Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect
Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350
www.elsevier.com/locate/cogsys

Narrative structure in the mind: Translating Genette’s


narrative discourse theory into a cognitive system

Taisuke Akimoto
Kyushu Institute of Technology, 680-4 Kawazu, Iizuka, Fukuoka 820-8502, Japan

Received 14 May 2019; accepted 17 August 2019


Available online 22 August 2019

Abstract

An episodic memory is considered to be mentally encoded or constructed information instead of a copy of the past events. However,
the mechanism that constructs episodic memory has not been systematically formulated in previous studies on cognitive architecture and
system. In this study, the term ‘‘story” is used, rather than episodic memory, to refer to a mental representation of temporally and lin-
guistically organized events and entities. The main difference between a story and an episodic memory is that a story involves a form of
mental representation, while the definition of an episodic memory is generally based on the function or content (i.e., it enables the rec-
ollection of past events). A story is considered as a uniform mental representation involving episodic memory, current situation, prospec-
tive memory, planned or imagined future, and fiction. To ensure systematic formulation of the story-form memory construction, this
study introduces Genette’s narrative discourse theory, which is a representative work in narratology. Genette provided a systematic clas-
sification of terms used to describe a narrative structure, with particular focus on how a narrative is structured on a text instead of what is
told. In this study, Genette’s narratological terms are analogically translated to methods of story-form memory construction in a cog-
nitive system. Furthermore, the narratological methods are rearranged using cognitive terms including temporal segmentation of the
world, generalization of stories, theory of mind, metacognition, self-formation, memory organization, and sociocultural aspect of cog-
nition. Thus, a story is characterized as integrative, multifaceted, and multifunctional information in the mind.
Ó 2019 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

Keywords: Story; Memory; Narratology; Genette; Cognitive system

1. Introduction systematically formulated in previous studies on cognitive


architecture and system. For example, in the Soar cognitive
In psychological studies on memory, an episodic mem- architecture, an episodic memory is considered as sequen-
ory is generally defined as a memory that enables the recol- tial snapshots of the agent’s working memory (Nuxoll &
lection or remembrance of past events or experiences Laird, 2004, 2012). Faltersack, Burns, Nuxoll, and
(Tulving, 1983, 2002). It is not assumed as a copy of the Crenshaw (2011) presented an episodic memory structure
past events themselves and is instead assumed as corre- that involves a hierarchy from lower primitive elements
sponding to mentally encoded or constructed information to higher compound elements. In the ICARUS cognitive
including reinterpretation, sensemaking, abstraction, gen- architecture, an episodic memory is encoded via generaliza-
eralization, errors, and forgetting. However, the mecha- tion based on similarity with previous episodes (Ménager &
nism that constructs episodic memory has not been Choi, 2016). The aforementioned episodic memories
involve only few linguistic aspects. On the other hand,
Bölöni (2011) presented an episodic memory model includ-
E-mail address: akimoto@ai.kyutech.ac.jp

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2019.08.007
1389-0417/Ó 2019 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350 343

