Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/20973577

Self-focused Attention in Clinical Disorders: Review and a Conceptual


Model

Article  in  Psychological Bulletin · April 1990


DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.156 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

849 1,291

1 author:

Rick E. Ingram
University of Kansas
140 PUBLICATIONS   9,127 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Attachment security View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Rick E. Ingram on 16 February 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
19^0,Vol. 107, No. 2, 156-176 0033-2909/90/500.75

Self-Focused Attention in Clinical Disorders:


Review and a Conceptual Model

Rick E. Ingram
San Diego State University

Working largely independently, numerous investigators have explored the role of self-focused atten-
tion in various clinical disorders. This article reviews research examining increased self-focused
attention in these disorders. Results indicate that regardless of the particular disorder under investi-
gation, a heightened degree of self-focused attention is found. Hence, as ordinarily conceptualized,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

self-focused attention has little discriminatory power among different psychological disorders. Using
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

information processing constructs, a somewhat different model of self-focused attention is proposed,


and it is suggested that certain deviations in this process constitute a psychopathological kind of
attention. A meta-construct model of descriptive psychopathology is then outlined to examine how
certain aspects of attention can be considered specific to certain disorders and others common to
different disorders.

Descriptive psychopathology efforts have profited from ex- Greenberg, 1987), anxiety (Carver & Scheier, 1986), test anxi-
amination of a number of cognitive variables. One cognitive ety(Sarason, 1975,1986; Wine, 1971, 1982), and social anxiety
variable that has generated much enthusiasm for such efforts is (Buss, 1980).
the construct of self-focused attention. Although this construct With the growing number of studies documenting associa-
was originally derived from social-psychological theory and re- tions between self-focused attention and a variety of clinical dis-
search on self-evaluation (Duval & Wicklund, 1972), Carver orders, it is important to consider how this particular cognitive
(1979) has denned it in this way: process may be related to so many different dysfunctional states
at the same time. Indeed, it would seem incumbent on any the-
When attention is self-directed, it sometimes takes the form of fo-
ory relating self-focused attention to a particular disorder to
cus on internal perceptual events, that is, information from those
sensory receptors that react to changes in bodily activity. Self-focus specify not only the relations between self-focusing and that dis-
may also take the form of an enhanced awareness of one's present order, but also how such attention can be related to other pre-
or past physical behavior, that is, a heightened cognizance of what sumably different disorders. It is not clear from such theoretical
one is doing or what one is like. Alternatively, self-attention can
approaches if self-focused attention is meaningful in any real
be an awareness of" the more or less permanently encoded bits of
information thai comprise, for example, one's attitudes. It can even sense if it is not specific to the disorder. With this issue in mind,
be an enhanced awareness of temporarily encoded bits of informa- this article (a) reviews the empirical evidence for various disor-
tion that have been gleaned from previous focus on the environ- ders and increased self-focused attention, (b) examines theoreti-
ment; subjectively, this would be experienced as a recollection or cal issues regarding the nature of self-focused attention, and (c)
impression of that previous event, (p. 1255)
builds on the empirical body of cognitive clinical, social-psy-
Self-focused attention has thus been defined as an awareness chological, and experimental cognitive attention theory and re-
of self-referent, internally generated information that stands in search to propose a descriptive framework for viewing the role
contrast to an awareness of externally generated information of self-focused attention processes in diverse states of psychopa-
derived through sensory receptors. thology. '
Clinically, the cognitive literature now abounds with reports
demonstrating a positive relation between a heightened degree
of attention focused on the self and diverse psychopathological 1
This review is generally limited to published data on adult psycho-
states. For example, several theoretical conceptualizations of logical disorders. Theory, data, and clinical descriptions, however, have
self-focused attention (e.g., Carver, 1979; Carver & Scheier, suggested that heightened self-focused attention is related to a variety
1981; Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Hull & Levy, 1979) have been of primarily physical disorders, including but not necessarily limited
related to a variety of specific clinical disorders such as alcohol to chronic pain (Turk, Meichenbaum, & Oenest, 1983) and coronary
abuse (Hull, 1981), depression (Lewinsohn, Hoberman, Teri, & disease (Scherwitz, Graham, Grandits, Buehler, & Billings, 1986; Scher-
Hautzinger, 1985; Musson & Alloy, 1988; Pyszczynski & witz et al., 1983). Increased self-focused attention has similarly been
related to psychological disorders of childhood (Dodge, 1985; Exner,
1973). Furthermore, some unpublished work has indicated that hypo-
manic individuals also evidence increased levels of self-focused atten-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rick tion (Shdo, 1989). Indeed, the apparent ubiquitousness of this process
E. Ingram, Doctoral Training Facility, Department of Psychology, San in disorder makes it difficult to find anything dysfunctional that is not
Diego State University, San Diego, California 92182-0551. accompanied by increased self-focused attention.

156
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 157

Clinical Disorders Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1985) had subjects work on both
an ostensible self-focus task (i.e., completing a task in front of
Depression a large mirror) and a non-self-focused task. Following this pro-
cedure, subjects were asked to rate their liking and preference
Several authors have discussed the theoretical possibility that for each task. Likability results indicated that nondepressed
an excessive degree of self-focused attention occurs in depres- subjects liked the self-focus task significantly more after success
sion and may mediate a subset of depressive features (e.g., Lew- than after failure, and depressed subjects liked the self-focus
insohn et al., 1985; Musson & Alloy, 1988; Pyszczynski & task more after failure than after success. Furthermore, de-
Greenberg, 1987). The first empirical study to demonstrate a pressed subjects preferred the self-focus task less after success
relation between depression (at a subclinical level) and self-fo- than did nondepressed subjects. No depressed-nondepressed
cused attention was reported by Smith and Greenberg (1981), preference differences were found in the failure condition.
who found a significant relation between the Private Self-Con- In a second study, Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1986) re-
sciousness subscale of the Self-Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein, peated this methodology with the exception that subjects were
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Scheier, & Buss, 1975), a dispositional measure of self-focusing, given a choice of which task to work on. Results suggested that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and a shortened version of the Minnesota Multiphasic Person- depressed subjects who succeeded spent less time working on
ality Inventory Depression Scale (Dempsey, 1964). These re- the self-focused task and were also less likely to initially choose
sults were subsequently replicated and extended in two studies this task than were nondepressed success subjects and depressed
by Ingram and Smith (1984). In the first study, the correlation failure and control (no-feedback) subjects. No differences were
between dispositional self-focusing and depression was repli- found among any of the nondepressed conditions. Although
cated in three different college student samples, using a different they did not actually assess self-focused attention, Pyszczynski
measure of depression (i.e., the Beck Depression Inventory, or and Greenberg (1985, 1986) interpreted these results to indi-
BDI; Beck, 1967). In the second study, depressed and nonde- cate that depressed individuals evidence a particular self-focus-
pressed college students were compared on a state measure of ing style.
self-focusing, in this case, a sentence completion scale (the Self- In another study (Greenberg & Pyszczynski, 1986), de-
Focus Sentence Completion Scale, or SFSC; Exner, 1973) that pressed and nondepressed college students were provided with
uses the number of self-references in subject completions as an either success or failure, and self-focusing was then assessed by
index of self-focused attention. As expected from the previous the number of self-referent responses on a sentence completion
studies, depressed subjects produced significantly more self-fo- scale. Results indicated that both depressed and nondepressed
cused responses than did nondepressed subjects. Two experi- subjects evidenced significantly greater self-focusing immedi-
ments reported by Smith, Ingram, and Roth (1985) again found ately following failure than they did following success. In a fol-
significant relations between measures of depression and self- low-up experiment, this procedure was repeated with the ex-
focused attention in college students. Finally, a recent study by ception that self-focused attention was assessed immediately
Larsen and Cowan (1988) also found a link between subclinical following the feedback and after a time delay. Although both
depression and self-focused attention in a naturalistic setting. In depressed and nondepressed subjects again evidenced an in-
this study, daily ratings of depression and self-focused attention crease in self-focusing after failure, nondepressed subjects de-
over a 2-month period were found to correlate significantly. creased self-focusing on delayed assessment and depressed sub-
Some research has also examined the association between in- jects were more likely to retain their initially high levels. Thus,
creased self-focused attention and clinical depression. Ingram, although both depressed and nondepressed individuals appear
Lumry, Cruet, and Sieber (1987), for instance, found that clini- to have similar self-focusing tendencies following failure, these
cally depressed outpatients had significantly greater levels of effects dissipate for nondepressed individuals, but are main-
state self-focused attention than did a sex- and age-matched tained over a longer period of time for depressed people.
community control sample. Interestingly, the level of self-fo- Only one published study has not found an increased level of
cused attention in this sample was strikingly similar to that of self-focused attention in depression. In the study by Exner
the subclinical sample of Ingram and Smith (1984). The key (1973) reporting the development of the SFSC, depressed sub-
difference in these samples was in the favorability of the self- jects were not found to have more self-focused responses on the
focused responses: Whereas subclinical subjects had roughly scale than nondepressed controls. This study must be regarded
equal numbers of positive and negative self-focused responses, cautiously, however, as raters were not blind to subject status
clinical subjects had significantly more negative than positive and interrater reliability data were not reported.
responses.
Several researchers have suggested that a particular self-fo- Depressive Features
cusing style may be characteristic of depression. Research has
found that self-focusing can be aversive after a negative event In their 1981 paper, Smith and Greenberg noted a number of
that cannot be rectified, and is thus avoided when possible (see parallels between the naturally occurring features of depression
Carver & Scheier, 1981). In a series of studies, Pyszczynski and and the characteristics of essentially normal individuals who,
Greenberg (1985, 1986; Greenberg & Pyszczynski, 1986) ex- within the context of experimental social-cognitive research,
amined avoidance in depressed and nondepressed college stu- had been induced into a state of self-focused attention. Smith
dents following either success or failure feedback. For instance, and Greenberg (1981) suggested that these parallels may indi-
after manipulating success and failure on an anagram task, cate that self-focused attention mediates several depressive phe-
158 RICK. E. INGRAM

nomena, most notably affective intensity, dysfunctional causal their problem as more serious, view luck as less of a factor, and
attributions, enhanced accuracy of some kinds of self-reports, take more responsibility for the problem and its treatment than
and lowered self-esteem.2 Several studies have evaluated the re- would subjects in the non-self-focused condition. Although re-
lation among depression, self-focused attention and these fea- sults revealed that the comparison group attributed more re-
tures. sponsibility internally for problem responsibility and treat-
Affect. Gibbons et al. (1985) assessed the relation between ment, no effects involving the self-focus manipulation were
levels of affect and self-focused attention in individuals already found for either group. In Study 2, this procedure was essen-
experiencing salient emotion. The general strategy was to ex- tially repeated with the different groups of subjects described
perimentally heighten self-focused attention and then to assess earlier, and again no effects involving the self-focusing manipu-
possible changes in level of affect. In Experiment 1, an inpatient lation were found. Thus, these studies could find no evidence
psychiatric group composed primarily of affective disorder pa- of a link between attributions and self-focused attention in any
tients and a recovering alcoholic inpatient comparison group psychiatric sample.
were placed in either an elevated self-focused condition (expo- This lack of attributional results was echoed in a study re-
sure to mirror) or a non-self-focused condition. Affect was then ported by Smith et al. (1985). Examining a group of college
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

