Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/228881291

Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits: A review and extension

Article  in  Journal of Consumer Marketing · January 2007


DOI: 10.1108/07363760710720975

CITATIONS READS
299 7,284

4 authors, including:

Celso Augusto de Matos Cristiana Trindade Ituassu


Universidade do Vale do Rio dos Sinos Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo da Fundação Getulio Vargas
66 PUBLICATIONS   2,272 CITATIONS    22 PUBLICATIONS   310 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Carlos Alberto Vargas Rossi


Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul
43 PUBLICATIONS   2,230 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Consumer Debt - Endividamento de Consumidores View project

Services Marketing and Strategy View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Carlos Alberto Vargas Rossi on 17 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits:
a review and extension
Celso Augusto de Matos
Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul (PPGA-EA-UFRGS), Brazil
Cristiana Trindade Ituassu
Federal University of Minas Gerais (UFMG), Brazil, and
Carlos Alberto Vargas Rossi
Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul (PPGA-EA-UFRGS), Brazil

Abstract
Purpose – The purpose of this research is to propose and test a model that integrates the main predictors of consumers’ attitude and behavioral
intentions toward counterfeits; to help companies understand the main factors influencing consumer behavior toward counterfeits and create effective
anti-piracy strategies.
Design/methodology/approach – An integrated model is proposed following the studies by Ang et al. and Huang et al. A survey with 400
consumers was conducted in the Brazilian market and the Structural Equation Modeling technique was used to test the hypothesized relationships.
Findings – The main contribution of the paper is to show that consumer intentions to buy counterfeited products are dependent on the attitudes they
have toward counterfeits, which in turn are more influenced by perceived risk, whether consumers have bought a counterfeit before, subjective norm,
integrity, price-quality inference and personal gratification. The paper reinforces the mediator role of attitude in the relationship between these
antecedents and behavioral intentions. Moreover, previous experience with counterfeits consumption does not have a direct effect on behavioral
intentions, but only an indirect effect through attitude.
Practical implications – The paper contributes to inform policy makers and managers of brands about the main predictors of consumer’s attitudes
toward counterfeits. In this way, ads intended to discourage consumption of counterfeits could use the perceived risk as the main message appeal.
Originality/value – This paper investigates the key antecedents and consequences of consumer attitudes toward counterfeits by integrating and
testing two recent models dealing with this subject in the marketing literature.

Keywords Attitudes, Consumer behaviour, Counterfeiting, Risk assessment, Brazil

Paper type Research paper

An executive summary for managers and executive (trademarks, patents and copyrights) constitutes product
readers can be found at the end of this article. counterfeiting.”
Actions to limit counterfeits can arise from both supply and
Introduction demand side, considering the tactics companies employ to
deter counterfeits (Chaudhry et al., 2005) and the motivations
Counterfeiting is a significant and growing problem that make a counterfeit an interesting option for some
worldwide, occurring both in less and well developed customers (Huang et al., 2004; Ang et al., 2001). Because
countries. In the USA economy, the cost of counterfeiting is research addressing counterfeit purchasing from the
estimated to be up to $200 billion per year (Chaudhry et al., consumer’s perspective is still incipient, especially
2005). Considering the countries worldwide, almost 5 percent considering the antecedents of the construct “attitudes
of all products are counterfeit, according to the International toward counterfeits”, this study focuses on the demand side.
Anticounterfeiting Coalition (IACC, 2005) and the The aim is to propose and to test a model that deals with the
International Intellectual Property Institute (IIPI, 2003). main predictors of consumer attitudes toward counterfeits
A number of definitions have been used for product and their intentions to buy such products, integrating the
counterfeiting. In this paper, we use the one given by Cordell main findings existing in the literature.
et al. (1996) and also used by Chaudhry et al. (2005): “any The article is presented in five parts. First, a brief review of
unauthorized manufacturing of goods whose special the main antecedents and consequences of the consumer
characteristics are protected as intellectual property rights attitudes toward counterfeits were examined, resulting in a
conceptual model to be tested. Second, valid scales for the
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at constructs considered in the model were identified in the
www.emeraldinsight.com/0736-3761.htm literature. Third, the conceptual model was tested by means
of the structural equation modeling. Fourth, a discussion of
the main results is presented, comparing findings with
Journal of Consumer Marketing
24/1 (2007) 36– 47
q Emerald Group Publishing Limited [ISSN 0736-3761] The authors are thankful for the support provided by the Brazilian
[DOI 10.1108/07363760710720975] Funding Council for Research (Conselho Nacional de Pesquisa – CNPq.)

36
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

previous studies. Finally, the conclusions are presented, Internet shoppers and non-shoppers, Donthu and Garcia,
including the main contribution of the study and strategies 1999). In the context of counterfeits, Huang et al. (2004)
managers can use in order to reduce consumer attitudes found a significant inverse relationship between risk
toward counterfeits. averseness and attitude. Following these authors’ rationale,
we expected that:
Theoretical background and conceptual model H2. Consumers who are more (less) risk averse will have
unfavorable (favorable) attitude toward counterfeits.
Consumer attitude toward counterfeits
“Attitude” is “. . .a learned predisposition to behave in a As stated in H2, consumers believe that counterfeits are sold
consistently favorable or unfavorable manner with respect to a with lower prices and poorer guaranties. Because of this, the
given object” (Schiffman and Kanuk, 1997, p. 167). Indeed, risk variable is as important as the price-quality inference.
according to Bagozzi et al. (2002, p. 4), the most widely The concept of perceived risk more often used in marketing
accepted definition of attitude conceives of it as an evaluation, literature defines risk in terms of the consumer’s perceptions
for example: “. . .a psychological tendency that is expressed by of the uncertainty and adverse consequences of buying a
evaluating a particular entity with some degree of favor or product or service (Dowling and Staelin, 1994). Hence,
disfavor.” (Eagly and Chaiken, 1993, p. 1). consumers judge what are the chances that a problem might
Attitude is considered to be highly correlated with one’s occur and also what will be the negative consequences of such
intentions, which in turn is a reasonable predictor of behavior problem, and this judgment will influence every stage of the
(Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980). In these authors’ rationale (i.e. consumer decision-making process. As the nature of these
theory of reasoned action), not only the attitude one has problems vary, the risk might include different components,
toward an object will affect his/her intentions toward it, but such as performance, financial, safety, social, psychological,
also what influences one receives from his/her reference group and time/opportunity dimensions (Havlena and DeSarbo,
will be important, namely the subjective norms. In summary, 1991).
intentions to perform a behavior will be influenced by Albers-Miller (1999) found a significant role of the risk
individual and interpersonal level factors. factor on the purchasing of counterfeits. In this context, a
In the context of this study, consumer evaluation of consumer may consider that:
counterfeits will be an important predictor of his/her intention
.
the product will not perform as well as an original item
to buy a counterfeit, as well as how much agreement about and there will be no warranty from the seller;
this behavior he/she receives from his/her reference group. In
.
choosing a counterfeit will not bring the best possible
this way, what factors influence consumer evaluation of a monetary gain;
counterfeit becomes the focus of the investigation. Based on
.
the product may not be as safe the original one
the literature review, the main predictors are presented below.
. the selection of a counterfeit will affect in a negative way
how others perceive them; and
Price quality inference .
he/she will waste time, lose convenience or waste effort in
As the two main differences consumers perceive between a having to repeat a purchase. In this sense, it is
counterfeit and an original product are the lower prices and hypothesized that:
the poorer guaranties, price and risk constructs are likely to be
important factors related to attitude toward counterfeits H3. Consumers who perceive more (less) risk in
(Huang et al., 2004). In fact, previous studies have shown that counterfeits will have unfavorable (favorable) attitude
price difference is an important variable when choosing a toward counterfeits.
counterfeit (Cespedes et al., 1988; Cordell et al., 1996).
Inference of quality by the price level is a common belief Integrity
among consumers and an important factor in consumer Consumer purchase of a counterfeit is not a criminal act, but
behavior (Chapman and Wahlers, 1999). In this sense, as consumer participation in a counterfeit transaction
consumers’ tendency to believe that “high (low) price means supports illegal activity (i.e. counterfeit selling), consumer’s
high (low) quality” becomes even more important when there respect for lawfulness might explain how much engagement
is little information about the product quality or the consumer he/she will have in buying counterfeits. Indeed, research
is unable to judge product quality (Tellis and Gaeth, 1990). shows that consumers’ willingness to purchase counterfeit
As proposed also by Huang et al. (2004), considering that products is negatively related to attitudes toward lawfulness
counterfeits are usually sold at lower prices, the greater the (Cordell et al., 1996). In this sense, those consumers who
relationship price-quality for the consumer, the lower his/her have lower ethical standards are expected to feel less guilty
perception of quality for the counterfeits. For this reason, it is when buying a counterfeit (Ang et al., 2001). Rather, they
expected that: rationalize their behavior in a way to reduce the cognitive
H1. A consumer who more strongly believes in the price- dissonance of an unethical behavior. Using this rationale, we
quality inference has a more negative attitude toward expect that:
counterfeits. H4. Consumers who attribute more (less) integrity to
themselves will have unfavorable (favorable) attitude
Risk averseness and perceived risk in counterfeits purchasing toward counterfeits.
Risk averseness is defined as the propensity to avoid taking
risks and is generally considered a personality variable Personal gratification
(Bonoma and Johnston, 1979; Zinkhan and Karande, Personal gratification concerns the need for a sense of
1990). This psychological consumer trait is an important accomplishment, social recognition, and to enjoy the finer
characteristic for discriminating between buyers and non- things in life (Ang et al., 2001). There are conflicting results in
buyers of a product category, especially a risky one (e.g. this aspect in the literature because Bloch et al. (1993) suggest

