Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy © 2015 American Psychological Association

2016, Vol. 8, No. 3, 259 –266 1942-9681/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000073

Shattering World Assumptions: A Prospective View of the Impact of


Adverse Events on World Assumptions
Eric R. Schuler and Adriel Boals
University of North Texas

Shattered Assumptions theory (Janoff-Bulman, 1992) posits that experiencing a traumatic event has the
potential to diminish the degree of optimism in the assumptions of the world (assumptive world), which
could lead to the development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Prior research assessed the assumptive
world with a measure that was recently reported to have poor psychometric properties (Kaler et al., 2008).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The current study had 3 aims: (a) to assess the psychometric properties of a recently developed measure
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of the assumptive world, (b) to retrospectively examine how prior adverse events affected the optimism
of the assumptive world, and (c) to measure the impact of an intervening adverse event. An 8-week
prospective design with a college sample (N ⫽ 882 at Time 1 and N ⫽ 511 at Time 2) was used to assess
the study objectives. We split adverse events into those that were objectively or subjectively traumatic
in nature. The new measure exhibited adequate psychometric properties. The report of a prior objective
or subjective trauma at Time 1 was related to a less optimistic assumptive world. Furthermore,
participants who experienced an intervening objectively traumatic event evidenced a decrease in
optimistic views of the world compared with those who did not experience an intervening adverse event.
We found support for Shattered Assumptions theory retrospectively and prospectively using a reliable
measure of the assumptive world. We discuss future assessments of the measure of the assumptive world
and clinical implications to help rebuild the assumptive world with current therapies.

Keywords: trauma, subjective trauma, shattered assumptions, prior trauma history, psychometrics

Shattered Assumption theory (Janoff-Bulman, 1989, 1992) cen- consists of 32 items that are divided into eight subscales that
ters on the destructive nature of a trauma on an individual’s belief include: benevolence of the world, benevolence of people, con-
system or assumptive world that focuses on beliefs related to the trollability, justice, self-worth, randomness, self-control, and luck.
world as benevolent, the world as meaningful, and the self as Prior research that has assessed the hypothesis that a trauma affects
worthy. Exposure to a traumatic event may provide information the assumptive world has reported mixed findings, which may be
about the nature of the world that is incongruent with the pre- due to measurement and design issues (Kaler et al., 2008). Indi-
trauma assumptions, which has the potential to shatter these beliefs viduals with prior trauma exposure reported less optimistic as-
(Janoff-Bulman, 1992). Individuals may attempt to rebuild their sumptive worlds compared with individuals with no prior traumas
assumptive worlds to incorporate the traumatic experience (e.g., in a cross-sectional study (Janoff-Bulman, 1989). In addition,
the world is mostly good, but some people cannot be trusted); breast cancer survivors (diagnosed 5 years earlier) reported less
however, it is during this process that the symptoms of posttrau- control and that the world was more random compared with the
matic stress disorder (PTSD) are believed to arise (Janoff-Bulman, matched control group (Tomich & Helgeson, 2002). However,
1992). Prior research assessing if a trauma affects the assumptive because these studies were cross-sectional, the change in the world
world are highlighted along with methodological and measurement assumptions pre- and posttrauma could not be assessed.
issues. The present study empirically assesses the psychometrics of Subsequently, some longitudinal research has attempted to as-
a new measure of the assumptive world and tests whether an sess if changes in the world assumptions were caused by either the
intervening traumatic event can affect the beliefs in a prospective experience of a trauma or by the development of PTSD symptoms.
design.1 Ginzburg (2004) had participants who were recovering from a
myocardial infarction and a matched control group completed the
Previous Research into Shattered Assumptions WAS and again 7 months later. Participants who both experienced
a myocardial infraction and developed PTSD at Time 2 reported
The most widely used measure of the assumptive world is the lower levels of luck, self-worth, and randomness compared with
World Assumptions Scale (WAS; Janoff-Bulman, 1989), which both the matched control and those who did not develop PTSD 7
months later (Ginzburg, 2004). The changes in assumptions were

This article was published Online First July 27, 2015.


Eric R. Schuler and Adriel Boals, Department of Psychology, University 1
The term prospective design is used here in regards to assessing the
of North Texas. impact of an intervening traumatic event after accounting for the impact of
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Adriel prior traumas. It is important to note that our design is not a truly
Boals, Department of Psychology, University of North Texas, 1155 Union prospective design because our sample does contain individuals with prior
Circle #311280, Denton, TX 76203. E-mail: adriel@unt.edu trauma exposure.

259
260 SCHULER AND BOALS

reportedly due to the development of PTSD rather than trauma with measures of optimism, satisfaction in life, and self-esteem,
exposure (Ginzburg, 2004). Ginzburg (2004) noted that not having reflecting poor convergent validity (Kaler et al., 2008). The use of
a pre-event measure of the assumptive world and the specificity of a measure with low reliability may diminish the accuracy of the
the trauma may limit generalizability. Prior stressful life events results and may account for the “inconsistencies in the empirical
were reported to impact the assumptive world of mothers whose literature” (Kaler et al., 2008, p. 332). On the basis of the theory,
children were undergoing a bone morrow transplant (Rini et al., there would be substantive differences in optimism (and large
2004). Mothers may have known their child’s diagnoses before effect sizes) after a trauma or when comparing the degree of
Time 1; as such, their assumptions may have already been altered optimism between trauma victims and a control group (Janoff-
(Rini et al., 2004). Without pre-event measures of the assumptive Bulman, 1989, 1992). Prior research has reported low effect sizes;
world, trauma’s impact on these beliefs could not be fully as- however, given the poor psychometric properties of the WAS, the
sessed. effect sizes may have been underestimated. As such, it is unclear
One methodological concern that could account for inconsistent what the expected effect sizes are.
findings is the use of cross-sectional, retrospective designs (Kaler In an unpublished dissertation, Kaler (2009) developed the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