examined by using a shortened state version of the State-Trait students, Smith et al. assessed relations among depression,
Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, chronic self-focusing, and attributional style as respectively
1970), where items were divided into those assessing positive measured by the BDI, the Private Self-Consciousness subscale
and negative affect. No self-focus manipulation effects were of the Self-Consciousness Scale, and the Attributional Style
found for either positive or negative affect in the comparison Questionnaire (ASQ; Peterson et al., 1982). Although the ex-
group. Psychiatric subjects, however, showed significant in- pected significant relation was found between depression and
creases in negative affect in the self-focus condition. negative internal attributions as measured by the ASQ, no rela-
In Experiment 2 of Gibbons et al. (1985), self-focused atten- tion was found between self-focusing and negative internal attri-
tion was again experimentally manipulated in a depressed sub- butions, r(l 11) = .04. Thus, although limited to a very small
group that was identified from within a general psychiatric in- number of studies, the data so far have offered little to suggest
patient group. These subjects were compared with general med- that increased self-focused attention mediates the attributional
ical patients who were nondepressed. This time the primary patterns observed in depression.
measures of affect were the positive and negative affect items of Self-report accuracy. Whereas studies have not found evi-
the Depression, Anxiety, and Hostility scales of the Multiple dence that self-focused attention mediates attributions in de-
Affect Adjective Checklist (MAACL; Zuckerman & Lubin, pression, some have provided evidence to suggest that self-fo-
1965). Results again indicated no significant effects of the self- cused attention may mediate the increased self-report accuracy
focusing manipulation for control subjects for either positive or that is sometimes seen in depression (see Gibbons, 1983). In
negative affect and no self-focusing effects for any kind of posi- Study 1 reported by Gibbons et al. (1985), patients' self-reports
tive affect within the group of depressed subjects. For negative of the number of their hospitalizations and of the duration of
affect, however, the self-focus manipulation exacerbated depres- their problems were compared with hospital records of these
sive, anxious, and hostile affect in the depressed inpatient variables. For number of hospitalizations, the ratio of reported
group. Thus, in both of these experiments, a self-focused atten- hospitalizations to actual hospitalizations was analyzed. A sig-
tion manipulation was reliably linked to increases in negative nificant main effect was found, suggesting that self-focus ma-
affect for individuals already experiencing negative affect, sug- nipulation subjects' reports in both the psychiatric and alco-
gesting that one variable that self-focusing may mediate in de- holic groups corresponded more to their actual number of hos-
pression is the level of negative affect experienced. Similar pitalizations. No significant differences were found regarding
affect-exacerbating results in depression have been found in a hospitalization duration self-reports. This pattern of accuracy
recent study by Gibbons (1987). findings was replicated in Study 2, where correlations between
Attributions. Although several researchers have assessed at- hospital records and self-reports of hospitalizations and prob-
tributions for task performance within studies of depression lem duration were significantly higher for self-focused subjects
and self-focused attention (e.g., Pyszczynski & Greenberg, than were the correlations for non-self-focused subjects. Gib-
1985), only a limited amount of research has directly assessed bons (1987) also found that a self-awareness induction im-
whether depressive attributions are mediated by self-focusing. proved the accuracy of self-reports in depressed college stu-
For example, although Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1985) ex- dents.
amined main effects involving attributions, they did not assess Further evidence of self-focused attention mediation of accu-
whether attributions were related to either self-focused atten- racy comes from a study by Musson and Alloy (1988). In at-
tion or depression. In the study previously described, Gibbons tempting to examine possible mediating variables for depres-
ct al. (1985) asked patients attribution-related questions about sive realism phenomena, Musson and Alloy (1988) had de-
(a) the seriousness of their problem, (b) to what extent luck (as pressed and nondepressed undergraduates participate in an
opposed to personal responsibility) played in their problem, (c)
their perception of their responsibility for the problem, and (d) 2
Other variables possibly mediated by self-focused attention within
their perception as to their responsibility for treatment of the depression have also been proposed (e.g., task performance and social
problem. It was expected that if self-focused attention mediates skills deficits; Ingram & Smith, 1984). Only variables receiving some
attributions, subjects in the self-focused condition would view empirical assessment are reported here.
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 159

illusion of control paradigm. Subjects completed tasks in a self- racy and affective fluctuations, there is little evidence to suggest
focus condition (mirror), a distraction condition (number that self-focused attention plays an important role in the attri-
tracking), or a neutral control condition. Results indicated in- butional and self-esteem deficits seen in depression. However,
creases in accuracy for nondepressed subjects in the self-focus relatively few studies have attempted to assess self-focusing me-
condition, whereas depressed subjects experienced decreases in diation within depression, and more definitive conclusions re-
accuracy in the distraction condition. Musson and Alloy (1988) garding which depressive features are mediated by self-focusing
also assessed the adequacy of their attention manipulations so and which are not must await future research.
that results could be credibly attributed to actual changes in
levels of self-focused attention. Thus, data from the Musson and Anxiety
Alloy (1988) and Gibbons et al. (1985) studies support the sug-
gestion that increased self-focusing may mediate those in- Although approached from a somewhat different perspective
stances in which depressed individuals are more accurate than than depression research, the hypothesis that anxiety disorders
nondepressed individuals. are characterized by excessive levels of self-focused attention
has a long history. In 1952, for example, Mandler and Sarason
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Self-esteem. Self-esteem is a variable that has been both the-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

oretically (e.g., Beck, 1967; Ingram, 1984) and empirically described test-anxious individuals in a performance situation
linked to depression (see Becker, 1979). Researchers have also as "self rather than task oriented" (p. 166). Numerous other
suggested that depressive self-esteem may be mediated by theoretical approaches have also suggested that increases in self-
heightened self-focused attention (Pyszczynski & Greenberg, focused attention accompany anxiety states (e.g., Cacioppo,
1987; Smith & Greenberg, 1981). However, despite several self- Glass, & Merluzzi, 1979; Deffenbacher, 1980; Easterbrook,
focused attention studies that have assessed depression and self- 1959; Houston, 1977; Liebert& Morris, 1967; Mandler & Wat-
esteem variables within the same experiment (e.g., Brockner, son, 1966; Sarason, 1972, 1975, 1986; Sutton-Simon & Gold-
Hjelle, & Plant, 1985; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1985), only fried, 1979; Wine, 1971, 1980, 1982). The present section
limited attempts have been made to examine the relations comments on those empirical studies that have attempted to
among all three variables.3 Specifically, Smith et al. (1985) as- directly examine if anxiety is characterized by increased self-
sessed self-esteem as defined by real-ideal self discrepancies. In focused attention.4
a series of correlations with a sample of college students, self-
esteem was found to be reliably associated with both depression Test Anxiety
and self-focused attention. When partial correlations were ex-
Several early studies examined whether test-anxious individ-
amined, statistically removing the influence of self-focused at-
uals experienced more self-focused attention when working on
tention from the association between depression and self-es-
a task. Mandler and Watson (1966) administered digit symbol
teem did not substantially alter this correlation. Likewise, re-
tasks to high and low test-anxious subjects and asked them,
moving the influence of depression did not alter the relation
"How often during the testing situation did you find yourself
between self-focusing and self-esteem. These results therefore
thinking about how well or bad you were doing?" (p. 276). Re-
provide little support for the suggestion that self-focused atten-
sults indicated that high anxiety subjects reported substantially
tion is an important source of variance in the relation between
more of these thoughts than did low anxiety subjects—findings
self-esteem and depression.
that were subsequently replicated by Neale and Katahn (1968).
To the extent that thinking about how one is doing (and not
Summary of Depression Studies about the task) can be considered self-focused, these results sug-
gest that anxiety states are associated with self-focused atten-
Current depression models using self-focused attention con-
tion. Marlett and Watson (1968) also found that ninth-grade
structs (e.g., Lewinsohn et al., 1985; Pyszczynski & Greenberg,
subjects with high test anxiety reported more self-focusing on a
1987) differ in a number of regards. A common element in each
of these theories, however, is that depression is characterized by
an increase in self-focused attention. Empirical research exam- 3
Brockner et al. (1985), for example, examined self-esteem as an in-
ining the relation between self-focused attention and depression dependent variable, and Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1985) assessed
suggests both strong and tentative conclusions. This research changes in self-esteem as a function of depression level and success-
strongly indicates a reliable relation between heightened self- failure but did not evaluate directly any relation between self-focusing
focused attention and depression. A variety of studies assessing processes and self-esteem.
4
self-focused attention and depression with diverse measures and It should be noted that many studies of anxiety have assessed a vari-
methodologies all converge on the finding of an increased, and ety of types of cognitions. An early study by Doris and Sarason (1955),
perhaps prolonged, level of self-focused attention in depression, for example, found that high test-anxious subjects blamed themselves
after a failure experience more than did low test-anxious subjects. Al-
regardless of whether the depression constitutes a clinical or
though results such as these have been discussed as evidence for self-
subclinical degree of disturbance (although other forms of de-
focusing (e.g., Wine, 1971), and may indeed be related to self-focusing,
pression such as bipolar disorder have not yet been examined). there is little evidence in these studies directly linking anxiety to self-
Evidence regarding the mediational properties of self-fo- focusing. In addition, other studies (e.g., Geen, 1976) have inferred the
cused attention in depression is less clear-cut. Findings thus far presence of self-focused attention in anxious individuals, but have pro-
suggest the tentative conclusion that although self-focused at- vided no measures of this process. Consequently, these studies are not
tention may partially mediate some kinds of self-report accu- addressed in this review.
160 RICK E. INGRAM

questionnaire following a task than did low test-anxious sub- about how their performance was related to their abilities in
jects. Similarly, Ganzer (1968) found that test-anxious subjects general, and specifically suggested that, in the mirror condition,
made more self-evaluative and apologetic task-irrelevant com- test-anxious subjects reported more task-irrelevant intrusive
ments while working on a serial verbal learning task than did thoughts and fewer task-relevant thoughts than did low test-anx-
low anxious subjects. ious subjects. Moreover, low test-anxious subjects in the mirror
Self-focused attention in test anxiety was examined experi- condition reported spending a greater percentage of time think-
mentally in a study by Deffenbacher (1978). Subjects worked on ing about the task than did test-anxious subjects.
difficult anagram problems in either a high stress (evaluative) or To the extent that reports of cognitive intrusions, self-abili-
a low stress (nonevaluative) context. Following completion of ties, and percentage of task focus reflect levels of increased self-
these tasks, self-focusing relevant measures were obtained of (a) focused attention, these results raise the prospect that the ma-
estimated time spent thinking about the task (as opposed to nipulation increased self-focusing in test-anxious subjects and
task-irrelevant thinking), (b) anxiety-generated cognitive inter- decreased self-focusing in low test-anxious subjects and that,
ference, (c) attention directed toward physiological arousal, (d) in turn, these differences in self-focused attention influenced
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

worry (task-irrelevant distractions), and (e) task-generated dis- subjects' task performance (i.e., lessened self-focus in low test-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tractions (e.g., focusing on irrelevant task aspects such as think- anxious subjects may have facilitated attention toward the task).
ing back to unsolved anagrams). Results suggested a pattern of Although non-mirror-condition means were not reported,
self-focusing showing that high anxious-high stress individuals Carver et al. (1983) did report such a possibility when they
spent less time generating task-relevant cognitions, reported noted that low test-anxious subjects in the mirror condition re-
more anxiety and task-generated cognitive interference, experi- ported spending more time thinking about the task (and pre-
enced more task-irrelevant cognitive distractions, and correctly sumably less time thinking about themselves) than did low test-
solved fewer anagrams. This same self-focusing pattern, how- anxious subjects in the baseline condition. Indeed, the experi-
ever, did not obtain for high anxious-low stress subjects, sug- mental instructions may have facilitated decreased self-focusing
gesting that increased self-focusing may emerge as a function of for low test-anxious subjects because they were explicitly in-
a Trait Anxiety X Situation interaction. structed not to pay attention to the mirror. In any event, it is
The Anxiety X Situation interaction may account for the lack clear that test-anxious subjects in the mirror condition reported
of significant findings in several studies examining the correla- cognitive responses that are consistent with definitions of self-
tion between measures of test anxiety and dispositional self-fo- focused attention, whereas low test-anxious subjects did not.
cusing (i.e., the Private Self-Consciousness subscale of the Self- On the basis of Carver and Scheier's model, Carver et al. (1983)
Consciousness Scale). For example, Carver and Glass (1976), might have suggested that the mirror increased self-focused at-
Smith et al. (1985), and Turner, Scheier, Carver, and Ickes tention for low test-anxious subjects, which subsequently initi-
(1978) could not find evidence of an association between self- ated an increased task focus. There were no data reported, how-
focused attention and test anxiety. Because all of these studies ever, to support this proposal. The possibility that these kinds
used unselected samples of college student subjects, there is no of manipulations can have very different attentional focusing
reason to believe that both subjects' anxiety and heightened effects on different kinds of people (i.e., Person x Situation in-
self-focused attention had been activated by any emotion-pro- teractions) is discussed in a later section.
voking situation. As Smith et al. (1985) noted, although self-
reports of the tendency to experience anxiety during tests may Social and Generalized Anxiety
be uncorrelated with self-focusing, the actual state of anxiety
does indeed appear to be accompanied by increased self-fo- Although the majority of studies examining self-focused at-
cused attention. tention in anxiety have addressed test anxiety, several self-fo-
Two other studies have examined self-focused attention in cused attention studies have investigated other types of anxiety.
test anxiety. Slapion and Carver (1981) sought to investigate the Hope and Heimberg (1985), for instance, found significant re-
effect of self-focusing on test performance by exposing male and lations among the Social Anxiety, Public Self-Consciousness,
female test-anxious and non-test-anxious subjects to a self-fo- and Private Self-Consciousness subscales of the Self-Conscious-
cusing manipulation (mirror presence) on one of two testing ness Scale in a diagnosed sample of social phobics, indicating
trials. The manipulation was found to enhance actual perfor- that increased self-focused attention characterizes individuals
mance for both male and female test-anxious subjects relative experiencing clinically significant anxiety. In the same study,
to low anxious subjects on the first trial, and it marginally en- Hope and Heimberg (1985) also found a positive correlation
hanced performance for test-anxious male subjects on the sec- between private self-consciousness and generalized anxiety as
ond trial. Although group means were not reported, test-anx- assessed by the STAI, although interestingly, no significant rela-
ious subjects apparently reported more self-focusing than did tion was found between private self-consciousness scores and a
low test-anxious subjects as indicated by statistically significant measure of social anxiety. Support for heightened self-focused
differences on posttest questions regarding subjects' degree of attention in social anxiety was also found in a study by Hope,
self-focused attention. Heimberg, Zollo, Nyman, and O'Brien (1987). In this study,
In a similar study by Carver, Peterson, Follansbee, and college student subjects were selected for high and low social
Scheier (1983), the difficulty of an anagram task and mirror anxiety; they then kept a diary of their thoughts during social
presence were varied for high and low female test-anxious sub- interactions. Content analyses of these diaries, examining spe-
jects. Results suggested that test-anxious subjects thought more cifically for self-focused versus non-self-focused thoughts, sug-
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 161