ing conceptual information. Recently, several studies translated into the structure of a cognitive system. In Sec-
focused on narrative structure in an episodic memory. tion 4, Genette’s categories are reinterpreted as methods
Anderson (2015) discussed the mental processes of con- of story-form memory construction. Section 5 rearranges
structing an episodic memory including temporal segmen- narratological methods using cognitive terms. Finally, Sec-
tation of event and creating a relationship between tion 6 concludes the study.
events. León (2016) formalized the relational structure of
a narrative memory that connects narrative objects in 2. Genette’s narrative discourse theory
terms of kernels and satellites. Although the aforemen-
tioned studies captured a partial aspect of episodic mem- This section provides a brief overview of Genette’s nar-
ory, it is necessary to explore a unified computational rative discourse theory (Genette, 1980). The theory was
theory of episodic memory construction. developed based on analyzing a novel À la recherche du
In this study, the term ‘‘story” is used, rather than epi- temps perdu (In Search of Lost Time) written by Marcel
sodic memory, to refer to a mental representation of tem- Proust. The basic analytical framework is explained in Sec-
porally and linguistically organized events and entities. tion 2.1. Subsequently, categorization of narrative dis-
The main difference between a story and an episodic mem- course structure is described in Section 2.2.
ory is that a story involves the form of the mental represen-
tation, while the definition of an episodic memory is 2.1. Analytical framework
generally based on the function or content (i.e., it enables
the recollection of past events). As described in Akimoto In order to deal with the structural analysis of a narra-
(2018), a story is considered as a uniform mental represen- tive, Genette distinguished a narrative into three aspects,
tation involving episodic memory, autobiographical mem- namely discourse (narrative), story, and narrating. A dis-
ory, current situation, prospective memory, planned or course refers to the text of a novel or narrative itself. A
imagined future, and fictional or hypothetical story. They story refers to the content information recounted in the dis-
include long-term, short-term, and working memories. course. Thus, it contains chronologically organized events.
Hence, the generative cognition of story constitutes a com- Narrating refers to the action of producing a narrative by a
mon basis for a cognitive system or an integrative autono- narrator(s) and narratee(s). In this context, a narrator and
mous artificial intelligence. The importance of story narratee correspond to persons inscribed in the narrative
cognition in artificial intelligence was explored by Schank text and not the author and reader of a narrative. In the
(1982, 1990). Winston (2012)’s strong story hypothesis also aforementioned three aspects, only a discourse exhibits a
argues for the generality of story in an intelligence. materiality while a story and narrating are interpreted from
In order to ensure a systematic formulation of the story- the discourse. Hence, the object of the analysis corresponds
form memory construction, this study introduces Genette to a discourse.
(1980)’s narrative discourse theory. It is a representative The categorization of structural properties of a narrative
work in narratology (which is a discipline that examines discourse is constructed based on the relationship among
fundamental structures, principles, and properties of narra- the discourse, story, and narrating. Specifically, it consists
tives). The theoretical background of narratology includes of the following three broad categories (see also Fig. 1):
structuralism, semiology, Russian formalism, linguistics, tense refers to the relationships between the temporal
and literary studies. Narratological theories are applied in aspects of a discourse and story; mood refers to the modal-
artificial intelligence studies on narrative generation ities of expressing the story in the discourse; and voice
(Gervás, Lönneker-Rodman, Meister, & Peinado, 2006; refers to the situation of narrating, in the relationships with
Lönneker, 2005; Ogata, 2016), interactive narrative the discourse and story.
(Montfort, 2007), narrative analysis (Mani, 2013), and cog-
nitive architecture (Anderson, 2015; León, 2016; Akimoto, 2.2. Categorization of narrative discourse structure
2018; Szilas, 2015; Samsonovich & Aha, 2015).
Genette’s narrative discourse theory is characterized by Genette established hierarchically organized terms to
its systematicity. He provided a hierarchical classification describe a narrative structure. The terms are arranged as
of terms to describe a narrative structure, with particular
focus on how a narrative is structured on a text instead
of what is told. The aim of this study involves formulating Narrating
the methods of story-form memory construction via ana- Voice
logically applying Genette’s systematic theory. Further-
more, the narratological methods are rearranged using
cognitive terms. Story Discourse
The rest of the paper is organized as follows: Section 2 Tense (narrative)
provides an overview of Genette’s narrative discourse the- Mood
ory including the analytical framework and structural cat-
egorization. In Section 3, Genette’s analytical framework is Fig. 1. Analytical framework of the narrative discourse theory.
344 T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350