et al., 2008). Cross-sectional designs can be problematic when World Assumptions Questionnaire (WAQ) to provide researchers
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

assessing for changes in the assumptive worlds in that pre-event with a reliable and valid measure of the assumptive world. The aim
measures cannot be collected (Kaler et al., 2008). Retrospective of the Kaler (2009) study was to create and assess the psychomet-
studies ask individuals to determine how much a trauma has rics of the WAQ. The WAQ comprises four subscales (Controlla-
violated their assumptive world after the fact and makes the bility of Events, Comprehensibility and Predictability of People,
assumption that these pretrauma views can be accessed by the Trustworthiness and Goodness of People, and Safety of the World)
individual (Edmondson et al., 2011). However, when individuals that are consistent with Shattered Assumptions theory. Kaler
are asked to make temporal comparisons of their pretrauma selves, (2009) implemented the study design and procedures from the
there may be a derogation of the earlier self (McFarland & Alvaro, Kaler et al. (2008) study. Undergraduate participants (N ⫽ 270)
2000). The previously discussed studies implemented retrospec- completed Time 2 of the study, and 36 individuals reported an
tive designs, which could explain some of the inconsistent find- intervening traumatic event and less optimistic assumptions of the
ings. world (Kaler, 2009). The WAQ subscales were reported to have
The aforementioned studies utilized cross-sectional and longi- adequate internal reliability across time points (ranged from .74 to
tudinal designs to assess whether experiencing a traumatic event .82) and test–retest reliability (ranged from .68 to .74) (Kaler,
can affect the assumptive world. A stronger research design would 2009, p. 47). The WAQ was hypothesized to have small to mod-
be to measure the assumptive worlds before and after a traumatic erate positive correlations with similar constructs and small to
event. College students were given the WAS at Time 1 and again moderate negative correlations with measures related to distress
approximately 8 weeks later to assess if they reported experiencing (Kaler, 2009). The WAQ exhibited adequate convergent validity
a traumatic event in the intervening time (Kaler et al., 2008). Kaler with self-report measures of the constructs of satisfaction with life
et al. (2008) posited that participants who were exposed to multiple (rs ranged from .11 to .36) and positive affect (rs ranged from .02
traumatic events would evidence less optimistic assumptions. This to .34) (Kaler, 2009, p. 47). Convergent validity for the specific
was based on the finding of Breslau, Chilcoat, Kessler, and Davis subscales were as follows: perceived control over the events was
(1999) that experiencing multiple traumas was associated with an correlated with the Control of Events subscale (r ⫽ .38), attach-
increased likelihood of PTSD. Of the 655 participants who com- ment was uncorrelated with the Comprehensibility and Predict-
pleted Time 2, 56 reported an intervening trauma (Kaler et al., ability of People subscale (rs ranged from ⫺.07 to .01), Trustwor-
2008). A majority of the intervening events were a sudden death of thiness and Goodness of People was correlated with a measure of
a loved one, an assault, or a serious injury (Kaler et al., 2008). trust (r ⫽ .69), and the Safety of the World subscale was nega-
However, there were no differences in the assumptions of victims tively correlated with a measure of the future likelihood of expe-
of an intervening trauma and nonvictims (Kaler et al., 2008). Kaler riencing a negative event (r ⫽ ⫺.19) (Kaler, 2009, p. 47). The
et al. (2008) suggested that the null findings were believed to be WAQ subscales had small to moderate negative correlations with
from the unreliable properties of the WAS. negative affect (rs ranged from ⫺.42 to ⫺.15), distress (rs ranged
Most research that has attempted to test the hypothesis that a from ⫺.46 to ⫺.17), and PTSD symptom severity (rs ranged
traumatic event shatters an individual’s assumptive world has from ⫺.36 to ⫺.24) (Kaler, 2009, p. 47). Kaler (2009, p. 47)
relied on the WAS (Janoff-Bulman, 1989), including the afore- reported that the WAQ subscales had adequate discriminate valid-
mentioned prospective study (Kaler et al., 2008). The WAS had ity based on the near-zero correlations with social desirability (rs
small correlations with the number of lifetime traumas, distress, ranged from ⫺.09 to .04). Kaler (2009) reported that the four
depression, and PTSD symptom severity (Kaler et al., 2008). factor structure had adequate model fit. On the basis of Kaler
Higher scores on the WAS (more optimism) should be related to (2009), the WAQ has good psychometric properties and is able to
less distress, PTSD symptom severity, and depression. Because of measure the core assumptions.
the small correlations, there was little support for the relationship The WAQ was recently used in an 8-week prospective design to
between violations to the assumptive world and PTSD symptoms. assess if an intervening traumatic event would cause reliable
The WAS was reported to have low internal reliability (ranged changes in the assumptive world, PTSD symptom severity, mental
from .60 to .83) and test–retest reliability (ranged from .38 to .65) and physical health, and distress (Anders, Frazier, & Shallcross,
in the control sample (Kaler et al., 2008). With the lack of temporal 2014). A potentially traumatic event was any highly distressing
reliability, assessing the shattering effect may be hindered by event that was nominated on a traumatic history that included
measurement error. The WAS had small to near-zero correlations events that do not meet the A1 criteria (e.g., being bullied) based
SHATTERED ASSUMPTIONS 261