gested that socially anxious subjects were significantly more Alcohol Abuse
self-focused during their social interactions than were nonso-
cially anxious subjects. Hull (1981) has offered a provocative hypothesis concerning
the relation between self-focused attention and alcohol use.
Specifically, he proposed that alcohol interferes with the cogni-
Summary of Anxiety Studies tive processes essential to achieving a state of self-focused atten-
tion. Inasmuch as self-focused attention is thought to be neces-
Despite a long theoretical history, substantial intuitive ap- sary for critical self-evaluation and negative affect, inhibiting
peal, and the logical inferences of many studies, relatively few self-focusing by consuming alcohol should therefore provide a
studies have directly examined self-focused attention in anxiety. sense of relief or "escape" from the psychological effects of neg-
Those studies that have made such an attempt, however, gener- ative personal outcomes.
ally offer support for such a relation. Although correlations Hull has garnered support for this proposal in a series of stud-
among self-report measures in unselected samples have been ies. For instance, Hull, Levenson, >bung, and Sher (1983) found
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

minimal, during task performance test-anxious subjects have evidence that alcohol consumption does indeed appear to de-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

consistently been found to report higher levels of self-focused crease self-focusing. In the first experiment reported, subjects
attention. Further in line with Person X Situation conceptual- consumed either alcohol or tonic and then gave speeches about
izations, evidence has suggested that test-anxious individuals themselves. These speeches were then rated for frequency of
may be more responsive to self-focusing stimuli and are thus self-statements. As predicted, subjects who consumed alcohol
more likely to attend internally in certain situations than are made fewer self-focused statements than did subjects who had
non-test-anxious people. consumed only tonic. These findings were replicated in a second
The evidence regarding social and generalized anxiety is also experiment where alcohol consumption expectancy was manip-
generally supportive of a relation between increased self-fo- ulated by providing subjects with either alcohol or tonic and
cused attention and anxious affect. In individuals with clinical telling them either that they were receiving alcohol or that they
levels of social anxiety, self-focusing does appear to be height- were receiving tonic. Results indicated that actual alcohol con-
ened and related to generalized level of anxiety. The few data sumption, but not the expectation of alcohol consumption, re-
on generalized anxiety also support a relation between this kind duced self-focused attention. Finally, Hull et al. (1983) also ad-
of affective distress and heightened self-focused attention. Over- ministered the self-consciousness scale and then gave either al-
all then, these initial data are largely supportive, although more cohol or tonic to subjects. Following this procedure, subjects
studies examining social or generalized anxiety are clearly participated in a depth-of-processing incidental-recall task
needed. (Craik & Tulving, 1975). To analyze the data, a median split
On the basis of the available evidence, few researchers would was performed on private self-consciousness scales to separate
argue that anxiety states do not encompass a higher than typical subjects relatively high and low in dispositional self-focusing.
level of self-focused attention. Because the first two sections of Findings suggested that high private self-conscious subjects re-
this article addressed anxiety and depression, at least one criti- called more self-relevant words in the tonic condition than did
cal issue must be noted. Evolving from divergent theoretical an- the low self-conscious subjects. Alcohol consumption, however,
tecedents, the bodies of literature in depression and anxiety decreased self-relevant recall for private self-conscious subjects
have developed largely in isolation from each other, with the relative to the tonic condition, high private self-conscious sub-
apparent assumption that increased self-focused attention is a jects. Together, then, these experiments support the hypothesis
relatively unique factor in each disorder. The problem is more that alcohol interferes with cognitive processing of self-relevant
than just theoretical, however. Depression and anxiety are so information, and thus, presumably, self-focused attention.
highly correlated (Gotlib, 1984) that any study purporting to In another study, Hull and \bung (1983) tested the notion
select anxious subjects is also selecting depressed subjects and that individuals consume alcohol to decrease self-focusing. Sub-
vice versa (Ingram, 1989; Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hammen, & jects high or low in dispositional self-focusing received either a
Ingram, 1987). Results may thus be due to depressive affect, success or a failure experience and were then given an opportu-
anxious affect, or some unique combination of the two. Hence, nity to participate in a "wine tasting experiment." Alcohol con-
virtually every study discussed thus far that pertains to either sumption for low self-focused subjects was unaffected by suc-
anxiety or depression is undoubtedly confounded with the pres- cess or failure. On the other hand, failure increased the amount
ence of "other" affect. Ingram (in press) separated a sample of of wine consumed by high self-focused subjects. Thus, consis-
subjects into a depressed and not generally anxious group and tent with Hull's (1981) model, individuals who were highly self-
a generally anxious but not depressed group. Results on two focused were more likely to drink after encountering a negative
different measures of self-focused attention (the Private Self- personal outcome, presumably to lessen their self-focusing and
Consciousness subscale and the Test of Attentional and Inter- hence their negative affect.
personal Style; Nideffer, 1976) indicated no differences between In a subsequent study of alcohol treatment relapse, Hull,
these groups, although both evidenced significantly greater self- Young, and Jouriles (1986) assessed private self-consciousness
focused attention than a normal control group. Therefore, the and negative life events immediately before discharge from a
little "specificity" data available indicate that self-focused at- detoxification program. High self-conscious subjects who re-
tention is separately related to both depression and anxiety ported many negative events were found to be particularly
states. likely to relapse to previous drinking levels within the first three
162 RICK E. INGRAM

months (i.e., 70% relapse), whereas high self-conscious subjects periments, subjects who were characterized by predominantly
who had predominately positive life events were significantly negative affect (they had psychiatric diagnoses) reported in-
less likely to relapse (14%). For low self-conscious subjects, creased anxiety and decreased positive affect (Experiment 1),
quality of life events was unrelated to relapse. and increased depression, anxiety, and hostility (Experiment 2)
Hull's data consistently support the hypothesized relation be- when their self-focused attention was experimentally height-
tween increased self-focused attention and alcohol consump- ened.
tion. Not only does alcohol consumption appear to inhibit self- Several studies have examined the intensification of fear as a
focusing, but evidence also indicates that individuals high in result of self-focused attention. Carver, Blaney, and Scheier
self-focusing will drink more to lower negative self-evaluation (1979) selected subjects who were moderately fearful of snakes
states, presumably because of alcohol's effect on self-focusing. and then separated them according to whether they expressed
Furthermore, an individual's level of chronic self-focus appears relative doubt or confidence regarding their ability to approach
to interact with the quality of life events experienced to success- a snake. They found that subjects whose self-focused attention
fully predict alcoholism relapse. was experimentally manipulated reported more arousal and a
greater sense of inadequacy than did non-self-focused subjects.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Furthermore, subjects who were doubtful withdrew earlier in


Vulnerability
approaching the snake when they were in the self-focused con-
The evidence reviewed to this point clearly indicates that self- dition.
focused attention is a component of several dysfunctional These findings were echoed to some degree in two experi-
states. There is also evidence to suggest that chronically high ments by Scheier, Carver, and Gibbons (1981). In the first exper-
levels of self-focused attention may serve as a vulnerability fac- iment, a mirror manipulation was found to decrease the ap-
tor by placing individuals at risk for the onset or prolonged proach behavior of subjects who were preselected on the basis
maintenance of dysfunction. Two vulnerability dimensions can of high fear of snakes. Subjects with low fear of snakes were
be identified. The first can be considered an intensification fac- unaffected by the self-focusing manipulation. In the second ex-
tor, suggesting that levels of salient internal dimensions should periment, subjects high and low in private self-consciousness
be magnified by self-focusing. With regard to psychopathology, were given an opportunity to participate in an electric shock
self-focused attention intensification processes would serve to experiment for "humanitarian" reasons in which they would
exacerbate or prolong the negative state. receive either mild (low fear condition) or strong (high fear con-
The second factor is the initiation of psychologically dysfunc- dition) shocks. No shock effects were found for low private self-
tional states. Instead of maintaining existing negative function- conscious subjects. High private self-conscious subjects, on the
ing, initiation refers to the creation of this functioning. Initia- other hand, were significantly less willing to participate in the
tion is undoubtedly related to intensification, but may be com- high fear than in the low fear condition. Additionally, for high
posed of functionally quite different processes. Self-focused private self-conscious subjects, the high fear manipulation in-
attention, however, may contribute to both the intensification tensified reports of negative emotional arousal relative to those
and initiation of psychopathology. Thus, with regard to initia- of low fear subjects.
tion, self-focused attention would serve to contribute to the
constellation of variables that bring about disorder. In particu- Initiation Effects
lar, chronically high levels of self-focused attention across a va-
riety of situational contexts may constitute a cognitive diathesis As noted, the initiation hypothesis represents a diathesis-
in a diathesis-stress relation with psychopathology. Moreover, stress approach to disorder onset that suggests that stressful life
self-focused attention may serve a substantially different func- events interact with a chronic self-focusing diathesis factor to
tion depending on whether it is examined from an intensifica- produce the onset of dysfunction. Studies reviewed in the previ-
tion perspective or from an initiation perspective. ous section examined whether self-focused attention intensified
affect that was either already present or had been directly in-
Intensification Effects duced; that is, the affect was already present or subjects had
been specifically instructed to try to create a particular affective
A variety of studies have suggested that self-focusing intensi- state. Such studies, however, do not allow for an evaluation of
fies negative affect. Scheier and Carver (1977), for example, the individual's naturally occurring processes in the onset of
found that experimentally self-focused male subjects rated affect. Some individuals, for example, may have certain vulner-
slides of nude women as more attractive than did non-self-fo- abilities, whereas others may evidence protective factors. Fewer
cused subjects, a finding that they interpreted as support for the studies have examined the contribution of increased self-fo-
hypothesis that self-focused attention can "increase awareness cused attention to the onset of negative affect when an emotion
of and reactions to positive affect" (p. 629). Scheier and Carver has not been created for subjects through a direct mood induc-
(1977) also found that a different group of subjects high in ex- tion, but instead when subjects have instigated (or reduced)
perimentally induced self-focused attention reported greater their own affective responses to stressful events.
levels of negative affect after a Velten mood-induction proce- Hull and his colleagues (Hull & Young, 1983; Hull et al.,
dure than did non-self-focused subjects. 1986) provided some initial evidence that individuals who are
The experiments by Gibbons et al. (1985) reported earlier high in dispositional self-focused attention are more prone than
also support the intensification hypothesis. In both of these ex- normally self-focused individuals to experience negative affect
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 163

in response to negative events. As part of an experiment de- second study that showed that women become more self-fo-
scribed earlier, Hull and Young (1983) provided subjects with cused in response to video focusing manipulations than do
either a success or a failure experience; after this experience, men. In the final study reported, Ingram et al. (1988) tested
they completed the MAACL and then participated in an ostensi- whether differences in reactivity to focusing stimuli as a func-
ble wine-tasting experiment. Of particular interest in the pres- tion of sex role were related to exacerbated negative affective
ent context, MAACL scores indicated that, after experiencing responses. After an initial depression screening to ensure no
failure, high private self-conscious subjects reported greater to- preexperimental group differences in existing depressive affect,
tal negative affect than did low private self-conscious subjects. male and female subjects were classified into masculine, femi-
This effect was particularly pronounced for high self-conscious nine, and androgynous sex role groups. Half of the subjects
subjects who also had low self-esteem. were assigned to a mirror condition, and all subjects received a
In another study, Hull, Levenson, and \bung (1986) exam- failure induction on an "interpersonal task" as a negative affect
ined both verbal reports and physiological indices of anxiety in prompt. Following this induction, subjects completed the
high and low private self-conscious subjects in response to a MAACL and the BDI. The BDI was scored according to clusters
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stressful speech manipulation. As expected on the basis of a of item subsets that had been determined from a previous factor
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

