Yokoyama 2013

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

Unconscious processing of direct gaze: Evidence from an ERP study


Takemasa Yokoyama a,b,n, Yasuki Noguchi a, Shinichi Kita a
a
Departiment of Psychology, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan
b
Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Humans detect faces with direct gaze more rapidly than they do faces with averted gaze. Evidence
Received 31 July 2012 suggests that the visual information of faces with direct gaze reaches conscious awareness faster than
Received in revised form that of faces with averted gaze. This suggests that faces with direct gaze are effectively processed in the
20 March 2013
brain before they reach conscious awareness; however, it is unclear how the unconscious perception of
Accepted 1 April 2013
faces with direct gaze is processed in the brain. To address this unanswered question, we recorded event-
Available online 17 April 2013
related potentials while observers viewed faces with direct or averted gaze that were either visible or
Keywords: rendered invisible during continuous flash suppression. We observed that invisible faces with direct gaze
Direct gaze elicited significantly larger negative deflections than did invisible faces with averted gaze at 200, 250,
Interocular suppression
and 350 ms over the parietofrontal electrodes, whereas we did not observe such effects when facial
Unconscious
images were visible. Our results suggest that the visual information of faces with direct gaze is preferen-
Event-related potentials
Conscious awareness tially processed in the brain when they are presented unconsciously.
& 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction they do not (Friesen & Kingstone, 1998; Langton, Watt, & Bruce,
2000; Vecera & Rizzo, 2006; Yokoyama, Noguchi, & Kita, 2012).
Eye gaze of a conspecific conveys rich signals to an observer. Therefore, faces with direct gaze capture the attention of obser-
Thus, gaze plays a critical role in visual processing of a face. vers, whereas faces with averted gaze shift the observer's attention
Research on gaze perception deals mainly with two types of gaze: to the direction indicated by the averted gaze.
averted gaze, which is not directed at the observer, and direct Because the processing of faces with direct and averted gaze
gaze, which is directed at the observer. These two types of gaze are functionally different, the neural processing of these two types
have different effects on the perception and cognition of observers. of gaze is dissociated in the brain. Neuroimaging studies indicate
This functional dissociation between faces with direct and that the processing of faces with direct gaze is more closely
averted gaze is typically seen in their influences on attention of associated with the fusiform gyrus, right superior temporal sulcus
observers. It is well known that faces with direct gaze give more (STS), medial prefrontal cortex, and amygdala than the processing
salient visual signals to observers than do faces with averted gaze; of faces with averted gaze (Calder et al., 2002; George, Driver, &
thus, faces with direct gaze are more likely to capture the spatial Dolan, 2001; Kampe, Frith, & Frith, 2003; Kawashima et al., 1999;
attention of observers compared with averted gaze (Senju & Pageler et al., 2003; Pelphrey, Viola, & McCarthy, 2004; Sato,
Johnson, 2009; Yokoyama, Ishibashi, Hongoh, & Kita, 2011). In a Yoshikawa, Kochiyama, & Matsumura, 2004; Wicker, Perrett,
visual search task, faces with direct gaze among faces with averted Baron-Cohen, & Decety, 2003). In contrast, the processing of faces
gaze are detected faster and more accurately than faces with averted gaze is related to the left STS and intraparietal sulcus
with averted gaze among faces with direct gaze (Conty, Tijus, (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000; Wicker, Michel, Henaff, & Decety, 1998).
Hugueville, Coelho, & George, 2006; von Grunau & Anston, 1995). Hence, faces with direct and averted gaze are different not only
On the other hand, faces with averted gaze shift the observer's regarding behavior, but also regarding how they are processed in
spatial attention to the direction indicated by the averted gaze. In a the observer's brain.
gaze-cuing paradigm, observers can respond to a target more Regarding the unconscious perception of faces with direct gaze,
quickly when faces with averted gaze in the center of the display Stein, Senju, Peelen, and Sterzer (2011) used continuous flash
indicate a peripheral location of the upcoming target than when suppression (CFS) to examine whether faces with direct or averted
gaze were more effectively processed without conscious aware-
ness. Those authors used CFS to render facial images invisible at
n
Corresponding author at: Kobe University, Department of Tokyo, 1-1 Rokkodai-
the beginning of the trials, and then ramped up the contrast of
cho, Nada-ku, Kobe, Hyogo 657-8501 Japan. Tel./fax: +81 78 803 5562. those images until participants were consciously aware of them.
E-mail address: yokoyama@lit.kobe-u.ac.jp (T. Yokoyama). It was found that participants became aware of faces with direct