on a pilot. Anders et al. (2014) had participants (N ⫽ 1,082) structure of the WAQ has not been assessed using a confirmatory
complete measures at Time 1 and Time 2. At Time 2, 153 partic- factor analysis outside of the unpublished dissertation. Third, we
ipants reported experiencing a highly distressing, potentially trau- examined objective and subjective traumatic events and their im-
matic event since Time 1 on the measure of potentially traumatic pact on world assumptions. In the current study, an objective
events for the 8-week interim (Anders et al., 2014). There was a trauma was an event nominated on a trauma history questionnaire
decrease in the Control WAQ subscale, but no differences or without necessarily meeting the DSM–IV–TR’s A1 and A2 criteria
reliable change on the other subscales (Anders et al., 2014). (APA, 2000, p. 463). The data were collected while the DSM–IV–
However, there were changes in distress and mental and physical TR’s (APA, 2000) criteria for PTSD were still in use; however, at
wellbeing (Anders et al., 2014). The WAQ subscales were reported the end of data collection, the field shifted to the DSM 5th edition
to have good internal reliability (Anders et al., 2014); however, (DSM-5; APA, 2013), which has a narrower criterion for the
further psychometric assessments are needed. diagnosis of PTSD. As such, the DSM–IV–TR criteria were imple-
Most prior research has focused on the impact of Diagnostic and mented. Subjective traumas are events in the top quartile of CES
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition—Text scores (Boals et al., 2015). There were three specific hypotheses:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Revision (DSM–IV–TR; American Psychiatric Association [APA],


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2000) defined traumatic events on the assumptive world. Adverse Hypothesis 1: The WAQ would exhibit adequate psychomet-
events (e.g., the dissolution of a serious relationship) may not meet ric properties related to internal reliability, test–retest reliabil-
the A1 and A2 criteria of the DSM–IV–TR; however, they could be ity, and convergent validity. To assess the convergent validity,
traumatic for the individual if it became an core part of the the WAQ would be related to less depression, negative affect,
individual’s life story, called subjective traumas (Boals & event centrality, negative thoughts about the world, and PTSD
Schuettler, 2009; Boals, Southard-Dobbs, & Blumenthal, 2015). In symptoms, but be related to more life satisfaction and positive
the previously mentioned study by Anders et al. (2014), potentially affect.
traumatic events that were high in distress but may not meet the
Hypothesis 2: Individuals who have experienced a prior sub-
DSM–IV–TR criteria for PTSD could be considered subjective
jective or objective traumatic event would have a less opti-
traumas. A way to assess if an event was subjectively traumatic is
mistic assumptive world (lower WAQ scores) compared with
to see if the event has become a central part of the life story, called
individuals with no prior traumatic experience.
event centrality (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006). A subjective trauma is
one that has become highly central to their life story, reflected by Hypothesis 3: Individuals who experienced an intervening
high scores on the Centrality of Events Scale (CES; Berntsen & traumatic event (either objective or subjective) between Time
Rubin, 2006). Highly central events were determined by a quartile 1 and Time 2 would evidence a less optimistic assumptive
split on the CES, with the highest 25% reflecting subjective world compared with individuals who did not.
traumas (Boals et al., 2015). Highly central events were reported to
correspond to higher levels of PTSD symptom severity and dis-
tress (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006). Subjective and objective traumatic Method
events could potentially shatter the world assumptions.
Participants
Present Study
Participants (N ⫽ 882) were recruited from a midsized univer-
In the present study, we tested Shattered Assumptions theory sity in the southern United States. Participants received partial
through the use of several methodological improvements over past course credit in their psychology courses for participating. There
studies. We replicated the design used in Anders et al. (2014) and were no exclusionary criteria for this study. The demographic data
Kaler et al. (2008) to obtain a baseline measure of the assumptive of gender, ethnicity, and age for Time 1 and Time 2 are reported
world and had participants complete Time 2 approximately 8 in Table 1.
weeks later to assess if there was an intervening adverse life event
and if the assumptive world was affected. Although 8 weeks is a
Measures
relatively short time for a potentially traumatic event to occur, this
time frame was selected to replicate the methodology of Anders et The WAQ (Kaler, 2009) consists of 25 items that correspond to
al. (2014) and Kaler et al. (2008). The 8-week interim allowed 4 the three core assumptions posited by Janoff-Bulman (1992). The
weeks to collect Time 1 data at the beginning of the semester and participants rate their agreement toward statements about their
3 weeks to collect Time 2 data before the participant pool closed. assumptions of the world using 6-point scales ranging from 1
At Time 1, participants completed the WAQ and measures of (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree), with seven items being
similar constructs and measures of distress to assess convergent reverse coded. Lower scores on the WAQ reflect less optimistic
and discriminant validity. Second, we used the WAQ to assess the assumptions. An example item is “People often behave in unpre-
assumptive world, which appears to be a reliable measure of the dictable ways.” The WAQ subscales been reported to have ade-
assumptive world. However, the WAQ was only used in a couple quate internal reliability (ranges from .64 to .83) (Anders et al.,
of studies (Anders et al., 2014; Kaler, 2009) and further psycho- 2014), good test–retest reliability (subscales ranged from .68 to
metric assessment is needed. Internal reliability and test–retest .74), and adequate convergent and discriminate validity (Kaler,
reliability were assessed to determine if WAQ had adequate psy- 2009). In the current study, the WAQ total scores had an internal
chometric properties. In the current study, the WAQ total scores reliability of .82 at Time 1 and ␣ ⫽ .84 at Time 2 and had a
were used instead of the four subscales because the underlying test–retest reliability of .67.
262 SCHULER AND BOALS

