Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

J Orofac Orthop

DOI 10.1007/s00056-017-0097-x

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Effects of malocclusions on facial attractiveness and their


correlations with the divine proportion
Auswirkungen von Malokklusionen auf die Gesichtsattraktivität
und Korrelationen mit den Proportionen des Goldenen Schnitts
Seden Akan1 • Ayse Gul Torgut1 • Hüsamettin Oktay1

Received: 5 April 2016 / Accepted: 21 March 2017


 Springer Medizin Verlag GmbH 2017

Abstract subnasale-menton/stomion-menton and nasion-menton/na-


Purpose The aim of this study was to evaluate the effects sion-trichion in the nonattractive group were found to be
of malocclusions on facial attractiveness and to determine different from the divine proportion.
if it was correlated with the divine proportion. Conclusions Sagittal skeletal malocclusions evaluated by
Methods Standard frontal facial photos were taken from ANB angle are not effective on facial attractiveness. Facial
335 subjects in natural head position. Facial attractiveness ratios used in this study have little effect on attractiveness,
of the subjects was evaluated by 10 dental students using a and are different from the divine proportion.
10-point visual analog scale (VAS). All of the evaluations
were arranged in order, and 30 attractive subjects with the Keywords Facial proportion  Divine proportion  Esthetic
highest score and 30 nonattractive subjects having the perception  Malocclusions  Anthropometry
lowest score were chosen. On the frontal photos of these
subjects, 13 landmarks were determined and 12 ratios were Zusammenfassung
measured. Two way ANOVA was used to determine the Ziel Evaluiert werden sollten die Auswirkungen von
effects of malocclusions on facial attractiveness, Student’s Malokklusionen auf die Gesichtsattraktivität. Ferner sollte
t test was used to compare the facial proportions of the bestimmt werden, ob Korrelationen mit dem Goldenen
attractive and nonattractive subjects, and one sample t test Schnitt bestehen.
was used to define the relationships between the divine Methoden Von 335 Individuen wurden Standardportrait-
proportion and facial ratios. fotos in natürlicher Kopfposition angefertigt. Die
Results No statistically significant difference was found Gesichtsattraktivität wurde von 10 Zahnmedizinstudenten
between malocclusion groups in terms of facial attrac- mit einer 10-Punkte-VAS (visuelle Analogskala) bewertet
tiveness. Of the 12 facial ratios, significant differences und in eine Rangfolge geordnet. Die 30 attraktivsten
were found between attractive and nonattractive subjects Individuen mit den höchsten und die 30 nichtattraktiven
regarding trichion-menton/nasion-menton, subnasale-men- Individuen mit den niedrigsten Scores wurden in die Studie
ton/stomion-menton, nasion-subnasale/stomion-menton, aufgenommen. Auf den en-face Aufnahmen dieser Teil-
nasion-subnasale/nasal width, and trichion-menton/right- nehmenden wurden 13 Referenzpunkte und 12 Verhält-
left frontotemporale ratios. All of the ratios except nasion- niswerte bestimmt. Um die Effekte von Malokklusionen
subnasale/stomion-menton in the attractive group and auf die Gesichtsattraktivität zu evaluieren, wurde
eine Varianzanalyse (ANOVA) durchgeführt, zum Ver-
Assistant Professor Seden Akan.
gleich von attraktiven und nichtattraktiven Individuen der
Student-t-Test und zur Definition der Beziehungen zwi-
& Seden Akan schen Goldenem Schnitt und fazialen Verhältniswerten der
sedenakandt@hotmail.com Einstichproben-t-Test.
1 Ergebnisse Zwischen den Gruppen mit Malokklusionen
Department of Orthodontics, School of Dentistry, Istanbul
Medipol University, Atatürk Bulv. No: 27, Unkapanı, Fatih, fand sich kein statistisch signifikanter Unterschied hin-
Istanbul 34083, Turkey sichtlich der Gesichtsattraktivität. Signifikante Unterschiede