subcategories of the aforementioned three broad cate- * Zero focalization: A nonfocalized discourse with-
gories. The hierarchical categorization of structural terms out information regulation based on a specific
and their brief explanations are provided below (see also perspective.
Prince (2003) for more strict definitions of the narratolog- * Internal focalization: A discourse is composed
ical terminologies): from a character’s perspective including the char-
acter’s mental actions.
 Tense: Relationships between the temporal aspects of a * External focalization: A discourse presents only
discourse and story. the external behavior of characters (i.e., charac-
– Order (anachronies): Relationships between the ters’ mental actions are not recounted).
chronological order of events in a story and the order
in which the events are recounted in a discourse.  Voice: Situations of narrating in the relationship with
There are two major subcategories as follows: the discourse and story.
* Analepsis: Going back to past events from a pre- – Time of the narrating: Relationships between the tem-
sent temporal position via a flashback, recollec- poral positions of narrating and the narrated story.
tion, or other methods. * Subsequent: A past-tense narrative.
* Prolepsis: Going forward to future events from a * Prior: A predictive narrative (generally presented
present temporal position via a flashforward, pre- in the future tense).
diction, or other methods. * Simultaneous: A present-tense narrative.
– Duration (anisochronies): Relationships between the * Interpolated: Temporal position of the narrating
length of a discourse (for e.g., words, lines, and is interpolated between the moments of action.
pages) and amount of time of the recounted events – Narrative level: The notion can be understood as the
(for e.g., seconds, minutes, hours, days, months, nesting structure of narrating and is explained as ‘‘a
and years). narrative narrated within a narrative.” A first-level
* Summary: A sequence of events is briefly narrative is produced in its external level (extradie-
recounted via relatively short text. getic position).
* Scene: A sequence of events is recounted in detail – Person: Relationships between a narrator and charac-
via relatively long text. ters in the story.
* Pause: The story’s temporal progress is stopped * First-person: A narrator appears in the story as a
in a discourse via the narrator’s commentaries or (central or secondary) character.
descriptions of objects, things, or situations. * Third-person: A narrator does not appear in the
* Ellipsis: A certain time period in a story is explic- story.
itly or implicitly omitted in the discourse (for e.g.,
‘‘some years passed”).
– Frequency: Relationships of frequency between the
occurrences of an event(s) in a story and recounted 3. Translating the analytical framework into a cognitive
number in a discourse. system
* Singulative narrative: An event is recounted once,
i.e., a normal narrative. This section translates the relationship among discourse,
* Repeating narrative: The same event is recounted story, and narrating into a cognitive system. Fig. 2 illus-
two or more times. trates an overview of the framework. First, the three
* Iterative narrative: Similar events are recounted aspects of a narrative are replaced into representational
once (e.g., ‘‘every day of the week I went to bed and procedural elements of a cognitive system: the story
early”). corresponds to a mental representation containing infor-
mation of structured events; discourse corresponds to an
 Mood: Modalities of expressing a story in the discourse. expressive structure of a narrative; and narrating corre-
– Distance: A mode of regulating narrated information sponds to the action of producing stories and discourses.
in terms of the degree of the narrator’s mediation. With respect to the aspect of narrating, the narrator essen-
The notion can be understood as a contrast between tially corresponds to the agent itself. The narratee is
mimesis or showing (smaller distance) and diegesis or unspecified although it corresponds to an environment or
telling (larger distance). an objective that directs the production of a story and
– Focalization: A mode of regulating narrated informa- discourse.
tion based on a choice or not of a restrictive perspec- In the relationship with physical and social environ-
tive. It should be noted that Genette distinguished a ments, a story that forms a temporal structure of a current
perspective from a narrator because a narrator can situation constitutes a foundation of a higher-level action–
narrate a story via considering another character’s perception system (Akimoto, 2018). Conversely, narrative-
perspective. The three basic types of focalization are communication with another person or agent is always
listed below. mediated via a discourse.
T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350 345

Agent’s Cognitive System


character’s action or a stative information including
description and commentary.1 An event can contain a
sequence of two or more sub-events: ½ei ðe1 ; . . . ; ej Þ. A rela-
Narrating
(production) tionship ri relates two events: ½relationshipðep ; eq Þ. Table 1
shows an example of memory containing three simple sto-
ries. The example is subsequently used to explain story con-
struction methods.
In this study, the order is not applied to the construction
Story Discourse
method for a story itself; instead, it exhibits a close rela-
(memory) (expressive
tionship with a memory organization. Therefore, we start
structure)
with duration, and the relevance of temporal ordering to
a memory is discussed in the last subsection.

