Babalola 2017

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

J Bus Ethics

DOI 10.1007/s10551-017-3524-4

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When
Leader Conscientiousness Relates to Ethical Leadership
Mayowa T. Babalola1 • Michelle C. Bligh2 • Babatunde Ogunfowora3 •

Liang Guo4 • Omale A. Garba5

Received: 31 July 2016 / Accepted: 25 March 2017


 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2017

Abstract While previous research has established that implications of our findings and suggest ways in which
employees who have a more conscientious leader are more organizations can better foster ethical leadership.
likely to perceive that their leader is ethical, the underlying
mechanisms and boundary conditions of this linkage Keywords Ethical leadership  Leader conscientiousness 
remain unknown. In order to better understand the rela- Moral reflectiveness  Decision-making autonomy
tionship between leader conscientiousness and ethical
leadership, we examine the potential mediating role of
leader moral reflectiveness, as well as the potential mod- Introduction
erating role of decision-making autonomy. Drawing from
social cognitive theory, results from two samples of Over the last decade, high-profile incidents of leaders’
workgroup leaders and their immediate reports situated in ethical failure in organizations such as Enron, WorldCom,
Africa and Asia show that leader conscientiousness is and Tyco have increased both scholars’ and practitioners’
positively related to leader moral reflectiveness, which in attention to the ethical aspects of leadership. Despite this
turn, is positively associated with employees’ assessment attention, and the increasing pressure on corporate leaders
of ethical leadership. Furthermore, and consistent with our to behave ethically, new incidents of ethical failure con-
hypothesis, results from the two samples show that leader tinue to emerge, such as the recent Volkswagen emission
decision-making autonomy moderates the indirect path test scandal. Brown et al. (2005) define ethical leadership
from leader conscientiousness to ethical leadership through (EL) as ‘‘the demonstration of normatively appropriate
moral reflectiveness, such that only morally reflective conduct…and the promotion of such conduct to followers
leaders who have high (versus low) decision-making through two-way communication, reinforcement, and
autonomy at work engage in ethical leadership behaviors. decision-making’’ (p. 120). Scholars studying ethical
In our discussion, we highlight the theoretical and practical leadership behavior in organizations have empirically
demonstrated its positive links with important work out-
comes such as employee ethical conduct (Mayer et al.
& Liang Guo
2012), organizational citizenship behavior (Babalola et al.
Liang.Guo@neoma-bs.fr 2017; Newman et al. 2014; Ogunfowora 2014a), voice and
employee cynicism (Pelletier and Bligh 2008; Walumbwa
1
Peter Faber Business School, Centre for Sustainable HRM and Schaubroeck 2009), reduced workplace conflicts (Ba-
and Well-being, Australian Catholic University, Melbourne,
Australia
balola et al. 2016), performance (Zhu et al. 2015), and
2
(ethical) job applicant attraction (Ogunfowora 2014b).
Claremont Graduate University, Claremont, CA, USA
Although the majority of this work has focused on the
3
Haskayne School of Business, University of Calgary, consequences of ethical leadership, comparatively less
Calgary, Canada
research has addressed its antecedents (e.g., Kalshoven
4
NEOMA Business School, Rouen, France et al. 2011; Mayer et al. 2012; Walumbwa and Schau-
5
Boston University, Boston, MA, USA broeck 2009; Zhu et al. 2016). For instance, Mayer et al.

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

(2012) and Zhu et al. (2016) have shown that morally on social cognitive theory (SCT; Bandura 1986, 1991),
oriented individual differences are associated with per- which offers a promising theoretical framework for deep-
ceptions of ethical leadership. Specifically, leaders with ening our knowledge about this relationship. A core pre-
high moral identity (i.e., those who self-identify and mise of SCT is that personality shapes an individual’s
believe themselves as ethical; Mayer et al. 2012) and high behavior through proximal reflective mechanisms. Since
moral attentiveness (i.e., those who chronically perceive conscientiousness taps into aspects of individuals’ thoughts
and consider morality and moral elements in their experi- and behaviors that are deemed morally oriented (Horn et al.
ences; Zhu et al. 2016) are more likely to be perceived as 2004), we use SCT as a theoretical basis to argue that
ethical leaders. While interesting insights have emerged leader conscientiousness is likely to lead to ethical lead-
from these few studies, they still remain limited in that they ership behaviors by stimulating moral reflectiveness, which
do not provide deeper knowledge of the psychological refers to the extent to which leaders contemplate moral
mechanisms and boundary conditions associated with the matters in their daily experiences and decisions (Reynolds
emergence of ethical leadership. Thus, extant research 2008).
studies insufficiently address the question of why and when Furthermore, we propose that the leader’s specific work
some leaders engage in ethical leadership behaviors. This is context may facilitate or hinder the extent to which his or
the primary goal of the current study. her moral reflectiveness ultimately results in observable
A limited but growing stream of research on leader ethical leadership behaviors. SCT suggests that the envi-
personality traits suggests that leader conscientiousness and ronment or situational context in which an individual finds
agreeableness are important antecedents of ethical leader- him/herself is a crucial factor that either strengthens or
ship behavior (Kalshoven et al. 2011; Walumbwa and constrains the extent to which domain-specific cognitive
Schaubroeck 2009). Scholars (e.g., Den Hartog 2015) have reflections invoke relevant behavioral actions (Bandura
however argued that, of the Big Five personality traits, 1991). From this perspective, the impact of a leader’s
conscientiousness seems most crucial for the explicit eth- moral cognitive reflections may depend on situational
ical focus that distinguishes ethical leadership from related contexts that encourage or inhibit the behavioral expression
leadership behaviors, primarily due to its moral cognitive of moral reflectiveness. We propose that decision-making
foundations (Costa and McCrae 1992). While establishing autonomy is one such contextual factor that may hinder or
and explicitly focusing on conscientiousness as an ante- foster the extent to which leader moral reflectiveness
cedent of ethical leadership is an important first step, this translates into observable ethical leadership behaviors.
approach is likely to be limited in utility and overly sim- According to Brown et al. (2005), ethical leadership
plistic for two major reasons. First, whereas Walumbwa involves more than simply possessing ethical personal
and Schaubroeck (2009) found a moderate association qualities (i.e., ethically oriented traits and cognitions);
between conscientiousness and ethical leadership, Kal- leaders must also engage in ‘‘moral manager’’ behaviors
shoven et al. (2011) reported evidence of a relatively such as setting high ethical standards and establishing
weaker relationship. This suggests that the strength of this reinforcements to uphold those standards. However, a
relationship may vary depending on situational or other leader’s capacity to engage in moral management behav-
moderating factors. Second, scholars have debated for iors is significantly limited if he/she lacks autonomy to
several decades whether personality traits such as consci- make relevant decisions such as determining the work
entiousness hold any value for reliably predicting leader- unit’s ethical standards or the rewards (punishments) for
ship behavior (Barrick and Mount 2005; Judge et al. 2002). upholding (violating) them. It is therefore surprising that
In light of varying effects and entrenched debates, Barrick leader decision-making autonomy has received limited
and Mount (2005) emphasized that ‘‘systematically and theoretical and empirical attention to date in the ethical
carefully studying mediating and moderating effects is leadership literature. As such, the second goal of our
precisely where we need to go in personality research’’ (p. research is to explore the moderating role of leader deci-
369). Thus, understanding the processes and contexts that sion-making autonomy in understanding when leader
determine when and how employees are more likely to conscientiousness (and moral reflectiveness) translates to
recognize conscientious leaders as ethical is crucial to ethical leadership behavior.
developing our theoretical understanding of ethical lead- Our research offers at least two major contributions to
ership, as well as to aid practical efforts to recruit and research on ethical leadership. First, in contrast to past
develop ethical leaders in organizations. research focusing on the direct relationship between con-
Accordingly, our goal in the present research is to shed scientiousness and ethical leadership, we examine moral
light on the relationship between leader conscientiousness reflectiveness as a potential mediatory mechanism that
and ethical leadership in light of existing debates in the underlies this relationship. Second, our research answers a
personality trait approach to leadership. To do so, we draw recent call to identify potential boundary conditions of