37
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

that consumers choosing a counterfeit see themselves as less Method


well off financially, less confident, less successful and lower
status than counterfeit non-buyers; on the other hand, result A survey was conducted among consumers in two big
found by Ang et al. (2001) showed no significant influence of Brazilian cities. They were interviewed in the streets close to
personal gratification on consumer attitudes toward the points where counterfeited products were being sold.
Interviewers were trained by the authors in administrating the
counterfeits. Because of this, we do not hypothesize the
survey instrument and were instructed to include in the
direction of the relationship, but only that:
sample people with different profile, considering age, gender,
H5. Consumers’ sense of accomplishment will affect their
education and income. A question concerning whether
attitude toward counterfeits.
participant had already bought a counterfeited product was
also included in the instrument. Data collection was
Subjective norm performed both on week and weekend days. A total of 400
Subjective norm is a social factor referring to the perceived individuals answered the survey instrument and were used in
social pressure to perform or not to perform a given behavior the data analysis.
(Ajzen, 1991). Consumers may be informationally Based on the literature, the authors built the survey
susceptible, when expertise from others influences their instrument, using scales that were already validated in
choice (e.g. when one does not know the product category), previous research. Table I summarizes the items used for
and also normatively susceptible, when they are more each construct, as well as the authors used as reference.
interested in making a good impression to others (Bearden Participants answered these items using Likert scales varying
et al., 1989). Regarding counterfeits, friends and relatives may from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). Only
act as inhibitors or contributors to the consumption, the scale of behavioral intentions used a different format, with
depending on how much this behavior is approved by them. anchors varying from 1 (very unlikely) to 7 (very likely). In
Hence, it is expected that: this study it was not specified any counterfeited product in
H6. Consumers perceiving that their friends/relatives particular. Questions considered the expression
approve (do not approve) their behavior of buying a “counterfeited products” in general because the aim at this
counterfeit will have favorable (unfavorable) attitude moment was to assess consumer attitudes toward
toward counterfeits. counterfeited products overall.
After data collection, the analysis followed these steps:
Previous experience
.
descriptive statistics for the scale items and for the
Research has shown that counterfeits buyers are different demographic variables;
from non-buyers, with the former viewing such purchases as
.
missing values and outliers detection;
less risky, trusting stores that sell counterfeits and not viewing
.
linearity between the scale items;
this purchase as unethical (Ang et al., 2001). Hence in this
.
dimensionality using Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA);
.
reliability and validity of the scale items using internal
study, it is expected that:
consistency coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha), as well as
H7A. Consumers who have already bought (have never
composite reliability and average variance extracted as
bought) a counterfeit have more favorable
suggested in the measurement literature (Fornell and
(unfavorable) attitude toward counterfeits.
Larcker, 1981; Gerbing and Anderson, 1988); and
H7B. Consumers who have already bought (have never .
estimation of model parameters and interpretation.
bought) a counterfeit have more favorable
(unfavorable) behavioral intentions toward
counterfeits. Results
Descriptive analysis
The following profile was found among the participants: 230
Behavioral Intentions (57.6 percent) were female, 74 (18.5 percent) were 20 years
The link attitude-behavioral intentions has been extensively old or less, 86 (21.5 percent) were between 21 and 25 years,
examined in the marketing literature. According to the 47 (11.8 percent) between 26 and 30 years, 86 (21.6 percent)
Theory of Reasoned Action, attitude is positively correlated were between 30 and 40 years and 106 (26.6 percent) had 41
with behavioral intentions, which in turn is an antecedent of years or more. In terms of education, 208 (52 percent) had
the real behavior (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980). Indeed, already completed high school, followed by those who had
research has found support on this relationship (Kim and not, 120 (30 percent). Concerning personal income, the
Hunter, 1993). In the context of counterfeits, it is expected majority (171 or 43 percent) said they received monthly up to
that: R$500.00 (equivalent to US$230.00), followed by 149 (37
H8: Consumers with more favorable (unfavorable) percent) in the range between R$500.00 and R$1,000 (149 or
attitudes toward counterfeits will have more favorable 37 percent) and 79 (20 percent) with more than R$1,000 by
(unfavorable) behavioral intentions toward these month. Most of the participants (279 or 70 percent) affirmed
products. that they had already bought a counterfeit.
The scale items presented means varying from 2.15 (item “I
like shopping for gray market goods”) to 6.88 (item “I admire
responsible people”). In general, the scale means indicate that
Conceptual model
respondents manifested unfavorable attitudes toward
Based on the theoretical background just presented, Figure 1 counterfeited products and low behavioral intentions toward
shows the model proposed and submitted to empirical test. them.