123
S. Akan et al.

zwischen den attraktiven und nichtattraktiven Teilnehmen- Facial attractiveness is an essential part of social inter-
den wurden bei den folgenden der 12 Verhältnisse in den action. Most orthodontic and orthognathic procedures
Gesichtsproportionen ermittelt: Trichion-Menton/Nasion- involve marked esthetic changes. Thus, orthodontists
Menton, Subnasale-Menton/Stomion-Menton, Nasion-Sub- should evaluate their patients from both the frontal and
nasale/Stomion-Menton, Nasion-Subnasale/Nasenbreite lateral views. Many authors [2, 26] have reported the
und Trichion-Menton/rechts-linke frontotemporale importance of esthetics from the frontal view, and
Verhältnisse. Alle Verhältniswerte – mit Ausnahme von orthodontists have shifted their focus from the sagittal
Nasion-Subnasale/Stomion-Menton in der Gruppe mit plane to the frontal plane when evaluating their patients
attraktiven und Subnasale-Menton/Stomion-Menton und and planning and assessing orthodontic treatments; this is
Nasion-Menton/Nasion-Trichion in der Gruppe mit nicht- because the patients generally consider themselves in terms
attraktiven Teilnehmenden – erwiesen sich als nicht den of beauty from the front or three-quarter profile, almost
Proportionen des Goldenen Schnitts entsprechend. never in profile [15].
Schlussfolgerungen Sagittale skelettale Malokklusionen, Because of the variations among gender and ethnic and
evaluiert mittels ANB-Winkel, haben keine Auswirkung racial groups, the use of proportional measurements for
auf die Gesichtsattraktivität. Die in dieser Studie unter- photographs is more useful for the study of facial esthetics
suchten fazialen Verhältnisse stehen nicht im Zusammen- compared to using absolute values in numerical facial
hang mit Attraktivität und unterscheiden sich von den analysis [17]. The divine proportion has existed from all
Proportionen des Goldenen Schnitts. eternity in mathematics and in the physical universe, but
precise information is not available as to when humankind
Schlüsselwörter Gesichtsproportionen  Goldener first discovered and applied it. Throughout history, it was
Schnitt  Ästhetische Wahnehmung  Malokklusionen  most likely rediscovered many times. The concept of the
Anthropometrie ‘‘divine proportion’’ concept is expressed as the ratio of large
and small and its value is ‘1.618’; that is, the smaller part is
62% of the size of the larger. In orthodontics, Ricketts
Introduction evaluated the divine proportion in relation to facial attrac-
tiveness for the first time and reported that facial esthetics
In common orthodontic practice, the treatment goal is to should be based on a scientific basis rather than a personal
produce an ideal occlusion with a full complement of teeth, perception basis [27, 28]. Ricketts’s articles appear to be key
and this would, it was believed, produce optimal facial publications in the fields of orthodontics and oral surgery for
esthetics. At this point, a question has arisen about whether facial esthetics [16]. Afterward, Baker and Woods [3] and
attractive people always have a normal or ideal dentofacial Shell and Woods [33] were unable to establish significant
relationship. Facial esthetics can be defined as an appre- correlations between changes in the divine proportion and
ciation of beauty and attractiveness. Judgments about facial esthetic rating after orthognathic treatment. Thus, they stated
attractiveness are complex and vary greatly between indi- that there was little or no relationship between the divine and
viduals and different cultures, as stated in Margaret esthetic proportions. However, as there is great interest in the
Hungerford’s classic statement, ‘‘Beauty is in the eye of the divine proportion as a measure of facial esthetics in general,
beholder’’ [12]. Although it is still a subjective evaluation, and since several authors still consider the divine proportion
in several groups, mutual appreciation indicates the pres- to be an important factor in facial esthetics, there is still a
ence of similar characteristics in people’s esthetic need to evaluate the relationship between facial esthetics and
perceptions. divine proportions [13, 16].
Limited soft tissue esthetic evaluations through The importance of malocclusions to facial esthetics has
cephalometric radiographic records gave rise to a quest for been described by many authors [25, 29]. Some of them
numerical and proportional evaluations using photographic have evaluated esthetics by manipulating photographs
records [3, 7, 9, 23, 27, 33]. Photographic records have through the computer, and they have discovered that faces
some advantages such as they are cheaper and allow better with aligned teeth were significantly more attractive than
assessment of facial harmony [24]. However, standardiza- those which did not possess this feature [25]. Others
tion in obtaining facial photographs became necessary to assessed the malocclusion types of the selected attractive
create scientific measurements [3, 9]. With this purpose, people in terms of the divine ratio [29].
the natural head position (NHP) has been used effectively Until now, an analysis of the relationship between facial
because it is a standardized and reproducible position esthetics as perceived by a panel of judges and the divine
[7, 21].