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

ethical leadership antecedents (Den Hartog 2015) by develops through reflectiveness in that domain, starting as a
investigating the moderating role of leader decision-mak- result of relevant personality traits and ending with
ing autonomy. In doing so, we contribute to the literature behavioral actions. We draw on SCT to suggest that ethical
by illuminating circumstances in which morally reflective leadership behaviors are rooted in a similar process of
leaders may not necessarily demonstrate or be seen as personality–cognitive reflection–behavioral action. Given
displaying ethical leadership behaviors. Taken together, that the Big Five trait most often seen as the standard for
our theoretical model (see Fig. 1) addresses Barrick and moral evaluation is conscientiousness (Horn et al. 2004),
Mount’s (2005) call to build and test theory about the we argue that leader conscientiousness (a personality trait)
mechanisms and contexts that explain why and when per- is likely to stimulate leader moral reflectiveness (a morally
sonality may foster specific leadership behaviors. inclined cognitive reflection), which subsequently induces
ethical leadership behavior (a morally informed behavioral
action), with the strength of this indirect relationship
Theoretical Background and Hypotheses depending on a situational factor that constrains the
expression of ethical leadership behavior (i.e., decision-
Overview of Social Cognitive Theory making autonomy).
In building conceptual support for our model, we draw
‘‘Most human behavior, being purposive, is regulated by on Reynolds’ (2008) work on moral attentiveness, a
forethought’’ (Bandura 1991, p. 248). According to SCT sociocognitive construct that captures the extent to which
(Bandura 1986, 1991), an individual’s specific cognitive people habitually attend to moral issues in their social
reflection in any given domain is central to predicting his or interactions. Reynolds proposes that there are two aspects
her behavioral course of action in that domain. SCT sug- to moral attentiveness: perceptual (the extent to which an
gests that domain-relevant behaviors depend first on indi- individual chronically searches for and focus on the moral
vidual personality, but also on the cognitive reflection that aspects of social information) and reflective (a more
accompanies the specific trait. In this way, SCT concep- nuanced cognitive process that emphasizes the use of
tualizes domain-relevant behaviors as a process that morality in examining social information). We focus on

Fig. 1 Theoretical model

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

moral reflectiveness in our model because, while percep- Leader Moral Reflectiveness and Ethical Leadership
tual moral attentiveness is considered an automatic
response and primarily entails information coding, moral At the heart of SCT (Bandura 1986, 1991) is the idea that
reflectiveness is more intentional and emphasizes behav- reflection and forethought in a specific domain induces
ioral action (Reynolds 2008). This is important because actionable behaviors in that particular targeted domain
ethical leadership entails more behavioral action than (Bandura 1986, 1991). As a result, moral reflectiveness
information coding (Brown et al. 2005), which is an should prompt leaders’ demonstration and encouragement
important but insufficient driver of behavioral action. of ethical behaviors as they fulfill their leadership roles and
Moreover, our focus on moral reflectiveness builds upon responsibilities. Examples of such morally informed lead-
past work on the link between ethical leadership and the ership behaviors include acting in the best interests of
broader moral attentiveness construct (e.g., Zhu et al. employees, actively listening to what they have to say,
2015). We propose that a more nuanced examination of discussing the implications of unethical behaviors with
this construct should enrich our understanding of why them, and generally being a role model for ethical conduct
conscientious leaders are more likely to demonstrate ethi- (Brown et al. 2005). These ethical leader behaviors are
cal leadership behaviors at work. We now turn to this issue more likely to ‘‘shine through’’ to others when leaders
in more detail below. consider moral matters on a regular basis. Indeed, previous
research has generally supported the notion that individ-
ual’s moral reflectiveness is an important basis for the
Leader Conscientiousness and Moral Reflectiveness
internalization of morally oriented values and behaviors
(Kohlberg 1981; Reynolds 2008). Thus, consistent with
To be ‘‘conscientious’’ is to be governed by one’s con-
prior research and SCT, we argue that leaders’ chronic
science, a foundation for moral evaluation (Horn et al.
attention to and consideration of morality and moral mat-
2004). Individuals low in conscientiousness are seen as
ters in daily work interactions should lead to the demon-
undependable, careless, and thoughtless. In contrast, con-
stration of normatively appropriate conduct and the
scientious individuals are reliable, self-disciplined, careful,
promotion of ethics to employees as reflected in their
and thorough (McCrae and Costa 1987). Conscientious
decision-making (i.e., ethical leadership).
individuals pay careful attention to detail, are deliberate
In line with the above argument, a recent study by Kim
rather than haphazard in their decision-making, have a
et al. (2014) found that leaders who reflected on moral
strong sense of moral obligation, and act based on their
matters in their experiences pursued morally informed
conscience (Judge et al. 2009). These attributes suggest a
behaviors to protect and save their organization’s resour-
possible link between conscientiousness and moral reflec-
ces, as well as engaged in actions that contribute to envi-
tiveness, which Reynolds (2008) defines as the extent to
ronmental sustainability. Zhu et al. (2016) also argued and
which an individual is guided by moral consideration in his
found that individuals who are high on overall moral
or her daily experiences and decisions. Given that moral
attentiveness (including both perceptual and reflective
reflectiveness is a consciously informed, self-controlled
moral attentiveness) were more likely to be perceived as
process (Haidt 2001; Reynolds 2008), we propose that
ethical leaders. Therefore, we argue that enhanced moral
leaders with high conscientiousness routinely reflect on the
reflectiveness associated with leaders’ conscientiousness
extent to which their behavior is morally appropriate. In
should guide and motivate leaders to engage in ethical
addition, they are likely to subsequently use such reflection
leadership behaviors. Taken together, since ethical lead-
as a guide for their behavioral actions since they tend to
ership is regarded as an explicit demonstration and
value honesty and think carefully before acting.
expression of a leader’s moral value of being a moral
Accordingly, as conscientious leaders go about their
person and moral manager, moral reflectiveness should
daily lives, they likely become more conscious and
serve as an internal reflective process that not only guides
thoughtful regarding the normative aspects of their actions.
the leader to display ethical conducts but also motivates
Conscientious leaders will therefore pursue morality,
him or her to encourage and promote ethical behavior in
reflect moral values (Collins and Schmidt 1993), and more
the workplace.
regularly reflect on morality in their daily experiences
(Kim et al. 2014). Therefore, based on the above theoret- Hypothesis 2 Leader moral reflectiveness is positively
ical arguments and empirical evidence, we expect that for related to perceptions of ethical leadership.
people occupying leadership roles, conscientiousness will
A key proposition in our model is the mediating role of
be associated with higher levels of moral reflectiveness:
leader moral reflectiveness in expanding our understanding
Hypothesis 1 Leader conscientiousness is positively of the relationship between leader conscientiousness and
related to leader moral reflectiveness. ethical leadership. To this point, we have argued that leader