Table 1 To assess the participant’s past traumatic history, the Stressful


Descriptive Statistics of Gender, Ethnicity, and Age Reported at Life Events Screening Questionnaire (SLESQ; Goodman, Corc-
Time 1 and Time 2 oran, Turner, Yuan, & Green, 1998) was used. The SLESQ con-
sists of 11 specific categories of traumatic events and two general
Time 1 Time 2 events to assess the lifetime exposure to traumatic life events in
Variable N % N % nontreatment-seeking participants. The SLESQ has been found to
have adequate convergent validity and test–retest reliability, and it
Gender
Men 255 28.90 150 29.40
can differentiate between A1 and non-A1 criteria for PTSD diag-
Women 627 71.10 361 70.60 nosis in a college sample of women (Goodman et al., 1998). A
Total 882 100.00 511 100.00 modified version of the SLESQ (items that ask about childhood
Ethnicity traumas were removed) was used to assess for an intervening
Caucasian 451 51.10 277 54.20 trauma at Time 2.
African American 129 14.60 65 12.70
Asian 58 6.60 29 5.70 The PTSD Checklist–Specific (PCL-S; Weathers, Litz, Herman,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hispanic 200 22.70 119 23.30 Huska, & Keane, 1993) was used to assess the severity of PTSD
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Native American 5 0.60 4 0.80 symptoms. Participants nominated their most traumatic event and
Other 39 4.40 17 3.30 then rated the frequency they have experienced each item of the 17
882 100.00 511 100.00
items using a 5-point scale that ranged from 1 (not at all) to 5
Mean SD Mean SD (extremely). Wilkins, Lang, and Norman (2011) synthesized the
psychometric properties of the PCL-S from 19 studies. The PCL-S
Age (in years) 19.89 3.68 19.87 3.52 was reported to have adequate test–retest reliability (.66), conver-
Note. Self-reported demographic information collected from the partici- gent validity with other measures of PTSD symptom severity, and
pants at Time 1 and Time 2. an internal consistency (⬎.75) (Wilkins et al., 2011). In the current
study, the internal reliability was ␣ ⫽ .95 at Time 1 and ␣ ⫽ .96
at Time 2.
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, The CES (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006) is a measure of the degree
Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) assesses participants’ degrees of positive to which a nominated event has become an integral part of the life
and negative emotions on two separate subscales, each consisting story. Participants were asked to use their most adverse life event
of 10 single-word items. Participants are asked how often they in reference to the items. The CES consists of 20 items that
experienced the emotion in the last 2 weeks using 5-point scales participants rated how strongly they agree with each statement
ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 (extremely). Watson using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally
et al. (1988) reported adequate psychometric properties for the agree). Prior research has reported that the CES has high internal
PANAS. In the current study, the internal reliability for the posi- reliability across college samples (range: .93 to .95) and was
tive affect subscale was ␣ ⫽ .89 at Time 1 and ␣ ⫽ .92 at Time positively correlated with PTSD symptom severity (r ⫽ .38) and
2. The internal reliability for the negative affect subscale of the depression symptoms (r ⫽ .23) (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006). In the
PANAS was ␣ ⫽ .87 at Time 1 and ␣ ⫽ .86 at Time 2. current study, the internal reliability was ␣ ⫽ .97 at Time 1 and
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener, Emmons, ␣ ⫽ .98 at Time 2.
Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) is a measure of how much a person feels The Negative Cognitions about the World (NCW) subscale of
happy with how things are in their life. The scale consists of five the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI; Foa, Elhers, Clark,
items in which participants rate their agreement using 7-point Tolin, & Orsillo, 1999) was used to assess the specific negative
scales ranging from 1 (the statement is absolutely untrue) to 7 (the cognitions related to an adverse event. The NCW subscale contains
statement is absolutely true). The SWLS was reported to have seven items in which participants rate their agreement using a
good convergent and discriminate validity (Pavot & Diener, 1993). 7-point scale ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree).
In the current study, the internal reliability of ␣ ⫽ .87 at Time 1 Beck et al. (2004) reported that the PTCI had adequate structural
and was ␣ ⫽ .90 at Time 2. and convergent validity. In the current study, the internal reliability
The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales (DASS-21; Lovi- at Time 1 was ␣ ⫽ .92 and ␣ ⫽ .92 at Time 2.
bond & Lovibond, 1995) is a 21-item scale that is composed of In the demographic questionnaire, participants were asked to
three subscales (depression, anxiety, and stress). Each of the three click and/or type in answers that corresponded to their gender, age,
subscales was measured with seven items. Participants rate how and ethnicity.
frequently in the last 2 weeks they experienced each of the items
using 4-point scales ranging from 0 (does not apply) to 3 (very
Procedure
much so). The DASS-21 has previously been reported to have
good internal consistency and concurrent validity (Antony, Biel- Participants completed the measures as part of a larger online
ing, Cox, Enns, & Swinson, 1998). For the current study, the two study. At Time 1, participants completed all questionnaires in the
items about suicide ideation were removed because of the online order listed above. At Time 1, if the participant nominated expe-
nature of the study and not having a protocol in place if partici- riencing multiple traumatic events on the SLESQ, then they were
pants reported high suicide ideation. Using this modified DASS asked which was their most traumatic and to use that event in
affects the scoring of the measure with a lower maximum and reference to completing the CES and PCL-S. If there was no prior
means. The modified DASS had an internal reliability of .94 at trauma nominated on the SLESQ, then participants were asked
Time 1 and .96 at Time 2. what their most stressful life event was and to use that event in
SHATTERED ASSUMPTIONS 263