Action–Perception Narrative 4.1. Duration


communication
The four subcategories of duration (i.e., summary, scene,
Physical and social environments pause, and ellipsis) are translated as follows.
Summary and scene correspond to a change in the level
Fig. 2. Framework of story construction in a cognitive system. of detail of a sequence of events. In summary, two or more
events are abstracted into a comprehensive event. For
Genette’s theory does not clarify whether a story refers example, s2 changes to ðe2:1 ; ½e2;x ðe2;2 ; e2;3 ÞÞ, where e2;x
to events and things themselves (in an external world) or (e.g., ‘‘We had coffee at a coffee shop.”) is newly generated
a mental representation of events and things. In this study, from e2;2 –e2;3 . If the sub-events (e2;2 –e2;3 ) are forgotten, then
a story is clearly positioned as a mental representation. s2 corresponds to ðe2;1 ; e2;x Þ.
Hence, it can be considered that a discourse as well as a In scene, an event is detailed into two or more sub-
story is linguistically constructed. The aforementioned per- events. For example, s2 changes to
spective blurs the boundary between a discourse and story. ðe2;1 ; ½e2;2 ðe2;2a ; e2;2b ; e2;2c Þ; e2;3 Þ, where e2;2a –e2;2c (e.g. ‘‘We
However, the matter appears to constitute the essential nat- walked to a coffee shop. We entered the coffee shop. We
ure of the mutual relationship between the memory (story) ordered two cups of coffee and two shortcakes.”) corre-
and expression (discourse). spond to newly generated events from e2;2 .
In the framework, a cyclic relationship exists between a Pause linguistically composes of a situational or scenery
story and discourse, i.e., a story is reconstructed via struc- information or commentary that is associated with an
turing a discourse based on that story. The cyclic relation- event. For example, s1 changes to ðe1;1 ; e1;2 ; e1;2d ; e1;3 Þ where
ship involves the following three cognitive processes of e1;2d refers to a scenery description of the beach (e.g., ‘‘The
story construction: construction of a story via interaction sea was sparkling in the morning sun.”).
with the environment; reconstruction of a story via exter- Ellipsis can be interpreted as a choice of information that is
nally or internally narrating it; and construction of a story discarded from a story. For example, if e2;2 –e2;3 in s2 are dis-
from a narrative via another person or agent. Various carded (consciously or unconsciously), then the story short-
aspects of the discourse structure are reflected into a story ens to ðe2;1 Þ, ‘‘Yesterday, I met an old friend at a park.”
via the aforementioned cycle.
4.2. Frequency
4. Translating the narratological categories into story
construction methods Only iterative narrating can be translated into a method
of memory construction from the subcategories of
In this section, the categories of narrative structure are frequency.2 In this method, two or more similar stories or
translated into methods of story construction in a cognitive events are compressed into a single story. For example,
system. In dealing with the issue, we focus on processes s1 and s3 can be transformed into a compressed story
wherein a relatively raw (simple) story is reconstructed by s4 , ‘‘(Every morning recently) e4;1 : I wake up early. e4;2 : I
narrative methods. In order to provide a formal description take a walk. e4;3 : I eat breakfast.” The story is contained
of the methods, we define a minimal representation formal-
ism of a memory containing stories.
A memory M denotes a tuple hS; T i, where S denotes a 1
In narratology, a description of situational or scenery information and
set of stories fs1 ; . . . ; sn g and T denotes a set of times a commentary by a narrator is distinguished from an event. However, in
ft1 ; . . . ; tm g. Time ti contains one or more stories: this formalism, they are treated as the same type of representational
element for purposes of simplicity.
½timefsi g. A story si is represented by a tuple hE; Ri, where 2
Singulative narrative is a normal form of story construction. The act of
E denotes a sequence of events ðe1 ; . . . ; el Þ and R denotes a repeating can be considered as a memory rehearsal although it does not
set of relationships fr1 ; . . . ; rk g. An event ei represents a produce a copy of an event into a story.
346 T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350

Table 1
Example memory.
M ¼ fSfs1 ; s2 ; s3 g; T ft1 ; t2 gg
s1 ¼ fEðe1;1 ; e1;2 ; e1;3 Þ; Rfgg
s2 ¼ fEðe2;1 ; e2;2 ; e2;3 Þ; Rfgg
s3 ¼ fEðe3;1 ; e3;2 ; e3;3 Þ; Rfgg
t1 ¼ ½yesterday fs1 ; s2 g
t2 ¼ ½today fs3 g
Contents of stories (in English):
s1 : ‘‘(Yesterday) e1;1 : I woke up early in the morning. e1;2 : I took a walk at a beach. e1;3 : I ate breakfast.”
s2 : ‘‘(Yesterday) e2;1 : I met an old friend at a park. e2;2 : We went to a coffee shop. e2;3 : We chatted over our coffee.”
s3 : ‘‘(Today) e3;1 : I woke up early in the morning. e3;2 : I took a walk in a park. e3;3 : I ate breakfast.”