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

conscientiousness is positively linked to moral reflective- moral manager (Brown et al. 2005; Mayer et al. 2012).
ness and that leader moral reflectiveness in turn is posi- This entails behavioral actions such as defining success not
tively associated with ethical leadership. That is, moral just by the results but also the way they are obtained,
reflectiveness serves as the proximal mechanism that disciplining employees who violate ethical standards,
underlines the distal relationship between leader consci- asking ‘‘what is the right thing to do?’’ when making
entiousness and ethical leadership behavior. decisions, and setting an example of how to do things the
Given the morally laden definition of conscientiousness right way in terms of ethics. We argue that these ethical
(Becker 1998; Costa and McCrae 1992) and the social leadership behaviors are more likely to occur when morally
cognitive origin of moral reflectiveness (Reynolds 2008), reflective leaders have high decision-making autonomy.
the application of SCT in understanding the relations For instance, leaders, in order for ethical leaders to address
between conscientiousness and ethical leadership suggests the core question of ‘‘what is the right thing to do’’ when
that the effect of leader conscientiousness (a distal indi- faced with a critical decision, it is important for them to
vidual personality predictor) on moral reflectiveness (a possess decision-making latitude (Brown et al. 2005). As
moral-specific forethought) shapes ethical leadership (a such, having decision-making autonomy should enhance
leader’s morally informed behavioral action). This is the extent to which morally reflective leaders can better
because a leader’s concern about morality underscores his take others’ considerations into account since they are able
or her motivation to demonstrate normatively appropriate to act upon such inputs. As a result, decision-making
behaviors in the workplace (Brown et al. 2005). Therefore, autonomy should enhance the extent to which morally
we propose that leader conscientiousness stimulates ethical reflective leaders engage in observable ethical leadership
leadership behavior through its linkage with moral behaviors.
reflectiveness. In contrast, low levels of decision-making autonomy
suggest that the leader has little discretion or control in
Hypothesis 3 Leader moral reflectiveness mediates the
terms of his or her responsibilities and behavioral strategies
relationship between leader conscientiousness and ethical
for fulfilling those responsibilities (Hackman and Oldman
leadership
1976). Under this condition, a leader’s moral reflectiveness
may not always lead to observable ethical leadership
The Moderating Role of Decision-Making behavior because he/she is unable to take meaningful
Autonomy actions, including those with ethical implications. In
organizations, leaders are often faced with situations that
We further propose that the strength of the mediatory effect may question their stance of being fair and moral (Gino and
of moral reflectiveness depends on the leader’s decision- Mogilner 2014). Even though conscientious leaders value
making autonomy. As earlier noted, one of the basic tenets honesty in their personal and work life (Costa and McCrae
of SCT (Bandura 1986, 1991) is that the ‘‘situation’’ an 1992), and may reflect more frequently on moral issues in
individual finds him/herself is an important conditional their daily decisions and experiences, being in a work sit-
factor that either strengthens or constrains the behavioral uation where they do not have sufficient freedom to make
manifestation of one’s cognitive reflection or forethought. decisions is likely to create less opportunities for them to
Reynolds (2008) has similarly proposed that moral reflec- behave in ways consistent with their morally inclined
tiveness should be more influential in contexts where an reflection. As such, the relationship between leader moral
individual must recognize a moral issue, consider its reflectiveness and ethical leadership should be weaker in
implications, deliberate alternate options, and ultimately these contexts. In other words, low decision-making
announce a course of action. In this paper, we propose that autonomy is likely to weaken the extent to which a con-
the behavioral expression of moral reflectiveness is likely scientious leader’s moral reflectiveness leads to the suc-
to be either strengthened or constrained by the leader’s cessful demonstration ethical leadership behavior as
decision-making autonomy—i.e., the extent to which the perceived by subordinates. Lastly, we expect that decision-
leader has freedom to make decisions about job-related making autonomy will have no significant influence on the
issues (Karasek 1979). Specifically, ethical leadership ethical leadership conduct of leaders who are low on
behaviors are more likely to occur if a morally reflective conscientiousness. Generally, these leaders are unlikely to
leader, having given much thought and consideration to engage in ethical leadership behaviors because they are less
ethical issues, has greater autonomy to make pertinent likely to habitually reflect on issues of morality in their
decisions on the job. day-to-day work life. For these individuals, therefore,
The extant literature suggests that beyond being a moral having greater or less decision-making autonomy should
person, the unique strength of an ethical leader is to have minimal impact on the extent to which they engage in
actively manage morality on a day-to-day basis by being a ethical leadership behaviors.

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

Hypothesis 4 The indirect relationship between leader unmatchable or unavailable corresponding direct reports,
conscientiousness and ethical leadership through moral thereby yielding an effective overall response rate of 56%.
reflectiveness is moderated by decision-making autonomy, As such, the final sample consisted of 28 workgroup leaders
such that moral reflectiveness engenders ethical leadership and 115 direct reports. The size of each workgroup ranged
when decision-making autonomy is high, but not when from three to seven members, with an average group size of
decision-making autonomy is low. 4.5 workgroup members. The average organizational tenure
was four years, and employees reported spending an average
Research Overview of 2.6 years with their immediate leader.

We test our hypothesized model in two samples using two Measures


different populations and methodologies (i.e., multisource
and three-wave field studies). In Sample 1, we examine the Surveys were administered in English, the official language
proposed model using a cross-sectional sample of leaders in Nigeria. All items were measured on a 5-point scale
and their direct reports in Nigerian organizations. We then (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).
replicate and test this model in Sample 2 using a three-wave
multisource sample of leaders and their direct reports in Leader Conscientiousness We measured leader conscien-
Chinese organizations. Given that both Nigeria and China tiousness using a four-item scale in the Mini International
are situated in a cultural context where decision-making Personality Item Pool (Mini-IPIP; Donnellan et al. 2006).
tends to be hierarchically structured (Hofstede, 2001), they Work group leaders rated the following items: ‘‘I get chores
provide two ideal samples for testing our model (Fig. 1). done right away’’, ‘‘I like order’’, ‘‘I make mess of things’’
(Reverse-coded), and ‘‘I often forget to put things back in
their proper place’’ (Reverse-coded). The internal consis-
Method tency reliability was .77. Past empirical research (Kim et al.
2014) has demonstrated the convergent validity of the Mini-
Sample 1 IPIP with Goldberg’s (1999) conscientiousness scale.