0028-3932/$ - see front matter & 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.002
1162 T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168

gaze faster than they became aware of faces with averted gaze. Table 1A.
Therefore, the authors concluded that faces with direct gaze were Mean and standard deviation of luminance and RMS contrast in Experiment 1
(behavioral experiment).
effectively processed without conscious awareness, which implies
that these faces gain preferential access to the conscious aware- Direct gaze Rightward gaze Leftward gaze
ness of faces.
Although Stein et al. (2011) study suggested that invisible faces Mean s.d. Mean s.d. Mean s.d.
with direct gaze induced stronger neural responses compared
Luminance 14.350 0.555 14.425 0.531 14.422 0.533
with faces with averted gaze, little is known about the uncon- Contrast (RMS) 5.000 0.000 4.997 0.004 4.997 0.004
scious neural processing of faces with direct gaze in the brain. To (cd/m2)
address this, we used electroencephalography (EEG) and recorded
event-related potentials (ERPs) evoked by images of faces with
direct gaze rendered invisible through CFS. Our aim was to identify The experiment used a one-factor repeated-measure design
the neural correlate of increased sensitivity to invisible faces with with two levels of gaze direction: direct and averted gaze. An
direct gaze compared with averted gaze. The high temporal experimental block consisted of 80 trials, in which the two
resolution of EEG enabled us to measure the speed at which the conditions were intermixed randomly. Ten practice trials preceded
invisible gaze information reached different parts of the brain. the experimental trial.
Fig. 1A shows an example of a trial sequence. Each trial cycled a
fixation display (1000 ms) as a preflash period; this was followed by
2. Experiment 1 (behavioral experiment) the presentation of a rapid sequence of high-contrast colored
Mondrian patterns at 20 Hz to the dominant eye of the participant.
Before performing the EEG experiments, we replicated the The colored Mondrian patterns were used as continuous flashes that
study of Stein et al. (2011) to confirm that the preferential rendered low-contrast images invisible (Noguchi, Yokoyama, Suzuki,
processing of faces with direct gaze relative to averted gaze also Kita, & Kakigi, 2012; Stein, Peelen, & Sterzer, 2012; Tsuchiya & Koch,
occurred in our study. 2005). To determine eye dominance in participants, we used a
variation of the Porta test (Roth, Lora, & Heilman, 2012); three
2.1. Methods participants had dominant right eyes. Conversely, a facial image
was gradually introduced to the nondominant eye, and the contrast
2.1.1. Participants of the facial image was ramped up linearly from 0% to 100% over 1 s.
Five participants (three men and two women; age range, 20–31 Facial images appeared either at the upper-left or upper-right corner
years) were recruited from the Psychology Department of Kobe of the white frame. The continuous flashes and facial images were
University. Informed consent was received from each participant presented until participants responded, or for 10 s. Participants
after the nature of the study had been explained. All participants pressed the left or right key as quickly and accurately as possible
had normal or corrected-to-normal acuity of vision and had no when any part of the facial image became visible.
history of neurological diseases. All individuals were naïve to the
purposes of this experiment. Approval for the experiment was
obtained from the ethics committee of Kobe University, Japan. 2.2. Results