in a new time t3 : ½every-morning-recentlyfs4 g. The origi- ‘‘e20 ;1 : An old friend met me at a park. e20 ;2 : He felt nostalgic
nal stories can be forgotten, and in this case about me. . . .”. A perspective is distinguished from the nar-
M ¼ fSfs2 ; s4 g; T ft1 ; t3 gg. rator, and thus the first-person character does not change
in this version.
4.3. Distance
4.5. Voice
Example stories in Table 1 contains only a sequence of
simple events without relational information. Given that In Genette’s theory, voice considers the aspect of narrat-
the stories involve only a few mediations of the narrator ing and not the structure of a discourse and the story itself.
and that they appear to constitute showing as opposed to As shown in Fig. 3, subcategories of voice (i.e., narrative
telling, it is assumed that the stories are created from a rel- level, time of the narrating, and person) can be translated
atively small distance. A basic method to increase the dis- into a set of elements to form a meta-story structure via
tance involves adding a relationship between story interpreting the notion as background conditions in the
elements because a relationship is considered as an imma- cognitive action of constructing a story.
terial object created by a narrator’s mentality. Conversely, Narrative level corresponds to an essential aspect of the
decreases in the relational information from a story meta-story structure to create a division between a story
decrease the distance. and cognitive action to produce the story. It is represented
We provide several examples of the idea. If a causality via a nesting structure and a first-level story is produced in
between events is added into s1 , then it changes to its external (extradiegetic) level by the narrator.
fEðe1;1 ; e1;2 ; e1;3 Þ; Rfr1 ½reasonðe1;1 ; e1;2 Þgg, which implies Time of the narrating corresponds to the temporal rela-
‘‘e1;1 : Because I woke up early in the morning, e1;2 : I took tionship between a story’s temporal position and present
a walk at a beach. e1;3 : I ate breakfast.”. A creation time of producing or remembering the story. The informa-
of a new event to provide a reason for an action tion is essentially reflected in the linguistic tense (i.e., past,
increases the distance to s1 , e.g., fEðe1;1 ; e1;2r ; e1;2 ; e1;3 Þ; future, or present) when a story is expressed as a narrative.
Rfr1 ½reason ðe1;2r ; e1;2 Þgg which implies for example, Person corresponds to whether the narrator appears in a
‘‘. . .e1;2r : Because the morning sun was calling me, e1;2 : I took story as a character. In an agent’s cognitive system, the
a walk at a beach. . . .”. basic narrator corresponds to the agent itself. However,
Another method to increase the distance is to add a another person or agent (as a character in a story) can
commentary about a story element (e.g., ‘‘. . .e1;2 : I took a become the narrator of a second-level story. For example,
walk at a beach. e1;2c : I believe that walking is good for if s20 (the friend’s internal focalization presented in Sec-
mental health. . . .”). In this example, the narrator’s value tion 4.4) is nested as the friend’s story, then it changes to
judgment is added into the story. ‘‘I met K at a park. I felt nostalgic about K.   ” (K refers
to the original ‘‘me”).
4.4. Focalization In order to represent the meta-story structure, story for-
malism is expanded into si ¼ ½vðsi Þ. The term v denotes a
In a story construction process, focalization works as a tuple hnarrator; narTime; leveli, where narrator indicates
perspective-based regulation of information to be con- the narrator by its identical symbol, narTime corresponds
tained in the story. In an agent’s cognitive system, a natural to a temporal position of si relative to the time of the nar-
focalization considers the internal focalization of the agent rating (e.g., ‘‘past,” ‘‘present,” or ‘‘future”), and level indi-
itself. Hence, an important issue in focalization involves cates the extradiegetic level of si (e.g., it corresponds to
taking a different perspective from the agent’s internal per- ‘‘zero” when si denotes a first-level story, and ‘‘first: sx ”
spective. It generates a different version of a story. For when si denotes a second-level story contained in sx ). The
example, a different version of s2 (i.e., s20 ) is produced via meta-story structure is dynamically generated when a story
simulating the internal focalization of the old friend, e.g., is remembered or focused.
T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350 347

Person
(appearance in the story or not)

First-level story

Second-level story
(a mental story about
another person or
agent.)