reference to the CES and PCL-S. To replicate the methodology of ability at both time points (.82 and .84, respectively) and test–retest
Anders et al. (2014) and Kaler et al. (2008), Time 2 was completed reliability (.67). To assess convergent validity at Time 1, WAQ
approximately 8 weeks after Time 1. During the completion of the scores were correlated with the DASS, PCL-S, PANAS-Negative
Trauma History Questionnaire at Time 2, participants were asked, Affect subscale, PANAS-Positive Affect subscale, SWLS, CES,
“Since the completion of the Time 1 survey, have you experienced and the NCW subscale of the PTCI (see Table 2). The number of
a traumatic or negative life event?” If participants reported an objective traumas was predicted to be negatively correlated with
intervening event, then that event was used in reference to the Time 1 WAQ scores, suggesting that additional traumas could
PCL-S and CES. If there were no reported intervening traumatic further diminish the assumptive world (Kaler et al., 2008). There
events, then participants were asked if they experienced a stressful was a small correlation between Time 1 WAQ scores and the
life event. This procedure was approved by the Institutional Re- number of objective traumas at Time 1. The WAQ evidenced good
view Board of the University of North Texas. convergent validity based on the correlational table (see Table 2).
The WAQ had moderately negative correlations with negative
Results affect, NCW, and PCL-S while having a positive relationship with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the SWLS. The correlations of Time 1 WAQ scores and measures


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

At Time 1, 882 participants completed the online study. Of the


of SWLS, Positive Affect, number of prior traumas, and CES were
initial sample, 243 participants dropped out of the study (27.70%
significant, but below the expected r ⱖ |.30|. Table 3 reports the
attrition rate) at Time 2. At Time 2, 126 participants nominated a
frequencies and types of prior and intervening objective traumatic
prior traumatic event instead of an intervening event; this was
based on the fact that the dates of the events reported had occurred events that were nominated. In summary, the WAQ evidenced
before the completion of Time 1. These participants who did not adequate psychometric properties.
follow the instructions at Time 2 were removed from the Time 2 To test the second hypothesis that individuals who have expe-
data analyses. During data cleaning, two participants were re- rienced a prior adverse event will have a less optimistic assumptive
moved from the data set because of selecting the same response on world (lower WAQ Time 1 scores) compared with individuals
every measure. This left a sample of 511 participants who com- with no prior traumatic event, we conducted two independent-
pleted Time 2. To assess for differences between completers and sample t tests. We classified adverse events into one of two
noncompleters in the Time 1 data that could account for the nonindependent groups: (a) objective traumatic events (reported
attrition rate, a series of independent t tests and ␹2 tests were one of the events on the SLESQ) or (b) subjective traumatic events
conducted on the measures of WAQ, CES, gender, and ethnicity. (evidenced a CES total score ⬎73.00, the threshold for the top
There were no statistically significant differences between com- quartile split). Table 4 reports the sample sizes, means, standard
pleters and noncompleters on CES, t(878) ⫽ 1.20, p ⫽ .231; on deviations, and t test results for both of the groups. As noted in
WAQ, t(879) ⫽ .96, p ⫽ .338; or based on gender, ␹2(1) ⫽ .29, Table 4, there were significant differences in WAQ scores between
p ⫽ .600. There was a nonsignificant trend of Caucasian partici- the groups for both classifications.
pants being less likely to attrite, ␹2(5) ⫽ 11.03, p ⫽ .051. On the The third hypothesis of the study was that individuals who
basis of these analyses, there were no differences between com- reported an intervening adverse event (using both nonindependent
pleters and noncompleters at Time 2. classifications) between Time 1 and Time 2 would have less
The measures of interest were assessed for internal reliability at optimistic assumptions compared with those without an interven-
both time points to determine the stability of the measures. The ing event. At Time 2, 75 participants reported an intervening
means, standard deviations, and correlations for the WAQ and the objective trauma and 44 experienced a subjective trauma. Changes
measures of interest are reported in Table 2. To assess the first in the WAQ between time points and groups were assessed by two
hypothesis that the WAQ would have adequate internal reliability, separate multiple regressions. The intervening variables of trauma
Cronbach’s ␣ was examined. The WAQ had good internal reli- for each category were coded as 0 (no event) or 1 (self-reported

Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlational Matrix of the Measures of Convergent Validity