Participants and Procedure Moral Reflectiveness We measured leader moral reflec-


tiveness using a 5-item scale developed by Reynolds
Sample 1 consisted of workgroup leaders and their direct (2008). The sample items include, ‘‘I often reflect on the
reports from three companies in the Nigerian consulting, moral aspects of my decisions’’, and ‘‘I regularly think
hospitality, and financial industries. Prior to survey about the ethical implications of my decisions’’. The
administration, human resource personnel in each of the internal consistency reliability was .75.
participating firms, who also served as our internal con-
tacts, provided a list of workgroup leaders and employees Decision-Making Autonomy We assessed decision-mak-
who worked directly under them from which we randomly ing autonomy using Morgeson and Humphrey’s (2006)
selected potential participants. Participants were informed 3-item scale. Sample items include, ‘‘This job gives me a
through a cover letter that the purpose of the survey was to chance to use my personal initiative or judgment in car-
examine effective leadership development, and that their rying out the work’’, and ‘‘This job provides me with sig-
responses would be confidential. We also emphasized the nificant autonomy in making decisions’’. The internal
voluntary nature of participation. We administered two reliability of the scale was .88.
separate questionnaires to workgroup leaders and their
direct reports. The questionnaires included a customized Ethical Leadership Employees provided ratings of ethical
personal code based on the list received from the HR leadership using Brown et al.’s (2005) 10-item scale.
officer in order to enable us link the two surveys. With the Sample items include: my manager/supervisor… ‘‘makes
help of our contacts, surveys were distributed on-site and fair and balanced decisions’’, and ‘‘defines success not just
directly returned to the research team after completion. To by results but also the way that they are obtained’’. The
thank participants for their participation, they were entered internal reliability of the scale was .89.
into a drawing to win one of four online shopping vouchers
priced at approximately US$15 each.
Of the 50 workgroup leader and 200 direct report surveys Analytic Strategy and Levels of Analysis
distributed, 34 leaders and 133 direct reports returned
completed questionnaires (approximately 68% response First, we sought to establish our measurement model. We
rate). Six leader surveys were discarded because of either carried out multilevel confirmatory factor analyses in

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

Mplus (MCFA; Muthén and Asparouhov 2009) to ensure procedures (Tucker et al. 2016; Zyphur and Oswald 2015).
that the scales loaded on their intended constructs. We used Mplus provides 95% confidence intervals around the esti-
MCFA because employee responses to the ethical leader- mated parameters.
ship scale are not independent, given that our participants
were nested in teams. In our hypothesized factor structure,
we modeled within- and between-group variances in ethi- Results
cal leadership, while other constructs completed by the
supervisor were modeled as group-level constructs. To Measurement Model
minimize potential estimation issues, we created three
randomly distributed parcels for the ethical leadership scale The results of the multilevel CFA showed that our
(Landis et al. 2000). We compared our hypothesized four- hypothesized factor structure was a good fit to the data,
factor model (Model 1) with two alternate models: Model v2 (86) = 89.95, p [ .05, CFI = .99, TLI = .98,
2—a three-factor model that combined ethical leadership RMSEA = .02, SRMRwithin = .02, and SRMRbetween =
and moral reflectiveness into one construct; and Model 3— .12. All fit indices are within acceptable ranges, with the
a one-factor model where all items loaded on a single exception of SRMRbetween, which captures the fit at the
construct. group level. We suspect that this is likely due to the rela-
Lastly, we tested our theoretical model using linear and tively small sample size at the group level. For instance,
hierarchical regression analyses. We further specified a although the model estimation terminated normally, Mplus
multilevel moderated mediation model in Mplus (Preacher suggested caution due to the fewer number of groups
et al. 2010), which allowed us to simultaneously test our (relative to the number of parameters estimated).
theoretical model. This approach is preferable to OLS Nonetheless, the hypothesized model was significantly
moderated mediation regression analyses (e.g., using the better than the alternate models, including
2 2
PROCESS macro) because it does not assume indepen- Model 2 (Dv (3) = 11.19, p \ .05 [v (89) = 106.87,
dence of observation in the data. Moreover, Preacher et al. p [ .05, CFI = .94, TLI = .93, RMSEA = .04,
(2010) have discussed the advantages of Mplus over tra- SRMRwithin = .01, and SRMRbetween = .13]), and Model 3
ditional multilevel modeling paradigms (e.g., HLM) for (Dv2 (6) = 112.66, p \ .05 [v2 (92) = 227.67, p \ .05,
testing mediation in a multilevel context. For instance, CFI = .54, TLI = .46, RMSEA = .11, SRMRwithin = .15,
unlike HLM, Mplus allows one to directly test relationships and SRMRbetween = .31]).
among ‘‘Level 2’’ variables. We tested for indirect and
conditional indirect effects using Bayesian estimation in Test of Hypotheses
Mplus. This procedure uses the Markov Chain Monte Carlo
(MCMC) process to iteratively estimate the indirect and Table 1 presents the correlations, means, and standard
conditional indirect parameters (Zyphur and Oswald 2015). deviations of the variables in this sample. We carried out a
This process (iterations = 10,000 in the present study) is series of regression analyses in Mplus to test our
similar in principle to nonparametric bootstrapping hypotheses. The results showed that leader

Table 1 Descriptive statistics


Mean SD 1 2 3
and correlations among study
variables (Sample 1) Individual level
1 Leader conscientiousness – – –
2 Leader moral reflectiveness – – .50** –
3 Leader decision-making autonomy – – .09 -.05 –
4 Employee ratings of ethical leadership 2.65 0.73 .40** .39** .11
Group level
1 Leader conscientiousness 3.04 1.00 –
2 Leader moral reflectiveness 2.78 .80 .49** –
3 Leader decision-making autonomy 2.73 1.30 .12 .08 –
4 Employee ratings of ethical leadership 2.69 .59 .48** .43* .15
At the individual level, n = 115. At the group level, n = 28. At the individual level, each member of the
group received the same score for the group leader’s conscientiousness, moral reflectiveness, and decision-
making autonomy
* p \ .05; ** p \ .01

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

Table 2 Results of multilevel


Moral Ethical Ethical Ethical
moderated mediation analyses
reflectiveness leadership (C) leadership (C) leadership (C)
using Bayesian estimation
procedures (Sample 1) Step 1 Step 2 Step 3
B SD B SD B SD B SD

Leader conscientiousness (A) .39** .15 .21 .12 .10 .12


Leader moral reflectiveness (B) .34* .14 .21 .16 -.46 .28
Decision-making autonomy (D) -.59** .24
B9D .23** .08
R2 .21** .21** .33** .75**
Indirect effect B SD 95% confidence interval
Low CI High CI

A?B?C .07 .07 -.05 .24


Conditional indirect effect
Levels of the moderator: decision-making autonomy (D)
A ? B ? C, when D is low -.04 .08 -.22 .10
A ? B ? C, when D is average .07 .06 -.04 .21
A ? B ? C, when D is high .18** .10 .01 .39
Estimates are unstandardized coefficients. In lieu of standard errors (SE), Bayesian estimation procedures in
Mplus provide ‘‘posterior standard deviation (SD)’’ estimates
* p \ .05; ** p \ .01

conscientiousness was positively related to moral reflec-


tiveness, B = .39, p \ .01, 95% CI [.10, .68], providing
support for Hypothesis 1. In support of Hypothesis 2, lea-
der moral reflectiveness was positively associated with
employee ratings of ethical leadership, B = .34, p \ .05,
95% CI [.06, .63]. Hypothesis 3, which predicted that
moral reflectiveness mediates the link between leader
conscientiousness and ethical leadership, was not sup-
ported, B = .07, p = .09, 95% CI [-.05, .24].
Next, we tested our proposed moderated mediation
model. The results showed that decision-making autonomy
did moderate the indirect effect of leader conscientiousness
on ethical leadership perceptions through moral reflec-
tiveness, B = .23, p \ .01, 95% CI [.07, .40]. This pro-
vides support for Hypothesis 4 (Table 2). Tests of simple
slopes showed that moral reflectiveness significantly
mediated the relationship between leader conscientiousness
and subordinate perceptions of ethical leadership when
leader decision-making autonomy was high (B = .18,
p \ .01, 95% CI [.01, .39] but not when it was low
(B = -.04, p [ .05, 95% CI [-.22, .10]. A graphical Fig. 2 Indirect effects of leader conscientiousness on ethical leader-
depiction of this interaction is shown in Fig. 2. The graph ship perceptions (through moral reflectiveness) at different levels of
leader decision-making autonomy (Sample 1)
shows that when leader decision-making autonomy is high,
the slope of the relationship between leader conscien-
tiousness and ethical leadership (through moral reflective- Although we used two sources of data collection in our
ness) is positive and strong, and relatively weak when first sample—leader and their direct reports—the sample is
leader decision-making autonomy is low. relatively small and cross-sectional in nature. Therefore,