self-focused attention initiation hypothesis, high private self- analysis. Results indicated that regardless of actual sex, the mir-
conscious subjects showed heightened affective and physiologi- ror manipulation was associated with the greatest amount of
cal reactions to the speech, suggesting that an increased ten- negative affect for feminine subjects. Androgynous subjects also
dency toward self-focusing places individuals at risk for in- experienced some heightened negative affect in the mirror con-
creased negative affective responses to negative events. dition, and, interestingly, masculine subjects showed somewhat
In the first of several experiments reported by Ingram (1988), of a decrease in negative affect in the self-focusing condition.
high and low private self-conscious subjects were presented with Feminine subjects also had the highest scores for self-retribu-
either success or failure feedback, and then both affective and tion and social irritation BDI item subsets. Thus, these results
cognitive reactions were assessed. This study, however, also con- supported the hypothesis that sex-linked differences in the read-
trolled for the positive association between private self-con- iness to self-focus can serve to help initiate negative affect as a
sciousness and negative affect by screening subjects to ensure response to stressful events.
that only nondepressed subjects were selected. Findings indi- Most generally, results of this sort support diathesis-stress in-
cated that low private self-conscious subjects were largely un- terpretations of self-focus initiation effects. That is, it seems rea-
affected by the success-failure manipulation, whereas high pri- sonable to suggest that diathesis variables (sex linked in this
vate serf-conscious subjects showed significantly more negative case) interact with certain situations to produce varying levels
affect and thinking in the failure condition than in the success of situational self-focused attention that can ultimately result in
condition. The results of this study thus support the initiation exacerbated negative affective responses to stress. Other studies
hypothesis; subjects who were more likely to be self-focused have also provided data to suggest that certain individuals re-
across situations were also more likely to think and feel nega- spond differently to environmental tasks typically thought to
tively after experiencing a negative personal outcome. uniformly produce heightened self-focused attention. Carver et
In a second experiment by Ingram (1988), four groups of sub- al. (1983), for instance, reported data indicating that a mirror
jects were monitored weekly over a 2'/2-month period. The manipulation increased self-focused attention for high test-anx-
group of primary interest was a chronically self-focused but ious individuals, and there was some suggestion that self-focus-
nondepressed (vulnerable) group. These subjects were com- ing for low test-anxious subjects was decreased by the mirror.
pared with groups of self-focused and depressive affect subjects, Thus, there may be any number of variables associated with
depressive affect and non-self-focused subjects, and nonde- particular subject groups that are more likely predisposed to
pressed and non-self-focused control subjects. Over the course increased self-focusing responsivity and, ultimately, to the dys-
of this period, vulnerable subjects reported experiencing sig- function to which this internal attention might contribute.
nificantly more negative affect than did the normal control Both theoretically and empirically it is important for future
group, even though they started the experiment with similar research to examine the mechanisms that play a role in increas-
levels of negative affect. These data thus support self-focused ing or decreasing self-focused attention. One perspective on the
attention as a negative affect vulnerability factor in a naturalis- mediation of self-focusing concerns control processes. These
tic situation. Interestingly, the chronic self-focused group with processes refer to the potentially volitional aspects of cognitive
preexisting depressive affect showed the highest degree of nega- processes such as self-focused attention (see Shiffrin & Schnei-
tive feelings of any group over the monitoring period, suggesting der, 1977) and, as such, represent how individuals voluntarily
that the added component of self-focused attention with al- act to process information. Some individuals, for example, may
ready depressed subjects served to intensify their negative feel- simply be more drawn to particular self-focusing stimuli be-
ings and symptoms. cause they are perhaps predisposed to self-focusing. Conversely,
The initiation hypothesis may also help to account for sex others may simply attempt to ignore such stimuli as they find
differences in affective disorders. In a series of studies, Ingram. them an impediment to the tasks at hand. Still others may be
Cruet, Johnson, and Wisnicki (1988) found evidence of differ- very aware of the stimulus, but may actively "work" to not be-
ential sex-linked self-focusing patterns. In the first study, a mir- come self-focused, especially if the stimulus has initially caused
ror prompt was found to increase self-focused attention for fe- them some discomfort. During times when self-focusing may
male but not male subjects. These findings were replicated in a be aversive, these efforts may represent cognitive "coping" at-
164 RICK E. INGRAM

tempts that lead the individual either to not increase self-fo- (1981) reported several studies examining depressives, binge
cused attention or to decrease it. Thus, rather than adjust be- eaters, alcoholics, and "compulsives" and "impulsives" that
havior or cognitive standards in response to self-focused atten- found evidence of heightened self-focused attention relative to
tion (Carver & Scheier, 1981, 1983;Duval&Wicklund, 1972), normals. This section reviews available data and theoretical
it may be that some individuals instead adjust their attention perspectives on several diverse psychopathological states. Al-
from an internal to an external focus. though these data are limited in terms of quantity, and much is
Apart from the theoretical and empirical significance of di- necessarily of indirect relevance, it is nevertheless quite sugges-
athesis-stress perspectives on self-focusing initiation effects, tive of relations between self-focused attention and divergent
these data also point to significant methodological considera- psychological disorders.
tions. As is evident from the preceding discussion, the assump-
tion that self-focusing stimuli automatically produce self-focus- Schizophrenia
ing is untenable; for some kinds of individuals self-focused at-
tention is increased, whereas for other kinds this attention is In an early study examining the psychometric properties of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

decreased. At the very least, explicit manipulation checks must the Self-Focus Sentence Completion Scale, Exner (1973) found
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

be reported in studies designed to induce self-focusing. Going that both process and reactive schizophrenic patients produced
beyond this simple confirmation of intended states, however, more responses defined as self-focused than did nonpsychiatric
there is a need for future research to address more precisely the control individuals. As this same instrument has been used to
specific processes that are affected by self-focusing stimuli, how establish the relation between self-focused attention and other
these processes are affected, and what their determinants in disorders, this constitutes some empirical evidence that schizo-
different groups of people are (i.e., Person x Situation interac- phrenia may also be characterized by excessive self-focused at-
tion determinants). tention.
Unfortunately, beyond Exner's (1973) original research, few
other empirical studies have addressed this relation. Many of
Summary of Vulnerability Studies
the classic descriptions of cognition in schizophrenia, however,
Studies of both the initiation and intensification of affect square well with a self-focus hypothesis. In reviewing the cogni-
clearly support both of these self-focusing functions; an in- tive research in schizophrenia, for example, Magaro (1980)
crease in self-focused attention appears to reliably exacerbate noted that for a variety of schizophrenia theorists, "an aspect
existing affect. On the other hand, the data suggest that height- of the schizophrenia problem is seen as residing in some atten-
ened self-focused attention, typically operationalized as chronic tional mechanism" (p. 167). Similarly, Nuechterlein and Daw-
self-focused attention, interacts with other situational and per- son (1984) have argued that schizophrenic deficits are due to
son variables to precipitate the onset of negative affect. Together maladaptive allocation of attention. When such attentional
these studies support self-focused attention as a cognitive diath- constructs are combined with schizophrenics' inability to at-
esis variable; individuals who are self-focused are more vulner- tend to "appropriate" stimuli as well as their classically recog-
able to the negative effects of negative situations, and those who nized distortion in the processing of reality (see Chapman &
are already experiencing negative affect are vulnerable to an Chapman, 1973), these observations suggest the intuitively ap-
exacerbation of the affect. pealing possibility that schizophrenics are instead attending to
Although the data are quite consistent in suggesting that self- internally generated self-stimuli at the expense of relevant exter-
focused attention can act as both an intensification and an initi- nal stimuli. In light of these observations, therefore, it is con-
ation variable, it is much less clear as to the nature of the mecha- ceivable that an overreliance on self-focused attention may be
nisms that underlie these functions. In the case of initiation, for an important concept in accounting for certain cognitive as-
example, it may be that stressful events create negative affect, pects of schizophrenia.
which is in turn intensified by the individuals' chronic self-fo-
cused attention (Carver & Scheier, 1981). Alternately, it may be Psychopathy
that self-focused attention serves to help trigger negative cogni-
tive schemas in response to negative events that then precipitate In the same study that suggested that schizophrenics were
dysfunctional behavior, including dysfunctional affect (Ingram characterized by increased self-focused attention, Exner (1973)
et al., 1987). Given the consistency thus far in the data describ- found that a group of individuals diagnosed as psychopaths,
ing chronic self-focused attention as a vulnerability factor, re- who had a history of assaultiveness and had been institutional-
search efforts that examine the path by which self-focused at- ized by court order, also evidenced increased self-focused atten-
tention helps to initiate negative affect and thus place individu- tion relative to normal controls. Thus, the limited data available
als at heightened risk are important. also suggest an increased level of self-focused attention in psy-
chopathy. Moreover, the long-term history of egocentric func-
Other Psychopathological Processes tioning of these individuals (DSMII1-R; American Psychiatric
Association [APA], 1987) further suggests that a chronic self-
Although self-focused attention in some areas of psycho- focus is not unlikely. These data are particularly noteworthy in
pathological functioning has been examined extensively, there that psychopathic functioning tends to be characterized by a
are numerous other psychopathological processes that have re- lack of depressive and anxious affect, two states that are sepa-
ceived little direct empirical attention. For example, Weintraub rately related to self-focused attention. Although some have
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 165

suggested that the cognitive style of psychopathic individuals is Overall Summary: The Incidence of Self-Focused
externally oriented and that introspection is avoided (see Attention in Clinical Disorders
Checkley, 1976; Hare, 1970), this appears to be more in terms
of external blame for negative events and in perceiving the envi- In evaluating the evidence for a particular variable in psycho-
ronment as hostile and dangerous. In fact, these inaccurate per- pathology, it is important to distinguish among supportive evi-
ceptions of external reality may be a reflection of an excessive dence, equivocal (neither supportive nor nonsupportive) evi-
attention to the self at the expense of attention to external data. dence, and nonsupportive (contradictory) evidence. The studies
In considering these other psychopathological processes, it examined in the present review are remarkably consistent in
may be that the quality of the self-focused attention differs from providing supportive evidence. Additionally, the few studies not
state to state. According to such a viewpoint, for example, the evidencing positive outcomes were much more likely to pro-
content of self-focused attention in schizophrenia may vary duce inconclusive rather than contradictory evidence. Thus, the
from the content of the attention in anxiety. Theoretical con- weight of the available data clearly suggests an association be-
tent-process distinctions are discussed at a later point in this tween disorder (or vulnerability to disorder) and self-focused
attention regardless of the particular disorder. Indeed, it appears
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

article.
difficult to find a psychological disorder that is not character-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ized by a heightened degree of self-focused attention. This con-


Undifferentiated Psychopathological States clusion seems to hold true for both relatively extensively re-
searched disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety, alcohol abuse) as
Several early studies have suggested that varying forms of well as for less extensively researched disorders (schizophrenia,
psychopathology are characterized by heightened self-focused psychopathy, Undifferentiated disorders).5 Such consistency is
attention. Unfortunately, these studies have generally been un- particularly striking when one considers that internal attention
clear as to the precise type of psychopathology under consider- research in these different disorders has derived from disparate
ation. For instance, Lorenz and Cobb (1953) reported evidence, theoretical backgrounds and has used diverse methodologies.
as assessed by greater use of personal pronouns in speech sam-
ples, of increased self-focused attention by "psychoneurotic"
Causal Status of Self-Focused Attention
patients. They also reported increases in self-focused attention
in Psychopathology
by manic patients. Gottschalk, Gleser, and Hambidge (1957)
found similar empirical evidence of exacerbated self-focused A fundamental issue in this literature is whether self-focused
attention in hospitalized psychotic patients. In their discussion attention can be said to be a causal variable in any of the disor-
of negative affectivity, Watson and Clark (1984) have also noted ders with which it is associated. Causal mediation pathways can
that persons high in such affectivity are more likely than those take different forms (see Hollon, DeRubeis, & Evans, 1987),
low in negative affectivity to focus on themselves. Thus, the neg- and several pathways are possible. The first is that increased
ative affectivity construct, which is general rather than specific, self-focused attention serves as one of a constellation of vari-
along with these early empirical studies, offers further evidence ables that bring about the onset of a disorder (causal status). A
of the prevalence of heightened self-focused attention across di- second possibility is that increased self-focused attention serves
verse disorders and emotional states. to govern the disorder once initiated (mediational status). This
perspective is advocated by depression models that view exces-
sive self-focused attention as a consequence of some disruption
Therapeutic Change Processes in the individual's life. Once this attention is activated by the
disruption, it then plays a significant role in mediating a variety
A natural extension of findings that self-focused attention is
of depressive features. There is also no reason for the first two
related to psychological disorders is that attentional processes
possibilities to be mutually exclusive. Thus, an additional possi-
may also be important in considerations of alleviation of disor-
bility is that self-focused attention plays a dual role for which it
der. Natale, Dahlberg, and Jaffe (1978), for instance, have noted
is implicated in both the onset and governance of the disorder
an inverse relation between the use of self-references and im-
process. A final possibility is that self-focused attention is a
provement in psychotherapy. Although few treatment methods
mere consequence of disorder processes and thus exerts no sig-
have been developed with the specific intent of targeting self-
nificant influence on the disorder per se.
focused attention, other researchers have suggested that among
These causal possibilities may vary as a function of the disor-
the effective change mechanisms of therapy are those methods
der under consideration; for any specified psychopathological
that induce a decrease in self-focused attention (Gibbons et al.,
state, self-focused attention may be causal, mediational, causal
1985; Ingram & Hollon, 1986; Schmitt, 1983). This proposal is
based on the assumption that if self-focused attention is related
to clinical disorders, and if these disorders improve as a function 5
A lack of extensive research should not be confused with weak or
of intervention, then some intervention methods may work by
contradictory data. Although a disorder such as schizophrenia may not
decreasing self-focusing. This is highly speculative, of course, as have received sufficient attention with regard to a variable such as self-
it is not possible to identify basic psychopathological mecha- focusing, it cannot be concluded that the relation is therefore weak. Al-
nisms from therapy outcome alone. Yet, given the empirical evi- though research clearly needs to assess self-focused attention in these
dence on self-focused attention and clinical disorders, such a less-examined disorders directly, the extant data do point to an associa-
hypothesis does seem plausible. tion.
166 RICK E. INGRAM