38
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Figure 1 Conceptual model for attitude toward counterfeited products

Comparing the scores of the scales with the demographic parameters, when testing the conceptual model, and
variables, no significant differences were found between man compared the stability of the results.
and women, neither between different ranges of age,
education nor income. Dimensionality
Due to the data collection process (i.e. personal interview), The scales used to measure the constructs of the conceptual
questionnaires did not present missing values because model were submitted to the Exploratory Factor Analysis
interviewers were instructed to collect all the information (EFA) in order to test their dimensionality. Using Principal
from each participant. These data were submitted to the Axis Factoring as the extraction method and Oblimin as the
outliers analysis suggested by Hair et al. (1998, p. 69), namely rotation method and eigenvalues greater than one, seven
computing the Mahalanobis distance and excluding cases factors were extracted (see Table IV). A simulation with other
with significant high values. Using this procedure, 17 cases methods (e.g. Principal Components and Varimax rotation)
were identified and excluded from the data set. These cases produced similar results.
Results presented in Table IV suggested that dimensionality
were characterized as having lower means in the items
was not found in only one case, between the constructs of
referring to perceived risk and higher values in the propensity
attitude and behavioral intentions, which grouped together in
to buy counterfeited products, when compared to the rest of
one factor.
the sample. Analysis of variance showed significant differences
in these means ( p , 0.05). Reliability and validity
Linearity analysis was performed by checking the Scales were analyzed in terms of their reliability, by means of
correlations between all items used in the questionnaire. the internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) and composite
Considering items from the same construct, the higher value reliability (Fornell and Larcker, 1981), and also validity,
found was 0.76 (i.e. items bi1-bi2 from Table I), suggesting considering convergent and discriminant validity.
there was no problem of multicollinearity in the data (see Reliability, initially assessed by Cronbach’s alpha, was
Tables II and III). Asymmetry was found in all variables when computed in two stages: considering the original items of each
analyzing the normality graphs for each of them. scale and after the exclusion of those with lower correlation
Nevertheless, data were not submitted to transformations. with the scale. After this refinement, values ranged from 0.46
Rather, the authors used different methods for estimating the (risk averseness) to 0.87 (integrity).

39
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Table I Descriptive statistics


Scale Mean SD
Price quality inference (Lichtenstein et al., 1993, Huang et al., 2004)
PQ1 Generally speaking, the higher the price of a product, the higher the quality 3.86 2.08
PQ2 The price of a product is a good indicator of its quality 3.64 1.99
PQ3 You always have to pay a bit more for the best 4.80 2.06
Risk averseness (Huang et al., 2004; Donthu and Garcia, 1999)
RA1 When I buy something, I prefer not taking risks 5.44 1.83
RA2 I like to be sure the product is a good one before buying it 6.38 0.97
RA3 I don’t like to feel uncertainty when I buy something. 6.38 1.08
Attitude toward counterfeited products (Huang et al., 2004)
AT1 Considering price, I prefer gray market goods 3.13 2.26
AT2 I like shopping for gray market goods 2.15 1.60
AT3 Buying gray market goods generally benefits the consumer 2.76 1.98
AT4 There’s nothing wrong with purchasing gray market goods. 2.83 2.07
AT5 Generally speaking, buying gray market goods is a better choice 2.27 1.67
Subjective norm (Ajzen, 1991)
SN1 My relatives and friends approve my decision to buy counterfeited products 3.47 2.10
SN2 My relatives and friends think that I should buy counterfeited products 2.95 2.04
Behavioral intentions (Zeithaml et al., 1996)
Considering today, what are the chances that you. . .
BI1 . . .think about a counterfeited product as a choice when buying something 2.70 1.87
BI2 . . .buy a counterfeited product 2.68 1.92
BI3 ...recommend to friends and relatives that they buy a counterfeited product 2.20 1.68
BI4 . . .say favorable things about counterfeited products 2.19 1.61
Perceived risk (Dowling and Staelin, 1994)
PR1 The risk that I take when I buy a counterfeited product is high 5.93 1.65
PR2 There is high probability that the product doesn’t work 6.14 1.42
PR2 Spending money with a counterfeited product might be a bad decision 6.03 1.49
Integrity (Ang et al., 2001)
INT1 I consider honesty as an important quality for one’s character 6.86 0.40
INT2 I consider very important that people be polite 6.86 0.41
INT3 I admire responsible people 6.88 0.35
INT4 I like people that have self-control 6.47 1.06
Personal gratification (Ang et al., 2001)
PG I always attempt to have a sense of accomplishment 6.60 0.82

Another measure of reliability was also considered, namely and the variance of the item explained by the construct (i.e.
the composite reliability. Results here were similar to those squared multiple correlation, SMC).
ones presented above, with lower values in the same scales of Some constructs presented values of AVE below the lower
risk averseness (0.48). In all other scales the lower value bound of 0.50 suggested in the literature (e.g. Fornell and
found was 0.76, which is superior to the minimum of 0.70 Larcker, 1981), although an improvement was reached after
suggested in the literature (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). This excluding the items with lambdas , 0.50. These constructs
pattern of low values for the risk averseness scale is similar to include: risk averseness (AVE ¼ 0:32), similar to what was
the results presented by Huang et al. (2004), although they found by Huang et al. (2004), when AVE ¼ 0:39, and attitude
found values greater than the ones present here. (AVE ¼ 0:45).
The regression weights found for each item when running Discriminant Validity was performed by comparing the
each constructs as a separate model were used as indication of shared variance between each pair of construct with the
Convergent Validity. This analysis, together with the reliability average variance extracted in each one of the pair (Fornell and
results presented above, was used to purify the constructs’ Larcker, 1981). Absolute values of correlation among
measures. Items with regression weights (i.e. lambdas) lower constructs ranges from 0.025 (risk averseness-behavioral
than 0.50 were excluded from the scale and the Cronbach’s intentions) to 0.883 (attitude-behavioral intentions). Only in
alpha, composite reliability and average variance extracted this case, the squared correlation (78 percent), indicating the
(AVE) were recalculated (see Table V). In conjunction with shared variance, was higher then the AVE in attitude (45
the value of the lambdas, it was also checked its significance percent) and in behavioral intentions (61 percent), then

40
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Table II Correlations between items