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1WAQ 75.71 12.25 —


1SWLS 23.21 6.74 .26ⴱⴱⴱ —
1DASS 12.62 11.02 ⫺.34ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.39ⴱⴱⴱ —
1NA 22.55 7.69 ⫺.37ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱⴱ .62ⴱⴱⴱ —
1PA 33.71 7.95 .10ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.06 —
1PCL 34.61 16.07 ⫺.35ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ .53ⴱⴱⴱ .44ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.02 —
1CES 56.77 21.80 ⫺.17ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.11ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱⴱ .22ⴱⴱⴱ .08ⴱ .51ⴱⴱⴱ —
1NCW 70.92 34.17 ⫺.40ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ .42ⴱⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.08ⴱ .50ⴱⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱⴱ —
PT 1.67 1.60 ⫺.18ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱⴱ .19ⴱⴱⴱ .00 .41ⴱⴱⴱ .32ⴱⴱⴱ .39ⴱⴱⴱ —
2WAQ 78.52 12.31 .67ⴱⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.28ⴱⴱⴱ .15ⴱⴱ ⫺.24ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.16ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.38ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.14ⴱⴱ
2CES 44.16 21.38 .09ⴱ ⫺.13ⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱⴱ .13ⴱⴱ .02 .29ⴱⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱⴱ .16ⴱⴱⴱ .16ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.16ⴱⴱⴱ —
Note. 1 ⫽ Time 1, 2 ⫽ Time 2, NA ⫽ PANAS–Negative Subscale; PA ⫽ PANAS–Positive Subscale; NCW ⫽ Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory:
Negative Cognitions of the World; PT ⫽ Number of prior traumas.

p ⬍ .050. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .010. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
264 SCHULER AND BOALS

Table 3
Frequencies of the Most Traumatic Prior and Intervening Events on the SLESQ

Prior trauma Intervening trauma


Type of traumatic or stressful life event nominations nominations

Had a threatening life illness 32 1


Life-threatening accident 69 4
Robbery/mugging 10 0
Death a family member or friend 151 8
Sexual assault 91 4
Physical abuse as a child 22 N/A
Physical abuse as an adult 31 3
Emotional abuse 106 44
Threatened with a weapon 12 2
Witnessing a death, injury, or assault 31 3
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

In a situation in which your life was in danger 18 1


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

In a situation in which you felt helpless 60 7


Other event 99 3
Note. The trauma category of physical abuse as a child was removed from the trauma history questionnaire at
Time 2 to reduce the chances of a Time 1 prior trauma being nominated. N/A ⫽ not applicable; SLESQ ⫽
Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire.

event). Regression models for each category were as follows: Time world as a trait construct and not directly affected by mood. Thus,
2 WAQ scores being predicted by Time 1 WAQ scores and the it appears the WAQ is a reliable and valid measure of the assump-
intervening trauma variable. If the intervening trauma variables tive world.
were statistically significant after controlling for Time 1 WAQ The second hypothesis of the negative impact of prior subjective
scores, then reporting an event accounted for unique variance of or objective trauma on an individual’s assumptive world was
Time 2 scores. Experiencing an intervening objective trauma was supported. The subjective measure of previous trauma had the
a significant predictor of Time 2 WAQ scores. The omnibus largest reduction in the assumptive world compared with the prior
regression results, coefficients, and squared semipartial correla- objective trauma. This reported finding that prior subjective or
tions are reported in Table 5. objective traumas can negatively impact an individual’s assump-
tive world is consistent with previous studies assessing differences
Discussion in the assumptive world based on objective measures of trauma
Empirical research to assess if experiencing a traumatic event (Janoff-Bulman, 1989, 1992; Park, Mills, & Edmondson, 2012).
may shatter the assumptive world has reported mixed findings Individuals who had subjective traumas had lower optimism in
(Kaler et al., 2008). The use of cross-sectional designs and mea- their assumptive world compared with individuals with low event
sures with poor psychometrics can diminish the ability to detect centrality. This suggests the importance of the individual’s sub-
changes in the assumption after a trauma (Kaler et al., 2008). The jective experience in trauma research. Furthermore, this finding
first hypothesis was to assess a new measure of the assumptive reflects that the assumptive world can be affected by a broad range
world—the WAQ. This hypothesis was supported in that the WAQ of traumatic events.
was reported to have good internal reliability and test–retest reli- The third hypothesis that an intervening traumatic event would
ability. Time 1 WAQ scores had moderate correlations with mea- result in a reduction of the optimism of the assumptive world was
sures of negative affect, PTSD symptom severity, negative cogni- partially supported. There were reported small effects of the inter-
tions, and depression. The correlations with the WAQ and vening objective traumas explaining the variance of the Time 2
satisfaction with life and positive affect were smaller than antici- WAQ scores beyond that of Time 1 scores. Experiencing a sub-
pated, but they were significant. The small correlation between jective traumatic event was trending toward statistical significance
positive affect and WAQ could be indicative of the assumptive (p ⫽ .060), which could have been due to the small number of

Table 4
Comparing Assumptive Worlds Between Prior and No Prior Events Using the Classifications of
Subjective and Objective Trauma

Group N M (SD) t df Cohen’s d


ⴱⴱ
Prior objective trauma 732 75.14 (12.39) 3.09 879 .28
No prior objective trauma 149 78.52 (11.17)
Prior subjective trauma 209 71.98 (13.25) 5.11ⴱⴱⴱ 877 .41
No prior subjective trauma 672 76.87 (11.70)
Note. Statistical analyses were independent-sample t tests.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .010. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
SHATTERED ASSUMPTIONS 265