and mediational, or consequential. Despite the relatively large developed to account for the specificity and nonspecificity of
number of studies that have examined self-focused attention, different levels of variables across different disorders.
few studies have sought to directly test the causal nature of self-
focused attention and how it may vary in different disorders. Conceptual Issues
Although the vulnerability data reviewed earlier do offer some
support for causal pathways in emotional distress, these data Attentional Constructs
are quite preliminary and are limited in scope to distress in-
Attention is a construct central to a variety of models of cog-
volving relatively mild depressive and anxious states. Sufficient
nition (see Parasuraman & Davies, 1984). Yet as central as this
data are therefore not currently available that allow for clear-
concept is, there is little consensus on what constitutes the na-
cut determinations of the causal status of self-focused attention
ture and general parameters of attention, and indeed, few basic
in differing psychopathological states. An explicit recognition
definitions have been offered (Carver & Scheier, 1981). This
of this deficit suggests that an important research agenda for
lack of conceptual clarity has engendered a variety of controver-
investigators will be to specifically test causal hypotheses in var-
sies regarding various aspects of attentional phenomena. For
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ious disorders.
instance, issues concerning late versus early selection (Kahne-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

man & Treisman, 1984), structural versus capacity theories


Theoretical Issues Arising From the Consistency of (Wickens, 1984), distinctions between automatic and con-
Descriptive Data Across Disorders trolled processing (Schneider, Dumais, & Shiffrin, 1984;
Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977), and differing definitions as to what
The association between self-focused attention and such a
constitutes automatic processing (e.g., Norman & Shallice,
wide variety of fundamentally different disorders presents a
1980) are a few of the major issues in contemporary attention
conceptual dilemma. For example, the actual theoretical and
theory and research. Recognizing the limited agreement con-
empirical utility of self-focused attention as a variable in psy-
cerning the basic processes of attentional phenomena, this sec-
chopathology is limited by its wealth of associations to different
tion outlines some broad assumptions concerning the nature of
disorders. In terms of understanding the essential process of a
attention as a basis for discussing attention as it relates to self-
given psychopathology, it could easily be suggested that self-fo-
focusing and psychopathology. In the absence of clear-cut data
cused attention provides little meaningful information concern-
supporting any particular model or conceptualization of atten-
ing the critical processes that converge to determine the disor-
tion, these assumptions are both theoretically reasonable and
der. That is. if there are genuine differences in the fundamental
consistent with current attention conceptualizations.
aspects of various disorders, yet all are characterized by height-
ened self-focused attention, then the value of self-focused atten-
tion as a variable is severely limited, at least without further Cognitive Processing Capacity
conceptual elaboration. The finite limits on attention or processing capacity have
Although some researchers have suggested that self-focused been conceptualized in several different ways. Structural ap-
attention may be a core process in general dysfunction (Carver proaches to limited processing capacity view attention limits as
& Scheier, 1984), associations with numerous disorders also a function of a single mechanism, sometimes called a limited
represent a substantial conceptual problem for theoretical capacity central processor (LCCP; Kerr, 1973) that can handle
models of specific disorders that incorporate self-focused atten- only a single attentional operation at a time. When the LCCP is
tional constructs, particularly those models that view self-fo- engaged in a given task it is not available for attention to other
cused attention as central to the disorder. Such models, for ex- tasks until that operation is complete. Alternatively, capacity
ample, do not typically examine specificity issues regarding the theories of limited processing are less concerned with the struc-
construct but instead appear to assume specificity. The relation tural aspects of attention. Instead, these approaches focus on
of increased self-focused attention to varying disorders there- the flexibility of attention and characterize capacity as a limited
fore casts some doubt on the ability of such models to invoke pool of resources that can be allocated to various tasks (e.g.,
self-focused attention processes to meaningfully describe a Kahneman, 1973; Wickens, 1984). Available capacity can thus
given disorder. be split among different tasks, each using some proportion of
Despite these limitations, it appears that self-focused atten- the capacity; when capacity is not at its limit, information can
tion can provide meaningful information about psychological be added to awareness until that limit is reached. The conceptu-
disorders when given an appropriate theoretical context. Hence, alization presented here assumes a finite but flexible capacity
modifications building on the basic construct of self-focused at- model that allocates attentional resources to a limited number
tention are necessary to propose a meaningful contribution of of tasks.
this process to psychopathology. The second half of this article
therefore develops a conceptualization of self-focused attention
Schemata
within a framework that is somewhat different than current self-
focused attention models, but that is consistent with existing The cognitive schema is generally regarded as the structure
attention constructs. Using this conceptualization, a descriptive underlying attention (see Neisser, 1976). Despite its wide usage,
model of psychopathological self-focused attention is proposed. there is no clear-cut and universally agreed-on definition of the
Following this, it is shown how the elements of this dysfunc- schema construct (see Hollon & Kriss, 1984; Kihlstrom &
tional self-focused attention can be classified within a model Nasby, 1981; Nasby & Kihlstrom, 1986; Taylor & Crocker,
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 167

1981). Although many "different" types of schemata have been self-focused or they are not. Given this assumption there is little
described, the construct can be divided into two general catego- need to address potential quantitative aspects of the state. Pre-
ries of function. Declarative schemata (Kihlstrom & Nasby, sumably, a shift into the state for a sufficient duration will allow
1981; Nasby & Kihlstrom, 1986) contain some kind of proposi- for the observation of the state's psychological consequences,
tional knowledge or content. Alternatively, procedural schemata and there is thus little need for assessing how much self-focused
are those structures that contain operating information about attention has been achieved. From a theoretical standpoint, this
sequences of behavioral routines for a variety of actions ranging viewpoint derives from a fixed capacity model of attention that
from molecular (e.g., muscle movements involved in riding a implicitly assumes structural capacity limitations (e.g., an
bike; see Norman, 1981) to molar levels of behavior (e.g., exe- LCCP) and that is capable of processing only one task at a time.
cuting the appropriate behaviors in a given social situation; see Thus, although attentional focus can shift back and forth, per-
Abelson, 1981; Schank & Abelson, 1977). As used in the pres- haps rapidly enough to blur the distinction, the implication of
ent context, schemata are viewed as memory structures that are a structural viewpoint leads to a functional view of attentional
the basic unit of the information-processing system and thus direction as an either-or phenomenon. In contrast, the current
control attention. Subsequent discussion refers largely to self- model assumes a fixed but flexible model of attentional capac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

schemata; that is, declarative schemata that contain, organize, ity. Although this capacity can be engaged to the extent that no
and process information specifically relevant to the self. other processing is possible, available capacity is typically seen
as being allocated among multiple tasks, some self-focused and
Activation some not.

Although the cognitive schema represents the structural Directional Process Assumptions
mechanism linked to attention, the processing mechanism by
which the schema functions is embodied in the activation con- Degree of internal or external attention parameters. Assump-
cept. Individuals possess a variety of schemata; the particular tions of flexible capacity, or attention allocation, allow for theo-
schemata that influence information processing during any retically viewing direction as constituting a continuum with
specified instance are those that are excited or activated. Atten- complete internal and external attention falling at the respective
tion, or conscious awareness of information, is seen as occur- endpoints of the continuum and some balance between the two
ring when given schema components are activated above some at the midpoint. Individuals can therefore attend to a combina-
threshold level (see Anderson, 1983) either by external stimula- tion of internal and external attention simultaneously. Even
tion (e.g., an externally induced orienting response) or by when attending externally, for example, people can still have
spreading activation (Bower, 1981). Patterns of awareness are an awareness of themselves. Likewise, when attention is turned
therefore determined by the activation or excitation among inward, the external environment, although perhaps not sa-
different patterns of nodes within and across cognitive sche- lient, nevertheless receives some attention; one can drive a car
mata (Johnston & Dark, 1982; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). and be self-focused at the same time. Thus, in a flexible capacity
This activation can occur either by intentional means or in an system the typical attentional state can be characterized as a
unintentional and automatic fashion. balance between external and internal attention.
This view is analogous in many respects to Norman and Bo-
brow's (1975) description of conceptual versus data-driven pro-
Generalized Self-Focused Attention Assumptions
cesses (or top-down versus bottom-up processing). At one end
Although differing in some respects, extant models of self- of the continuum, processing is conceptually or internally
focused attention have a number of common assumptions. The driven; at the other end, the focus of attention is on the environ-
current approach shares several of these assumptions, particu- ment or the "data" in the environment. Presumably, optimal
larly in regard to a reliance on information-processing concep- functioning consists of a balance between the two. Neisser's
tualizations, although in other respects it uses a different level of (1976) suggestion that perception is characterized as an interac-
analysis and a somewhat different set of information-processing tion between cognitive structures and sensory data is also con-
constructs. sistent with a continuum conceptualization.
When viewed as a continuum, several assumptions concern-
ing internal attention can be articulated. For example, the dis-
Capacity Assumptions
tribution of attention resources can be seen as approximating a
Previous models of self-focused attention have generally im- normal curve; although attention can shift to a relatively inter-
plied a dichotomy between internally and externally directed nal or external focus, some balance between the two seems
attention; at any given time attention can emanate from either likely for optimal functioning. Internally focused attention can
internal or external sources, but not from both at the same time therefore be defined as any time this balance shifts to a greater
(e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981; Duval & Wicklund, 1972). Al- proportion of internal attention relative to external attention.
though such a description may be largely for the sake of conve- This attentional distribution viewpoint also clearly suggests
nience, such terminology nevertheless has important empirical that different degrees of internal attention are possible. Figure
and theoretical implications. Empirically, this assumption leads 1 provides a conceptual illustration: A slight shift can be consid-
to methodologies designed to shift the individual "into" this ered mildly self-focused, and proportionally greater shifts can
state; subsequent conclusions suggest that either subjects are be described as respectively moderate to extreme. The ordinate
168 RICK E. INGRAM

Relative
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Proportion:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Internal Focus 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100


External Focus 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0

Descriptive Extreme Moderate Mild Mild Moderate Extreme


Terms
Externally Focused Internally Focused

Figure 1. Illustration of a distributional model of internal and external attention. (The ordinate represents
the frequency of occurrence, based on theoretical perspectives suggesting that a relatively even balance is
optimal for psychologically healthy functioning. The abscissa reflects the possible proportions of internal
and external attention.)

in Figure 1 depicts the frequency of occurrence of the relative to relatively more internal focus) may not be the same as exces-
attentional proportions for average individuals across situa- sive self-focusing (a shift to a severe degree). Moreover, it is not
tions. However, the distribution is slightly skewed to illustrate clear from many studies of self-focused attention whether
that the "average" frequencies for the normal population are merely a shift to a self-focused state (degree parameter) or a
unknown and do not necessarily have to be balanced 50/50, prolonged shift to such a state (duration parameter) is of inter-
although an equal or close-to-equal balance seems most adap- est. Thus, whereas self-focused attention may imply a virtually
tive. Most normal individuals probably hover around the mid- total state of self-focused attention (structural assumptions), it
point most of the time, but shift toward the endpoints as various may also imply a shift in a self-focused direction, or a more
situations dictate. sustained duration of this shift. The current model provides a
Duration parameters. In addition to the degree of the shift means for making theoretical distinctions among these assump-
in attentional direction, one can also describe attentional direc- tions and parameters.
tion in terms of the duration of the shift. Shifts in either an Flexibility parameters. Previous approaches have suggested
internal or an external direction can range from an extremely that shifting attentional directions can occur quite rapidly. Un-
brief amount of time to a much more sustained amount of time. doubtedly one reason for the ability to engage in such rapid
It should further be noted that duration and degree parameters shifts is to accommodate various situational demands. Effective
are conceptually if not empirically independent; although a functioning in some situations requires a shift into a relatively
shift in an internal direction may be accompanied by a sus- externally focused state; other situations require shifts in an in-
tained duration, there is no theoretical reason why this must ternal direction. The individual's flexibility in being able to shift
be so. attentional directions is thus a parameter that has significant
Degree and duration parameters of attentional focus high- adaptive value. Such flexibility would appear to be executed by
light some of the theoretical ambiguity of previous self-focused a combination of automatic and controlled processes. That is,
attention research. Many studies, for instance, have inter- shifts in a particular direction are "drawn" by some situations,
changeably used terms such as self-focused or excessive, in- and other shifts can be controlled by the individual's decision
creased, or heightened self-focusing. Although it is clear that as to where to attend.
such terminology is for the sake of descriptive convenience, the
present model suggests that these descriptions confuse theoreti- Content Parameters
cal assumptions and parameters. If structural capacity con-
structs are assumed, then strictly speaking increased or height- Distinctions between attentional process and content are
ened self-focused attention is not possible, because these labels critical to understanding the causes and effects of self-focused
imply flexible capacity assumptions and consequent degrees of attention. Attention is a cognitive process of bringing to aware-
internal attention. In addition, heightened self-focusing (a shift ness sensations and perceptions arising both internally and ex-
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 169