123
The effects of malocclusions on facial attractiveness

proportion has never been performed in individuals with procedures and the 10-point visual analog scale (VAS).
skeletal malocclusion. The purposes of this study were to The jury watched the PowerPoint presentation in a silent
and comfortable room, and they scored the 335 subjects
• Assess the impact of sagittal skeletal relationships on
from 1 (least attractive) to 10 (most attractive) according to
facial attractiveness and
their own preferences. The VAS form used in the present
• Compare the facial proportions of the most attractive
study is shown in Fig. 2.
and nonattractive subjects with each other and with the
In the second part, the mean VAS scores of the subjects
divine proportion.
were determined, and they were arranged in order
according to their absolute values. The 30 most attractive
subjects with the highest scores and the 30 least attractive
Materials and methods subjects with the lowest scores were chosen from the
sample, and these children were used for further analyses.
In the present study, which was approved by the insti- In order to determine the divine proportions of the attrac-
tutional ethical board (108400987-96-85), 335 subjects tive and nonattractive subjects, 13 landmarks and 12 linear
of Caucasian origin (133 males, 202 females; mean age measurements were used on the photographs (Fig. 3;
15.72 ± 4.03 years) were randomly selected from 490 Table 2). The measurements were carried out using ImageJ
untreated individuals who applied to the Department of software (version 1.44; National Institutes of Health,
Orthodontics, School of Dentistry at Istanbul Medipol Bethesda, MD, USA).
University. The power analysis showed that 279 subjects Data analysis was performed using the SPSS software
were sufficient for this study [a = 0.05; f(effect package program (SPSS for Windows, version 16.0, SPSS
size) = 0.25; power (1-b error probability) = 0.95]. To Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Two-way ANOVA was used for
compensate for possible dropouts during the trial, we the statistical evaluation of the VAS scores between the
decided to enroll more subjects. Inclusion criteria were groups. Student’s independent samples t test was used for
no previous history of orthodontic treatment, tooth comparison of the ratios of attractive and nonattractive
extraction except for third molars, cosmetic surgery on subjects, and one-sample t test was used for comparison of
the face, and craniofacial syndrome. Facial photographs the divergence amounts of the ratios from the divine pro-
were taken from all subjects using the Olympus D 720, portion in both groups. A p B 0.05 values were accepted as
4.2–42.0 mm macro lens in a natural head position, and significant.
they were standardized using biometric photograph
templates created for female and male individuals
through superimposition at the facial midline and orbital Results
line (Fig. 1). Color images of all the subjects were
converted to grayscale using the Adobe Photoshop pro- Descriptive data and comparison results of the panel
gram (CS4 version 7.0) to eliminate beauty interferers assessment are presented in Table 3 and Fig. 4. As can be
such as skin and hair color, and they were then randomly seen from this table, there was no significant difference in
placed into a PowerPoint presentation to be displayed to terms of facial attractiveness between skeletal relationship
the judges. The exposure time of each photo was set at (p = 0.407) and gender (p = 0.884) groups. Distribution of
10 s. the most attractive and nonattractive subjects to the skeletal
Preorthodontic cephalometric records of each patient malocclusion groups and the comparison results of their
were assessed, and the patients were classified into differ- scores are shown in Table 4. Although no statistically sig-
ent malocclusion groups according to their ANB angles nificant difference was found between the malocclusion and
(ANB angle = 0–4, skeletal Class I;[4, skeletal Class II; gender groups in both attractive and nonattractive subjects,
\0, skeletal Class III) and Wits appraisals. The numbers the majority of the nonattractive and attractive subjects were
of subjects in Class I, II, and III skeletal malocclusion in Class II, Class I, and Class III groups, respectively.
groups were 174, 125, and 36, respectively. Descriptive Statistical data and comparison results of attractive and
values of the groups according to ANB angles and Wits nonattractive subjects regarding facial ratios are presented
appraisals are shown in Table 1. in Table 5. Of the 12 ratios, R2 (Tr-Me/Na-Me), R4 (Sn-
In the first part of the study, the panel of judges con- Me/St-Me), R5 (Na-Sn/St-Me), R11 (Na-Sn/Right-Left lat.
sisted of 10 Caucasian students who were beginning their nasal), and R12 (Tr-Me/Ftr-Ftl) showed statistically sig-
dental education (8 women and 2 men with a mean age of nificant differences between attractive and nonattractive
20.85 ± 1.43). The students were well informed about the subjects.