Table 5
Variance of Time 2 WAQ Total Scores by Time 1 Scores and Intervening Trauma Variable
2
DV Predictor F statistic Radj b SE B SSPC
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
T2 WAQ T1 WAQ F(2, 508) ⫽ 215.65 .46 .67 .03 .66 .44
Objective ⫺3.22ⴱⴱ 1.13 ⫺.09 .01
T2 WAQ T1 WAQ F(2, 508) ⫽ 209.94ⴱⴱⴱ .45 .67ⴱⴱⴱ .03 .66 .44
Subjective ⫺2.72 1.43 ⫺.06 .00
Note. DV ⫽ dependent variable; T1 ⫽ Time 1; T2 ⫽ Time 2; WAQ ⫽ World Assumptions Questionnaire;
Objective ⫽ intervening objective trauma variable; Subjective ⫽ intervening subjective trauma; Radj
2
⫽ adjusted
R2; b ⫽ unstandardized coefficient; SE ⫽ standard error of the unstandardized coefficient; B ⫽ ␤ weight;
SSPC ⫽ squared semipartial correlation; Trauma ⫽ intervening trauma reported between Time 1 and Time 2.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .010. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

intervening highly subjective traumas (n ⫽ 44). Alternatively, it of this finding, the assumptive world may be weakened rather than
could be that not enough time had passed since the intervening shattered by a trauma.
subjective trauma for it to become a highly central life event. The The current study offers empirical evidence of the shattering of
effect sizes were small, but there was unique variance explained by the assumptive world after an intervening objectively traumatic
the intervening trauma variables. On the basis of these results, event using a prospective design. There were significant differ-
experiencing intervening objective trauma was related to reduc- ences in the assumptive world based on having a prior trauma
tions in the assumptive world. This pattern of findings is consistent history, which replicated prior studies (Janoff-Bulman, 1989). It is
with Shattered Assumptions theory. interesting to note that prior subjective events had a larger effect
There were some important limitations in the current study to compared with subjective intervening events. A subjective trauma
consider. There was a large diversity of the types of traumas; potentially has an initially small impact and grows larger over time
therefore, no direct comparisons of which assumptions were shat- if it becomes a central part to the life story. This suggests that
tered by specific adverse events or the impact of childhood trau- research should consider the impact of subjective traumas on the
mas could be made. Although the use of an undergraduate conve- assumptive world. Clinicians can work with clients to specifically
nience sample is a limitation, individuals in this sample are not rebuild the assumptive world after a traumatic event. Prolonged
knowingly at high risk for experiencing a traumatic event (e.g., Exposure (PE) therapy (Peterson, Foa, & Riggs, 2011) and Cog-
soldiers and first responders may expect to encounter potentially nitive Processing Therapy (CPT; Williams, Galovski, Kattar, &
traumatic situations; Frazier et al., 2011). Our replication of the Resick, 2011) can help individuals rebuild their assumptive world.
8-week interim of Kaler et al. (2008) and Anders et al. (2014) Part of PE focuses on reducing of negative cognitions (e.g.,” I
between time points may not have been enough time for partici- deserve what happened to me”) by having the individual become
pants to potentially experience an intervening trauma; as such, few habituated to the event (Peterson et al., 2011). CPT (Williams et
participants reported experiencing an event. This interim allowed al., 2011) focuses on altering incongruent information of the
us to collect data from our sample during the same semester as trauma with the pretrauma beliefs, which parallels Shattered As-
Time 1; however, future research could benefit from a longer sumptions. Onsite clinicians could work with groups at high risk
interim. At the end of data collection, the field transitioned from for experiencing a trauma (e.g., first responders) to develop flex-
the DSM–IV–TR to the DSM–5; as such, we were unable to use the ibility in their beliefs to become more resilient. By better under-
updated criteria. The fact that we found that prior subjective standing how subjective and objective traumatic events can impact
traumas were reported to negatively impact the WAQ scores at assumptions of the world, clinical treatments can be improved to
Time 1 suggested that it is important to consider how these broader better rebuild these shattered assumptions.
traumas can also shatter the assumptive world. An additional
limitation was that the WAQ total scores were used instead of the
four WAQ subscales. The specific nature of the trauma could References
affect some assumptions whereas others are unchanged (Janoff- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
Bulman, 1992), which may obscure effects when looking at vari- ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.
ous traumas or using total scores. However, there were significant American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man-
differences in the assumptive world when using a summed total ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
score to assess the impact of prior and intervening adverse events. Anders, S. L., Frazier, P. A., & Shallcross, S. L. (2014). Changes in
There was no direct comparison between the WAQ and the WAS functioning following potentially traumatic life events in college stu-
based on the findings of Kaler et al. (2008). Future studies should dents. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy,
6, S99 –S106.
assess the WAQ factor structure and test whether the type of
Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P.
trauma can affect certain assumptions. Our results supported that
(1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of
an adverse event can affect the assumptive world, but the effect the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community
sizes were smaller than what was expected based on the theory sample. Psychological Assessment, 10, 176 –181.
(Janoff-Bulman, 1989, 1992). It is important to note that changes Beck, J. G., Coffey, S. F., Palyo, S. A., Gudmundsdottir, B., Miller, L. M.,
on measures pre- and postevent in longitudinal designs are gener- & Colder, C. R. (2004). Psychometric properties of the Posttraumatic
ally small (Anders et al., 2014; Bonanno et al., 2002). On the basis Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): A replication with motor vehicle accident
266 SCHULER AND BOALS