ternally. Internally focused attention refers to the direction of a greater likelihood of maladaptive functioning; mildly self-fo-
this process. The sensations and perceptions, once in awareness, cused attention would be unlikely to be associated with psycho-
are the content of this attention. Previous theorists have drawn pathology, whereas more extreme shifts would be more apt to
similar distinctions. Carver and Scheier (1981), for instance, characterize a disorder. Sustained internal attention (duration
made a related point in noting that internal attention can be parameters) is also a necessary element of self-absorption and
divided into two subjects: self-relevant and non-self-relevant. A suggests that internal attention is more likely to be dysfunc-
content distinction can also be made between attention focused tional when it is sustained for protracted periods of time. Fi-
on private self-aspects and attention focused on public self-as- nally, self-absorption also encompasses cognitive intransigence
pects (see Nasby, 1989). Similarly, a central aspect of Carver (flexibility parameters). Recalling that adaptive attentional fo-
and Scheier's conceptualization of the functioning of self-fo- cusing is a flexible process, this parameter indicates that self-
cused attention, the individual's task expectations, refers to a absorption is characterized by a rigid reliance on internal atten-
content as opposed to a process parameter. Thus, theoretical tion across a variety of situations. Internal attention is clearly
explication of self-focused attention requires not only reference appropriate in many situations; however, it is also clearly less
to a particular cognitive process (internally directed attention),
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

appropriate in some situations (e.g., performance situations


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

but also to a particular kind of content (self-relevant). where attention to task parameters is necessary for effective per-
As noted by Carver and Scheier (1981), self-content can be formance). These process aspects thus suggest that self-absorp-
distinguished into general and specific categories. Whereas self, tion is defined by excessive, sustained, and rigid attention to
a superordinate category, refers only to attentional content that information emanating from internal sources.6
is self-relevant, more specific categories of self-focused atten-
tion content are possible and refer to the many different aspects Conceptual Characteristics of Self-Absorption:
of the self that can be attended to. Carver (1979) argued, for Content Variables
instance, that autonomic bodily activity, awareness of past or
present behaviors, or memories of previous self-relevant events Although internal attention represents the process, the gen-
are all examples of specific self-content. eral content of self-absorption is self-relevant by definition and
thus quite nonspecific. The specific content (or subject) of self-
absorbed attention in psychopathology can vary considerably.
Pathological Self-Focused Attention: Self-Absorption
Given that schemata are thought to be the structures underlying
As previously discussed, data suggesting that elevated self- attentional processes, several clinical theorists (e.g., Beck, 1976;
focused attention characterizes numerous disorders portend a Kuiper, Olingei; & MacDonald, 1988;Teasdale, 1988)have pro-
critical conceptual dilemma for models seeking to meaningfully posed models that would suggest that the specific content of
account for attentional processes in dysfunction. For example, self-absorption in varying disorders is determined by the prepo-
the theoretical and practical significance of self-focused atten- tent schema. As such, although the process of self-absorption
tion as it is typically denned may be diminished for descriptive characterizes diverse psychopathological states and is common
psychopathology efforts because it cannot distinguish funda- to generally dysfunctional states, the key to cognitively differ-
mentally different psychopathological conditions. Two further entiating these states lies in the particular schema, or cognitive-
theoretical clarifications are therefore proposed. First, using the affective network (Bower, 1981), that is accessed (e.g., self-de-
attentional parameters outlined earlier, a particular kind of self- grading content in depression, fear or harm in anxiety, disorga-
focused attention is proposed that invariably accompanies dis- nized self-content in schizophrenia, grandiosity in mania).
ordered functioning. Second, the elements of self-focused atten- Hence, the self-relevant content of self-absorption is viewed as
tion are related to a metaconstruct model of psychopathology a function of the particular schema operating in the disordered
that provides a means for describing both the specificity and state (for empirical evidence on some of these distinctions, see
nonspecific! ty of variables across disorders. Beck, Brown, Steer, Eidelson, & Riskind, 1987; Clark, 1986;
The maladaptive self-focused attention to be described is re- Ingram etal., 1987).
ferred to as self-absorption. This label is quite arbitrary in the Self-absorption can therefore be distinguished in several ways
sense that many other labels could as easily have been invoked. from traditional concepts of self-focused attention in the nor-
The term self-absorption is simply aimed at capturing the dys- mal population. In addition to variations in the degree and du-
functional quality of maladaptive self-focused attention. ration of internal attention states, it is important to note the
subtle but important distinction between self-absorption and
the notion of chronic self-focused attention (e.g., Carver &
Conceptual Characteristics of Self-Absorption:
Scheier, 1981). The idea of chronic self-focused attention sug-
Process Variables
gests that individuals may be inclined to focus internally across
Three attention process parameters were outlined: degree pa- a variety of different situations; the current model adds an in-
rameters, duration parameters, and flexibility parameters. Self-
absorption is defined as a dysfunctional shift in the combination 6
It is important to note that although the description of these param-
of these parameters. In particular, shifts to internal, sustained, eters is consistent with existing research, each represents attentional
and inflexible attention are proposed to characterize psycho- constructs that have been developed to account for this research. Pro-
pathological states of functioning. For instance, excessive inter- spective studies examining these proposals are thus necessary to accu-
nal attention (degree parameters) is proposed to correspond to rately test the role of these parameters in psychopathology.
170 RICK E. INGRAM

flexibility dimension to this process. Thus, chronic self-focused Carver and Scheier's Control Theory
attention per se is not dysfunctional; an inability to shift out of
this state in response to situational demands is. Experimentally, In addition to specifying normal self-regulatory functions,
these parameters should also be distinguishable and testable. Carver and Scheier have also applied their model to dysfunc-
Although indices of self-focused attention are not as yet partic- tional states, most notably anxiety (e.g., Carver & Scheier,
ularly sophisticated, social-cognitive research has used several 1986). Although Carver and Scheier's model and the current
measures to assess this state. Thus, in principle, different de- model are similar in some respects, they differ in other impor-
grees of self-focused attention should be able to be assessed by tant regards. Derived from Duval and Wicklund's (1972) origi-
measures that provide some quantification of the process (e.g., nal work on self-awareness, control theory suggests that an im-
the Self-Focus Sentence Completion Scale). Similarly, these portant element in self-regulation is self-evaluation. Thus,
measures should provide a means to assess the duration of self- when the individual enters a state of self-focused attention, an
focused attention. Greenberg and Pyszczynski (1986), for in- evaluation process comparing behavior and behavioral stan-
stance, provided a relevant illustration of this in their study, dards is spontaneously engaged. In the case of dysfunction,
Carver and Scheier proposed a sequential process where self-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

which suggested that over a period of time, subclinically de-


focused attention necessarily and invariably leads to a focus on
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pressed individuals sustained a higher level of self-focused at-


tention than a nondepressed group. Finally, it is also possible to outcome expectancies. Such expectancies are centrally impor-
develop experimental situations where it is adaptive to shift tant in dysfunction in that disorders such as depression and
one's focus from internal to external sources (e.g., adequate task anxiety are characterized by unfavorable task expectations.
performance, successful interaction with another person). Such In addition to the process parameters previously discussed,
experimental situations allow for tests of the intransigence of the primary difference between Carver and Scheier's (1981,
attentional processes in disordered individuals. 1982) control theory and the current model is in regard to the
centrality of negative task expectations in dysfunction. Whereas
Carver and Scheier suggested that self-focus inevitably leads to
Comparisons With Other Models Applying expectancy assessment, the current model views expectancies
Self-Focused Attention to Dysfunction as just one of a number of content dimensions that can become
salient when an individual is self-focused. Accordingly, the role
Although models of self-focused attention inevitably share a of expectancies is considerably less important in the present
number of features, they can also be distinguished on several model. This position is taken for two reasons. First, there is little
grounds. With regard to self-focused attention in dysfunctional evidence to suggest that self-evaluation or expectancy assess-
states, there are three current models that are particularly rele- ment always follows from self-focused attention. In the domain
vant to the present proposal: Hull and Levy (1979), Carver and of psychopathology, extant data have not suggested that expec-
Scheier (1981, 1982), and Pyszczynski and Greenberg (1987). tancy assessment spontaneously occurs in response to self-fo-
The central aspects of these models are briefly reviewed and cused attention; most studies either specifically prompt inquir-
their differences with the present approach are discussed. ies concerning self-evaluation or attempt to manipulate expec-
tancies. Hull (1981) has made a similar point in his discussion
of self-focused attention in alcohol abuse. Second, empirical
Hull and Levy's Model of Self-Awareness studies have generally been unable to provide statistically reli-
able evidence that expectancies play such a key role in self-fo-
In many respects, Hull and Levy's (1979) model is the most cused attention and dysfunction (e.g., Burgio, Merluzzi, &
similar to the current set of proposals. Hull and Levy proposed Pryor, 1986; Carver et al., 1983; Strack, Blaney, Ganellen, &
that self-awareness relates to a person's manner of organizing Coyne, 1985).
information for encoding. In particular, they suggested that self-
focused attention corresponds to information that is encoded Pyszczynski and Greenberg's Self-Regulatory
specifically in relation to its self-relevance. Hull (1981) further Model of Depression
suggested that alcohol consumption interferes with the pro-
cesses essential to achieving a state of self-focused attention. In line with Carver and Scheier's (1981,1982) approach, Pys-
Such a theory is consistent with suggestions that a variety of zczynski and Greenberg (1987) suggested that the disruption
negative states are associated with increased self-focused atten- that leads to depression instigates a self-focused attention pro-
tion and that some individuals may thus choose to drink to di- cess that engages a self-evaluation process. These factors lead to
minish this self-focused attention and the negative affect that a self-regulatory cycle from which the depressed individual does
goes along with it. The key difference between Hull's model and not disengage. As a result of this perseveration, depressed indi-
the current model resides in the fact that Hull's model is in- viduals develop a self-focusing "style" that results in self-esteem
tended to be more specific and does not explicitly discuss the deficits, exacerbated negative affect, and task performance
myriad of negative states that are associated with self-focused deficits. Pyszczynski and Greenberg further speculated that the
attention. However, given the focus of this model on alcohol as depressive self-focusing style can account for "negative auto-
relief for negative states, it is not necessary to account for the matic thoughts, anxiety, sleeplessness, poor concentration, an-
relation between self-focused attention and these various nega- ger, aches and pains, fatigue, and psychomotor retardation" (p.
tive states. 133). Moreover, they suggested that the self-focusing style
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 171

causes a motivation to maintain a negative self-image in which Hollon & Kriss, 1984; Kihlstrom & Nasby, 1981). Building on
positive thoughts, feelings, signs of competence, and personal these proposals to differentiate the various aspects of what con-
value become anxiety provoking. In short, Pyszczynski and stitutes cognition, Ingram and Kendall (1986), Ingram and Hol-
Greenberg suggested that self-focusing is the central and inte- lon (1986), and Kendall and Ingram (1987) have proposed a
grative process in virtually every category of depressive symp- taxonomy for categorizing classes of cognitive constructs. Con-
toms and features. temporary cognitive variables can thus reasonably be described
There are several similarities between Pyszczynski and as belonging to a given category: structures, propositions, opera-
Greenberg's (1987) model and the current framework. For ex- tions, or products.
ample, although they are conceptualized somewhat differently,
the concepts of sustained attention and perseveration are sim- Cognitive Structures
ilar. Although there are also several differences between these
approaches, there are two major distinctions. First, the present These constructs represent the architectural properties of the
model does not view attentional focus as the central process in information-processing system within which data are stored.
virtually all depressive symptomatology that is suggested by the Although "contentless" themselves, these structures reflect the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mechanisms that organize information and encompass con-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