123
S. Akan et al.

Fig. 1 Frontal photographs and their standardizations with biometric templates


Abb. 1 Standardisierung der en-face Aufnahmen mit biometrischen Templates

The results of one sample t test indicating the diver- orthodontic tooth movement affects existing facial bal-
gence of facial ratios from the divine proportion are shown ance [2]. According to orthodontists, sagittal malocclu-
in Table 6 and Fig. 5. Only one ratio (R5; Na-Sn/St-Me) sion and facial beauty are interdependent concepts [35].
was identical to the divine proportion of 1.618 in the It has been suggested that when teeth are aligned and
attractive subjects, and two ratios (R4; Sn-Me/St-Me and occlusion is corrected to osseous cephalometric stan-
R6; Na-Me/Na-Tr) in the nonattractive subjects. All of the dards, optimal facial esthetics will be obtained [36].
other ratios in both groups were statistically different from However, this fact does not necessarily imply that a
1.618 (p \ 0.05). good dentofacial relationship can give facial attractive-
ness to the subjects, as there may be numerous attractive
patients who do not present a perfect Class I craniofacial
Discussion and occlusal relationship [10].
Physical appearance has important effects on human
It has been generally accepted that orthodontic tooth self-esteem, and previous studies [2, 35] have shown that
movement can alter facial esthetics. Since orthodontists attractive people leave a better first impression than less
are interested in facial beauty, they need to know how attractive subjects even when they have malocclusions.

123
The effects of malocclusions on facial attractiveness

Tab. 1 Descriptive values of the groups according to ANB angles


and Wits appraisals
Tab. 1 Deskriptive Werte der Gruppen nach ANB-Winkel und Wits-
Appraisal
N Mean SD Min Max

ANB
Class I
Female 109 1.50 0.90 0 4
Male 65 1.60 1.01 0 4
Class II
Female 73 7.48 1.89 5 14
Male 52 7.54 2.03 5 13
Class III
Female 20 -3.45 1.67 -7 -1
Male 16 -3.94 2.74 -12 -1
Total 335 3.20 3.98 -12 14
Wits
Class I
Female 109 0.99 0.70 0 2
Male 65 1.03 0.73 0 3
Class II
Female 73 3.85 1.00 3 7
Male 52 3.88 0.96 3 6
Class III
Female 20 -2.25 1.12 -5 -1 Fig. 3 Landmarks used in this study. Tr trichion, Na nasion, Ftr
Male 16 -2.69 1.82 -8 -1 frontotemporale right, Ftl frontotemporale left, Med cant medial
Total 335 1.70 2.17 -8 7 canthus, Lat Na lateral nasal, Sn stomion, Chi cheilion, Me menton
Abb. 3 In der Studie verwendete Referenzpunkte. Tr Trichion, Na
Nasion, Ftr Frontotemporale rechts, Ftl Frontotemporale links, Med
cant medialer Kanthus, Lat Na Alare, Sn Stomion, Chi Cheilion, Me
Menton

mentioned above. A panel of judges was constituted


from 10 Caucasian students beginning dental education
(8 women and 2 men). As they are continuing with a
basic medicine education, they do not have any knowl-
edge of esthetic and cephalometric evaluation or of
Fig. 2 The visual analog score (VAS) form used in present study facial proportions. For this reason, the panel of students
Abb. 2 In der Studie verwendete visuelle Analogskala (VAS)
was thought to be objective. In the present study, the
panel of judges consisted of more women than men.
Some studies [6, 11] have shown that the gender of
Frontal facial photos have often been used in research
panel members was not decisive for their ratings in the
studies because they are easy to standardize and, more
literature. However, it has also been shown that women
importantly, the subjects always see themselves and
are less critical than men in another study [34].
other subjects from the front or in three-quarter profile,
One of the aims of the present study was to evaluate
almost never in profile [30]. For this reason, the esthetic
whether attractive subjects always possessed an ideal
concept should be reconsidered especially before
craniofacial relationship. The results showed that 43% of
orthognathic surgical operations in which esthetic cor-
the most attractive subjects with the highest scores had a
rection is the main purpose. Some studies [35, 36] have
skeletal Class I relationship, 43% had a skeletal Class II,
evaluated facial esthetics using profile images in the
and 14% had a skeletal Class III. As for the nonattrac-
literature. In the present study, facial attractiveness was
tive subjects with the lowest scores, the percentage
evaluated using frontal facial photos for the reasons
distributions of the subjects in the skeletal Class I, II,

123
S. Akan et al.

Tab. 2 Ratios analyzed in the present study


Tab. 2 In der Studie analysierte Verhältnisse
Numerator Denominator
Tr-Sn: Na-Me: Sn-Me: St-Me: Na-Tr: Right–left Right–left Ftr- Right–left
height of morphological height of height of forehead Cheilion: lat nasal: Ftl: med canth:
the upper height of the the lower the heigt mouth nose width face Inter-eye
face face face mandible width width width