Self Self Another


(as narrator) (as character) person or agent

Time Time

Time of the narrating

Fig. 3. Meta-story structure including narrative level, time of the narrating, and person.

4.6. Order 5. Rearrangement in cognitive terms

Events in a story are chronologically organized, and The aforementioned narrative methods and structures
thus the temporal order of events can be essentially manip- are relevant to various cognitive issues and are categorized
ulated only in the discourse dimension. Hence, we do not as follows:
apply temporal ordering to a story construction method. Temporal segmentation and abstraction. Stories are con-
However, the mechanism of forming a connection between structed in different degrees of duration between scene and
events or stories over the chronological order and a tempo- summary. The phenomenon arises from flexibility in
ral gap is relevant to the organization of stories in a mem- abstracting events with different densities of temporal seg-
ory. Therefore, we provide a short consideration on a ments of the world.
structure underlying temporal ordering in a discourse. Generalization. Similar stories are compressed into a
We use only one example that associates s2 with an event story via iterative narrating. The phenomenon corresponds
in s3 via a flashback: ðe3;1 ; e3;2 ; ðe2;1 ; e2;2 ; e2;3 Þ; e3;3 Þ. The to generalization that captures a shared information or
structure is expressed as ‘‘Today, e3;1 : I woke up early in structure among stories.
the morning. e3;2 : I took a walk in a park. (Yesterday, Non-verbal memory. Processes of story construction
e2;1 : I met an old friend at the park. e2;2 : We went to a coffee include verbalization of non-verbal memory such as detail-
shop. e2;3 : We chatted over our coffee.) e3;3 : I ate break- ing events via remembering an experience by scene and
fast.”. The structure can be explained as an association constructing a scenery description accompanying an event
mediated by the ‘‘park.” It is considered that association by pause. In order to enable this type of cognition, it is nec-
among stories in the human mind is mediated by various essary for non-verbal (or unverbalized) memory to be asso-
types of similarities and relatedness including semantic, ciated with linguistically structured stories.
structural, non-verbal, and instance-level. In order to gen- Memory organization. Temporal ordering of events
erate these types of phenomena, it is necessary to organi- including remembering a story over the chronological
cally organize stories in a memory. The issue is relevant order and temporal gap is underpinned via an organic
to memory retrieval including index- and similarity-based memory organization.
approaches (Schank, 1982; Kolodner, 1983; Thagard, Sensemaking and relating. Stories are constructed in dif-
Holyoak, Nelson, & Gochfeld, 1990; Forbus, Gentner, & ferent degrees of distance. The process of increasing a
Law, 1994), and our initial idea about story association is story’s distance via the creation of relationships among
presented in Akimoto (2019). story elements is similar to subjective sensemaking.
Theory of mind and point of view. In an agent’s cognitive
4.7. Summary of story construction methods system, a story is essentially constructed from its own per-
spective. However, different versions of stories are gener-
Story construction methods including meta-story struc- ated via simulating another individual’s internal
ture and temporal order as derived from Genette’s theory focalization (perspective). The phenomenon is relevant to
are summarized in Table 2. Fig. 4 lists their graphical the theory of mind, i.e., the ability to imagine another indi-
explanations excluding the meta-story structure. vidual’s mental state.
348 T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350

Table 2
Summary of story construction methods.
Narratological terms Operation or structure
Summary Generating a comprehensive event from two or more events
Scene Generating two or more detailed events from an event
Pause Adding a situational or scenery description or commentary associated with an event
Ellipsis Discarding part of a story
Iterative narrating Compressing two or more similar stories or events into a single story
Distance (increase) Adding a relational information to a story
Distance (decrease) Removing a relational information from a story
(Internal) Focalization Generating a different version of a story via simulating another individual’s perspective
Narrative level* Division between a story and its production via a nesting structure
Time of the narrating* Relationship between the time of a story and time of producing or remembering the story
Person* Whether the narrator appears in a story as a character
Order** Associating a part of a story with another part or story over the chronological order and a temporal gap
*
Meta-story structure.
**
Association among stories.

Summary Scene Pause Ellipsis


e e
e e e e e e
e e e e e e d

Iterative narrating Distance Focalization


r r
s
s
e e e
s s s c s’

Order (as association among stories)


s: Story
s s e: Event
d: Description of situational or scenery information
c: Commentary
s
s r: Relationship

Fig. 4. Graphical explanations of story construction methods derived from Genette’s narrative discourse theory.