perseveration model. Indeed, research has suggested that some


of the depressive features proposed to result from self-focused cepts such as the linkages and associations among stored memo-
attention (e.g., attributions [Gibbons et al., 1985], self-esteem ries. Cognitive structure constructs include short- and long-
[Smith et al., 1985]) are unrelated. In other instances, virtually term memory, sensory-iconic storage, and cognitive networks.
no studies have attempted to address the multitude of features
thought to result from self-focusing. It seems unlikely, however, Cognitive Propositions
that the attentional variables described by Pyszczynski and
Propositions refer to the information, or content, that is rep-
Greenberg can account for such numerous and diverse depres-
resented within cognitive structures. Semantic and episodic
sive symptoms.
knowledge can be considered examples of cognitive proposi-
The second major distinction concerns the specificity of self-
tions or content. Together, cognitive structures and content are
focused attention and depression. Although self-regulatory the-
typically referred to as schemata.
ory provides an elaborate account of the relation between self-
focusing and diverse aspects of depression, some of which are
presumably unique to depression, it is silent with regard to the Cognitive Operations
other psychopathological states in which self-focused attention
This category includes process constructs representing the
is characteristic. Although the present framework is intended
various procedures by which the cognitive system functions to
specifically to account for these diverse states, it is unclear as to
manipulate information, such as encoding and retrieving infor-
how self-regulatory theory can accomplish this task and at the
mation. Conceptualized as the process by which either inter-
same time describe processes that appear to be specific to de-
nally or externally derived information is brought to awareness,
pression.
attention is a construct belonging within the operational cate-
gory.
Self-Absorption as Applied to a Model of
Psychopathology: The Meta-Construct Model Cognitive Products
In further attempting to explicate the role that self-absorp- Cognitive products are an end result of the operation of the
tion plays in dysfunction, it is important to consider that there information-processing system and encompass the individual's
are a number of diverse factors that contribute significantly to cognitions. In a sense, cognitive products represent the "sur-
the development and course of psychopathology. This final sec- face" manifestations of the individual's cognitive propositions
tion illustrates how the content and processes elements of self- (Hollon & Kriss, 1984). This level of analysis includes concepts
absorption can be classified according to a model that provides such as attributions (see Peterson & Seligman, 1984) and self-
a means for categorizing the specific and nonspecific variables statements (Kendall & Hollon, 1981).
in disordered functioning. Before relating this model to self-
focused attention, the basic constructs of the approach are Partitioning the Variance in Psychopathology
noted. Specifically, this model integrates two conceptual sys-
tems that have been described separately elsewhere: a taxonomy Given the diversity of cognitive constructs that have been ap-
of the classes of cognitive constructs (Ingram & Kendall, 1986) plied to psychopathology, it is inevitable that there will be over-
and a variance partition model (Ingram & Kendall, 1987; Ken- lap of at least some constructs among fundamentally different
dall & Ingram, 1987). disorders. This overlap does not necessarily imply that various
constructs are invalid or not useful simply because they lack
specificity (cf. Coyne & Gotlib, 1983). Rather, overlap has im-
Cognitive Taxonomy
portant theoretical and empirical implications in its own right.
The fundamental difference between cognitive structures and To examine these implications, however, theoretical frame-
processes are generally well known and have been clinically works are needed that account for variables that are either spe-
elaborated in several proposals (e.g., Goldfried & Robins, 1983; cific to certain psychopathologies or that overlap among many
172 RICK. E. INGRAM

psychopathologies. Ingram and Kendall (1987) and Kendall and Table 1


Ingram (1987) have proposed that a useful approach to this Components of Self-Absorption Viewed According to the
problem is an analogy to statistical techniques that partition Meta-Construct Model of Psychopathology
variance. For example, variance in an analysis of variance (AN-
Psychopathological variance
OVA) procedure is partitioned into variance that is unique to a
Level of
given factor (a main effect), variance that is common or shared analysis Critical features Common features
among different factors (an interaction), and variance due to
individual differences (error variance). Similarly, factor analytic Cognitive
techniques break a large number of items into underlying Structural
Prepositional Psychopathology-
sources of variance. Some of these sources are common across specific beliefs
resulting factors, and some are specific to given factors. By Operational Sustained, internal,
adopting a conceptual analogy based on such statistical parti- and rigid
tion methods, the expression of a given psychopathology can be attention
Products Psychopathology- Predominant self-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

thought of as a function of several converging sources of vari-


specific thoughts relevant thoughts
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ance that are either specific to the disorder or common to disor- Behavioral
dered functioning in general. Thus, psychopathology variance Affective
can be partitioned in a conceptual sense in much the same way Biochemical
that variance is partitioned empirically by an ANOVA or factor
Note. Levels of analyses are not limited to those in this table.
analysis; the diversity of cognitive constructs that have been
studied in different disorders can be categorized according to
whether they appear to describe variables unique to a disorder
or whether they appear to contribute to a variety of disorders. psychopathology. The intention of this model is to facilitate the
Hence, the variance in psychopathology could be conceptually classification of the myriad components that characterize psy-
partitioned as follows: chopathology. It can be applied to a given psychopathology to
examine theoretically or empirically derived components of the
Expression Critical disorder at each of the various levels, or, as in the present case,
of given = psychopathological it can be used to classify a set of variables characterizing a par-
disorder features ticular psychopathological process.
Common Error The meta-construct model is illustrated in Table 1. As can be
+ psychopathological + (individual seen from this table, the cognitive level of analysis is further
features differences) divided into the categories suggested by the cognitive taxonomy.
Hence, a particular cognitive construct can be classified de-
In such an analogy, critical psychopathological features are pending on whether it is proposed as a structural, prepositional,
the main effects, or sources of specific variance, and thus reflect operational, or product variable. In addition, each construct,
those constructs that are relatively unique to a particular disor- based on existing theory and empirical data, can be further cat-
der. Within the general domain of maladaptive functioning, egorized according to whether it describes a variable specific
these features differentiate one disorder from another. Common (critical) to a particular disorder or whether it is common across
psychopathological features represent interactions or variance disorders. As can also be seen from this table, a variety of levels
that is shared among independent variables (i.e., different disor- of analysis using this framework are possible. The present dis-
ders). These variables characterize a variety of diverse disorders, cussion is, of course, limited to the cognitive level, specifically
and although they do not differentiate various disorders, they as it pertains to self-absorption.
do differentiate maladaptive functioning from adaptive func- When applied to self-absorption, the meta-construct model
tioning. Thus, these variables are important even though they suggests several classifications, which are illustrated in Table 1.
are of little use in determining what kind of nonadaptive func- When one recalls that the process of self-absorption is defined
tioning has occurred. Finally, error variance, or individual as internal attention that is excessive, rigid across situations,
differences, represents the unique factors that any given individ- and prolonged, these components are most appropriately
ual brings to daily functioning or dysfunctioning. Thus, despite viewed as operational variables. Because the data suggest that
the presence of theoretically predictable critical and common these operations occur in many disorders, they are further con-
features, any type of psychopathology will be expressed in a sidered to be common features of psychopathology. To the ex-
somewhat different way because of the particular characteristics tent that certain psychopathological symptoms may be gov-
of the individual involved. Because error variance is unpredict- erned by attentional processes, such commonality suggests that
able by definition, and is presumably less important than sys- self-absorption may account for the considerable symptom
tematic sources of variance, it is not discussed further. overlap that is observed across diverse disordered states (DSM
HI-R,APA, 1987).
Mela-Construct Model The remaining aspects of self-absorption are defined as con-
tent variables. The most general aspect of this content, predom-
The cognitive taxonomy and variance partition frameworks inantly self-relevant thinking, constitutes a product variable
together form the basis of a meta-construct model of descriptive that is common to disorders in general. As noted previously,
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 173

however, the particular kinds of self-relevant cognitions are References


schema-based and thus unique to the specific disorder (critical
features). Finally, because these thoughts are assumed to be the Abelson, R. P. (1981). The psychological status of the script concept.
surface manifestations of the individual's beliefs and stored in- American Psychologist, 36. 5-729.
formation, these beliefs represent the propositional level of American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical
analysis. Thus, the pathological equivalent of self-focused atten- manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Au-
thor.
tion, self-absorption, is defined in such a way that several of its
Anderson,!. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Jour-
components are likely to occur across a multitude of disorders.
nal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22,261-295.
At the same time, it is proposed that the specific content or sub-
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical
ject of the attention will be a reflection of the schema operating aspects. New \brk: Hoeber.
in the particular disorder and thus will be specific to the dis- Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New
order. York: International University Press.
It is apparent that although the components of self-absorp- Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Steer, R. A., Eidelson, J. I., & Riskind, J. H.
tion fit into this conceptual scheme in several different areas, a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1987). Differentiating anxiety and depression: A test of the cognitive


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

number of categories are blank. This reflects the fact that any content specificity hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96,
specific psychopathology is a multidetermined state and that a 179-183.
variety of interacting cognitive (as well as other) factors contrib- Becker, J. (1979). Vulnerable self-esteem as a predisposing factor in de-
pression disorders. In R. A. Depue (Ed.), The psychobiology of de-
ute to various disorders in a complex fashion. Hence, any given
pressive disorders: Implications for the effects of stress (pp. 317-334).
disorder is not a disorder of self-absorption but, rather, self-ab-
New York: Academic Press.
sorption appears to constitute a cluster of variables that contrib-
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,
ute to different disorders. 129-148.
Brockner, J., Hjelle, L., & Plant, R. W. (1985). Self-focused attention,
self-esteem and the experience of depression. Journal of Personality,
Summary 53, 425-435.
Burgio, K. L., Merluzzi, T. V., & Pryor, J. B. (1986). Effects of expec-
Numerous investigators, working largely independently, have tancy and self-focused attention on social interaction. Journal of Per-
explored the role of self-focused attention in diverse disorders. sonality and Social Psychology, SO, 1216-1221.
Similarly, theoretical models describing the role of self-focused Buss, A. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco:
W. H. Freeman.
attention in particular disorders have been advanced, also
Cacioppo, J. T, Glass, C. R., & Merluzzi, T. V. (1979). Self-statements
largely independently. Given the sheer number of empirical and
and self-evaluations: A cognitive response analysis of heterosexual
theoretical descriptions of self-focused attention in psychologi-
anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3, 249-262.
cal dysfunction, no synthesis of this diverse yet remarkably con- Carver, C. S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention processes.
sistent literature across psychopathological domains has been Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1251-1281.
proposed. After reviewing the literature, this article suggests Carver, C. S., Blaney, P. H., & Scheier, M. F. (1979). Focus of attention,
that self-focused attention is a nonspecific process in psychopa- chronic expectancy, and responses to a feared stimulus. Journal of
thology. Although this characterization appears accurate, it also Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1186-1195.
diminishes the potential importance of this variable for efforts Carver, C. S., & Glass, D. (1976). The self-consciousness scale: A dis-
to describe the key processes in psychopathology. Furthermore, criminant validity study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 40, 169-
if self-focused attention is important in psychopathology, it is 172.
Carver, C. S., Peterson, L. M., Follansbee, D. J., & Scheier, M. F. (1983).
unclear what differentiates "normal" self-focused attention
Effects of self-directed attention on performance and persistence
from attentional processes that contribute to dysfunction. By
among persons high and low in test anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and
suggesting deviations in the degree, duration, and flexibility of
Research, 7, 333-354.
self-focused attention, however, it is possible to describe a con- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A
ceptual variant of this normal process, self-absorption, that is control-therapy approach to human behavior. Berlin, Federal Repub-
proposed to invariably accompany psychopathological func- lic of Germany: Springer-Verlag.
tioning. Finally, to illustrate how self-absorption can occur Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful concep-
across such a diversity of disorders, a classification model of tual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology.
psychopathology, the meta-construct model, was briefly re- Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111-135.
viewed. This model suggests that the process aspects of self-ab- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1983). A control theory approach to
sorption (i.e., excessive, sustained, and rigid attention) can be human behavior and implications for problems in self-management.
In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and
seen as common variables in disorders, whereas the content
therapy (Vol. 2, pp. 127-194). New York: Academic Press.
variables that appear to differentiate various disorders are
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1984). Self-focused attention in test anxi-
unique to the particular psychopathological schema that con-
ety: A general theory applied to a specific phenomenon. In H. M. van
trols the more generalized attentional process. Future theory der Ploeg, R. Schwarzer, & C. D. Spielbeiger (Eds.), Advances in test
and research is necessary to confirm or disconfirm the veracity anxiety research (Vol. 3, pp. 3-20). Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets &
of the categories proposed by the model, the placement of the Zeitlinger.
various components of self-absorption within these categories, Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1986). Functional and dysfunctional
and, if appropriate, to fill in the remaining gaps. responses to anxiety: The interaction between expectancies and self-
174 RICK E. INGRAM

focused attention. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in Hare, R. D. (1970). Psychopathy: Theory and research. New York: Wi-
anxiety and motivation (pp. 111-141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ley.
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1973). Disordered thought in schizo- Hollon, S. D., DeRubeis, R. J., & Evans, M. D. (1987). Causal media-
phrenia- New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. tion of change in treatment for depression: Discriminating between
Checkley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby. nonspecificity and noncausality. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 139-
Clark, D. A. (1986). Cognitive-affective interaction: A test of the "speci- 149.
ficity" and "generality" hypotheses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, Hollon, S. D., & Kriss, M. (1984). Cognitive factors in clinical research
10, 607-623. and practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 35-76.
Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, 1. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression: Hope, D. A., & Heimberg, R. G. (1985). Public and private self-con-
A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472-505. sciousness in a social phobic sample. Paper presented at the meeting
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and retention of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Hous-
of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: ton.
General, 104, 268-294. Hope, D. A., Heimberg, R. G., Zollo, L. J., Nyman, D. J., & O'Brien,
Deffenbacher, J. L. (1978). Worry, emotionality, and task-generated in- G. T. (1987). Thought listing in the natural environment: Valence and
focus of listed thoughts among socially anxious and nonanxious sub-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