Tr-Me: R1 R2 R12
physiognomical
height of face
Tr-Sn: height of R3
the upper face
Na-Me: R6
morphological
height of the
face
Sn-Me: height of R4
the lower face
St-Me: height of
the mandible
Med cant-Me: R7
eye–chin
distance
Na-Sn: nose R5 R11
height
Right–left R8
cheilion: mouth
width
Right–left lat
nasal: nose
width
Ftr-Ftl: face width R9 R10

and III groups were 30, 50, and 20, respectively. The subjective perception, changing according to cultural
ANOVA results showed that the malocclusion groups differences from community to community [4, 5, 18].
and gender differences had no effect on facial attrac- Matoula and Pancherz [20] investigated skeletofacial
tiveness. In other words, attractive subjects might not morphology of attractive and nonattractive subjects and
always have a Class I skeletal relationship. Recently, found that attractive females had a larger ANB angle and
Rodrı́guez et al. [29] evaluated attractive subjects with a Wits appraisal, causing a more convex soft tissue profile.
Class I molar relationship, and they concluded that As a result, they concluded that the attractiveness of a
attractive patients did not always have a Class I molar face could hardly be explained by objective parameters.
relationship. Although similar results were obtained in In the present study, although not statistically significant,
the present study, it has been noted that skeletal classi- the results also showed a slight tendency for males to be
fication by ANB angle was used in the present study, as judged better than females in the Class III group and for
dental malocclusions may have been affected by local females to be judged more attractive in the Class II
factors such as tooth size and shape, dental caries, group. This phenomenon could be accepted as a com-
extractions, and missing teeth as well as skeletal mon social trend, and it was also discovered by Macı́as-
relationship. Gago et al. [19] who stated that attractive females tended
There is no universal standard for facial attractiveness to fall into Class II, whereas males tended to fall into
no matter what race, age, gender, or other variables are Class III.
considered. In the literature, several studies regarding the The second aim of this study was to evaluate whether
assessment of facial attractiveness are available. Many there was a difference between the ratios of attractive and
of them have reported that facial attractiveness was a nonattractive faces and whether they were identical to the

123
The effects of malocclusions on facial attractiveness

divine proportion. Angle [1] claimed that beautiful faces (Table 4). Only the R2 ratio (Tr-Me/Na-Me) was higher in
are compatible with divine proportion, and Ricketts [27] attractive faces, while R4 (Sn-Me/St-Me), R5 (Na-Sn/St-
found a relationship between the divine proportion and Me), R11 (Na-Sn/Right-Left lat. nasal), and R12 (Tr-Me/
facial beauty in young women. The results of the present Ftr-Ftl) ratio values were higher in nonattractive faces.
study showed that 5 of the 12 ratios were different in Ferrario et al. [8] and Sforza et al. [32] suggested that a
attractive and nonattractive faces at a statistically signifi- large forehead, a small nose, a wide face, and a large mouth
cant level. Other ratios were similar in both groups were common characteristics for a better esthetic appear-
ance. However, it has been accepted that there was a low
relationship between the two dimensional ratios and angles
Tab. 3 Descriptive statistics and ANOVA results of visual analog
and facial attractiveness [16, 31].
scale (VAS) scores of total sample
Tab. 3 Deskriptive Statistik und ANOVA-Ergebnisse der VAS (vi- It is highly interesting that most of the ratios inves-
suelle Analogskala)-Scores beim gesamten Kollektiv tigated in this study differ from the divine proportion.
N Mean SD Min Max One ratio in attractive subjects and the two ratios in
nonattractive subjects are identical to the divine pro-
Class I portion. These results are compatible with those of
Female 109 3.77 1.02 1.30 6.50 previous studies [24, 29, 31], which could not find any
Male 65 3.73 0.96 2.10 5.50 similarity between attractive people’s ratios and the
Total Cl I 174 3.99 0.99 1.30 6.50 golden ratio. As opposed to our findings, Ricketts [27]
Class II and Kawakami et al. [14] observed that the human face
Female 73 3.92 1.36 1.50 7.80 has several structures in the golden proportion relating to
Male 52 3.43 1.15 1.10 6.70 esthetic perception.
Total Cl II 125 3.71 1.29 1.10 7.80 As a result, it may be said that attractive faces may
Class III not present the divine proportion or vice versa [3], and
Female 20 3.24 1.16 1.80 6.00 thus the primary goal of orthodontic treatment should
Male 16 3.70 1.38 2.10 7.10 not involve making the patient’s craniofacial measure-
Total Cl III 36 3.44 1.14 1.10 7.80 ments close to the divine proportion [33]. The following
Total 335 3.71 1.14 1.10 7.80 statement belonging to Holdaway [10] explains this truth
very well, ‘‘It would be very difficult to correct the
Fgender 0.021, p 0.884; Fmalocclusion 0.902, p 0.407; Fgender and malocclu-
sion interact 2.874, p 0.058
malocclusion without losing something in the way of
SD standard deviation, Cl class facial beauty.’’ He also claimed that not everyone had an
ideal occlusion even in a sample of beauty queens.