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

we sought to test our model with a dyadic dataset of leaders them responded (overall response rate of 68.73%). At time
and their direct reports, while separating measurements in 3, employees were invited to rate the ethical leadership
time in order to limit common method bias and strengthen behavior of their workgroup manager. Of the 1,316 direct
the order of occurrence of the tested relationships in using report surveys distributed, we received 723 (approximately
an additional sample (Podsakoff et al. 2012). a 55% response rate). Of the returned surveys, six
uncompleted leader surveys were discarded. We also
Sample 2 eliminated nine workgroups in which group members did
not fully complete the questionnaires. Therefore, our final
In this sample, we addressed two critical limitations of sample size consists of 163 workgroup managers and 714
Sample 1. First, we recruited a significantly larger sample group members.
of workgroups. This increases our statistical power to The size of each workgroup ranges from 3 to 23 indi-
detect effects and also addresses the MCFA estimation viduals, with an average group size of 4.38 (SD = 3.84).
issues associated with the small number of workgroup in The average age of group members and managers is 28.57
Sample 1. In addition, we employed a time-lagged design and 36.51, respectively. The average organizational tenure
in Sample 2 to address the potential effects of common is around 4.10 years, with an average of 3.23 years with
source bias associated with the cross-sectional design of their immediate leader. Both group members and managers
Sample 1. work relatively long hours per week (mean = 54.21 and
62.74 h, respectively). Finally, 94% of group members
Participants and Procedure completed at least post-secondary studies and 37.1% of
group managers hold postgraduate degrees.
Sample 2 consisted of 163 workgroups from seven ICT
companies located in Beijing (Northern China), Chengdu Measures
(Western China), and Guangzhou (Southern China), three
major Chinese high-tech metropolitan clusters. We sur- We used the same measures from Sample 1. All items were
veyed both work group managers and members of work measured on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree,
groups in functional areas such as IT operations, finance, 7 = strongly agree) because prior studies report that in
accounting, sales and marketing, and human resources. Asian cultures, people are more likely to select the mid-
Prior to the survey administration, we invited human points and avoid extreme responses on 5-point Likert scales
resource personnel to provide a list of group managers as (Chen et al. 1995; Hamamura et al. 2008). Higher scale
well as employees who work directly under them. A cover granularity can also reduce response bias.
letter was used to explain the purpose of the study and We followed the back-translation procedure (Brislin
emphasize the voluntary nature of participation and the 1980) to translate the scales from English to Chinese: a
strict confidentiality of their responses. We administered professional translator first translated the questionnaires
two separate questionnaires to workgroup managers and from English to Chinese. Then, a different translator pro-
their direct reports in three phases. Whereas workgroup ceeded with the reverse translation (i.e., translated the
managers were surveyed at Time 1 and Time 2 (three scales from Chinese back to English). Finally, the first
weeks after Time 1), their direct reports were surveyed at translator compared the two scales to check whether the
Time 3 (three weeks after workgroup managers completed back-translation scales were similar to the original ones.
the time 2 surveys). The questionnaires included a personal We also pretested the questionnaires with ten individuals to
ID based on the list received from the HR personnel of make sure all items were clearly understood.
each firm in order to enable us link the surveys. With the The alpha values of leader conscientiousness at Time 1,
help of our contacts, surveys were distributed on-site and moral reflectiveness and decision-making autonomy at
directly returned to the research team after completion. To Time 2, and ethical leadership at Time 3 were .78, .92, .89,
thank those who participated across the three different and .83, respectively.
phases, they were entered into a drawing to win one of
twenty restaurant vouchers priced at 200 RMB (approxi-
mately US$31) each. Results
Of the 259 workgroup managers who were invited at
Time 1 to provide ratings of their conscientiousness and Measurement Model
demographic information, we received responses from 182
(a response rate of 70.27%). These individuals were then Multilevel CFA showed that our hypothesized factor structure
sent another survey to rate their moral reflectiveness and was an excellent fit to the data, v2 (129) = 138.83, p [ .05,
decision-making autonomy at Time 2, of which only 178 of CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .01, SRMRwithin = .001,

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

and SRMRbetween = .04. The hypothesized model was sig- 1, the graphical depiction of the relationship (Fig. 3) shows
nificantly better than the alternate models, including Model 2 that when leader decision-making autonomy is high, the
(Dv2 (3) = 250.22, p \ .01 [v2 (132) = 299.21, p \ .01, slope of the relationship between leader conscientiousness
CFI = .89, TLI = .87, RMSEA = .04, SRMRwithin = .01, and ethical leadership (through moral reflectiveness) is
and SRMRbetween = .09]), and Model 3 (Dv2 (5) = 410.18, positive and strong, and relatively weak when leader
p \ .01 [v2 (134) = 513.00, p \ .01, CFI = .75, TLI = .71, decision-making autonomy is low (Table 4).
RMSEA = .06, SRMRwithin = .01, and SRMRbetween =
.17]). These results provide strong evidence in support of the
proposed measurement model in this sample. Discussion

Test of Hypotheses In the present research, we attempt to reevaluate the rela-


tionship between leader conscientiousness and ethical
Table 3 presents the correlations, means, and standard leadership in light of the existing debates in the literature
deviations of the variables in this sample. The results regarding the extent to which personality may predict
showed that leader conscientiousness was positively related observed leadership behavior (Judge et al. 2002; Barrick
to moral reflectiveness, B = .68, p \ .01, 95% CI [.46, .90], and Mount 2005). In doing so, we delve more deeply into
providing support for Hypothesis 1. In support of Hypothesis why and when conscientious leaders may influence
2, leader moral reflectiveness was positively associated with employees’ perceptions of ethical leadership. Specifically,
employee ratings of ethical leadership, B = .32, p \ .01, we drew on SCT (Bandura 1986, 1991) to develop and test
95% CI [.21, .44]. Hypothesis 3, which predicted that moral an integrated moderated mediation model of how and
reflectiveness mediates the link between leader conscien- under what conditions leader conscientiousness may lead
tiousness and ethical leadership, was supported in this to observable demonstrations of ethical leadership behav-
sample, B = .19, p \ .01, 95% CI [.09, .29]. Next, we tested iors. As predicted, our research findings suggest that leader
the proposed moderated mediation model. We found once conscientiousness is positively related to moral reflective-
again that leader decision-making autonomy moderated the ness, which subsequently engenders ethical leadership as
indirect effect of leader conscientiousness on ethical lead- perceived by employees. However, the indirect effect of
ership perceptions through moral reflectiveness, B = .10, leader conscientiousness on ethical leadership was condi-
p \ .01, 95% CI [.02, .18]. tional on the leader’s decision-making autonomy at work.
This provides support for Hypothesis 4. Tests of simple Decision-making autonomy enhanced the strength of the
slopes showed that moral reflectiveness significantly indirect effect of leader conscientiousness on ethical
mediated the relationship between leader conscientiousness leadership through moral reflectiveness, such that the
and subordinate perceptions of ethical leadership when relationship was stronger for leaders with high decision-
leader decision-making autonomy was high (B = .30, making autonomy rather than those with low decision-
p \ .01, 95% CI [.16, .46], but not when it was low making autonomy. These findings suggest a number of
(B = .08, p [ .05, 95% CI [-.04, .21]. Similar to Sample meaningful theoretical and practical implications.

