D. (2011). Perceived proposer personality characteristics and gender
ings of opposite-sex individuals as a function of time, since the scarcity
differences in acceptance of casual sex offers. Journal of Personality and Social
of potential partners due to time restrictions may increase their Psychology, 100(2), 309–329.
perceived attractiveness (Cialdini, 2009a; Lynn, 1992), but seems Cornil, Y., Chandon, P., & Krishna, A. (2017). Does Red Bull give wings to vodka?
Placebo effects of marketing labels on perceived intoxication and risky attitudes and
implausible in explaining a rise in patrons’ self-rated attractiveness.
behaviors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(4), 456–465.
Moreover, commodity theory cannot explain why the closing time effect Diener, E., Wolsic, B., & Fujita, F. (1995). Physical attractiveness and subjective well-
should be restricted to patrons who are single (Madey et al., 1996). being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 120–129.
Among the previous explanations for the closing time effect, reactance Dufner, M., Rauthmann, J. F., Czarna, A. Z., & Denissen, J. J. (2013). Are narcissists
sexy? Zeroing in on the effect of narcissism on short-term mate appeal. Personality
theory, as originally suggested by Pennebaker et al. (1979), appears best and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(7), 870–882.
suited to explain why it occurs and why it might differ depending on bar Dunn, M. J., & Hill, A. (2014). Manipulated luxury-apartment ownership enhances
patrons’ relationship status. However, reactance theory cannot easily opposite-sex attraction in females but not males. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology,
12(1), 1–17.
explain why men and women should be differentially influenced by the Dunn, M. J., & Searle, R. (2010). Effect of manipulated prestige-car ownership on both
effect depending on their relationship status. As such, the theory that sex attractiveness ratings. British Journal of Psychology, 101(1), 69–80.