PQ1 PQ2 PQ3 RA1 RA2 RA3 DU AT1 AT2 AT3 AT4 AT5 SN1
PQ1 1.00
PQ2 0.59 * * 1.00
PQ3 0.40 * * 0.36 * * 1.00
RA1 0.09 0.08 0.14 * * 1.00
RA2 0.02 0.06 0.12 * * 0.19 * * 1.00
RA3 0.07 0.03 0.10 0.18 * * 0.30 * * 1.00
DU 2 0.11 * 20.12 * 20.07 2 0.09 20.04 20.5 1.00
AT1 0.02 20.07 20.08 2 0.07 0.00 0.02 0.47 * * 1.00
AT2 0.02 0.05 20.09 2 0.10 20.04 20.01 0.36 * * 0.47 * * 1.00
AT3 0.09 0.02 0.00 0.00 20.09 0.00 0.27 * * 0.40 * * 0.48 * * 1.00
AT4 2 0.15 * * 20.13 * * 20.11 * 2 0.19 * * 20.07 20.22 * * 0.18 * * 0.14 * * 0.25 * * 0.12 * 1.00
AT5 0.02 0.01 20.12 * 2 0.13 * 20.09 20.16 * * 0.24 * * 0.36 * * 0.46 * * 0.52 * * 0.28 * * 1.00
SN1 2 0.13 * 20.10 20.13 * 2 0.04 0.02 20.06 0.25 * * 0.29 * * 0.20 * * 0.17 * * 0.19 * * 0.34 * * 1.00
SN2 2 0.07 20.07 20.13 * 0.04 20.02 0.03 0.16 * * 0.22 * * 0.18 * * 0.15 * * 0.18 * * 0.26 * * 0.59 * *
B11 0.02 20.03 20.01 0.01 20.03 0.03 0.38 * * 0.48 * * 0.47 * * 0.44 * * 0.17 * * 0.42 * * 0.35 * *
B12 0.06 0.05 20.02 2 0.03 0.00 0.04 0.42 * * 0.54 * * 0.49 * * 0.43 * * 0.18 * * 0.41 * * 0.32 * *
B13 2 0.02 20.06 20.14 * * 2 0.14 * * 20.09 20.06 0.35 * * 0.44 * * 0.51 * * 0.37 * * 0.24 * * 0.48 * * 0.26 * *
B14 0.05 0.01 20.13 * 2 0.17 * * 20.05 20.06 0.35 * * 0.47 * * 0.54 * * 0.45 * * 0.23 * * 0.55 * * 0.29 * *
PR1 0.00 20.01 0.11 * 0.14 * * 0.06 0.02 0.06 20.14 * * 20.20 * * 2 0.14 * * 2 0.15 * * 20.19 * * 2 0.06
PR2 0.07 20.01 0.07 0.04 0.04 0.03 2 0.10 * 20.19 * * 20.31 * * 2 0.26 * * 2 0.19 * * 20.34 * * 2 0.14 * *
PR3 0.02 20.01 0.04 0.08 20.02 0.05 2 0.20 * * 20.32 * * 20.34 * * 2 0.33 * * 2 0.16 * * 20.41 * * 2 0.24 * *
PG 0.00 0.03 0.13 * * 0.08 0.08 0.16 * * 2 0.04 0.07 20.04 0.02 2 0.04 20.12 * 0.01
INT1 2 0.03 20.04 0.09 0.18 * * 0.09 0.17 * * 2 0.14 * * 20.09 20.17 * * 2 0.08 2 0.09 20.29 * * 2 0.02
INT2 0.00 20.02 0.12 * 0.27 * * 0.13 * 0.14 * * 2 0.06 20.04 20.18 * * 2 0.11 * 2 0.11 * 20.28 * * 0.00
INT3 2 0.02 20.03 0.13 * 0.23 * * 0.18 * * 0.19 * * 2 0.05 20.05 20.16 * * 2 0.18 * * 2 0.10 * 20.32 * * 2 0.03
INT4 0.08 20.02 0.10 * 0.03 0.16 * * 0.05 0.01 0.01 0.02 2 0.03 2 0.03 20.09 2 0.13 * *
Notes: * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed); * * Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)

Table III Correlations between items


SN2 B11 B12 B13 B14 PR1 PR2 PR3 PG INT1 INT2 INT3 INT4
PQ1
PQ2
PQ3
RA1
RA2
RA3
DU
AT1
AT2
AT3
AT4
AT5
SN1
SN2 1.00
B11 0.29 * * 1.00
B12 0.23 * * 0.76 * * 1.00
B13 0.31 * * 0.48 * * 0.60 * * 1.00
B14 0.29 * * 0.52 * * 0.61 * * 0.66 * * 1.00
PR1 20.04 20.12 * 20.19 * * 20.22 * * 20.25 * * 1.00
PR2 20.13 * 20.34 * * 20.32 * * 20.33 * * 20.32 * * 20.43 * * 1.00
PR3 20.17 * * 20.40 * * 20.44 * * 20.40 * * 20.41 * * 20.32 * * 2 0.62 * * 1.00
PG 20.02 0.04 0.00 20.10 * 20.05 0.05 0.16 * * 0.12 * 1.00
INT1 20.09 0.01 20.06 20.26 * * 20.21 * * 0.09 0.13 * 0.09 0.33 * * 1.00
INT2 20.08 0.00 20.07 20.21 * * 20.26 * * 0.17 * * 0.11 * 0.11 * 0.23 * * 0.67 * * 1.00
INT3 20.11 * 20.01 20.07 20.19 * * 20.25 * * 0.17 * * 0.13 * * 0.13 * * 0.24 * * 0.69 * * 0.74 * * 1.00
INT4 0.00 20.08 20.08 0.02 20.04 0.08 0.06 0.10 0.08 0.14 * * 0.20 * * 0.20 * * 1.00
Notes: * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed); * * Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)

41
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Table IV Dimensionality test by exploratory factor analysis


Factors
Items Attitude/behavioral intentions Integrity Price-quality relation Perceived risk Subjective norm Risk aversion
BI2 0.827 0.077 0.035 20.025 2 0.017 2 0.089
BI1 0.722 0.158 0.013 20.039 2 0.103 2 0.051
BI4 0.714 20.129 0.008 20.026 2 0.040 0.051
AT1 0.680 0.042 2 0.059 0.068 2 0.026 2 0.044
AT2 0.673 20.056 0.005 20.050 0.063 0.021
BI3 0.644 20.110 2 0.070 20.047 2 0.052 0.036
AT3 0.596 20.015 0.079 20.039 0.043 0.035
AT5 0.436 20.167 0.062 20.123 2 0.169 0.180
INT2 2 0.009 0.863 0.021 0.008 2 0.013 0.031
INT3 0.022 0.850 2 0.027 0.031 0.038 2 0.037
INT1 2 0.001 0.793 2 0.025 20.004 0.039 0.021
PQ1 0.108 20.055 0.796 0.067 0.017 0.037
PQ2 2 0.037 20.059 0.755 20.067 2 0.026 0.039
PQ3 2 0.017 0.087 0.490 0.015 0.041 2 0.066
PR2 0.100 20.049 0.001 0.986 0.019 0.049
PR3 2 0.204 20.044 2 0.025 0.586 0.063 0.019
PR1 2 0.023 0.071 0.017 0.453 2 0.059 2 0.023
SN1 0.031 0.052 2 0.020 0.001 2 0.788 0.058
SN2 0.004 20.098 2 0.013 0.021 2 0.733 2 0.066
RA3 0.112 20.043 2 0.060 0.008 0.051 2 0.679
RA2 2 0.023 0.010 0.004 20.034 2 0.046 2 0.427
RA1 2 0.057 0.152 0.103 0.005 2 0.077 2 0.293
AT4 0.123 0.036 2 0.110 20.097 2 0.118 0.291
Variance explained 20.1 9.3 6.3 3.9 2.8 2.4
Notes: Total variance explained: 50.54%; item “int4” was excluded because it load alone as a different factor; Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling
Adequacy ¼ 0:833; Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity: x2 ¼ 3:351; df ¼ 253; p , 0.000