2.1.2. Stimuli and procedure Fig. 1B shows the mean reaction time of the two gaze-direction
Visual stimuli were displayed on a Sony MultiScan 17sf II conditions. A t test analysis revealed the presence of a significant
14.1 in. CRT display with a resolution of 1024  768 pixels. Displays difference between the two conditions (t4 ¼−3.438, p o0.05). This
and data collection were controlled with MATLAB using the result provides evidence that the preferential processing of faces
Psychophysics toolbox (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) on a Dell with direct gaze, compared with faces with averted gaze, occurred
Optiplex 360 computer running Microsoft Windows XP (refresh in our study. Based on our behavioral results, we measured ERPs to
rate, 60 Hz). investigate the neural mechanisms underpinning the enhanced
To create dichoptic viewing, we presented stimuli at two sensitivity to process unconsciously faces with direct gaze.
different locations on the screen. Two white frames (6.1  6.11)
were displayed side by side on the screen, with one frame
presented to each eye. A mirror stereoscope fused stimuli at those
two locations. We used four pictures of faces (two males and two 3. Experiment 2 (EEG experiment)
females) from the ATR DB99 database (ATR-Promotions, Kyoto,
Japan). The gaze direction of each pictured person was leftward, 3.1. Methods
straight, and rightward. The overall luminance and contrast levels
of all pictures were adjusted using MATLAB. After controlling the The method described in Experiment 2 was similar to that used
luminance and root mean square (RMS) contrast levels, we in Experiment 1, with the exception of the following details.
performed an analysis of variance (ANOVA), which indicated the
absence of significant differences between direct-gaze, rightward-
gaze, and leftward-gaze pictures regarding luminance levels 3.1.1. Participants
(F2,11 ¼ 0.019, p ¼0.9815, n.s.) or RMS contrast levels (F2,11 ¼ 0.500, Twelve participants (nine men and three women; age range,
p ¼0.6224, n.s.). The mean and standard error values of luminance 20–29 years) were recruited from the Psychology Department of
and RMS contrast are presented in Table 1A. Direct-gaze pictures Kobe University. Informed consent was received from each parti-
were used for the direct-gaze condition, and leftward- and cipant after the nature of the study had been explained. All
rightward-gaze pictures were used for the averted-gaze condition. participants had normal or corrected-to-normal acuity of vision
In the averted-gaze condition, images of leftward gaze were and had no history of neurological diseases. All individuals were
positioned in the upper-left corner of the frame, whereas images naïve to the purposes of this experiment. Approval for the
of rightward gaze were positioned in the upper-right corner of experiment was obtained from the ethics committee of Kobe
the frame. University, Japan.
T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168 1163

Table 1B.
Mean and standard deviation of luminance and RMS contrast in high-contrast and
low-contrast pictures in Experiment 2 (EEG experiment).

Direct gaze Rightward gaze Leftward gaze

Mean s.d. Mean s.d. Mean s.d.

High-contrast pictures
Luminance 26.528 0.099 26.542 0.141 26.590 0.126
Contrast (RMS) 14.564 0.080 14.558 0.075 14.604 0.059

Low-contrast pictures
Luminance 8.780 0.192 8.735 0.302 8.690 0.217
Contrast (RMS) 1.525 0.036 1.518 0.016 1.511 0.027
(cd/m2)