Table 3 Descriptive statistics and inter-correlations among study variables (Sample 2)


Mean SD 1 2 3

Individual level
1 Leader conscientiousness – – –
2 Leader moral reflectiveness – – .42** –
3 Leader decision-making autonomy – – .22** .13** –
4 Employee ratings of ethical leadership 4.25 1.16 .34** .42** .24**
Group level
1 Leader conscientiousness 5.32 .88 –
2 Leader moral reflectiveness 4.42 1.39 .44** –
3 Leader decision-making autonomy 3.13 1.49 .19* 0.05 –
4 Employee ratings of ethical leadership 4.25 1.10 .28** .40** .21**
At the individual level, n = 714. At the group level, n = 163. At the individual level, each member of the group received the same score for the
group leader’s conscientiousness, moral reflectiveness, and decision-making autonomy
* p \ .05; ** p \ .01

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

Theoretical Implications

Our study contributes to theory and emerging research on


ethical leadership antecedents in several ways. First, as
previously discussed, although prior research has linked
leader conscientiousness with ethical leadership behaviors
(Kalshoven et al. 2011; Walumbwa and Schaubroeck
2009), research on ethical leadership has largely focused on
examining the consequences of such leadership behavior.
Despite promising initial evidence, this stream of research
still lacks a strong theoretical framework that addresses the
debates in the leadership literature as to when and precisely
why conscientiousness may predicts leadership behaviors
(Judge et al. 2002). Accordingly, we developed a social
cognitive perspective to advance this emerging stream of
research on ethical leadership by exploring both distal
(leader conscientiousness) and proximal (moral reflective-
ness) antecedents and consequently, illustrating how this
particular leader personality trait and related cognitive
reflection may lead to perceptions of ethical leadership.
Fig. 3 Indirect effects of leader conscientiousness on ethical leader- Our research findings in this regard thus help to demon-
ship perceptions (through moral reflectiveness) at different levels of strate conscientiousness as a potentially beneficial person-
leader decision-making autonomy (Sample 2)
ality trait for predicting ethical leadership. By applying

Table 4 Results of multilevel moderated mediation analyses using Bayesian estimation procedures (Sample 2)
Moral reflectiveness Ethical leadership (C) Ethical leadership (C) Ethical leadership (C)
Step 1 Step 2 Step 3
B SD B SD B SD B SD

Leader conscientiousness (A) .68** .11 .17 .10 .15 .10


Leader moral reflectiveness (B) .32** .06 .28** .06 -.05 .14
Decision-making autonomy (D) -.30 .18
B9D .10** .04
R2 .19** .16** .19** .28**
Indirect effect B SD 95% confidence interval
Low CI High CI

A?B?C .19** .05 .09 .29


Conditional indirect effect
Levels of the moderator: decision-making autonomy (D)
A ? B ? C, when D is low .08 .06 -.04 .21
A ? B ? C, when D is average .19** .05 .09 .30
A ? B ? C, when D is high .30** .08 .16 .46

The italics are used to uniquely distinguish the values of the (R) from other normal coefficients
Estimates are unstandardized coefficients. In lieu of standard errors (SE), Bayesian estimation procedures in Mplus provide ‘‘posterior standard
deviation (SD)’’ estimates
* p \ .05; ** p \ .01

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

SCT (Bandura 1986, 1991), we explicate the psychological decision-making autonomy. Hence, moral reflectiveness
process that makes conscientious leaders more likely to may be a necessary but insufficient precursor of moral
exhibit ethical leader behaviors by highlighting the critical behavior. This finding suggests that future research inves-
role that moral reflectiveness plays in this process. Our tigating how individual moral reflectiveness relates to
findings are consistent with the hypothesized model, in that moral behaviors may benefit from incorporating situational
leaders who are higher in conscientiousness are inclined to factors such as decision-making autonomy into their
be more morally reflective, and in turn, demonstrate more models.
leadership behaviors that employees perceive as ethical. As
such, we extend SCT by specifically measuring a domain-
Practical Implications
specific cognitive forethought/reflection and its relationship
to morally specific leadership behaviors of great concern to
This research also offers important practical knowledge
both scholars and practitioners. In sum, drawing on SCT,
that can facilitate and guide the development of ethical
we highlight that ethical leadership stems from leaders’
leadership in organizations. Given the complex linkages
reflection on morality in their daily experiences. This
among leader conscientiousness, moral reflectiveness, and
finding underscores the need for organizational processes,
ethical leadership behavior, our findings suggest that it is
training activities, and both leader- and follower-initiated
not sufficient to only hire conscientious leaders who are
conversations that foster closer attention to the moral
cognitively disposed to showing ethical behaviors. Rather,
implications of even seemingly mundane management
encouraging them to pay attention to moral issues in their
decisions.
experience is also an important consideration. By doing so,
The present research also contributes to the literature by
such leaders can benefit from their cognitive reflectiveness
examining leader decision-making autonomy as a situa-
on morality and moral matters as it can importantly shape
tional moderator of ethical leadership antecedents. The
observable ethical behavior. As such, practical investiga-
inclusion of this important leadership constraint as a
tions aimed at developing ethical leadership could devote
moderator in our model was crucial given that the literature
adequate sessions to getting participants to role-play, dis-
on ethical leadership has generally overlooked the role that
cuss, and reflect on moral issues around them.
situational factors may play (Den Hartog 2015). As Den
Our findings also indicate that giving leaders the free-
Hartog suggested, models proposing trait antecedents of
dom to make decisions also strengthens the proximal
ethical leadership may be insufficient and may limit
motivational mechanism of a leader’s increased moral
insights that could be gained from such research without
reflectiveness. Hence, the degree of decision-making
accounting for the role of a leader’s job context. Accord-
autonomy is an important consideration both for organi-
ingly, and in line with the SCT framework (Bandura
zations seeking to recruit conscientious leaders and for
1986, 1991), our findings suggest that high decision-mak-
programs designed to facilitate ethical leadership. Without
ing autonomy strengthens the extent to which a leader’s
such consideration, the potential benefit of recruiting and
proximal cognitive reflection (i.e., moral reflectiveness)
selecting conscientious leaders could be attenuated, as such
translates into ethical leadership, while low decision-
leaders require relevant job contexts to maximize their
making autonomy constrains transmission of leader moral
potential and further develop and role model their ethical
reflectiveness into ethical leadership. This suggests that
leadership.
future research may also examine other potential situa-
tional variables, such as work demands (e.g., time pressure,
organizational bottom line mentality), perceived organiza- Limitations and Directions for Future Research
tional support for ethical leadership, and perceptions of the
political risk or consequences of a decision, among others. Despite these contributions, our study is not without limi-
More generally, our research also contributes to SCT tations. First, we did not receive permission from the
(Bandura 1986, 1991) by extending and empirically testing organizations to collect data longitudinally in Sample 1,
the basic tenets of Reynolds’ (2008) moral reflectiveness which would have been preferable given the causal direc-
model. Reynolds provided a framework for understanding tion implied in our model. Hence, because of the cross-
how reflecting on morality and moral matters (i.e., moral sectional nature of our research design, we cannot draw
reflectiveness) can lead to moral behavior. Extending this strong causal conclusions. We addressed this limitation in
model, we proposed and empirically confirmed the Sample 2 by temporally separating the variables in the
importance of moral reflectiveness in the domain of causal chain using a time lag of three weeks between each
workplace ethical leadership. Specifically, moral reflec- phase. In doing so, our research design also helps to reduce
tiveness facilitates ethical leadership consistently in both of common method bias (Podsakoff et al. 2012). Moreover,
our samples, yet only when leaders have high levels of where inferred, the relationship directionality of our model