failing to suggest discriminant validity (see results in Table ML, the most frequently used method (Thompson, 2002), are
VI). Using another criteria (Bagozzi and Philips, 1982) and presented (see Table VII).
constraining this correlation to unity produced a significant Considering the antecedents of attitudes, significant paths
change in the goodness-of-fit statistic (Dx2 ¼ 25:389; df ¼ 1; were found for perceived risk ( p , 0.000), integrity ( p ,
p , 0.000). We used as additional evidence of discriminant 0.002), personal gratification ( p , 0.015), subjective norm ( p
validity between these constructs further evidence obtained , 0.000) and the dummy ( p , 0.000), supporting H3, H4,
when testing the full model (i.e. significant predictors of H5, H6, H7A respectively. Only risk averseness was a non-
attitude were not significant predictors of behavioral significant antecedent ( p , 0.933), failing to support H2.
intentions), which will be presented ahead. Contrary to expectations, however, results showed a positive
relationship between price-quality and attitude ( p , 0.009),
Estimation of model parameters failing to support H1.
After the process of scale refinement presented above, the Results revealed also that attitude toward counterfeits is
conceptual model with the remaining indicators (see Figure 1) most significantly affected by the following constructs:
was submitted to test. Results using both the maximum- perceived risk (b ¼ 20:487); dummy “have bought a
likelihood (ML) method and the generalized least squares counterfeit before or not” (b ¼ 0:347), subjective norm
(GLS) show that the fit indexes approximate acceptable levels (b ¼ 0:245), integrity (b ¼ 0:157), price-quality inference
using ML (i.e. GFI ¼ 0:898; AGFI ¼ 0:854; NFI ¼ 0:874; (b ¼ 0:149) and personal gratification (b ¼ 0:109). In this
CFI ¼ 0:917; PACFI ¼ 0:703; RMSEA ¼ 0:064) and using order, these variables are the most important for explaining
GLS (i.e. GFI ¼ 0:928; AGFI ¼ 0:897; NFI ¼ 0:666; consumer attitude toward counterfeited products.
CFI ¼ 0:806; PACFI ¼ 0:622; RMSEA ¼ 0:045), When assessing the variables influencing behavioral
considering the measurement literature (e.g., Byrne, 2001). intentions, it was found that attitude was significant ( p ,
In both methods, however, chi-square parameter was 0.000; b ¼ 0:891), supporting H8; but the dummy “have
significant ( p , 0.000). Since chi-square is sensitive to bought a counterfeit before or not” was not ( p , 0.493;
sample size, relative chi-square (x2/df) is commonly suggested b ¼ 0:033), not supporting H7B. This result is interesting
in the measurement literature. Using ML method, a value of because it shows a mediating effect of attitude in the
2.575 was found for the relative chi-square, which is in the relationship between the dummy, a proxy of consumer
acceptable level of 2 or 3 to 1 (Arbuckle, 1997). previous experience with counterfeits, and intentions to buy a
Because similar results were also found for the coefficients counterfeit. In other words, experience influences attitudes,
when using both ML and GLS, only those produced by the which in turn influences behavior.

42
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Table V Summary of constructs’ measures


Scale
Cronbach’s alpha (a)
Composite reliability (CR) Standardized Squared multiple
Average variance extracted (AVE) Items regression weights t p correlations (SMC)
Price-quality relation pq1 0.810 0.657
a 5 0:71; CR 5 0:72; AVE 5 0:48 pq2 0.726 8.162 0.000 0.527
a
a 5 0:74; CR 5 0:76; AVE 5 0:62 pq3 * 0.497 7.519 0.000 0.247
Attitude toward counterfeit at1 0.583 0.340
a 5 0:70; CR 5 0:74 AVE 5 0:38 at2 0.712 9.332 0.000 0.507
a
a 5 0:75; CR 5 0:76; AVE 5 0:45 at3 0.692 9.215 0.000 0.478
at4 * 0.301 4.911 0.000 0.091
at5 0.686 9.179 0.000 0.471
Behavioral intentions bi1 0.803 0.644
a 5 0:85; CR 5 0:86; AVE 5 0:61 bi2 0.911 18.168 0.000 0.830
bi3 0.676 13.711 0.000 0.457
bi4 0.700 14.302 0.000 0.490
Perceived risk pr1 * 0.471 0.222
a 5 0:71; CR 5 0:74; AVE 5 0:51 pr2 0.918 6.876 0.000 0.843
a
a 5 0:76; CR 5 0:77; AVE 5 0:63 pr3 0.676 8.282 0.000 0.458
Integrity int1 0.789 0.622
a 5 0:53; CR 5 0:80; AVE 5 0:54 int2 0.849 17.237 0.000 0.721
a
a 5 0:87; CR 5 0:88; AVE 5 0:70 int3 0.877 17.497 0.000 0.770
int4 * 0.221 4.094 0.000 0.049
Subjective norm sn1 0.680
a 5 0:74; CR 5 0:75; AVE 5 0:60 sn2 0.860 7.58 0.000 0.463
Risk averseness ra1 * 0.339 0.115
a 5 0:39; CR 5 0:48; AVE 5 0:24 ra2 0.558 3.358 0.001 0.311
a
a 5 0:46; CR 5 0:48; AVE 5 0:32 ra3 0.540 3.432 0.001 0.292
Notes: * Excluded due to low weights (, 0.5); a values for the purified scale; Fit statistics after purification process (measurement model):
chi  square ¼ 403:50; df ¼ 155; p , 0.000; GFI ¼ 0:901; AGFI ¼ 0:853; CFI ¼ 0:919; RMSEA ¼ 0:065

In order to test which of the exogenous construct from behavioral intentions (M havebought ¼ 0:29,
Figure 1 would have a significant influence on behavioral M haveneverbought ¼ 20:68, p , 0.000), tend to disagree with
intentions, an alternative model was also tested, in which the notion that “high price means high quality”
constructs are modeled to influence both attitudes and (M havebought ¼ 20:08, M haveneverbought ¼ 0:20, p , 0.012),
behavioral intentions. As a result, it was found that most agree more on the question that their friends and relatives
variables significantly affect attitudes but not behavioral approve their decision to buy counterfeits (M havebought ¼ 0:15,
intentions (i.e. price-quality inference, subjective norm, M haveneverbought ¼ 20:35, p , 0.000), perceive less risk in the
perceived risk, integrity, dummy and personal gratification), counterfeit (M havebought ¼ 20:11, M haveneverbought ¼ 0:26, p ,
and only one did not affect either of them (i.e. risk 0.001) and have lower scores on integrity
averseness). This finding can be interpreted as an indication (M havebought ¼ 20:06, M haveneverbought ¼ 0:14, p , 0.065).
of parsimony in the original conceptualized model and it also These results support H7.
reinforces the mediating role of attitudes in the relationship
between the key reviewed antecedents and the behavioral
intentions. It is also an interesting result because, considering Discussion
the high amount of shared variance between attitudes and Although gray market has grown worldwide, research from a
behavioral intentions, one would expect that constructs with a demand perspective remains scarce as emphasized by Huang
significant effect on attitudes would also have a significant et al. (2004). In order to fill this void, this paper aimed to
effect on behavioral intentions. This not being the case investigate the key antecedents of consumer attitudes toward
suggests the discriminant validity between attitudes and counterfeits, as well as the influence of this attitude on the
behavioral intentions. behavioral intentions toward these products. Based on recent
When comparing consumers who have bought a counterfeit marketing literature, this article integrates two conceptual
with those who affirmed that have never bought a counterfeit, models, one proposed and tested by Huang et al. (2004), who
it was found that the former have more favorable attitudes considered price-consciousness, price-quality inference and
(M havebought ¼ 0:28, M haveneverbought ¼ 20:66, p , 0.000) and risk averseness as antecedents of consumer attitudes, and the