The mean and standard error values of the luminance and RMS
contrast are presented in Table 1B.
Fig. 2 shows an example of a trial sequence. Each trial cycled
through a fixation display (1000 ms) as a preflash period, followed
by the presentation of a rapid sequence of high-contrast colored
Mondrian patterns to the dominant eye of the participant. To
determine eye dominance in participants, we used a variation of
the Porta test (Roth et al., 2002). All participants had dominant
right eyes. Each colored Mondrian pattern appeared for 50 ms, 26
different patterns were used in each trial, and the duration of the
flash period was 1300 ms. Conversely, the images presented to the
nondominant eye differed depending on seven types of trials
that were intermixed randomly: high-contrast direct gaze, high-
contrast rightward gaze, high-contrast leftward gaze, low-contrast
direct gaze, low-contrast rightward gaze, low-contrast leftward
gaze, and gray trials. Stimuli were presented to the nondominant
eye for 500 ms. In the high-contrast conditions, participants
perceived the gaze direction of the target faces consciously
because of the high luminance contrast of those images. In the
low-contrast conditions, however, the luminance contrast of the
facial images was too low for them to be perceived consciously
under CFS. In the gray trials, no images were presented throughout
the flash period. The duration of the presentation of all images to
the nondominant eye was between 600 and 1100 ms after the
Fig. 1. A trial sequence and the results in the Experiment 1. (A) An example of the
onset of the continuous flashes; thus, neural responses to the facial
sequence of events in a typical trial in the Experiment 1. A facial image (direct or
averted gaze) and colored Mondrian pattern were presented to non-dominant and images were not confounded with those induced by the onset
dominant eye, respectively. Those images were fused by a mirror stereoscope. A of continuous flashes. Subsequently, we presented continuous
contrast of a facial image was gradually ramped up, and participants judged flashes to the nondominant eye in the last 200 ms of each trial
whether a facial image appeared at upper left or upper right of the frame. to prevent the perception of afterimages.
(B) Results in the Experiment 1. The horizontal axis indicates reaction time that
participants responded to averted gaze(s), and the vertical axis indicates reaction
After the trials, participants were required to answer two
time that participants responded to direct gaze(s). All participants (n¼ 5) questions sequentially. The first question probed target detection
responded to direct gaze more quickly than averted gaze. (subjective measurement): participants pressed one key when
images were visible and another key when images were invisible.
The second question probed the participants’ ability to identify
accurately the gaze direction of facial images (objective measure-
3.1.2. Stimuli and procedure ment). Even if participants answered “no” to the first question, the
We used six pictures (three men and three women) from the ATR second question was presented to exclude the possibility of the
DB99 database (ATR-Promotions, Kyoto, Japan), and gaze direction unconscious processing of invisible stimuli. An experimental
was leftward, straight, and rightward for each pictured person. To block consisted of 84 trials in which the seven conditions (12
create visible and invisible conditions, we prepared high- and low- trials for each condition) were intermixed randomly. Six blocks
contrast pictures. The overall luminance and contrast levels of the were conducted.
pictures were adjusted using Adobe Photoshop 6.0 for both high- and
low-contrast pictures. After controlling the luminance and RMS
contrast levels, we performed ANOVA, which indicated the absence 3.1.3. EEG measurement and data analysis
of significant differences between direct-gaze, rightward-gaze, and EEG was recorded from 19 scalp electrodes (FP1, FP2, F3, Fz, F4,
leftward-gaze pictures regarding luminance levels (F2,17 ¼0.065, F7, F8, C3, Cz, C4, T3, T4, T5, T6, P3, Pz, P4, O1, and O2) (EEG1200;
p¼0.9375, n.s.) or RMS contrast levels (F2,17 ¼ 1.519, p¼ 0.2508, n.s.) Nihon Koden, Tokyo, Japan). EEG was recorded continuously at a
for high-contrast pictures, and absence of significant differences rate of 500 Hz and referenced with an average potential measured
between direct-gaze, rightward-gaze, and leftward-gaze pictures from the right and left earlobes. Neural activity in response to the
regarding luminance levels (F2,17 ¼0.207, p¼0.8154, n.s.) or RMS facial images was examined by measuring visual-evoked poten-
contrast levels (F2,17 ¼0.341, p¼ 0.7162, n.s.) for low-contrast pictures. tials (VEPs) that were time locked to the onset of those images. We
1164 T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168

epoched and averaged EEG waveforms ranging from −700 to results of tasks 1 and 2 suggest that the CFS blocked the
500 ms relative to the onset of the facial images. EEG signals in a conscious perception of the low-contrast facial images, whereas
period of −50 to 0 ms were used as a baseline. Epochs with signal the high-contrast facial images broke the CFS and were processed
variations exceeding 100 μV were excluded from the analysis. consciously.
Finally, VEPs were band-pass filtered at 0.5–30.0 Hz. Because some
participants reported conscious detection of the low-contrast
facial images during CFS in a small portion of trials, EEG wave-
forms in such trials were eliminated from the analysis, to ensure
the quality of unconsciousness.