survivors. Psychological Assessment, 16, 289 –298. http://dx.doi.org/ Kaler, M. E. (2009). The World Assumptions Questionnaire: Development
10.1037/1040-3590.16.3.289 of a measure of the assumptive world. Doctoral dissertation. Retrieved
Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2006). The Centrality of Event Scale: A from ProQuest. (Order No. 3371864)
measure of integrating a trauma into one’s identity and its relation to Kaler, M. E., Frazier, P. A., Anders, S. L., Tashiro, T., Tomich, P., Tennen,
post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Behavior Research H., & Park, C. (2008). Assessing the psychometric properties of the
and Therapy, 44, 219 –231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.01.009 World Assumptions Scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 326 –332.
Boals, A., & Schuettler, D. (2009). PTSD symptoms in response to trau- http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20343
matic and non-traumatic events: The role of respondent perception and Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative
A2 criterion. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 458 – 462. http://dx.doi emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales
.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.09.003 (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour
Boals, A., Southard-Dobbs, S., & Blumenthal, H. (2015). Adverse events Research and Therapy, 33, 335–343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-
7967(94)00075-U
in emerging adulthood are associated with increases in neuroticism.
McFarland, C., & Alvaro, C. (2000). The impact of motivation on temporal
Journal of Personality, 83, 202–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy
comparisons: Coping with traumatic events by perceiving personal
.12095
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

growth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 327–343.


Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., Tweed, R. G., Haring,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.327
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

M., Sonnega, J., . . . Nesse, R. M. (2002). Resilience to loss and chronic


Park, C. L., Mills, M. A., & Edmondson, D. (2012). PTSD as meaning
grief: A prospective study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal violation: Testing a cognitive worldview perspective. Psychological
of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1150 –1164. http://dx.doi.org/ Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4, 66 –73. http://dx.doi
10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1150 .org/10.1037/a0018792
Breslau, N., Chilcoat, H. D., Kessler, R. C., & Davis, G. C. (1999). Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the Satisfaction with Life Scale.
Previous exposure to trauma and PTSD effects of subsequent trauma: Psychological Assessment, 5, 164 –172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-
Results from the Detroit Area Survey of Trauma. The American Journal 3590.5.2.164
of Psychiatry, 156, 902–907. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.6.902 Peterson, A. L., Foa, E. B., & Riggs, D. S. (2011). Prolonged exposure
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The therapy. In B. A. Moore & W. E. Penk (Eds.), Treating PTSD in military
Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, personnel: A clinical handbook (pp. 42–58). New York, NY: Guilford
71–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 Press.
Edmondson, D., Chaudoir, S. R., Mills, M. A., Park, C. L., Holub, J., & Rini, C., Manne, S., DuHamel, K. N., Austin, J., Ostroff, J., Boulad, F., . . .
Bartkowiak, J. M. (2011). From shattered assumptions to weakened Redd, W. H. (2004). Changes in mothers’ basic beliefs following a
worldviews: Trauma symptoms signal anxiety buffer disruption. Journal child’s bone marrow transplantation: The role of prior trauma and
of Loss and Trauma, 16, 358 –385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15325024 negative life events. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 325–333. http://
.2011.572030 dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:JOTS.0000038481.17167.0d
Foa, E. B., Elhers, A., Clark, D. M., Tolin, D. F., & Orsillo, S. M. (1999). Tomich, P. L., & Helgeson, V. S. (2002). Five years later: A cross-
The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI) development and vali- sectional comparison of breast cancer survivors with healthy women.
dation. Psychological Assessment, 11, 303–314. http://dx.doi.org/ Psycho-Oncology, 11, 154 –169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pon.570
10.1037/1040-3590.11.3.303 Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and vali-
Frazier, P., Gavian, M., Hirai, R., Park, C., Tennen, H., Tomich, P., & dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Tashiro, T. (2011). Prospective predictors of Posttraumatic Stress Dis-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
order symptoms: Direct and mediated relations. Psychological Trauma:
Weathers, F., Litz, B., Herman, D., Huska, J., & Keane, T. (1993). The
Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 3, 27–36. http://dx.doi.org/
PTSD Checklist (PCL): Reliability, validity, and diagnostic utility. Paper
10.1037/a0019894
presented at the annual convention of the International Society for
Ginzburg, K. (2004). PTSD and world assumptions following myocardial
Traumatic Stress Studies, San Antonio, TX.
infarction: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, Wilkins, K. C., Lang, A. J., & Norman, S. B. (2011). Synthesis of the
74, 286 –292. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.74.3.286 psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist (PCL) military, civilian,
Goodman, L. A., Corcoran, C., Turner, K., Yuan, N., & Green, B. L. and specific versions. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 596 – 606. http://dx
(1998). Assessing traumatic event exposure: General issues and prelim- .doi.org/10.1002/da.20837
inary findings for the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire. Williams, A. M., Galovski, T. E., Kattar, K. A., & Resick, P. A. (2011).
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 521–542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A: Cognitive processing therapy. In B. A. Moore & W. E. Penk (Eds.),
1024456713321 Treating PTSD in military personnel: A clinical handbook (pp. 59 –73).
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1989). Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic New York, NY: Guilford Press.
events: Applications of the scheme construct. Social Cognition, 7, 113–
136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.113 Received May 20, 2014
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychol- Revision received June 8, 2015
ogy of trauma. New York, NY: Free Press. Accepted June 13, 2015 䡲

You might also like