6
T. Otterbring and K. Rolschau Personality and Individual Differences 179 (2021) 110921

Fang, X., Singh, S., & Ahluwalia, R. (2007). An examination of different explanations for Lyvers, M., Cholakians, E., Puorro, M., & Sundram, S. (2011). Beer goggles: Blood alcohol
the mere exposure effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 97–103. concentration in relation to attractiveness ratings for unfamiliar opposite sex faces in
Fisher, M., Cox, A., Bennett, S., & Gavric, D. (2008). Components of self-perceived mate naturalistic settings. Journal of Social Psychology, 151(1), 105–112.
value. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 2(4), 156–168. Madey, S. F., Simo, M., Dillworth, D., Kemper, D., Toczynski, A., & Perella, A. (1996).
Fox, J., & Rooney, M. C. (2015). The Dark Triad and trait self-objectification as predictors They do get more attractive at closing time, but only when you are not in a
of men’s use and self-presentation behaviors on social networking sites. Personality relationship. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18(4), 387–393.
and Individual Differences, 76, 161–165. Margraf, J., Meyer, A. H., & Lavallee, K. L. (2013). Well-being from the knife?
Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1995). Persuasion knowledge: Lay people’s and researchers’ Psychological effects of asthetic surgery. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(3),
beliefs about the psychology of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 22(1), 239–252.
62–74. Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories
Furnham, A. (1988). Lay theories: Everyday understanding of problems in the social sciences. approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American
Oxford: Pergamon. Psychologist, 61(3), 192–203.
Gao, L., Wheeler, S. C., & Shiv, B. (2009). The “shaken self”: Product choices as a means Nezlek, J. B. (1999). Body image and day-to-day social interaction. Journal of Personality,
of restoring self-view confidence. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 29–38. 67, 793–817.
Gladue, B. A., & Delaney, H. J. (1990). Gender differences in perception of attractiveness Nida, S. A., & Koon, J. (1983). They get better looking at closing time around here, too.
of men and women in bars. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(2), 378–391. Psychological Reports, 52(2), 657–658.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Sundie, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Miller, G. F., & Kenrick, D. T. Otterbring, T. (2020). Appetite for destruction: Counterintuitive effects of attractive faces
(2007). Blatant benevolence and conspicuous consumption: When romantic motives on people’s food choices. Psychology & Marketing, 37(11), 1451–1464.
elicit strategic costly signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), Otterbring, T. (2021). Evolutionary psychology in marketing: Deep, debated, but fancier
85–102. with fieldwork. Psychology & Marketing, 38(2), 229–238.
Gupta, N., Etcoff, N. L., & Jaeger, M. M. (2015). Beauty in mind: The effects of physical Otterbring, T., & Mitkidis, P. (2018). Commentary: Folk-economic beliefs: An
attractiveness on psychological well-being and distress. Journal of Happiness Studies, evolutionary cognitive model. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1120.
17(3), 1313–1325. Otterbring, T., Ringler, C., Sirianni, N. J., & Gustafsson, A. (2018). The Abercrombie &
Halberstadt, J., Rhodes, G., & Catty, S. R. (2003). Subjective and objective familiarity as Fitch effect: The impact of physical dominance on male customers’ status-signaling
explanations for the attraction to average faces. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in consumption. Journal of Marketing Research, 55(1), 69–79.
psychology research (pp. 91–106). New York: Nova Science Publishers. Otterbring, T., Sundie, J., Li, Y. J., & Hill, S. (2020). Evolutionary psychological
Hald, G. M., & Høgh-Olesen, H. (2010). Receptivity to sexual invitations from strangers consumer research: Bold, bright, but better with behavior. Journal of Business
of the opposite gender. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(6), 453–458. Research, 120, 473–484.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Pennebaker, J. W., Dyer, M. A., Caulkins, R. S., Litowitz, D. L., Ackreman, P. L.,
Hill, E. M., Nocks, E. S., & Gardner, L. (1987). Physical attractiveness: Manipulation by Anderson, D. B., & McGraw, K. M. (1979). Don’t the girls get prettier at closing time:
physique and status displays. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8(2), 143–154. A country and western application to psychology. Personality and Social Psychology
Holtzman, N. S., & Strube, M. J. (2010). Narcissism and attractiveness. Journal of Bulletin, 5(1), 122–125.
Research in Personality, 44(1), 133–136. Peskin, M., & Newell, F. N. (2004). Familiarity breeds attraction: Effects of exposure on
Johnco, C., Wheeler, L., & Taylor, A. (2010). They do get prettier at closing time: A the attractiveness of typical and distinctive faces. Perception, 33(2), 147–157.
repeated measures study of the closing-time effect and alcohol. Social Influence, 5(4), Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior:
261–271. Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2),
Jonason, P. K., Jones, A., & Lyons, M. (2013). Creatures of the night: Chronotypes and 186–201.
the Dark Triad traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 538–541. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., & Cason, M. J. (2009). The “booty call”: A compromise between attribution process. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 173–220.
men’s and women’s ideal mating strategies. Journal of Sex Research, 46(5), 460–470. Schmitt, D. P. (2003). Universal sex differences in the desire for sexual variety: Tests
Jones, B. T., Jones, B. C., Thomas, A. P., & Piper, J. (2003). Alcohol consumption from 52 nations, 6 continents, and 13 islands. Journal of Personality and Social
increases attractiveness ratings of opposite-sex faces: A possible third route to risky Psychology, 85(1), 85–104.
sex. Addiction, 98(8), 1069–1075. Schmitt, D. P., Jonason, P. K., Byerley, G. J., Flores, S. D., Illbeck, B. E., O’Leary, K. N., &
Karremans, J. C., & Verwijmeren, T. (2008). Mimicking attractive opposite-sex others: Qudrat, A. (2012). A reexamination of sex differences in sexuality: New studies
The role of romantic relationship status. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 reveal old truths. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 135–139.
(7), 939–950. Sprecher, S., DeLamater, J., Neuman, N., Neuman, M., Kahn, P., Orbuch, D., &
Kelley, H. H. (1992). Common-sense psychology and scientific psychology. Annual McKinney, K. (1984). Asking questions in bars: The girls (and boys) may not get
Review of Psychology, 43(1), 1–24. prettier at closing time and other interesting results. Personality and Social Psychology
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in Bulletin, 10(3), 482–488.
recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Sundie, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Vohs, K. D., & Beal, D. J.
Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121–1134. (2011). Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen: Conspicuous consumption as a
Learner’’s Dictionary. (2019). Parts of the day: Early morning, late morning, etc Accessed sexual signaling system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4),
at: http://www.learnersdictionary.com/qa/parts-of-the-day-early-morning-late-mor 664–680.
ning-etc. (Accessed 6 November 2020). Surbey, M. K., & Brice, G. R. (2007). Enhancement of self-perceived mate value precedes
Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differences in preferences for short- a shift in men’s preferred mating strategy. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(3), 513–522.
term mates: What, whether, and why. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90 Townsend, J. M., & Levy, G. D. (1990). Effects of potential partners’ costume and
(3), 468–489. physical attractiveness on sexuality and partner selection. The Journal of Psychology,
Lippa, R. A. (2009). Sex differences in sex drive, sociosexuality, and height across 53 124(4), 371–389.
nations: Testing evolutionary and social structural theories. Archives of Sexual Umberson, D., & Hughes, M. (1987). The impact of physical attractiveness on
Behavior, 38(5), 631–651. achievement and psychological well-being. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50(3),
Longo, L. C., & Ashmore, R. D. (1995). The looks-personality relationship: Global self- 227–236.
orientations as shared precursors of subjective physical attractiveness and self- Vazire, S., Naumann, L. P., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Portrait of a
ascribed traits. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25(5), 371–398. narcissist: Manifestations of narcissism in physical appearance. Journal of Research in
Lopes, A., Skoda, K., & Pedersen, C. L. (2019). Smartphone battery levels and sexual Personality, 42(6), 1439–1447.
decision-making among men who have sex with men. Sexuality and Culture, 23(4), Walter, K. V., Conroy-Beam, D., Buss, D. M., Asao, K., Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P., …
1301–1314. Amjad, N. (2020). Sex differences in mate preferences across 45 countries: A large-
Lynn, M. (1991). Scarcity effects on value: A quantitative review of the commodity scale replication. Psychological Science, 31(4), 408–423.
theory literature. Psychology & Marketing, 8(1), 43–57. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and
Lynn, M. (1992). The psychology of unavailability: Explaining scarcity and cost effects Social Psychology, 9(2, Pt.2), 1–27.
on value. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 13(1), 3–7. Zeigler-Hill, V., & Myers, E. M. (2011). An implicit theory of self-esteem: The
consequences of perceived self-esteem for romantic desirability. Evolutionary
Psychology, 9(2), Article 147470491100900.

You might also like