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Behavioral data


Fig. 3A shows the behavioral results of task 1 (detection task)
averaged across the 12 participants. The mean ( 7 SE across
participants) detection rates for the high-contrast pictures were
99.7 7 0.2% (high-contrast direct gaze), 98.1 7 0.3% (high-contrast
rightward gaze), and 99.6 7 0.3% (high-contrast leftward gaze).
This result indicates that participants were able to detect the
high-contrast facial images even during CFS. Conversely, the
detection rates for the low-contrast pictures were 0.9 7 0.5%
(low-contrast direct gaze), 0.9 7 0.4% (low-contrast rightward
gaze), and 0.7 7 0.3% (low-contrast leftward gaze), which were
comparable to the false-alarm rates recorded in the gray condi-
tion (0.7 7 0.3%), in which no facial image was presented. We
performed one-way ANOVA to test for the presence of significant
differences between detection rates in the low-contrast condi-
tions and false-alarm rates in the gray condition. There were no
significant differences between the low-contrast direct gaze, low-
contrast rightward gaze, low-contrast leftward gaze, or gray
condition (F3,47 ¼0.169, p ¼0.9167). Thus, the detection rates in
the low-contrast conditions and false-alarm rates in the gray
condition were not significantly different. These data indicate
that participants could not consciously perceive the low-contrast
images regardless of whether gaze direction was direct, right-
ward, or leftward. Subsequently, we used a gaze direction
discrimination task to confirm that the low-contrast images were Fig. 3. Behavioral results of tasks 1 and 2. All error bars denote standard errors
invisible to participants (Fig. 3B). The accuracy of the high- (SEs). (A) Results of task 1 (detection task). The presentation of facial images with
contrast condition was almost 100% (98.3 7 0.1%), which was direct, rightward, and leftward gazes with a high luminance contrast during the
continuous flashes broke the suppression and came into the consciousness of
significantly higher (t11 ¼190.1, p o 0.001) than the chance level
participants. In contrast, almost all facial images of direct, rightward, and leftward
(33.3%). In contrast, the accuracy of the low-contrast condition gaze with a low luminance contrast were invisible throughout the trial. We set the
was 34.3 7 1.8%, which was not significantly different from the gray condition, in which no facial image was presented, as a control. False-alarm
chance level (t11 ¼0.34, p ¼0.73). To test that observers were not rates in the condition were comparable to the detection rates observed in the low-
able to discriminate the gaze direction of facial images in the contrast conditions. (B) Results of task 2 (discrimination task). The mean accuracies
in the high-contrast and low-contrast conditions were 98.3% and 34.3%, respec-
low-contrast conditions, we performed ANOVA to compare the tively. There were no significant differences between the low-contrast conditions
three low-contrast conditions and found no significant differ- and the chance level (33.3%), which indicates that facial images presented in low-
ences between the three conditions (F2,35 ¼1.194, p ¼0.321). The contrast conditions were totally suppressed during the flash period.

Fig. 2. An example of the sequence of events in a typical trial. Two different images were presented to the dominant and nondominant eyes of participants and a mirror
stereoscope fused those images. After the presentation of a fixation screen for 1 s, continuous flashes of 20 Hz were given to the dominant eye, which rendered a target facial
image invisible to the nondominant eye. After presentation of continuous flashes, participants were required to perform two tasks: detection and discrimination tasks.
T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168 1165

Fig. 4. Depiction of the 19 electrode positions over the scalp and the grand-averaged ERPs evoked by high-contrast conditions (positive: upward). The zeros in the horizontal
axis denote the onset of the presentation of facial images.

3.2.2. VEP waveforms data of the low-contrast rightward- and leftward-gaze conditions,
Because no facial images were presented in the gray condition, making ERPs in the averted-gaze condition as a control. Subse-
the ERPs induced by the gray condition should reflect neural quently, we performed a t test analysis at each channel to explore
responses to continuous flashes, not faces. To isolate the ERP which channels exhibited significant differences between direct
components related to gaze perception, we subtracted EEG wave- and averted gaze in the invisible conditions. There were significant
forms in the gray condition from those of the other gaze condi- differences between direct and averted gaze at F4 (t11 ¼ −2.23;
tions. We separated the high-contrast conditions from the low- po 0.05), C4 (t11 ¼−2.46; p o0.05), Fz (t11 ¼−2.25; p o0.05), and
contrast conditions to investigate VEPs from the 19 points on each Cz (t11 ¼−2.26; p o0.05) (see gray-shaded windows in Fig. 5). The
participant's scalp. Fig. 4 shows the grand-averaged VEPs at all 19 responses at those electrodes are enlarged in Fig. 6A. We then
electrodes for the high-contrast conditions, and Fig. 5 shows the created 50 ms time windows (Fig. 6B) and performed 2 (direct/
grand-averaged VEPs at all 19 electrodes for the low-contrast averted: gaze direction)  4 (channel)  8 (time window) repeated-
conditions. Some studies have suggested that, compared with measures ANOVA. We found significant main effects of gaze
faces with averted gaze, faces with direct gaze elicit a larger N1 direction and time window (F1,714 ¼ 11.545, po0.001 for gaze
in the posterior channels, such as O1, O2, T5, and T6 (Conty, direction; F7,714 ¼3.661, po0.001 for time window), but not
N'Diaye, Tijus, & George, 2007; Pesciarelli, Sarlo, & Leo, 2011) when of channel (F3,714 ¼ 0.171, p¼0.916). The interaction between gaze
the two types of gaze are presented consciously. Thus, we direction and time window was significant (F7,714 ¼2.109, po0.05);
averaged the EEG waveforms in the high-contrast conditions however, we did not find a significant interaction between gaze
across the four electrodes listed above, and applied one-factorial direction and channel (F3,714 ¼ 0.426, p¼ 0.735) or between channel
repeated-measures ANOVA to the averaged ERP amplitude within and time window (F21,714 ¼0.180, p¼1.000). Because the main
a 150–230 ms latency range (Taylor, George, & Ducorps, 2001). This effects of channel and interactions including channel were not
analysis revealed an absence of significant main effects between significant, we factored out the effects of channel and performed
the three conditions (F2,35 ¼ 0.353, p¼ 0.7068). In contrast, regard- two-way ANOVA (gaze direction and time window). There were
ing the unconscious processing of face stimuli, previous studies significant main effects of gaze direction (F1,741 ¼ 11.889, po0.001)
reported a negative deflection of EEG signals to faces presented and time window (F7,741 ¼3.770, po0.001), and a significant
unconsciously between 200 and 300 ms after stimulus onset interaction between gaze direction and time window (F7,741 ¼
(Liddell, Williams, Rathjen, Shevrin, & Gordon, 2004). Consistent 2.172, po0.05). To examine which of the time windows exhibited
with this report, we observed in some electrodes that the ERP significant differences between direct and averted gaze, we performed
components of the low-contrast direct-gaze condition were larger a post hoc analysis of interaction using a Bonferroni correction.
than the ERP components of the low-contrast rightward- and There were significant differences between direct and averted
leftward-gaze conditions at 200–300 ms. To analyze those compo- gaze at time windows of 200 ms (corrected p o0.005), 250 ms
nents selective to direct gaze statistically, we first averaged the (corrected po 0.001), and 350 ms (corrected po 0.05) (Fig. 6C).
1166 T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168