43
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Table VI Discriminant validity by squared correlations and AVE are similar in content when comparing these two models:
price-consciousness and value consciousness, meaning the
PQ RA SN PR IN AT BI concern for paying lower prices, subject to some quality
Price-quality (PQ) 0.624 constraint (Lichtenstein et al., 1993). In their study, Ang et al.
Risk aversion (RA) 0.011 0.322 (2001) found that consumers who were more value conscious
Subjective norm (SN) 0.023 0.002 0.601 had a more favorable attitude towards piracy than less value
Perceived risk (PR) 0.002 0.004 0.084 0.63 conscious consumers. On the other hand, in Huang et al.
(2004)’s study, the price-consciousness construct was not
Integrity (IN) 0.001 0.102 0.003 0.029 0.705
significant. In the present research, this construct was not
Attitude (AT) 0.003 0.012 0.188 0.361 0.088 0.447
included. It remains, however, as an interesting relationship to
Behavioral intentions (BI) 0.002 0.001 0.216 0.356 0.030 0.780 0.611
be tested in future research. In the sense of combination of
Notes: Numbers in italic are the AVE for the construct; for scales with only factors from both models, subjective norm was considered as
two items (e.g. SN); the loadings used for computing AVE were those representing the social influence. The authors also included a
obtained when modeling all constructs correlated with each other new construct in the model in order to better explain the risk
(measurement model) component, which was the risk consumers perceive when they
buy a counterfeit. This variable was not considered in either
of the two models presented above. Perceived risk is more
Table VII Parameter estimation specific than risk averseness because the former deals with
how much risk consumer perceive when s/he buys a
counterfeit while the latter indicates only the consumer
Standardized Critical propensity to take risks overall.
Regression Standard weights ratios Although some constructs had a relative small average
Relations weights errors (b) (t) p variance extracted (e.g., risk averseness) and some of the fit
AT ˆ PQ 0.115 0.044 0.149 2.604 0.009 indexes obtained in the model only approximate the desired
AT ˆ RA 0.013 0.15 0.005 0.084 0.933 level (e.g. GFI should be higher than 0.9), 82.5 percent of the
AT ˆ SN 0.2 0.049 0.245 4.114 0.000 variance in the behavioral intentions and 64 percent of the
variance in the attitudes could be explained by the variables in
AT ˆ PR 2 0.582 0.08 2 0.487 27.262 0.000
the model (i.e. considering Squared Multiple Correlations,
AT ˆ IN 2 0.733 0.239 2 0.157 23.067 0.002
SMC for these constructs). These values are rather
AT ˆ DU 1.126 0.168 0.347 6.722 0.000
considerable, especially considering previous research (e.g.,
AT ˆ PG 0.208 0.085 0.109 2.435 0.015
in the study by Ang et al., 2001, adjusted R2 was equal to 0.44
BI ˆ AT 0.857 0.079 0.891 10.812 0.000 for the model predicting purchase intentions).
BI ˆ DU 0.103 0.15 0.033 0.685 0.493 Results from this extended model revealed that perceived
pq2 ˆ PQ 0.667 0.169 0.643 3.937 0.000 risk was the most important variable to predict consumer
pq1 ˆ PQ 1 0.915 attitude toward counterfeits, followed by the fact that he/she
ra3 ˆ RA 1 0.648 have bought a counterfeit before or not, subjective norm,
ra2 ˆ RA 0.673 0.219 0.465 3.073 0.002 integrity, price-quality inference and personal gratification.
sn2 ˆ SN 0.752 0.102 0.672 7.372 0.000 Consumers who perceived more risk in the counterfeits had
sn1 ˆ SN 1 0.871 unfavorable attitudes toward them, in line with previous
pr3 ˆ PR 1 0.858 research dealing with perceived risk (e.g. Dowling and Staelin,
pr2 ˆ PR 0.803 0.076 0.724 10.606 0.000 1994). Those consumers who had already bought a
int2 ˆ IN 0.985 0.056 0.843 17.444 0.000 counterfeit had favorable attitude toward it. Those
int1 ˆ IN 1 0.798 consumers whose relative and friends approve their decision
int3 ˆ IN 0.96 0.054 0.876 17.863 0.000 to buy counterfeits have more favorable attitudes, a result
at1 ˆ AT 1 0.656 convergent with the predictions of the Theory of Planned
at2 ˆ AT 0.741 0.064 0.694 11.558 0.000 Behavior (Ajzen, 1991). Consumers who considered
at3 ˆ AT 0.825 0.078 0.627 10.611 0.000 important values as honesty, politeness and responsibility
at5 ˆ AT 0.743 0.067 0.664 11.133 0.000 tended to have a negative attitude toward counterfeits, in
bi1 ˆ BI 1 0.773 accordance with previous research (e.g. Ang et al., 2001,
bi2 ˆ BI 1.119 0.065 0.844 17.138 0.000 Cordell et al., 1996), but those who seek to have a sense of
bi3 ˆ BI 0.837 0.057 0.731 14.574 0.000
accomplishment have positive attitudes, different from Ang
et al.’s study, which found a positive but non significant effect.
bi4 ˆ BI 0.847 0.055 0.772 15.511 0.000
Finally, those consumers who considered the price as an
Notes: SMC: attitude ¼ 0:642; behavioral intentions ¼ 0:825 indication of quality had more favorable attitudes toward
counterfeits, which is contrary to the results found by Huang
et al. (2004) and the prediction made in H1. In this case of the
other proposed by Ang et al. (2001), who considered social price-quality inference, it was hypothesized that consumers
factors (i.e. informative susceptibility and normative considering “high price, high quality” and “low price, low
susceptibility) and personality factors (i.e. value quality” would have unfavorable attitudes toward counterfeits
consciousness, integrity and personal gratification) as because of its inferior price. Results suggested that this was
antecedents of consumer attitudes. not the case, or at least that consumers who are used to buy
The study presented here considered as antecedents a counterfeits may apply this rule to the gray market in itself
combination of these factors presented above. Two constructs (i.e. those counterfeits of inferior price are perceived as low