Fig. 4 Graphical presentation


of the panel assessment ratios of
the malocclusions. Cl class
Abb. 4 Graphische Darstellung
des Panels zum Assessment der
Malokklusionen. Cl Klasse

123
S. Akan et al.

Tab. 4 Descriptive statistics and ANOVA results of visual analog Tab. 5 Descriptive statistics and comparison results of the ratios
scores (VAS) of attractive and nonattractive subjects computed in the two groups
Tab. 4 Deskriptive Statistik und ANOVA-Ergebnisse der VAS (vi- Tab. 5 Deskriptive Statistik und errechnete Ergebnisse des Ver-
suelle Analogskala)-Scores von attraktiven und nichtattraktiven gleichs der Verhältnisse in den beiden Gruppen
Teilnehmenden
Ratio Attractive Nonattractive p
N Percentage Mean SD Min Max
Mean SD Mean SD
Nonattractive (30)
R1 1.481 0.570 1.515 0.084 0.079
Class I
R2 1.477 0.710 1.407 0.717 0.000***
Female 6 20 1.88 0.34 1.30 2.20
R3 2.100 0.269 1.939 0.367 0.057
Male 3 10 2.13 0.06 1.10 2.20
R4 1.479 0.077 1.616 0.362 0.048*
Total Cl I 9 30 2.01 0.18 1.10 2.20
R5 1.613 0.159 1.798 0.439 0.035*
Class II
R6 1.482 0.169 1.596 0.265 0.052
Female 8 26.7 1.85 0.24 1.50 2.20
R7 2.703 0.196 2.818 0.649 0.359
Male 7 23.3 1.70 0.35 1.10 2.20
R8 1.395 0.100 1.360 0.132 0.257
Total Cl II 15 50 1.80 0.23 1.10 2.20
R9 1.886 0.132 1.882 0.211 0.931
Class III
R10 3.071 0.188 3.008 0.219 0.238
Female 4 13 2.05 0.17 1.80 2.20
R11 1.749 0.127 1.921 0.282 0.003**
Male 2 7 2.10 0.00 2.10 2.20
R12 1.892 0.104 1.961 0.128 0.027*
Total Cl III 6 20 2.07 0.14 1.80 2.20
Attractive (30) SD standard deviation
Class I * p B 0.05; ** p B 0.01; *** p B 0.001
Female 9 30 5.93 0.35 5.50 6.50
Male 4 13 5.48 0.05 5.40 5.50
Total Cl I 13 43 5.65 0.18 5.40 6.50 Tab. 6 Descriptive statistics and results of one sample t test indi-
Class II cating the divergence of ratios from the divine proportion
Tab. 6 Deskriptive Statistik und Ergebnisse des Einstichproben-t-
Female 11 36 6.07 0.81 5.40 7.80
Tests zur Darstellung der Abweichung von Verhältnissen im Gesicht
Male 2 7 6.10 0.85 5.50 6.70 von denen des Goldenen Schnitts
Total Cl II 13 43 6.08 0.82 5.40 7.80
Ratio Attractive Nonattractive
Class III
Female 2 7 5.85 0.21 5.70 6.00 Mean SD p Mean SD p
Male 2 7 6.35 1.06 5.60 7.10 R1 1.481 0.570 0.001 1.515 0.084 0.001
Total Cl III 4 14 6.01 1.01 5.60 7.10 R2 1.477 0.710 0.001 1.407 0.717 0.001
For nonattractive group; Fgender 0.193, p 0.665; Fmalocclusion 3.190, R3 2.100 0.269 0.001 1.939 0.367 0.001
p 0.059; Fgender and malocclusion interact 1.381, p 0.271. For attractive R4 1.479 0.077 0.001 1.616 0.362 0.973*
group; Fgender 0.006, p 0.938; Fmalocclusion 1.053, p 0.365; Fgen- R5 1.613 0.159 0.877* 1.798 0.439 0.033
der and malocclusion interact 0.947, p 0.402
R6 1.482 0.169 0.001 1.596 0.265 0.654*
SD standard deviation, Cl class
R7 2.703 0.196 0.001 2.818 0.649 0.001
R8 1.395 0.100 0.001 1.36 0.132 0.001
R9 1.886 0.132 0.001 1.882 0.211 0.001
According to him, a Miss America had a slightly con-
R10 3.071 0.188 0.001 3.008 0.219 0.001
cave skeletal pattern, a runner-up Miss Universe had a
R11 1.749 0.127 0.001 1.921 0.282 0.001
less prominent chin, and a Miss Virginia had a Class II
R12 1.892 0.104 0.001 1.961 0.128 0.001
malocclusion. This knowledge shows that each human
face is individually determined, carrying information * p C 0.05
that allows the identification of the single person [32].
Even among attractive faces, there are a variety of facial
characteristics that do not repeat the same standard [22]. facial attractiveness, and moreover, the divine proportion
The results of the present study indicate that sagittal is not a standard for esthetic perception. Consequently, it
skeletal malocclusions do not have any effect on frontal can be said that ‘‘beauty is subjective.’’