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

was grounded in theory and empirical evidence. Future predict the between-level variance component of this
research may however utilize a longitudinal research construct). Previous simulation studies generally indicate
design where all variables are measured repeatedly or an that increasing the number of groups, rather than number of
experimental design to strengthen causal claims. individuals in each group, is necessary to ensure sufficient
Second, although our focus on the mediating role of power to detect between-group effects (Kreft 1996). Hof-
moral reflectiveness rather than perceptual moral atten- mann (1997) however notes that sample size requirements
tiveness in the relationship between leader conscientious- should be less for detecting between-group main effects,
ness and ethical leadership was theoretically motivated, it compared to ‘‘cross-level’’ interaction effects. Thus, we
would have been helpful to also include perceptual moral believe that our small sample size in Sample 1 is a clear
attentiveness in order to test whether (or not) a similar limitation, although it may not be as significant given the
pattern may hold. For instance, compared to moral reflec- simplicity of our mediation model (between-group effects
tiveness, perceptual moral attentiveness is less likely to involving three constructs). Indeed, past multilevel studies
fluctuate (i.e., it is more likely to be static; Kim et al. 2014; with similar or more complex models have published
Sturm 2015). Moreover, as previous research demonstrates, results based on similar sample sizes (e.g., Eisenbeiss et al.
although perceptual moral attentiveness helps individuals 2008; Hofmann et al. 2003; Hirst et al. 2009). Nonetheless,
recognize moral dilemmas and recall morality-related we addressed this limitation by recruiting a much larger
behaviors in others, unlike moral reflectiveness it does not sample of workgroups in Sample 2 (n = 163). With greater
necessarily translate into moral behaviors (Reynolds 2008; statistical power, the results from Sample 2 provide full
Sturm 2015). Thus, it seems quite unlikely that perceptual support for mediation. Moreover, results from both samples
moral attentiveness will be more relevant than moral showed that the indirect effect of leader conscientiousness
reflectiveness in our model since ethical leadership focuses on ethical leadership through leader moral reflectiveness
more on the behavioral actions of leaders (Brown et al. was strongest under conditions of high (versus low) leader
2005). Nevertheless, we encourage future research decision-making autonomy. The replication of this mod-
exploring the link between leader conscientiousness and erated mediation relationship across both samples is
ethical leadership to include perceptual moral reflective- encouraging and serves to further allay our sample size and
ness in order to further substantiate this claim. Relatedly, statistical power concerns.
our findings regarding the significant relationship between Finally, although our findings provide support for the
conscientiousness and moral reflectiveness seem to be generalizability of leader conscientiousness in predicting
inconsistent with Reynolds’ (2008) study that found a EL in non-Western cultures (in this case, Nigeria and
nonsignificant relationship. This suggests the possibility of China), future research should replicate and examine the
a moderating variable, which points to an interesting generalizability of our findings in other cultures. Similarly,
direction for future research. For instance, trait activation such research could also consider cross-cultural variation
theory (Tett and Burnett 2003) suggests that personality in EL emergence. Insights from the GLOBE studies sug-
traits require trait-relevant situations for their expressions. gest that the behavioral enactment of EL may vary by
Research has shown that low time pressure, for example, is cultural context, yet the importance of EL as a critical
an important cue that conscientious leaders may require to aspect of leadership does not (Chhokar et al. 2007; House
be effective (Ng et al. 2008). As such, it is possible that et al. 2004). In other words, existing evidence suggests that
conscientious leaders also need less time pressure to enable EL is universally desirable, and we would expect that our
them become moral reflective, thus offering a potential theoretical arguments and findings are generalizable to
resolution for the nonsignificant finding in Reynolds other contexts such as Western cultures, especially in
(2008). We encourage future research to investigate this organizations where decision-making is hierarchically
possibility. structured. In such contexts, our findings suggest that the
Third, we found somewhat inconsistent support for our leader’s level of autonomy will be a critical aspect to
mediation hypothesis across the two samples. Specifically, consider; however, the nature and range of that autonomy
we found that moral reflectiveness mediated the link will likely differ significantly based on both the organiza-
between leader conscientiousness and ethical leadership in tional and cultural context.
Sample 2, but this mediation effect was marginally sig- Generally speaking, the research on the extent to which
nificant (p = .09) in Sample 1. We believe that the rela- conscientiousness predicts ethical leadership is still devel-
tively small group-level sample size (n = 28) in Study 1 oping in the ethical leadership literature and seems
may have contributed to this discrepancy. In our model, the promising based on our findings. However, much can still
proposed mediation effect primarily occurs at the group or be learned in this area. For instance, our research findings
‘‘between’’ level of analysis (although ethical leadership showing that morally reflective leaders are less likely to be
varies both within and between groups, our goal was to perceived as ethical leaders when their decision-making

123
M. T. Babalola et al.

autonomy is low raises an important question for future Compliance with Ethical Standards
research. Given that the ethical leadership literature largely
Conflict of interest The authors have no conflict of interest.
relies on subordinates’ perceptions of such leadership
behavior, it would be interesting to further examine when Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all indi-
leaders fail to behave ethically under conditions of low vidual participants in the study.
decision-making autonomy (reflecting on ‘‘actual’’ leader
ethical behavior rather than ‘‘perceptions’’ of ethical
leadership). For instance, it may be that such situations References
may constrain the perception of ethical leadership and
Babalola, M. T., Stouten, J., Euwema, M., & Ovadje, F. (2016). The
perhaps not necessarily constrain actual leader ethical relation between ethical leadership and workplace conflicts: The
behavior. Clearly, exploring this line of research would be mediating role of employee resolution efficacy. Journal of
an interesting next step. For example, is it that morally Management. doi:10.1177/0149206316638163.
reflective leaders do not behave ethically in low decision- Babalola, M. T., Stouten, J., Camps, J., & Euwema, M. (2017). When
do ethical leaders become less effective: The moderating role of
making autonomy contexts, or that they are just not ‘‘per- perceived leader ethical conviction on employee discretionary
ceived’’ by employees as demonstrating ethical leadership reactions to ethical leadership. Journal of Business Ethics.
under such conditions? Although ‘‘actual’’ ethical behav- doi:10.1007/s10551-017-3472-z.
iors demonstrated by leaders are very important, research Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
has shown that individuals are more strongly influenced by Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organi-
their own ‘‘perceptions’’ of others’ behavior than its actual zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248–287.
‘‘objective’’ nature (Lewin, 1951). Regardless, future Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (2005). Yes, personality matters:
investigation may refine our outlook on ethical leadership Moving on to more important matters. Human Performance, 18,
111–118.
in organizations and further open scholarly discussions Becker, T. E. (1998). Integrity in organizations: Beyond honesty and
regarding the extent to which we need to study ‘‘actual’’ vs. conscientiousness. Academy of Management Review, 23,
‘‘perceived’’ ethical leadership. Importantly, such research 154–161.
should also specifically test whether there is a differential Brislin, R. W. (1980). Translation and content analysis of oral and
written material. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 2(2),
effect between actual and perceived ethical leadership. 349–444.
Brown, M. E., Treviño, L. K., & Harrison, D. (2005). Ethical
leadership: A social learning perspective for construct develop-
ment and testing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Conclusion Processes, 97, 117–134.
Chen, C., Lee, S. Y., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Response style and
In light of the long lasting debates in the personality cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales among East Asian
and North American students. Psychological Science, 6,
approach to leadership literature, our research offers 170–175.
important contributions to the literature. We shed new light Chhokar, J. S., Brodbeck, F. C., & House, R. J. (2007). Culture and
on the mechanisms and boundary conditions that lead leadership across the world: The GLOBE book of in-depth
conscientious leaders to be perceived as ethical leaders, studies of 25 societies. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group.
Collins, J. M., & Schmidt, F. L. (1993). Personality, integrity, and
thus demonstrating that the link between leader conscien- white collar crime: A construct validity study. Personnel
tiousness and ethical leadership is more complex than Psychology, 46, 295–311.
assumed by previous research. In line with SCT, we Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality
illustrate the importance of leader moral reflectiveness as a Inventory Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
mediator, and decision-making autonomy as a moderator, Den Hartog, D. N. (2015). Ethical leadership. Annual Review of
in the relationship between conscientiousness and ethical Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2,
leadership. Further, our research suggests that, to aid the 409–434.
effectiveness of practical interventions that can potentially Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006).
The Mini-IPIP scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big
support ethical leadership, there is a need for morally Five factors of personality. Psychological Assessment, 18,
reflective leaders to have autonomy in making decisions at 192–203.
work. We hope this research reinforces the need for Eisenbeiss, S. A., Van Knippenberg, D., & Boerner, S. (2008).
researchers to devote more attention to the antecedent Transformational leadership and team innovation: Integrating
team climate principles. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93,
conditions associated with ethical leadership, ultimately to 1438–1466.
help avoid the potentially disastrous impacts of its absence Gino, F., & Mogilner, C. (2014). Time, money, and morality.
for both organizations and employees. Psychological Science, 25, 414–421.