Stories Discourses Stories


as as as
mental information social information mental information
communicated among
individuals

Mind Society Mind


(cognitive system) (social systems) (cognitive system)

Fig. 5. Narrative-based interpenetration between an agent’s cognitive system and a society.


T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350 349

Metacognition and consciousness. The self as narrator lated based on Genette’s systematic categorization of nar-
(narrator-self) and self as character (character-self) are dis- rative structure. Further, the proposed narratological
tinguished based on meta-story structure derived from methods and structures are interrelated with cognitive
voice. The distinction constitutes the basis for structural issues, including temporal segmentation of the world, gen-
modeling of metacognition, namely cognition about one’s eralization of stories, theory of mind, point of view,
own cognition. This structure may also lead to the organi- metacognition, self-formation, memory organization, and
zational understanding of consciousness. sociocultural aspect of cognition. Thus, a story can be con-
Self-formation. In psychological and philosophical stud- sidered as integrative, multifaceted, and multifunctional
ies, narrative and autobiographical memory are often information in one’s mind. Hence, a story-centered
regarded as the foundation of self-formation. The pro- approach to a cognitive system exhibits the advantage of
posed narrative methods and structures, particularly voice- capturing integrative working of the mind. Our proposed
based metacognition and sensemaking, will be the basis for theory acts as the foundation to develop a story-centered
computational modeling of self-formation. cognitive system or architecture.
Sociocultural aspect of cognition. Narratives (discourses) The proposed theory describes the general aspects of a
consist of social information communicated in various story structure in the mind. However, it does not provide
social fields such as education, art, mass and social media, the cognitive process and knowledge underlying the
and family. Styles of narrative composition are developed production and manipulation of stories. In computational
by an individual mind and societies via co-creative commu- narrative generation studies, various approaches such as
nication among individuals. Therefore, the proposed the- goal-oriented planning, case-based reasoning, analogical
ory involves a sociocultural aspect of cognition. In the reasoning, schema-based top–down cognition, and
proposed framework (see Fig. 2), the relationship between combination or transformation of existing stories have
stories and discourses is formulated as a cyclic relationship. been suggested. We consider that generative story
Based on this structure, the interaction between an individ- cognition is a combination of various cognitive processes,
ual mind (cognitive system) and societies (social systems) including the ones mentioned above. Hence, in a future
can be understood as an interpenetration built on narra- study, we will explore fundamental principles for the
tives, as illustrated in Fig. 5. This system view on societies unification of generative story cognition.
and the relationship between societies and minds is rooted
in Luhmann’s social systems theory (Luhmann, 2012–
Funding
2013).3
Computational modeling of generative cognition of sto-
This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant
ries exhibits potential to lead to an integrative view on the
No. JP18K18344.
collaboration of the aforementioned different cognitive
aspects.
References
6. Conclusion
Akimoto, T. (2018). Stories as mental representations of an agent’s
subjective world: A structural overview. Biologically Inspired Cognitive
In search of a unified computational theory of story-
Architectures, 25, 107–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bica.2018.07.003.
form memory construction, this study analogically trans- Akimoto, T. (2019). Key issues for generative narrative cognition in a
lated Genette’s narrative discourse theory into a cognitive cognitive system: Association and blending of stories. In Story-enabled
system. First, the three basics of a narrative, i.e., discourse, intelligence, AAAI 2019 spring symposium.
story, and narrating, are replaced by representational and Anderson, T. S. (2015). From episodic memory to narrative in a cognitive
architecture. In Proceedings of 6th workshop on computational models
procedural elements in a cognitive system. Second, story
of narrative (pp. 2–11).
construction methods and meta-story structures are formu- Bölöni, L. (2011). An investigation into the utility of episodic memory for
cognitive architectures. In Advances in cognitive systems, AAAI 2011
3 fall symposium (pp. 42–49).
Luhmann (2012–2013) incorporated the autopoiesis theory, which was
Faltersack, Z., Burns, B., Nuxoll, A., & Crenshaw, T. L. (2011). Ziggurat:
originally developed in the field of biology by Maturana and Varela
Steps toward a general episodic memory. In Advances in cognitive
(1980), into a theory for describing modern societies. In Luhmann’s social
systems, AAAI 2011 fall symposium (pp. 106–111).
systems theory, a person and the society are divided into different
Forbus, K. D., Gentner, D., & Law, K. (1994). MAC/FAC: A model of
autonomous systems, i.e., mental and social systems. The modern society
similarity-based retrieval. Cognitive Science, 19, 141–205. https://doi.
is characterized as a complex interrelationship among functionally
org/10.1016/0364-0213(95)90016-0.
differentiated subsystems including politics, economy, art, education,
Genette, G. (1980). Narrative discourse: An essay in method. NY: Cornell
science, religion, and family. The only element forming a social system is
University Press (Trans. Jane E. Lewin. Original work published
communication, whereas the communication is basically conducted via
1972).
mental systems. A social system autonomously operates through recursive
Gervás, P., Lönneker-Rodman, B., Meister, J. C., & Peinado, F. (2006).
chains of communication in which communication produces new com-
Narrative models: Narratology meets artificial intelligence. In Pro-
munication. Each of the social and mental systems is operationally closed,
ceedings of satellite workshop: Toward computational models of literary
however, it incorporates stimulations from other systems (i.e., environ-
analysis. 5th international conference on language resources and
ments). Hence, mental and social systems are closely coupled in the form
evaluation (pp. 44–51).
of interpenetration.
350 T. Akimoto / Cognitive Systems Research 58 (2019) 342–350