123
The effects of malocclusions on facial attractiveness

Fig. 5 Graphical presentation


of the divergence of facial ratios
from the divine proportion
Abb. 5 Graphische Darstellung
der Abweichungen der fazialen
Verhältniswerte von den
Goldenen-Schnitt-Proportionen

Conclusions orthodontic/orthognathic surgical treatment. The Int J Adult


Orthodon Orthognath Surg 16:108–120
4. Ben-Bassat Y, Dinte A, Brin I, Koyoumdjisky-Kaye E (1992)
The results of this study can be summarized as follows: Cephalometric pattern of Jewish East European adolescents with
clinically acceptable occlusion. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop
• Most of the investigated ratios showed no statistically 102:443–448
significant difference between attractive and nonattrac- 5. Bishara SE, Abdalla EM, Hoppens BJ (1990) Cephalometric
tive subjects. comparisons of dentofacial parameters between Egyptian and
• Nearly all investigated facial ratios diverged greatly North American adolescents. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop
97:413–421. doi:10.1016/0889-5406(90)70113-Q
from the divine proportion. 6. De Smit A, Dermaut L (1984) Soft-tissue profile preference. Am
• Sagittal skeletal malocclusions determined by the ANB J Orthod 86:67–73
angle have no effect on the subjects’ frontal facial 7. Ferrario VF, Sforza C, Miani A, Tartaglia G (1993) Craniofacial
attractiveness. morphometry by photographic evaluations. Am J Orthod
Dentofacial Orthop 103:327–337. doi:10.1016/0889-5406(93)
70013-E
Compliance with ethical guidelines 8. Ferrario VF, Sforza C, Poggio CE, Tartaglia G (1995) Facial
morphometry of television actresses compared with normal
Conflict of interest S. Akan, A.G. Torgut, and H. Oktay declare that women. J Oral Maxillofac Surg 53:1008–1014 (discussion
they have no conflicting interests. 1014–1005)
9. Herzberg BL (1952) Facial esthetics in relation to orthodontic
Ethical approval All procedures performed in this study involving treatment. Angel Orthod 22:9
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of 10. Holdaway RA (1983) A soft-tissue cephalometric analysis and its
the Istanbul Medipol University institutional research committee use in orthodontic treatment planning. Part I. Am J Orthod
(with the number of 108400987-96-85) and with the 1964 Helsinki 84:1–28
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. 11. Howells DJ, Shaw WC (1985) The validity and reliability of
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants ratings of dental and facial attractiveness for epidemiologic use.
included in the study. Am J Orthod 88:402–408
12. Hungerford MW (1890) Molly Bawn. Smith, Elder and Com-
Funding This study was not funded by any company or commitee. pany, London
13. Jefferson Y (2004) Facial beauty-establishing a universal stan-
dard. Int J Orthod 15:9–22
14. Kawakami S, Tsukada S, Hayashi H, Takada Y, Koubayashi S
References (1989) Golden proportion for maxillofacial surgery in orientals.
Ann Plast Surg 23:417–425
1. Angle EH (1899) Classification of malocclusion. Dental Cosmos 15. Kerns LL, Silveira AM, Kerns DG, Regennitter FJ (1997)
41:248–264 Esthetic preference of the frontal and profile views of the same
2. Arnett GW, Bergman RT (1993) Facial keys to orthodontic smile. J Esthet Dent 9:76–85
diagnosis and treatment planning. Part I. Am J Orthod Dentofa- 16. Kiekens RM, Kuijpers-Jagtman AM, van’t Hof MA, van’t Hof
cial Orthop 103:299–312. doi:10.1016/0889-5406(93)70010-L BE, Maltha JC (2008) Putative golden proportions as predictors
3. Baker BW, Woods MG (2001) The role of the divine proportion of facial esthetics in adolescents. Am J Orthod Dentofacial
in the esthetic improvement of patients undergoing combined Orthop 134:480–483. doi:10.1016/j.ajodo.2006.10.041