44
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

quality and those ones of higher prices as higher quality). This Conclusions and suggestions
can be an important alternative explanation especially if one
considers that 70 percent of the respondents in this study had This research contributes to the existing literature by
already bought a counterfeit. extending and testing the key antecedents of the consumer
Only the construct risk averseness did not have a significant attitudes toward counterfeits. It also identifies the relative
influence on attitudes, which is different from the result importance of each antecedent in predicting attitudes. It is
presented by Huang et al. (2004). This finding is interesting if argued that these attitudes act as mediator in the relation
one considers that perceived risk was the most important between the constructs considered and the behavioral
predictor. A possible explanation is the difference in meaning intentions. Another contribution is related to the practical
between them and also the easiness with each respondents implications of this paper, when it considers the strategies
probably related the perceived risk items to the context of the managers can use for dealing with the problem of consumers
research, maybe having difficult to do so in the risk averseness loosing loyalty in the brands and turning into counterfeits.
items. This difference, however, should be considered in As suggestions for future research, one could test the model
future investigations. presented here in different product categories (e.g. CDs,
Because previous research reviewed did not mention the DVDs, clothes, toys etc) and check for possible differences. It
direct effect of the antecedents of attitudes on purchase/ is also recommended that new important variables be
behavioral intentions, one important contribution of this paper included in the model, which can be done by searching for
is also to show that the significant predictors of attitudes moderators and boundary conditions. This might be the case
presented above do not have a direct influence on consumer’s of consumer involvement with the product, for instance. It
behavioral intentions, even though attitudes and behavioral should be expected that when consumers are more involved
intentions are highly correlated in this study. This is an with the product, he/she should be more worried about the
evidence of the mediator role of attitude: the key constructs buying decision and have a higher risk aversion.
affect attitudes, which in turn affect behavioral intentions.
The relative importance of these predictors can also References
contribute to the policy makers or managers of international
brands who should use the perceived risk as the main appeal Ajzen, I. (1991), “The theory of planned behavior”,
in the messages intended to discourage consumption of Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
counterfeits. Another important point is that those consumers Vol. 50 No. 2, pp. 179-211.
who have bought a counterfeit have more favorable attitudes Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (1980), Understanding Attitudes and
when compared to those who have not. This is a real threat Predicting Social Behavior, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs,
for the original brands, because once consumers experiment NJ.
the counterfeit, they tend to have a favorable attitude and then Albers-Miller, N.D. (1999), “Consumer misbehavior: why
have a positive behavioral intentions. The results suggested, people buy illicit goods”, Journal of Consumer Marketing,
however, that this experience does not have a direct effect on Vol. 16 No. 3, pp. 273-87.
behavioral intentions. In this way, it is possible to influence Ang, S.H., Cheng, P.S., Lim, E.A.C. and Tambyah, S.K.
attitudes consumers have toward counterfeits through other (2001), “Spot the difference: consumer responses towards
variables, for example, by influencing the (negative) perceived counterfeits”, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 18 No. 3,
social acceptance consumer will have when buying a pp. 219-35.
counterfeit. This would be the practical implication of the Arbuckle, J.L. (1997), AMOS User’s Guide: Version 3.6, SPSS,
significant effect of the construct subjective norm. Another Chicago, IL.
alternative could be trying to influence consumer personality, Bagozzi, R. and Philips, L.W. (1982), “Representing and
such as integrity, although this one is more difficult to change, testing organizational theories: a holistic construal”,
encouraging consumers to consider values as responsibility Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 27 No. 3, pp. 459-90.
and honesty in their life. It was found that consumers who Bagozzi, R.P., Gürhan-Canli, Z. and Priester, J.R. (2002), The
attempt to have a sense of accomplishment had more Social Psychology of Consumer Behavior, Open University
favorable attitudes toward counterfeits. These consumers Press, Buckingham.
may consume counterfeits as an opportunity to experiment a Bearden, W.O., Netemeyer, R.G. and Teel, J.E. (1989),
product innovation, which can be more salient for electronic “Measurement of consumer susceptibility to interpersonal
products. This could be even more significant for low-income influence”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15 No. 4,
consumers, which can be investigated in future research. pp. 473-81.
In terms of demographic variables, results are convergent Byrne, B.M. (2001), Structural Equation Modeling With
with previous research when finding that consumers of AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming,
counterfeits have more favorable attitudes and behavioral Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.
intentions (Ang et al., 2001). This might be a problem for Bloch, P.H., Bush, R.F. and Campbell, L. (1993),
those trying to reduce the counterfeit consumption, because if “Consumer ‘Accomplices’ in product counterfeiting;
these consumers are satisfied with the performance of the a demand side investigation”, Journal of Consumer
pirated product, there will be more resistance for attitude Marketing, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 27-36.
change. It was also found that these consumers: Bonoma, T.V. and Johnston, W.J. (1979), “Decision making
.
do not use price as a reference of quality; under uncertainty: a direct measurement approach”,
.
consider that the reference groups approve their decision Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 6 No. 2, pp. 177-91.
to buy counterfeits, which can be viewed as a strategy to Chapman, J. and Wahlers, A. (1999), “revision and empirical
reduce cognitive dissonance; and test of the extended price-perceived quality model”, Journal
.
are not afraid that the counterfeit will not work properly. of Marketing Theory and Practice, Vol. 7 No. 3, pp. 53-64.

45
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

Cespedes, F.V., Corey, E.R. and Rangan, V.K. (1988), “Gray Zeithaml, V.A., Parasuraman, A. and Berry, L.L. (1996),
markets: causes and cures”, Harvard Business Review, “The behavioral consequences of service quality”, Journal
Vol. 66 No. 4, pp. 75-83. of Marketing, Vol. 60, April, pp. 31-43.
Chaudhry, P., Cordell, V. and Zimmerman, A. (2005), Zinkhan, G.M. and Karande, K.W. (1990), “Cultural and
“Modeling anti-counterfeiting strategies in response to gender differences in risk-taking behavior among American
protecting intellectual property rights in a global and Spanish decision markers”, The Journal of Social
environment”, Marketing Review, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 59-72. Psychology, Vol. 131 No. 5, pp. 741-2.
Cordell, V., Wongtada, N. and Kieschnick, R.L. Jr (1996),
“Counterfeit purchase intentions: role of lawfulness Further reading
attitudes and product traits as determinants”, Journal of
Blackwell, R.D., Miniard, P.W. and Engel, J.F. (1995),
Business Research, Vol. 35 No. 1, pp. 41-53.
Consumer Behavior, The Dryden Press, Orlando, FL.
Donthu, N. and Garcia, A. (1999), “The Internet shopper”,
Hubbard, R. and Armstrong, J.S. (1994), “Replications and
Journal of Advertising Research, Vol. 39 No. 3, pp. 52-8.
Extensions in marketing: rarely published but quite
Dowling, G.R. and Staelin, R. (1994), “A model of perceived
contrary”, International Journal of Research in Marketing,
risk and intended risk-handling activity”, Journal of
Vol. 11, pp. 233-48.
Consumer Research, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 119-34.
Eagly, A.H. and Chaiken, S. (1993), The Psychology of
Attitudes, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Fort Worth, TX. About the authors
Fornell, C. and Larcker, D.F. (1981), “Evaluating structural Celso Augusto de Matos is a marketing doctoral candidate at
equation models with unobservable variables and the School of Management at the Federal University of Rio
measurement error”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 18 Grande do Sul, Brazil (PPGA-EA-UFRGS). His research
No. 1, pp. 39-50. interests are in the areas of attitude formation and change,
Gerbing, D.W. and Anderson, J.C. (1988), “Un updated consumer behavior in services, and marketing research. His
paradigm for scale development incorporating research has been published in the International Journal of
unidimensionality and its assessment”, Journal of Consumer Studies, Brazilian Administration Review, ACR
Marketing Research, Vol. 25, May, pp. 186-92. conference and in a number of Brazilian journals and
Hair, J.F. Jr, Anderson, R.E., Tatham, W. and Black, W.C. proceedings. He is the corresponding author and can be
(1998), Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th ed., Prentice Hall, contacted at: celsomatos@yahoo.com.br
Upper Saddle River, NJ. Cristiana Trindade Ituassu holds a master’s degree from the
Havlena, W.J. and DeSarbo, W.S. (1991), “On the Federal University of Minas Gerais, Brazil (CEPEAD-
measurement of perceived consumer risk”, Decision UFMG). Her research interests are in the areas of country
Sciences, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 927-39. image, consumer behavior, and advertising. Her research has
Huang, J.H., Lee, B.C.Y. and Ho, S.H. (2004), “Consumer been published in a number of Brazilian proceedings.
attitude toward gray market goods”, International Marketing Dr Carlos Alberto Vargas Rossi is a professor at the School
Review, Vol. 21 No. 6, pp. 598-614. of Management at the Federal University of Rio Grande do
International Anticounterfeiting Coalition (2005), “Facts on Sul, Brazil (PPGA-EA-UFRGS). His research interests are in
fakes”, available at: www.iacc.org/Facts.html (accessed the areas of strategic marketing, consumer behavior, and
November 30, 2005). marketing theory. His research has been published in the
International Intellectual Property Institute (2003), International Journal of Consumer Studies, Brazilian
“Counterfeit goods and the public’s health and safety”, Administration Review, ACR conference and in a number of
Brazilian journals and proceedings.
available at: www.iacc.org/IIPI.pdf (accessed November 30,
2005).
Kim, M.S. and Hunter, J.E. (1993), “Relationships among Executive summary and implications for
attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior: a meta- managers and executives
analysis of past research, Part 2”, Communication Research,
This summary has been provided to allow managers and executives
Vol. 20 No. 3, pp. 331-64.
a rapid appreciation of the content of this article in toto to take
Lichtenstein, D.R., Ridgway, N.M. and Netemeyer, R.G.
advantage of the more comprehensive description of the research
(1993), “Price perceptions and consumer shopping
undertaken and its results to get the full benefit of the material
behavior: a field study”, Journal of Marketing Research, present.
Vol. 30 No. 2, pp. 234-45.
Schiffman, L.G. and Kanuk, L.L. (1997), Consumer Behavior, There is plenty of evidence to prove that the sale of
8th ed., Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. counterfeit goods is a growing cause for concern. Around 5
Tellis, G.J. and Gaeth, G.J. (1990), “Best value, price- percent of products worldwide are believed to be fake, while
seeking, and price aversion: the impact of information and the problem costs the US economy over $200 billion every
learning on consumer choices”, Journal of Marketing, year. Despite this level of concern, scant research exists into
Vol. 54, April, pp. 34-45. the purchasing of pirated goods – particularly from a
Thompson, B. (2002), “Ten commandments of structural consumer perspective.
equation modeling”, in Grimm, L.G. and Yarnold, P.R.
(Eds), Reading and Understanding More Multivariate Influential factors
Statistics, American Psychological Association, Previous studies have, however, identified several factors that
Washington, DC. may influence consumer intention towards counterfeit