Fig. 5. Depiction of the 19 electrode positions over the scalp and the grand-averaged ERPs evoked by the low-contrast conditions (positive: upward). The zeros in the
horizontal axis denote the onset of the presentation of facial images. Specific responses for direct gaze only were observed in the four parietofrontal channels (Fz, F4, Cz, and
C4; green circles). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

4. Discussion Why did we obtain differential responses to direct vs. averted


gaze in the invisible condition only? One possibility is the
This study was the first to investigate the neural correlate of involvement of feedback neural signals that are uniquely induced
increased sensitivity to the unconscious processing of faces with by visible stimuli. Feedback neural signals are neural activations
direct gaze. We recorded EEG during CFS to address this question. that are essential for visual awareness (Dehaene, Changeux,
The effect of the unconscious perception of faces with direct gaze Naccache, Sackur, & Sergent, 2006; Lamme, 2003). Although the
appeared at time windows of 200, 250, and 350 ms after stimulus conscious perception of visual stimuli evokes brain responses that
onset at the parietofrontal electrodes. No specific neural responses are distributed widely across the entire brain, those strong
to invisible faces with direct gaze were observed at the occipital or responses sometimes mask subtle differences among conditions.
temporal electrodes, which might be consistent with previous For instance, Jiang and He (2006) used functional magnetic
findings reporting a lack of aftereffects induced by invisible faces resonance imaging to compare the neural responses to neutral
with direct gaze (Stein et al., 2012). Taken together, our findings and fearful faces that were either visible or rendered invisible by
suggest that faces with direct gaze are processed effectively under CFS. In the visible (no CFS) condition, those authors found
an unconscious condition, and that this effect occurs 200, 250, significant hemodynamic responses in the STS to both neutral
and 350 ms after the unconscious representation of faces with and fearful faces. In contrast, under the invisible condition, the
direct gaze. same STS region indicated a significant response only to fearful
Previous studies found large negative deflection at 200–300 ms faces. Because the fusiform face area (FFA) was similarly activated
at the parietofrontal electrodes when fearful faces were presented by the invisible neutral and fearful faces, the overall result
subliminally (Kiss & Eimer, 2008; Liddell et al., 2004). The large obtained in the invisible condition indicates a clear functional
EEG waveforms observed in these studies are similar to our distinction between the FFA (structural encoding) and STS (expres-
waveform data of faces with direct gaze. Given that the presenta- sional or emotional analyses). Jiang and He (2006) concluded that
tion of fearful faces and faces with direct gaze evokes emotional this clear distinction was caused by an absence of awareness of
responses in observers (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010; Senju & Johnson, facial information, which minimized the cortical responses asso-
2009), the EEG components of 200–300 ms in response to faces ciated with the conscious perception of faces.
with direct gaze observed in the present study might reflect We suggest that significant responses to invisible faces with
unconscious emotional responses. Our data clearly demonstrated direct compared with averted gaze can be explained by a lack of
a latency of the processing of unconscious faces with direct gaze, consciousness-related activity under unconscious conditions. In
but did not reveal the brain regions that were related to the fact, the results of previous neurophysiological studies that inves-
processing; thus, additional neuroimaging studies are necessary to tigated the effect of visible direct and averted gaze are incon-
elucidate the specific brain regions that are mainly associated with sistent. Some studies have suggested that faces with direct gaze
the processing unconscious faces with direct gaze. evoked larger VEPs than did faces with averted gaze (Conty et al.,
T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168 1167