123
S. Akan et al.

17. Koury ME, Epker BN (1992) Maxillofacial esthetics: anthropo- 28. Ricketts RM (1982) Divine proportion in facial esthetics. Clin
metrics of the maxillofacial region. J Oral Maxillofac Surg Plast Surg 9:401–422
50:806–820 29. Rodriguez GLC, Cambron ZH, Vargas RM (2014) Relationship
18. Lusterman EA (1963) The esthetics of the occidental face: a between facial golden ratio and malocclusion in Mexican patients
study of dentofacial morphology based upon anthropologic cri- who attended the Orthodontics Clinic at Facultad de Odontologı́a
teria. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 49:826–850 de la Universidad Tecnológica de México during 2009 with facial
19. Macias Gago AB, Romero Maroto M, Crego A (2012) The per- aesthetics criteria evaluated with Marquardt mask. Revista
ception of facial aesthetics in a young Spanish population. Eur J Mexicana de Ortodoncia 2:9–17
Orthod 34:335–339. doi:10.1093/ejo/cjr014 30. Ronchi P, Guariglia A (2011) Surgical orthodontic treatment of
20. Matoula S, Pancherz H (2006) Skeletal Morphology of attractive class III malocclusions. In: Naretto S (ed) Principles in contem-
and nonattractive faces. Angle Orthod 76:204–210 porary orthodontics. In Tech, Rijeka, Croatia, p 419
21. Moorrees CF (1994) Natural head position—a revival. Am J 31. Rossetti A, De Menezes M, Rosati R, Ferrario VF, Sforza C
Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 105:512–513. doi:10.1016/S0889- (2013) The role of the golden proportion in the evaluation of
5406(94)70014-1 facial esthetics. Angle Orthodt 83:801–808. doi:10.2319/111812-
22. Naini FB, Moss JP, Gill DS (2006) The enigma of facial beauty: 883.1
esthetics, proportions, deformity, and controversy. Am J Orthod 32. Sforza C, Laino A, D’Alessio R, Grandi G, Binelli M, Ferrario
Dentofacial Orthop 130:277–282. doi:10.1016/j.ajodo.2005.09. VF (2009) Soft-tissue facial characteristics of attractive Italian
027 women as compared to normal women. Angle Orthod 79:17–23.
23. Peck H, Peck S (1970) A concept of facial esthetics. Angle doi:10.2319/122707-605.1
Orthod 40:284–318. doi:10.1043/0003-3219(1970)040\0284: 33. Shell TL, Woods MG (2004) Facial aesthetics and the divine
ACOFE[2.0.CO;2 proportion: a comparison of surgical and non-surgical class II
24. Peron APLM, Morosini IC, Correia KR, Moresca R, Petrelli E treatment. Aust Orthod J 20:51–63
(2012) Photometric study of divine proportion and its correlation 34. Tedesco LA, Albino JE, Cunat JJ, Slakter MJ, Waltz KJ (1983) A
with facial attractiveness. Dental Press J Orthod 17:124–131 dental-facial attractiveness scale. Part II. Consistency of percep-
25. Phillips C, Beal KN (2009) Self-concept and the perception of tion. Am J Orthod 83:44–46
facial appearance in children and adolescents seeking orthodontic 35. Tweed CH (1944) Indications for the extraction of teeth in
treatment. Angle Orthod 79:12–16. doi:10.2319/071307-328.1 orthodontic procedure. Am J Orthod Oral Surg 42:22–45
26. Proffit WR (2000) The soft tissue paradigm in orthodontic 36. Tweed CH (1954) Frankfort mandibular incisor angles in diag-
diagnosis and treatment planning: a new view for a new century. nosis, treatment planning and prognosis. Angle Orthod
J Esthet Dent 12:46–49 24:121–169
27. Ricketts RM (1982) The biologic significance of the divine
proportion and Fibonacci series. Am J Orthod 81:351–370

123

You might also like