Kolodner, J. L. (1983). Maintaining organization in a dynamic long-term Akimoto (Eds.), Computational and cognitive approaches to narratology
memory. Cognitive Science, 7, 243–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364- (pp. 1–74). PA: IGI Global. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-5225-0432-
0213(83)80001-9. 0.ch001.
León, C. (2016). An architecture of narrative memory. Biologically Prince, G. (2003). A dictionary of narratology (revised ed.). NE: University
Inspired Cognitive Architectures, 16, 19–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. of Nebraska Press.
bica.2016.04.002. Samsonovich, A. V., & Aha, D. W. (2015). Character-oriented narrative
Lönneker, B. (2005). Narratological knowledge for natural language goal reasoning in autonomous actors. In Goal reasoning: papers from
generation. In Proceedings of the 10th European workshop on natural the ACS workshop, technical report GT-IRIM-CR-2015-001
language generation (pp. 91–100). (pp. 166–181).
Luhmann, N. (2012–2013). Theory of society (vols. 1–2). CA: Stanford Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory: A theory of reminding and
University Press (Trans. R. Barrett. Original work published 1997). learning in computers and people. NY: Cambridge University Press.
Mani, I. (2013). Computational modeling of narrative. CA: Morgan & Schank, R. C. (1990). Tell me a story: A new look at real and artificial
Claypool. memory. NY: Charles Scribner’s Sons.
Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The Szilas, N. (2015). Towards narrative-based knowledge representation in
realization of the living. MA: D. Reidel Publishing Company. cognitive systems. In Proceedings of 6th workshop on computational
Ménager, D. H., & Choi, D. (2016). A robust implementation of episodic models of narrative (pp. 133–141).
memory for a cognitive architecture. In Proceedings of the 38th annual Thagard, P., Holyoak, K. J., Nelson, G., & Gochfeld, D. (1990). Analog
conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 620–625). retrieval by constraint satisfaction. Artificial Intelligence, 46, 259–310.
Montfort, N. (2007). Generating narrative variation in interactive fiction https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(90)90018-U.
(Ph.D. thesis). University of Pennsylvania. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Oxford
Nuxoll, A. M., & Laird, J. E. (2004). A cognitive model of episodic University Press.
memory integrated with a general cognitive architecture. In Proceed- Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review
ings of international conference on cognitive modeling (pp. 220–225). of Psychology, 53, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
Nuxoll, A. M., & Laird, J. E. (2012). Enhancing intelligent agents with psych.53.100901.135114.
episodic memory. Cognitive Systems Research, 17–18, 34–48. https:// Winston, P. H. (2012). The right way. Advances in Cognitive Systems, 1,
doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2011.10.002. 23–36.
Ogata, T. (2016). Computational and cognitive approaches to narratology
from the perspective of narrative generation. In T. Ogata & T.

You might also like