Fig. 6. EEG waveforms and time sequence at four channels. (A) EEG waveforms of direct gaze and averted gaze. Averted-gaze data were created by averaging low-contrast
rightward and leftward gaze data. (B) Mean amplitude of EEG responses over time. EEG waveforms from 0 to 400 ms were divided into eight time windows of 50 ms and the
mean amplitude within each time window was plotted as a function of time. (C) Average across four channels in panel B. Amplitude of direct gaze was more negatively
deflected than amplitude of averted gaze at time-window of 200, 250, and 350 ms after stimulus onset (*p o .05, **p o .005, ***p o.001).

2007; Senju, Tojo, Yaguchi, and Hasegawa, 2005), whereas other conditions. Eliminating the feedback process, therefore, might
studies found the opposite effects (Puce, Smith, & Allison, 2000; enable us to identify the distinct processing of faces with direct
Watanabe, Miki, & Kakigi, 2002). Another group found no differ- and averted gaze using EEG.
ences between VEPs in response to faces with direct and averted In conclusion, we demonstrated the existence of a neural
gaze (Taylor, George, & Ducorps, 2001). The inconsistency between correlate of increased sensitivity to the unconscious processing
previous results may be attributable to the consciousness-related of faces with direct gaze. This effect was observed 200, 250, and
neural activity induced by visible gaze, which may have masked 350 ms after stimulus presentation. The results of our study are
the small differences between the direct- and averted-gaze consistent with those of a previous behavioral study that indicated
1168 T. Yokoyama et al. / Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 1161–1168