terference in test anxiety: An empirical test of attentional theory.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Journalof Educational Psychology, 70, 248-254. jects. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Advance-
Deffenbacher, J. L. (1980). Worry and emotionality in test anxiety. In ment of Behavior Therapy, Boston.
I. G. Sarason (Ed.), Test anxiety: Theory, research and applications Houston, B. K. (1977). Dispositional anxiety and the effectiveness of
(pp. 111-128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. cognitive coping strategies in stressful laboratory and classroom situ-
Dempsey, P. (1964). A unidimensional scale for the MMPI. Journal of ations. In C. D. Spielberger & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety
Consulting Psychology, 28, 364-370. (Vol. 4). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Dodge, K. A. (1985). Attributional bias in aggressive children. In P. C. Hull, J. G. (1981). A self-awareness model of the causes and effects of
Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy alcohol consumption. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 586-600.
(Vol. 4, pp. 75-110). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Hull, J. G., Levenson, R. W, &. Young, R. D. (1986). Reassessing the
Doris, J., & Sarason, S. B. (1955). Test anxiety and blame assignment relation between self-consciousness and affective states: Heightened
in a failure situation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, self-awareness of affect or heightened affect? Unpublished manu-
335-338. script, Darthmouth College.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-aware- Hull, J. G., Levenson, R. W., Young, R. D., & Sher, K. J. (1983). Self-
ness. New York: Academic Press. awareness-reducing effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Per-
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and sonality and Social Psychology, 44, 461-473.
the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 83-201. Hull, J. G., & Levy, A. S. (1979). The organizational functions of the
Exner, J. E. (1973). The Self-Focus Sentence Completion Scale: A study self: An alternative to the Duval and Wicklund model of self-aware-
of egocentricity. Journalof Personality Assessment, 37, 437-455. ness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 756-768.
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private Hull, J. G., & Young, R. D. (1983). Self-consciousness, self-esteem, and
self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and success-failure as determinants of alcohol consumption in male so-
Clinical Psychology, 43, 522-527. cial drinkers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1097-
Ganzer, V. J. (1968). Effects of audience presence and test anxiety on 1109.
learning and retention in a serial learning situation. Journal of Per- Hull, J. G., Young, R. D., & Jouriles, E. (1986). Applications of the self-
sonality and Social Psychology, S, 194-199. awareness model of alcohol consumption: Predicting patterns of use
Geen, R. G. (1976). Test anxiety, observation, and range of cue utiliza- and abuse. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 790-
tion. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 253-259. 796.
Gibbons, F. X. (1983). Self-focused attention and self-report validity: Ingram, R. E. (1984). Toward an information processing analysis of de-
The "veridicality" hypothesis. Journal of Personality, 51, 517-542. pression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 443-478.
Gibbons, F. X. (1987). Mild depression and self-disclosure intimacy: Ingram, R. E., (1988, August). Chronic internal attention as a risk factor
Self and others' perceptions. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, for emotional distress. Paper presented at the 96th annual meeting of
361^380. the American Psychological Association, Atlanta, GA.
Gibbons, F. X., Smith, T. W, Ingram, R. E., Pearce, K., Brehm, S. S., Ingram, R. E. (1989). Affective confounds in social-cognitive research.
& Schroeder, D. J. (1985). Self-awareness and self-confrontation: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 715-722.
Effects of self-focused attention on members of a clinical population. Ingram, R. E. (in press). Attentional nonspecificity in depressive and
Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 662-675. generalized anxious affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research.
Goldfried, M. R., & Robins, C. (1983). Self-schema, cognitive bias, and Ingram, R. E., Cruet, D., Johnson, B., & Wisnicki, K. S. (1988). Self-
the processing of therapeutic experiences. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Ad- focused attention, gender, gender role, and vulnerability to negative
vances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (Vol. 2, pp. 33- affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 967-978.
80). New York: Academic Press. Ingram, R. E., & Hollon, S. D. (1986). Cognitive therapy of depression
Gotlib, I. H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in univer- from an information processing perspective. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.),
sity students. Journalof Abnormal Psychology, 93, 19-30. Information processing approaches to clinical psychology. Orlando,
Gottschalk, L. A., Gleser, G. C, & Hambidge, G. C. (1957). Verbal FL: Academic Press.
behavior analysis: Some content and form variables in speech rele- Ingram, R. E., & Kendall, P. C. (1986). Cognitive clinical psychology:
vant to personality adjustment. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, Implications of an information processing perspective. In R. E. In-
77,300-311. gram (Ed.), Information processing approaches to clinical psychology
Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1986). Persistent high self-focus after (pp. 3-21). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
failure and low self-focus after success: The depressive self-focusing Ingram, R. E., & Kendall, P. C. (1987). The cognitive side of anxiety.
style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1039-1044. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 523-536.
SELF-FOCUSED ATTENTION 175

Ingram, R. E., Lumry, A., Cruet, D., & Sieber, W. (1987). Attentional of the self-schema. Journal oj'Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
processes in depression disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 117-123.
11, 351-360. Nasby, W., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Cognitive assessment of personal-
Ingram, R. E., & Smith, T. W. (1984). Depression and internal versus ity and psychopathology. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), Information process-
external focus of attention. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 139- ing approaches to clinical psychology (pp. 219-239). Orlando: Aca-
152. demic Press.
Johnston, W. A., & Dark, V. J. (1982). In defense of intraperceptual Natale, M., Dahlberg, C. C., & Jaffe, J. (1978). The relationship of de-
theories of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human fensive language behavior in patient monologues in the course of psy-
Perception and Performance, 8, 407-421. choanalysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34,466-470.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren- Neale, J. M., & Katahn, M. (1968). Anxiety, choice and stimulus uncer-
tice-Hall. tainty. Journal of Personality, 36, 235-245.
Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.
and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties Nideffer, R. M. (1976). Test of attentional and interpersonal style. Jour-
of attention (pp. 29-61). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 394-404.
Kendall, P. C, & Hollon, S. D. (1981). Assessing self-referent speech: Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

view, 88, 1-15.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Methods in the measurement of self-statements. In P. C. Kendall &


S. D. Hollon (Eds.), Assessment strategies for cognitive-behavioral in- Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-
terventions (pp. 85-118). New York: Academic Press. limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44-64.
Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T, Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action: Willed and
R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck automatic control of behavior (Technical Report No. 8006). San
Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289- Diego: University of California, Center for Human Information Pro-
299. cessing.
Kendall, P. C, & Ingram, R. E. (1987). The future for cognitive assess- Nuechterlein, K. H., & Dawson, M. E. (1984). Information processing
ment of anxiety: Let's get specific. In L. Michelson & L. M. Ascher and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizo-
(Eds.), Anxiety and stress disorders: Cognitive-behavioral assessment phrenic disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10, 160-202.
and treatment (pp. 89-104). New York: Guilford Press. Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (1984). Varieties of attention. Or-
Kerr, B. (1973). Processing demands during mental operations. Memory lando, FL: Academic Press.
and Cognition, I, 401-412. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk
Kihlstrom, J. F., & Nasby, W. (1981). Cognitive tasks in clinical assess- factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91,
347-374.
ment: An exercise in applied psychology. In P. C. Kendall & S. D.
Hollon (Eds.), Assessment strategies for cognitive-behavioral inter- Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C, Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky,
ventions (pp. 287-317). New York: Academic Press. G. L, & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). The attributional style question-
naire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287-300.
Kuiper, N. A., Olinger, L. J., & MacDonald, M. R. (1988). Vulnerability
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1985). Depression and preference for
and episodic cognitions in a self-worth contingency model of depres-
self-focusing stimuli after success and failure. Journal of Personality
sion. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 289-
and Social Psychology, 49, 1066-1075.
309). New York: Guilford Press.
Pyszczynski, T, & Greenberg, J. (1986). Evidence for a depressive self-
Larsen, R. L, & Cowan, G. S. (1988). Internal focus of attention and
focusing style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 95-
depression: A study of daily experience. Motivation and Emotion, 12,
106.
237-249.
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory perseveration
Lewinsohn, P. M., Hoberman, H., Teri, L., & Hautzinger, M. (1985).
and the depressive self-focusing style: A self-awareness theory of reac-
An integrative theory of depression. In S. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.),
tive depression. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122-138.
Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 331-359). New York: Aca-
Sarason, I. G. (1972). Experimental approaches to test anxiety: Atten-
demic Press.
tion and the uses of information. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety:
Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional com-
Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 2, pp. 96-117). New York:
ponents of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psycho-
Academic Press.
logical Reports, 20. 975-978.
Sarason, I. G. (1975). Anxiety and self-preoccupation. In I. G. Sarason,
Lorenz, M., & Cobb, S. (1953). Language behavior in psychoneurotic
&C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 2, pp. 129-151).
patients. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 69, 684-693.
New York: Hemisphere.
Magaro, P. A. (1980). Cognition in schizophrenia and paranoia: The Sarason, I. G. (1986). Test anxiety, worry, and cognitive interference. In
integration of cognitive processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in anxiety and motivation
Mandler, G., & Sarason, S. B. (1952). A study of anxiety and learning. (pp. 19-33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 166-173. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, andunder-
Mandler, G., & Watson, D. L. (1966). Anxiety and the interruption of standing:An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ:
behavior. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 22- Erlbaum.
49). New York: Academic Press. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1977). Self-focused attention and the
Marlett, N. J., & Watson, D. (1968). Test anxiety and immediate or experience of emotion: Attraction, repulsion, elation, and depres-
delayed feedback in a test-like avoidance task. Journal of Personality sion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 625-636.
and Social Psychology, 8, 200-203. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Gibbons, F. X. (1981). Self-focused at-
Musson, R. F., & Alloy, L. B. (1988). Depression and self-directed atten- tention and reactions to fear. Journal of Research in Personality, 15,
tion. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 193- 1-15.
220). New York: Guilford Press. Scherwitz, L., Graham, L. E., Grandits, G., Buehler, J., & Billings, J.
Nasby, W. (1989). Private and public self-consciousness and articulation (1986). Self-involvement and coronary heart disease incidence in the
176 RICK E. INGRAM

Multiple Risk Factor Intervention Trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, Taylor, S. E., & Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic bases of social information
48, 187-199. processing. In E. T. Higgins, P. Hermann, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The
Scherwitz, L., McKelvain, R., Laman, C., Patterson, J., Dutton, L., Yu- Ontario Symposium on personality and social psychology (Vol. I, pp.
sim, S., Lester, 5., Kraft, I., Rochelle, D., & Leachman, R. (1983). 46-82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Type A behavior, self-involvement, and coronary atherosclerosis. Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 47-57. Cognition andEmotion, 2, 247-274.
Schmitt, J. P. (1983). Focus of attention in the treatment of depression. Turk, D. C., Meichenbaum, D., & Genest, M. (1983). Pain and behav-
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 20, 457-463. ioral medicine: A cognitive-behavioral perspective. New York: Guil-
Schneider, W., Dumais, S. T, & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). Automatic con- ford Press.
trol processing and attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies Turner, R., Scheier, M., Carver, C., & Ickes, W. (1978). Correlates of
(Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 1-27). Orlando, FL: Academic self-consciousness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 42, 285-289.
Press. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition
Shdo, C. (1989). Increased self-focused attention in hypomania. Unpub- to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96,
lished master's thesis, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA. 465-490.
ShirTrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic hu- Weintraub, W. (1981). Verbal behavior: Adaptation and psychopathol-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ogy. New York: Springer.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

man information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic at-


tending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127-190. Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Para-
suraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63-102).
Slapion, J. J., & Carver, C. S. (1981). Self-directed attention and facilita-
tion of intellectual performance among persons high in test anxiety. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Wine. J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 5. 115-121.
Bulletin, 76, 92-104.
Smith. T. W.. & Greenbcrg, J. (1981). Depression and self-focused at-
Wine, J. D. (1980). Cognitive-attentional theory of test anxiety. In I. G.
tention. .\foti'vation and Emotion, 5, 323-331.
Sarason (Ed.), Test anxiety: Theory, research, and applications (pp.
Smith, T. W., Ingram, R. E., & Roth, D. L. (1985). Self-focused atten-
349-385). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tion and depression: Self-evaluation, affect, and life stress. Motivation
Wine, J. D. (1982). Evaluation anxiety: A cognitive-attentional con-
andEmotion, 9, 381-389.
struct. In H. W. Krohne & L. Laux (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Test man-
anxiety (pp. 207-219). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
ual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1965). Manual for the Multiple Affect
Psychologists Press.
Adjective Checklist. San Diego, CA: Educational & Industrial Testing
Strack, S., Blaney, P. H., Ganellen, R. J., & Coyne, J. C. (1985). Pessi- Service.
mistic self-preoccupation, performance deficits, and depression.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1076-1085. Received August 5,1987
Sutton-Simon, K., & Goldfried, M. R. (1979). Faulty thinking patterns Revision received April 11, 1989
in two types of anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, J, 193-203. Accepted April 20, 1989 •

View publication stats

You might also like