46
Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits Journal of Consumer Marketing
Celso Augusto de Matos et al. Volume 24 · Number 1 · 2007 · 36 –47

products. The factors operate at individual and interpersonal particularly given the high number of respondents who had
level and include: previously bought such products.
. Price-quality correlation. A belief that many consumers The assumption that higher levels of risk aversion would
equate product quality from its price and the level of have a similar effect was also unproven. De Matos et al.
guarantee offered. Specifically, many theorists believe that express particular surprise at the latter findings given that
the typically lower cost of pirated goods leads the perceived risk was discovered to be the most influential of all
consumer to assume lower quality also. factors considered. Prior purchase was next, followed by
.
Risk aversion and perceived risk. Research has proven that subjective norms, integrity, price-quality inference and
some consumers possess a personality trait making them personal gratification. Importantly, evidence suggests that
averse to risk taking and has shown this trait to be a these factors influence consumer attitude towards pirated
significant factor in whether or not the consumer will goods but do not directly influence purchase intention. The
make purchases deemed risky. Buying online is one authors note the complexity of this issue given that attitude
example that many people regard as involving risk. Some was found to influence intention.
consumers perceive more specific risks that can relate not Most other findings were as anticipated and concurred with
only to price and quality but also to safety, finance, previous studies. For instance, consumers who valued honesty
performance, social or psychological. and responsibility generally display a negative attitude towards
.
Integrity. The ethical standards of the consumer. counterfeits. Likewise, there was also considerable support for
.
Subjective norm. How pressure from relatives and friends the theory that peer approval or disapproval affects consumer
can lead individuals to adopt certain attitudes and attitude.
behaviors. The results of the survey enabled de Matos et al. to build a
.
Previous experience. Whether or not the consumer has profile of a consumer most likely to buy fake goods. Such a
previously bought pirated goods. consumer would probably have a more favorable attitude and
.
Personal fulfillment. How individuals perceive themselves. purchase intention towards counterfeits, tend to disagree with
Earlier studies have provided conflicting evidence in the notion that higher price means better quality, perceive less
regard to this factor. Some researchers found little risk, places lower value on integrity. In the survey itself, these
evidence of links between personal fulfillment and respondents also revealed higher levels of agreement in
counterfeit goods, while others discovered that those relation to the question about peer approval of their
buying such products regarded themselves as less purchases.
confident, being lower achievers, having less money and
being of inferior standing than non-buyers. Implications and suggestions for future research
.
Behavioral intentions. Other studies have indicated strong The authors believe that the study can aid marketers in the
links between attitude and intention, and that intention is battle to dissuade consumers from purchasing counterfeit
a reliable predictor of actual behavior. products. Given that perceived risk was most influential, they
Two existing conceptual models each considered the impact suggest that the most effective results may be possible if this
of some of the above factors. De Matos et al. extend this work factor is at the core of advertising campaigns. However, if the
by combining the models and investigating a range of consumer has previously bought pirated goods the challenge
hypotheses relating to all the factors. becomes more difficult and is compounded further when he
or she is satisfied with these purchases. In this instance, de
Survey and findings Matos et al. propose that trying to influence the social
The authors conducted a survey in areas of two large Brazilian acceptance factor may be the best tactic to adopt. Attempting
cities where counterfeit goods were on sale. Interviews were to influence integrity is another option, although the authors
carried out in the streets and the sample obtained included acknowledge the difficulties inherent in persuading
respondents with different age, gender, income and education individuals to reflect on the values they hold.
profiles. In response to a question about previous purchases, In view of the inconclusiveness, further investigation of the
70 percent of the 400 surveyed admitted they had already price-quality link could prove fruitful, as might additional
bought fake goods. For other questions, respondents exploration into the relationship between risk averseness and
answered on a scale ranging from completely agree to perceived risk. The authors also point out that the survey
completely disagree – apart from behavioral intentions, where focused on general rather than specific counterfeit products
answers ranged from very likely to very unlikely. The and suggest that consideration of different product categories
responses indicated that no real importance could be may be informative. For example, they speculate that lower
attached to gender, age, income level or education. income consumers who value personal accomplishment may
Contrary to expectation, the study did not provide experience innovation through the purchase of counterfeit
sufficient evidence to imply that consumers who firmly electrical or other hi-tech products. Inclusion of other
believe in the price-quality link will have a more negative variables such as consumer involvement with the product
attitude towards counterfeit products. Indeed, many may likewise prove to be significant.
participants who considered that quality is indicated by
price revealed more rather than less favorable attitudes. The (A précis of the article “Consumer attitudes toward counterfeits:
authors speculate that consumers might in fact apply the a review and extension”. Supplied by Marketing Consultants
price-quality inference within the counterfeit market itself – for Emerald.)

To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com


Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints

47

View publication stats

You might also like