that the visual information of faces with direct gaze reached visual Langton, S. R., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, I. I. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the
awareness faster than that of faces with averted gaze. We believe direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(2), 50–59doi:
S1364661399014369.
that the current study will provide important clues for the Liddell, B. J., Williams, L. M., Rathjen, J., Shevrin, H., & Gordon, E. (2004). A temporal
elucidation of the neural mechanism underlying the processing dissociation of subliminal versus supraliminal fear perception: an event-related
of faces with direct gaze. potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 479–486, http://dx.doi.
org/10.1162/089892904322926809.
Noguchi, Y., Yokoyama, T., Suzuki, M., Kita, S., & Kakigi, R. (2012). Temporal
dynamics of neural activity at the moment of an emergence of a conscious
percept. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
Acknowledgement
Pageler, N. M., Menon, V., Merin, N. M., Eliez, S., Brown, W. E., & Reiss, A. L. (2003).
Effect of head orientation on gaze processing in fusiform gyrus and superior
We greatly thank Mr. Shintaro Kimijima (Kobe University, temporal sulcus. NeuroImage, 20(1), 318–329, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-
8119(03)00229-5.
Japan) for his technical supports. We also thank our laboratory Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transform-
members for support in carrying out this work. This study was ing numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.
supported by a grant to the first author (T.Y) from the Grant-in-Aid Pelphrey, K. A., Viola, R. J., & McCarthy, G. (2004). When strangers pass—processing
of mutual and averted social gaze in the superior temporal sulcus. Psychological
for JSPS Fellows [11J02271], the second author (Y.N) from the
Science, 15(9), 598–603.
Gran-in-Aid for Young Scientists (A) [22680022], and the third Pesciarelli, F., Sarlo, M., & Leo, I. (2011). The time course of implicit processing of
author (S.K) from the Strategic Information and Communications facial features: an event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 49(5),
1154–1161, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.003.
R&D Promotion Programme (SCOPE) [101707012].
Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low
road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–782, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/Nrn2920.
References Puce, A., Smith, A., & Allison, T. (2000). Erps evoked by viewing facial movements.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(1), 221–239, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
026432900380580.
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436. Roth, H. L., Lora, A. N., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Effects of monocular viewing and
Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., Keane, J., Scott, S. K., Owen, A. M., Christoffels, I., & eye dominance on spatial attention. Brain, 125, 2023–2035.
Young, A. W. (2002). Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia, 40(8), Sato, W., Yoshikawa, S., Kochiyama, T., & Matsumura, M. (2004). The amygdala
1129–1138. processes the emotional significance of facial expressions: an fMRI investiga-
Conty, L., N'Diaye, K., Tijus, C., & George, N. (2007). When eye creates the contact! tion using the interaction between expression and face direction. NeuroImage,
ERP evidence for early dissociation between direct and averted gaze motion 22(2), 1006–1013, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/J.Neuroimage.2004.02.030.
processing. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3024–3037, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/J. Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009). The eye contact effect: mechanisms and
Neuropsychologia.2007.05.017. development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 127–134, http://dx.doi.org/
Conty, L., Tijus, C., Hugueville, L., Coelho, E., & George, N. (2006). Searching for 10.1016/J.Tics.2008.11.009.
asymmetries in the detection of gaze contact versus averted gaze under Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Yaguchi, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Deviant gaze processing in
different head views: a behavioural study. Spatial Vision, 19(6), 529–545. children with autism: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1297–1306, http:
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, //dx.doi.org/10.1016/J.Neuropsychologia.2004.12.002.
preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy. Trends in Stein, T., Peelen, M. V., & Sterzer, P. (2012). Eye gaze adaptation under interocular
Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007. suppression. Journal of Vision, 12(7), 1, http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/12.7.1.
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is Stein, T., Senju, A., Peelen, M. V., & Sterzer, P. (2011). Eye contact facilitates
triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490–495. awareness of faces during interocular suppression. Cognition, 119(2), 307–311,
George, N., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Seen gaze-direction modulates fusiform http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/J.Cognition.2011.01.008.
activity and its coupling with other brain areas during face processing. Neuro- Taylor, M. J., George, N., & Ducorps, A. (2001). Magnetoencephalographic evidence
Image, 13(6), 1102–1112, http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/Nimg.2001.0769. of early processing of direction of gaze in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 316(3),
von Grunau, M., & Anston, C. (1995). The detection of gaze direction: a stare-in-the- 173–177.
crowd effect. Perception, 24(11), 1297–1313. Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative
Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096–1101, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/
identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Nn1500.
Neuroscience, 3(1), 80–84, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/71152. Vecera, S. P., & Rizzo, M. (2006). Eye gaze does not produce reflexive shifts of
Jiang, Y., & He, S. (2006). Cortical responses to invisible faces: dissociating attention: evidence from frontal-lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 44(1), 150–-
subsystems for facial-information processing. Current Biology, 16(20), 2023–2029, 159doi: S0028-3932(05)00168-5 [pii]10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.010.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/J.Cub.2006.08.084. Watanabe, S., Miki, K., & Kakigi, R. (2002). Gaze direction affects face perception in
Kampe, K. K. W., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: signals conveying humans. Neuroscience Letters, 325(3), 163–166.
communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated Wicker, B., Michel, F., Henaff, M. A., & Decety, J. (1998). Brain regions involved in the
with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), perception of gaze: a PET study. NeuroImage, 8(2), 221–227, http://dx.doi.org/
5258–5263. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0357.
Kawashima, R., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., Nakamura, A., Hatano, K., Ito, K., Fukuda, H., Wicker, B., Perrett, D. I., Baron-Cohen, S., & Decety, J. (2003). Being the target of
Kojima, S., & Nakamura, K. (1999). The human amygdala plays an important another's emotion: a PET study. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 139–146.
role in gaze monitoring—a PET study. Brain, 122, 779–783. Yokoyama, T., Ishibashi, K., Hongoh, Y., & Kita, S. (2011). Attentional capture by
Kiss, M., & Eimer, M. (2008). ERPs reveal subliminal processing of fearful faces. change in direct gaze. Perception, 40(7), 785–797, http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/
Psychophysiology, 45(2), 318–326, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007. P7003.
00634.x. Yokoyama, T., Noguchi, Y., & Kita, S. (2012). Attentional shifts by gaze direction in
Lamme, V. A. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in voluntary orienting: evidence from a microsaccade study. Experimental Brain
Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18. Research, 223(2), 291–300, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3260-z.

You might also like