123
The Mind is Willing, but the Situation Constrains: Why and When Leader Conscientiousness…

Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public-domain, person- Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. (2006). The Work Design
ality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five- Questionnaire (WDQ): Developing and validating a comprehen-
factor models. Personality Psychology in Europe, 7, 7–28. sive measure for assessing job design and the nature of work.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2014.05.003. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1321–1339.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the Muthén, B., & Asparouhov, T. (2009). Multilevel regression mixture
design of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and analysis. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A:
Human Decision Processes, 16, 250–279. Applied Statistics, 172, 639–657.
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social Newman, A., Kiazad, K., Miao, & Cooper, B. (2014). Examining the
intuitionist approach to moral judgement. Psychological Review, cognitive and affective trust-based mechanisms underlying the
108, 814–834. relationship between ethical leadership and organizational citi-
Hamamura, T., Heine, S. J., & Paulhus, D. L. (2008). Culture zenship: A case of the head leading the heart? Journal of
differences in response styles: The role of dialectical thinking. Business Ethics, 123, 113–123.
Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 932–942. Ng, K., Ang, S., & Chan, K. (2008). Personality and leader
Hirst, G., Van Knippenberg, D., & Zhou, J. (2009). A cross-level effectiveness: A moderated mediation model of leadership self-
perspective on employee creativity: Goal orientation, team efficacy, job demands, and job demands, and job autonomy.
learning behavior, and individual creativity. Academy of Man- Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 733–743.
agement Journal, 52, 280–293. Ogunfowora, B. (2014a). It’s all a matter of consensus: Leader role
Hofmann, D. A. (1997). An overview of the logic and rationale of modeling strength as a moderator of the links between ethical
hierarchical linear models. Journal of Management, 23(6), leadership and employee outcomes. Human Relations, 67,
723–744. 1467–1490.
Hofmann, D. A., Morgeson, F. P., & Gerras, S. J. (2003). Climate as a Ogunfowora, B. (2014b). The impact of ethical leadership within the
moderator of the relationship between leader-member exchange recruitment context: The roles of organizational reputation,
and content specific citizenship: Safety climate as an exemplar. applicant personality, and value congruence. The Leadership
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 170–178. Quarterly, 25(3), 528–543.
Horn, J., Nelson, C. E., & Brannick, M. T. (2004). Integrity, Pelletier, K. L., & Bligh, M. C. (2008). The aftermath of organiza-
conscientiousness, and honesty. Psychological Reports, 95, tional corruption: Employee attributions and emotional reac-
27–38. tions. Journal of Business Ethics, 80(4), 823–844.
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012).
(Eds.). (2004). Culture, leadership, and organizations: The Source of method bias in social science research and recom-
GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. mendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology,
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Illies, R., & Gerhardt, M. G. (2002). 63I, 539–569.
Personality and leadership: A qualitative and quantitative Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general
review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 765–780. multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation.
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Kosalka, C. J. (2009). The bright and Psychological Methods, 15, 209–233.
dark sides of leader traits: A review and theoretical extension of Reynolds, S. J. (2008). Moral attentiveness: Who pays attention to
the leader trait paradigm. The Leadership Quarterly, 20, moral aspects of life? Journal of Applied Psychology, 93,
855–875. 1027–1041.
Kalshoven, K., Den Hartog, D. N., & De Hoogh, A. B. (2011). Ethical Sturm, R. E. (2015). Decreasing unethical decisions: The role of
leader behavior and big five factors of personality. Journal of morality-based individual differences. Journal of Business
Business Ethics, 100, 349–366. Ethics. doi:10.1007/s10551-015-2787-x.
Karasek, R. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude and mental Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based
strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science interactionist model of job performance. Journal of Applied
Quarterly, 24, 285–308. Psychology, 88, 500–517.
Kim, A., Kim, Y., Han, K., Jackson, S. E., & Ployhart, R. E. (2014). Tucker, S., Ogunfowora, B., & Ehr, D. (2016). Safety in the c-suite:
Multilevel influences on voluntary workplace green behavior: How chief executive officers influence organizational safety
Individual differences, leader behavior, and coworker advocacy. climate and employee injuries. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Journal of Management. doi:10.1177/0149206314547386. 101(9), 1228–1239.
Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development. San Walumbwa, F. O., & Schaubroeck, J. (2009). Leader personality traits
Francisco: Harper and Row. and work group psychological safety. Journal of Applied
Kreft, I. G. G. (1996). Are multilevel techniques necessary? An Psychology, 94, 1275–1286.
overview, including simulation studies. Working paper, Califor- Zhu, W., He, H., Treviño, L. K., Chao, M. M., & Wang, W. (2015).
nia State University, Los Angeles, CA. Ethical leadership and follower voice and performance: The role
Landis, R. S., Beal, D. J., & Tesluk, P. E. (2000). A comparison of of follower identifications and entity morality beliefs. The
approaches to forming composite measures in structural equation Leadership Quarterly, 26, 702–718.
models. Organizational Research Methods, 3, 186–207. Zhu, W., Treviño, L. K., & Zheng, X. (2016). Ethical leaders and their
Mayer, D. M., Aquino, K., Greenbaum, R. L., & Kuenzi, M. (2012). followers: The transmission of moral identity and moral
Who displays ethical leadership, and why does it matter? An attentiveness. Business Ethics Quarterly, 26, 95–115.
examination of antecedents and consequences of ethical leader- Zyphur, M. J., & Oswald, F. L. (2015). Bayesian estimation and
ship. Academy of Management Journal, 1, 151–171. inference: A user’s guide. Journal of Management, 41, 390–420.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor
model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.

123

You might also like